UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations UCLA UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Exempted: Spatial Clustering of Intentionally Unvaccinated Children in California and its Potential Consequences for Measles Transmission Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/3bt9k541 Author Gromis, Ashley Publication Date 2017 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Exempted: Spatial Clustering of Intentionally Unvaccinated Children in California and its Potential Consequences for Measles Transmission A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Sociology by Ashley Renee Gromis 2017 © Copyright by Ashley Renee Gromis 2017 ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION Exempted: Spatial Clustering of Intentionally Unvaccinated Children in California and its Potential Consequences for Measles Transmission by Ashley Renee Gromis Doctor of Philosophy in Sociology University of California, Los Angeles, 2017 Professor Ka Yuet Liu, Chair Rates of non-medical vaccine exemptions have been increasing in the U.S. since the early 1990s. These exemptions tend to be spatially clustered rather than randomly occurring in the population. In contrast to long-standing public health findings, under-vaccination due to non- medical exemptions tends to be found among children with educated, affluent, non-Hispanic white parents rather than those with relatively disadvantaged backgrounds. This dissertation uses data on Personal Beliefs Exemptions (PBEs) in schools in California from 1998-2014 and an empirically-calibrated large-scale simulation experiment to understand how spatial clusters of these exemptions form and their potential consequences for measles transmission. This dissertation emphasizes the importance of interaction in social networks in structuring both the local context surrounding parents‘ vaccine decisions and the patterns of ii physical contact through which disease spreads. Major findings show that residential sorting of parents into neighborhoods and schools based on socio-demographic characteristics associated with school-level PBE rates provides only a partial explanation for broader patterns of clustering. Additional analyses provide evidence of spillover effects between PBE rates and socio- demographic characteristics, particularly percent non-Hispanic white children, on PBE rates in nearby schools. A large-scale simulation of measles transmission using a synthetic population of youth in California in 2014 is then used to examine the potential consequences of PBEs for disease spread. Schools serve as hotspots for measles transmission regardless of the spatial locations of PBEs, although clustering of these exemptions within households and charter schools increase the opportunities for infections. Surprisingly, spatial pockets of PBEs may provide slight protection against measles transmission when outbreak sizes are small and the disease is introduced randomly into the population. Yet, clustering of exemptions can reduce protective effects of herd immunity to large measles epidemics, even when overall vaccination coverage in the population is high. This dissertation concludes by drawing connections between findings in the empirical chapters and linking the results to the larger theoretical context of social interaction in networks. Practical implications for vaccine policy and efforts to control vaccine-preventable disease are discussed. Overall, this project seeks to illustrate how decisions perceived as personal by parents contribute to collective health outcomes. iii The dissertation of Ashley Renee Gromis is approved. Gabriel Rossman William G. Roy Edward T. Walker Peter Bearman Ka Yuet Liu, Committee Chair University of California, Los Angeles 2017 iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Acknowledgments xii Vita xvi Chapter 1: Introduction 1 Research Questions 5 Personal Belief Exemptions in California 6 Outline of Chapters 12 Significance 14 Figures 15 Chapter 2: The Importance of Social Networks and Space in Vaccine Refusals Debates 17 Chapter Introduction 18 Evaluation of Risk 19 Trust in Healthcare Institutions 23 Homophily in Social Networks 26 (i) Selection 26 (ii) Confounding Effects 27 (iii) Social Influence 28 Social Ties 30 Biological Contagion 34 Disease Spread in Small-Worlds 36 v Robustness