Dynamics of Cover Test Eye Movements

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Dynamics of Cover Test Eye Movements 0093-7002/83/6008-0712$02.00/0 Vol. 60, No.8, pp. 712-724 AMERICAN JOURNAL OF OPTOMETRY & PHYSIOLOGICAL OPTICS Printed in U.S.A. Copyright © 1983 AMERICAN ACADEMY OF OPTOMETRY Dynamics of Cover Test Eye Movements ELI PELI- GLEN McCORMACKt The New Eng/and College of Optometry, Boston, Massachusetts ABSTRACT The cover test is the most commonly used The dynamics of unilateral cover test eye technique for the detection and diagnosis of heterophoric and strabismic oculomotor imbal­ movements, never before systematically in­ I vestigated with an objective recording system, ance. ,2 The unilateral cover test, in which one are shown to be more complex than textbook eye is briefly covered and then uncovered, is accounts of them. We administered the cover used principally to reveal the presence of a strab­ test to nine heterophoric subjects by means of ismic deviation. The alternating cover test, in electromechanical occluders. Eye movements which the cover is repeatedly switched from one were recorded using the infrared photoelectric eye to the other, is used to quantify the devia­ technique. Saccadic and vergence move­ tion. This report concerns the dynamics of uni­ ments of the fixating eye were observed in lateral cover test eye movements in normally almost all records when the occluded eye was binocular patients. The standard clinical descriptions of unilat­ uncovered. These movements were found in l 2 esophores and exophores and in both large eral cover test eye movements • assert that and small phoria cases. Such movements when one eye is covered (the "cover phase") it were previously described in other asymmetric will deviate to its heterophoric posture, while vergence tasks and appear to obey Hering's the noncovered eye maintains steady fixation. law of equal innervation. Uncovering the dom­ When the cover is removed (the "uncover inant eye, in cases .qf clear dominancy, re­ phase") the formerly covered eye regains fixa­ sulted in shorter latency and larger amplitude tion, and again it is assumed that the fixating saccades than did uncovering the nondomi­ eye does not move. The required movement is nant eye. These large saccades were fre­ therefore an asymmetric vergence in which the quently of unequal amplitude in each eye. "fixating" eye should remain steady while the Trained subjects appear to use dynamic covered eye performs the entire vergence. Such overshoots to increase this saccadic inequal­ descriptions portray the static position of the ity and thereby attain vergence during sac­ eyes before and after occlusion, and not the cades. Movements after the application of a actual trajectory of the eyes that lead to their cover to one eye, while grossly similar to text­ static position. Thus, Griffin notes that a slight book descriptions of them, are found to con­ "~ick" ~f the fixating eye may be noted in pa­ tain small vergence drifts and refixation (cor­ tients wIth large phorias. recting) saccades in the nonoccluded eye. If the fixating eye of heterophores made no movement during the cover test (i.e., in small heterophorias) then the cover test eye move­ Key W?rds: cover test, asymmetric vergence, ment pattern would be in violation of Hering's dynamic overshoots, heterophoria, unequal law of equal innervation3 and run counter to a saccades number of studies on asymmetric vergence eye 4 movements. ---6 Because the intermittent occlu­ sion of the cover test is a stimulus condition not previously used in asymmetric vergence studies, Presented at the 1981 Annual Meeting of the Amer­ and because there are no reports available on ican Academy of Optometry, Orlando, Florida. the dyn~mics of cover test eye movements using Received AprilS, 19S2; revision received March 29, a recordmg technique sufficient to resolve these 1983. dynamics, we decided that a reassessment of * Electrical Engineer, MSEE, Research Associate, cover test eye movements using a high-speed Student. objective recording technique was needed to re- t Optometrist, Ph.D., Member of Faculty. solve the question of the general conformity of 712 August 1983 Cover Test Eye Movements-Peli & McCormack 713 unilateral cover test eye movements to Hering's phase differs from the accommodation-only con­ law and to the known characteristics of asym­ dition because of the absence of any accommo­ metric vergence eye movements. dative stimulus change in the cover phase. Semmlow and Heerema7 measured, in normal The other extreme of the vergence eye move­ observers, movements of the covered eye after a ment spectrum demands a vergence response unilateral cover. Their method of applying the despite a constant accommodative demand, and cover when the eyes are at the phoria position is known as the "disparity-only" case. Disparity­ of gaze was intended only to demonstrate certain only stimuli may be presented haploscopi­ accommodative/convergence relationships, and cally,4.13,14 or by the interposition of a prism limits the conclusions one may draw from their before one eye (e.g., the 4 t. base-out test). results about the normal dynamics of cover test Hering's law of equal innervation seems to hold eye movements. A few studies of cover test eye best for the disparity-only condition because movement dynamics based on visual observa­ almost every. response is found to contain an 9 tions have been published,s-lO Pickwe1l ,10 no­ irrelevant saccade of large magnitude. Many of ticed, in patients with high heterophoria, move­ these disparity-only movements also show what ment of the noncovered eye during the uncover appears to be a non-Hering's law inequality phase of the test. He found that the fixating eye of "irrelevant" saccade amplitude6 in which the frequently makes a saccade away from the point covered eye. generates the large saccade. (Ken­ of regard which is followed by an approximately yon et a1. 15, 16 have also observed this inequality symmetric vergence movement back to the point in accommodation-only experiments.) These of regard. This first movement of the fixating unequal saccades have the effect of reducing the eye, named the "irrelevant" movement or "flick" fusional vergence response required to meet the by PickwelPo as it did not reflect the demand of disparity demand in the disparity-only case. the stimulus, was assumed to be an expression The vergence demand of the uncover phase of of Hering's law. Pickwell10 noted that irrelevant the unilateral cover test is very similar to the saccades were not reported in earlier studies. asymmetric disparity-only demand. In both Irrelevant movements have been demon­ cases, the accommodative stimulus is held con­ strated in a variety of experiments on asymmet­ stant and the disparity of the target, subtended ric vergence.4-6 The vergence stimuli in some of entirely at the nonaligned eye, is the only verg­ these experiments are aligned on the visual axis ence stimulus. These two vergence stimuli differ of one eye, thus demanding no change in the qualitatively only with respect to the intermit­ position of the aligned eye when vergence de­ tency of binocular vision in the cover test. If the mands are varied. We shall compare the cover absence of binocular vision does not affect the test response to these types of eye movements. subsequent eye movements in the uncover phase Ono and Nakamizo4 studied asymmetric verg­ of the cover test, then we can expect these ence eye movements throughout a spectrum of movements to be similar to those found in the accommodation and convergence conditions. disparity-only condition and that the irrelevant They found movement of the aligned eye in saccade characteristics should be present in almost all trials. The magnitude of this saccadic small phoria cases as well as large phoria cases. movement varied with the different conditions. Both of these expectations were realized. One extreme of the asymmetric vergence spec­ trum is represented by the Johannes Mueller 4 METHODS experiment • .'i,11,12 in which the eye not aligned with the far and near targets is constantly oc­ Nine subjects, aged 24 to 35 years, all had cluded. This case is known as the "accommo­ normal binocular vision and 6/6 (20/20) or bet­ dation only" condition4 as no disparity-driven ter visual acuity while wearing their habitual vergence is possible with one eye occluded. Most distance corrections. One was tested while wear­ of the movement is performed by the covered ing soft contact lenses, two wore spectacles, and eye, which manifests an asymmetric vergence the remaining six subjects were nearly emme-' which appears to violate Hering's law. The re­ tropic. Six subjects were exophoric and three sults show, as noted by Ono and N akamizo, 4 were esophoric. Informed consent was obtained that the "fixating" eye moves toward the oc­ from all subjects before their participation. cluded eye, but its movement is much smaller. Eye movements were recorded using an in­ Thus, some type of irrelevant or Hering-type frared photoelectric technique.17 The electronics movement is maintained even under these mon­ for the photoelectric eye movement monitor ocular conditions. The accommodation-only were assembled in our laboratory. Infrared sen­ condition is similar to the cover phase of the sors and emitters were fitted to an adjustable cover test by virtue of the absence of binocular spectacle frame mounting device (Rehder Devel. visual control over either movement. The cover Co., Castro Valley, CAl. A small bite-board was 714 AM J OPTOM & PHYSIOL OPTICS Vol. 60, No.8 used to stabilize both head movements and the these cases, the experimenter waited (up to 8 viewing distance of 30 em. For calibration pur~ sec in some cases) for the completion of the eye poses the subjects tracked a horizontally moving movement, as determined by on-line monitor­ spot on a cathode ray tube.
