Hemispatial Neglect, Balance and Eye-Movement Control Paresh Malhotra, Elizabeth Coulthard and Masud Husain

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Hemispatial Neglect, Balance and Eye-Movement Control Paresh Malhotra, Elizabeth Coulthard and Masud Husain Hemispatial neglect, balance and eye-movement control Paresh Malhotra, Elizabeth Coulthard and Masud Husain Purpose of review Introduction Disorders of spatial awareness and balance following The neglect syndrome is a common disorder following stroke are common but often under-diagnosed. They lead to stroke, particularly prominent and severe after right- poor outcome and frequently coexist. Here we focus on hemisphere lesions. Patients with hemispatial neglect recent progress in the understanding of the mechanisms often fail to be aware of or acknowledge objects or people underlying these disorders and potential therapeutic on their contralesional side (the left side for patients with advances. right brain damage), attending instead to ipsilesional Recent findings items (the right side for right-hemisphere stroke Right-hemisphere networks are important for both spatial patients). Many such patients have intact visual fields attention and postural awareness. Neglect patients show and can initiate eye or limb movements contralesionally. multiple oculomotor impairments including reduced But despite this they appear selectively to attend to items saccade amplitude and difficulty retaining spatial locations on their ipsilesional side. across saccades. There has been controversy regarding the brain regions associated with neglect, although most From a clinical perspective hemispatial neglect repre- studies show the right inferior parietal lobe to be crucial and sents an important disorder because enduring neglect is new imaging modalities have provided insight into neglect associated with a poor functional outcome following caused by subcortical stroke. The ‘pusher syndrome’ is a stroke [1]. In addition to its clinical importance, neglect poorly understood balance disorder where patients push has also attracted a great deal of interest because of the towards their paretic side, resulting in falls. It may involve insights it provides into the brain networks that underlie impairment of subjective verticality but experimental studies spatial awareness and cognition [2,3], as well as non- have reported diverse findings. Advances in treatment for spatial functions such as the ability to sustain vigilant neglect include the successful use of prism adaptation and attention [4]. Here we review recent studies that have pilot data suggesting noradrenergic stimulation may begun to investigate the relationship and impact of the improve search in selected patients. cognitive deficits in neglect with eye movement and Summary balance control in these patients. In addition, we consider New experimental techniques have provided insight into the recent controversies regarding the anatomical basis of the debilitating disorders of spatial and postural awareness that neglect syndrome, the relationship of these findings to often follow stroke. There are currently no widely used current knowledge about the cortical control of balance therapies for neglect but both new behavioural techniques and eye movements, the clinical impact of neglect on and pharmacological methods are promising. patients and novel treatments for the disorder. More general recent reviews of neglect are also available for Keywords the interested reader [5–8]. parietal lobe, pusher syndrome, stroke, unilateral neglect, vestibular Eye-movement studies Oculomotor studies in neglect have tended to concen- Curr Opin Neurol 19:14–20. ß 2006 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. trate on lateralized saccadic impairments, but more Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, UCL, Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK recently there has been interest in a failure to update and Imperial College, Division of Neuroscience, Charing Cross Hospital, London W6 8RF, UK representations of space across saccades, leading to recur- Correspondence to Paresh Malhotra, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, UCL, sive search in patients with neglect. Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK Tel: +44 (0)207 679 5499; fax: +44 (0)207 916 8517; e-mail: [email protected] Contralesional saccades in neglect Current Opinion in Neurology 2006, 19:14–20 Patients with hemispatial neglect often spontaneously Abbreviations fail to explore contralesional space in everyday life, confining their eye movements instead to their ipsi- IPL inferior parietal lobe IPS intraparietal sulcus lesional side. It is vital to understand though that the SPV subjective postural verticality demarcation between the neglected and the non- STG superior temporal gyrus SVV subjective visual verticality neglected ‘field’ of vision need not be neatly through TPJ temporoparietal junction the vertical meridian of the visual field, or the midline of the search display (see Fig. 1). The degree of neglect, as ß 2006 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins 1350-7540 assessed for example by fixation patterns [9], varies 14 Copyright © Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited. Hemispatial neglect, eye movements and balance Malhotra et al. 15 Figure 1 Eye-movement scan path of a right-hemisphere patient stimuli [17]. The authors argued that paying attention to with neglect searching for letter Ts embedded among distractor an item at central fixation leads to shrinkage of the letter Ls effective visual field, worse to the contralesional side. Thus when searching a display with high attentional demands, right-hemisphere patients with neglect may have a constricted effective field of vision, accounting for the generally small-amplitude saccades they make. Saccade amplitudes to the left may be particularly reduced under some circumstances, as demonstrated by Niemeier and Karnath [18]. These researchers used two interesting variants of a visual search paradigm on right-hemisphere patients with and without neglect. In the first task subjects freely searched for the letter A in a random array of letters. The subjects’ scan paths when performing this task were recorded and used to generate the second ‘replay’ condition. In this second task, partici- pants viewed the same display but had to follow a red Black dots show fixations and white lines denote saccades. Note that neglect of the left side of the array is not strictly down the vertical square, which took exactly the same path as the fixations meridian of the display. In this case the patient found some Ts well to the made by subjects in the previous task, thus replaying it. left of centre in the upper part of the screen. The figure also vividly In the first (free search) condition, neglect patients illustrates how such patients with neglect often refixate locations they have already searched on the right, at the expense of exploring the left of tended to search on the right side of array and ignore the array. Such recursive ipsilesional search may, in some patients, be the left, but there was no major discrepancy between due to a failure to keep track of spatial locations that have already been leftward and rightward saccade amplitude or frequency. explored (from [10]). However, in the replay task, saccades were of reduced amplitude, particularly when leftward, despite the pre- between patients, and even within the same patient scribed eye movements being identical to the first task. depending upon the task and stimulus characteristics This finding suggests the contralesional saccadic deficit [10]. Thus a greater degree of contralesional neglect in neglect may be most prominent for stimulus-driven or is often found when patients are asked to search a dense, reflexive saccades (replay task) than for volitional move- cluttered visual scene compared to a non-cluttered one ments (free search condition). [10]. Moreover, there is often a gradient of neglect, such that the probability of finding a target in contralesional Spatial working memory and saccadic control space falls off gradually with distance, rather than When searching a visual scene, neglect patients not only patients demonstrating an abrupt failure to detect targets fail to explore contralesional space, they often keep at the midline [9,10]. Even in the dark, the resting refixating items on their ipsilesional side [9] (for example, position of gaze of right-hemisphere patients with neglect see Fig. 1). Many of these refixations are associated with a is far to the right of the midline [11]. failure to keep track of spatial locations across saccades, with patients revisiting locations they have previously When asked to shift their gaze contralesionally, neglect visited, unaware that they have looked there before patients demonstrate a variety of impairments, often [10,16,19]. Mannan and colleagues [10] monitored being slower to initiate saccades in that direction or eye movements while patients searched for target letter making small, multistep saccades [12–14]. When search- Ts