of Networks to Disease Spread 36 Social Behavior and Biological Contagion 38 Conclusion 40 Chapter 3: Clustering of Personal Belief Exemptions across Kindergartens in California, 1998-2014 43 Chapter Introduction 44 Personal Belief Exemptions 45 Costs and Benefits of Vaccination 47 Clustering of PBEs across Schools 53 Data and Methods 54 Data 55 Methods 57 Results 61 Discussion 69 Conclusion 71 Tables 72 Figures 76 Appendix 80 Chapter 4: The Structure of Spatial Dependence in Yearly Personal Beliefs Exemption Rates across Kindergartens in California, 1998-2014 88 Chapter Introduction 89 Spatial Dependence in PBE Rates 90 Spatial Heterogeneity 91 vi Unmeasured Covariates 92 Spatial Externalities 92 Spatial Diffusion 93 Data 96 Analytic Strategy 97 Spatial Neighbors 102 Results 104 Discussion 110 Conclusion 112 Figures 113 Appendix 120 Chapter 5: How Spatial Clustering of Non-Medical Vaccine Exemptions Affects Locations of Measles Transmission in Social Networks 125 Chapter Introduction 126 Social Contact and Disease Spread in Networks 127 Measles Transmission 129 Hypotheses 131 Synthetic Population 132 Simulation Model 135 Results 139 Schools and Childcare Centers 140 Households 143 Physicians 144 vii Neighborhoods and Malls 145 Discussion 148 Conclusion 151 Tables 152 Figures 153 Appendix 159 Chapter 6: Effects of Non-Medical Vaccine Exemptions on Overall Size of Measles Outbreak 161 Chapter Introduction 162 Spatial Clusters of PBEs as Disease Cores 164 Hypotheses 165 Data and Methods 167 Results 170 Discussion 176 Conclusion 178 Figures 179 Appendix 184 Chapter 7: Summary and Conclusion 186 Bibliography 195 viii LIST OF TABLES Table 3.1: Correlates of PBEs among public school kindergartens, 1998-2014 72 Table 3.2: Three-level model for correlates of PBEs among public school kindergartens, 2006-2014 73 Table 3.3: Effects of school type on PBEs among kindergartens, 1998-2014 74 Table 3.4: Effect of racial segregation on PBEs rates among public kindergartens 75 Table A3.1: Correlates of PBEs among public school kindergartens, 5 year lag, 1998-2014 80 Table A3.2: Correlates of PBEs among public school kindergartens, 1998-2010 81 Table A3.3: Correlates of logged PBE rates among public school kindergartens, 1998-2010 82 Table A3.4: Physician effects on PBEs, by physician type, 1998-2014 83 Table A3.5: Percent renters effects on PBE rates in public kindergartens, 1998-2014 84 Table A3.6: Effect of household income on PBE rates in public kindergartens, 1998-2014 85 Table A3.7: Correlates among public school kindergartens, additional local business variables, 1998-2014 86 Table A4.1: Yearly observations, by school type 120 Table A4.2: LaGrange Multiplier tests for spatial dependence, 1998-2014 121 Table 5.1: Descriptive Statistics of Synthetic Population 152 ix LIST OF FIGURES Figure 1.1: Statewide PBE rate in California kindergartens by school type, 1992-2014 15 Figure 1.2: County PBE rates in California kindergartens, 2014-2015 school year 16 Figure 3.1: PBE rates in California kindergartens, 1992-2014 76 Figure 3.2: Adjusted spatial clusters of PBEs across public schools, 2014 77 Figure 3.3: Effect of percent non-Hispanic white by school type 78 Figure 3.4: Effect of distance to nearest public school kindergarten in PBE cluster on PBE rates in childcare centers and private schools 79 Figure A3.1: Adjusted spatial clusters of PBEs by mother‘s education and percent non- Hispanic white, 1998-2013 87 Figure 4.1: Spatial concentration and dispersion of PBE rates in California kindergartens, 1998-2014 113 Figure 4.2: Diagrams of spatial relationships in autocorrelation among school PBE rates in spatial error, spatial lag, and spatial Durbin lag models 114 Figure 4.3: Local variation in covariate effects in GWR models, 1998-2014 115 Figure 4.4: Reduction in AIC (compared to OLS models) for spatial autocorrelation models 117 Figure 4.5: Direct and indirect effects of covariates 118 Figure 4.6: Effect of having neighboring schools with high PBE rates in the previous year 119 Figure A4.1: Reduction in AIC (compared to OLS) for spatial autocorrelation models, nearest 5 to 50 neighbors for all schools and schools of the same type 122 Figure A4.2: Effect of having neighboring schools with high PBE rates in the previous year, as measured by yearly standardized rates 123 Figure A4.3: Effect of neighboring