Spatial Disorientation: Decades of Pilot Fatalities

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Spatial Disorientation: Decades of Pilot Fatalities REVIEW ARTICLE Spatial Disorientation: Decades of Pilot Fatalities Randy Gibb , Bill Ercoline , and Lauren Scharff G IBB R, E RCOLINE B, S CHARFF L. Spatial disorientation: decades of tion community. Although we primarily address military pilot fatalities. Aviat Space Environ Med 2011; 82:717 - 24. aspects of SD, the problem also exists in both the com- Spatial disorientation (SD) has been a contributing factor in aviation mishaps for decades and efforts to mitigate SD have not been propor- mercial and general aviation sectors. The accurate per- tionate to the danger it poses to pilots. We argue that SD contributes to ception of one’s orientation in space is vital for safe nearly 33% of all mishaps with a fatality rate of almost 100%. However, aviation operations and, despite assumed improvements SD has not garnered the respect and awareness it requires from leader- ship and pilots because of historically inaccurate reporting within acci- in training programs and technology, SD-related mishaps dent investigations and under-reporting of SD data in research. Over 30 continue to occur and the most common types of SD ex- research studies and 10 mishap case studies are presented to portray periences have not drastically changed over the decades. SD’s role in aviation accidents since 1913. Research and training are SD is defi ned by Benson ( 6 ) as, “ the pilot fails to sense recommended to improve pilot respect and awareness for SD-inducing scenarios that would include pilot recognition and successful recovery correctly the position, motion, or attitude of his aircraft Delivered by Ingenta to: Baylor Universityor of himself College within of theMedicine fi xed coordinate system provided using SD-specifi c simulators. Consequently,IP: funding 128.249.1.198 is needed to furtherOn: Mon, 20 Jun 2016 17:46:49 SD research, to fund SD training, as well as amendCopyright: the current Aerospace mishap Medicalby the surface Association of the Earth and the gravitational vertical ” investigation process to better articulate SD contributions in aviation (p. 277). The 2005 United States Air Force Manual of accidents. Keywords: aviation mishaps , aviation accidents , aviation spatial orienta- Instrument Flying Procedures, 11-217 Volume I ( 45 ), tion , aviation safety , aviation training simulators . defi ned SD as “ the erroneous percept of any of the pa- rameters displayed by the aircraft control and perfor- mance instruments … regardless of a pilot’s experience or HE DAY THE MUSIC died … , ” as written and profi ciency, sensory illusion can lead to differences be- “ T sung by Don McClean, references the February tween instrument indications and what the pilot “ feels ” 1959 tragic airplane crash in which singers Buddy Holly, the aircraft is doing ” (p. 355). It is important to note the Ritchie Valens, and JP Richardson (aka, The Big Bopper) multisensory contributions to SD. Both vision and equi- were killed. The cause of the crash is believed to have librium/vestibular perceptions contribute (inter actively) been spatial disorientation (SD) by the pilot during the to spatial orientation perception, with vision accounting dark night takeoff while attempting to interpret a newly for nearly 80% in the aerospace environment ( 35 ). designed attitude indicator display. Unfortunately, even In contrast, for many years within the aviation com- such a high profi le crash did not prompt a serious effort munity, SD was often thought of only within the context to reduce SD-related mishaps for several decades. In the of vestibular illusions. It was not until Gillingham’s 1992 fall of 1984, Col. (Dr.) Grant McNaughton, USAF Flight landmark paper ( 19 ) published in the Journal of Vestibu- Safety Center, decided to produce a safety video for lar Research that “ tied the loose ends ” of the complete SD use in military fl ying squadrons. The video was titled problem together. However, there are still many exam- “ Spatial Disorientation — Still a Killer! ” Col. McNaughton, ples where it is obvious that the role of SD is not being a fl ight surgeon and private pilot, understood the prob- recognized, as noted in two 2009 SD mishaps presented lem of spatial disorientation in aviation and wanted to below. Another example of the failure to understand the do something that would help USAF pilots be better multisensory concept of SD is seen in the U.S. Naval prepared for SD. Aviation Safety offi ce ( 46 ) release of aeromedical causes Now, almost three decades later, the aviation commu- nity has still not substantially reduced the likelihood of in mishaps from 1990 to 2008. The #1 causal factor SD-related mishaps; Col. McNaughton has long since was SD and then listed at #4 were “ visual illusions. ” retired, but the issue of SD remains a threat to all fl ying Visual illusions should be considered a form of SD. The communities. Williams and Johnson in 2010 ( 52 ), also interplay between vision, proprioceptive, and vestibular from the USAF Flight Safety Center, sum up their recent SD fi ndings