Color, Light and Metamerism

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Color, Light and Metamerism Color, Light and Metamerism 1.1 Welcome Notes: Welcome to Color, Light and Metamerism, presented by The Sherwin-Williams Company. Thank you for your interest as we share important properties that affect the built environment in an array of many ways. 1.2 IDCEC Best Practices Notes: NO AUDIO RECORDED - CLICK NEXT TO CONTINUE This CEU is registered with the Interior Design Continuing Education Council (IDCEC) for continuing education credits. This credit will be accepted by the American Society of Interior Designers (ASID), International Interior Designers Association (IIDA) and Interior Designers of Canada (IDC). The content included is not deemed or construed to be an approval or endorsement by IDCEC of any material or construction or any method or manner of handling, using, distributing or dealing in any material or product. Questions related to specific materials, methods and services should be directed to the instructor or provider of this CEU. This program is registered for 0.1 CEU/HSW value. The IDCEC class-code is: CEU-103657-R1 This CEU will be reported on your behalf to IDCEC and you will receive an email notification. Please log in and complete the electronic survey for this CEU. Certificates of completion will be automatically issued once you have submitted the online survey for this CEU. Attendees who do not belong to ASID, IIDA or IDC and do not have a unique IDCEC number will be provided with a Certificate of Completion after this CEU. 1.3 Copyright Notes: This presentation is protected by U.S. and International copyright laws. Reproduction, distribution, display and use of the presentation without written permission of Sherwin-Williams is prohibited. 1.4 Course Description Notes: This course will describe in detail the phenomenon known as Metamerism. Principles of color and light will be explored to give the participant a greater awareness of these elements, as well as their effect on each other in the built environment. At the conclusion of the course, the participant shall be able to apply these concepts to his or her design. 1.5 Learning Objectives Notes: By the end of this course, you should be able to: Examine the properties of color, how we see color and its relationship to the inhabitant of the space. Gain knowledge and insight into the study of Metamerism. Develop an understanding between the relationship of color and light in a space. Gain knowledge for the use of color in projects. 1.6 Color Notes: Mr. Demetrious Smith, Architect, is flying north to Cleveland, Ohio for a business trip. He resides in a warm, sunny part of the southeast United States. Looking at the upcoming weather report for Cleveland, which includes colder temperatures and snow, he realizes he needs to purchase a heavier suit for the trip. At a nearby department store, he finds an ideal ensemble: dark brown pants, with a dark brown sport coat and a pumpkin-colored dress shirt. Best of all, it was all on sale. Mr. Smith has a short-notice, but very important out-of-town presentation to a new potential client the next afternoon and desires to make an outstanding first impression. As he is getting ready the next morning in Cleveland, he notices in the mirror something peculiar… The pants and shirt look great, but the jacket purchased is not brown, it is, in fact - purple! 1.7 Color Notes: Prismatic Design just completed the construction of a boutique hotel. The finishes, including stunning damask lobby wall-covering and charcoal-colored ceramic tile, have all been installed. The interior designer, Ms. Charlotte Carr, is on her way to the jobsite for a meeting the furniture installer, who has begun the process of placing furniture throughout the space. Upon walking into the lobby, Charlotte views something very out-of-place. The steel-colored fabric on the sectional seating, which looked gorgeous at the showroom, appears to have a yellowish almost dirty cast, not at all the desired look with the red and gray space. 1.8 Color Notes: The shift in color described in each scenario is referred to as Metamerism. It is the phenomenon that occurs when color changes under different light sources. Today, we will explore the relationship between Color, Light and Metamerism. 