<<

Dispersal and Integration in Female

by

Kara Kristina Walker

Department of Evolutionary Anthropology Duke University

Date:______Approved:

______Anne Pusey, Supervisor

______Brian Hare

______Susan Alberts

______Christine Drea

Dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Evolutionary Anthropology in the Graduate School of Duke University

2015

ABSTRACT

Dispersal and Integration in Female Chimpanzees

by

Kara Kristina Walker

Department of Evolutionary Anthropology Duke University

Date:______Approved:

______Anne Pusey, Supervisor

______Brian Hare

______Susan Alberts

______Christine Drea

An abstract of a dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Evolutionary Anthropology in the Graduate School of Duke University

2015

Copyright by Kara Kristina Walker 2015

Abstract

In chimpanzees, most females disperse from the community in which they were born to reproduce in a new community, thereby eliminating the risk of inbreeding with

close kin. However, across sites, some females breed in their natal community, raising

questions about the flexibility of dispersal, the costs and benefits of different strategies

and the mitigation of costs associated with dispersal and integration. In this dissertation

I address these questions by combining long-term behavioral data and recent field

observations on maturing and young adult females in Gombe National Park with an

experimental manipulation of relationship formation in captive in the .

To assess the risk of inbreeding for females who do and do not disperse, 129

chimpanzees were genotyped and relatedness between each dyad was calculated. Natal

females were more closely related to adult community males than were immigrant

females. By examining the parentage of 58 surviving offspring, I found that natal

females were not more related to the sires of their offspring than were immigrant

females, despite three instances of close inbreeding. The sires of all offspring were less

related to the mothers than non-sires regardless of the mother’s residence status. These

results suggest that chimpanzees are capable of detecting relatedness and that, even

when remaining natal, females can largely avoid, though not eliminate, inbreeding.

Next, I examined whether dispersal was associated with energetic, social,

iv

physiological and/or reproductive costs by comparing immigrant (n=10) and natal (n=9) females of similar age (10 – 18 ) using 2358 hours of observational data. Natal and immigrant females did not differ in any energetic metric. Immigrant females received from resident females more frequently than natal females. Immigrants spent less time in and more time self-grooming than natal females. Immigrant females primarily associated with resident males, had more social partners and lacked close social allies. There was no difference in concentrations of fecal glucocorticoid metabolites in immigrant and natal females. Immigrants gave birth 2.5 years later than natal females, but the survival of their first offspring did not differ. These results show that immigrant females in Gombe National Park do not face energetic deficits upon transfer, but they do enter a hostile social environment and have a delayed first birth.

Next, I examined whether chimpanzees use condition- and phenotype- dependent cues in making dispersal decisions. I examined the effect of social and environmental conditions present at the time females of known age matured (n=25) on the females’ dispersal decisions. Females were more likely to disperse if they had more male maternal relatives and thus, a high risk of inbreeding. Females with a high ranking mother and multiple maternal female kin tended to disperse less frequently, suggesting that a strong female kin network provides benefits to the maturing daughter. Females were also somewhat less likely to disperse when fewer unrelated males were present in the group. Habitat quality and intrasexual competition did not affect dispersal decisions.

v

Using a larger sample of 62 females observed as adults in Gombe, I also detected an

effect of phenotypic differences in personality on the female’s dispersal decisions;

extraverted, agreeable and open females were less likely to disperse.

Natural observations show that apes use grooming and play as social currency, but no experimental manipulations have been carried out to measure the effects of these behaviors on relationship formation, an essential component of integration. Thirty

chimpanzees and 25 were given a choice between an unfamiliar who

had recently groomed or played with them over one who did not. Both species showed a

preference for the human that had interacted with them, though the effect was driven by

males. These results support the idea that grooming and play act as social currency in

great apes that can rapidly shape social relationships between unfamiliar individuals.

Further investigation is needed to elucidate the use of social currency in female apes.

I conclude that dispersal in female chimpanzees is flexible and the balance of

costs and benefits varies for each individual. Females likely take into account social cues

present at maturity and their own phenotype in choosing a settlement path and are

especially sensitive to the presence of maternal male kin. The primary cost associated

with philopatry is inbreeding risk and the primary cost associated with dispersal is

delay in the age at first birth, presumably resulting from intense social competition.

Finally, apes may strategically make use of affiliative behavior in pursuing particular

relationships, something that should be useful in the integration process.

vi

Dedication

This dissertation is dedicated to Richard Albert Myren. As an academic himself, my grandfather was one of the few who understood the enormity of completing a dissertation and he was immensely proud that I was following in his footsteps.

vii

Contents

Abstract ...... iv

List of Tables ...... xii

List of Figures ...... xiv

Acknowledgements ...... xvi

1. Introduction ...... 1

1.1 Dispersal ...... 1

1.1.1 Dispersal in ...... 2

1.2 Social Organization ...... 6

1.2.1 Female Competition ...... 8

1.3 Chimpanzee Dispersal ...... 12

1.4 Conclusion ...... 20

1.5 Specific Contributions from Co-Authors ...... 22

2. Chimpanzees reproduce with genetically dissimilar mates regardless of dispersal status ...... 23

2.1 Methods ...... 27

2.1.1 Study Site ...... 27

2.1.2 Genotyping and Paternity Analysis ...... 27

2.1.3 Relatedness Analysis ...... 28

2.2 Results ...... 31

2.2.1 Paternity and Inbreeding ...... 31

2.2.2 Relatedness between natal or immigrant females and resident males ...... 35

viii

2.2.3 Relatedness between natal and immigrant breeding dyads ...... 35

2.2.4 Relatedness between breeding and non-breeding dyads ...... 37

2.3 Discussion ...... 41

3. Dispersal is socially, but not energetically, costly in female chimpanzees of Gombe National Park...... 47

3.1 Methods ...... 59

3.1.1 Study Site and Subjects ...... 59

3.1.2 Data Collection ...... 64

3.1.2.1 Instantaneous Samples ...... 64

3.1.2.2 Travel Distance ...... 65

3.1.2.3 Feeding ...... 65

3.1.2.4 Social Behavior ...... 66

3.1.2.5 Physiological Data ...... 70

3.1.2.6 Reproductive Costs ...... 72

3.1.3 Analyses ...... 74

3.1.3.1 Travel Costs Post-Transfer ...... 75

3.1.3.2 Diet Quality and Effort ...... 75

3.1.3.3 Social Costs ...... 76

3.1.3.4 Physiological Costs ...... 76

3.1.3.5 Reproductive Costs ...... 77

3.2 Results ...... 78

3.2.1 Travel Costs ...... 78

ix

3.2.1.1 Dispersal ...... 78

3.2.1.2 Travel Costs Post-Transfer ...... 79

3.2.2 Feeding Costs ...... 80

3.2.3 Social Costs ...... 82

3.2.3.1 Grooming ...... 82

3.2.3.2 Aggression ...... 84

3.2.3.3 ...... 88

3.2.3.4 Combined Association Index ...... 89

3.2.4 Physiological Costs...... 92

3.2.5 Reproductive Costs ...... 95

3.3 Discussion ...... 102

4. What factors influence dispersal decisions in chimpanzees? ...... 114

4.1 Methods ...... 125

4.1.1 Study Site ...... 125

4.1.2 Frequency of Dispersal ...... 126

4.1.3 Condition Dependent Dispersal ...... 128

4.1.3.1 Presence of Female Kin ...... 129

4.1.3.2 Inbreeding Risk ...... 130

4.1.3.3 Unrelated Males ...... 131

4.1.3.4 Intrasexual Competition ...... 131

4.1.3.5 Habitat Quality ...... 133

4.1.4 Personality ...... 133

x

4.1.4.1 Subjects ...... 135

4.1.5 Data Analysis ...... 136

4.1.5.1 Condition Dependent Dispersal ...... 136

4.1.5.2 Personality ...... 136

4.2 Results ...... 137

4.2.1 Frequency of Dispersal ...... 137

4.2.2 Condition Dependent Dispersal ...... 141

4.2.3 Personality ...... 143

4.3 Discussion ...... 145

5. Experimental evidence that grooming and play are social currency in chimpanzees and bonobos ...... 154

5.1 Methods ...... 158

5.1.1 Test Procedure ...... 161

5.1.1.1 Baseline ...... 162

5.1.1.2 Test Session ...... 163

5.1.1.3 Interaction Period ...... 164

5.1.2 Coding and Analysis ...... 164

5.2 Results ...... 166

5.3 Discussion ...... 169

6. Conclusion ...... 175

References ...... 186

Biography ...... 216

xi

List of Tables

Table 1: Paternities and related demographic and genetic information for 62 offspring. 33

Table 2: List of subjects with demographic information...... 62

Table 3: List of subjects by data set, study period and observation hours...... 63

Table 4: Females used in analyses of FGM levels ...... 72

Table 5: Mean and standard deviation for time spent traveling and resting...... 80

Table 6: Linear mixed model results for traveling metrics showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes...... 80

Table 7: Mean and standard deviation of three foraging metrics ...... 81

Table 8: Linear mixed model results for three foraging metrics showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes...... 81

Table 9: Mean and standard deviation for seven grooming metrics...... 83

Table 10: Linear mixed model results for grooming metrics showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes...... 83

Table 11: Mean and standard deviation for three aggression metrics...... 85

Table 12: Linear mixed model results for received aggression showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes...... 85

Table 13: Frequency of aggression against immigrant and natal females by all sexually mature Kasekela females present in the second study period (2011-2014)...... 87

Table 14: Mean and standard deviation for combined association index...... 89

Table 15: Linear mixed model results for combined association index showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes...... 89

Table 16: Top three association (CAI) and grooming partners by sex and maternal relatedness for each female during the study period...... 91

Table 17: Mean and standard deviation for FGM ...... 93

xii

Table 18: Linear mixed model results for fecal glucocorticoid metabolites showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes ...... 93

Table 19: Predictor variables...... 129

Table 20: Origin and Adult Community for all females surviving to adulthood in Kasekela and Mitumba during the study period...... 139

Table 21: Means, standard deviations and results of Mann-Whitney U tests for dispersers and non-dispersers across five measures...... 142

Table 22: Nominal logistic regression model selection results with effect sizes for each term...... 143

Table 23: Means, standard deviations and t-test results for six personality domains in dispersers and non-dispersers in the less conservative dataset ...... 143

Table 24: Means, standard deviations and t-test results for six personality domains in dispersers and non-dispersers in the conservative dataset ...... 144

Table 25: List of subjects by species, sex, age and condition...... 160

Table 26: Results of the GEE model ...... 167

Table 27: Results of the supplementary GEE model, restricted to males but including periods (first four/last four trials)...... 167

xiii

List of Figures

Figure 1: Female core areas in Gombe National Park from 2000 – 2002. A) Representative core areas for a high, middle and low ranking female within the community range. B) Core areas for all females present in Kasekela during this time. Figure reprinted from Pusey & Schroepfer-Walker (2013)...... 10

Figure 2: A) Distribution of Queller & Goodnight R for all male-female dyads in the population. B) Relatedness between Kasekela immigrant female-male dyads and natal female-male dyads (p < 0.01). Error bars represent standard error...... 35

Figure 3: Relatedness between immigrant mothers and sires compared to relatedness between natal mothers and sires (A) across communities (p = NS) and (B) in Kasekela only (p = NS). Error bars represent standard error...... 36

Figure 4: Mean relatedness between non-breeding and breeding dyads at each conception in (A) Kasekela (p < 0.01) and (B) Mitumba (p = NS). Error bars represent standard error...... 38

Figure 5: Relatedness between breeding and non-breeding dyads in Kasekela by residence status (Immigrant, p = 0.03; Natal, p = 0.075). Error bars represent standard error...... 39

Figure 6: Distribution of Queller & Goodnight R between immigrant female-male dyads in Kasekela (KK) and Mitumba (MT)...... 40

Figure 7: Mean percent time in- and out-grooming by partner sex and residence status for natal and immigrant females. Error bars represent standard error...... 84

Figure 8: Received aggression per 1000 hours for natal and immigrant females separated by the sex of the aggressor. Error bars represent standard error...... 86

Figure 9: Copulation Rate/100 Hours in immigrant females (grey bars) and two natal females; Flirt (triangles) and Eowyn (squares). Error bars represent the standard deviation in copulation rate for nine immigrant females...... 88

Figure 10: Distribution of the Combined Association Index by residence status and sex of the association partner...... 90

xiv

Figure 11: Mean FGM levels by residence status and estrous state. FGM levels in resident estrous females are significantly greater than in immigrant anestrous and estrous females. Error bars represent standard error...... 94

Figure 12: Kaplan-Meier survival analysis. A- Proportion of females giving birth by a particular age for immigrant (red) and natal (blue) females (p = 0.07) using conservative inclusion criteria. B- Proportion of females giving birth for the first time by a particular age for immigrant and natal females (p < 0.0001) using less conservative inclusion criteria...... 96

Figure 13: Kaplan-Meier survival analysis. A- Proportion of females mating with an adult male by a particular age for immigrant and natal known-aged females (p = NS). B- Proportion of females giving birth for the first time by a particular age (excluding sterile females) for immigrant and natal known-aged females (p = 0.02)...... 98

Figure 14: Kaplan-Meier survival analysis on the time from immigration to first birth for all females (excluding sterile females) in Kasekela (red) and Mitumba (blue) (p = NS). .. 99

Figure 15: Kaplan-Meier survival analysis. Proportion of offspring surviving to a particular age for immigrant (red) and natal females (blue) in the conservative (A) and less conservative data sets (B) (p = NS)...... 101

Figure 16: Z-Transformed means for each predictive factor by dispersal status. Error bars represent standard error...... 142

Figure 17: Mean personality measures across six personality domains for non-dispersers and dispersers in the less conservative data set (A) and the conservative data set (B). Extraversion differs in both data sets while agreeableness and openness differ in the less conservative data set (p < 0.5). Error bars represent standard error...... 144

Figure 18: The Y-axis represents the proportion of trials the experimenter who played with the subject was chosen over an experimenter who did not in the baseline and test conditions for both species, separated by sex in the play condition. Error bars represent standard error...... 168

Figure 19: The Y-axis represents the proportion of trials the experimenter who groomed the subject or “actor” was chosen over an experimenter who did not in the baseline and test conditions for both species separated by sex in the groom condition. Error bars represent standard error...... 168

xv

Acknowledgements

This dissertation would not have been possible without the help and support of numerous colleagues, friends and family members. Each chapter represents a collaborative effort between myself and fellow scientists interested in chimpanzee socioecology. The first three chapters build upon over 50 years of field work at Gombe

National Park, . This work was made possible by and the ongoing support of the Jane Goodall Institute and the Gombe Stream Research Center (GSRC).

Permission for this work was granted by the Tanzania National Parks Authority, the

Tanzania Commission for Science and and the Tanzanian Wildlife Research

Institute. Construction of the long-term database was supported by grants from the NSF

(DBS-9021946, SBR-9319909, BCS-0452315, IOS-LTREB-1052693).

When on the ground in Gombe I relied upon the local expertise of Deus Mjungu and Anthony Collins, Directors of Research at GSRC. Upon my arrival in Gombe for the first time in 2011, the field staff at GSRC, under the of Gabo Paulo, took me under their wing and patiently worked with me as I learned the names and faces of over

75 chimpanzees in two communities. After I struck out on my own with my field team, the research staff continued to provide invaluable assistance in tracking and following the chimpanzees. My field team of Amri Yahaya, Audax Gabo and Nasibu Zubery contributed thousands of observation hours trekking after skittish young females, a group even the veteran field assistants avoid following. My team stuck with it for over

xvi

three years and did an excellent job of habituating the young immigrants and doing the

less desirable activities such as collecting fecal samples and conducting urine

tests. Without them I would still be stuck in a vine tangle in Gombe wondering where

the chimpanzees disappeared to. When back at Duke, several eager and dedicated

undergraduate students helped me enter and analyze over 3000 hours of data. Patricia

DeLacey was especially invested in this project and entered the bulk of the data. My

field work was supported by the Duke Graduate School, the Duke Chapter of Sigma Xi

and the Leakey Foundation.

Emily Wroblewski guided me through the confusing world of genetics and was

my primary collaborator in quantifying inbreeding and relatedness among the Gombe

chimpanzees. I also benefitted from the expertise of and her colleagues

Miguel Ramirez and Rebecca Rudicell, who genotyped hundreds of fecal samples to

allow for genetic analysis of the Gombe chimpanzees. Genetic analyses were supported by grants from the NIH (R01 AI50529, R01 AI58715, P30 27767, F32 AI 085959-03).

Carson Murray paved the way for this study on young females, proving that they could

indeed be followed with a lot of perseverance and endurance. She generously gave me

access to her data set on young females collected during her dissertation work and has

guided my scientific thoughts on the immigration and integration processes. I was

provided the privilege of working with Alex Weiss upon receipt of a graduate student

fellowship at NESCent and he encouraged me to apply his knowledge of chimpanzee

xvii

personality to female transfer, an emerging area of research across species. Under his

guidance I entered the fraught world of personality studies and came out the other end

with a thorough understanding of the field. Finally, I must thank my lab mates for

keeping me sane, answering all of my mundane statistical inquires and helping me

through hours of endless query making in Access. Ian Gilby and Steffen Foerster bore

the brunt of my questions on statistics and Access while Emily Boehm, Joseph Feldblum

and Christopher Krupenye were always available for quick consults, engaging

discussion and, most importantly, to provide necessary distraction when needed.

My work at Lola ya and the Tchimpounga Chimpanzee Rehabilitation

Center was made possible by Claudine Andre and Rebeca Atencia, respectively. I

received permission for this work through the Ministry of Scientific Research and

Technological Innovation in Republic of Congo and the Ministry of Research and the

Ministry of the Environment in the Democratic Republic of Congo. This work was

supported by grants from the NSF (BCS-08-27552-02, BCS-10-25172) to Brian Hare. I

could not have conducted these experiments without the input and expertise of my co-

author, Victoria Wobber, and the dedicated care takers who participated as

experimenters. In particular, Delphin Bilua proved invaluable in working with finicky bonobos at Lola ya Bonobo. Evan McLean, Alexandra Rosati and Jingzhi Tan provided excellent company in Congo and showed me the ropes of cognitive research. I also thank

Aisha Goulab and Carley Mobley who coded the experimental data, and Sunil

xviii

Sunchidaran who provided statistical help. Shinya Yamamoto and two anonymous

reviewers provided helpful comments as this work was getting published.

I would like to thank my committee members, Susan Alberts and Christine Drea,

who have provided valuable feedback on this project and have helped mold my

scientific thinking from start to finish. My advisors, Anne Pusey and Brian Hare, deserve

more thanks than I can give. Brian took a chance on me as a lab manager and stood by

me as I applied to the graduate program. He gave me incredible opportunities as a lab

manager and early graduate student to get my feet wet in cognitive research and has been of continued support as I transitioned to working primarily on wild chimpanzees.

Anne has kept me grounded throughout my career and has been patient with me as I

navigated through graduate school. I can only one day hope to know as much as she

does about chimpanzee social behavior and about the individual lives of the Gombe

chimpanzees. Lisa Jones has been a constant source of companionship, support and

guidance in all departmental matters, and deserves special mention.

Last, but not least, I am incredibly thankful to my family and friends. Without

their support none of this would be possible. Lauren Gonzales and I entered the

department as the sole members of the class of 2015 and we have stuck by each other’s

side through all of the ups and downs of completing a PhD. I couldn’t imagine having a

more supportive cohort mate. Additional graduate students who came before and after

me became great friends and a natural support system where we could complain and

xix

celebrate together. My family cheered me on as I pursued my crazy dream of becoming an anthropologist and kept me going until the end. My parents, Nina and Terry

Schroepfer, allowed me to pursue my interests in primates , even as a young child, and never held me back when I kept coming to them with wild ideas to travel to far flung places and earn numerous degrees. My sisters, Katrina and Rebecca, have always been there to provide advice and keep me moving forward.

As it turns out, I began my graduate career and my relationship with my husband, Christopher Walker, at the same time. Chris was a ahead in the program and eased my transition from a lab manager to a graduate student. We learned together and supported each other through each requirement and project along the way.

Somewhere in between classes, research and field trips we managed to get married and start a family, none of which would have been possible without creative thinking and unending support from him and others. Though he is an expert in he embraced the world of chimpanzee research and eagerly traveled to Gombe to meet the stars of my research. And to conclude, I am deeply grateful to my son, Bennett Walker, whose impending arrival kept me motivated to finish this dissertation in the allotted five years. May he grow up to love and science as much as his parents!

xx

1. Introduction

1.1 Dispersal

Dispersal, a key component to socioecology, is the simple act of moving

from one location to another. Natal dispersal, when an individual moves from its place

of origin to the place it reproduces (Howard, 1960; Greenwood, 1980), occurs most

frequently and plays a major role in population regulation, spatial distribution and

social systems. In most vertebrates, only a portion of the population disperses while the

remainder stay in their natal area to breed. In some animals, including many birds and

, this division is primarily marked by sex. The identity of the dispersing sex is

thought to relate to the social system of the organism; in species where males take an

active role in provisioning young and defending a territory they have the most to gain

through philopatry so females disperse (Greenwood, 1980). In contrast, in polygnyous

species, including many social mammals, males provide little to no parental care and the benefits of philopatry accrue to the females who invest heavily in offspring (Greenwood,

1980). Alternatively, some argue that the identity of the dispersing sex is better

explained by the presence of a shared common ancestor and that mating systems only

play a limited role in predicting which sex disperses (Di Fiore and Rendall, 1994; Perrin

and Mazalov, 1999).

The evolutionary explanation for sex-biased dispersal commonly includes some

combination of inbreeding avoidance and intrasexual competition (Greenwood, 1980;

1

Dobson, 1982; Moore and Ali, 1984; Pusey, 1987; Perrin and Mazalov, 1999). In mammals, intrasexual competition cannot adequately explain near complete sex-biased dispersal and inbreeding avoidance must play a substantial evolutionary role in dispersal (Pusey, 1987; Pusey and Wolf, 1996). Game theoretical models support the assertion that inbreeding avoidance, over competition, drives the of sex-biased natal dispersal patterns across animals (Gandon, 1999; Perrin and Mazalov, 1999).

Finally, female biased dispersal is more common in mammalian species where male tenure exceeds the age at which daughter’s reach maturity, providing empirical support for the importance of inbreeding in dispersal (Clutton-Brock and Lukas, 2012).

Which sex disperses has important consequences for the ensuing social system.

Members of the philopatric sex will be more related to each other than the dispersing sex and consequently are expected to have higher rates of cooperation (Wrangham, 1980), which can affect food acquisition, defense and other aspects of sociality. Dispersal also influences bonding, status of the sexes, mate acquisition, group defense and ranging patterns (Ghiglieri, 1987; Marlowe, 2004). Accordingly, to understand a species’ socioecology one must first understand its dispersal patterns.

1.1.1 Dispersal in Primates

In group-living primates, like most social mammals, male-biased dispersal is the prevalent pattern and is commonly found in strepsirrhines, some platyrrhines and

2

cercopithecoids (Pusey and Packer, 1987). Female primates invest heavily in

reproduction through gestation and lactation (Trivers, 1972) and, correspondingly, have

more to gain through familiarity and the presence of female kin allies (Wrangham,

1980). However, within each exceptions occur and are useful in understanding

general tenets of dispersal theory. Bisexual dispersal, where some members of both

sexes disperse, is somewhat common in species and is found in several langur

species (Stanford, 1991; Sterck et al., 2005), some howler monkeys (Glander, 1992;

Crockett and Pope, 1993), Milne-Edwards sifakas (Morelli et al., 2009), woolly monkeys

(Di Fiore et al., 2009), (Harcourt et al., 1976; Robbins et al., 2004) and hamadryas baboons (Hammond et al., 2006). In some instances the context of dispersal varies for each sex. For example, members of one sex may be evicted or kidnapped while the other disperses voluntarily or one sex may transfer between groups while the other enters a solitary phase (Pusey and Packer, 1987). Female dispersal coupled with male philopatry is rare and has only been described in (Struhsaker, 2010), muriquis (Printes and Strier, 1999), spider monkeys (Asensio et al., 2008), Costa Rican monkeys

(Boinski and Mitchell, 1994), chimpanzees (Pusey and Packer, 1987) and bonobos (Kano,

1992). In some species where females disperse, their diets consist of low quality but evenly distributed foods that are not easily defended by groups of females, reducing the benefit to female philopatry (Sterck et al., 1997). However, spider monkeys, muriquis

and African apes are all large-bodied frugivorous species where resources are clumped

3

and within-group competition is high. Competition may be reduced through fission-

fusion social systems though the occurrence of female dispersal in these species may be better explained by male reproductive strategies (Pusey and Packer, 1987).

The clade is marked by variability in both dispersal patterns and social

systems; however all African apes show some degree of female dispersal (Pusey and

Packer, 1987). Because dispersal lays the foundation for the ensuing social system,

dispersal patterns in extant apes are especially relevant to understanding human

evolution (Ghiglieri, 1987; Koenig and Borries, 2012). The study of dispersal patterns in

hominin evolution is limited by methodological considerations but creative work in the

field indicates , and possibly their ancestors, traditionally show female or bisexual dispersal. Early anthropological surveys found that 70% of catalogued human

societies are characterized by male philopatry and female dispersal (Murdock, 1967),

including 62% of hunter-gatherer societies (Ember, 1978). Genetic studies show some

evidence of increased diversity in mtDNA as compared to the Y-, the

expected result if males are philopatric and females disperse (Seielstad et al., 1998;

Pilkington et al., 2008), though the signal is stronger in agricultural populations than in

hunter-gatherers. In the first analysis of dispersal in fossil hominins,

africanus and robustus females have more diverse isotopic signatures than

comparable males, indicating that females originated from outside the region of death,

4

while males did not (Copeland et al., 2011). A similar analysis of Neolithic skeletons found similar results (Bentley, 2013).

In contrast, recent analyses on a subset of hunter-gatherer societies for which more data exist support a bilocal pattern of dispersal where marital residence is flexible and can change over an individual’s lifetime (Marlowe, 2004; Hill et al., 2011). On a global scale there is no difference in Y-chromosome or mtDNA variation that can be attributed to sex-biased dispersal (Wilder et al., 2004). A recent review of hominoid dispersal that reanalyzes behavioral and fossil evidence finds more support for a dispersal system similar to that seen in gorillas for early hominins (Koenig and Borries,

2012).

Since the ape-human split, the hominin lineage has acquired incredibly advanced social cognition that supports high levels of culture, communication and collaboration

(Tomasello and Herrmann, 2010). Many hypothesize that these advancements are linked to changes in the hominin dispersal pattern (Chapais, 2010; Hill et al., 2011). If individual hominins maintained consanguinal relationships after leaving their natal community or group, affiliative or tolerant intergroup encounters may have become more frequent (Rodseth et al., 1991). The maintenance of these relationships may explain the high level of intergroup affinity observed in human groups when compared to non- human primate groups (Rodseth et al., 1991; Chapais, 2010; Hill et al., 2011). This pattern could emerge under a system of female biased or bisexual dispersal and simply requires

5

that individuals can recognize their maternal, paternal and affinal kin (Rodseth et al.,

1991, Hill et al., 2011, Kramer & Greaves, 2011). Such a system allows for differential interactions in intergroup encounters and would promote cooperation between particular groups (Ghiglieri, 1987; Chapais, 2010; Hill et al., 2011).

As shown above, there is currently disagreement in characterizing the historical dispersal pattern of early humans. Given the inherent biases in relying on modern day hunter-gatherers (Marlowe, 2005), scant fossil evidence and inconclusive genetic evidence, our closest living relatives serve as a useful referential model in which to study dispersal (Ghiglieri, 1987; Koenig and Borries, 2012). Specifically, studying dispersal in African apes can be useful in reconstructing the ancestral condition and allow for the identification of phylogenetically conserved and derived traits in hominin socioecology. There is currently a growing body of long-term data from multiple chimpanzee field sites that is ripe for a thorough analysis of the dispersal and integration period. Such an analysis will contribute to the debate on dispersal in hominins by clarifying patterns seen in chimpanzees, one of our closest living relatives.

1.2 Chimpanzee Social Organization

Chimpanzee social organization differs from that of most primate species in three ways. First, chimpanzees have a fission-fusion social system; individuals form permanent social groups but travel in parties that vary in size and composition

6

throughout the day (Aureli et al., 2008). Parties can be comprised of mixed sex, same sexed or lone individuals (Nishida, 1968; Goodall, 1986; Boesch, 1996). Second, as discussed above, males are philopatric while females disperse around sexual maturity

(Pusey and Packer, 1987). Last, males are more gregarious than females and range over a wider area while females spend more time in small parties or alone and, especially in the eastern sub species, limit their ranging to a subset of the group range (Nishida, 1968;

Wrangham and Smuts, 1980; Goodall, 1986; Wrangham, 2000; Mitani et al., 2002;

Williams et al., 2002b; Emery Thompson et al., 2007; Lehmann and Boesch, 2008).

Males strive for high dominance rank and will engage in fierce male-male competition to rise in the hierarchy and gain better access to fertile females (Goodall,

1986; Muller, 2002; Boesch et al., 2006; Wroblewski et al., 2009). Females also compete and can be ranked, though they do so in a more nuanced fashion and rarely participate in loud, aggressive contests that are common among males. Dominant females also accrue benefits; including a higher quality foraging area (Murray et al., 2007) and diet

(Murray et al., 2006), less seasonal variation in weight (Pusey et al., 2005) and, ultimately, increased reproductive success (Pusey et al., 1997). The in chimpanzees is highly promiscuous; females mate with most or all males during a typical cycle (Goodall, 1986; Nishida and Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, 1987). Females are more restrictive in their mating patterns during their peri-ovulatory period though whether

7

this is due to female choice or sexual coercion is an active area of debate (Stumpf and

Boesch, 2006; Muller et al., 2007; Feldblum et al., 2014).

1.2.1 Female Competition

Female chimpanzees initially proved difficult to study because they were hard to locate and often traveled alone. Therefore early studies emphasized the gregarious of male chimpanzees and over simplified female chimpanzees as solitary foragers that face little competition (Wrangham, 1979). New evidence shows that female chimpanzees strategically compete with each other to maximize access to resources

(Pusey and Schroepfer-Walker, 2013). Ranging patterns in female chimpanzees reflect this competition. When females are not sexually receptive they spend more time traveling alone or in small groups with other females and less time in mixed sex-parties

(Nishida, 1968; Wrangham and Smuts, 1980; Mitani et al., 2002; Lehmann and Boesch,

2008). While males travel widely throughout the community range, females primarily forage in a restricted portion of the range (Wrangham and Smuts, 1980; Hasegawa, 1990;

Wrangham et al., 1992). The degree to which females restrict their ranges varies considerably between West and East African sites. In East Africa, anestrous females concentrate their foraging in distinct, though overlapping core areas that only comprise a small portion of the community range (Wrangham and Smuts, 1980; Wrangham et al.,

1992; Williams et al., 2002b). Females show high site fidelity to these areas though will shift or shrink their core areas as the community boundaries and group composition

8

change across time (Williams et al., 2002b). Such restricted ranging does not occur at Taï

National Park in where females make use of the majority of their range

(Lehmann and Boesch, 2005). As ripe-fruit specialists, large groups of chimpanzees can

quickly exhaust food patches requiring travel to a new site. Females with offspring

travel more slowly than unencumbered males and are at a disadvantage in food

competition (Wrangham, 2000). Females reduce this competition by traveling in smaller

parties and concentrating their foraging in familiar areas where they can reduce travel

time and maximize efficiency (Pusey and Schroepfer-Walker, 2013).

The location of an individual female’s core area has long-term consequences. In

the Kasekela community of Gombe National Park, where these ranging patterns have been studied in detail, high ranking females occupy smaller and higher quality core areas that are often located in the center of the community range, making encounters with hostile neighbors unlikely and limiting travel distance between food resources

(Murray et al., 2007). In the Gombe chimpanzees, dominance rank is correlated with several measures of reproductive success thus linking core area quality and fitness

(Pusey et al., 1997). At Kanyawara, where ranging patterns have also been a focus of study, there is a direct correlation between core area quality and reproductive success

(Emery Thompson et al., 2007). As female density within a community increases, available resources will decrease for each female. Accordingly, females should be motivated to exclude rivals from their core area and do so in Kasekela in Gombe (Miller

9

et al., 2014). Females may also push out competitors from ideal locations as they rise in rank or acquire kin allies.

Figure 1: Female core areas in Gombe National Park from 2000 – 2002. A) Representative core areas for a high, middle and low ranking female within the community range. B) Core areas for all females present in Kasekela during this time. Figure reprinted from Pusey & Schroepfer-Walker (2013).

The unique dispersion system of female chimpanzees encourages indirect competition primarily through avoidance behaviors. However, females do compete directly and will use aggression when the pay-off is expected to be large. Overt 10

aggression between females most often occurs in direct feeding contexts (Goodall, 1986;

Muller, 2002; Wittig and Boesch, 2003) where the winner gains immediate access to food.

However, females also use well-placed aggression when the rewards will accrue in the long-term by removing or weakening future competitors (Pusey and Schroepfer-Walker,

2013). Resident females are hostile towards new immigrants (Nishida, 1979; Pusey, 1979;

Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a) and will form coalitions to repel newcomers (Townsend et al., 2007; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a; Pusey et al., 2008).

Females also occasionally commit infanticide, which has been interpreted as a mechanism to remove a future competitor (Goodall, 1977; Townsend et al., 2007; Pusey et al., 2008). Aggressive interactions are hypothesized to be rare among females due to the risk of bodily harm to both the individual and her offspring and are therefore, only used when the payoff is substantial.

Bonds between females may serve as a mechanism to mitigate female-female competition. Strong bonds between mothers and their adult daughters were first recognized at Gombe (Goodall, 1986) and stable bonds between unrelated females have now been found at Taï (Lehmann and Boesch, 2009), Kanyawara (Gilby and Wrangham,

2008), Ngogo (Langergraber et al., 2009; Wakefield, 2013) and Gombe (Foerster et al.,

2015). In some primate species, strong bonds between individuals are correlated with increases in reproductive success (Silk, 2007; Silk et al., 2009, 2010; Schülke et al., 2010) and though not yet studied, chimpanzees may reap similar benefits. In particular, allies

11

that range in the same neighborhood should be helpful in reducing aggressive competition and lead to reproductive benefits.

1.3 Chimpanzee Dispersal

Female dispersal has been confirmed at all chimpanzee field sites while adult male dispersal is extremely rare and has only occurred at Bossou in West Africa

(Sugiyama, 1999, 2004), though juveniles and adolescents have immigrated with female relatives in several instances (Goodall, 1986; Nishida et al., 2003). Philopatry is thought to be especially beneficial to male chimpanzees because they can cooperate with kin and cohort mates to defend a large territory that contains many females (Williams et al.,

2004). Thus, in order to avoid breeding with close relatives, females must disperse to neighboring communities. Inbreeding is generally detrimental to overall fitness through the accumulation of deleterious homozygous alleles and adverse effects have been identified in chimpanzees, both in captive (Ralls and Ballou, 1982) and wild populations

(Constable et al., 2001). As a consequence of the dispersal system, community males are expected to be more related to one another than females are to one another. This pattern has been documented at Mahale (Inoue et al., 2008) and Gombe (Schroepfer-Walker et al., 2013), though evidence is mixed at Taï (Vigilant et al., 2001) and at Budongo no such difference occurs (Lukas et al., 2005).

12

Upon reaching sexual maturity, females have some flexibility in choosing where

to breed. In the majority of studied communities, over 90% of females disperse to a

different breeding community (Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Nishida et al.,

2003; Reynolds, 2005; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a; Langergraber et al., 2009). The Kasekela

community in Gombe National Park is an exception to this pattern. Approximately half

of all females born into Kasekela over a fifty-year period have reproduced in their natal

community (Pusey et al., 1997; unpublished data). In rare cases, some females will

secondarily transfer after giving birth either in their natal or immigrant community

(Nishida, 1979; Goodall, 1986; Emery Thompson et al., 2006). This pattern primarily

occurs when a group dissolves and has been seen in Mahale and Gombe (Nishida, 1979;

Rudicell et al., 2010) and is presumed to have occurred in Budongo (Emery Thompson et

al., 2006). Overall, these patterns suggest that females assess their dispersal options prior

to making a settlement decision.