with a very poignant statement (and echo From the USAF Academy, CO. This manuscript was received for review in January 2011 . It was from the past), “ the only way to save your life from a accepted for publication in March 2011 . leading killer of fi ghter pilots is to prevent it ” (p. 21). In Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Col. Randall Gibb, six decades what have we learned? What are we doing Behavioral Sciences & Leadership, USAF Academy, 2354 Fairchild Dr., about this “ known killer ” ? Ste. 6L-166, USAF Academy, CO; [email protected] . Reprint & Copyright © by the Aerospace Medical Association, This paper summarizes current research and mishaps Alexandria, VA. involving SD, an issue that cuts across the entire avia- DOI: 10.3357/ASEM.3048.2011 Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine x Vol. 82, No. 7 x July 2011 717 SPATIAL DISORIENTATION — GIBB ET AL. systems is vital for accurate perception of a pilot’s ori- fl ight accomplishing a high-altitude strafe while wear- entation; thus all play a key role in understanding the ing NVGs. That night the illumination was defi ned as causes of and countermeasures for SD. “ low ” and the pilots overestimated their height above Perhaps the long history and commonness of SD have the featureless terrain. The primary causal factor deter- led the aviation community to become numb and desen- mined by the AIB was the incorrect calculation of the sitized to its threat, to the point of it being considered target elevation. Five contributing factors were also pre- “ the cost of doing business. ” For example, in 2000, sented by the AIB: misperception of operational condi- Lessard ( 26 ) simply stated that SD “ remains a problem tions, erroneous expectation of a typical night strafi ng for current pilots as it was for early aviators ” (p. 27). attack, inexperience executing night strafi ng, channel- We argue that SD contributes to at least 25 – 33% of all ized attention, and an improper crosscheck. Despite the aircraft mishaps and it results in the highest number of investigation presenting many aspects of SD in terms of fatalities. Other research reports generally do not credit an extremely dark night, featureless terrain, and NVG (blame) SD to that high of a causal/contributing percent- visual limitations, SD again was not formally listed in age. However, given the underreporting and inaccuracy the report. Are these really examples of ‘ pilot error ’ or of SD in mishap reports, and its correlation with con- might mission requirements simply be exceeding human trolled fl ight into terrain, loss of control, inadvertent physiological/perceptual capabilities? fl ight into weather, and loss of situational awareness In contrast to the two above mishaps and their lack of ( 47 ), SD’s actual impact may even be higher than one- formal SD acknowledgment is a recognized SD mishap third. that occurred in 2008 involving an F-16, from which the We are not the only ones who are pushing for height- pilot successfully ejected and survived ( 1 ). The experi- ened awareness of SD. In a 2002 keynote address, enced pilot was fl ying a nighttime NVG training mis- Benson ( 7 ) clearly stated that SD has continued to plague Delivered by Ingenta to: Baylor Universitysion off theCollege coast of of Medicine Florida. The AIB determined the pilots for 50 years and, despite improvedIP: 128.249.1.198 understanding On: Mon, cause 20 Jun of the2016 accident 17:46:49 was SD due to environmental con- of its etiology and enhanced pilot displays,Copyright: SD continues Aerospace Medicalditions ofAssociation a limited horizon over featureless terrain com- to kill pilots. He provided a historical perspective of SD bined with excessive maneuvering. The pilot surviving and detailed aviator SD mishaps going back to 1913. De- this SD mishap certainly helped investigators determine spite the fact that today’s pilots have instruments/visual the contributing causes to the accident compared with displays to help maintain orientation, it is apparent that the two previously discussed mishaps. However, de- aviation’s extreme demands on pilots exceed human spite the fatalities in the other mishaps, suffi cient infor- sensory-perceptual-cognitive capabilities, even with new mation regarding aspects of SD was presented in the technology. In fact, at times the new technology plays a investigative report. The question becomes why SD did contributing factor in SD (see below). not make its way into the executive summary and offi - Despite warnings such as Benson’s and others ’ , SD- cial list of contributing causes. related mishaps are still occurring and, unfortunately, Our objective in writing this paper is to highlight the SD is often not formally recognized as a contributing continuing danger of SD and offer solutions. More im- factor in mishaps. For example, in 2009 ( 2 ), a $21M F-16 portantly, we want to enlighten readers that lives and aircraft and pilot on a night training sortie wearing night resources lost could have possibly been saved had a vision goggles (NVGs) impacted the ground while meaningful investment been made in treating SD as the accomplishing a high-altitude strafe.