Recommended publications
  • Management of Microtropia
    Br J Ophthalmol: first published as 10.1136/bjo.58.3.281 on 1 March 1974. Downloaded from Brit. J. Ophthal. (I974) 58, 28 I Management of microtropia J. LANG Zirich, Switzerland Microtropia or microstrabismus may be briefly described as a manifest strabismus of less than 50 with harmonious anomalous correspondence. Three forms can be distinguished: primary constant, primary decompensating, and secondary. There are three situations in which the ophthalmologist may be confronted with micro- tropia: (i) Amblyopia without strabismus; (2) Hereditary and familial strabismus; (3) Residual strabismus after surgery. This may be called secondary microtropia, for everyone will admit that in most cases of convergent strabismus perfect parallelism and bifoveal fixation are not achieved even after expert treatment. Microtropia and similar conditions were not mentioned by such well-known early copyright. practitioners as Javal, Worth, Duane, and Bielschowsky. The views of Maddox (i898), that very small angles were extremely rare, and that the natural tendency to fusion was much too strong to allow small angles to exist, appear to be typical. The first to mention small residual angles was Pugh (I936), who wrote: "A patient with monocular squint who has been trained to have equal vision in each eye and full stereoscopic vision with good amplitude of fusion may in 3 months relapse into a slight deviation http://bjo.bmj.com/ in the weaker eye and the vision retrogresses". Similar observations of small residual angles have been made by Swan, Kirschberg, Jampolsky, Gittoes-Davis, Cashell, Lyle, Broadman, and Gortz. There has been much discussion in both the British Orthoptic Journal and the American Orthoptic journal on the cause of this condition and ways of avoiding it.
    [Show full text]
  • Routine Eye Examination
    CET Continuing education Routine eye examination Part 3 – Binocular assessment In the third part of our series on the eye examination, Andrew Franklin and Bill Harvey look at the assessment and interpretation of binocular status. Module C8290, one general CET point, suitable for optometrists and DOs he assessment of binocular previous question. Leading questions function is often one of the should be avoided, especially when weaker areas of a routine, dealing with children. If they are out if observation of candidates of line ask the patient ‘Which one is out in the professional qualifica- of line with the X?’ Ttions examination is any guide. Tests are You should know before you start the done for no clearly logical reason, often test which line is seen by which eye. because they always have been, and in If you cannot remember it from last an order which defeats the object of the time, simply look at the target through testing. Binocular vision seems to be one the visor yourself (Figure 1). Even if of those areas that practitioners shy away you think you can remember, check from, and students often take an instant anyway, as it is possible for the polarisa- dislike to. Many retests and subsequent tion of the visor not to match that of the remakes of spectacles are the result of a Mallett Unit at distance or near or both, practitioner overlooking the effects of a Figure 1 A distance fixation disparity target especially if the visor is a replacement. change of prescription on the binocular If both eyes can see both bars, nobody status of the patient.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Vision
    BINOCULAR VISION Rahul Bhola, MD Pediatric Ophthalmology Fellow The University of Iowa Department of Ophthalmology & Visual Sciences posted Jan. 18, 2006, updated Jan. 23, 2006 Binocular vision is one of the hallmarks of the human race that has bestowed on it the supremacy in the hierarchy of the animal kingdom. It is an asset with normal alignment of the two eyes, but becomes a liability when the alignment is lost. Binocular Single Vision may be defined as the state of simultaneous vision, which is achieved by the coordinated use of both eyes, so that separate and slightly dissimilar images arising in each eye are appreciated as a single image by the process of fusion. Thus binocular vision implies fusion, the blending of sight from the two eyes to form a single percept. Binocular Single Vision can be: 1. Normal – Binocular Single vision can be classified as normal when it is bifoveal and there is no manifest deviation. 