among distractors. Subjects were asked to click a ing a visual scene, they do not necessarily make fewer response button to indicate whenever they identified a saccades in the contralesional direction [15,16], but their new target T (one they had not previously found). Right- eye movements are generally of small amplitude [15]. hemisphere neglect patients with lesions involving either This behaviour may be related to constriction of the the intraparietal sulcus (IPS) or the inferior frontal gyrus ‘effective field of vision’ when searching a dense array frequently misjudged targets on the right as new when for small targets among distractors. A recent study of they had in fact fixated and clicked on them before. In right-hemisphere-lesioned patients who had recovered neglect patients
Recommended publications
  • Four Effective and Feasible Interventions for Hemi-Inattention
    University of Puget Sound Sound Ideas School of Occupational Master's Capstone Projects Occupational Therapy, School of 5-2016 Four Effective and Feasible Interventions for Hemi- inattention Post CVA: Systematic Review and Collaboration for Knowledge Translation in an Inpatient Rehab Setting. Elizabeth Armbrust University of Puget Sound Domonique Herrin University of Puget Sound Christi Lewallen University of Puget Sound Karin Van Duzer University of Puget Sound Follow this and additional works at: http://soundideas.pugetsound.edu/ot_capstone Part of the Occupational Therapy Commons Recommended Citation Armbrust, Elizabeth; Herrin, Domonique; Lewallen, Christi; and Van Duzer, Karin, "Four Effective and Feasible Interventions for Hemi-inattention Post CVA: Systematic Review and Collaboration for Knowledge Translation in an Inpatient Rehab Setting." (2016). School of Occupational Master's Capstone Projects. 4. http://soundideas.pugetsound.edu/ot_capstone/4 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Occupational Therapy, School of at Sound Ideas. It has been accepted for inclusion in School of Occupational Master's Capstone Projects by an authorized administrator of Sound Ideas. For more information, please contact [email protected]. INTERVENTIONS FOR HEMI-INATTENTION IN INPATIENT REHAB Four Effective and Feasible Interventions for Hemi-inattention Post CVA: Systematic Review and Collaboration for Knowledge Translation in an Inpatient Rehab Setting. May 2016 This evidence project, submitted by Elizabeth Armbrust,
    [Show full text]
  • 26 Aphasia, Memory Loss, Hemispatial Neglect, Frontal Syndromes and Other Cerebral Disorders - - 8/4/17 12:21 PM )
    1 Aphasia, Memory Loss, 26 Hemispatial Neglect, Frontal Syndromes and Other Cerebral Disorders M.-Marsel Mesulam CHAPTER The cerebral cortex of the human brain contains ~20 billion neurons spread over an area of 2.5 m2. The primary sensory and motor areas constitute 10% of the cerebral cortex. The rest is subsumed by modality- 26 selective, heteromodal, paralimbic, and limbic areas collectively known as the association cortex (Fig. 26-1). The association cortex mediates the Aphasia, Memory Hemispatial Neglect, Frontal Syndromes and Other Cerebral Disorders Loss, integrative processes that subserve cognition, emotion, and comport- ment. A systematic testing of these mental functions is necessary for the effective clinical assessment of the association cortex and its dis- eases. According to current thinking, there are no centers for “hearing words,” “perceiving space,” or “storing memories.” Cognitive and behavioral functions (domains) are coordinated by intersecting large-s- cale neural networks that contain interconnected cortical and subcortical components. Five anatomically defined large-scale networks are most relevant to clinical practice: (1) a perisylvian network for language, (2) a parietofrontal network for spatial orientation, (3) an occipitotemporal network for face and object recognition, (4) a limbic network for explicit episodic memory, and (5) a prefrontal network for the executive con- trol of cognition and comportment. Investigations based on functional imaging have also identified a default mode network, which becomes activated when the person is not engaged in a specific task requiring attention to external events. The clinical consequences of damage to this network are not yet fully defined. THE LEFT PERISYLVIAN NETWORK FOR LANGUAGE AND APHASIAS The production and comprehension of words and sentences is depen- FIGURE 26-1 Lateral (top) and medial (bottom) views of the cerebral dent on the integrity of a distributed network located along the peri- hemispheres.