Recommended publications
  • Development of Spatial Disorientation Demonstration Scenarios for Commercial Pilot Training (STI Paper Series)
    STI Paper No. 807 Development of Spatial Disorientation Demonstration Scenarios for Commercial Pilot Training (STI Paper Series) June 2020 David H. Klyde Amanda K. Lampton, Ph.D. P. Chase Schulze Systems Technology, Inc. Published as AIAA-2016-1180 AIAA SciTech Forum 4-8 January 2016, San Diego, CA www.systemstech.com AIAA 2016-1180 AIAA SciTech 4-8 January 2016, San Diego, California, USA AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference Development of Spatial Disorientation Demonstration Scenarios for Commercial Pilot Training David H. Klyde* Amanda K. Lampton, Ph.D.† P. Chase Schulze ‡ Systems Technology, Inc., Hawthorne, CA, 90250 A study of world-wide commercial jet transport accidents by The Boeing Company found the most common events to be loss of control associated with an inability of pilots to recover from upsets and unusual attitudes. A key component in some of these events was pilot spatial disorientation. In terms of non-visual illusion cases, the spatial disorientation most commonly took the form of somatogravic and/or somatogyral illusions. Somatogravic illusions are those associated with the false sensation of body tilt, while somatogyral illusions are those associated with the inability of the human to perceive extended rotations. Improved pilot training in these abnormal flight conditions, including the ability of commercial pilot training simulators to replicate spatial disorientation, is needed to reduce loss of control accidents. A Federal Aviation Association-sponsored program led by Systems Technology, Inc. is currently developing spatial disorientation demonstration training scenarios using the B747-400 flight simulator at NASA Ames Research Center. Scenarios for a missed approach/go around and a steep bank, constant altitude turn have evolved from evaluations in the simulator conducted over several weeks.
    [Show full text]
  • Unrecognized Spatial Disorientation Recognition: a Methodological Study
    Unrecognized Spatial Disorientation Recognition: A Methodological Study Rucha P. Shankersinh Vaghela Bapu Group of Institutions, Gandhinagar, India ABSTRACT the body balanced in a complex, ever-changing world. In terms of the triggers of SD, researchers have agreed that Pilots also experience spatial disorientation (SD) when flying. visual illusions in the three-dimensional space motion world One factor contributing to this issue is the lack of a validated are at the root of the condition. procedure for detecting Type I (unrecognized) SD. Most of the current literature focuses on recognized SD evaluation Under normal conditions, humans gather knowledge (via the methods, but there are few reports that concentrate on sensory system, vestibular system, and proprioception system unrecognized SD evaluation methods. We go through some of [7]) to assess their orientation and condition. The visual the issues with SD and compare the various SD assessment system is responsible for around 80% of the details required approaches. We report on the benefits and drawbacks of the for orientation [9]. The vestibular mechanism, which includes quantitative questionnaire assessment process and the the semicircular canal organ and the otolith organ, is situated behavior evaluation method in these books. We suggest a tool in the inner part of the ear and provides information for to diagnose unrecognized SD based on these analyses, which positioning [10]. Linear acceleration and gravity variations are improves SD estimation in many ways. Centered on the sensed by otolith lungs, while angular acceleration changes subjective report and the core of strain, we establish a new are sensed by semicircular canals [11]. In the processing of approach to studying unrecognized SD (CoP).