The onset of adolescence in female chimpanzees is marked by small infrequent

swellings of the clitoris that typically begin between 8 and 9 years and gradually

increase to full size by 12 years (Pusey, 1990). This marks the beginning of a suite of behavioral and physical changes that prepare a female for adulthood including a

decrease in play behavior and an increase in interest in infants and adult males (Nishida,

1983; Pusey, 1990). All females experience a period of sub-fecundity following the onset

of full cycles when, despite experiencing regular sexual swellings and mating with adult

13

males, they fail to conceive or sustain (Nishida et al., 2003; Emery

Thompson, 2013). After full cycles begin females decrease their time spent in association with their mothers and increase time spent with adolescent and adult males (Pusey,

1980, 1990; Stumpf et al., 2009). Young females mate in their natal community during this time and will do so with male relatives (Pusey, 1990; Stumpf et al., 2009). At Gombe, females dramatically decrease association time with males that they were close with during the juvenile period, presumably because these males were familiar and therefore, likely to be related (Pusey, 1980). This phase of regularly occurring cycles generally lasts for about one year, though it may continue for up to 8 years prior to immigration or settlement in the natal community (Pusey, 1990; Nishida et al., 2003; Emery Thompson,

2013). Mating within the natal community may provide necessary sexual experiences and allow females to identify high-quality males prior to immigration or conception

(Pusey, 1990). Emigration generally occurs between 11 and 13 years, an age that is fairly consistent across field sites (Emery Thompson, 2013). During this period, many females will commence visits to neighboring communities, typically setting out when in estrous and returning to their natal community when anestrous (Nishida, 1979; Pusey, 1979;

Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000). In the only detailed study yet conducted on the timing of dispersal, neither chronological age nor time since first maximal swelling was predictive of dispersal nor was dispersal associated with a shift in glucocorticoids in

Kanyawara females (Stumpf et al., 2009). Rather, females emigrated when they had a

14

positive energy balance and after decreasing association time with their mothers

(Stumpf et al., 2009). These findings suggest that the period prior to dispersal is not

stressful to young females but that females may time dispersal to coincide with optimal body condition, better preparing them to face difficult times ahead. Moreover, strong bonds between a young female and individuals in her natal community, most

importantly her mother, may delay time to dispersal (Stumpf et al., 2009). Most females

disperse with a full , ensuring that adult males in their transfer

community will find them maximally attractive (Deschner and Boesch, 2007). There is no

consistent evidence that young females are evicted from their natal community by

individuals of either sex. Instead, females appear to choose to transfer on their own

accord and dispersal is likely driven by an attraction to novel mates (Pusey, 1979;

Nishida, 1989).

Once arriving in a new group, young females must overcome significant

obstacles before beginning reproduction. Most urgently, they need to integrate socially

and establish their own core area. The integration period has not been well studied due

to the difficulty in habituating and following new arrivals. However, a gross

understanding of integration has emerged and is consistent across field sites. Resident

males are generally accepting of incoming females as new mating partners (Pusey, 1990;

Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a). In contrast, resident

females seem to regard immigrants as new feeding competitors and are almost

15

universally hostile to newcomers (Nishida, 1989; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a; Pusey et al.,

2008). Immigrants enter the female hierarchy at or near the bottom (Nishida, 1979;

Murray et al., 2006; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a) and are likely to face foraging deficiencies

as they navigate unfamiliar terrain and establish their own core area. This costly period

is expected to last several years and delay the onset of reproduction in dispersing

females (Nishida et al., 2003; Emery Thompson, 2013). Individual integration strategies,

such as quickly establishing relationships with dominant females, may allow particular

females to shorten the length of the integration period.

Females that do not transfer likely also incur integration costs, though they may be significantly reduced. Natal females must establish an adult foraging area and enter

the female . For individuals whose mother is still alive, she can be

an essential ally in the integration and settlement process. The process is likely to be

more difficult for natal orphans, though they may still benefit from pre-existing

relationships with community females. In some cases, females (especially those whose

mothers have died) establish their core areas in a different location from that of their

mothers (Pusey, 1980); this is hypothesized to reduce encounters with maternal brothers

who, when alone, range in their mother’s core area (Murray et al., 2008). These females

must then establish bonds with residents whom they have not previously associated

with at high levels, approximating the situation faced by immigrating females.

Regardless of settlement location, in Kanyawara, natal females do not experience the

16

high rates of aggression directed at immigrants (Kahlenberg et al., 2008a). Nevertheless, natal females also tend to enter the hierarchy near the bottom (Nishida, 1989; Wittig and

Boesch, 2003; Murray et al., 2006; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a) and should face the consequences of low-rank in foraging effort.

Given the high cost of dispersal, females should employ strategies to mitigate these costs and should be responsive to phenotypic and environmental cues in making dispersal decisions, a phenomenon seen in many species (Ims and Hjermann, 2001). If true, females should be able to assess their own condition as well as that of their social and physical environment to reproduce under the most favorable conditions. For some females this may necessitate dispersal while in others it will promote philopatry.

Philopatry eliminates the costs associated with dispersal but is expected to have varying reproductive consequences. In Kasekela, natal females have successfully reproduced and raised offspring to adulthood (Pusey et al., 1997). In contrast, natal females in the

Kanyawara (R. Wrangham, pers. comm.) and Taï (Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000) communities have poor reproductive output while reproductive success of natal females in other communities is unknown. If females’ reproduction will suffer by staying in their natal community then they may be able to mitigate the costs of dispersal by correctly timing their departure to maximize the possibility of success. Under each circumstance, some females may be better at maximizing their opportunities than are others. Such advantages could be gained socially if, for example an individual has a high-ranking

17

mother. Similarly, an individual could mature in a time of food abundance. Finally, an individual could benefit from an advantageous phenotype such as being larger or having a bold personality where they are more likely to be successful on one path over another. While not yet examined in chimpanzees, dispersal decisions in a variety of species are associated with similar patterns (Ims and Hjermann, 2001; Cote et al., 2010).

Chimpanzees are capable of advanced social cognition and they use flexible problem solving on a daily basis to navigate their social environment. Most importantly, chimpanzees have a fairly complex , providing a necessary foundation for advanced social cognition. Though chimpanzees do not understand false belief, they understand the goals and intentions of others and the knowledge and perception of others (Call and Tomasello, 2008). Chimpanzees are sensitive to reputation and can quickly learn both through direct and indirect encounters if an individual is likely to share with them (Russell et al., 2008; Subiaul et al., 2008; Herrmann et al., 2013).

Chimpanzees are skilled cooperators (de Waal, 1982; Watts, 2002; Melis et al., 2006a); they can discern the most suitable partner to cooperate with based on past experience

(Melis et al., 2006b) and can maintain cooperation through negotiation (Melis et al., 2009) and possibly reciprocity (Melis et al., 2008). In the wild, complex cooperation occurs during boundary patrols (Mitani and Watts, 2001a), possibly during group hunts

(Boesch, 1999) and through the maintenance of long-term alliances between individuals

(Watts, 2002). Last chimpanzees seem to be able to keep track of grooming exchanges

18

and may be able to trade grooming for political power, help and sex (Nishida and Hosaka, 1996; Watts, 2000, 2002; Mitani and Watts, 2001b). While advanced cognition is not necessary to integrate into a new community, it may facilitate the transition by allowing transferring individuals to react flexibly to the different social situations that they encounter during the integration period. Individual variation is expected to influence social integration when new immigrants arrive in a community.

Little is known about what cues individuals use in forming new relationships nor if they can use affiliative behavior as a type of social currency to quickly assess the suitability of potential partners.

Chimpanzees also show sophisticated physical cognitive abilities. In foraging and memory tasks chimpanzees make use of optimal search paths to locate hidden food

(Menzel, 1973; Sayers and Menzel, 2012), even taking into account the handling time of the objects, can remember locations over longer time periods (Mendes and Call, 2008) and make use of ‘episodic like’ memory to solve some spatial problems (Martin-Ordas et al., 2010). In the Taï forest, chimpanzees must transport hammers to nut cracking sites and appear to do so taking into account the weight of the , the energy content of the nut and the optimal route to the nut-cracking area (Boesch and Boesch-Achermann,

2000). The combination of social and physical cognitive skills may mitigate the energetic costs of dispersal in apes. Support for this assertion comes from ape reintroductions where mortality is frequently due to lethal encounters with wild individuals and

19

individuals appear to meet their nutritional requirements with ease (Goossens et al.,

2005; B. Hare, pers. comm.).

1.4 Conclusion

Unlike some primate species that exhibit female dispersal, female chimpanzees face high levels of within group feeding competition and correspondingly, are likely to incur high costs when dispersing and integrating into a new community. Though the majority of chimpanzee females disperse, research suggests females have some flexibility in making dispersal decisions and it is likely that these decisions are based on some sort of cost/benefit analysis. However, research in this area has been hampered by the high-number of dispersing females at most sites, making comparisons between dispersing and non-dispersing females difficult and in the difficulty in habituating and following young female chimpanzees. Second, virtually nothing is known about how chimpanzees form new relationships and how individual differences and strategies may affect the integration process once females arrive in a new community. Here, I take advantage of the high number of non-dispersing chimpanzees and the presence of two habituated communities in Gombe National Park to explore the costs and benefits of dispersal and to assess whether dispersal decisions are condition and phenotype dependent. Second, I make use of captive ape populations to identify the power of affiliative behavior in forming relationship preferences with strange individuals.

20

In chapter 1, I test the hypothesis that immigrants gain reproductive benefits by

residing with unrelated males while natal females face costs associated with inbreeding.

To do this, I first examine average relatedness between immigrant and natal females

with resident males to assess the potential for inbreeding by residence status. I then look

at relatedness between breeding pairs to assess if inbreeding actually occurs. Finally I

examine if chimpanzees can detect relatedness and breed with more unrelated

individuals. In chapter 2, I test the hypothesis that immigrant females incur costs beyond those already identified and that emigration actually delays reproduction. I conduct the first analysis of the energetic costs and build upon prior work examining the social and physiological costs of dispersal. I also quantify the reproductive costs associated with dispersal by calculating age at first birth and the survival of the first offspring in dispersing and non-dispersing females. In chapter 3, I test the hypothesis that dispersal decisions are condition and phenotype dependent. Females are expected to use external social and environmental cues and internal phenotypic cues to determine a course of action around sexual maturity. In chapter 4, I make use of captive ape populations to test the hypothesis that individuals can use affiliative behaviors as social currency in building relationships with strangers.

21

1.5 Specific Contributions from Co-Authors

Chapter 2

Anne Pusey developed long-term database utilized in analyses, discussed hypotheses, reviewed earlier drafts

Rebecca Rudicell extracted and genotyped DNA from fecal samples

Miguel Ramirez extracted and genotyped DNA from fecal samples

Beatrice Hahn provided technical expertise for analyses, reviewed earlier drafts

Emily Wroblewski conducted paternity assignment, assisted in analysis, reviewed earlier drafts Chapter 3

Carson Murray collected data in study period 1, discussed hypotheses, reviewed earlier drafts

Anne Pusey discussed hypotheses, reviewed earlier drafts

Chapter 4

Alexander Weiss collected personality data, assisted in personality analysis, reviewed earlier drafts

Anne Pusey developed long-term database utilized in analyses, discussed hypotheses as the paper developed, reviewed earlier drafts Chapter 5 1

Victoria Wobber helped conceive and design experiment, provided experimental expertise

Brian Hare discussed hypotheses as the paper developed, reviewed earlier drafts

1 Published as: Schroepfer-Walker, K., Wobber, V., & Hare, B. 2015. Experimental evidence that grooming and play are social currency in bonobos and chimpanzees. Behaviour. 152, 545 – 562. 22

2. Chimpanzees reproduce with genetically dissimilar mates regardless of dispersal status

Inbreeding depression, a reduction of fitness in the offspring of closely related mates, is a widely documented phenomenon thought to result largely from the homozygous expression of recessive deleterious alleles (Charlesworth and

Charlesworth, 1987, 1999; Keller and Waller, 2002). Inbreeding can adversely affect fitness through numerous pathways, notably via early death or reproductive disadvantage (Keller and Waller, 2002). In contrast, increased heterozygosity, whether wide or at specific loci, is often correlated with higher fitness (Chapman et al.,

2009; Szulkin et al., 2010). Selection should, therefore, generally favor the evolution of mechanisms to avoid inbreeding and promote mate selection of genetically different individuals.

In many animals, the likelihood of close inbreeding is reduced by the dispersal of members of one sex from the group or area before breeding (Pusey, 1987; Clutton-Brock,

1989; Perrin and Mazalov, 1999), but in some cases, close relatives continue to reside together as adults. In such instances, there is increasing evidence of behavioral avoidance of inbreeding (Pusey and Wolf, 1996). For example, although alpha male white-faced capuchins generally sire most or all infants in their group, they rarely breed with their daughters (Muniz et al., 2006). Similarly, African elephant males follow, mate guard and copulate less frequently with relatives (Archie et al., 2007). In rhesus

23

macaques, closely related maternal relatives mate but not during the most fertile period

(Manson and Perry, 1993). Post-copulatory mechanisms such as

(Ginsberg and Huck, 1989) and female-choice of sperm (Zeh and Zeh, 1997) are more

difficult to measure but also occur. Several experimental studies that require females to

mate with both related and unrelated individuals have found that the successful sire is

the least related individual (e.g. field crickets (Simmons et al., 2006), agile antechinus

(Parrott et al., 2007), house mice (Firman and Simmons, 2008)) and in the wild, post-

copulatory inbreeding avoidance is sometimes inferred because mating is common between relatives but inbred offspring are rare (e.g. blue tit (Foerster et al., 2006), hihi

(Brekke et al., 2011)).

Mate choice for overall genetic dissimilarity in wild populations is an emerging

area of study with variable results. In some populations, breeding dyads are generally

less related to each other than non-breeding dyads. In banner-tailed kangaroo rats

(Waser and De Woody, 2006) and Cunningham’s skinks (Stow and Sunnucks, 2004) the breeding male is less related to the female than other males in close proximity. In greater sac winged bats (Nagy et al., 2007) and reed buntings (Suter et al., 2007) females breed with extra-pair or group males when that individual is less related to them than their social mate. In contrast, no difference in overall relatedness was detected between breeding and non-breeding dyads in Atlantic salmon (Landry et al., 2001), American

pika (Peacock and Smith, 1997) and white-toothed shrews (Duarte et al., 2003).

24

Chimpanzees are an interesting species in which to examine inbreeding avoidance and mate choice. Though behavioral inbreeding avoidance has been extensively studied in non-human primates, few studies have addressed mate choice for overall genetic dissimilarity. Given the slow life histories and high cost of reproduction in primates, selection for genetic dissimilarity in mate choice should be adaptive.

Moreover, the unusual social and dispersal system of chimpanzees may have further selected for such a system. Chimpanzees live in permanent, multimale-multifemale, fission-fusion communities of up to 200 individuals (median 46.3) (Wrangham et al.,

2006). Like only a handful of other mammals (Lukas and Clutton-Brock, 2011), males remain in their natal community for life, whereas many, but not all, females transfer to other communities before breeding, a costly undertaking that likely delays reproduction

(Pusey and Packer, 1987; Nishida et al., 2003; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a). While separating many closely related adults of the opposite sex, these patterns result in the residence in the same community of some mothers with their adult sons, as well as adult siblings and other close relatives (Nishida, 1989; Constable et al., 2001; Schubert et al., 2013). If females frequently transfer between adjacent groups, there is a chance that they may also reside as adults with their mothers’ male relatives (Langergraber et al., 2009).

Extensive observation across field sites indicates that mating rarely occurs between maternal relatives but may be more frequent among paternal relatives (Goodall, 1986;

Constable et al., 2001; Stumpf et al., 2009; Wroblewski, 2010). Accordingly, close

25

inbreeding is rare and only one case has been detected in genetic studies of paternity across several field sites, evidence that there may be mate choice for genetic dissimilarity. However, no studies have yet examined whether mates are less related than expected.

Here we examine the parentage of 62 infants (including 28 with newly assigned paternities) to determine patterns of mate selection in two chimpanzee communities

(Kasekela and Mitumba) in Gombe National Park, Tanzania, that differ in size, number of adult males, and the proportion of females that disperse. The larger Kasekela community is unusual in that about 50% of females breed in their natal community

(Pusey et al., 1997). First, we examine the incidence of close inbreeding based on known pedigree information. Second, we test the hypothesis that natal females are generally more closely related to resident males by comparing relatedness between natal female- male dyads and immigrant female-male dyads. Third, we examine the extent to which natal females are able to avoid inbreeding by testing whether they are more closely related than immigrant females are to the sires of their offspring. Last, we test the hypothesis that because of the general advantages of heterozygosity, breeding dyads are less related than expected.

26

2.1 Methods

2.1.1 Study Site

Gombe National Park, in western Tanzania, contains three chimpanzee communities. Full day focal follows have been conducted on chimpanzees in the

Kasekela and Mitumba communities since 1973 and 1995 respectively and the transfer patterns of all females born into these communities are known. Individuals in the

Kalande community are known genetically from fecal sampling. All adult individuals and juveniles surviving to age 2 have been genotyped since 1995 in Kasekela and 2004 in

Mitumba. During this time period the community size in Kasekela ranged from 12 – 25 adult females (age 12+) and 10 – 12 adult males (age 12+). The Mitumba community ranged from 5 – 9 adult females and 2 – 5 adult males.

2.1.2 Genotyping and Paternity Analysis

We genotyped 151 chimpanzees from the Kasekela, Mitumba and Kalande communities at six to eleven microsatellite loci. Paternities for 34 offspring born into the

Kasekela community had previously been determined (Constable et al., 2001;

Wroblewski et al., 2009; Rudicell et al., 2010; Gilby et al., 2013). Typing at additional microsatellite loci resolved paternity for one individual previously sampled, which we update here, and we report paternities for 28 new individuals born into the Kasekela and Mitumba communities. This provides a total sample size of 62 offspring with

27

known paternities, 51 in the Kasekela community and 11 in the Mitumba community.

DNA was extracted from fecal samples for 23 individuals and from tissue samples from

5 individuals (whose bodies were recovered after death) for the newly determined paternities. We conservatively included all males at least nine years of age in both communities at the time of conception as candidate fathers, as well as any sampled male from Kalande. For each new paternity 100% of potential sires in Kasekela and Mitumba were sampled. Fathers were identified using the exclusion principle. Detailed methods are explained elsewhere (Wroblewski et al., 2009).

2.1.3 Relatedness Analysis

We classified offspring as inbred if the father was related to the mother at the level of half-siblings (R = 0.25) or above based on known pedigrees. In one case, strong evidence pointed to inbreeding but could not be conclusively determined because GM, the mother of offspring GIZ, was conceived in the Kasekela community in 1970 and when genotyping began in the 1990s GM’s mother was dead and only one candidate sire, EV, was still alive. However EV was identified as the father through likelihood analysis with 80% confidence under the most conservative parameters (Wroblewski et al., 2009). EV was conclusively determined to be the father of FE who in turn sired GIZ.

All other cases could be identified with a high degree of confidence as inbred or not inbred.

28

Genetic relatedness between all dyads was calculated using Queller &

Goodnight’s pairwise genetic relatedness estimator (Queller and Goodnight, 1989) in the

program Co-ancestry (Wang, 2011). The R values produced using Queller and

Goodnight’s estimator had high correlations (range 0.75 – 0.93) with 5 other relatedness

estimators calculated using Co-ancestry. Relatedness values also closely reflected

Hamilton’s R for individuals of known pedigree. Tests of relatedness were conducted in

Co-ancestry using a permutation procedure with 10,000 iterations where the observed

difference is compared to the simulated difference to test for significant differences between groups. To control for rank effects in the final analysis, a similar permutation

procedure was performed in R (R Core Team, 2014) that took into account both rank

(see below) and relatedness for each dyad.

To compare relatedness between immigrant female-male dyads and natal female- male dyads all adult females in the Kasekela community were categorized as immigrant or natal based on community of birth. All candidate males were born in the Kasekela community except one that immigrated into the community with a female assumed to be his sister at the estimated age of 4 years (Goodall, 1986). Females who were present on less than 10% of male follows during their tenure were considered peripheral and not included in the analysis. The Mitumba community was not used in this analysis because only one adult female is known to have been born in the community and her only offspring to date died in infancy before a fecal sample could be obtained.

29

We calculated overall relatedness between different classes of females and resident males by averaging relatedness values between all immigrant female-male dyads and all natal female-male dyads that overlapped as adults in each community. Relatedness between mothers and sires of 30 offspring (19 in Kasekela and 11 in Mitumba) born to immigrant mothers was compared to relatedness between mothers and sires of 28 offspring (all Kasekela) born to natal mothers. Two mothers of offspring born in

Mitumba (EDE & LOS) were of unknown provenance (they were present in the group as adults when observations began) and were, therefore, excluded from this analysis. We then repeated this analysis, restricting our data to immigrant female-male dyads and natal female-male dyads in Kasekela because no offspring of natal mothers were genotyped in

Mitumba.

Relatedness between non-breeding and breeding dyads was compared for 50 offspring in the Kasekela community and 11 offspring in the Mitumba community. All males present in the community, and 9 years old at the time of conception (estimated as

226 days prior to conception (Goodall, 1986; Wallis, 1997)) were included as potentially- breeding males. Rank for each candidate sire was calculated on the day of estimated conception using the occurrence of submissive pant grunts to calculate an Elo score

(Neumann et al., 2011). For each offspring, the mother and sire were classified as a breeding dyad while the mother with each other adult male present at conception was classified as a non-breeding dyad. Two offspring born in Mitumba (APL & LAM) prior to 2004 were excluded from this analysis because one or more candidate sires was not genotyped. This same analysis was then performed separately for offspring born to 30

immigrant (n = 19) and natal (n = 31) females in Kasekela to examine whether mate selection patterns were the same for immigrant and natal females. To assess if average relatedness between immigrant-male dyads differed by community, relatedness between all adult males and immigrant females in Mitumba and Kasekela were compared, following the same methods used to assess relatedness between males and immigrant or natal females in Kasekela.

Male rank influences the probability of paternity (Wroblewki et al. 2009).

Therefore, to be confident that selection of breeding partners was influenced by genetic distance rather than rank we ran a final analysis on the Kasekela sample, taking into account male rank and comparing relatedness between breeding and non-breeding dyads.

In Mitumba, rank was only known for the alpha male, preventing a systematic analysis of rank and relatedness in this community.

2.2 Results

2.2.1 Paternity and Inbreeding

By adding information on 28 new offspring to the 34 previously published cases we were able to examine the parentage of 62 individuals. Fathers could be assigned to all 28 offspring using exclusion, whereby they were the sole male able to contribute a complementary set of alleles to the offspring given the maternal genotype (Table 1). All new offspring were sired by within-community males except for one, whose mother

31

primarily resides in the southern Kalande community but conceived during a visit to

Kasekela (see discussion).

Two of the 28 infants were determined to be inbred (Table 1); one (GGL) the

offspring of paternal half-siblings and one (KAR) the offspring of maternal half-siblings.

Evidence points to a third case of inbreeding, also between paternal half-siblings

(offspring GIZ). Adding these three cases to the previously reported case of inbreeding between a mother-son pair (offspring FI) (Constable et al., 2001), offspring conceived by

close relatives account for 10% of all genotyped infants, and 16% of those born to

females who remained in their natal Kasekela community to breed since 1995. In

Mitumba, only one adult female is known to have remained in her natal community but

her only offspring to date died in infancy and was not genotyped. All other females in

Mitumba are immigrants.

32

Table 1: Paternities and related demographic and genetic information for 62 offspring.

Mother - Mother's New Comm Offspring Mother Sire Sire Analyses Status Paternity R Value KK CN CD WL Natal -0.151 1,2 KK COC CD FD Natal -0.005 1,2 KK DIA DL FO Natal 0.281 Y†† 1,2 KK DUK DL TN Natal -0.004 Y 1,2 KK FO FF WL Natal -0.189 1,2 KK FI FF FR Natal 0.369 1,2 KK FE FF EV Natal -0.049 1,2 KK FLI FF KS Natal 0.095 1,2 KK FU FN SL Natal -0.078 1,2 KK FND FN SL Natal -0.078 1,2 KK FAM FN SL Natal -0.078 1,2 KK FAD FN WL Natal -0.385 1,2 KK FFT FN WL Natal -0.385 Y 1,2 KK GGL GA FO Natal 0.339 Y 1,2 KK GAb1 GA AO Natal 0.190 Y 1,2 KK GAb2 GA SL Natal -0.210 Y 1,2 KK GLA GLD FU Natal -0.042 Y 1,2 KK GLIb1 GLI SL Natal -0.255 Y 1,2 KK GD GM AL Natal 0.083 1,2 KK GA GM WL Natal 0.021 1,2 KK GLI/GLD* GM FR Natal -0.151 1,2 KK GIM GM TB Natal -0.028 1,2 KK GIZ † GM FE Natal 0.186 Y 1,2 KK SR SA BE Natal -0.086 1,2 KK SN SA AO Natal -0.069 1,2 KK SAM SA FR Natal -0.092 1,2 KK SIR SA AO Natal -0.069 1,2 KK SAF SI AO Natal -0.104 Y 1,2 KK SHA SR WL Natal -0.307 1,2 KK TOM TG KS Natal -0.076 1,2 KK TAB TG FE Natal -0.192 Y 1,2 KK KAR TT TN Natal** 0.002 Y 1 KK BRZ BAH KS Imm -0.300 1,2 KK BAS BAH TN Imm -0.059 Y 1,2 33

KK ERI EZA KS Imm 0.295 1,2 KK EZAb1 EZA TN Imm -0.320 Y 1,2 KK IPO IMA WL Imm -0.468 Y 1,2 KK JK JF AL Imm 0.015 1,2 KK KEA KP WL Imm -0.102 1,2 KK MAM MAK GL Imm -0.088 1,2 KK NYO NUR FU Imm 0.000 Y 1,2 KK TG PI GB Imm -0.037 1,2 KK TN PI FR Imm -0.047 1,2 KK TZN PI FR Imm -0.047 1,2 KK SI SW WL Imm 0.113 1,2 KK SDB SW FR Imm -0.284 1,2 KK TOF TTA SL Imm -0.178 1,2 KK ZS TZ FR Imm -0.039 1,2 KK ZEL TZ KS Imm -0.179 1,2 KK ZIN TZ GL Imm -0.260 1,2 KK YAM YD WL Imm 0.031 1,2 MT APL AP VIN Imm -0.222 Y 1 MT AND AP EDG Imm 0.126 Y 1,2 MT ARI AP RUD Imm -0.008 Y 1,2 MT MAY DB RUD Imm 0.238 Y 1,2 MT FLW FS RUD Imm 0.099 Y 1,2 MT FAL FS EDG Imm 0.077 Y 1,2 MT FED FS EDG Imm 0.077 Y 1,2 MT KOM KON RUD Imm -0.164 Y 1,2 MT LAM LUC VIN Imm 0.172 Y 1 MT LTT LUC EDG Imm -0.325 Y 1,2 MT MIS MGA EDG Imm 0.028 Y 1,2 MT EDE EVA VIN Unk -0.327 Y 2 MT LOS LOR RUD Unk 0.067 Y 2 Comm (Community): KK=Kasekela, MT=Mitumba; Analyses: List of subjects used in comparison between natal and immigrant breeding dyads (1) and between breeding and non-breeding dyads (2). Bolded offspring are inbred at the 0.5 level. *Twins, confirmed to have same father, treated as one conception **TT is transient, born in KK, currently resides in Kalande with occasional visits to KK †Strong but not conclusive evidence individual is inbred (see results) ††Paternity previously reported as FE (Wroblewski et al. 2009), typing at 9 additional loci, for a total of 20, shows father is FO (maternal brother to FE).

34

2.2.2 Relatedness between natal or immigrant females and resident males

Across communities, all adult female-adult male dyads (n = 659) had a mean relatedness of -0.01 (SD = 0.20; Figure 2A). To assess inbreeding risk we compared relatedness between natal or immigrant females to resident males in the Kasekela community. Overall, natal females were more closely related, on average, to the males of their community (n = 268 dyads, mean R = 0.02) than immigrant females were (n = 391 dyads, mean R = -0.03; p < 0.01; Figure 2B).

A) B)

Figure 2: A) Distribution of Queller & Goodnight R for all male -female dya ds in the population. B) Relatedness between Kasekela immigrant female-male dyads and natal female-male dyads (p < 0.01). Error bars represent standard error.

2.2.3 Relatedness between natal and immigrant breeding dyads

Including data from both communities, natal females (n=32 dyads, mean R = -

0.05) were not more related to the sires of their offspring than were immigrant females

35

(n = 30 dyads, mean R = -0.06; p = NS; Figure 3A). There also was not a difference when analysis was restricted to dyads from the Kasekela community (Figure 3B) to account for a lack of natal breeding dyads in the Mitumba community (natal breeding dyads; n = 32 dyads, mean R = -0.05; immigrant breeding dyads: n = 19 dyads, mean R = -0.10; p = NS).

A) B)

Figure 3: Relatedness between immigrant mothers and sires compa red to relatedness between natal mothers and sires (A) across communities (p = NS) and (B) in Kasekela only (p = NS). Error bars represent standard error.

36

2.2.4 Relatedness between breeding and non-breeding dyads

In the Kasekela community, breeding dyads including both natal and immigrant females (n = 50 dyads, mean R = -0.07) were more distantly related than non-breeding dyads (n = 571 dyads, mean R = 0.01, p = 0.002; Figure 4A). This result did not change when controlling for the rank of the male (p = 0.015). In the Mitumba community there was no difference in relatedness between breeding dyads (n = 11 dyads, mean R = -0.01) and non-breeding dyads (n = 25 dyads, mean R = 0.03; p = NS; Figure 4B).

We then assessed whether this result was driven by natal females in Kasekela avoiding breeding with close relatives. To do this we analyzed immigrant and natal females separately (Figure 5). For immigrant females, breeding dyads (n= 19, mean R = -

0.10) were still significantly more distantly related than non-breeding dyads (n = 211 dyads, mean R = -0.01, p = 0.03) and for natal females there was a trend in the same direction (breeding dyads: n=31, mean R = -0.04; non-breeding dyads: n = 360, mean R =

0.02, p = 0.075).

To examine if Mitumba chimpanzees are less genetically diverse, and therefore, more limited in opportunities for choice based on genetic dissimilarity we compared immigrant female-male dyads in the Kasekela and Mitumba communities (Figure 6).

There was no difference in relatedness (Kasekela-n = 391 dyads, mean R = -0.03;

Mitumba- n = 84 dyads, mean R = -0.03, p = NS), nor did the variance differ when tested

37

using the Levene test of variance (though a trend was detected showing that Mitumba had greater variance; p = 0.07).

A) B)

Figure 4: Mean relatedness between non -breeding and breedin g dyads at each

conception in (A) Kasekela (p < 0.01) and (B) Mitumba (p = NS). Error bars represent standard error.

38

Figure 5: Relatedness between breeding and non-breeding dyads in Kasekela by residence status (Immigrant, p = 0.03; Natal, p = 0.075). Error bars represent standard error.

39

Figure 6: Distribution of Queller & Goodnight R between immigrant female- male dyads in Kasekela (KK) and Mitumba (MT).

40

2.3 Discussion

We found that, as predicted, females that reside in their natal community are

more closely related on average than immigrant females are to the males of the

community. We also identified three new cases of close inbreeding. However, the sires

of sampled offspring of all females, including natal females, were generally less closely

related to the mother than non-sires, providing evidence of inbreeding avoidance and

mate choice for genetic dissimilarity in chimpanzees.

The fact that natal females in Kasekela are more closely related to the resident

males than are immigrant females is in accordance with our knowledge from pedigree

and genetic information of the frequent presence in the community of sons, fathers and

half-siblings of these females. Previous studies found that while mating between certain

classes of relatives in this community is usually infrequent, particularly among mothers

and sons and maternal siblings (Pusey 2005), it can occur at high frequencies among

fathers and daughters, and paternal siblings (Wroblewski 2010). Adding to the

previously determined case of an inbred infant resulting from a forced son-mother

mating (Constable et al. 2001) we identified three new cases of close inbreeding, two between paternal siblings residing in the Kasekela community, and the third between maternal siblings. The third case (KAR) was unique in that, though the mother was born in Kasekela, she transferred briefly to Mitumba, and then is thought to have resided in the southern community of Kalande from 1996 – 2008 based on fecal sampling. In 2008,

41

following a drastic decline in the size of the Kalande community (Rudicell et al., 2010),

she began making lengthy visits back to her natal Kasekela community and conceived

an infant with her 16 year old maternal brother, born after she had emigrated. We may be underestimating the prevalence of inbreeding in the population because most infants that die prior to age 2 are not genotyped (19 of 23 offspring in Kasekela and 2 of 2 offspring in Mitumba); however 15 of these infants were born to immigrant mothers and thus were unlikely to be inbred. The fact that 50% of the four inbred offspring died in the first two years compared to an average infant mortality in the first two years of 29%

(Bronikowski et al. in prep) is suggestive of significant inbreeding depression but more data are needed. In summary, natal females face a higher risk of inbreeding than immigrant females and when inbreeding does occur, it is more frequent in natal females.

Despite the elevated risk and occasional cases of inbreeding, natal females in general are not more related to the sires of their offspring than immigrant females are to the sires of their offspring, indicating that natal females are generally able to avoid inbreeding. Moreover, in the Kasekela community, most females bred with sires that were less related to them than the average male. This suggests chimpanzees can detect genetic relatedness among potential mates and selectively breed with a mate whose genetic profile differs from their own.

Unlike in the larger Kasekela community, breeding dyads in the Mitumba community were not less related than non-breeding dyads. This raises interesting

42

questions about the influence of demography on mating patterns in chimpanzees. Since

2001, Mitumba has only contained 2-3 fully grown adult males (15+ years) and 5-9 adult females (12+ years), most of whom are immigrants, at any given time. Small population size may limit the range of genetic diversity in potential mates. Additionally the lack of natal females may remove the need for choice based on relatedness. However, we found that genetic diversity is similar in Kasekela and Mitumba and that choice for dissimilarity is still present in Kasekela when only considering immigrant females.

Rather, with fewer males overall and fewer females in estrous at any given time, high- ranking males in Mitumba are likely to be more successful at mate guarding. Indeed, this may account for the differences in alpha male success seen in the larger Kasekela and smaller Mitumba community; in Mitumba 77% of offspring were sired by the alpha male (unpublished data) compared to 30% in the Kasekela community (Wroblewski et al., 2009) . Thus, even if the limited number of males still provides a range of genetic diversity, the strong influence of the alpha male in Mitumba may constrain female choice for genetic dissimilarity. Though sexual coercion is known to be important in restricting female mate choice in eastern chimpanzees (Muller et al., 2010; Feldblum et al., 2014), the fission-fusion social system and alternative mating strategies such as consortships (Tutin, 1979; Wroblewski et al., 2009) may allow a greater degree of choice for females and lessen the degree of coercion in larger groups such as Kasekela.

Alternatively, in groups with many females, males may concentrate mating effort on

43

unrelated individuals, an opportunity not available, nor potentially necessary, in groups

with a limited number of females.

Inbreeding is particularly costly for mammalian females because of their high

investment in gestation and lactation (Trivers, 1972; Emlen and Oring, 1977; Emery

Thompson, 2013), especially in a slowly reproducing species such as chimpanzees, in

which interbirth intervals average at least five years (Emery Thompson, 2013). In

instances where relatives reside together as adults, females might, therefore, be expected

to invest more heavily in inbreeding avoidance. Accordingly, females frequently do not

respond to mating invitations from maternal male relatives and will often resist if a

relative persists in a mating attempt (Van Lawick-Goodall, 1968; Pusey, 1980; Goodall,

1986). In the only study of its kind, preliminary evidence shows that females in the

Kasekela community also resist mating attempts from paternal male relatives and

copulations with these relatives seem to be due to male effort (Wroblewski, 2010).

However, males also contribute to inbreeding avoidance and, once past sexual maturity,

rarely mate with their mother and infrequently pursue maternal sisters (Van Lawick-

Goodall, 1968; Pusey, 1980; Goodall, 1986). Our new genetic evidence builds upon prior behavioral evidence that chimpanzees do avoid inbreeding but our data cannot

discriminate between female and male effort, an issue that merits further investigation.

Though the mechanisms that chimpanzees use to determine relatedness are

unclear, these data suggest such mechanisms may be relatively sensitive in

44

discriminating degrees of relatedness. Such discrimination is an active area of research and likely involves several phenotypic cues such as physical resemblance (Parr and de

Waal, 1999; Parr et al., 2010) and odor similarity (Havlicek and Roberts, 2009;

Charpentier et al., 2010; Setchell and Huchard, 2010; Setchell et al., 2011) and experience cues such as association during the juvenile period (Pusey, 1980). Our data show that association and familiarity may be particularly important for discriminating close maternal relatives given that in the only case of inbreeding to occur between maternal siblings the pair had no contact prior to adulthood, but familiarity is a less likely to operate in discrimination between paternal and more distant relatives. Because mating does occur between relatives, but infants rarely result, post-copulatory sperm selection or cryptic female choice like that seen in agile antechinuses and house mice and suspected to occur in mandrills may also be important (Parrott et al., 2007; Firman and

Simmons, 2008; Setchell et al., 2013).