    [Show full text]
  • Absence of Hemispatial Neglect Following Right Hemispherectomy in a 7-Year-Old Girl with Neonatal Stroke
    Neurology Asia 2017; 22(2) : 149 – 154 CASE REPORTS Absence of hemispatial neglect following right hemispherectomy in a 7-year-old girl with neonatal stroke *1Jiqing Qiu PhD, *2Yu Cui MD, 3Lichao Sun MD, 1Bin QiPhD, 1Zhanpeng Zhu PhD *JQ Qiu and Y Cui contributed equally to this work Departments of 1Neurosurgery, 2Otolaryngology and 3Emergency Medicine, The First Hospital of Jilin University, Changchun, Jilin, China Abstract Neonatal stroke leads to cognitive deficits that may include hemispatial neglect. Hemispatial neglect is a syndrome after stroke that patients fail to be aware of stimuli on the side of space and body opposite a brain lesion. We report here a 7-year-old girl who suffered neonatal right brain stroke and underwent right hemispherectomy due to refractory epilepsy. Post-surgical observation of the child’s behavior and tests did not show any signs of hemispatial neglect. We concluded the spatial attention function of the child with neonatal stroke might be transferred to the contralateral side during early childhood. Key words: Epilepsy; hemispatial neglect; hemispherectomy; neonatal stroke; child. INTRODUCTION CASE REPORT Approximately one in every 4,000 newborns suffer The patient was a 7-year-old right-handed girl. from neonatal stroke, with a high mortalityrate.1-3 She was delivered with normal body weight. The survivors may develop varying degrees Gestation and delivery were uneventful. There of hemiparesis and low intelligence quotient was no family history of epilepsy. She had coma (IQ). Children with neonatal stroke, especially a few days after birth and was diagnosed with children with hemiplegia, have a high risk of neonatal stroke (Figure 1A-D).
    [Show full text]
  • BJNN Neglect Article Pre-Proof.Pdf
    Kent Academic Repository Full text document (pdf) Citation for published version Gallagher, Maria and Wilkinson, David T. and Sakel, Mohamed (2013) Hemispatial Neglect: Clinical Features, Assessment and Treatment. British Journal of Neuroscience Nursing, 9 (6). pp. 273-277. ISSN 1747-0307. DOI https://doi.org/10.12968/bjnn.2013.9.6.273 Link to record in KAR https://kar.kent.ac.uk/37678/ Document Version UNSPECIFIED Copyright & reuse Content in the Kent Academic Repository is made available for research purposes. Unless otherwise stated all content is protected by copyright and in the absence of an open licence (eg Creative Commons), permissions for further reuse of content should be sought from the publisher, author or other copyright holder. Versions of research The version in the Kent Academic Repository may differ from the final published version. Users are advised to check http://kar.kent.ac.uk for the status of the paper. Users should always cite the published version of record. Enquiries For any further enquiries regarding the licence status of this document, please contact: [email protected] If you believe this document infringes copyright then please contact the KAR admin team with the take-down information provided at http://kar.kent.ac.uk/contact.html 1 Hemispatial Neglect: Clinical Features, Assessment and Treatment Maria Gallagher1, David Wilkinson1 & Mohamed Sakel2 1School of Psychology, University of Kent, UK. 2East Kent Neuro-Rehabilitation Service, East Kent Hospitals University NHS Foundation Trust, UK. Correspondence and request for reprints to: Dr. David Wilkinson, School of Psychology, University of Kent, Canterbury, Kent, CT2 7NP, UK. Email: [email protected].
    [Show full text]
  • The Dizzy Patient: Don't Forget Disorders of the Central Vestibular
    REVIEWS The dizzy patient: don’t forget disorders of the central vestibular system Thomas Brandt1 and Marianne Dieterich1–3 Abstract | Vertigo and dizziness are among the most common complaints in neurology clinics, and they account for about 13% of the patients entering emergency units. In this Review, we focus on central vestibular disorders, which are mostly attributable to acute unilateral lesions of the bilateral vestibular circuitry in the brain. In a tertiary interdisciplinary outpatient dizziness unit, central vestibular disorders, including vestibular migraine, comprise about 25% of the established diagnoses. The signs and symptoms of these disorders can mimic those of peripheral vestibular disorders with sustained rotational vertigo. Bedside examinations, such as the head impulse test and ocular motor testing to determine spontaneous and gaze-evoked nystagmus or skew deviation, reliably differentiate central from peripheral syndromes. We also consider disorders of ‘higher vestibular functions’, which involve more than one sensory modality as well as cognitive domains (for example, orientation, spatial memory and navigation). These disorders include hemispatial neglect, the room tilt illusion, pusher syndrome, and impairment of spatial memory and navigation associated with hippocampal atrophy in cases of peripheral bilateral vestibular loss. Vertigo and dizziness are common complaints in disorders elicited by dysfunction within the temporo­ neuro logical patients. In a general academic emergency parietal cortex, thalamus, brainstem and cerebellum department, these symptoms were evident in 1–10% of can be classified by characteristic perceptual and ocular the overall attendances1,2, and they account for about motor manifestations, as well as sensorimotor control 13% of neurological consultations3,4. Up to 25% of of posture and gait.