schools with high PBE rates in the previous year, 1998-2014 124 Figure 5.1: Transmission settings for measles infections 153 x Figure 5.2: Ratio of percentage of infections to percentage of children enrolled in school and childcare centers 154 Figure 5.3: Distribution of outbreak sizes with and without shared vaccination status among siblings 155 Figure 5.4: Prominence of household transmission in reported measles outbreaks 156 Figure 5.5: Infections in children aged < 1 year 157 Figure 5.6: Average number of overall infections and percentage of infections acquired in malls and neighborhoods as proportion of contact increases 158 Figure A5.1: Transmission settings for measles infections, using 1 and 40 initial infections. 159 Figure A5.2: Transmission settings for measles infections, when PBEs are distributed as observed by siblings do not
Recommended publications
  • Investigating the Observability of Complex Contagion in Empirical Social Networks
    Investigating the Observability of Complex Contagion in Empirical Social Networks Clay Fink1, Aurora Schmidt1, Vladimir Barash2, John Kelly2, Christopher Cameron3, Michael Macy3 1Johns Hopkins University Applied Physics Laboratory 2Graphika, Inc. 3Cornell University NSF-IBSS, San Diego, CA, August 1-2, 2016 This work was funded by the Minerva Initiative through the United States Air Force Office of Scientific Research (AFOSR) under grant FA9550-15-1-0036. 1 / 15 Complex contagions and social movements Threshold-based, or complex, models of social contagion may partly explain the initiation of mass mobilizations and social movements 2 / 15 Prior work Threshold models of collective behavior and theoretical predictions (Granovetter 1978, 1973); (Centola, Macy 2007); (Barash, Cameron, Macy 2012) Observational Studies: focus on empirical adoption thresholds Coleman, et al. (1966); Valente (1996): empirical studies of social reinforcement for medical practices and diffusion of innovations; Romero, et al. (2011), Fink, et al. (2016): spread of hashtags on Twitter; State and Adamic (2015): adoption of Equal-Sign profile pictures on Facebook 3 / 15 Overestimation of adoption thresholds b c b c a a e d e d At time t none of a's neighbors By time t + dt all neighbors have adopted have adopted. If a now adopts, what was their actual adoption threshold? 4 / 15 This work We formulate comparable probabilistic models of simple and complex contagion to generate predictions of Twitter hashtag diffusion events Using the follow network of 53K Nigerian 2014 users
    [Show full text]
  • Mathematical Modeling of Complex Contagion on Clustered Networks
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Frontiers - Publisher Connector ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 15 September 2015 doi: 10.3389/fphy.2015.00071 Mathematical modeling of complex contagion on clustered networks David J. P. O’Sullivan *, Gary J. O’Keeffe, Peter G. Fennell and James P. Gleeson Mathematics Applications Consortium for Science and Industry, Department of Mathematics and Statistics, University of Limerick, Limerick, Ireland The spreading of behavior, such as the adoption of a new innovation, is influenced by the structure of social networks that interconnect the population. In the experiments of Centola [15], adoption of new behavior was shown to spread further and faster across clustered-lattice networks than across corresponding random networks. This implies that the “complex contagion” effects of social reinforcement are important in such diffusion, in contrast to “simple” contagion models of disease-spread which predict that epidemics would grow more efficiently on random networks than on clustered networks. To accurately model complex contagion on clustered networks remains a challenge because the usual assumptions (e.g., of mean-field theory) regarding tree-like networks are invalidated by the presence of triangles in the network; the triangles are, however, Edited by: crucial to the social reinforcement mechanism, which posits an increased probability of Javier Borge-Holthoefer, a person adopting behavior that has been adopted by two or more neighbors. In this Qatar Computing Research Institute, Qatar paper we modify the analytical approach that was introduced by Hébert-Dufresne et al. Reviewed by: [19], to study disease-spread on clustered networks.