1.9 Study of Color Notes: Color is as individual as the person. Color is a freedom of expression. As we begin, what comes to mind when you think of color? The study, or theory, of Color has intrigued humankind for a long time. 1.10 Color History Notes: Aristotle developed the first known theory of color during Ancient Greek times: “He postulated that God sent down color from the heavens as celestial waves.” He defined four colors taken from nature’s four elements: earth, fire, wind and water. Source: Color Theory, Causes of Color, October 28, 2009. http://www. webexhibits.org/causesofcolor/1B.html 1.11 Color History Notes: Leonardo da Vinci was the first to suggest an alternative in color theory, adding white and black to the four traditional colors: Yellow (earth), Red (fire), Blue (wind) and Green (water). He correctly identified white as the presence of all color, and black as the absence of it. 1.12 Color History Notes: The first detailed understanding of the science of color began in 1666 when Sir Isaac Newton identified colors as a spectrum produced by a glass prism. Using two prisms, he refracted white light and defined seven colors inside it of red, orange, yellow, green, cyan, ultramarine, and violet. 1.13 Disciplines of Color Notes: Today, several different disciplines and fields share in the study of colors that include: Natural Science, Theology, Psychology, Philosophy, Biology, Medicine, Engineering, and Art. Architects and designers use this research to apply color to the built environment. “Seldom, surely, is the psychological part of an appearance in nature so great as it is in the case of color.” It is virtually impossible to be exposed to color and not respond. 1.14 What is Color Notes: Color is, “the sensation caused by certain qualities of light that the eye recognizes and the brain interprets.” It is a wavelength of light that an object produces and the eye sees. 1.15 Primary Colors Notes: Spectral light is comprised of seven colors: red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo and violet. The color wheel is a circular demonstration of spectral light created to better understand the relationship between colors. Red, yellow and blue are the primary colors of the spectrum. These are pure colors that cannot be made from other colors, and they serve as the basis for all colors. Notice the way the colors, red, yellow and blue, spiral through seven colors each, creating secondary and tertiary colors. The secondary colors orange, green and purple are positioned halfway between the primary colors. The tertiary colors fill in the gaps to complete the color wheel. 1.16 Attributes of Color Notes: Color itself is the result of different wavelengths of light that stimulate certain parts of the brain. The primary colors of light are red, green and blue. When added together, these colors produce a white light, which is known as additive color mixing. 1.17 Complementary Colors Notes: The same white light is produced when a primary color of light is mixed with its true secondary complement. The complementary colors of light are cyan and red, magenta and green and yellow and blue. In this course, we will further explore light and examine the human eye and how it sees color. 1.18 The Eye and Color Notes: The eye sees color through the retina, which is the inner surface of the eye. The retina is an intricate network of receptor cells and neurons where light is converted into neutral impulses that are forwarded to the brain. There are two types of receptors in the retina: rods and cones. Rods react to brightness, but not color. Cones see color and detailed vision. There are about twelve million cones total in both eyes, and there are three types of cones: red, green and blue. Theses cones contribute to a sensation that your brain recognizes as a color. The three cones can amazingly see about 7 million colors. 1.19 The Eye and Color Notes: The retina views objects as opposite-down or mirrored. It is the brain that reverses the phenomenon, so we can view objects in their proper orientation. Additionally, the brain takes the two images seen by two eyes and combines them into one singular picture. Finally, the brain pieces missing information and makes corrections for an eye that may be problematic. This process, from the retina registering light wavelengths and transmitting signals to the brain, takes a mere 1/25th of a second. Interestingly, 20% of the light signals produced by this electro-chemical process will not even reach the brain. Instead, they will go to the pituitary gland, the master endocrine gland, to assist with its processes in the human body. 1.20 Color Blindness Notes: Color blindness is an inherited condition linked to the X chromosome. As you know, men have only one X chromosome and women have two. For a woman to be considered colorblind, both X chromosomes would be affected. This is a rare occurrence and explains why color blindness in varying degrees is more prevalent in men at 8% of the population [compared] to .4% of the population of women. It is thought that color blindness is due to the lack or reduced number of a certain type of cone. 1.21 Color Wavelengths Notes: How fast do you drive to work? Or, maybe a better question, how fast do you drive on the way home? Electromagnetic waves travel through space at 186,282 miles- per second (299,792 km/sec).