    [Show full text]
  • Interagency Aviation Accident Prevention Bulletin 19-05 Spatial
    OAS-43A (4/18) Interagency Aviation Accident Prevention Bulletin No. IA APB 19-05 Date: September 18, 2019 Page 1 of 2 Subject: Spatial Disorientation, Vertigo, and Head Movement/Position Changes Area of Concern: Flight Safety Distribution: All Aviation Activities Discussion: A recent SAFECOM (#19-0520) describes a situation where a pilot began to feel ill while conducting heli-torch operations on a prescribed burn. The pilot described a vertigo sensation and feeling queasy while flying through smoke and opted to land and shutdown the helicopter. Once safely on the ground, the pilot felt the need to rest for about 20 minutes before continuing the mission. While the helicopter was shutdown, prescribed fire personnel were unable to make radio contact with the pilot. As a result, the Helicopter Manager requested that an Emergency Medical Technician (EMT) on the burn crew hike to the landing area to assess the pilot’s condition. After assessing the pilot, the decision was made to discontinue the heli-torch mission for the day and for the pilot be evaluated by a higher level of care. The pilot visited a physician the following morning and was cleared to return to work. It is not uncommon for those flying in fire operational environments to encounter these types of symptoms. Prior to feeling ill, the pilot had been operating in an area with visual horizons that varied in slope, elevation and angle, along with smoke laying over the ridgetops. The mission profile required constant change in airspeeds ranging from hovering to 50 knots, varying altitudes in order to cross ridgelines, and frequent turns due to the small size of the burn area.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Factors & Aviation Medicine January-February 1992
    F L I G H T S A F E T Y F O U N D A T I O N HUMAN FACTORS & AVIATION MEDICINE Vol. 39 No. 1For Everyone Concerned with the Safety of Flight January/February 1992 Inflight Spatial Disorientation Pilots must learn to recognize the visual and sensory signs of approaching problems interpreting motion, position and attitude and to minimize their effects on the safety of flight. by Melchor J. Antuñano, M.D. KRUG Life Sciences/U.S. Air Force School of Aerospace Medicine and Stanley R. Mohler, M.D. Wright State University/Aerospace Medicine Program Spatial orientation refers to a person’s ability to perceive focal vision, which is involved in the recognition and motion, position and attitude in relation to the surround- identification of viewed objects (resulting from the stimu- ing environment. This capability depends upon the re- lation of the central region of the retina, the fovea, and ception, integration and interpretation of sensory inputs immediately adjacent areas). The second is peripheral from visual, vestibular (inner ear), muscular and skin vision, which provides spatial visual reference (resulting receptors. The genetic design of the spatial orientation from the stimulation of the retina beyond the foveal mechanism copes with usual human activities (sitting, area). Foveal vision provides the fine, detailed sense of standing, walking, running, and jumping, for example). the visual image. Peripheral vision is more sensitive to However, this mechanism can encounter interpretation motion and also provides overall broad field of vision problems when outside visual references are lost and an position cues. The great importance of peripheral vision illusory environment exists.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Orientation, Adaptation, and Motion Sickness in Real and Virtual Environments
    forum Spotlight On: Simulator Sickness Spatial Orientation, Adaptation, and Motion Sickness in Real and Virtual Environments Reason and Brand (1975) traced the first written report diopulmonary function, phasic skin conductance, and of sea sickness to Hippocrates, 2400 years ago. They also peripheral blood flow) has proven helpful for under- noted that motion sickness occurs in many situations standing the physiology of motion sickness under labo- involving either passive body motion or active interac- ratory conditions but is insensitive, nonspecific, unreli- tion with the world via indirect sensorimotor interfaces able, and/or restrictive under operational conditions (e.g., prism spectacles). The best advice for avoiding (Cowings, Suter, Toscano, Kamiya, 8c Naifeh, 1986; motion sickness in such situations (without medication) Golding, 1992; Graybiel 8c Lackner, 1980; Lawson 8c is to refrain from moving your head, to keep your eyes Lackner, 1992; Stern, Koch, Leibowitz, Linblad, Shu- closed, and to move