    [Show full text]
  • Associative Diffusion and the Emergence of Cultural Variation
    Beyond “Social Contagion”: Associative Diffusion and the Emergence of Cultural Variation∗ Amir Goldberg Stanford University Sarah K. Stein Stanford University July 16, 2018 Abstract Network models of diffusion predominantly think about cultural variation as a prod- uct of social contagion. But culture does not spread like a virus. In this paper, we pro- pose an alternative explanation which we refer to as associative diffusion. Drawing on two insights from research in cognition—that meaning inheres in cognitive associations between concepts, and that such perceived associations constrain people’s actions— we propose a model wherein, rather than beliefs or behaviors per-se, the things being transmitted between individuals are perceptions about what beliefs or behaviors are compatible with one another. Conventional contagion models require an assumption of network segregation to explain cultural variation. In contrast, we demonstrate that the endogenous emergence of cultural differentiation can be entirely attributable to social cognition and does not necessitate a clustered social network or a preexisting division into groups. Moreover, we show that prevailing assumptions about the effects of network topology do not hold when diffusion is associative. ∗Direct all correspondence to Amir Goldberg: [email protected]; 650-725-7926. We thank Rembrand M. Koning, Daniel A. McFarland, Paul DiMaggio, Sarah Soule, John-Paul Ferguson, Jesper Sørensen, Glenn Carroll, Sameer B. Srivastava, Jerker Denrell, Robert Gibbons and participants of the MIT conference on Categories and Shared Mental Models for helpful comments and suggestions. The usual disclaimer applies. Introduction Contemporary societies exhibit remarkable and persistent cultural differences on issues as varied as musical taste and gun control.
    [Show full text]
  • Complex Contagion and the Weakness of Long Ties
    Complex Contagion and the Weakness of Long Ties Damon Centola and Michael Macy Cornell University September 12, 2005 Complex Contagion and the Weakness of Long Ties Abstract The strength of weak ties is that they tend to be long – they connect socially distant locations. Recent research on “small worlds” shows that remarkably few long ties are needed to give large and highly clustered populations the “degrees of separation” of a random network, in which information can rapidly diffuse. We test whether this effect of long ties generalizes from simple to complex contagions – those in which the credibility of information or the willingness to adopt an innovation requires independent confirmation from multiple sources. Using Watts and Strogatz’s original small world model, we demonstrate that long ties not only fail to speed up complex contagions, they can even preclude diffusion entirely. Results suggest that the spread of collective actions, social movements, and risky innovations benefit not from ties that are long but from bridges that are wide enough to transmit strong social reinforcement. Balance theory shows how wide bridges might also form in evolving networks, but this turns out to have surprisingly little effect on the propagation of complex contagions. We find that hybrid contagions, in which a critical mass of low-threshold nodes trigger the remaining high threshold nodes, can propagate on perturbed networks. However, of greater importance is the finding that wide bridges are a characteristic feature of spatial networks, which may account in part for the widely observed tendency for social movements to diffuse spatially. 2 “All politics is local.” – Rep.
    [Show full text]
  • The Complex Contagion of Doubt in the Anti-Vaccine Movement
    COMPLEX CONTAGION OF DOUBT THE COMPLEX CONTAGION OF DOUBT IN THE ANTI-VACCINE MOVEMENT Damon Centola, Ph.D. “FUD” is the fear, uncertainty, and doubt that IBM salespeople instill in the minds of potential customers who might be considering [competitors’] products…. The idea, of course, was to persuade buyers to go with safe IBM gear rather than competitors’ equipment. The Jargon File (cited in Raymond, 1991) INTRODUCTION Measles virus The measles virus is a “simple contagion” that is transmitted through contact between an infected person and a susceptible person. When someone who is newly infected becomes contagious, that person can transmit the disease to someone else who is susceptible, who in turn can transmit it to another, and so on. The result: One highly connected person can trigger an epidemic. Information can act like a simple contagion as well. If I tell you recent news about the availability of a new measles, mumps, and rubella (MMR) vaccine, you can easily repeat it to someone who can then repeat it to someone else. Each new contact and repetition leads to more transmission of the information. The result is the same: One highly connected person can accelerate word-of-mouth transmission of news, allowing it to spread “virally” across a community (Centola & Macy, 2007). But anti-vaccine sentiment is different. It is a “complex contagion.” Simply hearing a piece of anti-vaccine propaganda does not change a person’s beliefs. Rather, people need to be convinced—the hallmark of a complex contagion—through contact with several peers who can reinforce the legitimacy of a point of view.