2. Anomalous - Binocular Single vision is anomalous when the images of the fixated object are projected from the fovea of one eye and an extrafoveal area of the other eye i.e. when the visual direction of the retinal elements has changed. A small manifest strabismus is therefore always present in anomalous Binocular Single vision. Normal Binocular Single vision requires: 1. Clear Visual Axis leading to a reasonably clear vision in both eyes 2. The ability of the retino-cortical elements to function in association with each other to promote the fusion of two slightly dissimilar images i.e. Sensory fusion. 3. The precise co-ordination of the two eyes for all direction of gazes, so that corresponding retino-cortical element are placed in a position to deal with two images i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • Strabismus: a Decision Making Approach
    Strabismus A Decision Making Approach Gunter K. von Noorden, M.D. Eugene M. Helveston, M.D. Strabismus: A Decision Making Approach Gunter K. von Noorden, M.D. Emeritus Professor of Ophthalmology and Pediatrics Baylor College of Medicine Houston, Texas Eugene M. Helveston, M.D. Emeritus Professor of Ophthalmology Indiana University School of Medicine Indianapolis, Indiana Published originally in English under the title: Strabismus: A Decision Making Approach. By Gunter K. von Noorden and Eugene M. Helveston Published in 1994 by Mosby-Year Book, Inc., St. Louis, MO Copyright held by Gunter K. von Noorden and Eugene M. Helveston All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without prior written permission from the authors. Copyright © 2010 Table of Contents Foreword Preface 1.01 Equipment for Examination of the Patient with Strabismus 1.02 History 1.03 Inspection of Patient 1.04 Sequence of Motility Examination 1.05 Does This Baby See? 1.06 Visual Acuity – Methods of Examination 1.07 Visual Acuity Testing in Infants 1.08 Primary versus Secondary Deviation 1.09 Evaluation of Monocular Movements – Ductions 1.10 Evaluation of Binocular Movements – Versions 1.11 Unilaterally Reduced Vision Associated with Orthotropia 1.12 Unilateral Decrease of Visual Acuity Associated with Heterotropia 1.13 Decentered Corneal Light Reflex 1.14 Strabismus – Generic Classification 1.15 Is Latent Strabismus
    [Show full text]
  • Surgical Classification of Squints with a Vertical Deviation
    Br J Ophthalmol: first published as 10.1136/bjo.39.3.129 on 1 March 1955. Downloaded from Brit. J. Ophthal. (1955) 39, 129. COMMUNICATIONS SURGICAL CLASSIFICATION OF SQUINTS WITH A VERTICAL DEVIATION* BY ALFREDO VILLASECA Hospital Salvador, Santiago, Chile VERTICAL squints, either pure or associated with horizontal squints, are of great practical importance, since binocular single vision can only be attained if there is perfect parallelism of the visual axis in both the horizontal and vertical directions. The statistics given by various authors show that vertical deviations are very frequent: White and Brown (1939) found the following distribution in 1,062 cases : un- complicated lateral deviation in 347 patients, uncomplicated vertical deviation in 358, and combined vertical and lateral deviation in 357. Dunnington and Regan (1950) found that 50 per cent. of 79 cases of concomitant convergent strabismus showed a vertical component. Scobee (1951), in 457 cases of convergent strabismus, found 195 (43 per cent.) copyright. with a vertical component. In his patients the frequency of involvement (paresis) of the vertically acting muscles was as follows: Muscle No. of Cases Superior rectus ... ... ... ... ... 60 Superior oblique ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 32 Inferior rectus ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 25 of the same eye 20 Both depressors (superior oblique and inferior rectus) http://bjo.bmj.com/ Both superior obliques ... ... ... 15 Both inferior recti ... ... ... ... ... ... 13 Superior rectus of one eye and inferior rectus of the other eye ... ... 11 Both superior recti ... ... ... ... ... ... ... 7... Both elevators (superior rectus and inferior oblique) of the same eye ... 6 Superior oblique and inferior rectus of both eyes ... ... ... 2 Inferior oblique ... ... ... ... ... ... ... ...1 Miscellaneous groupings ... .. ... ... ... ... ... 3 He makes no specific mehtion of patients with concomitant hypertropia or on September 23, 2021 by guest.