    [Show full text]
  • Published Version: Bultitude, JH, Downing, PE & Rafal, RD 2013, 'Prism Adaptation Does Not Alter Configural Processing of Faces', F1000 Research, Vol
    Citation for published version: Bultitude, JH, Downing, PE & Rafal, RD 2013, 'Prism adaptation does not alter configural processing of faces', F1000 Research, vol. 2, pp. 215. https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.2-215.v1 DOI: 10.12688/f1000research.2-215.v1 Publication date: 2013 Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link to publication Publisher Rights CC BY University of Bath Alternative formats If you require this document in an alternative format, please contact: [email protected] General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the public portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 30. Sep. 2021 F1000Research 2013, 2:215 Last updated: 05 JAN 2015 RESEARCH ARTICLE Prism adaptation does not alter configural processing of faces [v1; ref status: indexed, http://f1000r.es/1wk] Janet H. Bultitude1, Paul E. Downing2, Robert D. Rafal2 1Oxford Centre for Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Brain (FMRIB), Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK 2Wolfson Centre for Clinical and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Bangor University, Gwynedd, LL57 2AS, UK v1 First published: 14 Oct 2013, 2:215 (doi: 10.12688/f1000research.2-215.v1) Open Peer Review Latest published: 14 Oct 2013, 2:215 (doi: 10.12688/f1000research.2-215.v1) Referee Status: Abstract Patients with hemispatial neglect (‘neglect’) following a brain lesion show difficulty responding or orienting to objects and events on the left side of space.
    [Show full text]
  • Apraxia, Neglect, and Agnosia
    REVIEW ARTICLE 07/09/2018 on SruuCyaLiGD/095xRqJ2PzgDYuM98ZB494KP9rwScvIkQrYai2aioRZDTyulujJ/fqPksscQKqke3QAnIva1ZqwEKekuwNqyUWcnSLnClNQLfnPrUdnEcDXOJLeG3sr/HuiNevTSNcdMFp1i4FoTX9EXYGXm/fCfl4vTgtAk5QA/xTymSTD9kwHmmkNHlYfO by https://journals.lww.com/continuum from Downloaded Apraxia, Neglect, Downloaded CONTINUUM AUDIO INTERVIEW AVAILABLE and Agnosia ONLINE from By H. Branch Coslett, MD, FAAN https://journals.lww.com/continuum ABSTRACT PURPOSEOFREVIEW:In part because of their striking clinical presentations, by SruuCyaLiGD/095xRqJ2PzgDYuM98ZB494KP9rwScvIkQrYai2aioRZDTyulujJ/fqPksscQKqke3QAnIva1ZqwEKekuwNqyUWcnSLnClNQLfnPrUdnEcDXOJLeG3sr/HuiNevTSNcdMFp1i4FoTX9EXYGXm/fCfl4vTgtAk5QA/xTymSTD9kwHmmkNHlYfO disorders of higher nervous system function figured prominently in the early history of neurology. These disorders are not merely historical curiosities, however. As apraxia, neglect, and agnosia have important clinical implications, it is important to possess a working knowledge of the conditions and how to identify them. RECENT FINDINGS: Apraxia is a disorder of skilled action that is frequently observed in the setting of dominant hemisphere pathology, whether from stroke or neurodegenerative disorders. In contrast to some previous teaching, apraxia has clear clinical relevance as it is associated with poor recovery from stroke. Neglect is a complex disorder with CITE AS: many different manifestations that may have different underlying CONTINUUM (MINNEAP MINN) mechanisms. Neglect is, in the author’s view, a multicomponent disorder 2018;24(3,
    [Show full text]
  • A Dictionary of Neurological Signs.Pdf
    A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS THIRD EDITION A DICTIONARY OF NEUROLOGICAL SIGNS THIRD EDITION A.J. LARNER MA, MD, MRCP (UK), DHMSA Consultant Neurologist Walton Centre for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Liverpool Honorary Lecturer in Neuroscience, University of Liverpool Society of Apothecaries’ Honorary Lecturer in the History of Medicine, University of Liverpool Liverpool, U.K. 123 Andrew J. Larner MA MD MRCP (UK) DHMSA Walton Centre for Neurology & Neurosurgery Lower Lane L9 7LJ Liverpool, UK ISBN 978-1-4419-7094-7 e-ISBN 978-1-4419-7095-4 DOI 10.1007/978-1-4419-7095-4 Springer New York Dordrecht Heidelberg London Library of Congress Control Number: 2010937226 © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2001, 2006, 2011 All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 Spring Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are subject to proprietary rights. While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of going to press, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omissions that may be made.