In conclusion, our results show that inbreeding is not an inevitable outcome for female chimpanzees that reside with relatives. Rather chimpanzees are able to detect relatedness and generally avoid breeding with close kin. By remaining in their natal community, females may then be able to take advantage of familiar resources (Williams et al., 2002b; Murray et al., 2007) and avoid the female aggression they face upon immigration into a new community (Nishida, 1989; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a; Pusey et al.,

2008). However, given the increased risk of inbreeding faced by natal females and the

45

high death rate of inbred offspring, dispersing to another community with no or few

relatives provides females with a more complete means of avoiding inbreeding. Finally, breeding generally occurs between less related dyads regardless of dispersal status,

suggesting that genetic dissimilarity contributes to mate choice

46

3. Dispersal is socially, but not energetically, costly in female chimpanzees of Gombe National Park.

Dispersal, leaving one group for another, is a costly endeavor encompassing losses associated with energy, time, risk and opportunity that benefits the individual through a decrease in inbreeding risk or increased access to mates among other factors

(Bonte et al., 2012). In social mammals, natal dispersal further requires integrating into a novel social environment before beginning reproduction. Dispersal is sex-biased in most social mammals; typically males leave their group of birth to reproduce in neighboring groups (Greenwood, 1980). Chimpanzees are unusual among social mammals in that males are philopatric and most, but not all, females disperse around sexual maturity

(Pusey and Packer, 1987). Ultimately, this pattern is likely determined by the benefits that accrue to philopatric males in cooperating to defend a large territory (Williams et al., 2004). Accordingly, females must disperse to avoid inbreeding. However, some females are able to remain in their natal community to breed and avoid costs associated with dispersal. To understand dispersal patterns in female chimpanzees, we must first quantify the costs and benefits for dispersers and non-dispersers.

Chimpanzees are ripe-fruit specialists and their food resources are patchily distributed. Access to resources is influenced by an individual’s social status (Murray et al., 2006; Kahlenberg et al., 2008b). Therefore, dispersal in female chimpanzees is

47

hypothesized to be especially costly (Pusey, 1980; Nishida, 1989) but to provide benefits in virtually eliminating the risk of inbreeding (Pusey, 1980). Chimpanzees live in large fission-fusion communities where males are more gregarious and cooperate to defend a community range (Nishida, 1979; Goodall, 1986). Females sometimes travel alone with their dependent offspring but will join large parties, especially when in estrous. In eastern chimpanzees, female also restrict their ranging when anestrous to distinct but overlapping core areas within the larger community range (Wrangham and Smuts, 1980;

Wrangham et al., 1992). Dispersing females typically transfer sometime after experiencing their first full estrous swelling between ages 10 and 13 and generally remain in their transfer community for life (Pusey, 1979; Nishida et al., 2003; Stumpf et al., 2009). A gross understanding of the social consequences of dispersal in female chimpanzees has emerged from several long-term research sites but we know little about more subtle social changes and nothing about energetic costs post-dispersal due to the difficulty in following females throughout the transfer period. Female chimpanzees are likely to incur energetic and social costs during each phase of dispersal; initiation, transfer and settlement, all or some of which are likely to exert physiological changes and delay reproduction.

In chimpanzees, costs associated with movement during transfer are largely unknown because individuals have rarely been followed from one community to another. In several primate species, and other causes of mortality increase

48

when individuals visit areas unfamiliar to them (e.g. vervet monkeys (Isbell and Young,

1993)) and during actual dispersal events (e.g. savannah baboons (Alberts and Altmann,

1995); Milne-Edwards sifakas (Tecot et al., 2013)). Predation rates among chimpanzees are generally low but do vary between sites (Goodall, 1986; Boesch, 1991; Tsukakara,

1993) and human proximity and conflict is increasing across the chimpanzee range

(Kormos and Boesch, 2003; Plumptre et al., 2010). For a large bodied chimpanzee, unfamiliar conspecifics may present a greater risk than other predators. Chimpanzees sometimes kill strangers, including females, when numbers are in their favor (Wilson et al., 2014). Finally, traveling through unfamiliar and/or unsuitable habitat is likely to decrease foraging efficiency (see below) and increase contact with novel diseases. Taken together, journeying between communities is likely a risky endeavor. These risks may be minimal if individuals transfer between adjacent or nearby communities, a common occurrence among primate species (e.g. Milne-Edwards sifakas (Morelli et al., 2009), vervet monkeys (Cheney and Seyfarth, 1983), mountain gorillas (Fossey and Harcourt,

1977)).

In the few instances where female chimpanzees have been followed between communities, transfers between adjacent communities are common. Three of six females that matured in the K-group of Mahale National Park transferred to the adjacent M- group (Nishida, 1979). Similarly, in the early decades at Gombe National Park, several females that matured in Kasekela were later observed in the adjacent Mitumba

49

community, though four females disappeared in good health and may have left the park

(this volume, Chapter 4). On the other , studies of population differentiation at

several sites show that communities separated by up to 600 kilometers can be genetically

similar and share mtDNA haplotypes (Morin et al., 1994; Goldberg and Ruvolo, 1997;

Inoue et al., 2013). The presence of shared genetic profiles suggests substantial long-

distance gene flow between areas and one study estimates that 3 – 4 females migrate between populations each generation (Goldberg and Ruvolo, 1997). Irrespective of

movement costs, females likely incur travel costs upon arrival in a new community as

they learn the boundaries of their new territory, identify ideal foraging areas and

frequently associate with males.

Dispersing individuals are also likely to suffer costs from loss of feeding

efficiency when they start to forage in unfamiliar areas. As time spent in a particular

area increases, animals learn about the physical and biotic features, including habitat

quality, and can maximize resource exploitation (Piper, 2011). Fitness benefits of site

familiarity exist in a variety of avian taxa (e.g. grouse (Yoder, 2004); swallows (Brown et

al., 2008), loons (Piper et al., 2008)) and are thought to occur in others (e.g. rabbits (Letty

et al., 2003), elephant seals (Haley, 1994)). Immigrating chimpanzee females are, then,

expected to suffer from a lack of familiarity in their new community.

Individuals may be able to reduce costs associated with a lack of familiarity by

transferring with a positive energy balance, dispersing socially rather than locationally

50

or by making temporary visits prior to immigration. Female chimpanzees, like male blue monkeys (Ekernas and Cords, 2007), are more likely to transfer when environmental conditions are favorable (Stumpf et al., 2009), suggesting they prepare for future foraging deficiencies. They may also reduce costs by undertaking visits to potential transfer communities. Young females at Mahale and Gombe have been observed visiting neighboring communities (Nishida, 1979; Pusey, 1979, 1980), and are suspected to do so in the Kanyawara community in (Stumpf et al., 2009) and at Taï

National Park (Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000). These visits can last for a few days or a few months and may allow females time to assess the foraging options in a potential transfer community. However, visits are likely costly and may simply shift familiarity costs rather than reduce them. On the other hand, if communities have overlapping home ranges, females may be able to disperse socially but not locationally (Isbell and van Vuren, 1996). By doing so, they would be able to maintain familiar foraging areas and at the same time, reduce the costs of inbreeding associated with philopatry.

Chimpanzee studies that examine feeding across ranks and reproductive states can also elucidate potential foraging costs associated with settlement. Female chimpanzees compete over resources and dominant females accrue feeding benefits. In

Tai, dominant females win contests over food (Wittig and Boesch, 2003) while in

Kasekela dominant females have a higher quality core area (Murray et al., 2007) and diet

(Murray et al., 2006) and suffer fewer seasonal fluctuations in body mass (Pusey et al.,

51

2005). Resident females will defend their core foraging areas (Miller et al., 2014) and behave aggressively towards immigrants (see below) and as a result, low-ranking

females tend to avoid high-ranking females when in the same neighborhood (Williams

et al., 2002b; Murray et al., 2007). Immigrant females generally enter the dominance

hierarchy at the bottom (Nishida, 1979; Murray et al., 2006; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a) and

likely rank below natal females of similar age (Kahlenberg et al., 2008a) who further benefit from familiarity and the presence of higher-ranking kin allies. Thus, costs

associated with feeding are unlikely to arise solely from a loss of familiarity and may

also be due to female-female feeding competition and the effects of low dominance rank.

Reproductive state may also contribute to feeding costs. Though cycling females are not

as energetically taxed as pregnant and lactating females (Murray et al., 2009; Emery

Thompson et al., 2010; Emery Thompson, 2013), swollen females face feeding deficits

due to time spent socializing and traveling with males (Wrangham and Smuts, 1980;

Goodall, 1986; Williams et al., 2002b).

In contrast to potential travel and feeding costs of transfer, some costs associated

with social integration during settlement are well established. As in many well-studied

species (e.g. Belding’s ground (Sherman, 1981), vervet monkeys (Cheney and

Seyfarth, 1982), olive baboons (Packer, 1979)), incoming strangers are frequently

attacked by resident chimpanzees. In particular, immigrant females are harassed by

resident females. These unfriendly relationships were first noted in the Kasekela

52

community (Pusey, 1980) and subsequently observed in Mahale (Nishida, 1989), Taï

(Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000) and Kanyawara (Kahlenberg et al., 2008a).

Aggressive encounters can be severe, often involve female-female coalitions and, in rare cases, prevent females from settling in a community (Townsend et al., 2007; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a; Pusey et al., 2008). Injuries incurred during attacks can lead to death and/or lifelong consequences (Williams et al., 2008).

Immigrant females employ counterstrategies to reduce costs associated with female aggression. Immigrants tend to associate primarily with males during the initial post-transfer period and males often intervene on behalf of immigrants during female- female aggressive encounters (Pusey, 1990; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000;

Kahlenberg et al., 2008a). Males are generally thought to be attracted to immigrants as new mating partners, explaining the observed patterns of behavior (Pusey, 1979;

Nishida, 1989). However, across sites males tend to prefer older parous females over nulliparous females and copulation rates with nulliparous females are generally low

(Tutin, 1979; Wrangham, 2002; Watts, 2007). In eastern chimpanzees, male aggression towards females is associated with sexual coercion and males are likewise more aggressive towards older, parous females (Muller et al., 2007). If immigrant females are attractive mating partners, they are likely to receive more aggression from males than other low-ranking females of similar age but less than older, resident females, increasing the cost of social integration.

53

Following immigration, females must establish a foraging area and to do so need to integrate socially with residents. Ideally, females should attempt to establish their core areas near the center of the range to avoid potential encounters with neighboring groups. To do so, females likely need to establish relationships with resident females who reside in these areas and can serve as future allies. However, this appears to be a difficult undertaking as resident females are hostile towards residents. Thus, beyond directed aggression, immigrants are likely to experience other social costs during the settlement period related to the loss of kin allies. In Kasekela, low ranking females avoid high ranking females (Williams et al., 2002a, 2002b; Murray et al., 2006) and low-ranking females are more likely to associate with each other than middle or high-ranking females

(Foerster et al., 2015). Low-ranking females settle away from high-ranking females and range more widely (Murray et al., 2007), putting them at higher risk for intergroup encounters.

Finally, immigrant females are also expected to experience changes in their social and grooming networks as a result of being socially isolated and pushed to the periphery. Adolescent females frequently associate and groom with their mother, even after sexual maturity (Goodall, 1986; Pusey, 1990; Stumpf et al., 2009). As adolescence progresses females seek out more partners but, in many instances, still receive over 90% of their grooming from their mother (Pusey, 1990). This bond is severed at transfer and new immigrants may make use of several strategies to acquire grooming time in their

54

new community. Females may follow a strategy similar to that in bonobos where they concentrate their efforts on a single senior female (Idani, 1991). On the other hand, they may seek out many partners to fulfill their grooming needs; a tactic that may also hasten the identification of suitable and receptive social partners. Nonetheless, immigrants may fail to receive the same level of grooming that they did prior to transfer. A loss in grooming time may also represent a health cost. Grooming is thought to fulfill hygienic as well as social needs in chimpanzees; similar to patterns observed in baboons (Akinyi et al., 2013). Given the hostile reception from resident females, immigrants likely initially concentrate their grooming effort on receptive males and are unlikely to have resident females as preferred grooming partners.

Social costs have been observed in most female-dispersing species, though they vary by degree. In horses, immigrants received more aggression from mares in their transfer group than in their natal group (Monard and Duncan, 1996) and in Geoffroy’s spider monkeys, a frugivorous primate with a similar social system to the chimpanzee, females are very aggressive towards immigrants (Asensio et al., 2008). In contrast, in other primates such as red colobus (Struhsaker, 2010) and muriquis (Printes and Strier,

1999) overt aggression towards immigrants has not been reported, though in muriquis resident females frequently displace new immigrants (Printes and Strier, 1999). Among other apes, in the closely related bonobo, females occasionally receive aggression from residents but are primarily ignored (Idani, 1991) and in the more distantly related

55

mountain , resident females can be aggressive towards immigrants (Watts, 1991) but are generally only intolerant (Watts, 1994).

Due to the amalgamation of dispersal costs, including energetic deficits and loss

of social predictability and control, immigrants may be chronically stressed. Acute stress

responses are adaptive, however chronic stress leads to physiological decline and is

associated with muscle wasting, immunosuppression and a reduction in successful

reproduction (Sapolsky, 1992). Glucocorticoids (cortisol in primates) are a steroid

hormone released in response to unpredictable stimuli (i.e. stressors) and are commonly

used to measure both chronic and acute stress responses in individuals (McEwen and

Wingfield, 2003; Romero et al., 2009). In Kanyawara females, immigrants have higher

cortisol concentrations than similarly aged natal females (Kahlenberg et al., 2008a) and

though cortisol concentrations increase with age, they peak during the transfer period

(Emery Thompson et al., 2010). Evidence suggests females are sensitive to social stress.

Female-female aggression during the estrous period was associated with higher cortisol

concentrations in Kanyawara females (Emery Thompson et al., 2010) and in Kasekela

low-ranking lactating females are attacked more frequently by males and have higher

cortisol concentrations (Markham et al., 2014). Evidence for the importance of nutritional

stress is mixed. In Kasekela, diet quality is not linked to differences in cortisol among

lactating females (Markham et al., 2014) but at Kanyawara lactating females had higher

cortisol in times of low-fruit availability (Emery Thompson et al., 2010) and quality of

56

the foraging neighborhood is negatively correlated with cortisol concentrations (Emery

Thompson et al., 2010).

Ultimately, costs associated with dispersal may delay the start of an individual’s

reproductive career and decrease survival in the resultant offspring. Even if the time between leaving the natal community and entering the transfer community is

minimized the costs of dispersal seem to delay first birth. In Mahale, age at first birth for

26 immigrant females of estimated age exceeded that of four females who did not

disperse by 1 year (Nishida et al., 2003). Better information is available on the time from

immigration to first birth, an interval that at Mahale, is 3 plus years (Nishida et al., 2003)

and at Taï is 2.65 years (Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000). Throughout this interval,

females cycle regularly and are sexually receptive but do not give birth to offspring. This

suggests the costly period may extend several years post-immigration and decreased

access to resources and lack of social support may affect the first offspring’s survival

even if a female is able to carry an infant to term. For instance, natal females tend to be

slightly higher ranking and theoretically have access to their mother’s higher-quality

familiar core area; both of which are associated with an increase in infant survival

(Pusey et al., 1997; Emery Thompson et al., 2007). Therefore, it seems likely that the first

offspring of immigrants may have a lower survival rate than their natal peers.

Reproduction is delayed in dispersing females of some other species (e.g. Columbian

ground squirrels (Wigget and Boag, 1993), red howler monkeys (Crockett and Pope,

57

1993)) but not in others (e.g. horses (Monard and Duncan, 1996), Thomas’ langurs

(Sterck et al., 2005), gorillas (Robbins et al., 2009)). Given the long interbirth interval of

approximately five years and the effort required to gestate and wean an offspring the

delay and difference in survival may translate to a meaningful difference in lifetime

reproductive success.

Here, I conduct a comprehensive analysis of the energetic and social costs

associated with transfer in female chimpanzees in the Kasekela and Mitumba

communities of Gombe National Park. I take advantage of the fact that approximately

half of all females remain in their natal community to breed to compare extensive field

observations on immigrant and natal females of similar age. I predict that in comparison

to natal females, immigrant females 1) travel further and spend more time traveling on a

daily basis 2) have a lower quality diet, higher diet breadth and spend more time

foraging 3) primarily associate with males 4) experience more frequent female-female, but not male-female, aggression 5) spend less time grooming with more partners and 6) have higher cortisol levels due to chronic stress. Most importantly, immigrant females will experience a delay in time to first birth and survival of the first infant will be reduced.

58

3.1 Methods

3.1.1 Study Site and Subjects

Gombe National Park is a narrow park in western Tanzania, bounded by the

eastern shore of Lake Tanganyika and a rift escarpment 1500 meters above the lake. The

park is characterized by a series of steep valleys that fall from the escarpment to the lake.

Habitat varies from evergreen forests in the valleys to and grasslands on the

ridge tops (Goodall, 1986). The environment is highly seasonal with a dry season from

May – October. Three chimpanzee communities are found within the park. The northern

Mitumba community was habituated in the 1990s and at the time of this study contained between 26 and 29 individuals (5 – 6 adult males and 8 – 9 adult females). The central

Kasekela community has been habituated since the 1960s and contained between 58 and

64 (11 – 15 adult males and 22 – 24 adult females) individuals at the time of study. The

southern Kalande community has been monitored since the 1990s and virtually all

individuals are known genetically. Demographic information is available for all

chimpanzees going back to the 1960s in the Kasekela community and the 1980s in the

Mitumba community.

To assess dispersal costs, I analyzed data from two time periods for which

detailed data were available on young females, including new immigrants: data

collected by KSW and assistants from May 2011-May 2014 and data collected by CMM

and assistants from December 2002 – September 2004. Five natal and six immigrant

59

females were the subject of intensive full-day focal sampling by KSW and assistants

(Table 2, Table 3) and together, comprise the main data set used in all analyses and

referred to as Data Set 1. In addition, staff at the Gombe Stream Research Center (GSRC)

conduct near-daily full-day focal follows on some adult individuals in the Kasekela and

Mitumba communities (Wilson, 2012). Follows generally focus on older adult females but between 2011 – 2014, seven of the ten females targeted by KSW were also followed by GSRC researchers. These data supplement data collected by KSW and when

appropriate are added to Data Set 1 and together are referred to as Data Set 1a.

An additional eight females, 4 natal and 4 immigrant, were the subject of 1-hour

focal sampling by CMM and assistants from December 2002-September 2004 (Table 2,

Table 3). When possible, these data are used in analyses to expand sample size and are

referred to as Data Set 2.

Females that were residing in their birth community, were nulliparous at the

start of the study period and had mated with a male over the age of 12 years were

assigned to the natal female group. One female, Eowyn, immigrated into Kasekela at

~3.8 years of age with her mother and was assigned to the natal group on the basis of her

long tenure as a juvenile in the community. The remaining natal females were known to

researchers since birth. Natal females ranged in age from 10 – 16. Females that were

inferred to be nulliparous, had mated with an adult male and had immigrated into the

community during or in the two years prior to the start of the study were assigned to the

60

immigrant group. Four immigrant females were known as infants and/or juveniles in

their home community (Kasekela and Mitumba), including two, Yamaha and Flirt, who

transferred during the main study period and were followed in both their natal and

transfer communities. Three immigrant females are known to have been born to mothers

residing in Kalande and one additional female was sampled genetically in Kalande before moving to Kasekela and is assumed to have been born there (Rudicell et al., 2010).

The origin of the remaining immigrants is unknown.

61

Table 2: List of subjects with demographic information.

Subject Date of Mom Date of Residence Birth Adult Birth Alive† Immigration Status Community Community Golden Jul -98 Y - N KK KK Glitter Jul -98 Y - N KK KK Flirt Jul -98 N Jun -12 N/I KK MT Yamaha Jul -98 N Mar -11 N/I KK MT Eowyn Apr -00* N - N KL KK Makiwa Jul -94* U Feb -11 I KL KK Rumumba Jul -97* N Jun -09 I MT KK Chem a Jul -02* U Jul -12 I UK KK Wema Jul -02* U Dec -13 I UK MT Tanga Apr -89 Y - N KK KK Sherehe Jan -91 Y - N KK KK Schweini Apr -91 Y - N KK KK Gaia Feb -93 Y - N KK KK Bahati Jul -88* N Oct -01 I MT KK Malaika Jul -89* Y Jul -02 I KL KK Nuru Jul -90* U Aug -02 I KL KK Eliza Jul -92* U Jan -04 I KL KK †Indicates if the subject’s mother was alive when she reached sexual maturity: Y=yes, N=no, U=unknown. Residence Status: N = Natal, I = Immigrant; Community: KK = Kasekela, MT = Mitumba, KL = Kalande, UK = Unknown, *Estimated birth date.

62

Table 3: List of subjects by data set, study period and observation hours.

Subject Data Study Observation KSW/CMM GSRC Focal Set Period Community Focal Hours Hours Golden 1 2010 -14 KK 360 352 Glitter 1 2010 -14 KK 361 280 Flirt 1 2010 -14 KK/ MT 84/108* 107/75* Yamaha 1 2010 -14 KK/ MT 0/144* 21/420* Eowyn 1 2010 -14 KK 234 33 Makiwa 1 2010 -14 KK 248 31 Rumumba 1 2010 -14 KK 273 111 Chema 1 2010 -14 KK 206 - Wema 1 2010 -14 KK 74 - Tanga 2 2002 -04 KK 46 - Sherehe 2 2002 -04 KK 11 - Schweini 2 2002 -04 KK 20 - Gaia 2 2002 -04 KK 17 - Bahati 2 2002 -04 KK 50 - Malaika 2 2002 -04 KK 48 - Nuru 2 2002 -04 KK 50 - Eliza 2 2002 -04 KK 14 - * Indicates observation time in birth and adult communities respectively.

63

3.1.2 Data Collection

KSW and assistants conducted focal follows, ranging in length from 1 – 10 hours

(median = 8 hours), on all natal and immigrant females in Data Set 1 for a total of 2092 observation hours. Focal samples began either at the location of the night nest or when an individual was located when searching the forest and ended at a predetermined time or when an individual was lost. Samples were distributed throughout the day and observations were made at a distance of 7 – 10 meters. Observers made an effort to cycle through each female before repeating a target but this was not always possible because of the social organization and large range of chimpanzees. GSRC follows were conducted in similar fashion except that follows typically ended when the target nested for the evening (Goodall, 1986). CMM and two assistants conducted 1 hour focal follows on females in Data Set 2 for a total of 256 observation hours. Focal follows were conducted such that no female was followed more than twice in one day and at least 2 hours passed between successive follows on the same individual.

3.1.2.1 Instantaneous Samples

To construct an activity budget, the behavior of the focal individual was noted on five minute intervals in Data Set 1 and on 10 minute intervals in Data Set 2 and transformed into % time spent engaging in each behavior. These data were then used to compare time spent in solitary activities including travel, rest and self-groom between

64

natal and immigrant females. Females were only included in analyses if they were observed for 10 or more hours in a given year, season and estrous state.

3.1.2.2 Travel Distance

Travel distance was calculated for Data Set 1a by dividing the meters travelled during a focal follow by the duration of the focal follow to obtain a rate of m/hour. For follows conducted by KSW, the location of the subject was marked every 30 minutes with a Garmin GPSMap 62s. For follows conducted by GSRC staff, the location of the subject was marked on a map every fifteen minutes to an approximate accuracy of 133 meters (Gilby, 2004). To account for the difference in sampling frequency, points at even numbered intervals in the GSRC data were excluded. Travel distance was only considered for follows that lasted four or more hours to avoid follows when a subject engaged in a single activity.

3.1.2.3 Feeding

All occurrence sampling was used to record feeding information in Data Set 1 and Data Set 1a. The start and end time, species and part consumed were noted for each feeding bout. In Data Set 2, feeding information was recorded at 10 minute intervals and only the part consumed was recorded. These data were then used to calculate three foraging metrics following Murray et al. (2006): diet breadth, diet quality and time spent foraging. Following established ecological theory I interpreted an increase in species consumed per season (diet breadth) as a general decrease in diet quality. This measure

65

was calculated for each female by dividing the number of species consumed in a

particular season and year by time observed. Because the species consumed was not

noted in Date Set 2, this analysis was restricted to Data Set 1a. Leaves and pith are

considered to be low quality food items in the chimpanzee diet (Conklin-Brittain et al.,

1998; Basabose, 2002) and in the Kasekela community, previous study shows that an

increase in the percent of leaves and pith consumed by an individual was correlated

with a decrease in body mass (Gilby et al., 2006). I, therefore, used the % of leaves and

pith in the diet as a measure of diet quality. This metric was calculated for each female by dividing the time spent foraging on leaves and pith by the total time spent foraging

in a particular year and season multiplied by 100. Finally we measured overall foraging

effort, controlling for party size. Previous study shows that chimpanzees spend less time

foraging in larger parties (Wrangham and Smuts, 1980) so I considered foraging effort in

small (1-4 adolescents or adults) and large parties (5 or more adolescents or adults),

following Murray et al. (2006). Foraging effort for each female was calculated by

dividing the total time spent foraging in party size x by the total time observed in party

size x multiplied by 100. In both data sets, females were only included in analyses if they

were observed for 10 hours or more in each season and year.

3.1.2.4 Social Behavior

In Data Sets 1 & 2, all occurrence sampling was used to record detailed

information on grooming time, aggressive events and copulations when the partner was

66

an adolescent or adult (10 years of age or greater). At adolescence both male and female chimpanzees begin to integrate into the adult community and participate frequently in copulatory and aggressive behavior. This age group is the peer group for most natal and immigrant females and is, therefore, considered along with adult individuals when looking at social interactions.

Every five minutes in Data Set 1 the identity of the target’s nearest neighbor and all adolescent and adult chimpanzees within five meters were recorded. Every 15 minutes party composition (individuals traveling together and within 100 meters at the time of the scan) was recorded, noting the estrous state of all cycling females. Estrous state was rated on a five point, 0 to 1 scale; 0 indicates a fully deflated swelling and 1 indicates a fully inflated swelling. Females with a score of 0-0.75 were considered anestrous and those with a 1, estrous.

To assess differences in grooming time and partner distribution in immigrant and natal females I calculated % total time engaged in social grooming, % time grooming others (out-grooming), % time being groomed (in-grooming) and % time self- grooming for each female when anestrous and when estrous. I also calculated the time spent social grooming with adolescent and adult males and females separately, as a percentage of time spent together with at least one member of that class. I calculated the number of grooming partners per female divided by total observation time to control for variation in observation time. I identified the top three partners for each female by time

67

spent grooming and noted the sex of each partner. For each female, I calculated received aggression from adolescent and adult males per thousand hours spent with individuals of that class when anestrous and when estrous. Aggression included threats, chases and physical encounters involving biting and hitting clearly directed towards a particular individual. The same rate and definition was used to ascertain rates of female-female aggression for each targeted female. To further assess patterns of female-female aggression I calculated dominance rank for all females present in Kasekela during the

2011 – 2014 study period. Chimpanzees emit unidirectional submissive vocalizations called pant grunts that are used to assign rank (Bygott, 1979). Pant grunts were used to calculate the mother’s Elo rank (Neumann et al., 2011) using a modified equation specific to female chimpanzees (Franz et al., in prep.). I then quantified the number of attacks by each female in the group against immigrant and natal females. Rank information was not available for Mitumba. Copulation rates per hundred hours spent with adolescent or adult males, while estrous, were calculated for each female.

To measure social association patterns between the target and other adolescent and adult individuals, I calculated a combined association index for each dyad following

Gilby & Wrangham (2008). This index is comprised of three social metrics: dyadic party association, 5 meter proximity and nearest neighbor index. To calculate dyadic party association I modified the Simple Ratio Index by simply dividing the number of 15 minute intervals the focal subject spent with each individual (P SubjectChimpA ) by the total

68

number of intervals that the focal subject was observed (P Subject ). Using this modification, only dyads that contained an immigrant or natal female were considered. I then calculated the SRI for each subject-adolescent/adult dyad in the community.

SRI SubjectChimpA = P SubjectChimpA /P Subject (1)

The 5 meter proximity index was calculated by dividing the total number of

intervals the subject was within 5 meters of each chimpanzee (I SubjectChimpA ) by the number of intervals the dyad was in the same party (P SubjectChimpA ); this controls for the number of times two individuals were in the same party, making the index independent of party- level association. A dyad that was rarely in the same party, but when together, spent most of their time within 5 meters would have a low SRI but a high 5M index. Proximity information was taken every five minutes and party information was taken every 15 minutes so the number of intervals in the same party was multiplied by three.

5M SubjectChimpA = I SubjectChimpA /P SubjectChimpA (2)

The nearest neighbor index was calculated by dividing the total number of

intervals the subject was nearest neighbor (N SubjectChimpA ) with each chimpanzee by the number of intervals the dyad was within 5 meters of each other (I SubjectChimpA ); again this

controls for the number of times two individuals were within 5 meters, making the

index independent of the 5M index.

NN SubjectChimpA = N SubjectChimpA /I SubjectChimpA (3)

69

Finally the combined association index (CAI) was calculated first by dividing

each metric by the mean across dyads for that metric. This allows for meaningful

comparison across indices, sexes and communities. Numbers greater than 1 for each

measure indicate a dyad affiliated more often than the average dyad while those under 1

indicate dyads affiliated less often than the average dyad. Each index was then added

together and divided by three.

NN SubjectChimpA = (SRI SubjectChimpA /SRI Mean + 5M SubjectChimpA /5M Mean + NN SubjectChimpA /NN Mean )/3 (4)

3.1.2.5 Physiological Data

Fecal glucocorticoid metabolites (FGM) were measured from non-invasively collected fecal samples from all females in the group, including resident females; sample collection began in 2010 so is only available for Data Set 1. Resident females were defined as females who had given birth in the community prior to the start of the study or had resided in the community as adults for 10+ years. Once collected, samples were stored in a freezer until on-site extraction which was conducted using a biologically and physiologically validated technique (Murray et al., 2013) that circumvents the difficulties in delayed extractions. In short, 5.0 ml of 90% ethanol was added to 0.5 g of feces into 16 x 100 mm tubes. The tubes were shaken by hand for 30 seconds, rotated for 2 hours and then centrifuged for 20 minutes at 1500 rpm. One ml aliquots of the resultant supernaturant were stored in 12 x 75 ml tubes and dried in a sealed case with desiccant

70

to prevent contamination. When samples were dry, they were capped and shipped to the Santymire Lab at the , Chicago, IL. Dried samples were reconstituted in 1 ml PBS immediately before analysis and 3-4 glass beads were added and sonicated for 20 minutes to ensure that all the dried hormone extract was in solution. FGM concentration was quantified using the cortisol enzyme immunoassay

(Wasser et al. 2000). In the assay, cortisol antiserum and horseradish peroxidase ligands were used at dilutions of 1:8500 and 1:20000 respectively.

Samples used for analyses were collected between Jan 2010 and Mar 2014. Only females that were experiencing monthly cycles, excluding pregnant or lactating females, were included in the final analysis and each female was assigned to a residence status

(Table 4). Natal and immigrant females follow the above definitions.

71

Table 4: Females used in analyses of FGM levels

Female Age in Jan 2010 Residence Status # of Samples Golden 11.5 N 30 Glitter 11.5 N 41 Flirt (KK) 11.4 N 32 Yamaha (KK) 11.4 N 6 Eowyn 9.7 N 31 Makiwa 15.5* I 38 Rumumba 12.5* I 78 Yamaha (MT) 11.4 I 29 Flirt (MT) 11.4 I 8 Chema 7.5* I 12 Bahati 21.5* R 5 Dilly 23.5 R 18 Eliza 17.5* R 58 Fanni 28.8 R 30 Ki para 25.5* R 21 Nasa 21.5* R 40 Sandi 36.2 R 50 Schweini 18.7 R 5 Sifa 31.5* R 32 Tanga 20.7 R 66 Trezia 31.5* R 32 Vanilla 21.5* R 5 *Estimated DOB

3.1.2.6 Reproductive Costs

To assess age at first birth in female chimpanzees, I identified 25 natal and 38 immigrant females from the long-term Gombe archive that resided in Kasekela or

Mitumba as adults and whose ages were known or estimated. Females were required to have reached 10 years of age, were known or estimated to be nulliparous at group entry and have no significant gaps in daily sightings prior to first parturition. This sample

72

includes four sterile females who had resided in a community as adults for 10+ years

without giving birth. Using conservative inclusion criteria, I included all females that fit

the above criteria and were followed to death or disappearance. Using a less

conservative approach, I excluded sterile females (n = 4) and nulliparous females who

matured in Kasekela and disappeared prior to age 14 and were assumed to have

immigrated (n = 5). In three cases, females could not be definitively assigned a residence

status. Two females were occasionally seen as juveniles in Kasekela but fully integrated

into the community as adults. In the conservative data set these females are considered

natal and in the less conservative data set, immigrant. One female, Eowyn, immigrated

into Kasekela at 3.8 years and was considered an immigrant in the conservative data set

and natal in the less conservative data set.

In instances where females were not known from birth, age at entry was

estimated based on body condition and size, using known age females as guidelines,

and is thought to be accurate plus or minus two years (Strier et al., 2010). Age at first birth or censored age was calculated for each female. Seven females never gave birth

prior to death or disappearance and a further six are still alive but not yet parous for a

total of 13 censored intervals.

Though age estimates are thought to be accurate, I identified a subset of 20

individuals (13 natal, 7 immigrant) whose birthdate was known to within six months,

who survived to adulthood and who remained in the park to breed to assess the same

73

patterns in females of known age. In this smaller dataset I was also able to assess differences in age at maturity to control for possible developmental differences between females who transfer and those who do not. The age at first mating with an adult male

(12+) was used to mark the attainment of sexual maturity (Pusey, 1990; Wallis, 1997).

One female, Moeza, was last seen in Kasekela in July of 1983 and not seen in Mitumba until July of 1985, making it possible, though unlikely, that researchers missed the birth of a prior offspring. Second, for 38 immigrant females to Kasekela and Mitumba, I calculated the interval between immigration and first birth to assess the length of the sterile window post-transfer and to identify potential differences between communities.

I also compared survival rates of the first offspring born to immigrant and natal females using the 50 females in the full dataset who had given birth to at least one offspring. By nature, this analysis excludes sterile females, females who disappeared without giving birth and living nulliparous females. This analysis was run on both the conservative and less conservative data sets. In this case, the only difference between datasets was the residence status of the three females discussed above.

3.1.3 Analyses

I used linear mixed models that controlled for repeated measures on the same individual to assess differences in travel, foraging and sociality metrics as well as changes in FGM levels, assigning significance at the 0.05 level. I first ran each model

74

with all potential fixed effects and removed the term with the highest p-value one by

one. If the reduced model did not improve by two or more AIC units the previous full

model was retained. I report p values for all terms that remained in the final model and

estimate effect sizes by comparing goodness of fit of the full model to a model without

the predictor variable. When predictor variables or interaction terms were non-binary I

conducted post-hoc Tukey HSD tests to identify significant pairwise differences. Model

specifics are discussed below for each outcome variable. All statistical tests were

performed with JMP, v. 11 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).

3.1.3.1 Travel Costs Post-Transfer

Separate models were run for travel time, rest time and travel distance. Each

model contained residence status as a main effect. Community, season, year, estrous

state, estrous state by residence status and season by residence status were also included

as main effects to control for additional sources of variance and potential interactions.

Subject ID was added as a random effect to control for repeated measures.

3.1.3.2 Diet Quality and Foraging Effort

Separate models were run for diet breadth, quality and foraging effort. For diet breadth and quality the models contained residence status, community, season, year and

season by residence status as main effects. For foraging effort party size was added as an

additional main effect. Subject ID was included in all models as a random effect.

75

3.1.3.3 Social Costs

Separate models were run for six grooming measures, three aggression measures and the combined association index. In all models residence status, estrous state, community and an interaction term residence status by estrous state were included as main effects. Subject ID was included as a random effect in all models. The main aggression model also included aggression type (male-female or female-female) as a main effect. The CAI model also contained sex of the dyad as a main effect. Copulation rate could not be compared between immigrant and natal females because only two natal females were frequently observed when in estrous and only descriptive statistics are reported. The proportion of female grooming and association partners was compared between natal and immigrant females using a Chi square analysis. Finally, I used the F-test for unequal variances to examine the variance in CAI values between immigrant and natal females.

3.1.3.4 Physiological Costs

FGM data were log-transformed prior to modelling. One model was run on FGM levels and included residence status, season, year, estrous state and an interaction term of residence status by estrous state as main effects. Because cortisol levels increase with age and can vary by time of day (Emery Thompson et al., 2010), age and time of collection were also included as main effects. Subject ID was included as a random effect.

76

3.1.3.5 Reproductive Costs

To test for differences in the age at which natal and immigrant females gave birth for the first time I used a Kaplan-Meier Survival Analysis and compared groups using the log-rank test. This analysis was run first on the conservative and less conservative full data sets and then only on females of known age. To control for possible differences in the age at sexual maturity, I also compared the age that females first mated with an adult male in natal and immigrant females of known age. Last, I used a Kaplan-Meier survival analysis to assess the length of the interval between immigration and first birth in immigrant females in Mitumba and Kasekela.

77

3.2 Results

3.2.1 Travel Costs

3.2.1.1 Dispersal

In total, of the 10 immigrant females targeted in this study, 8 transferred between adjacent communities and 2 did not. During the second study period (2011-2014) three females changed communities. One female, Flirt, was born into the Kasekela community, last seen in Kasekela in April of 2012 and first seen in Mitumba in June of

2012. Upon reaching maturity in 2010, Flirt was frequently absent for periods of a month or more, though she was never observed in Mitumba during these absences. Two unknown females, Chema and Wema, immigrated into the study population, one to

Kasekela and one to Mitumba. Both females were unrecognizable to field staff and subsequent DNA analysis determined that neither Chema’s microsatellite genotype nor her mtDNA haplotype matched any known individual within the park (Hahn,

Wroblewski unpublished data). Prior to her arrival in Kasekela, there were reports from areas northeast of the park of a lone individual matching Chema’s description, though her identity cannot be conclusively verified. Wema’s genotype also did not match any known individual; however she shared an mtDNA haplotype and 14 alleles across 18 microsatellites with resident female, Aphro. Kinship analysis rules out the possibility that Aphro is Wema’s mother but a more distant familial relationship may exist.