Recommended publications
  • Multispectral Image Reproduction Via Color Appearance Mapping
    International Journal of Signal Processing, Image Processing and Pattern Recognition Vol.7, No.4 (2014), pp.65-72 http://dx.doi.org/10.14257/ijsip.2014.7.4.06 Multispectral Image Reproduction via Color Appearance Mapping Ying Wang, Sheping Zhai and Zhongmin Wang School of Computer Science & Technology, Xi’an Univ. of Posts & Telecommunications, Xi’an China [email protected] Abstract To achieve the color consistent reproduction of multispectral images in the different viewing condition, a new method of multispectral image reproduction via color appearance mapping was proposed. Firstly, through the introduction of color appearance transformation, the color appearance description of the source spectral reflectance in the source viewing condition was obtained. Then by the construction of the inverse model, the high dimension spectra in the destination viewing condition were evaluated, which was color appearance matching but spectral mismatching with the source image. Finally, to improve the spectral precision of the reproduced spectra, the evaluated spectra were corrected by the method of metamerism correction based on the source spectra, and then the reproduced spectral image was obtained, which matched the source image in color appearance and in spectra when the reproduction viewing condition was different from the source. Experiments show that the perceptual color difference and the spectral error between the reproduced multi-spectral image and its original in the different viewing condition are small. The new method preserves the spectral information of the source multispectral image and achieves equal perceptual reproduction to the source image. Keywords: multispectral image reproduction, color appearance mapping, spectral evaluation, metamerism correction, viewing condition independent space 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Tetrachromatic Metamerism a Discrete, Mathematical Characterization
    Tetrachromatic Metamerism A Discrete, Mathematical Characterization Alfredo Restrepo Palacios Laboratorio de Senales,˜ Dept. Ing. Electrica´ y Electronica,´ Universidad de los Andes, Carrera 1 No. 18A-70; of. ML-427, Bogota´ 111711, Colombia Keywords: Metamerism, Tetrachromacy. Abstract: Two light beams that are seen as of having the same colour but that have different spectra are said to be metameric. The colour of a light beam is based on the reading of severel photodetectors with different spectral responses and metamerism results when a set of photodetectors is unable to resolve two spectra. The spectra are then said to be metameric. We are interested in exploring the concept of metamerism in the tetrachromatic case. Applications are in computer vision, computational photography and satellite imaginery, for example. 1 INTRODUCTION here, R denotes the set of the real numbers. Thus, in going from s(l) to c = [cw;cx;cy;cz], you Two light spectra are said to be metameric when the take four aperture samples of s and metamerism re- corresponding lights look of the same colour. For ex- sults when the photodetectors are unable to resolve ample, a spectral (i.e. of energy at a unique wave- two spectra. This is unavoidable if you consider that length) yellow light beam and an appropriate com- a set of four photoreceptors linearly2 maps the graph bination of spectral beam lights, green and red. We curve of each spectrum function s : [lmin;lmax] ! stress the point that it is pairs of spectra, and not pairs [0;¥) into a point on the ”16-tant3” R 4+ that we de- 4 of colors, that are metameric.
    [Show full text]
  • Observer Metamerism
    Observer Metamerism Toronto’s Graphic Arts Day Chris Bai Vice Chair, ICC Display Workgroup Senior Color Scientist, BenQ Corporation 2017/10/13 Overview • What is Observer Metamerism? • How about in Digital World? • Some More Research… • How to Tackle the Problem? • Result • Conclusion What is Observer Metamerism? • The phenomenon by which two materials that match under one circumstance appear different to different observers. Example - Painting How about in Digital World? • When BenQ 1st LED backlight color management monitor was announced in 2013. • Very exciting news! • Experienced users found out LED backlight monitor did not performed well in soft proofing scenario. ?? ? Experienced Observer Symptoms • Variations in blue, green and pale yellow tones, for