    [Show full text]
  • Comparison of Eccentric Fixation Measurements Using the Streak Target of an Ophthalmoscope and a Traditional Visuoscopy Target
    Comparison of Eccentric Fixation Measurements Using the Streak Target of an Ophthalmoscope and a Traditional Visuoscopy Target Jeffrey Cooper, MS, OD; Ilana Gelfond, OD; Pamela E. Carlson, OD; Brian Campolattaro, MD; and Frederick Wang, MD ABSTRACT Purpose: To compare measurements of eccentric eccentric fixation in children younger than 3 fixation using two different targets for fixation, a years compared to the traditional visuoscope tar- traditional visuoscope target and the streak target get (75% versus 30%). All of the patients older of an ophthalmoscope. than 3 years were testable using both targets, with both methods yielding the same results. All of the Patients and Methods: Monocular fixation was patients with a visual acuity of at least 20/20 also evaluated using visuoscopy in 47 patients ranging demonstrated central fixation using both tech- in age from 4 months to 22 years. Visuoscopy mea- niques. surements were compared using both the tradi- tional visuoscope target and the streak target of a Conclusions: Using the streak of a Welch Allyn Welch Allyn ophthalmoscope. The streak light was ophthalmoscope as a visuoscope target allows for rotated both horizontally and vertically to detect testing of eccentric fixation in children younger both horizontal and vertical eccentric fixation. than 3 years. Results: The streak target improved testability of J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus 2005;42:89-96. INTRODUCTION patients with strabismic amblyopia.4 Although there is no exact relationship between eccentricity and Measurement of monocular fixation is impor- visual acuity,3-6 generally the farther away from the tant in the diagnosis and treatment of amblyopia.1,2 fovea that patients fixate, the greater the decrease in Eccentric fixation is present in approximately 44% visual acuity.3,7-9 In addition, fixation becomes of all patients with amblyopia3 and in 30% of increasingly unsteady the farther away fixation is from the fovea.3,8 It has been assumed that as visual acuity improves during treatment, fixation will Drs.
    [Show full text]
  • Amblyopia Strabismus
    7 Strabismus, Amblyopia & Leukocoria Color index: 432 Team – Important – 433 Notes – Not important [email protected] Objectives (Weren’t provided) Strabismus Amblyopia Leukocoria Esotropia Pseudostrabismus Exotropia Strabismus Definition: Abnormal alignment of the eyes; the condition of having a squint. (Oxford dictionary) Epidemiology: 2%-3% of children and young adults. Prevalence in males=females Causes: Inherited pattern. Most patients fall under this category, so it is important to ask about family history. Idiopathic. Binocular Single Vision can be: Neurological conditions (Cerebral palsy, Hydrocephalus & -Normal – Binocular Single brain tumors). vision can be classified as Down syndrome. normal when it is bifoveal and Congenital cataract, Eyes Tumor. there is no manifest deviation. -Anomalous - Binocular Single Why we are concerned about strabismus? vision is anomalous when the 1. Double vision. Mainly in adults. because images of the fixatedchildren object and infants have a suppression feature which are projected from the fovea is not found in adults of one eye and an extrafoveal 2. Cosmetic. area of the other eye i.e. when 3. Binocular single vision. the visual direction of the retinal elements has changed. Consequences A small manifest strabismus is therefore always present in Amblyopia (lazy eye). In children anomalous Binocular Single Double vision. Usually in adults but you may vision. see it in children. E.g: if they have a tumor and they present with sudden esotropia and diplopia Tests for deviation: 1. Hirschberg test: 1mm from pupil center=15PD (prism diopter) or 7o. also known as corneal light reflex you shine the light at one arm length into both eyes and see the corneal reflex.