Nevertheless, Wema was never previously sampled within the park and is thought to

78

have originated outside. Coverage of all individuals, including those in Kalande is high, providing further support that they both originated outside the park. They arrived 18 months apart so it is unlikely that they traveled together.

3.2.1.2 Travel Costs Post-Transfer

Meters travelled per hour did not differ between natal and immigrant females

(Table 5). Community, estrous state and year remained in the final model and only community affected travel distance (Table 6); females in Kasekela travelled considerably further per hour than Mitumba females. When swollen, females tended to travel further though this was not significant. When analysis was restricted to Kasekela females, travel distance did not vary by any measured variable and only estrous state and year were retained in the final model.

Immigrant females did not spend more time travelling in their transfer communities than natal females of similar age (Table 5). Time spent traveling varied by estrous state and year; females spent more time traveling when fully swollen and reacted to yearly environmental and/or social differences (Table 6). Community was also retained in the model and females tended to spend more time traveling in Kaskela. Time spent resting also did not vary as a function of residence status but varied significantly according to season and year. Females rested more in the wet season.

79

Table 5: Mean and standard deviation for time spent traveling and resting.

Anestrous Estrous Metric Data Sets Natal Immigrant Natal Immigrant Travel Distance (m/hr) 1a 185 ± 96 175 ± 102 207 ± 70 196 ± 106 Travel Time (%) 1 & 2 16.8 ± 6.0 17.0 ± 5.8 21.0 ± 6.8 21.5 ± 7.6 Dry Season Wet Season Natal Immigrant Natal Immigrant Rest Time (%) 1 & 2 25.0 ± 8.9 27.9 ± 9.8 29.5 ± 9.0 32.0 ± 10.1

Table 6: Linear mixed model results for traveling metrics showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes.

Explanatory Variables: Outcome Residence Estrous Comm. Season Year Variable Status Travel Distance - p=0.07 p=0.039* - p=0.07 (All Females) ES=-0.001 ES=-0.002 ES=-0.002 Travel Distance - p=0.14 - p=0.23 (KK Only) ES=-0.001 ES=-0.001 Tra vel Time p=0.15 p< 0.0008* p=0.057 - p=0.026* ES=0.009 ES=0.034 ES=0.014 ES=0.054 Rest Time p=0.17 - - p=0.020* p=0.033* ES=0.010 ES=0.027 ES=0.071 Grey bars indicate the term was not included as a predictor. *Significant at p < 0.05

3.2.2 Feeding Costs

Foraging metrics are summarized in Table 7. No metric varied by residence status; immigrant and natal females had similar diet breadth, quality and effort (Table

8). Diet quality was primarily affected by season though community remained in the

80

final model. Diet quality increased in the wet season. Diet Breadth did not vary by any

measured variable and only season remained in the final model. Foraging effort varied by year, community and group size; females in small groups foraged more and

Mitumba females spent more time foraging than Kasekela females. While residence

status was not significant as a main effect the interaction between season and residence

status was significant. However post-hoc Tukey HSD comparisons did not reveal

significant differences between the terms, though immigrants forage more in the dry

season than in the wet season while no such difference exists for natal females.

Table 7: Mean and standard deviation of three foraging metrics

Dry Season Wet Season For aging Metric Data Sets Natal Immigrant Natal Immigrant Diet Breadth 1a 0.284 ± 0.095 0.326 ± 0.121 0.381 ± 0.175 0.340 ± 0.116 Diet Quality 1a & 2 23.13 ± 13.57 21.04 ± 11.69 17.53 ± 7.87 14.66 ± 8.25 Foraging Effort 1a & 2 42.86 ± 13.98 47.33 ± 1 8.16 43.37 ± 12.73 38.35 ± 15.49

Table 8: Linear mixed model results for three foraging metrics showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes.

Explanatory Variables Outcome Residence Community Season Year Party Season* Variable Status Size Res Status Diet Breadth - - p=0.172 - - ES=0.028 Diet Quality - p=0.145 p=0.039* - - ES=0.027 ES=0.037 Foraging p=0.38 p=0.017* p=0.54 p=0.016* p<0.001* p=0.030* Effort ES=-0.001 ES=-0.008 ES=-0.001 ES=-0.013 ES=-0.020 ES=-0.005 Grey bars indicate term was not included as a predictor. *Significant at p < 0.05

81

3.2.3 Social Costs

3.2.3.1 Grooming

Six grooming metrics were analyzed to quantify differences in time spent

grooming and grooming networks between immigrant and natal females; descriptive

statistics are summarized in Table 9 and model results are summarized in Table 10.

Rates of self-grooming did not vary by residence status or estrous state but the

interaction term between residence status and estrous state approached significance.

When anestrous, immigrant females spend more time self-grooming than natal females.

Time spent engaged in social grooming (% Time All Grooming) did vary by residence

status and the best fit model excluded all other main effects. Immigrant females spent

significantly less time engaged in social grooming than natal females. Neither in- nor

out-grooming with males varied by residence status (Figure 7). Instead both varied

significantly by community; females participated in more in- and out- grooming with

males in Mitumba than in Kasekela. For out-grooming with males the best fit model

included residence status, though not significant, and for in-grooming with males the best fit model included estrous state, though not significant. Time spent in- and out-

grooming females did vary by residence status (Figure 7). Immigrant females spent

significantly less time in- and out-grooming with females than natal females did. Out-

grooming with females also varied by estrous state; females spent more time grooming

females when anestrous than when estrous. A similar trend was detected for in-

grooming with females, though the term did not reach significance. The number of 82

partners per time observed did not vary by residence status and only estrous state was retained in the model, though it was not significant.

Table 9: Mean and standard deviation for seven grooming metrics.

Kasekela Mitumba Metric Data Set Natal Immigrant Immigrant % Time Self Grooming 1 & 2 3.60 ± 2.24 4. 39 ± 3.39 5.57 ± 2.00 % Time Social Grooming 1 & 2 5.41 ± 3.83 3.13 ± 1.87 3.46 ± 1.34 % Time out -Grooming Males 1 & 2 0.78 ± 0.70 1.45 ± 0.98 4.89 ± 2.90 % Time in -Grooming M ales 1 & 2 0.85 ± 1.01 1.59 ± 1.06 4.50 ± 4.28 % Time out -Grooming Females 1 & 2 4.09 ± 1.41 1.27 ± 0.87 1.23 ± 1.50 % Time in -Grooming Females 1 & 2 4.08 ± 2.56 1.08 ±0.89 1.19 ± 1.47 # of Partners 1 & 2 0.28 ± 0.16 0.27 ± 0.19 0.26 ± 0.10

Table 10: Linear mixed model results for grooming metrics showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes.

Explanatory Variables: Outcome Variable Residence Estrous Community Residence Status Status*E % Time Self Grooming p = 0.554 p = 0.92 - p = 0.06 ES = 0.006 ES = 0.010 ES = 0.031 % Ti me Social Grooming p = 0.036* - - - ES = -0.035 % Time out -Grooming Males p = 0.150 - p = 0.005* - ES = -0.022 ES = -0.092 % Time in -Grooming Males - p = 0.274 p = 0.009* - ES = -0.010 ES = -0.060 % Time out -Grooming Females p < 0.001* p = 0 .009* - - ES = -0.119 ES = -0.073 % Time in -Grooming Females p = 0.014* p = 0.070 - - ES = -0.050 ES = -0.031 # of Partners - p = 0.43 - - ES = 0.026 *Significant at p < 0.05

83

6 in-Grooming out-Grooming 5

4

3

2 % Time Time % Grooming 1

0 Female Male Female Male Natal Immigrant

Figure 7: Mean percent time in- and out-grooming by partner sex and residence status for natal and immigrant females. Error bars represent standard error.

3.2.3.2 Aggression

Three metrics were used to assess differences in rates of received aggression between immigrant and natal females (Table 11). Total aggression received did not differ by residence status and this term was excluded from the final model. Rather, total

received aggression varied by the sex of the aggressor and community (Table 12). Male-

female aggression was more frequent than female-female aggression and aggression

occurred more often in Mitumba than in Kasekela. Estrous state was also retained in the

model, though it was not significant. In an analysis of female-female aggression only,

residence status was significant; immigrants received aggression at higher rates than

84

natal females (Table 12, Figure 8). Community was also retained in the final model, though it was not significant. Male-female aggression did not vary by residence status and, though community and estrous state were retained in the final model, neither factor was significant (Table 12, Figure 8).

Table 11: Mean and standard deviation for three aggression metrics.

Kasekela Mitumba Metric Data Set Natal Immigrant Immigrant Aggression Rate –All 1 & 2 88.16 ± 111.25 92.85 ± 89.31 151.22 ± 124.78 Aggression Rate – FF 1 & 2 29.21 ± 33.75 80.92 ± 64.49 41.70 ± 53.16 Aggression Rate – MF 1 & 2 172.37 ± 131.28 104.79 ± 110.23 260.74 ± 51.41

Table 12: Linear mixed model results for received aggression showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes.

Explanatory Variables Outcome Variable Sex of Aggressor Status Estrous Community Aggression Rate – All p < 0.001* - p = 0.34 p = 0.048* ES = -0.023 ES = -0.001 ES = -0.007 Aggression Rate – FF p = 0.049* - p = 0.19 ES = -0.015 ES = -0.007 Aggression Rate – MF - p = 0.13 p = 0.11 ES = -0.008 ES = -0.008 Grey bars indicate the term was not included as a predictor. *Significant at p < 0.05

85

Figure 8: Received aggression per 1000 hours for natal and immigrant females separated by the sex of the aggressor. Error bars represent standard error.

Kasekela females threatened, chased or attacked immigrant females on 54 occasions and natal females on 12 occasions in Data Set 2 (Table 13). Coalitions occurred in two attacks against immigrant females. In one case, high ranking female Sparrow and her low-ranking daughter, Schweini teamed up to attack Chema and chased her vigorously for five minutes. In the second instance, Jiffy & Sifa, a strongly bonded pair of high ranking females with no dependent offspring (Foerster et al., 2015), threatened and attacked Makiwa. High-ranking female Fanni was most frequently aggressive towards immigrants but females of all ranks participated across the course of the study.

86

Table 13: Frequency of aggression against immigrant and natal females by all sexually mature Kasekela females present in the second study period (2011-2014).

# of Events Against… Ag gressor Residence Rank Immigrant Natal Status Females Females Sparrow R H 3 0 Gremlin R H 0 1 Fanni R H 10 0 Kipara R H 0 0 Sandy R H 1 1 Jiffy R H 1 0 Sifa R H 1 0 Trezia R M 7 0 Gaia R M 3 0 Tanga R M 1 0 Vanilla R M 1 0 Nasa R M 2 4 Bahati R M 0 1 Imani R L 5 0 Golden N L 0 0 Dilly R L 0 0 Nuru R L 0 3 Sch weini R L 1 0 Glitter N L 4 0 Eliza R L 5 1 Eowyn N (L) 0 0 Flirt N (L) 0 0 Rumumba I (L) 1 1 Makiwa I (L) 1 0 Chema I (L) 0 0 Residence Status: R=Resident, N=Natal, I=Immigrant. Rank: H=High, M=Middle, L=Low, (L)=not enough information to rank them; inferred to be low ranking due to age and parity.

87

3.2.3.3 Copulation

Only two natal females in data sets 1 & 2 experienced consistent and regular sexual cycles during the data collection period. This precluded statistical analyses on differences in copulation rate between immigrant and natal females. However, the copulation rate of both natal females (Flirt & Eowyn) exceeded the mean copulation rate with adult and adolescent males for immigrant females (Figure 9). In Flirt’s case, her copulation rate was more than four standard deviations above the mean in both categories.

Figure 9: Copulation Rate/100 Hours in immigrant females (grey bars) and two natal females; Flirt (triangles) and Eowyn (squares). Error bars represent the standard deviation in copulation rate for nine immigrant females.

88

3.2.3.4 Combined Association Index

The combined association index varied by residence status and sex of dyad partner (Table 14). Community and the interaction term residence status by sex were maintained in the final model, though not significant (Table 15). Immigrants had higher association indices than natal females and all target females had higher association indices with adolescent and adult males than with adolescent and adult females.

Immigrant females tended to have lower association rates with females than with males, when compared with natal females. The F-test found unequal variances in the distribution of CAIs by residence status (F = 1.93, p < 0.001). Natal females had higher variance in their CAIs, routinely having low CAIs with some individuals (generally unrelated females) and high CAIs with others (primarily maternal relatives). The distribution of CAIs for immigrant females was more normal (Figure 10).

Table 14: Mean and standard deviation for combined association index.

Natal Immigrant Metric Data Set Male Female Male Female CAI 1 1.0 ± 0.34 0.87 ± 0.66 1.34 ± 0.42 1.02 ± 0.34

Table 15: Linear mixed model results for combined association index showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes.

Explanatory Variables: Outcome Residence Status Sex Community Residence Variable Status*Sex CAI p < 0.001* p < 0.001* p = 0.22 p = 0.054 ES = -0.016 ES = -0.040 ES = -0.007 ES = -0.014 *Significant at p < 0.05

89

Figure 10: Distribution of the Combined Association Index by residence status and sex of the association partner.

In an analysis of the top three social partners by CAI for immigrant and natal

females (Table 16), immigrant females have a higher proportion of males among their

association partners than do natal females (Χ 2 = 5.396, p = 0.020). In the same analysis using the top three grooming partners for immigrant and natal females (Table 16), immigrant females have a higher proportion of males among their grooming partners than do natal females (Χ 2 = 9.76, p < 0.01). 90

Table 16: Top three association (CAI) and grooming partners by sex and maternal relatedness for each female during the study period.

Association Partners Grooming Partners Subject & Top Sex Maternal CAI Top Sex Mat ernal % Residence Partners Relative Partners Relative Grm Status Golden -N Gremlin F Mother 3.06 Glitter F Sister 37.2 Glitter F Sister 2.96 Gremlin F Mother 20.3 Gaia F Sister 2.42 Gaia F Sister 17.9 Glitter -N Golden F Sister 3.01 Golden F Sister 39.9 Gremlin F Mother 2.75 Gremlin F Mother 20.9 Gaia F Sister 1.98 Gaia F Sister 9.9 Flirt (KK) - Nasa F 2.13 Titan M 23.8 N Titan M 2.03 Nasa F 16.1 Sampson M 1.62 Frodo M Brother 12.9 Eowyn -N Imani F Sister 2.98 Imani F Sister 56.1 Faustino M 1.67 Makiwa F 7.9 Ferdinand M 1.6 Faustino M 5.2 Rumumba Ferdinand M 2.05 Fudge M 21.4 -I Fudge M 1. 95 Fanni F 12.2 Fundi M 1.73 Fundi M 11.0 Makiwa -I Ferdinand M 2.31 Fudge M 16.8 Kipara F 1.99 Chema F 15.5 Chema F 1.90 Eowyn F 12.9 Chema -I Titan M 2.21 Makiwa F 21.5 Ferdinand M 2.13 M 12.8 Sheldon M 1.95 Faustino M 11. 6 Yamaha -I Edgar M 1.68 Edgar M 29.1 Darbee F 1.30 Fansi M 15.3 Bima F 1.20 Darbee F 13.2 Flirt (MT) -I Edgar M 2.60 Edgar M 51.8 Apple M 1.58 Apple M 13.5 Fansi M 1.48 Fansi M 8.1 Wema -I Edgar M 2.03 Edgar M 16.6 Konyagi F 1.66 Darbee F 12.2 Lamba M 1.38 Londo M 10.6 Gaia -N Gremlin F Mother 59.0 Faustino M 26.0 Fanni F 7.0

91

Sherehe -N Schweini F Aunt 45.2 Sandi F Mother 38.1 Sparrow F Grandma 11.9 Schweini - Sparrow F Mother 69.4 N Nuru F 7.1 Faustino M 7.1 Tanga -N Patti F Mother 35.8 Faustino M 15.7 Schweini F 11.9 Bahati -I Goblin M 22.8 Fanni F 11.9 Faustino M 7.9 Malaika -I Sheldon M 22.9 Faustino M 18.1 Nuru F 12.4 Nuru -I Ferdinand M 44.9 Nasa F 29.6 Faustino M 12.2 Residence Status: N=Natal, I=Immigrant. Maternal Relative indicates if partner was the subject’s maternal relative. * CAI values were not computed in data set 2 and are left blank here. Residence Status: N=Natal, I=Immigrant.

3.2.4 Physiological Costs

FGM concentrations were affected by residence status, season, estrous state and

interactions between season*estrous and residence status*estrous (Table 17, Table 18).

Community, year, time of day and female age did not contribute to the final model.

FGM values varied significantly by residence status. A post-hoc Tukey HSD test found

that resident females had higher FGM concentrations than immigrant females (T = -3.25,

p < 0.01). FGM concentrations did not differ between immigrant and natal females or between resident and natal females. Season significantly affected FGM concentrations:

92

FGM is higher in the wet season. Though estrous state was retained in the final model, it was not significant. An interaction effect between season and estrous state was detected. Post-hoc Tukey tests found that FGM concentrations for anestrous females in the dry season differed significantly from anestrous (T = -7.65, p < 0.001) and estrous (T =

-5.08, p < 0.001) females in the wet season. FGM concentrations also differ between estrous and anestrous females in the wet season (T = -3.29, p < 0.01). A second interaction effect between residence status and estrous state was also detected. Post-hoc Tukey tests reveal a significant difference between FGM concentrations in immigrant anestrous and resident estrous females (T = -3.06, p = 0.028) and between immigrant estrous and resident estrous females (T = -3.79, p < 0.01; Figure 11).

Table 17: Mean and standard deviation for FGM

Dry Season Wet Season Metric Natal Immigrant Resident Natal Immigrant Resident FGM 19.5 ± 15.4 17.2 ± 8.2 19.5 ± 15.8 28.5 ± 18.4 23.9 ± 16.9 31.8 ± 28.1

Table 18: Linear mixed model results for fecal glucocorticoid metabolites showing the retained main effects with p values and estimated effect sizes .

Explanatory Variables Outcome Residence Season Estrous Season* Residence Variable Status Estrous Status*Estrous FGM p=0.012 * p<0.0001* p=0.37 p=0.014 * p=0.034 * ES=0.41 ES=-1.91 ES=0.10 ES=0.23 ES=0.27 *Significant at p < 0.05

93

Figure 11: Mean FGM levels by residence status and estrous state. FGM levels in resident estrous females are significantly greater than in immigrant anestrous and estrous females. Error bars represent standard error.

94

3.2.5 Reproductive Costs

The age at which females first give birth marginally differs between immigrant

(16.85 years ± 0.49, n = 38, 6 censored) and natal females (14.54 years ± 0.45, n = 25, 7 censored) when considering the conservative inclusion criteria (Χ 2 = 3.21, p = 0.07; Figure

12). This difference is significant when using the less conservative inclusion criteria

(immigrant: 16.52 years ± 0.43, n = 37, 4 censored; natal: 13.90 years ± 0.43, n = 20, 3 censored; Χ 2 = 17.28, p < 0.0001; Figure 12).

95

1.0 Immigrant Natal A 0.8

0.6

Surviving 0.4

0.2

0.0 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 Age at First Birth-Conservative B

Figure 12 : Kaplan -Meier survival analysis. A - Proportion of females giving birth by a particular age for immigrant (red) and natal (blue) females (p = 0.07) using conservative inclusion criteria. B- Proportion of females giving birth for the first time by[Figure] a particular age for immigrant and natal females (p < 0.0001) using less conservative inclusion criteria. 96

In an analysis of known age females only, using conservative inclusion criteria,

the age at which females reach sexual maturity (as measured by the age at first mating

with an adult male) does not differ between natal (11.20 years ± SE, n = 12, 0 censored

intervals) and immigrant females (10.94 years ± SE, n= 7, 0 censored intervals; Χ 2 = 0.09, p

= 0.77, Figure 13). The age at which females give birth for the first time also does not differ by residence status (Χ 2 = 1.03, p = 0.31). When the sterile female is excluded,

immigrants (16.51 years ± 1.55, n = 7, 1 censored) give birth for the first time later than

natal females (13.70 years ± 0.59, n = 12, 0 censored; Χ 2 = 3.97, p = 0.046; Figure 13).

97

Immigrant Natal A

Surviving

B

Figure 13 : Kaplan -Meier survival analysis. A - Proportion of females mating with an adult male by a particular age for immigrant and natal known-aged females (p = NS). B- Proportion of females giving birth for the first time by a particular age (excluding sterile females) for immigrant and natal known-aged females (p = 0.02).

98

Of 39 immigrants to Mitumba and Kasekela, the average time from immigration to first birth was 4.02 (±3.26) years. When two sterile individuals were excluded, this interval drops to 3.45 (± 2.17) years. Time to first birth did not differ by transfer community when including (Χ 2 = 1.62, p = 0.20, Figure 14) or excluding sterile females

(Χ 2 = 0.59, p = 0.44).

Kasekela Mitumba

Surviving

Figure 14 : Kaplan -Meier survival analysis on the time from immigrati on to first birth for all females (excluding sterile females) in Kasekela (red) and Mitumba (blue) (p = NS).

99

Survival of the first offspring did not differ between natal and immigrant females in both the conservative (Χ 2 = 0.02, p = 0.90) and less conservative data sets (Χ 2 = 0.40, p =

0.53; Figure 15). In the conservative data set the first offspring of immigrant females survived an average of 15.64 years (± 3.05, n = 32, 10 censored) and the first offspring of natal females survived an average of 14.61 years (± 4.20, n = 18, 4 censored). In the less conservative data set the first offspring of immigrant females survived an average of

16.65 years (± 2.97, n = 33, 10 censored) and the first offspring of natal females survived an average of 12.84 years (± 4.47, n = 17, 4 censored). Infant mortality was high; in the less conservative data set only 55% of first offspring born to immigrants and 47% of offspring born to natal females survived to age 5, the approximate age of weaning.

100

Immigrant Natal A

Surviving

B

Figure 15 : Kaplan -Meier survival analysis. Proportion of offspring surviving to a particular age for immigrant (red) and natal females (blue) in the conservative (A) and less conservative data sets (B) (p = NS).

101

3.3 Discussion

Contrary to my predictions, I found no evidence that dispersing females at

Gombe National Park experienced costs related to diet or travel. However, I did find differences in affiliative and agonistic interactions indicating that females experience costs associated with social integration. Puzzlingly, we did not find differences in the stress response, as measured by fecal glucocorticoid metabolites, between immigrant and natal females. Last, we did find that immigrant females experience reproductive delays but survival rates of the first offspring did not differ between natal and immigrant females.

In eastern chimpanzees, established females restrict their ranging when anestrous to a smaller portion of the community range (Wrangham and Smuts, 1980;

Hasegawa, 1990; Wrangham et al., 1992), remain loyal to these sites across years

(Williams et al., 2002b; Murray et al., 2007) and will actively defend these areas against other community females (Miller et al., 2014); suggesting they gain benefits from an intimate knowledge of a familiar foraging area (Pusey et al., 1997; Williams et al., 2002b;

Emery Thompson et al., 2007; Murray et al., 2007). Correspondingly, dispersing females were expected to face nutritional costs associated with leaving a familiar foraging territory and entering an unknown community range that is saturated or near-saturated with resident females. However, this study did not detect foraging shortfalls in dispersing females when compared to natal females of similar age with the exception

102

that immigrants may face seasonal differences in foraging effort that do not affect natal females.

There are several ways that females may be able to minimize feeding costs of dispersal. First, some may transfer socially but continue to use familiar foraging areas.

Of the ten immigrant females examined in this study eight of them transferred between adjacent communities. Four females came from the southern Kalande community and settled in the central Kasekela community. The Kalande population experienced a severe decline in the early 2000s (Rudicell et al., 2010) and since that time the Kasekela community has expanded southward and ranges deep into traditional Kalande territory.

Consequently, the southern portion of the Kasekela territory may be familiar to females emigrating from Kalande. By primarily dispersing socially rather than locationally

(Isbell and van Vuren, 1996), these females may have been able to reduce costs associated with an unfamiliar territory. In contrast, though Mitumba and Kasekela are adjacent communities, the primary feeding areas of each community are distinct for most of the year and locational benefits should be variable and depend on the ranging patterns of individual females. For example, Flirt ranged primarily in the northern half of Kasekela and visited areas of overlap while in her home community before transferring to Mitumba. On the other hand, Bahati, who transferred from Mitumba to

Kasekela, primarily ranges in the south of the Kasekela range and only rarely uses the small area of overlap between the communities. The remaining two females in the study

103

likely came from outside the park with no prior overlap in home ranges and were presumably unfamiliar with their new foraging area. Future studies with a more robust sample size should follow females from communities with differing degrees of overlap to assess if feeding costs increase with greater locational dispersal.

Second, females may minimize feeding costs via temporary visits prior to transfer. As females reach dispersal age some undertake visits, that can last up to several months, to nearby communities when they are in estrous and return home when anestrous (Nishida, 1979; Pusey, 1979, 1980). By visiting when in estrous and maximally attractive to community males (Deschner and Boesch, 2007), females can learn about available resources in potential transfer communities while avoiding potential conflicts with resident females (Pusey, 1979). Three females in the current study, Rumumba,

Makiwa and Yamaha, made documented visits to their transfer community prior to dispersal. In Makiwa’s case she made several visits, virtually all while fully swollen, to the Kasekela community over a period of five years before dispersing permanently.

Females likely gain valuable information on foraging and social opportunities, reducing costs when transfer actually occurs. Visits are unlikely to be free of cost and may represent a trade-off where females take on more costs during the initiation phase to decrease costs in the settlement phase (Bonte et al., 2012).

Finally immigrant females may have faced feeding deficits that were undetectable in the current study design. In particular feeding costs may be high in the

104

initial months after transfer but then level off quickly, even within the first year, as

females gain knowledge of the feeding territory. One female, Flirt, was followed

longitudinally from Kasekela to Mitumba. During her first year post-transfer her body

condition significantly deteriorated. Unfortunately, because she spent long periods of

her first year post-transfer missing from the group, it was impossible to collect data

during these absences. By her second year when she was seen more frequently, her

feeding behavior was similar to natal females, though she did have a high proportion of

leaves and pith in her diet. Every effort was made to follow females frequently

immediately after transfer but this often proved impossible because of the skittish nature

and unpredictable ranging patterns of immigrant females. Further effort should be made

to follow immigrants near daily through their first few months post-transfer before

conclusions can be reached on diet quality in immigrant females.

Though it was impossible to assess costs associated with the initial journey

systematically, these costs may be highly variable. The eight immigrants that transferred between adjacent communities presumably avoided costs associated with a lengthy

transfer journey. However, two females originated outside the park. The closest

confirmed population of chimpanzees in the neighboring region is ~12 - 16 kilometers

from the park boundary and the intervening landscape is a mix of wooded hillsides,

agricultural land, isolated settlements and villages (D. Mjungu, pers. comm). Despite

105

reforestation efforts, this journey would be arduous and villagers occasionally see chimpanzees near human settlements.

In contrast, travel costs upon settlement appear to be minimal. Prior work at multiple field sites show that incoming females primarily associate with resident males

(Pusey, 1990; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a). Males in most communities travel more widely than females and visit the edges of the range more frequently than do females (Wrangham and Smuts, 1980; Hasegawa, 1990;

Wrangham et al., 1992) so it was surprising that immigrants in this study did not travel further or for a greater proportion of each day than natal females. As in other studies

(Williams et al., 2002a), estrous females did spend more time traveling than anestrous females with a tendency to travel further, presumably reflecting increased travel in male-dominated parties. Females in the larger Kasekela community also traveled further, reflecting the larger community range size. An immigrant’s need to identify resource rich feeding areas was expected to further increase travel costs. The puzzling finding that travel costs did not differ by residence status may be explained by differing costs of locational and social dispersal, as discussed above, and future study should focus on the origin of immigrating females. Costs also may be more apparent within the initial months post-transfer and future study could concentrate on this time period.

As predicted I did identify several social costs associated with transfer. As in previous studies and observations from Gombe, Mahale and Kanyawara, immigrant

106

females were frequent targets of aggression from resident females. Immigrants experienced 2 - 3 times the amount of aggression targeted at natal females of similar age and received aggression from females of all dominance ranks. Though most aggression was non-physical in nature, more than 20% of interactions involved physical aggression that included biting and hitting and, on two occasions, coalitions of females attacked immigrants. Given that females of all ranks attack immigrants, immigrants are always at risk of attack when associating with residents. This is the first study to examine male aggression towards young females and immigrant females were not more frequently targeted by resident males. Immigrants appear to be equally attractive (or unattractive) as sexual partners based on the amount of aggression received. However, preliminary copulation rates suggest natal females may be more attractive. Further study is needed to parse this out given that copulation rates between natal females and resident males may have been driven by one aberrant female, rather than a real pattern.

There were considerable differences in social preferences based on residence status. Natal females primarily associated with their mother and maternal siblings. Of the two orphaned natal females, Eowyn maintained a strong association with her maternal sister but Flirt, despite having a maternal sister in the group, primarily associated with an unrelated female. Each natal female groomed frequently with her mother or primary female partner and, in most cases, spent over 70% of her time in association with them, prioritizing these relationships over all others. In contrast

107

immigrant females had more equal relationships with adult individuals and primarily associated with males. In each case their top social and grooming partner was an adult male and in some instances these relationships were strong, though they did not approach the level of mother-daughter dyads. Immigrant females spent less time grooming in all states and more time self-grooming when anestrous, than natal females did. The loss of their mother as a preferred social partner and their low social status in their new group likely contributes to this drop and represents a significant cost that could affect their health. This is further corroborated by looking at grooming time spent with males and females. While grooming rates with males did not differ between immigrant and natal females, immigrants groomed with females less frequently. The increase in self-grooming when anestrous may occur because when not attractive to adult males, immigrants struggle to fulfill their hygienic grooming needs and/or experience increased anxiety, possibly because they lose male protection from females when anestrous. In some instances immigrant females formed fairly strong bonds with other females, though these cases were the exception rather than the rule. Two immigrants formed a strong bond with each other and four immigrants had resident females among their top partners. Future studies will examine how relationship formation affects integration and time to first birth in immigrant females. In Mitumba, males were both more aggressive and more affiliative in some measures towards immigrant females. Mitumba had far fewer adult males, only one of whom was in his

108

prime and the effect of such demographic differences on social costs should be explored further.

Despite evidence of strong social costs associated with dispersal, fecal glucocorticoid metabolites were not elevated in immigrant females. Instead, immigrant females had the lowest FGM values overall and concentrations were slightly higher when anestrous, potentially reflecting their increased risk of female-female aggression when not attractive to males. Natal females had intermediate FGM concentrations across estrous states. Resident females, in contrast, had higher FGM concentrations overall and experienced a marked increase when in estrous. FGM concentrations were most sensitive to the season, rising in the wet season when food is more abundant but party size is larger, and were also affected by the year. That FGM concentrations markedly increased when resident females were in estrous follows previous work finding that older, parous females are frequent targets of male aggression due to sexual coercion

(Muller et al., 2007). In Kibale National Park, nulliparous females are less attractive to adult males (Muller et al., 2007; Watts, 2007) and are less frequently coerced (Muller et al., 2007). My data suggest a similar pattern may occur in natal and immigrant nulliparous females in Kasekela.

On the other hand, the lack of elevated cortisol in immigrants contrasts with findings from Kanyawara (Kahlenberg et al., 2008a). Several non-mutually exclusive factors could explain these results. First, at Kanyawara, natal and immigrant females

109

were not sampled in the same study period and yearly and seasonal differences may have contributed to elevated stress in immigrants. Furthermore, 4 of 5 sampled immigrant females transferred within 15 months of each other and female-female aggression rates were exceptionally high during this period. The single immigrant who transferred earlier experienced less aggression (though still elevated in comparison to natal females) and had an average stress level similar to that of natal females. Second, if immigrant females primarily face costs associated with sociality, as the current study suggests, the relative rarity of received aggression may make it difficult to detect elevated FGM in females because sample collection did not necessarily correspond to days when aggressive events occurred and the sample size may have made small differences in chronic stress levels difficult to detect.

The contrasting results from Kanyawara and Gombe suggest the sociopolitical climate plays an important role in the presence or absence of chronic stress in immigrants. Under the present social circumstances at Gombe, immigrant females seem to be able to cope with changes to their social environment but this may change if the community experiences high rates of immigration in a short time period. Moreover, natal females may also experience social stress, albeit of a different variety, as they seek to avoid mating with close male relatives. Finally, individual attributes such as psychological state, past experience with a stressor, genotype and overall health can all affect the magnitude and direction of a stress response and may explain these puzzling

110

results (Capitanio et al., 1998, 2005; Caldji et al., 2000; Mendoza et al., 2000; Sapolsky,

2005). In the future, studies should combine both longitudinal sample collection on females as they transfer with samples collected from females on days following an aggressive event to confirm or refute the current study results.

Finally, immigrant females experience reproductive delays but the survival of their first infant does not differ from that of natal females. In Kasekela, females who did not transfer gave birth 2.5 years earlier than those who did and following transfer, females experienced a sterile period that lasts, on average, 3.5 years. However, once an infant is born they face similar survival odds. Even early dispersers may not be able to erase the delay in time to first birth and all immigrants start the reproductive race disadvantaged by half an interbirth interval. In this slow-reproducing species such a delay may be difficult to make up. However one cannot discount the possibility that natal females may simply be deferring costs they avoided by not dispersing to later in life and that actual lifetime reproductive success does not differ by dispersal decision.

Mathematical modelling work done in gorillas suggests females who secondarily transfer can absorb a delay of up to eight months in conceiving if by dispersing to a new group they can increase the survival odds of their offspring (Robbins et al., 2009). Future study could determine if chimpanzees face similar odds.

Dispersing chimpanzee females in Gombe National Park may not accrue costs associated with travel and diet, however, they do face substantial social costs and a

111

delayed first birth. If the lack of energetic costs is confirmed, these patterns suggest that dispersing chimpanzees, much like dispersing females in folivorous species (Sterck et al., 2005), need not be concerned with energetic deficits upon arrival. The fission-fusion social system that permits females to forage solitarily and in small groups and the enhanced physical cognition that may facilitate foraging efficiency, coupled in some cases with a lack of locational dispersal, may allow immigrants sufficient flexibility in meeting their nutritional requirements post-dispersal such that any deficits can be strategically overcome. However, female chimpanzees face severe obstacles in entering an established social hierarchy and may need years to fully integrate into the community. That females disperse in all studied communities suggest a strong evolutionary pressure driving this behavior and may be best explained by a unique combination of benefits to male philopatry and long male tenure that forces females to disperse to avoid inbreeding (Williams et al., 2002b; Clutton-Brock and Lukas, 2012).

If one assumes that delaying the start of the reproductive window has far reaching effects, the benefits gained through dispersal must approximate or outweigh these costs. Previous work posits that the risk of inbreeding should be enough to drive dispersal in female chimpanzees (Pusey, 1980, 2005), given that male chimpanzees are long-lived and remain in their natal community for life (Goodall, 1986). While this may still be true, recent work (this volume) shows that the risk of inbreeding can be and is frequently overcome in natal females in the Kasekela community. An average female

112

may raise 1 – 4 offspring in her lifetime (Emery Thompson, 2013) but the most prolific

female on record is a natal Kasekela female, Fifi, who gave birth to seven surviving

offspring (Goodall and Pusey, 2015). This suggests that staying home can be unusually beneficial. Nevertheless, inbreeding does occur and the loss of a single infant may be enough to offset the delay in the start of reproduction incurred by immigrants. If inbred offspring survive to adulthood but are at a reproductive disadvantage or are infertile the costs of inbreeding could be substantially higher. In addition, the ability to stay in the natal community or the advantage to doing so, may vary by a female’s upbringing, personality and/or the social and environmental conditions present at maturity. In particular, core area availability and quality may shift the equation for some females.

These topics are the focus of the next chapter. Finally, with complete male philopatry, complete female philopatry is unsustainable. As more females do not disperse, there will come a point when females that leave will do better due to high inbreeding costs in the natal community and genes that promote dispersal will be passed on and maintained in the population. If dispersal responses are adaptive, females should be able to assess current conditions to determine if the complete elimination of inbreeding risk will outweigh the steep costs associated with transfer that delay the onset of reproduction.

113

4. What factors influence dispersal decisions in chimpanzees?

Dispersal theory posits that an individual’s decision to leave an area can be

dependent on both the environmental and social conditions present at the time and their

own internal state or phenotype (Bowler and Benton, 2005; Clobert et al., 2009). This

suggests animals respond to variation in the costs and benefits of dispersal over the

short-term (Clobert et al., 2001) and act accordingly in making a dispersal decision by

assessing conditions through the use of social cues and exploration (Clobert et al., 2009).