    [Show full text]
  • Proximity, Interactions, and Communities in Social Networks: Properties and Applications
    PROXIMITY, INTERACTIONS, AND COMMUNITIES IN SOCIAL NETWORKS: PROPERTIES AND APPLICATIONS. By Tommy Nguyen A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Major Subject: COMPUTER SCIENCE Examining Committee: Boleslaw K. Szymanski, Thesis Adviser Sibel Adal´ı,Member James A. Hendler, Member Gyorgy Korniss, Member Mohammed J. Zaki, Member Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute Troy, New York October 2014 (For Graduation December 2014) c Copyright 2014 by Tommy Nguyen All Rights Reserved ii CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES . vi LIST OF FIGURES . vii ACKNOWLEDGMENT . ix ABSTRACT . .x 1. INTRODUCTION . .1 1.1 Ranking Information in Social Networks . .2 1.2 Small Worlds and Social Stratification . .4 1.3 Summary of Contributions & Organization . .6 1.3.1 Organization . .7 2. LITERATURE REVIEW . 10 2.1 Ranking Techniques . 10 2.1.1 Web Conceptualization . 10 2.1.2 User Data & Trust Models . 11 2.1.3 Learning to Rank . 13 2.2 Small-world Problem . 15 2.2.1 Six Degrees of Separation . 15 2.2.2 Social Stratification . 16 3. SOCIAL NETWORK ANALYSIS . 18 3.1 Geography, Co-Appearance, & Interactions . 19 3.1.1 Data Collection . 19 3.1.2 Notations & Definitions . 20 3.1.3 Data Analysis & Results . 21 3.1.4 Limitations . 24 3.2 Incorporating Geography into Community Detection . 24 3.2.1 Clique Percolation Method . 25 3.2.2 Modularity Maximization . 26 3.2.3 Speaker-Label Propagation (GANXiS) . 27 3.3 Contrasting Communities to Null Models . 28 3.3.1 Techniques for Generating Covers . 29 iii 3.3.2 Measuring Covers & Communities .
    [Show full text]
  • The Routing of Complex Contagion in Kleinberg's Small-World
    The Routing of Complex Contagion in Kleinberg's Small-World Networks? Wei Chen1, Qiang Li2, Xiaoming Sun2, and Jialin Zhang2 1 Microsoft Research 2 Institute of Computing Technology, Chinese Academy of Sciences Abstract. In Kleinberg's small-world network model, strong ties are modeled as deterministic edges in the underlying base grid and weak ties are modeled as random edges connecting remote nodes. The probability of connecting a node u with node v through a weak tie is proportional to 1=juvjα, where juvj is the grid distance between u and v and α ≥ 0 is the parameter of the model. Complex contagion refers to the propagation mechanism in a network where each node is activated only after k ≥ 2 neighbors of the node are activated. In this paper, we propose the concept of routing of complex contagion (or complex routing), where at each time step we can select one eligible node (nodes already having two active neighbors) to activate, with the goal of activating the pre-selected target node in the end. We consider decentralized routing scheme where only the links connected to already activated nodes are known to the selection strategy. We study the routing time of complex contagion and compare the result with simple routing and complex diffusion (the diffusion of complex contagion, where all eli- gible nodes are activated immediately in the same step with the goal of activating all nodes in the end). We show that for decentralized complex routing, the routing time is lower bounded by a polynomial in n (the number of nodes in the network) for all range of α both in expectation and with high probability (in 1 α particular, Ω(n α+2 ) for α ≤ 2 and Ω(n 2(α+2) ) for α > 2 in expectation).