as economically as possible. This pert, 8c Stewart, 1985; Sunahara, Johnson, 8c Taylor, must sound hopelessly restrictive to anyone contemplat- 1964). In flight simulators (Kennedy, Lilienthal, Ber- ing virtual environment ( VE) systems in which users baum, Baltzley, 8c McCauley, 1989) and in our psycho- may play an active part, computers and specialized dis- physical experiments that involve real or apparent body plays are the user's interface to the world, and the experi- motion (e.g., Lackner 8c Texiera, 1977), it is uncom- enced world may purposely not correspond to the actual mon to observe severe symptoms—extreme nausea or world. As might be expected motion sickness is being vomiting, the skin appearing white as a sheet, and the reported in VEs that involve apparent self-motion clothes being soaked with perspiration.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Disorientation
    AOA and AOCOPM Aerospace Medicine Spatial Disorientation CAPT Kris Belland, MC, USN COMMANDER NAVAL AIR FORCES Force Surgeon Background 1980-90 USNA / PCOM / Gen Surgery NHO 1990-95 Flight Surgery / CVW-5 Japan / 14 Miramar CA 1995-96 Flight Training T-34 / 45, F/A-18 (Line and Staff Officer) 1,500 112 25 4.5yrs 3 Combat Cruises 1997-02 NSAWC / TOPGUN / USAF AWC Performance Maintenance, CBRNE, DEW, Childhood Cluster 2002-05 USS KITTY HAWK SMO (OIF) / THIRD Fleet Surgeon Senior Operational Screened 2009-13 COMNAVAIRFORCES Force Surgeon C4F NAVSOUTH Surgeon Operation Unified Response C7F LNO To CNFJ Operation Tomodachi Family Practice, Preventive Medicine – Aerospace Medicine MPH, MBA, MSS, CMBA, CPE, FAsMA 3 Terminal Objectives • Sensory System Objectives – Be familiar with the three primary sensory systems and how they interact to provide orientation information to the brain • Spatial Disorientation Objectives – Be familiar with spatial disorientation; the three types, prevention of and recovery from spatial disorientation. • Sensory Illusions Objectives – Be familiar with sensory illusions common to the aviation environment • PRESENTATION DOES NOT REPRESENT THE USN OR DOD BUT ARE THE OPINION OF THE AUTHOR Naval Aviation Mishap Rate 776 aircraft destroyed in 1954 Angled Carrier Decks 60 Naval Aviation Safety Center 50 NAMP est. 1959 22 - 1999 RAG concept initiated 31 - 2000 53 lives 40 NATOPS initiated 1961 Squadron Safety program 30 System Safety Designated Aircraft 20 ACT 10 HFC’s 0 Class A Hours Mishaps/100,000 Flight A Class 50 65 80 99 Fiscal Year Navy / Marine Corps Mishaps CY 1977-93 16 14 12 Human 10 8 6 4 Mechanical 2 0 1981 1977 1979 1983 1985 1987 1989 1991 1993 Class A, Hours FlightA, B,& C Mishaps/100,000 Class Year Aeromedical Causal Factors Reported in Mishaps & HAZREPS Spatial Disorientation Fatigue Meds/Illnesses Class A Loss of Consciousness Class B Visual Illusions Diet/Nutrition Class C Hypoxia Heat/Cold/Dehydration HAZREP Alcohol/Caffeine Barotrauma Other 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 CAPT L.
    [Show full text]
  • Interagency Aviation Accident Prevention Bulletin
    OAS-43A (4/18) Interagency Aviation Accident Prevention Bulletin No. IA APB 19-05 Date: September 18, 2019 Page 1 of 2 Subject: Spatial Disorientation, Vertigo, and Head Movement/Position Changes Area of Concern: Flight Safety Distribution: All Aviation Activities Discussion: A recent SAFECOM (#19-0520) describes a situation where a pilot began to feel ill while conducting heli-torch operations on a prescribed burn. The pilot described a vertigo sensation and feeling queasy while flying through smoke and opted to land and shutdown the helicopter. Once safely on the ground, the pilot felt the need to rest for about 20 minutes before continuing the mission. While the helicopter was shutdown, prescribed fire personnel were unable to make radio contact with the pilot. As a result, the Helicopter Manager requested that an Emergency Medical Technician (EMT) on the burn crew hike to the landing area to assess the pilot’s condition. After assessing the pilot, the decision was made to discontinue the heli-torch mission for the day and for the pilot be evaluated by a higher level of care. The pilot visited a physician the following morning and was cleared to return to work. It is not uncommon for those flying in fire operational environments to encounter these types of symptoms. Prior to feeling ill, the pilot had been operating in an area with visual horizons that varied in slope, elevation and angle, along with smoke laying over the ridgetops. The mission profile required constant change in airspeeds ranging from hovering to 50 knots, varying altitudes in order to cross ridgelines, and frequent turns due to the small size of the burn area.