    [Show full text]
  • 2015-03-Ocular Motility-Iiacobucci-Chap. 1
    Author: Ida Lucy Iacobucci, 2015 License: Unless otherwise noted, this material is made available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-Share Alike 4.0 License: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/ The University of Michigan Open.Michigan initiative has reviewed this material in accordance with U.S. Copyright Law and has tried to maximize your ability to use, share, and adapt it. Copyright holders of content included in this material should contact [email protected] with any questions, corrections, or clarification regarding the use of content. For more information about how to attribute these materials, please visit: http://open.umich.edu/education/about/terms-of-use. Chapter 1 BASIC TESTS TO EVALUATE OCULAR MOTILITY I. VISION ASSESSMENT IN CHILDREN UNDER FIVE YEARS OF AGE 1. Visual Acuity Timeline: - Newborn = 20/600, child will respond to bright lights by squeezing lids. - 1 month = 20/400 child will follow a large object of light. - 3-5 months = 20/200 child will reliably follow a target in all gazes. - 12 months = 20/40 (estimate) hand-eye coordination developing will grab objects. - 2-3 years = 20/30 child can identify/match various pictures. - 4+years = 20/20 child can reliably identify letters. 2. Obtaining Reliable VA - Always measure monocularly with adhesive eye patch. - Exception: Nystagmus, low vision - use opaque occluder. - Never use hand to occlude the eye. - Don’t give up too soon! 3. Preverbal Children a) Light - Used for children < 2 months. - Shine light into each eye and watch response. - Blinking, shielding, squeezing eyes, crying – all normal responses.
    [Show full text]
  • Binocular Vision Disorders
    Pediatric Clinical Pearls Valerie M. Kattouf O.D., FAAO, FCOVD Illinois College of Optometry Chicago, IL Pediatric Clinical Pearls Collecting exam data / Modification of exam techniques Determining the best treatment options Pediatric Refractive Error Amblyopia Strabismus Collecting Exam Data How do you modify the tests you already know how to do? Direct Observation Visual Acuity Assessment Fixate and Follow Target: Transilluminator / Face / Toy Evaluate: Response to light Note: Lid closure to bright light ≠ cortical function Tracking ability Comparison of ability of each eye When delayed Possible prematurity Possible generalized motor delay Possible Visual System Diagnosis Fixate and Follow Procedure Cover one eye Examiner hand/thumb, Parent’s hand Light directed at patient’s face Obtain fixation with non-occluded eye May tap/flash/move light Recording: F&F = able to fixate and follow target CSM Central – light reflex = central Steady – eye maintains alignment with stationary and moving object Maintenance – under binocular conditions does previously fixating eye maintain fixation Forced Choice Visual Acuity Tests Lea Symbols Ocular Motility Refractive Error Assessment Pediatric Trial Frame Use Administration of Cycloplegic Agent Cycloplegic Spray 0.5% Tropicamide 0.5% Cyclogel 2.5% Phenylephrine O’BRIEN Pharmacy 800-627-4360 Cycloplegic Spray Shelf life – approved for 120 days (stability and potency) Stable past that FDA typically approves no more than 30 days Can vary percentages based on needs of
    [Show full text]
  • Cover Test Scope (Staff): Community Health Staff Scope (Area): CAHS-CH, WACHS
    Community Health Clinical Nursing Manual PROCEDURE Cover test Scope (Staff): Community health staff Scope (Area): CAHS-CH, WACHS Child Safe Organisation Statement of Commitment The Child and Adolescent Health Service (CAHS) commits to being a child safe organisation by meeting the National Child Safe Principles and National Child Safe Standards. This is a commitment to a strong culture supported by robust policy documents to ensure the safety and wellbeing of children at CAHS. This document should be read in conjunction with this DISCLAIMER Aim To detect a manifest strabismus in preschool and school-aged children. Risk Undetected or unmanaged vision impairment can have a significant effect on a child’s health, psycho-social development, educational progress, and long term social and vocational outcomes.1 Background Amblyopia is decreased vision in one or both eyes due to abnormal development of vision in infancy or childhood. In amblyopia, there may not be an obvious problem of the eye. Vision loss occurs because nerve pathways between the brain and the eye aren't properly stimulated. As a result, the brain favours one eye, usually due to poor vision in the other eye. The brain “learns” to see only blurry images with the amblyopic eye even when glasses are used. The lay term for amblyopia is “lazy eye.” It is the leading cause of vision loss amongst children.2 Amblyopia is unique to children but is preventable if the child receives adequate treatment in childhood. The prevalence of amblyopia is approximately 2% of preschool children in Australia.3 Strabismus is the most common cause of amblyopia and is the term used to describe any anomaly of ocular alignment.