    [Show full text]
  • IJAGR Editorial Board Editor-In-Chief: Donald Patrick Albert (Geo [email protected]), Sam Houston State U
    IJAGR Editorial Board Editor-in-Chief: Donald Patrick Albert ([email protected]), Sam Houston State U. USA Associate Editors: Jonathan Comer, Oklahoma State U., USA Thomas Crawford, East Carolina U., USA G. Rebecca Dobbs, Western Carolina U., USA Sonya Glavac, U. of New England, Australia Carol Hanchette, U. of Louisville, USA Tony Hernandez, Ryerson U., Canada Jay Lee, Kent State U., USA Shuaib Lwasa, Makerere U., Uganda John Strait, Sam Houston State U., USA David Wong, George Mason U., USA International Editorial Review Board: Bhuiyan M. Alam, The U. of Toledo, USA David Martin, U. of Southhampton, UK Badri Basnet, The U. of Southern Queensland, Australia Luke Marzen, Auburn U., USA Rick Bunch, U. of North Carolina - Greensboro, USA Adam Mathews, Texas State U., USA Ed Cloutis, U. of Winnipeg, Canada Darrel McDonald, Stephen F. Austin State U., USA Kelley Crews, U. of Texas at Austin, USA Ian Meiklejohn, Rhodes U., South Africa Michael DeMers, New Mexico State U., USA Joseph Messina, Michigan State U., USA Sagar Deshpande, Ferris State U., USA William A. Morris, McMaster U., Canada Steven Fleming, United States Military Academy, USA Petri Pellikka, U. of Helsinki, Finland Doug Gamble, U. of North Carolina - Wilmington, USA François Pinet, Cemagref - Clermont Ferrand, France Gang Gong, Sam Houston State U., USA Wei Song, U. of Louisville, USA Carlos Granell, European Commission, Italy Sermin Tagil, Balikesir U., Turkey William Graves, U. of North Carolina - Charlotte, USA Wei Tu, Georgia Southern U., USA Timothy Hawthorne, Georgia State U., USA Brad Watkins, U. of Central Oklahoma, USA Bin Jiang, U. of Gävle, Sweden Dion Wiseman, Brandon U., Canada C.
    [Show full text]
  • Gamification for Youth Engagement
    GLOBAL BROADBAND AND INNOVATIONS ALLIANCE WHAT’S NEXT September 2020 WHAT’S NEXT: Would You Like to Play a Game? By Seth Corrigan, Senior Director of Assessment and Game-Based Learning, Southern New Hampshire University INTRODUCTION This chapter is designed to inform USAID and USAID broadly relational aspects of learning and behavior implementing partners of promising developments in change. Second, role playing games – both digital and the use of games and gamification to support civic board-based role-playing games – are gaining engagement. As communities come to increasingly face recognition for their capacity to support effective volatile, uncertain, complex, and ambiguous decision- deliberation and planning among community members. making environments, games and gamified experiences Examples of games and gamification efforts are will have a growing role in informing and gathering input discussed throughout. Third, while gamification of from the public, provide means for engaging citizens e-participation is not new, current uses often leverage in fruitful deliberation, establishing supportive norms for the approach to engage community members in idea civic engagement, and forming social ties valuable for generation, sensing activities as well as city planning public participation. Youth engagement is emphasized and policymaking. Lastly, newsgames are also noted throughout. Also discussed is the current evidence base for their ability to support the critical understanding of for use of games and gamification to support public complex systems and events; increasingly the focus of understanding of complex systems and issues, civic policymaking in environments that are volatile, uncertain, engagement as well as key opportunities and barriers complex, and ambiguous. Themes associated with civic to their use for that same purpose.