Condition and phenotype dependent dispersal has been identified in a variety of taxa but has rarely been studied in primates (Clobert et al., 2009; Cote et al., 2010). In

primates, natal dispersal, or dispersal from the place or group of one’s birth before breeding, shapes spatial distribution, population regulation and most importantly,

kinship and social structure within groups (Ghiglieri, 1987). Primates, due to their

extreme sociality, extended life histories and limited reproductive potential (Ross, 1998),

should be especially sensitive to internal and external cues present when making

dispersal decisions. Even among primates, chimpanzees ( troglodytes ) have extremely

slow life histories (Emery Thompson, 2013) and unsurprisingly, show some flexibility in

dispersal decisions. Males are philopatric and only disperse in rare cases; juveniles or

adolescents occasionally disperse with their mother or siblings (Goodall, 1986; Emery

114

Thompson et al., 2006). Dispersal in adult males has only been documented in one community of western chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes verus ) and probably results from habitat fragmentation and the unique socioecology of the community (Sugiyama, 1999,

2004). On the other hand, female dispersal has been documented at every study site; the majority of females leave the community they were born in around sexual maturity

(Pusey et al., 1997; Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000; Nishida et al., 2003; Sugiyama,

2004; Reynolds, 2005; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a; Langergraber et al., 2009). The proportion of non-dispersing females is typically under 10% but in the Kasekela community of

Gombe National Park approximately 50% of females breed in their natal community

(Pusey et al., 1997). Here we capitalize on the unusual dispersal pattern in Kasekela to assess condition and phenotype dependent dispersal in chimpanzees.

Across species, dispersal decisions can be influenced by a range of environmental and social factors that primarily include intraspecific competition, predation risk, kin interaction, inbreeding risk, mate choice and habitat quality (Clobert et al., 2009). Each of these factors, with the possible exception of predation, could reasonably affect dispersal in chimpanzees. Male chimpanzees gain unusual benefits by cooperating with kin to defend a joint territory (Williams et al., 2004) and consequently, females are thought to disperse to avoid mating with relatives (Bygott, 1979; Pusey,

1979, 1987). However in light of new findings that females that stay in their natal community may be able to overcome, though not eliminate, the risk of inbreeding

115

(Schroepfer-Walker et al., 2013) females may also be attuned to other conditions present

around sexual maturity and respond to levels of kin interaction, intrasexual competition,

mate choice and habitat quality. Predation on chimpanzees is rare, though variable

(Boesch, 1991; Tsukakara, 1993), and is expected to contribute only minimally, if at all, to

dispersal decisions.

Theoretical and behavioral work suggests maturing female chimpanzees are

especially sensitive to breeding with close relatives. Chimpanzees exhibit a range of behaviors that have been interpreted as mechanisms to avoid inbreeding including the avoidance of and resistance to mating attempts from relatives (Van Lawick-Goodall,

1968; Pusey, 1980; Goodall, 1986; Wroblewski, 2010). Females rarely mate with maternal relatives (but see Stumpf, et al. 2009), though the rate at which they mate with paternal relatives is highly variable (Nishida, 1979; Pusey, 1980; Wroblewski, 2010). Dispersing virtually eliminates the risk of close inbreeding and accordingly, females with male relatives in their natal group should be more likely to transfer to a new community. The number of male relatives in the community can vary considerably; some females have no brothers while others have as many as five (Goodall and Pusey, 2015). Thus, the risk of close inbreeding will differ for each female and will further depend on the degree to which individuals can recognize and avoid paternal kin (Parr et al., 2010; Wroblewski,

2010; Schroepfer-Walker et al., 2013). Puzzlingly, in Kasekela many females fail to disperse despite the presence of fathers and/or maternal and paternal brothers in the

116

group (unpublished data). Therefore, the existence of unrelated males in the group may be enough to overcome this cost and discourage dispersal when multiple unrelated mates are available. Even in the absence of relatives, females may be sensitive to mating opportunities within the home community and disperse if available mate choice is insufficient and the chances of increasing the mating pool are more likely outside the home community. However, given that mammalian females are theorized to be limited primarily by access to resources (Trivers, 1972), the benefits of increasing the mating pool may be minimal, especially if female mate choice is restricted by sexual coercion

(Muller et al., 2007; Feldblum et al., 2014). Nevertheless, At Taï National Park, immigrations into the community were more frequent when the community contained a large number of males (Boesch and Boesch-Achermann, 2000) and in the Mitumba community of Gombe National Park, 4 of 5 maturing females emigrated, each doing so when the community contained 3 or fewer males (unpublished data). In mountain gorillas, where females also disperse and conceptions are monopolized by the dominant males, females sometimes remain in their natal community if additional unrelated subordinate males are available and in each case these males sired their offspring

(Vigilant et al., 2015). Finally, in chimpanzees, if male relatives are high ranking or alpha it may be more difficult to avoid mating with these individuals because alpha males sire the greatest proportion of offspring and higher-ranking males tend to be more

117

aggressive and coercive (Vigilant et al., 2001; Muller and Wrangham, 2004; Wroblewski et al., 2009; Feldblum et al. 2013).

Intrasexual competition among females plays an important role in shaping female behavior (Pusey and Schroepfer-Walker, 2013). In western chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes verus ) females compete directly over food and dominant females tend to win such contests (Wittig and Boesch, 2003). In eastern chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ), females are less gregarious and engage in fewer contests over food

(Pusey and Schroepfer-Walker, 2013). Instead, females concentrate their ranging in overlapping core areas (Nishida, 1979; Wrangham and Smuts, 1980; Williams et al.,

2002a, 2002b; Emery Thompson et al., 2007) and primarily avoid competitors by spending time alone (Williams et al., 2002a; Murray et al., 2007). As the community range size changes and females enter or leave the group, the location and size of these areas can change, but they remain fairly stable throughout life (Williams et al., 2002b).

Each additional female in a group increases competition for other resident females

(Pusey and Schroepfer-Walker, 2013) and while there is no evidence that residents evict young females (Pusey, 1980), they aggressively attack new immigrants (Nishida, 1989;

Townsend et al., 2007; Kahlenberg et al., 2008a; Pusey et al., 2008), occasionally commit infanticide to remove future competitors (Townsend et al., 2007; Pusey et al., 2008) and defend their core areas against other females (Miller et al., 2014). Accordingly, the level of female-female competition in a community may promote transfer. In Mahale National

118

Park, females were more likely to disperse when female density was high and feeding competition was presumably intense (Nishida, 2012). On the other hand, females employ counterstrategies that minimize competition through the use of fission-fusion ranging patterns and core area placement (Pusey and Schroepfer-Walker, 2013) and a female’s ability to strategically alter her ranging and social patterns in the face of competition may dampen any effect that female-female competition has on an individual’s dispersal decision.

In contrast, kin interaction, and especially the presence and rank of female kin, may be important when considering dispersal. In many mammalian species, including most primates, females benefit from the presence of female kin through cooperation in feeding and social contexts (Sterck et al., 1997; Clutton-Brock, 2009). Furthermore, strong social bonds correlate with increased reproductive success in three cercopithecoids (Silk et al., 2009, 2010; Schülke et al., 2010) and similar effects are likely to occur in other social primates. Bonds between adolescent females and their mothers can remain strong even as daughters begin spending time with adult males (Pusey, 1990; Stumpf et al., 2009) but in most cases are severed when the daughter disperses. Strong bonds between adult mother-daughter pairs among females who did not disperse were first noted in the

Kasekela community at Gombe (Goodall, 1986) and have subsequently been confirmed in Kasekela (Foerester et al., 2015) and recorded at other field sites (Gilby and

Wrangham, 2008; Langergraber et al., 2009). Interestingly, at Kanyawara, females who

119

were tightly bonded with their mother tended to delay dispersal (Stumpf et al., 2009).

Individuals that form strong bonds with maternal female relatives may accrue benefits

from remaining in their natal community through overt cooperative behaviors such as

coalitions and by increasing their collective resource holding potential. These benefits

should be further enhanced when the mother is high-ranking. Dominant females have

foraging and reproductive advantages (Pusey et al., 1997; Murray et al., 2006; Emery

Thompson et al., 2007) that may be inherited by their daughters. In particular, the

quality of the mother’s core area, which tends to correlate with dominance rank (Murray

et al., 2007), may influence a daughter’s dispersal decision. If core area quality is high,

daughters may be able to forage within their mother’s core area and reap foraging and

social benefits not available to females whose mothers occupy low quality core areas

that cannot support an additional individual.

Finally habitat quality cannot be discounted. If an individual’s home community

is sub-optimal, dispersal from that community may be more likely (Bowler and Benton,

2005). This is difficult to determine in chimpanzees with large home ranges and few

study communities. However, temporal environmental variability may also be

important (Bowler and Benton, 2005) and food availability affects both natal and breeding dispersal rates in a variety of taxa (Kuussaari et al., 1996; Lurz and Garson,

1997; Kim, 2000). Chimpanzees are affected by food availability in numerous ways.

Party size, activity budgets and ranging patterns are influenced by fruiting patterns at

120

most studied sites (White and Wrangham, 1988; Matsumoto-Oda et al., 1998; Murray et al., 2006; Wittiger and Boesch, 2013). Detailed analysis at Gombe shows that, in

Kasekela, chimpanzees are heavier in the wet season when food is more abundant

(Pusey et al., 2005) and the proportion of low quality leaves and pith in the diet correlates with a decrease in body mass (Gilby et al., 2006). Furthermore, females reproduce more slowly when the range size is small, an effect thought to be driven by reduced food availability (Williams et al., 2004). At Kanyawara in Kibale National Park,

Uganda, female reproductive function increases in time of high fruit availability (Emery

Thompson and Wrangham, 2008). Given that habitat quality has direct consequences on reproductive success, females that mature during a time of reduced food availability, be it due to harsh environmental conditions or a shrinking home range, may be prone to seek out communities with a higher quality habitat.

Phenotypic variation is also expected to affect dispersal and some individuals may be better adapted to disperse than others (Bowler and Benton, 2005; Clobert et al.,

2009; Cote et al., 2010). In cases where individuals can be flexible in dispersal decisions it would be beneficial for individuals to assess their likelihood of success given their phenotype. The most straightforward example of phenotypic-dependent dispersal occurs when dispersers and non-dispersers have different morphological phenotypes.

For example, in naked mole rats, individuals who leave their natal colony to breed are larger and heavier than those who do not (O’Riain et al., 1996; Braude, 2000). If such

121

variation exists in chimpanzees it would be difficult to detect though there are anecdotal

reports from Gombe where a small female was repelled from a new community while a

large female was able to immigrate into the same community shortly thereafter (Pusey et

al., 2008).

Differences in behavior and personality have also been found between dispersers

and non-dispersers in a variety of species and are more easily assessed than body size in

wild chimpanzees. In addition to being larger in size, dispersing mole rats also

participated more frequently in locomotor and feeding activity and less frequently in

cooperative behaviors (O’Riain et al., 1996). Similarly, dispersing yellow-bellied

marmots were more likely to have a peripheral position in the social network (Blumstein

et al., 2009). Personality dependent dispersal has been identified in a handful of species

and is suspected to occur widely throughout the animal kingdom (Cote et al., 2010).

Personality, broadly defined as consistent individual differences in behavior, cognition

and affect across different contexts, exists in a variety of animal species (Gosling and

John, 1999; Gosling and Vazire, 2002; Briffa and Weiss, 2010; Weiss and Adams, 2013).

Particular aspects of personality (e.g. boldness, sociability & aggressiveness) have been

implicated in dispersal in species as diverse as Trinidad killifish (Fraser et al., 2001), bluebirds (Duckworth and Badyaev, 2007) and side-blotched lizards (Cote and Clobert,

2007).

122

Unsurprisingly, variation in personality has been detected in numerous captive

chimpanzee populations and is assumed to occur in wild populations (King and

Figueredo, 1997; Pederson et al., 2005; Weiss et al., 2007; Dutton, 2008; Weiss et al. 2009;

Massen et al., 2013). Though personality is best assessed by questionnaires that have been validated using pre-determined behavioral observations (Lilienfeld et al., 1999),

chimpanzee personality has been measured with some success solely using personality

questionnaires administered to caretakers who interact with individuals on a daily basis

(King and Figueredo, 1997). These surveys are derived from a large body of literature on

human personality (King and Figueredo, 1997), show evidence for interrater reliability

of personality scales (Weiss et al., 2009; Weiss & Adams, 2013), find consistent results

with different captive populations (King and Figueredo, 1997; Weiss et al. 2007; Weiss et

al., 2009) and can accurately predict behavior (Pederson et al., 2005). In chimpanzees, six

personality dimensions have been identified, five of which correspond to human

dimensions of personality. Agreeableness, conscientiousness, openness, neuroticism and

extraversion are present in both chimpanzees and humans. Chimpanzees have an

additional sixth dimension labeled dominance. These dimensions vary by sex and stage

of development, similar to patterns observed in humans (Weiss and King, 2015), but are

otherwise relatively stable across an individual’s lifetime (Dutton, 2008; King et al.,

2008). In general as individuals get older, personality changes reflect increases in

maturity, self-control and emotional stability (Roberts et al., 2008; Weiss and King, 2015).

123

Different facets of personality may prove influential at different stages of

dispersal. Openness to new experiences may encourage dispersal while extraversion,

dominance and agreeableness may promote integration into a new community after

dispersal. In particular, extraversion and agreeableness may aid individuals in

integrating and allow them to quickly form relationships with suitable social partners. In

captive chimpanzees extraversion is positively associated with affinitive behaviors

(Pederson et al., 2005). Furthermore, extraversion peaks in infancy and remains high

through adolescence before dropping off in adulthood (King et al., 2008), suggesting this behavior is adaptive during the transfer period. Measures of openness, such as tendency

to explore, correlate with dispersal propensity in a variety of species (Dingemanse et al.,

2003; Rasmussen and Belk, 2012; Debeffe et al., 2013). Openness also remains elevated

through adolescence in chimpanzees and may promote exploration around the

community boundaries, facilitating dispersal. Dominance, a personality trait associated

with agonistic behavior, emotional stability and assertiveness, may also aid in this

transition if females are able to better exploit the competitive environment in their new

community. Alternatively, extraversion and agreeableness may enhance cooperative behaviors within the home community and discourage dispersal by strengthening the

value of bonds to individuals that exhibit high levels of these traits.

Here, I use data from over 50 years of study in the Kasekela and Mitumba

communities in Gombe National Park to first determine the frequency and flexibility of

124

transfer from each community. I then use these data to investigate what factors predict

transfer decisions in female chimpanzees. I first examine conditional factors, including

kin interaction, inbreeding risk, mate choice and habitat quality, that promote dispersal

in 24 females that have matured in the Kasekela community. Of these five factors, I

predict that dispersing females will be at a higher inbreeding risk and have less kin

interaction than non-dispersing females while habitat quality, intrasexual competition

and mate choice will only differ marginally between dispersing and non-dispersing

females. I, then, examine personality’s effect on the transfer decisions of 62 females from both communities where I predict that dispersers will have high levels of extraversion,

dominance and openness.

4.1 Methods

4.1.1 Study Site

Gombe National Park is an area of 35 sq km bounded by the eastern shore of

Lake Tanganyika and a rift escarpment 1500 meters above the lake. The park is

characterized by a series of steep valleys that fall from the escarpement to the lake.

Habitat varies from evergreen forests in the valleys to woodlands and grasslands on the

ridge tops. The environment is highly seasonal with a dry season from May – October.

Three chimpanzee communities are found within the park. The central Kasekela

community has been habituated since the 1960s and group size has ranged from a

125

minumum of 38 individuals to a maximum of 62 (12 – 25 adult females and 7-14 adult males). The northern Mitumba community has been habituated since the 1990s and has ranged from a minumum of 19 individuals to a maximum of 29 (5 – 10 adult females and

2 – 5 adult males). The southern Kalande community has been monitored since the 1990s and individuals are known genetically. From the 1990s onwards, if individual females disperse within the park, they can be tracked from their natal to their transfer community. Staff at the Gombe Stream Research Center (GSRC) conduct near-daily full- day focal follows on adult individuals in the Kasekela and Mitumba communities. Data on party composition and location is recorded every 15 minutes and the behavior of the focal individual is recorded in narrative form (Goodall 1986).

4.1.2 Frequency of Dispersal

To ascertain dispersal patterns among female chimpanzees in Gombe National

Park I used the demographic archive to assign a dispersal status to each female that matured in or entered Kasekela beginning in 1965 and Mitumba beginning in 1990. By

1965 and 1990 respectively, observers had identified most community members and could accurately discern incoming from resident females. Females who were born into or observed as young juveniles in a community, lived past age 12 and remained there until presumed death or are currently adult were considered non-dispersers. Females who were born and/or observed in one community but left around the onset of sexual

126

maturity in good health were considered to have undergone natal dispersal, regardless

of whether they were subsequently observed in a different community. Females who

were unknown as juveniles but entered a community under observation and were

inferred to be nulliparous were also considered to have undergone natal dispersal and

those who entered with an offspring, or were inferred to be fully adult and probably

parous were considered to have undergone breeding dispersal. Females who gave birth

to their first offspring in the community where they were born but subsequently

transferred to a new community were also classified as undergoing breeding dispersal.

In rare cases, females could display both natal and breeding dispersal if they first

transferred when nulliparous and then transferred again after giving birth.

In addition to a dispersal status, each female was assigned a certainty of

dispersal. If a female was observed across a lifetime (either from birth or as a young juvenile), regardless of dispersal, her dispersal status was considered certain and

assigned a value of 1. Females that immigrated into a community under observation and

were inferred to be nulliparous or were known to have offspring were also assigned a

certainty of 1. By 1965, individuals could be identified with high confidence but to be

conservative, females thought to have immigrated into the community in 1965 were

assigned a certainty of zero because these females may have been present in the

community prior to this time as natal females but not yet observed. Females who

matured in a community of observation, disappeared around sexual maturity in good

127

health and were not seen again were also assigned a certainty of zero. While it is likely these females dispersed, death cannot be ruled out. Finally, females that were only seen occasionally or were difficult to classify were also given a zero and are discussed on a case by case basis.

4.1.3 Condition Dependent Dispersal

Condition dependent dispersal was analyzed by examining five variables potentially important in determining transfer decisions: presence of female kin, presence of male kin, presence of unrelated males, habitat quality and intrasexual competition

(Table 19). Twenty three females that were observed to be born into and reached sexual maturity in the Kasekela community were used in this analysis. An additional female,

Eowyn, transferred into the community at 3.8 years of age with her mother. Since she matured in Kasekela she was included in this analysis to bring the final count to 24. Four females that were first seen in Kasekela as juveniles in the 1960s and were presumed to have been born there were excluded because they reached sexual maturity prior to the onset of systematic data collection which precluded the calculation of some measures.

Females that were born/matured in and gave birth in Kasekela were considered non- dispersers, as was one female, Skosha, who never gave birth but resided in her natal community until her death at age 33. Two females, Pom and Dominie, gave birth to their first offspring in Kasekela and subsequently transferred. For the purposes of this

128

analysis they were considered natal females due to the location of their first birth.

Females that were observed or inferred to have left the community in good health around or shortly after the onset of sexual maturity were considered dispersers. Of the ten dispersing females, seven were subsequently observed in Mitumba and three dissappeared in good health. The date at which females were first observed to mate with an adult male (12 years plus) was used to mark the attainment of sexual maturity

(Pusey, 1990; Wallis, 1997). This date was then used to assess social and environmental factors present in the community at that time.

Table 19: Predictor variables.

Factor Description Female Kin Presence Mom’s Elo rank & # of maternal female relatives 12 + Maternal Brothers # of maternal brothers 5+ & alpha as brother Unrelated Males # of adult males (12+), excluding maternal brothers Intraspecific C ompetition Density of settled females & # of settling females Habitat Quality 99% MCP of range & % leaves/pith in community diet

4.1.3.1 Presence of Female Kin

I combined two measures to assess the presence and support potential of female kin. I first calculated the mother’s dominance rank when her daughter reached sexual maturity. Chimpanzees emit unidirectional submissive vocalizations called pant grunts that are used to assign rank (Bygott, 1979). Pant grunts were used to calculate the mother’s Elo rank (Neumann et al., 2011) using a modified equation specific to female

129

chimpanzees (Franz et al., in prep.) on the day her daughter first mated with an adult male. If the subject’s mother was deceased, she was assigned a rank of 0 to indicate an absence of maternal support. The rank of maternal sisters was not included because they were almost uniformly low ranking. I then calculated the number of adult maternal female relatives in the group (mothers and maternal sisters 12 years of age or older).

Both measures were z-transformed to put them on the same scale and added together to create a composite score for presence of female kin.

4.1.3.2 Inbreeding Risk

Paternity information was only known for a subset of females preventing a systematic analysis of the presence of close paternal relatives on transfer outcomes and only close maternal relatives were considered in determining inbreeding risk. To measure inbreeding risk I counted the number of maternal brothers alive at sexual maturity for each female. For 13 females with known paternity, none had full male siblings present in the group. The exact relationship (full or half siblings) could not be determined for females with unknown paternity but given the promiscuous mating system, I assumed all brothers were related to their sisters at the level of half-siblings.

Brothers were only included if they were five years of age or older. Infant mortality is high (Bronikowski et al., 2011) and though the exact timing of weaning is difficult to determine in chimpanzees, it likely occurs prior to or around five years of age (Plooij

130

and van de Rijt-Plooij, 1987; Emery Thompson et al., 2012; Smith et al., 2013). If a

female’s maternal brother was alpha when she reached sexual maturity, she was given

an additional point to reflect a further increase in inbreeding risk as alpha males sire a

greater proportion of offspring in most studied communities (Boesch et al., 2006;

Wroblewski et al., 2009; Newton-Fisher et al., 2010).

4.1.3.3 Unrelated Males

To measure mate choice, I counted the number of adult males 12 years of age or

older in the community when each female reached sexual maturity. Males as young as

12 have sired offspring in Gombe and are thus considered viable mates (Wroblewski et

al., 2009). Maternal brothers were excluded from these counts. Although fathers were

known for a subset of females, this information was not available for the majority because genetic sampling did not commence until 1995. Therefore, males known to be

fathers were, nevertheless, included.

4.1.3.4 Intrasexual Competition

I combined two measures to assess the level of intrasexual competition in the

community at sexual maturity for each female. I calculated the first measure, female

density, by dividing the number of settled adult females by the community range size in

the year leading up to maturity (see below). Settled females were defined as females 15

131

years or older and/or parous. Females are considered resident once they have given birth in a community but some females fail to give birth for years following maturity

and in some cases never do. To account for these cases, I considered all females 15 years

or older (the typical age at first birth is ~15 years) as settled in the community. As female

density rises in a community resources will become scarcer and female-female

competition will increase. I also included the number of settling females present at

sexual maturity for each female.

Settling females were defined as females between the ages of 8 and 15 who had

not yet reproduced or females who were seen on less than 10% of male follows. Females

that meet these criteria are presumably working to establish an adult presence and core

area in the community but could still transfer elsewhere. Maturing females may be

sensitive to the number of individuals trying to integrate into a community. If there are

many settling females and all are successful this would increase pressure on available

resources and potentially make the integration period difficult. In Kanyawara, when

many new females entered the group aggression increased four fold; a level of increase

not seen when there were fewer settling females (Kahlenberg et al., 2008a). To create a

composite score for intraspecific competition, both numbers were z-transformed and

then added together.

132

4.1.3.5 Habitat Quality

I combined two measures to assess habitat quality in the year leading up to sexual maturity as environmental conditions during this period may be especially potent to females as they begin to devote energetic reserves to reproductive behaviors.

Females at Kanyawara are more likely to transfer in favorable environmental conditions, possibly to ease the transition into a new community. Therefore, habitat quality at maturity may influence dispersal decisions. First, I calculated community range size by taking a 99% MCP around all points where focal chimpanzees in the Kasekela community were observed in the year prior to sexual maturity by GSRC researchers. In

Kasekela, food abundance is thought to increase with range size (Williams et al., 2004) so larger range sizes indicated increased habitat quality. I also calculated the percent of leaves and pith in the diet, a measure that, in Kasekela, negatively correlates with body mass (Gilby et al., 2006). To do this, I divided the time spent feeding on leaves or pith for all focal follows in the year prior to maturity by the total time spent feeding and subtracted the value from 1. Both scores were z-transformed and added together to create a composite score for habitat quality.

4.1.4 Personality

Personality of each chimpanzee was assessed using a 24-item questionnaire derived from the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire (Weiss et al., 2009, Appendix A)

133

as rated by 18 active or retired Tanzanian field assistants. In most instances, field

assistants had actively followed chimpanzees through much of their lives. Each

chimpanzee in the Kasekela community was rated by an average of 2.96 field assistants

and in the Mitumba community by an average of 5.88 field assistants. The 24 items were

chosen so that each of the six personality dimensions previously identified were

represented by four items that had a high loading factor in previous studies (Weiss et al.

in prep) and were weighted following Weiss et al. (2009). Each item of the questionnaire

included an adjective and a brief description to set the adjective in the context of

chimpanzee behavior (e.g. SOCIABLE: Subject seeks and enjoys the company of other

chimpanzees and engages in amicable, affable interactions with them). Scores to each

question were assigned on a 7 point scale (1 being ‘displays total absence or negligible

amounts of the trait, 7 being ‘displays extremely large amounts of the trait’). The

questionnaire was first translated into Swahili and back-translated into English to check

for errors prior to the start of data collection. Questionnaires were completed

individually and raters were not allowed to discuss their answers with others. Inter-

rater reliability was somewhat lower than in previous studies of captive chimpanzees but was similar to those found in human personality studies (Weiss et al. in prep). Rater consistencies [ ICC{3,k) ] ranged from 0.04 to 0.81. Items with an estimate of agreement below zero were dropped from the analyses.

134

4.1.4.1 Subjects

To analyze associations between personality and transfer outcomes I took

advantage of a larger dataset that includes 62 females that resided in Kasekela or

Mitumba as adolescents and/or adults during the study duration and had their

personalities rated. In addition to adding females from Mitumba, I also included

individuals of unknown origin who immigrated into either Kasekela or Mitumba as

sexually mature females. Females were categorized as non-dispersers if they matured

and/or gave birth in the community in which they were born (n = 16). Females were

categorized as dispersers if they were known to have entered one community from

another (n = 46). Four females disappeared around sexual maturity and were assumed to be dispersers.

Personality is fairly stable across an individual’s lifetime but some

developmental changes do occur (King et al., 2008). Extraversion and openness decline

somewhat with age while agreeableness, dominance and conscientiousness increase

(King et al., 2008; Weiss and King, 2015). Our dataset should be robust to such subtle

changes because most raters were present for an individual’s entire tenure in the group

and, therefore, personality ratings should reflect an individual’s average personality

over their lifespan. Nevertheless, to control for age as a potential confound we identified

25 females from the larger dataset who were known to raters prior to sexual maturity (as

135

defined by the date of first mating with an adult male) and used these females in a

conservative analysis of personality and dispersal.

4.1.5 Data Analysis

4.1.5.1 Condition Dependent Dispersal

To facilitate statistical analyses, scores for the five condition dependent dispersal

measures were z-transformed to put them all on the same scale. I first conducted Mann-

Whitney U tests on each z-transformed score, comparing non-dispersing and dispersing

females. I then used a nominal logistic regression on the binary measure of stay or go.

Each of the five dispersal factors were included in the model as main effects. I ran all

possible model combinations and used AICc model selection criteria to determine the best fit model. I considered the model with the lowest AICc value and all models within

two AICc units of the best fit as having substantial support. Mann-Whitney U Tests and

regression analyses were performed in JMP, v. 11 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).

4.1.5.2 Personality

Sample size precluded the use of linear mixed models to assess the individual

contribution of each personality domain on transfer outcome. Instead individual t-tests

were run on each personality domain using a significance value of 0.05. All analyses

were performed in R (R Core Team, 2014).

136

4.2 Results

4.2.1 Frequency of Dispersal

The dispersal status of all adult females observed in Kasekela and Mitumba is summarized in Table 20. Of 33 females born/matured in Kasekela, 19 females did not disperse and remained in their natal community for life or to the present. Four of these females (Candy, Spray, Little Bee and ) were difficult to track and/or classify and were assigned a certainty of 0. Candy’s mother, Caramel, was a peripheral female who was only sporadically seen in Kasekela with her dependent offspring, including

Candy. After Candy reached sexual maturity she became a full time resident of

Kasekela. Similarly, Spray’s mother Sprout was also a peripheral member of Kasekela.

When Spray was ~15 she permanently settled in Kasekela. Little Bee and Honey Bee were sisters who matured during an unstable period when the main Kasekela community cleaved in two, forming a new Kahama community. This community only survived for ~4 years before losing all of their males to intercommunity aggression. Both

Little Bee and Honey Bee went to the Kahama community with their mother. Little Bee returned permanently to the Kasekela community around sexual maturity and remained there for her lifetime. The younger Honey Bee, only returned on visits to Kasekela when

Kahama ceased to exist, disappearing four years later without producing any offspring.

137

Two females (Dominie and Pom) gave birth to their first offspring in Kasekela

and then dispersed thereafter. Twelve nulliparous females emigrated out of Kasekela,

seven of whom were followed to their transfer community. The remaining five

disappeared in good health and may have transferred outside of the park. One female

who initially emigrated to Mitumba (Tita) secondarily transferred to Kalande. When

including all females maturing in Kasekela, including those with a certainty of zero, 58%

remained in their natal community to breed and did not (or have not yet) undergo breeding dispersal. When non-dispersing females with a certainty of zero are excluded,

the rate drops to 52%. During the course of the study 34 females immigrated into

Kasekela, including three that were born in Mitumba. One female, Eowyn, immigrated

to Kasekela as an infant with her mother, Echo, and has remained there to breed. Two

females, Hope and Joanne, immigrated into the community with younger individuals

presumed to be their offspring.

Of five females born into Mitumba, one did not disperse and is breeding in her

natal community. The remaining four dispersed; three to Kasekela and one disappeared

in good health. In addition to the eight females that immigrated to Mitumba from

Kasekela (though one, Tita, subsequently left to Kalande), five additional females have

immigrated into Mitumba from unknown communities during the course of the study.

One immigrant female, Eva, was difficult to identify and as a result was assigned a

certainty of zero as she could have been present in the community as a natal female.

138

Table 20: Origin and Adult Community for all females surviving to adulthood in Kasekela and Mitumba during the study period.

Chimp Analyses DOB DOE/ Birth Adult Transfer Cert. DOI** Comm. Comm. Type Kasekela Born Candy 1 Jul -69* - KK KK None 0 Dilly 1,2 ,3L,3C Jun -86 - KK KK None 1 Fanni 1,2 ,3L,3C Mar -81 - KK KK None 1 Fifi 1,3 L Jul -58* - KK KK None 1 Gaia 1,2 ,3L,3C Feb -93 - KK KK None 1 Gigi 1,3 L Jul -54* - KK KK None 1 Gilka 1,3 L Jul -60* - KK KK None 1 Glitter 1,2,3 L,3C Jul -98 - KK KK None 1 Golden 1,2 ,3L,3C Jul -98 - KK KK None 1 Gremlin 1,2 ,3L,3C Nov -70 - KK KK None 1 Honey B 1 Jan -65 - KK KK None 0 Little B 1 Jul -60* - KK KK* None 0 Miff 1,3 L Jul -56* - KK KK None 1 Sandi 1,2 ,3L,3C Oct -73 - KK KK None 1 Schweini 1,2 ,3L,3C Apr -91 - KK KK None 1 Sherehe 1,2 ,3L,3C Jan -91 - KK KK None 1 Skosha 1, 2,3L,3C Mar -70 - KK KK None 1 Spray 1 Jul -69 - KK KK None 0 Tanga 1, 2,3L,3C Apr -89 - KK KK None 1 Dominie 1, 2,3L,3C Nov -72 Dec -91 KK KK/KL BD 1 Pom 1, 2,3L,3C Jul -65 Nov -83 KK KK/ MT BD 1 Aphro 1, 2,3L,3C Jun -73 Nov -88 KK MT ND 1 Darbee 1, 2,3L,3C Feb -84 Nov -98 KK MT ND 1 Flirt 1, 2,3L,3C Jul -98 Jun -12 KK MT ND 1 Flossie 1, 2,3L,3C Feb -85 Jan -96 KK MT ND 1 Moeza 1, 2,3L,3C Jan -69 Jul -83 KK MT ND 1 Yamaha 1, 2,3L,3C Jul -98 Mar -11 KK MT ND 1 Tita 1, 2,3L,3C Jan -84 Oct -94 KK MT /KL ND/B D 1 Bumble 1 Jul -54 * Jun -68 KK UK ND 0 California 1, 2,3L Aug -84 Mar -95 KK UK ND 0 Conoco 1, 2,3L Jan -91 Dec -01 KK UK ND 0 Sally 1 Jul -53 Feb -67 KK UK ND 0 Wunda 1, 2, 3L Dec -78 May -90 KK UK ND 0

139

Kasek ela Immigrants (unknown or KL origin) Athena 1, 3L Jul -52* May -65 UK KK ND 0 Caramel 1 Jul -54* Sep -75 UK KK BD 0 Chema 1 Jul -02* Jul -12 UK KK ND 1 Dove 1, 3L Jul -57* Feb -71 UK KK ND 1 Echo 1, 3L Jan -72* Sep -03 KL KL/ KK BD 1 Eliza 1, 3L Jul -92* Dec -03 UK KK ND 1 Eowyn 1, 2 Apr -00* Sep -03 KL/KK KK None* ** 1 Harmony 1, 3L Jul -61* Dec -73 UK KK ND 1 Hillary 1, 3L Jul -81* Oct -92 UK KK ND 1 Hope 1, 3L Jul -70* Oct -92 UK KK BD 0 Imani 1, 3L Jul -92* Feb -05 UK KK ND 1 1, 3L Jul -66* Jun -75 UK KK ND 1 Jiffy 1, 3L Jul -73* Aug -85 UK KK ND 1 Joanne 1, 3L Jul -56* Nov -78 UK KK BD 1 Kidevu 1, 3L Jun -66* Feb -79 UK KK ND 1 Kipara 1, 3L Jul -84* Mar -97 UK KK ND 1 Makiwa 1 Jul -94* Mar -06 KL KK ND 1 Malaika 1, 3L Jul -89* Jul -02 KL KK ND 1 Nasa 1, 3L Jul -88* Oct -00 UK KK ND 1 Nope 1, 3L Jul -50* Mar -65 UK KK ND 0 Nova 1, 3L Jul -53* Sep -65 UK KK ND 0 Nuru 1, 3L Jul -90* Aug -02 KL KK ND 1 Pallas 1, 3L Jul -52* Mar -65 UK KK ND 0 Patti 1, 3L Jul -61* Mar -71 UK KK ND 1 Sifa 1, 3L Jul -78* Aug -00 UK KK ND 1 Sparrow 1, 3L Jul -58* Sep -71 UK KK ND 1 Sprout 1 Jul -41 * Dec -66 UK KK BD 0 Titania 1, 3L Jul -85* Feb -98 UK KK ND 1 Wanda 1, 3L Jul -56* Sep -69 UK KK ND 1 Winkle 1, 3L Jul -58* Aug -68 UK KK ND 1 Yolanda 1, 3L Jul -83* Aug -97 UK KK ND 1

Mitumba Born Bima 1, 3L,3C Jul -92* - MT MT None 1 Rumumba 1, 3L,3C Jul -97* Jun -09 MT KK ND 1 Trezia 1, 3L,3C Jul -78* Feb -91 MT KK ND 1 Vanilla 1, 3L,3C Jul -88* Jan -05 MT KK ND 1 Aqua 1, 3L,3C Apr -92 Jun -03 MT UK ND 0

140

Mitumba Immigrants (unknown or KL origin) Konyagi 1, 3L Jul -84* Jul -94 UK MT ND 1 Eva 1 Jul -65* Jul -95 UK MT ND 0 1, 3L Jul -86* May -01 UK MT ND 1 Mgani 1, 3L Jul -91* Jun -04 UK MT ND 1 Wema 1 Jul -02* Dec -13 UK MT ND 1 Communities: KK=Kasekela, MT=Mitumba, KL=Kalande, UK=Unknown. Analyses (indicates what analyses female was included in): 1=Frequency of Dispersal, 2=Condition dependent dispersal, 3=Personality (L=Less conservative, C=Conservative). Dispersal Type: None=Non-Dispersers, ND=Natal Dispersal (prior to birth of first offspring), BD=Breeding Dispersal (following birth of first offspring). Certainty: 1=Certain, 0=Uncertain. *Estimated birthdate. **DOE=Date of Emigration, used for females born into Kasekela and Mitumba, corresponds to date last seen in natal community. DOI=Date of Immigration, used for females of unknown origin, corresponds to date first seen in observed community. ***Eowyn arrived with her mother as a juvenile into Kasekela and has remained there to breed.

4.2.2 Condition Dependent Dispersal

In an analysis using Mann-Whitney U tests no measure differed significantly between dispersers and non-dispersers (Table 21; Figure 16). Using nominal logistic

regression, our model selection procedure identified four indistinguishable models that

could explain the likelihood of dispersing (Table 22). Inbreeding risk appeared in every

model and was significant at the 0.05 level in each case. Females that had more male

maternal relatives present in the group at maturity were more likely to disperse. Habitat

quality and intrasexual competition appeared in none. Presence of female kin appeared

in two models, including the model with the lowest AICc value. Females with more

female relatives and/or a high ranking mother were less likely to disperse. The number

141

of unrelated males also appeared in two models. Females were more likely to disperse when there were more unrelated males in the group at maturity.