    [Show full text]
  • Download Preprint
    Prism adaptation and reach planning 1 Prism adaptation speeds reach initiation in the direction of the prism after-effect. Christopher L. Striemer1,2* & Carley A. Borza1,3 1. Department of Psychology, MacEwan University, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada 2. Neuroscience and Mental Health Institute, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada 3. Department of Educational Psychology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada *Corresponding Author: Christopher L. Striemer, PhD Associate Professor Department of Psychology MacEwan University 10700 – 104 Avenue Edmonton, Alberta, Canada, T5J 4S2 Email: [email protected] Phone: 780-633-3467 Fax: 780-497-5308 Prism adaptation and reach planning 2 Abstract: Damage to the temporal-parietal cortex in the right hemisphere often leads to spatial neglect – a disorder in which patients are unable to attend to sensory input from their contralesional (left) side. Neglect has been associated with both attentional and premotor deficits. That is, in addition to having difficulty with attending to the left side, patients are often slower to initiate leftward vs. rightward movements (i.e., directional hypokinesia). Previous research has indicated that a brief period of adaptation to rightward shifting prisms can reduce symptoms of neglect by adjusting the patient’s movements leftward, towards the neglected field. Although prism adaptation has been shown to reduce spatial attention deficits in patients with neglect, very little work has examined the effects of prisms on premotor symptoms. In the current study, we examined this in healthy individuals using leftward shifting prisms to induce a rightward shift in the egocentric reference frame, similar to neglect patients prior to prism adaptation. Specifically, we examined the speed with which healthy participants initiated leftward and rightward reaches (without visual feedback) prior to and following adaptation to either 17° leftward (n=16) or 17° rightward (n=15) shifting prisms.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Neglect
    Cognitive Primer Spatial Neglect patial neglect is a common syndrome following stroke, ● When asked to report ten objects around the room Smost frequently of the right hemisphere. Up to two- neglect patients may predominantly choose items thirds of acute right-hemisphere stroke patients demon- from the ipsilesional side of space. strate signs of contralesional neglect, failing to be aware of ● Tests of personal neglect include asking the patient to objects or people to their left in extrapersonal space. For mime combing their hair, shaving or making-up their example, when searching through a visual scene patients face. Right-hemisphere patients often fail to groom the with left neglect tend to look at elements on the right only left side of their face on such tasks, as well as in daily (Fig. 1). The syndrome may also involve ‘personal’ space, life. with patients neglecting their own contralesional body Andrew Parton is Post- Doctoral Research parts. Importantly, many patients are unaware they have Associate in the these problems (anosognosia). Perhaps unsurprisingly, Department of Visual therefore, enduring neglect is a poor prognostic indicator Neuroscience at Imperial College for functional independence following stroke. London, Charing Cross Hospital. His research interests are the cognitive neuroscience of attention, eye movements and spatial representation. Figure 2. In a clinical cancellation task a neglect patient tends to cross out only those targets (small stars) on the right of the page. Model Copy Masud Husain is Wellcome Trust Senior Fellow and Reader in Figure 1. Measurement of the eye-movements during search for letter Neurology at Imperial Ts among Ls of a typical patient with left neglect reveals a tendency to College London, repeatedly fixate items on the right while ignoring those on the left.