    [Show full text]
  • “Social Contagion”: Associative Diffusion and the Emergence Of
    Beyond “Social Contagion”: Associative Diffusion and the Emergence of Cultural Variation∗ Amir Goldberg Stanford University Sarah K. Stein Stanford University March 30, 2018 Abstract Network models of diffusion predominantly think about cultural variation as a prod- uct of social contagion. But culture does not spread like a virus. In this paper, we pro- pose an alternative explanation which we refer to as associative diffusion. Drawing on two insights from research in cognition—that meaning inheres in cognitive associations between concepts, and that such perceived associations constrain people’s actions— we propose a model wherein, rather than beliefs or behaviors per-se, the things being transmitted between individuals are perceptions about what beliefs or behaviors are compatible with one another. Conventional contagion models require an assumption of network segregation to explain cultural variation. In contrast, we demonstrate that the endogenous emergence of cultural differentiation can be entirely attributable to social cognition and does not necessitate a clustered social network or a preexisting division into groups. Moreover, we show that prevailing assumptions about the effects of network topology do not hold when diffusion is associative. ∗Direct all correspondence to Amir Goldberg: [email protected]; 650-725-7926. We thank Rembrand M. Koning, Daniel A. McFarland, Paul DiMaggio, Sarah Soule, John-Paul Ferguson, Jesper Sørensen, Glenn Carroll and Sameer B. Srivastava for helpful comments and suggestions. The usual disclaimer applies. Introduction Contemporary societies exhibit remarkable and persistent cultural differences on issues as varied as musical taste and gun control. This cultural variation has been a longstanding topic of sociological inquiry because it is central to how social order, and the inequalities it is founded on, is maintained (Lamont, Molnár, and Virág 2002; Bourdieu 1986; Gold- berg, Hannan, and Kovács 2016).
    [Show full text]
  • Complex Contagions in Kleinberg's Small World Model
    Complex Contagions in Kleinberg’s Small World Model Roozbeh Ebrahimi∗ Jie Gao Golnaz Ghasemiesfeh Stony Brook University Stony Brook University Stony Brook University Stony Brook, NY 11794 Stony Brook, NY 11794 Stony Brook, NY 11794 [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Grant Schoenebecky University of Michigan Ann Arbor, MI 48109 [email protected] ABSTRACT Categories and Subject Descriptors Complex contagions describe diffusion of behaviors in a social net- J.4 [Social and Behavioral Sciences]: Sociology—social networks, work in settings where spreading requires influence by two or more social contagion; G.2.2 [Discrete Mathematics]: Graph Theory— neighbors. In a k-complex contagion, a cluster of nodes are ini- random network models tially infected, and additional nodes become infected in the next round if they have at least k already infected neighbors. It has been Keywords argued that complex contagions better model behavioral changes Social Networks; Complex Contagion; Kleinberg’s Small World such as adoption of new beliefs, fashion trends or expensive tech- Model nology innovations. This has motivated rigorous understanding of spreading of complex contagions in social networks. Despite sim- ple contagions (k = 1) that spread fast in all small world graphs, 1. INTRODUCTION how complex contagions spread is much less understood. Previ- Social acts are influenced by the behavior of others while at ous work [11] analyzes complex contagions in Kleinberg’s small same time influencing them. New social behaviors may emerge world model [14] where edges are randomly added according to a and spread in a social network like a contagion.
    [Show full text]
  • False Beliefs: Byproducts of an Adaptive Knowledge Base? 131 Elizabeth J
    “This volume provides a great entry point into the vast and growing psycho- logical literature on one of the defining problems of the early 21st century – fake news and its dissemination. The chapters by leading scientists first focus on how (false) information spreads online and then examine the cognitive processes involved in accepting and sharing (false) information. The volume concludes by reviewing some of the available countermeasures. Anyone new to this area will find much here to satisfy their curiosity.” – Stephan Lewandowsky , Cognitive Science, University of Bristol, UK “Fake news is a serious problem for politics, for science, for journalism, for con- sumers, and, really, for all of us. We now live in a world where fact and fiction are intentionally blurred by people who hope to deceive us. In this tremendous collection, four scientists have gathered together some of the finest minds to help us understand the problem, and to guide our thinking about what can be done about it. The Psychology of Fake News is an important and inspirational contribu- tion to one of society’s most vexing problem.” – Elizabeth F Loftus , Distinguished Professor, University of California, Irvine, USA “This is an interesting, innovative and important book on a very significant social issue. Fake news has been the focus of intense public debate in recent years, but a proper scientific analysis of this phenomenon has been sorely lacking. Con- tributors to this excellent volume are world-class researchers who offer a detailed analysis of the psychological processes involved in the production, dissemina- tion, interpretation, sharing, and acceptance of fake news.
    [Show full text]