Table 21: Means, standard deviations and results of Mann-Whitney U tests for dispersers and non-dispersers across five measures.

Mean & Standard Deviation Mann -Whitney U Test Non -Dispersers Dispersers Z, p (n = 14) (n = 10) Intrasexual Competition 0.01 ± 0.74 -0.02 ± 1.32 -0.73, 0.46 Presence of Female Kin 0.21 ± 0.97 -0.30 ± 1.01 -1.29, 0.20 Inbreeding Risk -0.33 ± 0.54 0.46 ± 1.32 1.51, 0.1 3 Unrelated Males -0.09 ± 0.91 0.13 ± 1.08 0.51, 0.61 Habitat Quality -0.01 ± 1.01 0.01 ± 1.04 -0.03, 0.98

Figure 16: Z-Transformed means for each predictive factor by dispersal status. Error bars represent standard error.

142

Table 22: Nominal logistic regression model selection results with effect sizes for each term.

Model Unrelated Males Inbreeding Risk Presence of Female Kin AIC c ΔAIC c 1 -1.29* 0.88 32.95 0.00 2 -0.96* 33.18 0.22 3 -0.63 -1.24* 34.27 1.31 4 -0.70 -1.70* 0.92 34.33 1.37 * denotes term was significant at p < 0.05.

4.2.3 Personality

Personality was significantly associated with transfer outcomes (Figure 17). In both the less conservative (Table 23) and conservative datasets (Table 24) dispersing

females scored lower in extraversion (i.e. introverts). In the less conservative dataset,

dispersing females also scored lower in agreeableness and openness. In the conservative

dataset agreeableness and openness scores were lower for dispersing females and

approached, but did not reach, significance.

Table 23: Means, standard deviations and t-test results for six personality domains in dispersers and non-dispersers in the less conservative dataset

Non -Dispersers (n = 16) Dispersers (n = 46) T p Dominance 3.92±0.77 3.84±0.61 0.39 0.70 Agreeableness 4.95±0.71 4.23±0.69 3.56 <0.001 Neuroticism 3.36±0.67 3.68±0.56 -1.82 0.07 Openness 4.27±0.80 3.41±0.57 4.66 <0.001 Conscientiousness 4.64±0.71 4.73±0.54 -0.43 0. 63 Extraversion 4.87±0.62 4.14±0.61 4.13 <0.001

143

Table 24: Means, standard deviations and t-test results for six personality domains in dispersers and non-dispersers in the conservative dataset

Non -Dispersers (n = 14) Dispersers (n = 11) T p Dominance 3.81±0.73 3.79±0.78 0.05 0.96 Agreeableness 4.86±0.60 4.39±0.58 1.99 0.06 Neuroticism 3.35±0.75 3.61±0.61 -0.98 0.34 Openness 4.22±0.73 3.64±0.74 2.02 0.06 Conscientiousness 4.66±0.70 4.67±0.57 -0.02 0.99 Extraversion 5.08±0.64 4.18±0.62 3.56 <0.01

Figure 17 : Mean personality measures across six personality domains for non - dispersers and dispersers in the less conservative data set (A) and the conservative data set (B). Extraversion differs in both data sets while agreeableness and openness differ in the less conservative data set (p < 0.5). Error bars represent standard error.

144

4.3 Discussion

Females in Gombe National Park show flexibility in their dispersal patterns. The majority of females maturing in Kasekela do not disperse while those maturing in

Mitumba do disperse. Moreover, though rare, females sometimes disperse as parous females. As predicted, we detected both condition and phenotype dependent differences in females who did and did not transfer. Among female chimpanzees born into the

Kasekela community, data suggest that condition dependent dispersal decisions were primarily motivated by inbreeding avoidance but presence/dominance rank of female kin and mate choice may have also played a role. There was no evidence that our measures of intrasexual competition or habitat quality motivated dispersal decisions. In an assessment of personality differences between females who did and did not transfer in Kasekela and Mitumba, dispersing females scored low in extraversion, agreeableness and openness. Unfortunately, because of the long generation times of chimpanzees these analyses were based on a small number of females, in particular when investigating condition dependent dispersal, and the picture will become clearer as more females mature. Nevertheless, these results form a foundation for continued investigation of the proximate and phenotypic drivers of dispersal decisions in female chimpanzees.

The presence of weaned adult male maternal brothers, including if a brother was alpha male, at sexual maturity increased the odds of dispersal in Kasekela females.

Dispersal in chimpanzees is theorized to occur primarily to prevent inbreeding (Bygott,

145

1979; Pusey, 1979, 1987) and these results show for the first time that dispersal rates

increased when there were more opportunities for inbreeding within a group. Mating

sometimes occurs between sexually receptive females and their maternal brothers

(Pusey, 2005; Stumpf et al., 2009; Wroblewski, 2010), though the rate at which this occurs

can vary. Therefore, if relatives are present, dispersal is the most complete means to

eliminate the possibility of breeding with kin. The risk of inbreeding is expected to

increase if a female’s brother is alpha male when she reaches maturity. Across field

sites, alpha males sire a greater proportion of offspring than lower ranking males

(Vigilant et al., 2001; Wroblewski et al., 2009; Newton-Fisher et al., 2010) and at

Kanyawara and Kasekela, higher ranking males are more aggressive than lower-ranking

males (Muller and Wrangham, 2004; Feldblum et al. 2014). Taken together, females may

find it difficult to avoid copulating with their high-ranking brothers and having a brother as alpha male may serve as a potent cue to maturing females that the risk of

inbreeding would be elevated in their home community. The interaction between rank

and copulation between relatives should be explored in the future. Finally, not all

females have multiple male relatives in the group and these individuals may find it easy

to avoid a single relative allowing them to capitalize on other factors that may make

philopatry more advantageous. As data accumulate future analyses should also include

male paternal relatives.

146

The presence of a strong female kin network has previously been implicated in

the unusual dispersal patterns seen in the Kasekela chimpanzees; females with a high

ranking mother, with whom they are strongly bonded seemed to forego dispersal more

frequently, especially if the mother has a high quality core area (Pusey, 1983, 1990). The

current study confirmed the importance of the mother’s dominance rank and the

presence of female kin in promoting female philopatry. Among several studied species

of primates, strong social bonds are correlated with a variety of reproductive measures

(Silk et al., 2009, 2010; Schülke et al., 2010; Gilby et al., 2013) and the reproductive utility

of social bonds is predicted to be widespread in the primate family (Silk, 2007).

Therefore, it is likely that female chimpanzees that are centrally placed within the social

community and are strongly bonded to other individuals may accrue significant

advantages across a lifetime, especially if they can build upon relationships started as juveniles. These beneficial relationships then may significantly alter the cost/benefit ratio

in dispersal decisions in favor of philopatry. Though the presence and rank of female kin

seems to be especially important, three ‘orphans’, Eowyn, Dilly and Skosha, did not

transfer. Eowyn had an older maternal sister in the community and the two females

remained tightly bonded following the death of their mother (Chapter 2, this volume).

When Dilly was 5.5 years old, her mother unexpectedly transferred to Kalande and Dilly

did not accompany her, effectively leaving her orphaned with no other female relatives

in the group. Skosha’s mother died when she was 5, also leaving her with no female

147

relatives. However, both Dilly and Skosha formed strong relationships with other adult group members; Skosha with a parous female (Goodall, 1986) and Dilly with an adult male (unpublished data). This raises the possibility that cooperative support need not necessarily come from maternal kin and that strong relationships with unrelated individuals could mimic advantages accrued through having a living mother.

Alternatively, orphan females may benefit from the opportunity to acquire their mother’s vacant core area. This was not the case for Dilly and Skosha; the location of their adult core areas are distinct from those of their mothers. Eowyn only recently reproduced and the location of her adult core area is unknown. Young orphan females are likely at a competitive disadvantage due to body size, age and the lack of kin allies and under most circumstances, are unlikely to inherit a core area if orphaned prior to adulthood.

Here, I used mother’s dominance rank and the presence of female kin to measure female kin support but future efforts should also explore association patterns between adolescent females and other group members. In the Kanyawara community in Kibale

National Park a high proportion of females disperse but dispersal is delayed in females who are tightly bonded with their mother (Stumpf et al., 2009). This suggests that while dispersal is beneficial in this community, females may attempt to maximize the benefits of a stable social partner as long as possible. Particular behaviors that promote cooperation with others, such as those seen frequently in non-dispersing mole rats

148

(O’Riain et al., 1996), may also prove useful in predicting dispersal decisions and further

enhance the finding that kin support and cooperation can discourage dispersal. Future

work should also make use of social network measures to assess if an individual’s social

position affects dispersal as it does in yellow-bellied marmots, where individuals that

are highly socially embedded within a community, are less likely to disperse (Blumstein

et al., 2009).

Interestingly, the presence of unrelated males was associated with dispersal

decisions; females were more likely to stay when there were fewer available unrelated

mates, arguing against mate choice as a driver of dispersal. It is important, however, to

keep in mind that fathers may have been included as unrelated mates and with more

data on paternity this result may change. Nevertheless, with a larger sample size, it

would be informative to look for interaction effects between the number of unrelated

males and inbreeding avoidance. It could be the case that a simple threshold value is

needed to allow for sufficient mate choice among female chimpanzees. For example, one

natal Kasekela female, Fanni, gave birth to five offspring; three of whom were fathered by one unrelated male and two of whom were fathered by another. Despite having four

maternal brothers, her father and several additional unrelated males present in the

group, she was able to target her reproductive effort to two specific unrelated males. The

number of adult males in a community varies greatly, even within Gombe and further

study could address male demographic influences on mate choice. Over the past 20

149

years in Mitumba there have been five or fewer adult males present at any given time

and the dispersal rate has been much higher than in Kasekela, where there are typically

ten or more males.

When turning to phenotypic dependent dispersal I found evidence that

dispersing and non-dispersing females varied in certain personality traits. Most clear

was that extraverted females are less likely to disperse. There was also evidence that

agreeableness may play a similar role. These data show that the role of extraversion and

agreeableness in promoting interactions with others is more influential in the home

community than during the integration process into a transfer community. Theoretical

work suggests high dispersal costs can promote the rapid evolution of cooperative behaviors among non-dispersers (Bowler and Benton, 2005; Le Galliard et al., 2005) and

has been found empirically in mole rats (O’Riain et al., 1996). In yellow-bellied marmots,

individuals that were more socially embedded in their groups were less likely to

disperse (Blumstein et al., 2009). Various measures of increasing sociality also promote

philopatry among side-blotched lizards (Cote and Clobert, 2007), mosquito fish (Cote et

al., 2010, 2011) and humans (Jokela et al. 2008); three species with vastly different social

systems. However, in both lizards and humans, density differentially impacts

personality-dependent dispersal; social individuals are only more likely to stay in

groups or areas of high density. In chimpanzees, highly social individuals may be better

placed to exploit their familiar social environment and may have a more central role in

150

the social network, diminishing motivation to set out into the unknown. Meanwhile, introverted and disagreeable individuals may seek out a new group in the absence of strong ties holding them to their home community. In these cases, the benefits to dispersal, such as the elimination of inbreeding, may easily outweigh the costs given that their personality is likely to inhibit social interactions in their adult community, regardless of history and location.

Finally, there was some evidence to suggest openness varies between dispersers and non-dispersers. Interestingly, contrary to my prediction, high levels of openness were associated with non-dispersers. This contradicts a growing body of literature that has found associations between openness and/or exploratory tendencies and dispersal propensity (e.g. blue tits (Dingemanse et al., 2003), roe deer (Debeffe et al., 2013), southern leatherside chub minnows (Rasmussen and Belk, 2012)). This effect, however, may be most pronounced if dispersal requires overcoming challenging barriers (Cote et al. 2010) and in a captive study with few dispersal barriers openness was not associated with dispersal in mosquito fish. Perhaps in chimpanzees, transfer between adjacent or nearby communities is not regarded as a risky undertaking, especially in light of new evidence suggesting minimal energetic costs (previous chapter). This puzzling result could be clarified by contrasting females who disperse to nearby communities to those that travel further distances. Finally, the openness measure used here could be

151

misrepresenting an individual’s actual propensity to explore and take risks and effort

should be made to refine this measure using behavioral observations.

If dispersal is condition and phenotype dependent why do we see near complete

dispersal at most field sites? The simplest explanation is that outside of Kasekela, the benefits of dispersal clearly outweigh the costs for most females. So why has the equation shifted at Gombe? Many assume habitat isolation and/or banana provisioning is sufficient to explain the dispersal patterns seen at Gombe. While habitat fragmentation generally increases the cost of dispersal (though it can also increase the reward for long-distance movement) ( and Dytham, 1999), provisioning increases the benefits of staying (Ims and Hjermann, 2001; Kennedy and Ward, 2003); a combination that would promote female philopatry. However, rates of dispersal in

Kasekela have remained consistent since the advent of the study when forests still existed beyond Gombe’s borders. Meanwhile dispersal rates from Mitumba, where provisioning also took place, are similar to those noted elsewhere. Dispersal propensity evolves quickly under artificial selection (Roff and Fairbarn, 2001) and may have some heritable component (Ronce, 2007; Doligez et al., 2009), influencing dispersal in subsequent generations. However, at best, two generations of females have come of age since the 1960s and it seems unlikely that provisioning or habitat degradation would have altered the dispersal genotype in such a short time frame. Therefore, the dispersal patterns at Gombe are unlikely to be due solely to anthropogenic factors. Demography,

152

habitat and core area quality and chimpanzee density may also play a role. Chimpanzee

density at Gombe is above average (Wilson et al., 2014) and females may use this as an

indicator of habitat quality (Bowler and Benton, 2005), decreasing dispersal rates.

Correspondingly, in Kasekela, the high quality habitat may support more females and

allow daughters to range within their mother’s core area, even as adults. Though the

number of males in the group was associated with an increase in dispersal rates, there

was never fewer than 6 males in Kasekela, a number that may have been sufficient to

overcome inbreeding if females have few relatives in the group. In Mitumba, density is

similarly high, but mate choice is reduced. This alters the risk of inbreeding, which may be sufficient to promote dispersal. Such questions should be addressed empirically in the future by collating data from numerous field sites and could be used to identify dispersal reaction norms in chimpanzees to elucidate how dispersal behavior changes given a particular phenotype along an environmental and social gradient.

153

5. Experimental evidence that grooming and play are social currency in chimpanzees and bonobos 1

Apes, like all primates, rely on social relationships to survive and reproduce.

Evidence from several anthropoid species, including chimpanzees, shows that strong

individual relationships, both with kin and non-kin, provide adaptive benefits and in

many cases are correlated with reproductive success (Gilby et al., 2013; Schülke &

Ostner, 2008; Silk et al., 2009, Silk et al., 2010). Apes can manipulate social relationships,

including through the use of coalitions and alliances to increase rank within the

dominance hierarchy (e.g. de Waal, 1982; Goodall, 1986; Surbeck et al., 2011). Social

manipulation used to increase rank presumably benefits the individual as rank

correlates with measures of fitness (Kano, 1996; Pusey et al., 1997; Gerloff et al., 1999;

Boesch et al., 2006; Wroblewski et al., 2009).

Given the importance of social relationships for apes, observational work has been conducted to understand how these relationships are established and maintained.

A number of studies in captivity and the wild have shown how bonobos and chimpanzees use grooming to form and maintain social bonds (deWaal, 1982; Kano,

1992). There is evidence for reciprocal grooming in both species (Watts, 2002; Stevens et al., 2006). In chimpanzees there is evidence that male chimpanzees groom their alliance and hunting partners more frequently than non-alliance and hunting partners (Nishida

1 Published as: Schroepfer-Walker, K., Wobber, V., & Hare, B. 2015. Experimental evidence that grooming and play are social currency in bonobos and chimpanzees. Behaviour. 152, 545 – 562. 154

and Hosaka, 1996; Watts, 2000, 2002; Mitani and Watts, 2001b). Though bonds between

chimpanzees are strongest between male dyads (Gilby and Wrangham, 2008; Mitani,

2009), strong bonds can also form in male-female and female-female dyads (Gilby and

Wrangham, 2008; Langergraber et al., 2009; Lehmann and Boesch, 2009). Meanwhile bonobo mothers and sons seem to preferentially travel together and groom most

frequently in the wild (Kano, 1992; Furuichi, 1997; Hohmann et al., 1999; Surbeck et al.,

2011). Female bonobos form coalitions to compete against males but strong bonds between males have not been observed (Parish, 1996; Hohmann et al. 1999). Play has also been observed to be another way apes can form and maintain bonds and is important even in adulthood (Goodall, 1986; Palagi et al., 2004; Palagi and Paoli, 2007;

Nishida, 2012). Both species tend to play most frequently with kin and allies (Goodall,

1986; Palagi et al., 2004; Nishida, 2012).

Experiments have also been conducted to examine the cognitive abilities in apes that might play a role in relationship formation and maintenance (Tan and Hare, 2013).

Bonobos and chimpanzees are both skilled at solving instrumental tasks through cooperation with conspecifics (Melis et al., 2006a; Hare et al., 2007). Chimpanzees recruit help when cooperation is necessary and can quickly determine which of two partners is most skillful (Melis et al., 2006b). Chimpanzees are able to maintain cooperation when they encounter a conflict of interest through non-verbal negotiation although skills at reciprocity are inconsistent (Melis et al. 2008; Melis et al. 2009; Brosnan et al. 2009).

155

Bonobos retain a more juvenile level of individual tolerance into adulthood that

facilitates cooperation in obtaining food, something not seen in chimpanzees (Hare et al.,

2007). However, this leads to difficulty in inhibiting previously learned social

associations (Wobber et al., 2010). Bonobos also prefer to share food with strange bonobos over groupmates – a preference that may facilitate the extension of social

networks in a way not observed in other apes (Hare & Kwetuenda, 2010; Tan & Hare,

2013). A number of experiments have also studied the social preferences of apes using

human experimenters. Bonobos and chimpanzees show a preference for a human that

was trying to share food with another human over one who was trying to steal the food

during a triadic interaction (Russell et al., 2008; Subiaul et al., 2008; Herrmann et al.,

2013a). Chimpanzees also demonstrated an almost immediate reversal of preference

from a previously stingy to currently generous human in a reversal learning paradigm

in which it took them dozens of trials to demonstrate a reversal when a non-social cue

like color was used (Wobber and Hare, 2009). Taken together experimental studies

support the idea of bonobos and chimpanzees as flexible cooperators that monitor social

relationships closely and rapidly change their preferences – even potentially in

interactions with humans.

While observational studies have demonstrated the potential role of grooming

and play in social relationships and experiments have shown how flexible bonobos and

chimpanzees are in forming and maintaining cooperative relationships, no study has

156

ever experimentally examined grooming and play as a currency in establishing social

relationships in apes, where the amount of grooming or other affiliative behavior

received leads to a change in preference toward one social partner over the other. While

a strong role of grooming and play has long been suspected based on observational

work, all previous experimental studies require apes to show social preferences or solve

social problems for food rewards (although see Maclean & Hare, 2013). The Social

Currency hypothesis suggests that both grooming and play are valued in social

interactions and can be used to establish or shift social preferences depending on the

amount of play or grooming that occurs between individuals. The central prediction being that an individual can improve their social relationship with another group

member by grooming or playing with them. Therefore, an experimental manipulation of

grooming and play should show a shift in preference toward those individuals who play

and groom subjects the most. If confirmed a secondary question then becomes

uncovering the mechanism that might drive such exchanges (e.g. biological markets,

Noe, 1995)

Given the rapid shift in preference for cooperative partners observed in some

experiments (Melis et al., 2006b; Wobber and Hare, 2009; Herrmann et al., 2013), it may be apes alter their preferences extremely rapidly based on a relatively short social

exchange. In this experiment we will test the social currency hypothesis by manipulating

which of two social partners each subject interacts with before choosing which partner

157

they prefer to receive food from. If grooming and play act as social currency, subjects

should shift their preference toward an individual that recently played or groomed

them. In testing the social currency hypothesis it is also important to examine both Pan species since they have very different response to strangers related to establishing new relationships (Tan and Hare, 2013), they handle social stress related to relationship maintenance differently (Wobber et al., 2010), they exhibit temperament differences

(Herrmann et al., 2011) and they differ in social cognitive abilities (Herrmann et al., 2010;

Wobber et al., 2010). In this context, we predicted bonobos would show greater short- term shifts due to their increased tolerance in general, and especially towards strangers.

To control for differences in the two species general preference for interacting with conspecific strangers (i.e. chimpanzees are xenophobic while bonobos are xenophilic), human experimenters were used. When both species have been tested in the same context, they show the same strong preferences to interact with humans over playing alone and neither species has a xenophobic response to humans (Herrmann et al., 2011;

Maclean and Hare, 2013).

5.1 Methods

30 chimpanzees and 24 bonobos housed at the Tchimpounga Centre for

Chimpanzee Rehabilitation (Pointe Noire, Republic of Congo) and Lola ya Bonobo

(Kinshasa, Democratic Republic of Congo) participated in this experiment. Most subjects

158

arrived at the sanctuary as orphans and have been raised in mixed sex social groups

with access to large outdoor forested enclosures where they engage in species-typical behavior including grooming and playing (for details see Wobber and Hare, 2011).

Subjects interact with human caretakers on a daily basis when they return to their night

dormitories at sunset. Chimpanzees ranged in age from 8 – 23 years and bonobos from 6

– 23 years. All had limited exposure to the human experimenters prior to the

experimental conditions. A between-subjects design was utilized with 15 chimpanzees

and 13 bonobos completing the groom condition and 15 chimpanzees and 12 bonobos

completing the play condition (Table 25). Six subjects that were unable to complete the baseline session were dropped from all analyses.

159

Table 25: List of subjects by species, sex, age and condition.

Spp. Subject Sex Age Cond. Spp. Subject Sex Age Cond. C Elykia M 22 P C Tomy M 21 G C Jo M 22 P C Jacob M 19 G C Jay M 21 P C Tamishi M 18 G C Yoko M 14 P C Tchibanga M 13 G C Tabonga M 12 P C Wolo M 13 G C Chimpie M 12 P C Kefan M 11 G C Tiki M 10 P C Petit Prince M 10 G C Kimenga M 7 P C Lufumbu M 9 G C Pembele F 18 P C Mayebo F 22 G C Low-Low F 18 P C Ramses F 15 G C Diba F 15 P C Ouband F 11 G C Fanitouek F 11 P C Oumine F 11 G C Vitika F 10 P C Ulemvuka F 10 G C Lounama F 10 P C Makou F 9 G C Marcelle F 8 P C Mvouti F 8 G B Kiki M 14 P B Makali M 25 G B Lomami M 12 P B Api M 11 G B Illebo M 10 P B Boende M 11 G B Bandaka M 10 P B Bili M 10 G B Eleke M 8 P B Maniema M 9 G B Yolo M 8 P B Kasongo M 9 G B Kisantu F 14 P B Chibombo M 6 G B Bandundu F 14 P B Isiro F 14 G B Likasi F 10 P B Kalena F 13 G B Muanda F 8 P B Salonga F 13 G B Kinshasa F 6 P B Katako F 7 G B Sake F 6 P B Lukuru F 6 G B Masisi F 6 G Spp. (Species): C=Chimpanzee, B=Bonobo. Cond. (Condition): P=Play, G=Groom

160

5.1.1 Test Procedure

The test consisted of two phases and was conducted over 2 – 3 test sessions. All subjects began with a baseline preference test on day one followed by a test session that was divided across two more days. For a few subjects, the first test session occurred 30 minutes after the baseline session, rather than the following day, due to management constraints. Care was taken to assure an equal representation of species, sex and condition across the different testing schedules. Three human experimenters took part in this experiment. E1 and E2 took part in all preference tests and following the baseline preference test, one was designated as the actor. E3 centered the subject but otherwise did not interact with them in any way. E1 and E2 were unfamiliar individuals though the subjects did have limited experience with them in different capacities. It was not possible to control for gender and race between E1 and E2 (see discussion) due to experimenter availability in the two sanctuaries. Five human experimenters served as E1 and E2 throughout the experiment. KSW was an experimenter for each subject and was known to the apes through minimal exposure during three weeks of study at the sanctuaries a year prior to the current experiment. Upon arrival to complete the current set of experiments, KSW did not interact with the apes prior to testing. At Tchimpounga two caretakers also served as E1 or E2. The primary experimenter was a female caretaker who worked exclusively with the juvenile group, located in a geographically separate area from the sub-adult and adult animals. This caretaker had very limited exposure to

161

the sub-adult and adult individuals. Four older juveniles were tested at Tchimpounga

and for these subjects and four additional adults a male caretaker who had a limited role

with each group performed the role of second experimenter. At Lola ya Bonobo the

second male experimenter was held constant and was only known to the apes through a

three-week observational study he had conducted a month prior to the experiment. In

this capacity he did not ever have physical contact with the animals and could only

observe them from a distance of 10 or more meters. It is important to note that, overall,

subjects did not have an a priori preference for one experimenter over the other. KSW

served as the actor in 16/30 and 12/24 instances for the chimpanzees and bonobos

respectively.

5.1.1.1 Baseline

To assess any pre-existing preferences between E1 and E2 apes were given a baseline preference test. Subjects were brought one at a time into a testing room in their

night dormitory and were allowed to acclimate to the space. Preference test trials began

when E1 and E2 simultaneously gave the subject a slice of banana in the center of the

room, before stepping away from the mesh. E3 then re-centered the ape using a banana

slice. E1 and E2 stepped forward to the mesh and kneeled, 2 meters apart, each holding

half a banana in their outstretched hand as E3 stepped away from the testing room.

Subjects were allowed to choose to beg from either E1 or E2. Regardless of choice, the

162

subject never received the half banana. This procedure was then repeated for a total of

eight trials.

5.1.1.2 Test Session

The test session unfolded identically to the baseline preference with the addition

of an interaction period during each trial. The actor became the experimenter (E1 or E2)

that was least preferred in the baseline session. If the ape showed no preference then the

actor was chosen randomly. Trials again began with E1 and E2 giving the subject a banana slice in the center of the room. The non-actor then remained within arm’s reach

of the mesh and within a meter of the actor, facing the subject, throughout the

interaction period to control for effects of proximity. Crucially, while the non-actor

maintained proximity, s/he did not interact with the subject. Simultaneously, the actor began a 3-minute interaction period (see below) which varied by condition. Following

the interaction period, E3 centered the subject with a small piece of banana and E1 and

E2 positioned themselves on either side of the room, counterbalanced by trial, holding

half a banana. The subject was allowed to make a choice but was not provided with the banana. Each subject completed 8 test trials.

163

5.1.1.3 Interaction Period

Groom Condition. The actor sat in front of the mesh and engaged in grooming with the subject by sifting through the hair on the subject’s body parts that were within reach of the mesh while making the grooming lipsmack vocalizations. Grooming was not reciprocal and if the subject attempted to groom the actor then the actor shifted positions to widen the space between subject and experimenter. If the subject left the mesh the actor made verbal attempts to call the subject back. Time away from the mesh was coded and included in analysis.

Play Condition. The actor engaged the subject in high energy play that varied depending on individual preferences. Play could involve ticking, chase and poking and generally included all three. The actor alternated between a cheerful voice and their best attempt to mimic ape vocalizations. If the subject left the mesh the actor made verbal attempts to call the subject back. Time away from the mesh was included in the analysis.

5.1.2 Coding and Analysis

In both baseline and test trials choices were coded live by KSW and 30% of trials

were later confirmed through reliability coding using an observer blind to the conditions

and hypotheses and Cohen’s kappa was 0.958. In the baseline and test trials choice was

coded when the subject’s fingers crossed the mesh in front of the experimenter. To

control for motivation, time spent engaged was coded for each subject. Subjects were

164

free to terminate an interaction with the actor by moving away from the mesh that

allows human-ape interaction. Participants were considered engaged if they remained

within arm’s reach of the mesh barrier. For analysis, we used Poisson regression because

the data consisted of counts (number of times the subject picked the actor). Baseline and

test observations form repeated measures on the same individual, which are correlated.

Generalized estimating equations (GEEs) were used to account for the dependent

structure of the data. Inference focused on the treatment variable which had levels baseline and test condition (groom or play). Species, sex of the subject and sex of the

human experimenters were included in the model as main effects as well as an

interaction terms, condition by species and condition by sex of the subject. These

analyses used the geeglm package (Yan, 2002; Yan and Fine, 2004) in the R environment

for statistical computing version 3.1.0 (R Core Team, 2014). An additional model,

focusing on males only, was created to test for effects of period (first four vs. last four).

Differences in motivation were compared using an independent samples T-test

performed in JMP (“JMP Pro 10,” 2012). Finally to see if motivation affected choice we

ran an ordinary least squares regression on time spent unengaged and the change in

preference between the baseline and test conditions, also in JMP.

165

5.2 Results

Bonobos increased their preference for the actor in the play condition from 35% in the baseline to 52% in the test session (Figure 18) and in the groom condition from

39% in the baseline condition to 57% in the test condition (Figure 19). Chimpanzees increased their preference for the actor in the play condition from 39% in the baseline to

52% (Figure 18) in the test trials and increased their preference for the actor in the groom condition from 32% in the baseline condition to 47% in the test condition (Figure 19).

Species, condition and sex were included in the GEE model as well as interaction effects of species by condition and sex by condition (Table 26). There was no effect of species or experimenter sex but both condition and sex contributed to the model. The effect of sex was entirely driven by males (Figure 18, Figure 19). Males had a coefficient of 0.53 (se =

0.53, p < 0.001) in the groom condition and 0.62 (se = 0.08, p < 0.001) in the play condition. Females had a coefficient of 0.11 (se = 0.09, p = 0.25) in the groom condition and 0.01 (se = 0.17, p = 0.96) in the play condition. A second model that only included males looked at period effect (first four vs. last four). In this model, both groom and play were significant in both periods (Table 27) but the effect size diminished in the last four of the groom condition, indicating subjects were shifting their preference away from the actor in the final half of the session.

166

Table 26: Results of the GEE model

Coefficient P Value Species 0.26 Species x Condition 0.05 Experimenter Sex 0.20 Males Groom 0.53 <0.0001 Play 0.63 <0.0001 Fe males Groom 0.11 0.25 Play 0.01 0.96

Table 27: Results of the supplementary GEE model, restricted to males but including periods (first four/last four trials).

Coefficient P Value Period 1 Groom 0.73 <0.001 Play 0.55 <0.001 Period 2 Groom 0.31 0.01 Play 0.68 <0.001

167

0.9 Play Baseline 0.8 Test 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1

Proportion of Trials Actor is Selected is Actor of Trials Proportion 0 Males Females Males Females Bonobos Chimpanzees

Figure 18: The Y-axis represents the proportion of trials the experimenter who played with the subject was chosen over an experimenter who did not in the baseline and test conditions for both species, separated by sex in the play condition. Error bars represent standard error.

0.9 Groom Baseline 0.8 Test 0.7 0.6 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1

Proportion of Trials Actor is Selected is Actor Trials of Proportion 0 Males Females Males Females Bonobos Chimpanzees

Figure 19: The Y-axis represents the proportion of trials the experimenter who groomed the subject or “actor” was chosen over an experimenter who did not in the baseline and test conditions for both species separated by sex in the groom condition. Error bars represent standard error.

168

Chimpanzees were equally engaged in both conditions (t = -0.85, p = 0.80),

spending 94% and 91% of the time engaged in the play and groom conditions

respectively (i.e. remaining in proximity of the human experimenter). Bonobos were

more engaged in the play condition (t = -2.47, p = 0.026), spending 92% of their time

engaged compared to 75% in the groom condition. The level of engagement did not

affect the change in preference between the baseline and test conditions (play- R2 = 0.05, p = 0.29: groom- R2 = 0.06, p = 0.20).

5.3 Discussion

Chimpanzees and bonobos rapidly changed their preference for a novel

experimenter following an affiliative interaction, providing support for the Social

Currency hypothesis. Although play and grooming behavior likely arouse different

emotional states in an individual (Rosati and Hare, 2012), apes used both grooming and

play interactions equally as a currency to establish social relationships with human

experimenters. Bonobos and chimpanzees did not differ in their response to human

affiliative behaviors as social currency, despite differences in temperament (Hermann et

al., 2011), frequency of cooperative behaviors (Kano, 1992; Muller and Mitani, 2005) and

social cognitive abilities (Hermann et al., 2010; Wobber et al., 2010).

The change in preference was strong in both species in both conditions but was

primarily driven by males. Female chimpanzees and bonobos did not use play or

169

grooming as a social currency in this particular paradigm. We did not initially predict such a dichotomy, and in fact, would have predicted the sex difference to differ between species due to differential bonding patterns between the sexes (Hohmann et al., 1999;

Gilby and Wrangham, 2008). The observed pattern may be related to the shared socioecology between species where females disperse at adolescence (Pusey, 1980; Kano,

1992). In such a system males remain in their natal community for life and gain benefits from forming long-term relationships with kin and non-kin (Hohmann et al., 1999;

Mitani, 2009; Surbeck et al., 2011). As males mature, the ability to use affiliative behavior as a social currency to quickly ascertain the reputation of a partner should hasten the climb up the social ladder given the importance of allies in attaining rank (Nishida and

Hosaka, 1996; Surbeck et al., 2011). Males are also likely to benefit from rapidly establishing a rapport with sexually receptive females (Idani, 1991; Kahlenberg et al.,

2008). Dispersing females face different social challenges. Increasing evidence shows that females of both species do form differential bonds with both sexes and these relationships are likely to be adaptive (Gilby & Wrangham, 2008; Langergraber et al.,

2009; Lehmann & Boesch, 2009). Moreover, upon immigration females must establish relationships with unfamiliar individuals and should benefit from being able to form reputations based on social currency. Therefore, it is unlikely that females do not use affiliative behavior as a currency in forming new social interactions. Rather, it may be that females require more time to assess the value of a relationship and our short

170

experimental interactions here are insufficient to see a change in preference. These

results are more difficult to interpret in female bonobos. Unlike female chimpanzees

who are hostile towards immigrants and less social than males, bonds between bonobo

females are generally strong but not long-lasting (Parish, 1996; Hohmann et al., 1999),

immigrants seek out specific high ranking females when entering a new community

(Idani, 1991) and in experiments both male and female bonobos will pay a cost to have a

social interaction with a stranger (Tan & Hare, 2013). These lines of evidence suggest

they would show sensitivity to short-term affiliative interactions. Future research

comparing these species using conspecific partners or longer periods of interaction may

still reveal the expected species difference.

Subjects’ rapid shift in preference for a human that either played or groomed

with them can best be attributed to the social value of the experimenter’s affiliative behavior. Subjects were not differentially rewarded with food for their choices, making it difficult to explain a shift in preferences during the experiment based on anything but the interaction. Both species interacted with the experimenter similarly, choosing to maintain proximity throughout the interaction period and made choices on every trial.

Both experimenters maintained proximity to the subject’s room throughout each interaction period. Therefore the shift is not due to a lack of opportunity to interact equally with each experimenter. Although we were not able to hold constant the gender of the experimenters, this did not affect the outcome, primarily because the identity of

171

the actor interacting socially with the subject was determined by selecting the least

preferred experimenter for each subject in a baseline. While we observe apes quickly

shift preferences based on affiliative interactions, this experiment does not speak to the

precise cognitive mechanism involved. Future research will be needed to differentiate between mechanisms such as calculated reciprocity or physiological bonding, increased

familiarity or trust through physical contact.

Regardless of mechanism the ability to quickly establish relationships using

affiliative behaviors as social currency can have lifelong reproductive consequences for bonobos and chimpanzees. This is especially true for immigrating females who must establish a foraging territory and navigate a new social environment before reproducing

(Pusey & Schroepfer-Walker, 2013). Females who can integrate more rapidly into their transfer community are expected to have an advantage in early reproduction. Males should also benefit from prioritizing the establishment of certain relationships (Mitani,

2009). As new males mature and move up the hierarchy they can become essential allies for older males and established males should then compete for their attention. They should also be attuned to establishing relationships with females as affiliative relationships may be important for reproductive success. Moreover, cooperation among bonobos and chimpanzees is often predicated on a previously established relationship.

The ability to use both grooming and play as a social currency to guide partner choice should lead to stable and strong relationships among individuals.

172

Here we have established that chimpanzees and bonobos can use play and grooming as a social currency to form relationships. However, with further exploration, we expect to find differences in the speed, the developmental trajectory, the importance of different affiliative behaviors and the point at which social currency is overcome by the accumulation of social debt. We predict that the ability to use affiliative behavior to form relationships and assess reputation is likely to develop over the juvenile period and should be especially important during the adolescent period when females transfer to their adult communities and males must enter the established hierarchy. Though we did not assess developmental changes, informal observation suggests juveniles and adolescents (10 years and younger) of both species showed larger shifts of preference than adults in the groom condition than adult individuals. Reciprocity is thought to be important for maintaining cooperative interactions in chimpanzees and bonobos.