    [Show full text]
  • Hemispatial Neglect, Balint's Syndrome and Attention
    Hemispatial Neglect, Balint’s Syndrome and Attentionq Piers DL Howe, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, VIC, Australia Ó 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction 1 Forms of Neglect 1 Processing of Neglected Stimuli 2 Balint’s Syndrome 2 Conclusion 2 Further Reading 3 Glossary Anosognosia A neurological disorder, usually caused by Optic ataxia The inability to guide the hand toward an brain injury, characterized by the sufferers denying they object using visual information that cannot be explained have an impairment. by motor or sensory deficits. Balint’s syndrome A neurological disorder characterized Simultanagnosia The inability to perceive more than one by simultanagnosia and optic ataxia. Typically caused by object at a time. bilateral damage to the posterior parietal cortex. Somatoparaphrenia The belief that part of one’s body Extinction A neurological disorder where the ability to belongs to another person. attend to stimuli in a particular region of the visual field is impaired by the presence of stimuli outside that region. Introduction Hemispatial neglect, also known as unilateral neglect, hemineglect, neglect syndrome or spatial neglect, is a disabling brain disorder, typically caused by a brain lesion. Patients with neglect have difficulty paying attention to contralesional space and compensate by overly attending to ipsilesional space. It is most commonly caused by damage to the right cerebral cortex. Damage to the left cerebral cortex can also cause neglect but it is less common and typically less severe. Neglect is especially likely to be caused by lesions near the junction of the temporal lobe and the inferior parietal lobule. It can also be caused by lesions in the frontal cortex, in the white matter or in subcortical areas such as the thalamus and basal ganglia.
    [Show full text]
  • Speech Disorder and Behavioral Involvement in a Thalamic Stroke: A
    Caruso et al. Int J Brain Disord Treat 2015, 1:2 ISSN: 2469-5866 International Journal of Brain Disorders and Treatment Case Report: Open Access Speech Disorder and Behavioral Involvement in a Thalamic Stroke: A Case Report Paola Caruso*, Moretti R and Manganotti P Clinical Neurology, University of Trieste, Trieste, Italy *Corresponding author: Paola Caruso, MD, Neurologist, Clinical Neurology, University of Trieste, Azienda Ospedaliero Universitaria di Trieste, Trieste, IT, Italy, E-mail: [email protected] hospitalization antiplatelet therapy was started. Patient performed Abstract screening vascular examination including echocardiography carotid Data from literature on clinical manifestation of thalamic strokes Doppler and thrombophylic screening tests. Soon dizziness and have been published for ages. First in 1906 Dejerine e Roussy visual disorder ameliorated but then he showed palipsychism (which has spoken about sensory motor disturbances and have opened corresponds to an overlap of sequential cognitive processes in two the door to new pathologic disorders that may occur after thalamic or more domains); patient also showed severe perseverative behavior lesions. From 1925 behavior and speech disorders related with thalamic injury were described. Since then a classification of with increased sensitivity to interference, anterograde memory thalamic syndromes into four groups based on the four main arterial retrieval deficit with memory loss and produced a decreased and territories was accepted. As we know thalamic stroke account for invalid-output speech, characterized by unpredictable topic shifts, 11% of vertebra basilar infarct. Inferolateral territory infarctions with grammatically correct phrases, sometimes he accused naming are the most common injury (45%), followed by the paramedian difficulties and apathy with affective flatness.
    [Show full text]