    [Show full text]
  • A New Tool for Analyzing the Potential Influence of Vestibular Illusions By
    A New Tool for Analyzing the Potential Influence of Vestibular Illusions by Randall J. Mumaw1 (Associate Technical Fellow, Human Factors, Boeing), Eric Groen (Senior Scientist, TNO), Lars Fucke (Lead Engineer, Boeing), Richard Anderson (Senior Accident Investigator, Boeing), Jelte Bos (Senior Scientist, TNO; Professor, VU University) & Mark Houben (Research Scientist, TNO) Dr. Randy Mumaw, a cognitive scientist, has worked 15 years in aviation safety and accident investigation at Boeing. Recently, he has focused on loss of control events, especially those involving loss of situation awareness regarding airplane state. Dr. Eric Groen, aerospace physiologist, Prof. Dr. Jelte Bos, physicist, and Dr. Mark Houben, biomedical engineer, work as scientists at TNO and share research interest in the areas of pilot spatial disorientation, upset recovery, motion sickness, and flight simulator technology. Lars Fucke is an R&D Lead with Boeing (Madrid) working in the areas of system and flight deck operations safety. Richard Anderson has been a Boeing accident investigator since 1997. Role of Spatial Disorientation in Aviation Accidents In January of 2004, a 737-300 crashed minutes after take-off from Sharm el-Sheikh, Egypt. The departure was on a dark night, over the Red Sea, and there were few, if any, visible cultural landmarks that could be used to orient to the horizon. The Captain (the Pilot Flying, PF) had initiated a long left climbing turn, but partway through that turn the airplane had actually made a slow transition from a left bank to a right bank (20º and increasing slowly). The First Officer (FO) informed the Captain that they were turning right in this exchange: FO: Turning right, sir Captain: What? FO: Aircraft turning right Captain: Turning right? How turning right? At this point, the Captain was making control inputs on the wheel to roll further to the right, and continued doing so.
    [Show full text]
  • 6 Spatial Disorientation and Sensory Illusions Human
    human 6 Spatial Disorientation and performance Sensory Illusions and L 6.1 Introduction to Spatial imitations Disorientation The brain processes information from the (VFR) flights into instrument meteorological eyes, the vestibular system, the ears and conditions (IMC). These “VFR flight into the proprioceptors (sensory receptors in the IMC” accidents accounted for approximately muscles, tendons and joints) to determine 11% of the fatalities in that 8-year period. the direction of gravity (‘the vertical’) and 75% of all VFR flight into IMC accidents the position of the body in space. That is in that time period were fatal (Goh & what we call spatial orientation. Wiegmann, 2000). The most important information for The aviation accident records of other orientation must come from well-defined countries (e.g., United Kingdom and New external visual cues or visual references. If Zealand) also show similar trends, indicating these are not or not sufficiently present we that VFR flight into IMC is a major hazard in have a problem: we can become spatially general aviation. disorientated. Tests have proved that without adequate While on earth we may use our hands visual clues, directional control of an aircraft or ears to compensate for the loss of can be lost within 60 seconds during straight visual cues, the pilot can’t. In addition, and level flight and even quicker during a the vestibular and somatosensory senses turn. have become fully unreliable. Many fatal accidents have resulted and still result from The lessons learned from these accidents spatial disorientation in the air under bad and tests may already be clear: visual conditions.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Auditory Vection Speed and Directional Congruence on Perceptions of Visual Vection
    Old Dominion University ODU Digital Commons Psychology Theses & Dissertations Psychology Fall 2016 Effects of Auditory Vection Speed and Directional Congruence on Perceptions of Visual Vection Isabella Alexis Gagliano Old Dominion University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.odu.edu/psychology_etds Part of the Aerospace Engineering Commons, Biological Psychology Commons, and the Physiological Processes Commons Recommended Citation Gagliano, Isabella A.. "Effects of Auditory Vection Speed and Directional Congruence on Perceptions of Visual Vection" (2016). Master of Science (MS), Thesis, Psychology, Old Dominion University, DOI: 10.25777/rkb5-7c43 https://digitalcommons.odu.edu/psychology_etds/42 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Psychology at ODU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Psychology Theses & Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ODU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. EFFECTS OF AUDITORY VECTION SPEED AND DIRECTIONAL CONGRUENCE ON PERCEPTIONS OF VISUAL VECTION by Isabella Alexis Gagliano A.S. May 2011, Northern Virginia Community College B.S. May 2012, Old Dominion University A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of Old Dominion University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE PSYCHOLOGY OLD DOMINION UNIVERSITY DECEMBER 2016 Approved by: J. Christopher Brill (Director) Mark Scerbo (Member) James Paulson (Member) ABSTRACT EFFECTS OF AUDITORY VECTION SPEED AND DIRECTIONAL CONGRUENCE ON PERCEPTIONS OF VISUAL VECTION Isabella Alexis Gagliano Old Dominion University, 2016 Director: Dr. J. Christopher Brill Spatial disorientation is a major contributor to aircraft mishaps. One potential contributing factor is vection, an illusion of self-motion.