However, despite observational studies that note apes can exchange various commodities, including grooming, meat and coalitionary support (e.g. de Waal, 1997;

Koyama et al., 2006; Mitani, 2006; Gomes et al., 2009), experimental evidence is mixed and shows only weak support for contingent reciprocity (Melis et al., 2008; Brosnan et al., 2009). Melis et al. (2008) argue that short-term exchanges may be an inappropriate medium for assessing reciprocity in species that cooperate over long time periods. The current experiment was not designed to specifically address reciprocity but may be helpful in understanding the phenomenon because this experiment assesses interactions

173

between strangers, rather than between individuals with a relationship history. Apes may be willing to pay a higher price in an initial encounter with a stranger to gain information about that individual, even sacrificing food to learn about a new competitor or ally through a social interaction (Hare & Kwetuenda, 2010; Tan & Hare, 2013).

However, once the interaction begins, it may behoove an individual to watch their accumulation of ‘debt’. In this study we found limited evidence that chimpanzees may be attentive to the amount of grooming they receive with no reciprocation across a new

interaction. Chimpanzees were less likely to beg from the actor in the latter half of the

session (First 4 Trials 55%, Last 4 Trials 38%). This phenomenon was not observed in the

play condition (if anything, subjects continued to increase their preference across trials)

and suggests grooming may be perceived differently from play in initial short-term

interactions. Further work, addressing reciprocity among strangers, should be

undertaken to clarify the extent to which apes are capable of contingent reciprocity.

174

6. Conclusion

The general tenets of dispersal in chimpanzees began to emerge in the late 1970s

and have continued to be clarified as observation time has increased across years and

sites. However, a true understanding of the process proved elusive for three reasons: 1)

females were not followed from their community of origin to their community of

transfer and incoming females were hard to follow immediately after immigration, 2) in

most cases, immigrant females could not be compared to natal females of similar age

and 3) sample sizes were insufficient for hypothesis testing. In this dissertation I have begun to address these shortfalls and look at the entirety of the immigration and/or

settlement process; quantifying the costs and benefits, identifying dispersal strategies,

identifying factors that promote or discourage dispersal, and identifying behaviors that

promote relationship formation in a new community.

Inbreeding avoidance has long been implicated as a driver of dispersal patterns

in chimpanzees and here I provide further empirical support for its importance. Females

that do not disperse from their natal community are at a higher risk of inbreeding; they

remain with close kin and are typically more related to male group members than are

immigrant females. Three of four offspring identified as inbred were born to natal

females and the mortality rate for these individuals was high. Nevertheless, breeding

with close relatives is not an inevitable outcome for natal females and the majority of

non-dispersing females produced live offspring with unrelated mates. Furthermore,

175

most females, regardless of dispersal status, produced offspring with males who were less related to them than the average male. This indicates that they can detect genetic relatedness. The mechanism that allows discrimination of genetic difference is unknown and further work should expand upon previous findings that chimpanzees may be sensitive to visual cues in determining kin and explore other mechanisms such as olfaction. I also provide evidence for the importance of association in detecting maternal kin. The final case of close inbreeding occurred under the unusual circumstance in which an adult female that was born in Kasekela, but resided in Kalande, conceived in

Kasekela at a time when there were few available mates in Kalande. The infant’s father was her maternal brother who had been born after she had initially emigrated out of

Kasekela. In sum, females who forego dispersal are at a higher risk of inbreeding and the only means to eliminate this risk is to leave their natal community.

Dispersal is not without risks of its own. This work adds to a growing body of literature showing that resident females are aggressive towards incoming immigrants and that immigrants primarily seek out the company of males, who provide protection against such conflicts. Importantly, I have shown that incoming females incur other social costs that were not previously considered. In particular the association patterns of immigrant females differ drastically from those of natal females of similar age.

Immigrant females spend less time in social grooming overall and more time self- grooming when anestrous. This decrease is associated with a loss of female, rather than

176

male, grooming partners and can likely be attributed to the lack of female maternal kin in the new community. While grooming may primarily serve a social function, it also contributes to good hygiene and the decrease in grooming time could have both social and health effects that are costly to immigrant females. Immigrant females also associate much more widely than natal females and prefer resident males to resident females.

Though we don’t yet know the benefits of having strong social bonds with particular individuals, the loss of close social allies is likely to be to the detriment of immigrant females. This study was the first to look at energetic costs associated with dispersal and, interestingly, I did not detect any differences in feeding or travel between immigrant and natal females. These results suggest that females in Gombe National Park are able to overcome any costs associated with a loss of a familiar territory and feeding area. This may be accomplished through a combination of site visits prior to dispersal, social rather than locational dispersal, advanced physical cognition facilitating spatial awareness and memory, and the flexible fission-fusion social system that allows females to travel and forage independently, if need be. Alternatively, given the difficulty in following immigrant females immediately following transfer, such costs may exist but were undetectable in the current study design.

In contrast to other studies, fecal glucocorticoid metabolite levels did not differ in immigrant and natal females, suggesting both groups were equally stressed. Despite the increased frequency of aggression towards immigrants, physical attacks were still

177

relatively rare and immigrants may have been successful in coping physiologically with

the unpredictable social environment. Alternatively, natal females may also be dealing

with a stressful social environment as they must learn to avoid mating with adult male

relatives. Even though FGM levels were similar, immigration delays the age at first birth by half an interbirth interval, suggesting that the social effects of immigration are not cost-free. Such a delay in this slow reproducing species can affect lifetime reproductive success.

The primary dispersal options both come with substantial costs. Females that do not disperse risk breeding with a close relative and investing heavily in an offspring that has a high chance of mortality. Females that do disperse risk entering a hostile social environment that delays their age at first birth by over two years. Given these outcomes it is not surprising that females are flexible in their dispersal decisions and presumably make use of cues that help predict the outcome of either strategy. Among the social and environmental factors that may promote or discourage dispersal, the number of male maternal relatives in the group was the most important. The more male maternal relatives a female had in her home group, the more likely she was to disperse to a different community, further corroborating the importance of inbreeding avoidance.

However, other factors seem to promote philopatry in females. In particular, the presence of female kin and the rank of an individual’s mother may change the cost/benefit equation. If a maturing female’s mother is of high dominance rank and she

178

has other maternal relatives in the group, she is more likely to stay. Such females have remained in their natal community despite the presence of maternal brothers in the group. Thus, the advantages that a female kin network provide are expected to be substantial. Above all, dominant females are able to occupy high quality core areas that may be able to support additional family members. If daughters remain in their mother’s core area they may also be able to make use of cooperative efforts and coalitionary behavior to exclude other females from high quality areas. Future work should address the importance of core area quality on dispersal decisions. Finally, the number of available unrelated males in the group may affect dispersal decisions; though this result puzzlingly suggests females are less likely to disperse if they have fewer available unrelated mates.

Here, I also show that an individual’s phenotype affects dispersal decisions.

Particular phenotypes are likely to be advantageous under certain circumstances. Here I examined personality differences between females who do and do not disperse. Females who did not disperse were more extraverted and possibly more open and agreeable.

This suggests that females who are socially adept may accrue more benefits from philopatry through maintaining relationships they have cultivated as juveniles. On the other hand, females that are less sociable may find that, due to their introverted and disagreeable nature, there are few benefits to staying and that they will be better off dispersing to eliminate inbreeding.

179

Despite the flexibility in dispersal patterns, most females, at most field sites,

disperse. Therefore, the cost of dispersal in these communities must easily outweigh any benefits of staying. Thus, for these females, strategies used during integration become particularly important. The average time from immigration to first birth is 3.5 years, but is highly variable. In rare cases, females give birth within their first year post immigration. On the other extreme, it takes others eight or more years to give birth. The best way for chimpanzee females to increase their lifetime reproductive success is to maximize the length of the reproductive span. This requires starting as early as possible and living to old age. Therefore, immigrating females should attempt to integrate quickly into their new community to reduce the time to first birth. This is an area ripe for research, but little has been done to assess such strategies and the behaviors that promote relationship formation. Forming affiliative relationships with other females should be a top priority to reduce the risk of aggression. Cultivating relationships with potential mates is also likely to decrease integration time. Chimpanzees and bonobos possess an advanced theory of mind, allowing for the proliferation of social cognitive skills related to reputation, cooperation, negotiation and possibly reciprocity. Here, I have shown that chimpanzees and bonobos use both grooming and play as a social currency in building relationships. More specifically, they make use of direct social interactions to determine the suitability of a potential partner over a short time period, changing their preference for a human experimenter after an affiliative interaction.

180

While this makes intuitive sense it had never before been shown. Curiously, in the

experiments presented here, this effect was much stronger in male apes and was

virtually absent in female apes; though female chimpanzees did show some response to

grooming. However, this does not suggest females are not sensitive to short-term

affiliative interactions. Rather females, may be more discerning in their relationship

formation and need longer to form perceptions of new individuals. There was also some

evidence to suggest females may be more receptive to these short term interactions

when juveniles and adolescents as compared to adults. This follows behavioral

observations from the wild where adult females are reluctant to start new relationships

with incoming females while males quickly do so at any age. Only further work will

clarify the sex differences in the use of social currency.

In sum, dispersal in chimpanzees in Gombe National Park is not a rigid system

where all females disperse and all males are philopatric. Rather female chimpanzees are

remarkably flexible in deciding where to settle as adults. There are many options open

to each individual and dispersal decisions vary according to the social conditions

present at maturity as well as an individual’s own phenotype. The costs and benefits

vary for each female and the risk of inbreeding plays a substantial role in the decision

process. Such a pattern may be prevalent among African apes. Female gorillas also show

flexibility in dispersal patterns; some do not disperse while others do and both natal and breeding dispersal is common. Dispersal in bonobos is also female biased, though we

181

know little about the inherent flexibility due to a lack of observation hours. Therefore, the pattern of partial female dispersal may be an ancestral trait within the clade. In contrast, while gorillas and humans appear to show flexibility in male dispersal patterns, chimpanzees and bonobos show near complete male philopatry. Taken together, complete or near complete male-philopatry may be a derived trait in the Pan lineage.

This research represents a step forward in clarifying patterns of dispersal and integration in Great Apes. Future work should address dispersal flexibility and outcomes in all extant African great apes across multiple populations. This will augment creative work being done to identify dispersal patterns in fossil hominins and modern humans. Together, this will allow for a more thorough understanding of the changes that occurred in the human lineage, including those that may have led to our advanced culture and sociality.

182

Appendix A

CHIMPANZEE PERSONALITY TRAIT ASSESSMENT

Chimpanzee personality assessments can be made with this questionnaire by assigning a numerical score for all of the personality traits listed on the following pages. Make your judgments on the basis of your own understanding of the trait guided by the short clarifying definition following each trait. The chimpanzee’s own behaviors and interactions with other chimpanzees should be the basis for your numerical ratings. Use your own subjective judgment of typical chimpanzee behavior to decide if the chimpanzee you are scoring is above, below, or average for a trait. The following seven point scale should be used to make your ratings.

1. Displays either total absence or negligible amounts of the trait.

2. Displays small amounts of the trait on infrequent occasions.

3. Displays somewhat less than average amounts of the trait.

4. Displays about average amounts of the trait.

5. Displays somewhat greater than average amounts of the trait.

6. Displays considerable amounts of the trait on frequent occasions.

7. Displays extremely large amounts of the trait.

Please give a rating for each trait even if your judgment seems to be based on a purely subjective impression of the chimpanzee and you are somewhat unsure about it. Indicate your rating by placing a cross in the box underneath the chosen number.

Finally, do not discuss your rating of any particular chimpanzee with anyone else. As explained in the handout accompanying this questionnaire, this restriction is necessary in order to obtain valid reliability coefficients for the traits.

183

1. DOMINANT: Subject is able to displace, threaten, or take food from other chimpanzees. Or subject may express high status by decisively intervening in social interactions.

2. SOLITARY: Subject prefers to spend considerable time alone not seeking or avoiding contact with other chimpanzees.

3. IMPULSIVE: Subject often displays some spontaneous or sudden behavior that could not have been anticipated. There often seems to be some emotional reason behind the sudden behavior.

4. SYMPATHETIC: Subject seems to be considerate and kind towards others as if sharing their feelings or trying to provide reassurance.

5. STABLE: Subject reacts to its environment including the behavior of other chimpanzees in a calm, equable, way. Subject is not easily upset by the behaviors of other chimpanzees.

6. INVENTIVE: Subject is more likely than others to do new things including novel social or non-social behaviors. Novel behavior may also include new ways of using devices or materials.

7. DEPENDENT/FOLLOWER: Subject often relies on other chimpanzees for leadership, reassurance, touching, embracing and other forms of social support.

8. SOCIABLE: Subject seeks and enjoys the company of other chimpanzees and engages in amicable, affable, interactions with them.

9. THOUGHTLESS: Subject often behaves in a way that seems imprudent or forgetful.

10. HELPFUL: Subject is willing to assist, accommodate, or cooperate with other chimpanzees.

11. EXCITABLE: Subject is easily aroused to an emotional state. Subject becomes highly aroused by situations that would cause less arousal in most chimpanzees.

12. INQUISITIVE: Subject seems drawn to new situations, objects, or animals. Subject behaves as if it wishes to learn more about other chimpanzees, objects, or persons within its view.

184

13. DECISIVE: Subject is deliberate, determined, and purposeful in its activities.

14. INDIVIDUALISTIC: Subject’s behavior stands out compared to that of the other individuals in the group. This does not mean that it does not fit or is incompatible with the group.

15. RECKLESS: Subject is rash or unconcerned about the consequences of its behaviors.

16. SENSITIVE: Subject is able to understand or read the mood, disposition, feelings, or intentions of other chimpanzees often on the basis of subtle, minimal cues.

17. UNEMOTIONAL: Subject is relatively placid and unlikely to become aroused, upset, happy, or sad.

18. CURIOUS: Subject has a desire to see or know about objects, devices, or other chimpanzees. This includes a desire to know about the affairs of other chimpanzees that do not directly concern the subject.

19. VULNERABLE: Subject is prone to be physically or emotionally hurt as a result of dominance displays, highly assertive behavior, aggression, or attack by another chimpanzee.

20. ACTIVE: Subject spends little time idle and seems motivated to spend considerable time either moving around or engaging in some overt, energetic behavior.

21. PREDICTABLE: Subject’s behavior is consistent and steady over extended periods of time. Subject does little that is unexpected or deviates from its usual behavioral routine.

22. CONVENTIONAL: Subject seems to lack spontaneity or originality. Subject behaves in a consistent manner from day to day and stays well within the social rules of the group.

23. COOL: Subject seems unaffected by emotions and is usually undisturbed, assured, and calm.

24. INNOVATIVE: Subject engages in new or different behaviors that may involve the use of objects or materials or ways of interacting with others.

185

References

Akinyi, M.Y., Tung, J., Jeneby, M., Patel, N.B., Altmann, J., Alberts, S.C., 2013. Role of grooming in reducing tick load in wild baboons ( Papio cynocephalus ). Animal Behaviour. 85, 559-568.

Alberts, S.C., Altmann, J., 1995. Balancing costs and opportunities: Dispersal in male baboons. The American Naturalist. 145, 279–306.

Archie, E.A., Hollister-Smith, J.A., Poole, J.H., Lee, P.C., Moss, C.J., Maldonado, J.E., Fleischer, R.C., Alberts, S.C., 2007. Behavioural inbreeding avoidance in wild African elephants. Molecular Ecology. 16, 4138–4148.

Asensio, N., Korstjens, A., Schaffner, C., Aureli, F., 2008. Intragroup aggression, fission– fusion dynamics and feeding competition in spider monkeys. Behaviour. 145, 983–1001.

Aureli, F., Schaffner, C.M., Boesch, C., Bearder, S.K., Call, J., Chapman, C.A., Connor, R., Di Fiore, A., Dunbar, R.I.M., Henzi, S.P., Holekamp, K., Korstjens, A., Layton, R.H., Lee, P., Lehmann, J., Manson, J.H., Ramos-Fernandez, G., Strier, K.B., van Schaik, C.P., 2008. Fission-fusion dynamics: New research frameworks. Current Anthropology. 49, 627–654.

Basabose, A., 2002. Diet composition of chimpanzees inhabiting the montane forest of Kahuzi, Democratic Republic of Congo. American Journal of . 58, 1– 21.

Bentley, R.A., 2013. Mobility and the diversity of early Neolithic lives: Isotopic evidence from skeletons. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology. 32, 303–312.

Blumstein, D.T., Wey, T.W., Tang, K., 2009. A test of the social cohesion hypothesis: interactive female marmots remain at home. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 276, 3007–3012.

Boesch, C., 1991. The effects of predation on grouping patterns in forest chimpanzees. Behaviour. 117, 220–242.

Boesch, C., 1996. Social grouping in Taï chimpanzees. In: McGrew, W., Marchant, L., Nishida, T. (Eds.), Great Ape Societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 101–113. 186

Boesch, C., 1999. Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour. 48, 653– 667.

Boesch, C., Boesch-Achermann, H., 2000. The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest: Behavioral Ecology and Evolution. Oxford University Press, New York.

Boesch, C., Kohou, G., Nene, H., Vigilant, L., 2006. Male competition and paternity in wild chimpanzees of the Taï forest. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 130, 103–115.

Boinski, S., Mitchell, C.L., 1994. Male residence and association patterns in Costa-Rican squirrel monkeys ( Saimiri oerstedi ). American Journal of Primatology. 34, 157– 169.

Bonte, D., Van Dyck, H., Bullock, J.M., Coulon, A., Delgado, M., Gibbs, M., Lehouck, V., Matthysen, E., Mustin, K., Saastamoinen, M., Schtickzelle, N., Stevens, V.M., Vandewoestijne, S., Baguette, M., Barton, K., Benton, T.G., Chaput-Bardy, A., Clobert, J., Dytham, C., Hovestadt, T., Meier, C.M., Palmer, S.C.F., Turlure, C., Travis, J.M.J., 2012. Costs of dispersal. Biological Reviews. 87, 290–312.

Bowler, D.E., Benton, T.G., 2005. Causes and consequences of animal dispersal strategies: Relating individual behaviour to spatial dynamics. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 80, 205–225.

Braude, S., 2000. Dispersal and new colony formation in wild naked mole-rats: Evidence against inbreeding as the system of mating. Behavioral Ecology. 11, 7–12.

Brekke, P., Wang, J., Bennett, P.M., Cassey, P., Dawson, D.A., Horsburgh, G.J., Ewen, J.G., 2011. Postcopulatory mechanisms of inbreeding avoidance in the island endemic hihi ( Notiomystis cincta ). Behavioral Ecology. 23, 278–284.

Briffa, M., Weiss, A., 2010. Animal personality. Current Biology. 20, 912–914.

Bronikowski, A.M., Altmann, J., Brockman, D.K., Cords, M., Fedigan, L.M., Pusey, A., Stoinski, T., Morris, W.F., Strier, K.B., Alberts, S.C., 2011. Aging in the natural world: Comparative data reveal similar mortality patterns across primates. Science. 331, 1325–1328.

187

Bronikowski, A.M., Morris, W.F., Alberts, S.C., Altmann, J., Brockman, D.K., Fedigan, L.M., Pusey, A.E., Stoinski, T., Strier, K.B., Cords, M., In prep. Female and male life tables of seven wild primates.

Brosnan, S.F., Silk, J.B., Henrich, J., Mareno, M.C., Lambeth, S.P., Schapiro, S.J., 2009. Chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) do not develop contingent reciprocity in an experimental task. . 12, 587–597.

Brown, C.R., Brown, M.B., Brazeal, K.R., 2008. Familiarity with breeding habitat improves daily survival in colonial cliff swallows. Animal Behaviour. 76, 1201– 1210.

Bygott, J., 1979. Agonistic behavior, dominance and social structure in wild chimpanzees of the Gombe National Park. In: Hamburg, D., McCown, E. (Eds.), The Great Apes. Benjamin Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp. 405–428.

Caldji, C., Diorio, J., Meaney, M.J., 2000. Variations in maternal care in infancy regulate the development of stress reactivity. Biological Psychiatry. 48, 1164–1174.

Call, J., Tomasello, M., 2008. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 12, 187–192.

Capitanio, J.P., Mendoza, S.P., Lerche, N.W., 1998. Individual differences in peripheral blood immunological and hormonal measures in adult male rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta ): Evidence for temporal and situational consistency. American Journal of Primatology. 44, 29–41.

Capitanio, J.P., Mendoza, S.P., Mason, W.A., Maninger, N., 2005. Rearing environment and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal regulation in young rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta ). Developmental Psychobiology. 46, 318–330.

Chapais, B., 2010. The deep structure of human society: Primate origins and evolution. In: Kappeler, P., Silk, J.B. (Eds.), Mind the Gap. Springer Verlag, Berlin, pp. 19– 51.

Chapman, J.R., Nakagawa, S., Coltman, D.W., Slate, J., Sheldon, B.C., 2009. A quantitative review of heterozygosity-fitness correlations in animal populations. Molecular Ecology. 18, 2746–2765.

188

Charlesworth, B., Charlesworth, D., 1999. The genetic basis of inbreeding depression. Genetical Research. 74, 329–340.

Charlesworth, D., Charlesworth, B., 1987. Inbreeding depression and its evolutionary consequences. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 18, 237–268.

Charpentier, M.J.E., Crawford, J.C., Boulet, M., Drea, C.M., 2010. Message “scent”: lemurs detect the genetic relatedness and quality of conspecifics via olfactory cues. Animal Behaviour. 80, 101–108.

Cheney, D., Seyfarth, R.M., 1982. Recognition of individuals within and between groups of free-ranging vervet monkeys. American Zoologist. 22, 519–529.

Cheney, D.L., Seyfarth, R.M., 1983. Nonrandom dispersal in free-ranging vervet monkeys: Social and genetic consequences. The American Naturalist. 122, 392– 412.

Clobert, J., Danchin, E., Dhondt, A., Nichols, J., 2001. Dispersal. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Clobert, J., Le Galliard, J.F., Cote, J., Meylan, S., Massot, M., 2009. Informed dispersal, heterogeneity in animal dispersal syndromes and the dynamics of spatially structured populations. Ecology Letters. 12, 197–209.

Clutton-Brock, T., 1989. Female transfer and inbreeding avoidance in social mammals. Nature. 337, 70–72.

Clutton-Brock, T., 2009. Structure and function in mammalian societies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 364, 3229–3242.

Clutton-Brock, T., Lukas, D., 2012. The evolution of social philopatry and dispersal in female mammals. Molecular Ecology. 21, 472–492.

Conklin-Brittain, N., Wrangham, R.W., Hunt, K., 1998. Dietary response of chimpanzees and cercopithecines to seasonal variation in fruit abundance II: Macronutrients. International Journal of Primatology. 19, 971–998.

Constable, J.L., Ashley, M. V., Goodall, J., Pusey, A.E., 2001. Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees. Molecular Ecology. 10, 1279–1300.

189

Copeland, S.R., Sponheimer, M., de Ruiter, D.J., Lee-Thorp, J.A., Codron, D., le Roux, P.J., Grimes, V., Richards, M.P., 2011. Strontium isotope evidence for landscape use by early hominins. Nature. 474, 76–78.

Cote, J., Clobert, J., 2007. Social personalities influence natal dispersal in a lizard. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 274, 383–390.

Cote, J., Clobert, J., Brodin, T., Fogarty, S., Sih, A., 2010. Personality-dependent dispersal: Characterization, ontogeny and consequences for spatially structured populations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 365, 4065–4076.

Cote, J., Fogarty, S., Brodin, T., Weinersmith, K., Sih, A., 2011. Personality-dependent dispersal in the invasive mosquitofish: Group composition matters. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 278, 1670–1678.

Crockett, C., Pope, T., 1993. Consequences of sex differences in dispersal for juvenile red howler monkeys. In: Pereira, M., Fairbanks, L. (Eds.), Juvenile Primates. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL, pp. 104–118.

De Waal, F.B.M., 1982. Chimpanzee . The Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore.

De Waal, F.B.M., 1997. The chimpanzee’s service economy: Food for grooming. Evolution and Human Behavior. 18, 375–386.

Debeffe, L., Morellet, N., Cargnelutti, B., Lourtet, B., Coulon, A., Gaillard, J.M., Bon, R., Hewison, A.J.M., 2013. Exploration as a key component of natal dispersal: Dispersers explore more than philopatric individuals in roe deer. Animal Behaviour. 86, 143–151.

Deschner, T., Boesch, C., 2007. Can the patterns of sexual swelling cycles in female Taï chimpanzees be explained by the cost-of-sexual-attraction hypothesis? International Journal of Primatology. 28, 389–406.

Di Fiore, A., Link, A., Schmitt, C., Link, A., Spehar, S., 2009. Dispersal patterns in sympatric woolly and spider monkeys: Integrating molecular and observational data. Behaviour. 146, 437–470.

190

Di Fiore, A., Rendall, D., 1994. Evolution of social organization: A reappraisal for primates by using phylogenetic methods. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 91, 9941–9945.

Dingemanse, N.J., Both, C., van Noordwijk, A.J., Rutten, A.L., Drent, P.J., 2003. Natal dispersal and personalities in great tits ( Parus major ). Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 270, 741–747.

Dobson, F.S., 1982. Competition for mates and predominant juvenile male dispersal in mammals. Animal Behaviour. 30, 1183–1192.

Doligez, B., Gustafsson, L., Pärt, T., 2009. “Heritability” of dispersal propensity in a patchy population. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 276, 2829–2836.

Duarte, L.C., Bouteiller, C., Fontanillas, P., Petit, E., Perrin, N., 2003. Inbreeding in the greater white-toothed shrew, Crocidura russula . Evolution. 57, 638–645.

Duckworth, R.A., Badyaev, A.V., 2007. Coupling of dispersal and aggression facilitates the rapid range expansion of a passerine bird. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 104, 15017–15022.

Dutton, D. M., 2008. Subjective assessment of chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ) personality: reliability and stability of trait ratings. Primates. 49, 253 – 259.

Ekernas, L., Cords, M., 2007. Social and environmental factors influencing natal dispersal in blue monkeys, Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni . Animal Behaviour. 73, 1009–1020.

Ember, C.R., 1978. Myths about hunter-gatherers. Ethnology. 17, 439–448.

Emery Thompson, M., 2013. Reproductive ecology of female chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology. 75, 222–237.

Emery Thompson, M., Kahlenberg, S.M., Gilby, I.C., Wrangham, R.W., 2007. Core area quality is associated with variance in reproductive success among female chimpanzees at Kibale National Park. Animal Behaviour. 73, 501–512.

Emery Thompson, M., Muller, M.N., Kahlenberg, S.M., Wrangham, R.W., 2010. Dynamics of social and energetic stress in wild female chimpanzees. Hormones and Behavior. 58, 440–449.

191

Emery Thompson, M., Muller, M.N., Wrangham, R.W., 2012. The energetics of lactation and the return to fecundity in wild chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology. 23, 1234– 1241.

Emery Thompson, M., Newton-Fisher, N.E., Reynolds, V., 2006. Probable community transfer of parous adult female chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, . International Journal of Primatology. 27, 1601–1617.

Emery Thompson, M., Wrangham, R.W., 2008. Diet and reproductive function in wild female chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ) at Kibale National Park, Uganda. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 135, 171–181.

Emlen, S., Oring, L., 1977. Ecology, sexual selection and the evolution of mating systems. Science. 19, 215–223.

Feldblum, J., Wroblewski, E.E., Rudicell, R., Hahn, B., Paiva, T., Cetinkaya-Rundel, M., Pusey, A.E., Gilby, I., 2014. Sexually coercive male chimpanzees sire more offspring. Current Biology. 23, 2855–2860.

Firman, R.C., Simmons, L.W., 2008. Polyandry facilitates postcopulatory inbreeding avoidance in house mice. Evolution. 62, 603–611.

Foerster, K., Valcu, M., Johnsen, A., Kempenaers, B., 2006. A spatial genetic structure and effects of relatedness on mate choice in a wild bird population. Molecular Ecology. 15, 4555–4567.

Foerster, S., McLellan, K., Schroepfer-Walker, K., Murray, C.M., Gilby, I., Krupenye, C., Pusey, A.E., 2015. Social bonds in the dispersing sex: Partner preferences among adult female chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour. 105, 139-152.

Fossey, D., Harcourt, A., 1977. Feeding ecology of free-ranging mountain gorillas (Gorilla gorilla beringei ). In: Clutton-Brock, T. (Ed.), Primate Ecology: Studies of Feeding and Ranging Behaviour in Lemurs, Monkeys and Apes. Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 415–447.

Franz, M., Foerster, S., Murray, C., Pusey, A.E., In prep. Female chimpanzees queue for social status.

192

Fraser, D., Gilliam, J., Daley, M., Le, A., Skalski, G., 2001. Explaining leptokurtic movement distributions: Intrapopulation variation in boldness and exploration. The American Naturalist. 158, 124–135.

Furuichi, T., 1997. Agonistic interactions and matrifocal dominance rank of wild bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) at Wamba. International Journal of Primatology. 18, 855– 875.

Gandon, S., 1999. Kin competition, the cost of inbreeding and the evolution of dispersal. Journal of Theoretical Biology. 200, 345–364.

Gerloff, U., Hartung, B., Hohmann, G., Tautz, D., 1999. Intracommunity relationship, dispersal pattern and paternity success in a wild living community of bonobos (Pan paniscus ) determined from DNA analysis of faecal samples. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 266, 1189–1195.

Ghiglieri, M.P., 1987. Sociobiology of the great apes and the hominid ancestor. Journal of . 16, 319–357.

Gilby, I., 2004. Hunting and meat sharing among the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. University of Minnesota.

Gilby, I.C., Brent, L.J.N., Wroblewski, E.E., Rudicell, R.S., Hahn, B.H., Goodall, J., Pusey, A.E., 2013. Fitness benefits of coalitionary aggression in male chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 67, 373–381.

Gilby, I.C., Eberly, L.E., Pintea, L., Pusey, A.E., 2006. Ecological and social influences on the hunting behaviour of wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii . Animal Behaviour. 72, 169–180.

Gilby, I.C., Wrangham, R.W., 2008. Association patterns among wild chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ) reflect sex differences in cooperation. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 62, 1831–1842.

Ginsberg, J., Huck, U., 1989. Sperm competition in mammals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 4, 74–79.

Glander, K., 1992. Dispersal patterns in Costa Rican mantled howling monkeys. International Journal of Primatology. 13, 415–435.

193

Goldberg, T.L., Ruvolo, M., 1997. The geographic apportionment of mitochondrial genetic diversity in East African chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii . Molecular Biology and Evolution. 14, 976–984.

Gomes, C.M., Mundry, R., Boesch, C., 2009. Long-term reciprocation of grooming in wild West African chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 276, 699– 706.

Goodall, J., 1977. Infant killing and in free-living chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica. 28, 259–282.

Goodall, J., 1986. The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Belknap Press, Cambridge, MA.

Goodall, J., Pusey, A.E., 2015. Flo family (chimpanzee). In: Whelehan, P., Bolin, A. (Eds.), The International Encyclopedia of Human Sexuality. John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, NJ.

Goossens, B., Setchell, J.M., Tchidongo, E., Dilambaka, E., Vidal, C., Ancrenaz, M., Jamart, A., 2005. Survival, interactions with conspecifics and reproduction in 37 chimpanzees released into the wild. Biological Conservation. 123, 461–475.

Gosling, S.D., John, O.P., 1999. Personality dimensions in nonhuman animals: A cross- species review. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 8, 69–75.

Gosling, S.D., Vazire, S., 2002. Are we barking up the right tree? Evaluating a comparative approach to personality. Journal of Research in Personality. 36, 607– 614.

Greenwood, P.J., 1980. Mating systems, philopatry and dispersal in birds and mammals. Animal Behaviour. 28, 1140–1162.

Haley, M.P., 1994. Resource-holding power asymmetries, the prior residence effect, and reproductive payoffs in male northern elephant seal fights. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 34, 427–434.

Hammond, R.L., Handley, L.J.L., Winney, B.J., Bruford, M.W., Perrin, N., 2006. Genetic evidence for female-biased dispersal and gene flow in a polygynous primate. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 273, 479–484.

194

Harcourt, A.H., Stewart, K.S., Fossey, D., 1976. Male emigration and female transfer in wild . Nature. 263, 226–227.

Hare, B., Kwetuenda, S., 2010. Bonobos voluntarily share their own food with others. Current Biology. 20, 230–231.

Hare, B., Melis, A.P., Woods, V., Hastings, S., Wrangham, R.W., 2007. Tolerance allows bonobos to outperform chimpanzees on a cooperative task. Current Biology. 17, 619–623.

Hasegawa, T., 1990. Sex differences in ranging patterns. In: Nishida, T. (Ed.), The Chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains: Sexual and Life History Strategies. University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, Japan, pp. 99–114.

Havlicek, J., Roberts, S.C., 2009. MHC-correlated mate choice in humans: A review. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 34, 497–512.

Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Call, J., Tomasello, M., 2010. Differences in the cognitive skills of bonobos and chimpanzees. PLoS One. 5, 2–5.

Herrmann, E., Hare, B., Cissewski, J., Tomasello, M., 2011. A comparison of temperament in nonhuman apes and human infants. Developmental Science. 14, 1393–1405.

Herrmann, E., Keupp, S., Hare, B., Vaish, A., Tomasello, M., 2012. Direct and indirect reputation formation in nonhuman great apes ( Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Gorilla gorilla , Pongo pygmaeus ) and human children ( sapiens ). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 127, 63–75.

Hill, K.R., Walker, R.S., Bozicević, M., Eder, J., Headland, T., Hewlett, B., Hurtado, A.M., Marlowe, F., Wiessner, P., Wood, B., 2011. Co-residence patterns in hunter- gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science. 331, 1286–1289.

Hohmann, G., Gerloff, U., Tautz, D., Fruth, B., 1999. Social bonds and genetic ties: Kinship, association and affiliation in a community of bonobos ( Pan paniscus ). Behaviour. 136, 1219–1235.

Howard, W.E., 1960. Innate and environmental dispersal of individual vertebrates. American Midland Naturalist. 63, 152–161.

195

Idani, G., 1991. Social relationships between immigrant and resident bonobo ( Pan paniscus ) females at Wamba. Folia Primatologica. 57, 83–95.

Ims, R., Hjermann, D., 2001. Condition-dependent dispersal. In: Clobert, J., Danchin, E., Dhondt, A., Nichols, J. (Eds.), Dispersal. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 203–216.

Inoue, E., Inoue-Murayama, M., Vigilant, L., Takenaka, O., Nishida, T., 2008. Relatedness in wild chimpanzees: Influence of paternity, male philopatry, and demographic factors. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 137, 256–262.

Inoue, E., Tashiro, Y., Ogawa, H., Inoue-Murayama, M., Nishida, T., Takenaka, O., 2013. Gene flow and genetic diversity of chimpanzees in Tanzanian habitats. Primate Conservation. 26, 67–74.

Isbell, L.A., van Vuren, D., 1996. Differential costs of locational and social dispersal and their consequences for female group-living primates. Behaviour. 133, 1–36.

Isbell, L.A., Young, T.P., 1993. Social and ecological influences on activity budgets of vervet monkeys, and their implications for group living. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 32, 377–385.

Jokela, M., Elovainio, M., Kivimaki, M., Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L., 2008. Temperament and migration patterns in Finland. Psychological Science. 19, 831-837.

Kahlenberg, S.M., Emery Thompson, M., Muller, M.N., Wrangham, R.W., 2008a. Immigration costs for female chimpanzees and male protection as an immigrant counterstrategy to intrasexual aggression. Animal Behaviour. 76, 1497–1509.

Kahlenberg, S.M., Emery Thompson, M., Wrangham, R.W., 2008b. Female competition over core areas in Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii , Kibale National Park, Uganda. International Journal of Primatology. 29, 931–947.

Kano, T., 1992. The Last Ape: Pygmy Chimpanzee Behavior and Ecology. Press, Stanford, CA.

Kano, T., 1996. Male rank order and copulation rate in a unit-group of bonobos at Wamba, Zaire. In: McGrew, W., Marchant, L., Nishida, T. (Ed.), Great Ape Societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 135–143.

196

Keller, L.F., Waller, D.M., 2002. Inbreeding effects in wild populations. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 17, 230–241.

Kennedy, P.L., Ward, J.M., 2003. Effects of experimental food supplementation on movements of juvenile northern goshawks ( Accipiter gentilis atricapillus ). Oecologia. 134, 284–291.

Kim, K.W., 2000. Dispersal behaviour in a subsocial spider: Group conflict and the effect of food availability. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 48, 182–187.

King, J.E., Figueredo, A.J., 1997. The five-factor model plus dominance in chimpanzee personality. Journal of Research in Personality. 31, 257–271.

King, J.E., Weiss, A., Farmer, K.H., 2005. A chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ) analogue of cross-national generalization of personality structure: Zoological parks and an African sanctuary. Journal of Personality. 73, 389–410.

King, J.E., Weiss, A., Sisco, M.M., 2008. Aping humans: Age and sex effects in chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ) and human ( Homo sapiens ) personality. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 122, 418–427.

Koenig, A., Borries, C., 2012. Hominoid dispersal patterns and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology. 21, 108–112.

Kormos, R., Boesch, C., 2003. Regional Action Plan for the Conservation of Chimpanzees in West Africa. IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group and Conservation International, Washington, D.C.

Koyama, N.F., Caws, C., Aureli, F., 2006. Interchange of grooming and agonistic support in chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology. 27, 1293–1309.