    [Show full text]
  • THE MEASUREMENT of HETEROPHORIA Copyright
    Br J Ophthalmol: first published as 10.1136/bjo.25.5.189 on 1 May 1941. Downloaded from THE BRITISH JOURNAL OF OPHTHALMOLOGY MAY, 1941 COMMUNICATIONS THE MEASUREMENT OF HETEROPHORIA copyright. BY NIGEL CRIDLAND, D.M., D.O. PART II http://bjo.bmj.com/ Section I The diversity of the available methods is only equalled by the diversion which some of them engender. The fantastic elabora- tions of the instrument-maker have introduced further complexity, so that the patient, when faced by an imposing mass of machinery, may be easily forgiven for his failure to relax, and the element of on September 27, 2021 by guest. Protected voluntary effort becomes a thunder-cloud obscuring the entire heterophoric horizon. The wonder is that the results of measure- ments by different tests are not even more contradictory than they are. Perhaps no pit-head winding gear can wholly prevent truth fronm emerging from her well. Various groupings of the methods of revealing a latent deviation have been chosen by different writers on the subject, and all have ignored the primary dichotomy introduced by the position of rest which is to be attempted. True, only two methods fall into the group which essays the physiological position of rest, but that is no reason for denving them their distinction. All the remaining Br J Ophthalmol: first published as 10.1136/bjo.25.5.189 on 1 May 1941. Downloaded from 190 NIGEL CRIDLAND tests aim at determining the fusion-free position, and they fall con- veniently into four groups. There is first the group in which dissociation is effected bv limiting the field of vision of one or both eves.
    [Show full text]
  • Strabismus Measurements with Novel Video Goggles
    Zurich Open Repository and Archive University of Zurich Main Library Strickhofstrasse 39 CH-8057 Zurich www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2017 Strabismus measurements with novel video goggles Weber, Konrad P ; Rappoport, Daniel ; Dysli, Muriel ; Schmückle Meier, Tanja ; Marks, Guy B ; Bockisch, Christopher J ; Landau, Klara ; MacDougall, Hamish G Abstract: PURPOSE: To assess the validity of a novel, simplified, noninvasive test for strabismus using video goggles. DESIGN: Cross-sectional method comparison study in which the new test, the strabismus video goggles, is compared with the existing reference standard, the Hess screen test. PARTICIPANTS: We studied 41 adult and child patients aged ฀6 years with ocular misalignment owing to congenital or acquired paralytic or comitant strabismus and 17 healthy volunteers. METHODS: All participants were tested with binocular infrared video goggles with built-in laser target projection and liquid crystal display shutters for alternate occlusion of the eyes and the conventional Hess screen test. In both tests, ocular deviations were measured on a 9-point target grid located at 0±15° horizontal and vertical eccentricity. MAIN OUTCOME MEASURES: Horizontal and vertical ocular deviations at 9 different gaze positions of each eye were measured by the strabismus video goggles and the Hess screen test. Agreement was quantified as the intraclass correlation coefficient. Secondary outcomes were the utility of thegoggles in patients with visual suppression and in children. RESULTS: There was good agreement between the strabismus video goggles and the Hess screen test in the measurements of horizontal and vertical deviation (intraclass correlation coefficient horizontal 0.83, 95% confidence interval [0.77, 0.88], vertical 0.76, 95% confidence interval [0.68, 0.82]).
    [Show full text]