    [Show full text]
  • Spatial Disorientation in Flight a Handbook for Aircrew by A.J.Benson and E.Burchard Rs
    LNG P194518 N91G' AGARD-AG-170 o < • Q < < AGARDograph No. 170 on Spatial Disorientation in Flight A Handbook for Aircrew by A.J.Benson and E.Burchard rs NORTH ATLANTIC TREATY ORGANIZATION DISTRIBUTION AND AVAILABILITY ON BACK COVER AGARD-AG-170 NORTH ATLANTIC TREATY ORGANIZATION ADVISORY GROUP FOR AEROSPACE RESEARCH AND DEVELOPMENT (ORGANISATION DU TRA1TE DE L'ATLANTIQUE NORD) AGARDograph No. 170 SPATIAL DISORIENTATION IN FLIGHT A Handbook for Aircrew by A.J.Benson Head, Vestibular Physiology Royal Air Force Institute of Aviation Medicine Farnborough, Hants, UK and E.Burchard German Air Force, Medical Corps. 0001513 0 This AGARDograph was sponsored by the Aerospace Medical Panel of AGARD. THE MISSION OF AGARD The mission of AGARD is to bring together the leading personalities of the NATO nations in the fields of science and technology relating to aerospace for the following purposes: — Exchanging of scientific and technical information; — Continuously stimulating advances in the aerospace sciences relevant to strengthening the common defence posture; — Improving the co-operation among member nations in aerospace research and development; — Providing scientific and technical advice and assistance to the North Atlantic Military Committee in the field of aerospace research and development; — Rendering scientific and technical assistance, as requested, to other NATO bodies and to member nations in connection with research and development problems in the aerospace field; — Providing assistance to member nations for the purpose of increasing their scientific and technical potential; — Recommending effective ways for the member nations to use their research and development capabilities for the common benefit of the NATO community. The highest authority within AGARD is the National Delegates Board consisting of officially appointed senior representatives from each member nation.
    [Show full text]
  • Causes of Dizziness
    Causes of Dizziness SYMPTOMS By Vestibular Disorders Association, with edits by John King, PhD & Stacey Buckner, DPT Dizziness, vertigo, and disequilibrium are common symptoms reported by adults during visits to their doctors. They are all symptoms that can result DIZZINESS from a peripheral vestibular disorder (a dysfunction of the balance organs Dizziness, vertigo, of the inner ear) or central vestibular disorder (a dysfunction of one or and disequilibrium are more parts of the central nervous system that help process balance and common symptoms of spatial information). Although these three symptoms can be linked by vestibular disorders, which a common cause, they have different meanings, and describing them can arise from a number accurately can mean the difference between a successful diagnosis of causes. and one that is missed. Dizziness is a sensation of lightheadedness, faintness, or unstead-iness. Unlike dizziness, vertigo has a rotational, spinning component, and is the perception of movement, either of the self or surrounding objects. Disequilibrium simply means unsteadiness, imbalance, or loss of equilibrium that is often accompanied by spatial ARTICLE disorientation and dizziness, although it can occur independently. Almost everyone experiences a few seconds of spatial disorientation at some point– for example, when a person 011 watches a 3-D movie in the theater and momentarily perceives an illusion of moving or falling as the DID THIS ARTICLE images rush past. However, HELP YOU? frequent episodes of vertigo— SUPPORT VEDA @ whether lasting only for a few VESTIBULAR.ORG seconds or days on end are a primary sign of a vestibular dysfunction, especially when linked to changes in head and body position.
    [Show full text]