Kramer, K., Greaves, R., 2011. Postmarital residence and bilateral kin association among hunter-gatherers. . 22, 41-63.

Kuussaari, M., Nieminen, M., Hanski, I., 1996. An experimental study of migration in the Glanville fritillary butterfly, Melitea cinxia . Journal of Applied Ecology. 65, 791– 801.

Landry, C., Garant, D., Duchesne, P., Bernatchez, L., 2001. “Good genes as heterozygosity”: The major histocompatibility complex and mate choice in

197

Atlantic salmon ( Salmo salar ). Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 268, 1279– 1285.

Langergraber, K., Mitani, J., Vigilant, L., 2009. Kinship and social bonds in female chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ). American Journal of Primatology. 71, 840–851.

Le Galliard, J.-F., Ferrière, R., Dieckmann, U., 2005. Adaptive evolution of social traits: Origin, trajectories, and correlations of and mobility. The American Naturalist. 165, 206–224.

Lehmann, J., Boesch, C., 2005. Bisexually bonded ranging in chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes verus ). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 57, 525–535.

Lehmann, J., Boesch, C., 2008. Sexual differences in chimpanzee sociality. International Journal of Primatology. 29, 65–81.

Lehmann, J., Boesch, C., 2009. Sociality of the dispersing sex: The nature of social bonds in West African female chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes . Animal Behaviour. 77, 377– 387.

Letty, J., Aubineau, J., Marchandeau, S., Clobert, J., 2003. Effect of translocation on survival in wild rabbits ( Oryctlagus cuniculus ). Mammalian Biology. 68, 250–255.

Lilienfeld, S.O., Gershon, J., Duke, M., Marino, L., de Waal, F.B., 1999. A preliminary investigation of the construct of psychopathic personality (psychopathy) in chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 113, 365–375.

Lukas, D., Clutton-Brock, T.H., 2011. Group structure, kinship, inbreeding risk and habitual female dispersal in plural-breeding mammals. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 24, 2624–2630.

Lukas, D., Reynolds, V., Boesch, C., Vigilant, L., 2005. To what extent does living in a group mean living with kin? Molecular Ecology. 14, 2181–2196.

Lurz, P.W.W., Garson, P.J., 1997. Effects of temporal and spatial variation in habitat quality on red squirrel dispersal behaviour. Animal Behaviour. 427–435.

Maclean, E., Hare, B., 2013. Spontaneous triadic engagement in bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) and chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 127, 245- 255.

198

Manson, J.H., Perry, S.E., 1993. Inbreeding avoidance in rhesus macaques: Whose choice? American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 90, 335–344.

Markham, C., Santymire, R.M., Lonsdorf, E. V., Heintz, M.R., Lipende, I., Murray, C.M., 2014. Rank effects on social stress in lactating chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour. 87, 195–202.

Marlowe, F.W., 2004. Marital residence among foragers. Current Anthropology. 45, 277– 284.

Marlowe, F.W., 2005. Hunter-gatherers and human evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology. 14, 54–67.

Martin-Ordas, G., Haun, D., Colmenares, F., Call, J., 2010. Keeping track of time: Evidence for episodic-like memory in great apes. Animal Cognition. 13, 331–340.

Massen, J.J.M., Antonides, A., Arnold, A.M.K., Bionda, T., Koski, S.E., 2013. A behavioral view on chimpanzee personality: Exploration tendency, persistence, boldness, and tool-orientation measured with group experiments. American Journal of Primatology. 75, 947–958.

Matsumoto-Oda, A., Hosaka, K., Huffman, M.A., 1998. Factors affecting party size in chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. International Journal of Primatology. 19, 999–1011.

McEwen, B.S., Wingfield, J.C., 2003. The concept of allostasis in biology and biomedicine. Hormones and Behavior. 43, 2–15.

Melis, A.P., Hare, B., Tomasello, M., 2006a. Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: Tolerance constraints on cooperation. Animal Behaviour. 72, 275–286.

Melis, A.P., Hare, B., Tomasello, M., 2006b. Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. Science. 311, 1297–1300.

Melis, A.P., Hare, B., Tomasello, M., 2008. Do chimpanzees reciprocate received favours? Animal Behaviour. 76, 951–962.

Melis, A.P., Hare, B., Tomasello, M., 2009. Chimpanzees coordinate in a negotiation game. Evolution and Human Behavior. 30, 381–392.

199

Mendoza, S.P., Capitanio, J.P., Mason, W.A., 2000. Chronic social stress: Studies in non- human primates. In: Moberg, G.P., Mench, J.A. (Eds.), The Biology of Animal Stress. CAB International, New York, pp. 227–247.

Menzel, E.W., 1973. Chimpanzee spatial memory organization. Science. 182, 943–945.

Miller, J.A., Gilby, I.C., Schroepfer-Walker, K.K., Markham, C., Pusey, A.E., Murray, C.M., 2014. Competing for space: Wild female chimpanzees are more aggressive inside their core areas than outside. Animal Behaviour. 87, 147-152.

Mitani, J.C., 2006. Reciprocal exchange in chimpanzees and other primates. In: Kappeler, P., van Schaik, C.P. (Eds.), Cooperation in Primates and Humans. Springer, Berlin, pp. 107–119.

Mitani, J.C., 2009. Male chimpanzees form enduring and equitable social bonds. Animal Behaviour. 77, 633–640.

Mitani, J.C., Watts, D.P., 2001a. Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour. 138, 299–327.

Mitani, J.C., Watts, D.P., 2001b. Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat? Animal Behaviour. 61, 915–924.

Mitani, J.C., Watts, D.P., Lwanga, J.S., 2002. Ecological and social correlates of chimpanzee party size and composition. In: Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., Marchant, L. (Eds.), Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 102–111.

Monard, A.M., Duncan, P., 1996. Consequences of natal dispersal in female horses. Animal Behaviour. 52, 565–579.

Moore, J., Ali, R., 1984. Are dispersal and inbreeding avoidance related? Animal Behaviour. 32, 94–112.

Morelli, T.L., Wright, P.C., King, S., Pochron, S., 2009. The rules of disengagement: Takeovers, infanticide, and dispersal in a rainforest lemur, Propithecus edwardsi . Behaviour. 146, 499–523.

200

Morin, P.A., Moore, J.J., Chakraborty, R., Jin, L., Goodall, J., Woodruff, D.S., 1994. Kin selection, social structure, gene flow, and the evolution of chimpanzees. Science. 265, 1193–1201.

Muller, M.N., 2002. Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. In: Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., Marchant, L. (Eds.), Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 112–123.

Muller, M.N., Emery Thompson, M., Kahlenberg, S.M., Wrangham, R.W., 2010. Sexual coercion by male chimpanzees shows that female choice may be more apparent than real. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 65, 921–933.

Muller, M.N., Kahlenberg, S.M., Emery Thompson, M., Wrangham, R.W., 2007. Male coercion and the costs of promiscuous mating for female chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 274, 1009–1014.

Muller, M.N., Mitani, J.C., 2005. Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Advances in the Study of Behaviour. 35, 275–331.

Muller, M.N., Wrangham, R.W., 2004. Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: A test of the “challenge hypothesis.” Animal Behaviour. 67, 113– 123.

Muniz, L., Perry, S., Manson, J.H., Gros-, J., Vigilant, L., 2006. Father-daughter inbreeding avoidance in a wild primate population. Current Biology. 16, 156–157.

Murdock, G., 1967. Ethnographic atlas: A summary. Ethnology. 6, 109–236.

Murray, C.M., Eberly, L.E., Pusey, A.E., 2006. Foraging strategies as a function of season and rank among wild female chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ). Behavioral Ecology. 17, 1020–1028.

Murray, C.M., Gilby, I.C., Mane, S.V., Pusey, A.E., 2008. Adult male chimpanzees inherit maternal ranging patterns. Current Biology. 18, 20–24.

Murray, C.M., Heintz, M.R., Lonsdorf, E.V., Parr, L.A., Santymire, R.M., 2013. Validation of a field technique and characterization of fecal glucocorticoid metabolite analysis in wild chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ). American Journal of Primatology. 75, 57–64.

201

Murray, C.M., Lonsdorf, E.V., Eberly, L.E., Pusey, A.E., 2009. Reproductive energetics in free-living female chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ). Behavioral Ecology. 20, 1211–1216.

Murray, C.M., Mane, S. V., Pusey, A.E., 2007. Dominance rank influences female space use in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes : Towards an ideal despotic distribution. Animal Behaviour. 74, 1795–1804.

Nagy, M., Heckel, G., Voigt, C.C., Mayer, F., 2007. Female-biased dispersal and patrilocal kin groups in a with resource-defence polygyny. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 274, 3019–3025.

Neumann, C., Duboscq, J., Dubuc, C., Ginting, A., Irwan, A.M., Agil, M., Widdig, A., Engelhardt, A., 2011. Assessing dominance hierarchies: Validation and advantages of progressive evaluation with Elo-rating. Animal Behaviour. 82, 911–921.

Newton-Fisher, N.E., Emery Thompson, M., Reynolds, V., Boesch, C., Vigilant, L., 2010. Paternity and social rank in wild chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) from the Budongo Forest, Uganda. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 142, 417– 428.

Nishida, T., 1968. The social group of wild chimpanzees in Mahale Mountains. Primates. 9, 167–224.

Nishida, T., 1979. The social structure of chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains. In: Hamburg, D., McCown, E. (Eds.), The Great Apes. Benjamin Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp. 73–121.

Nishida, T., 1983. Alloparental behavior in wild chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. Folia Primatologica. 41, 1–33.

Nishida, T., 1989. Social interactions between resident and immigrant female chimpanzees. In: Heltne, P., Marquardt, L. (Eds.), Understanding Chimpanzees. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 68–89.

Nishida, T., 2012. Chimpanzees of the Lakeshore. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

202

Nishida, T., Corp, N., Hamai, M., Hasegawa, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., Hosaka, K., Hunt, K.D., Itoh, N., Kawanaka, K., Matsumoto-Oda, A., Mitani, J.C., Nakamura, M., Norikoshi, K., Sakamaki, T., Turner, L., Uehara, S., Zamma, K., 2003. Demography, female life history, and reproductive profiles among the chimpanzees of Mahale. American Journal of Primatology. 59, 99–121.

Nishida, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., 1987. Chimpanzees and bonobos: Cooperative relationships among males. In: Smuts, B., Cheney, D.L., Seyfarth, R.M., Wrangham, R.W., Struhsaker, T. (Eds.), Primate Societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 299–305.

Nishida, T., Hosaka, K., 1996. Coalition strategies among adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains, Tanzania. In: McGrew, W., Marchant, L., Nishida, T. (Eds.), Great Ape Societies. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 114–134.

O’Riain, M., Jarvis, J., Faulkes, C., 1996. A dispersive morph in the naked mole-rat. Nature. 380, 619–621.

Packer, C., 1979. Inter-troop transfer and inbreeding avoidance in Papio anubis . Animal Behaviour. 27, 1–36.

Palagi, E., Cordoni, G., Tarli, S.M.B., 2004. Immediate and delayed benefits of play behaviour: New evidence from chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ). Ethology. 962, 949–962.

Palagi, E., Paoli, T., 2007. Play in adult bonobos ( Pan paniscus ): Modality and potential meaning. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 134, 219–225.

Parish, A.R., 1996. Female relationships in bonobos ( Pan paniscus ): Evidence for bonding, cooperation, and female dominance in a male-philopatric species. Human Nature. 7, 61–96.

Parr, L., de Waal, F.B., 1999. Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees. Nature. 399, 647– 648.

Parr, L.A., Heintz, M., Lonsdorf, E.V., Wroblewski, E.E., 2010. Visual kin recognition in nonhuman primates (Pan troglodytes and Macaca mulatta ): Inbreeding avoidance or male distinctiveness? Journal of Comparative Psychology. 124, 343–350.

203

Parrott, M.L., Ward, S.J., Temple-Smith, P.D., 2007. Olfactory cues, genetic relatedness and female mate choice in the agile antechinus ( Antechinus agilis ). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 61, 1075–1079.

Peacock, M.M., Smith, A.T., 1997. Nonrandom mating in pikas, Ochotona princeps : Evidence for inbreeding between individuals of intermediate relatedness. Molecular Ecology. 6, 801–811.

Pederson, A.K., King, J.E., Landau, V.I., 2005. Chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ) personality predicts behavior. Journal of Research in Personality. 39, 534–549.

Perrin, N., Mazalov, V., 1999. Dispersal and inbreeding avoidance. The American Naturalist. 154, 282–292.

Pilkington, M.M., Wilder, J.A., Mendez, F.L., Cox, M.P., Woerner, A., Angui, T., Kingan, S., Mobasher, Z., Batini, C., Destro-Bisol, G., Soodyall, H., Strassmann, B.I., Hammer, M.F., 2008. Contrasting signatures of population growth for mitochondrial DNA and Y among human populations in Africa. Molecular Biology and Evolution. 25, 517–525.

Piper, W.H., 2011. Making habitat selection more “familiar”: A review. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 65, 1329–1351.

Piper, W.H., Walcott, C., Mager, J.N., Spilker, F.J., 2008. Nestsite selection by male loons leads to sex-biased site familiarity. The Journal of Animal Ecology. 77, 205–210.

Plooij, F.X., van de Rijt-Plooij, H.H.C., 1987. Growing independence, conflict and learning in mother-infant relations in free-ranging chimpanzees. Behaviour. 101, 1–86.

Plumptre, A., Rose, R., Nangendo, G., Williamson, E., Didier, K., Hart, J., Mulindahabi, F., Hicks, C., Griffin, B., Ogawa, H., Nixon, S., Pintea, L., Vosper, A., McClennan, M., Amsini, F., McNeilage, A., Makana, J., Kanamori, M., Hernandez, A., Piel, A., Stewart, F., Moore, J., Zamma, K., Nakamura, M., Kamenya, S., Idani, G., Sakamaki, T., Yoshikawa, M., Greer, D., Tranquilli, S., Beyers, R., Furuichi, T., Hashimoto, C., Bennett, E., 2010. ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ): Status Survey and Conservation Action Plan 2010-2020. IUCN, Gland, Switzerland.

204

Printes, R.C., Strier, K.B., 1999. Behavioral correlates of dispersal in female muriquis (Brachyteles arachnoides ). International Journal of Primatology. 20, 941–960.

Pusey, A.E., 1979. Intercommunity transfer of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park. In: Hamburg, D., McCown, E. (Eds.), The Great Apes. Benjamin Cummings, Menlo Park, CA, pp. 465–479.

Pusey, A.E., 1980. Inbreeding avoidance in chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour. 28, 543–552.

Pusey, A.E., 1983. Mother-offspring relationships in chimpanzees after weaning. Animal Behaviour. 31, 363–377.

Pusey, A.E., 1987. Sex-biased dispersal and inbreeding avoidance in birds and mammals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2, 295–299.

Pusey, A.E., 1990. Behavioural changes at adolescence in chimpanzees. Behaviour. 115, 203–246.

Pusey, A.E., 2005. Inbreeding avoidance in primates. In: Wolf, A., Durham, W. (Eds.), Inbreeding, Incest and the Incest Taboo. Stanford University Press, Stanford, CA, pp. 61–75.

Pusey, A.E., Murray, C.M., Wallauer, W., Wilson, M.L., Wroblewski, E.E., Goodall, J., 2008. Severe aggression among female Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. International Journal of Primatology. 29, 949–973.

Pusey, A.E., Oehlert, G., Williams, J.M., Goodall, J., 2005. Influence of ecological and social factors on body mass of wild chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology. 26, 3–31.

Pusey, A.E., Packer, C., 1987. Dispersal and philopatry. In: Smuts, B., Cheney, D., Seyfarth, R., Wrangham, R., Struhsaker, T. (Eds.), Primate Societies. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 250–266.

Pusey, A.E., Schroepfer-Walker, K.K., 2013. Female-female competition in chimpanzees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 368, 20130077.

Pusey, A.E., Williams, J., Goodall, J., 1997. The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science. 277, 828–831.

205

Pusey, A.E., Wolf, M., 1996. Inbreeding avoidance in animals. Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 11, 201–206.

Queller, D., Goodnight, K., 1989. Estimating relatedness using genetic markers. Evolution. 43, 258–275.

Rails, K., Ballou, J., 1982. Effects of inbreeding on infant mortality in captive primates. International Journal of Primatology. 3, 491–505.

Rasmussen, J.E., Belk, M.C., 2012. Dispersal behavior correlates with personality of a North American fish. Current Zoology. 58, 260–270.

Reynolds, V., 2005. The Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest: Ecology, Behavior and Conservation. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.

Robbins, A., Fawcett, K., Stoinski, T., Robbins, M., 2009. Does dispersal cause reproductive delays in female mountain gorillas? Behaviour. 146, 525–549.

Robbins, M.M., Bermejo, M., Cipolletta, C., Magliocca, F., Parnell, R.J., Stokes, E., 2004. Social structure and life-history patterns in western gorillas ( Gorilla gorilla gorilla ). American Journal of Primatology. 64, 145–159.

Roberts, B.W., Wood, D., Caspi, A., 2008. The development of personality traits in adulthood. In: John, O.P. (Ed), Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research. Guilford Press, New York, pp. 375 – 398.

Rodseth, L., Wrangham, R.W., Harrigan, A.M., Smuts, B.B., 1991. The human community as a primate society. Current Anthropology. 32, 221-254.

Roff, D., Fairbarn, D., 2001. The genetic basis of dispersal and migration, and its consequences for the evolution of correlated traits. In: Clobert, J., Danchin, E., Dhondt, A., Nichols, J. (Eds.), Dispersal. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 191–202.

Romero, L.M., Dickens, M.J., Cyr, N.E., 2009. The reactive scope model: A new model integrating homeostasis, allostasis, and stress. Hormones and Behavior. 55, 375– 389.

206

Ronce, O., 2007. How does it feel to be like a rolling stone? Ten questions about dispersal evolution. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics. 38, 231–253.

Rosati, A.G., Hare, B., 2012. Decision making across social contexts: competition increases preferences for risk in chimpanzees and bonobos. Animal Behaviour. 84, 869–879.

Ross, C., 1998. Primate life histories. Evolutionary Anthropology. 54–63.

Rudicell, R.S., Holland Jones, J., Wroblewski, E.E., Learn, G.H., Li, Y., Robertson, J.D., Greengrass, E., Grossmann, F., Kamenya, S., Pintea, L., Mjungu, D.C., Lonsdorf, E.V., Mosser, A., Lehman, C., Collins, D.A., Keele, B.F., Goodall, J., Hahn, B.H., Pusey, A.E., Wilson, M.L., 2010. Impact of immunodeficiency virus infection on chimpanzee population dynamics. PLoS Pathogens. 6, e1001116.

Russell, Y.I., Call, J., Dunbar, R.I.M., 2008. Image scoring in great apes. Behavioural Processes. 78, 108–111.

Sapolsky, R., 1992. Neuroendocrinology of the stress response. In: Becker, J. (Ed.), Behavioral Endocrinology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 284–324.

Sapolsky, R., 2005. The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science. 308, 648– 652.

Sayers, K., Menzel, C.R., 2012. Memory and foraging theory: Chimpanzee utilization of optimality heuristics in the rank-order recovery of hidden foods. Animal Behaviour. 84, 795–803.

Schroepfer-Walker, K.K., Pusey, A.E., Rudicell, R.S., Ramirez, M.A., Hahn, B.H., Wroblewski, E.E., 2013. Female select mates that are less related than expected among the Gombe chimpanzees. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 150, 246.

Schubert, G., Vigilant, L., Boesch, C., Klenke, R., Langergraber, K., Mundry, R., Surbeck, M., Hohmann, G., 2013. Co-residence between males and their mothers and grandmothers is more frequent in bonobos than chimpanzees. PLoS One. 8, e83870.

207

Schülke, O., Bhagavatula, J., Vigilant, L., Ostner, J., 2010. Social bonds enhance reproductive success in male macaques. Current Biology. 20, 2207–2210.

Schülke, O., Ostner, J., 2008. Male reproductive skew, paternal relatedness, and female social relationships. American Journal of Primatology. 70, 695–698.

Seielstad, M.T., Minch, E., Cavalli-Sforza, L.L., 1998. Genetic evidence for a higher female migration rate in humans. Nature Genetics. 20, 278–280.

Setchell, J.M., Abbott, K.M., Gonzalez, J.P., Knapp, L.A., 2013. Testing for post- copulatory selection for major histocompatibility complex genotype in a semi- free-ranging primate population. American Journal of Primatology. 75, 1021– 1031.

Setchell, J.M., Huchard, E., 2010. The hidden benefits of sex: Evidence for MHC- associated mate choice in primate societies. BioEssays. 32, 940–948.

Setchell, J.M., Vaglio, S., Abbott, K.M., Moggi-Cecchi, J., Boscaro, F., Pieraccini, G., Knapp, L.A., 2011. Odour signals major histocompatibility complex genotype in an Old World monkey. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 278, 274–280.

Sherman, P., 1981. Kinship, demography and Belding’s ground squirrel nepotism. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 8, 251–259.

Silk, J.B., 2007. Social components of fitness in primate groups. Science. 317, 1347–1351.

Silk, J.B., Beehner, J.C., Bergman, T.J., Crockford, C., Engh, A.L., Moscovice, L.R., Wittig, R.M., Seyfarth, R.M., Cheney, D.L., 2009. The benefits of social capital: Close social bonds among female baboons enhance offspring survival. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 276, 3099–3104.

Silk, J.B., Beehner, J.C., Bergman, T.J., Crockford, C., Engh, A.L., Moscovice, L.R., Wittig, R.M., Seyfarth, R.M., Cheney, D.L., 2010. Strong and consistent social bonds enhance the longevity of female baboons. Current Biology. 20, 1359–1361.

Simmons, L.W., Beveridge, M., Wedell, N., Tregenza, T., 2006. Postcopulatory inbreeding avoidance by female crickets only revealed by molecular markers. Molecular Ecology. 15, 3817–3824.

208

Smith, T.M., Machanda, Z., Bernard, A.B., Donovan, R.M., Papakyrikos, A.M., Muller, M.N., Wrangham, R.W., 2013. First eruption, weaning, and life history in living wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 110, 2787–2791.

Stanford, C.B., 1991. Social dynamics of intergroup encounters in the capped langur (Presbytis pileata ). American Journal of Primatology. 25, 35–47.

Sterck, E., Watts, D.P., van Schaik, C.P., 1997. The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Ecology. 41, 291–309.

Sterck, E., Willems, E., van Hooff, J., Wich, S.A., 2005. Female dispersal, inbreeding avoidance and mate choice in Thomas langurs ( Presbytis thomasi ). Behaviour. 142, 845–868.

Stevens, J.M.G., Vervaecke, H., De Vries, H., Van Elsacker, L., 2006. Social structures in Pan paniscus : Testing the female bonding hypothesis. Primates. 47, 210–217.

Stow, A.J., Sunnucks, P., 2004. Inbreeding avoidance in Cunningham’s skinks ( Egernia cunninghami ) in natural and fragmented habitat. Molecular Ecology. 13, 443– 447.

Strier, K.B., Altmann, J., Brockman, D.K., Bronikowski, A.M., Cords, M., Fedigan, L.M., Lapp, H., Liu, X., Morris, W.F., Pusey, A.E., Stoinski, T.S., Alberts, S.C., 2010. The Primate life history database: A unique shared ecological data resource. Methods in Ecology and Evolution. 1, 199–211.

Struhsaker, T., 2010. The Red Colobus. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Stumpf, R.M., Boesch, C., 2006. The efficacy of female choice in chimpanzees of the Taï Forest, Cote d’Ivoire. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 60, 749–765.

Stumpf, R.M., Emery Thompson, M., Muller, M.N., Wrangham, R.W., 2009. The context of female dispersal in Kanyawara chimpanzees. Behaviour. 146, 629–656.

Subiaul, F., Vonk, J., Okamoto-Barth, S., Barth, J., 2008. Do chimpanzees learn reputation by observation? Evidence from direct and indirect experience with generous and selfish strangers. Animal Cognition. 11, 611–623.

209

Sugiyama, Y., 1999. Socioecological factors of male chimpanzee migration at Bossou, . Primates. 40, 61–68.

Sugiyama, Y., 2004. Demographic parameters and life history of chimpanzees at Bossou, Guinea. American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 124, 154–165.

Surbeck, M., Mundry, R., Hohmann, G., 2011. Mothers matter! Maternal support, dominance status and mating success in male bonobos ( Pan paniscus ). Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 278, 590–598.

Suter, S.M., Keiser, M., Feignoux, R., Meyer, D.R., 2007. Reed bunting females increase fitness through extra-pair mating with genetically dissimilar males. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 274, 2865–2871.

Szulkin, M., Bierne, N., David, P., 2010. Heterozygosity-fitness correlations: A time for reappraisal. Evolution. 64, 1202–1217.

Tan, J., Hare, B., 2013. Bonobos share with strangers. PLoS One. 8, e51922.

Tecot, S.R., Gerber, B.D., King, S.J., Verdolin, J.L., Wright, P.C., 2013. Risky business: Sex differences in mortality and dispersal in a polygynous, monomorphic lemur. Behavioral Ecology. 24, 987–996.

Tomasello, M., Herrmann, E., 2010. Ape and human cognition: What’s the difference? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 19, 3–8.

Townsend, S.W., Slocombe, K.E., Emery Thompson, M., Zuberbühler, K., 2007. Female- led infanticide in wild chimpanzees. Current Biology. 17, 355–356.

Travis, J.M.J., Dytham, C., 1999. Habitat persistence, habitat availability and the evolution of dispersal. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 266, 723 – 728.

Trivers, R., 1972. Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell, B. (Ed.), Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man. Aldine, Chicago, IL, pp. 1871–1971.

Tsukakara, T., 1993. eat chimpanzees: The first evidence of predation by lions on wild chimpanzees. American Journal of Primatology. 29, 1–11.

210

Tutin, C.E.G., 1979. Mating patterns and reproductive strategies in a community of wild chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 6, 29–38.

Van Lawick-Goodall, J., 1968. The behaviour of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Animal Behaviour Monographs. 1, 161–311.

Vigilant, L., Hofreiter, M., Siedel, H., Boesch, C., 2001. Paternity and relatedness in wild chimpanzee communities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98, 12890–12895.

Vigilant, L., Roy, J., Bradley, B.J., Stoneking, C.J., Robbins, M.M., Stoinski, T.S., 2015. Reproductive competition and inbreeding avoidance in a primate species with habitual female dispersal. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 69, 1163 – 1172.

Wakefield, M.L., 2013. Social dynamics among females and their influence on social structure in an East African chimpanzee community. Animal Behaviour. 85, 1303–1313.

Wallis, J., 1997. A survey of reproductive parameters in the free-ranging chimpanzees of Gombe National Park. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility. 109, 297–307.

Wang, J., 2011. COANCESTRY: A program for simulating, estimating and analysing relatedness and inbreeding coefficients. Molecular Ecology Resources. 11, 141– 145.

Waser, P.M., De Woody, J.A., 2006. Multiple paternity in a philopatric rodent: The interaction of competition and choice. Behavioral Ecology. 17, 971–978.

Watts, D.P., 1991. Harassment of immigrant female mountain gorillas by resident females. Ethology. 89, 135–153.

Watts, D.P., 1994. Social relationships of immigrant and resident female mountain gorillas, II: Relatedness, residence, and relationships between females. American Journal of Primatology. 32, 13–30.

Watts, D.P., 2000. Grooming between male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. 1. Partner number and diversity and reciprocity. International Journal of Primatology. 21, 189–210.

211

Watts, D.P., 2002. Reciprocity and interchange in the social relationships of wild male chimpanzees. Behaviour. 139, 343–370.

Watts, D.P., 2007. Effects of male group size, parity, and cycle stage on female chimpanzee copulation rates at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. Primates. 48, 222–231.

Weiss, A., Adams, M.J., 2013. Differential behavioral ecology: The structure, life history, and evolution of primate personality. In: Carere, C., Maestripieri, D. (Eds), Animal Personalities: Behavior, Physiology and Evolution. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 96 – 123.

Weiss, A., Inoue-Murayama, M., Hong, K.W., Inoue, E., Udono, T., Ochiai, T., Matsuzawa, T., Hirata, S., King, J.E., 2009. Assessing chimpanzee personality and subjective well-being in Japan. American Journal of Primatology. 71, 283–292.

Weiss, A., King, J.E., 2015. Great ape origins of personality maturation and sex differences: A study of and chimpanzees. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 108, 4, 648 – 664.

Weiss, A., King, J.E., Hopkins, W.D., 2007. A cross-setting study of chimpanzee ( Pan troglodytes ) personality structure and development: Zoological parks and Yerkes National Primate Research Center. American Journal of Primatology. 69, 1264– 1277.

Weiss, A., Mjungu, D., Pusey, A.E. In prep. Personality in two wild chimpanzee populations.

White, F.J., Wrangham, R.W., 1988. Feeding competition and patch size in chimpanzee species Pan paniscus and Pan troglodytes . Behaviour. 105, 148 – 164.

Wigget, D., Boag, D., 1993. Annual reproductive success in three cohorts of Columbian ground squirrels: Founding immigrants, subsequent immigrants, and natal residents. Canadian Journal of Zoology. 71, 1577–1584.

Wilder, J.A., Kingan, S.B., Mobasher, Z., Pilkington, M.M., Hammer, M.F., 2004. Global patterns of human mitochondrial DNA and Y-chromosome structure are not influenced by higher migration rates of females versus males. Nature Genetics. 36, 1122–1125.

212

Williams, J.M., Liu, H., Pusey, A.E., 2002a. Costs and benefits of grouping for female chimpanzees at Gombe. In: Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., Marchant, L. (Eds.), Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 192–205.

Williams, J.M., Lonsdorf, E.V., Wilson, M.L., Schumacher-Stankey, J., Goodall, J., Pusey, A.E., 2008. Causes of death in the Kasekela chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. American Journal of Primatology. 70, 766–777.

Williams, J.M., Oehlert, G.W., Carlis, J.V., Pusey, A.E., 2004. Why do male chimpanzees defend a group range? Animal Behaviour. 68, 523–532.

Williams, J.M., Pusey, A.E., Carlis, J.V., Farm, B.P., Goodall, J., 2002b. Female competition and male territorial behaviour influence female chimpanzees’ ranging patterns. Animal Behaviour. 63, 347–360.

Wilson, M.L., 2012. Long-term studies of the chimpanzees of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. In: Kappeler, P., Watts, D.P. (Eds.), Long-Term Field Studies of Primates. Springer, Berlin, pp. 357–384.

Wilson, M.L., Boesch, C., Fruth, B., Furuichi, T., Gilby, I.C., Hashimoto, C., Hobaiter, C.L., Hohmann, G., Itoh, N., Koops, K., Lloyd, J.N., Matsuzawa, T., Mitani, J.C., Mjungu, D.C., Morgan, D., Muller, M.N., Mundry, R., Nakamura, M., Pruetz, J., Pusey, A.E., Riedel, J., Sanz, C., Schel, A.M., Simmons, N., Waller, M., Watts, D.P., White, F., Wittig, R.M., Zuberbühler, K., Wrangham, R.W., 2014. Lethal aggression in Pan is better explained by adaptive strategies than human impacts. Nature. 513, 414–417.

Wittig, R.M., Boesch, C., 2003. Food competition and linear dominance hierarchy among female chimpanzees of the Taï National Park. International Journal of Primatology. 24, 848–867.

Wittiger, L., Boesch, C., 2013. Female gregariousness in western Chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes verus ) is influenced by resource aggregation and the number of females in estrus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 67, 1097–1111.

Wobber, V., Hare, B., 2009. Testing the social hypothesis : Are also more skilled than chimpanzees in non-communicative social tasks? Behavioural Processes. 81, 423–428.

213

Wobber, V., Hare, B., Maboto, J., Lipson, S., Wrangham, R.W., Ellison, P.T., 2010a. Differential changes in steroid hormones before competition in bonobos and chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107, 12457–12462.

Wobber, V., Wrangham, R.W., Hare, B., 2010b. Bonobos exhibit delayed development of social behavior and cognition relative to chimpanzees. Current Biology. 20, 226– 230.

Wrangham, R.W., 1979. On the evolution of ape social systems. Social Science Information. 18, 336–368.

Wrangham, R.W., 1980. An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour. 75, 262–300.

Wrangham, R.W., 2000. Why are male chimpanzees more gregarious than mothers? A scramble competition hypothesis. In: Kappeler, P. (Ed.), Primate Males. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 278–313.

Wrangham, R.W., 2002. The cost of sexual attraction: Is there a trade-off in female Pan between sex appeal and received coercion? In: Boesch, C., Hohmann, G., Marchant, L. (Eds.), Behavioral Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 204–215.

Wrangham, R.W., Clark, A., Isabirye-Basuta, G., 1992. Female social relationships and social organization of Kibale Forest chimanzees. Topics in Primatology. 1, 81–98.

Wrangham, R.W., Smuts, B., 1980. Sex differences in the behavioral ecology of chimpanzees in the Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility. 28, 13–31.

Wrangham, R.W., Wilson, M.L., Muller, M.N., 2006. Comparative rates of violence in chimpanzees and humans. Primates. 47, 14–26.

Wroblewski, E.E., 2010. Paternity and father-offspring relationships in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii . University of Minnesota.

Wroblewski, E.E., Murray, C.M., Keele, B.F., Schumacher-Stankey, J.C., Hahn, B.H., Pusey, A.E., 2009. Male dominance rank and reproductive success in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii . Animal Behaviour. 77, 873–885.

214

Yan, J., 2002. Geepack: Yet another package for generalized estimating equations. R News. 2, 12–14.

Yan, J., Fine, J.P., 2004. Estimating equations for association structures. Statistics in Medicine. 23, 859–880.

Yoder, J.M., 2004. The cost of dispersal: Predation as a function of movement and site familiarity in ruffed grouse. Behavioral Ecology. 15, 469–476.

Zeh, J.A., Zeh, D.W., 1997. The evolution of polyandry II : Post-copulatory defences against genetic incompatibility. Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 264, 69–75.

215

Biography

Kara Kristina Walker (nee Schroepfer) was born on March 5, 1983 in Roseville,

CA. She completed elementary and high school in Bakersfield, CA before receiving a

Bachelor of Science in Animal Behavior and a Bachelor of Arts in French from Bucknell

University in Lewisburg, PA in 2005. She graduated cum laude and received the

Bucknell Prize in Animal Behavior for her research on reversal learning in primates. In

2008, Kara received a Master’s in French Studies with a concentration in international

development from the University of Wisconsin, Madison. While a doctoral candidate at

Duke University, Kara received five mentorship awards and a summer research travel

award from the Graduate School. In 2011, Kara received the Sally Hughes-Schrader

Sigma Xi Travel Grant. In 2013 she received a Leakey Foundation Research Grant and a

Margot Marsh Biodiversity Fund grant and in 2014 she received a NESCent Graduate

Student Fellowship. Kara was awarded the Dean’s Award for Excellence in Mentorship

in 2015 for her work mentoring Duke undergraduates.

List of Publications

Foerster S, McLellan K, Schroepfer-Walker KK , Murray C, Krupenye C, Gilby I, Pusey A. 2015. Dyadic associations reveal spatially independent partner preferences among adult female chimpanzees. Animal Behavior, 105, 139 – 152.

Schroepfer-Walker KK , Wobber V, Hare B. 2015. Experimental evidence that grooming and play are social currency in bonobos and chimpanzees. Behaviour, 152, 545 – 562.

216

Miller JA, Gilby IC, Schroepfer-Walker KK , Markham C, Pusey AP, Murray CM. 2014. Competing for space: wild female chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) are more aggressive inside their core areas than outside. Animal Behaviour. 87, 147-152.

Pontzer H, Raichlen DA, Gordon AD, Schroepfer-Walker KK, Hare B, Dunsworth HM, Wood BM, Isler K, Burhart J, Irwin M, Shumaker RW, Lonsdorf EV, Ross SR. 2014. Primate energy expenditure and life history. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111, 4, 1433-1437.

Pusey AE, Schroepfer-Walker KK . 2013. Female competition in chimpanzees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0077.

MacLean E, Mathews LJ, Hare B, Nunn CL, Anderson RC, Aureli F, Brannon EM, Call J, Drea CM, Emery NJ, Haun DBM, Herrmann E, Jacobs LF, Platt ML, Rosati AG, Sandel A, Schroepfer KK, Seed AM, Tan J, van Schaik CP, Wobber V. 2012. How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology. Animal Cognition, 15, 2, 223 – 238.

Rosati AG, Herrmann E, Kaminski J., Krupenye C, Melis AP, Schroepfer KK , Tan J, Warneken F, Wobber V, Hare B. 2012. Assessing the psychological health of captive and wild apes: A response to Ferdowsian et al. Journal of Comparative Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0029144.

Schroepfer KK, Rosati AG, Chartrand T, Hare B. 2011. Use of “entertainment” chimpanzees in commercials distorts public perception regarding their . PLoS One 6, 10, e26048. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0026048.

Judge PG, Evans DW, Schroepfer KK, Gross AC. 2011. Perseveration on a reversal learning task correlates with rates of self-directed behavior in nonhuman primates. Behavioral Research, 222, 57 – 65.

217