example. • Difference in perceived saturation. Slight hue Overall shift in blue saturation is different Slight hue shift in green Slight hue shift in pale yellow Viewing Booth LED Backlight Monitor (Simulated) (Simulated) What was Wrong? * • ∆E00 is more or less the same… Tested with IT8.7/4 1617 Patches: • LED Monitor: * •Avg. ∆E00 = 1.25 * •Max. ∆E00 = 4.35 • CCFL Monitor: * •Avg. ∆E00 = 1.20 * •Max. ∆E00 = 4.25 Not much difference from the values alone. Some More Research… • CIE Report* suggested: “A potential practical solution is to implement an observer- dependent color imaging workflow at the device level. … Conceptually this is similar to the device-dependent color imaging, a well-established color management concept. “ • But no spectral color management workflow was established
    [Show full text]
  • Sensor Interpixel Correlation Analysis and Reduction for Color Filter Array High Dynamic Range Image Reconstruction
    Sensor interpixel correlation analysis and reduction for color filter array high dynamic range image reconstruction Mikael Lindstrand The self-archived postprint version of this journal article is available at Linköping University Institutional Repository (DiVA): http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-154156 N.B.: When citing this work, cite the original publication. Lindstrand, M., (2019), Sensor interpixel correlation analysis and reduction for color filter array high dynamic range image reconstruction, Color Research and Application, , 1-13. https://doi.org/10.1002/col.22343 Original publication available at: https://doi.org/10.1002/col.22343 Copyright: This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non- commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made. © 2019 The Authors. Color Research & Application published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. http://eu.wiley.com/WileyCDA/ Received: 10 April 2018 Revised: 3 December 2018 Accepted: 4 December 2018 DOI: 10.1002/col.22343 RESEARCH ARTICLE Sensor interpixel correlation analysis and reduction for color filter array high dynamic range image reconstruction Mikael Lindstrand1,2 1gonioLabs AB, Stockholm, Sweden Abstract 2Image Reproduction and Graphics Design, Campus Norrköping, ITN, Linköping University, High dynamic range imaging (HDRI) by bracketing of low dynamic range (LDR) Linköping, Sweden images is demanding, as the sensor is deliberately operated at saturation. This exac- Correspondence erbates any crosstalk, interpixel capacitance, blooming and smear, all causing inter- gonioLabs AB, Stockholm, Sweden. pixel correlations (IC) and a deteriorated modulation transfer function (MTF).
    [Show full text]
  • Mean Observer Metamerism and the Selection of Display Primaries
    Mean Observer Metamerism and the Selection of Display Primaries Mark D. Fairchild & David R. Wyble RIT Munsell Color Science Laboratory A Conclusion... “There is another reason why it may be desirable to use desaturated primaries in a television receiver. It has been found in direct colorimetry that observer differences can be minimized by making the color triangle of the primaries no larger than is necessary to include the variation of chromaticities to be measured.” Wintringham, 1954 Observer Metamerism A metameric match for one observer is likely to mismatch for another. Terms Mean Observer Metamerism ≠ Mean-Observer Metamerism CIE TC1-36 • CIE 170-1:2006 • Cone Fundamentals (CMFs) • Function of Field Size • Function of Age • Mean Functions CIE 2006 Model Cone Cone Fundamentals Ocular Media Absorptivity Density Spectra f(age) f(field size) [−Dτ ,max,macula ⋅Dmacula,relative (λ )−Dτ ,ocul (λ )] l (λ) = α i,l (λ)⋅10 [−Dτ ,max,macula ⋅Dmacula,relative (λ )−Dτ ,ocul (λ )] m (λ) = αi,m (λ)⋅10 [−Dτ ,max,macula ⋅Dmacula,relative (λ )−Dτ ,ocul (λ )] s (λ) = αi,s(λ)⋅10 Macular Density f(field size) Notation • CIE2006(field size, age) • e.g. CIE2006(2,32) • 2-Degree Field, 32 Years Old Examples: Field Size L-, M-, & S-Cone Fundamentals (2- & 10-Deg. @ Age 32) 1.00 l-bar (2) 0.90 m-bar (2) s-bar (2) 0.80 l-bar (10) m-bar (10) 0.70 s-bar (10) 0.60 0.50 0.40 Relative Sensitivity Relative 0.30 0.20 0.10 0.00 390 440 490 540 590 640 690 740 Wavelength (nm) Examples: Age L-, M-, & S-Cone Fundamentals (10-Deg.
    [Show full text]
  • Color Appearance Models Second Edition
    Color Appearance Models Second Edition Mark D. Fairchild Munsell Color Science Laboratory Rochester Institute of Technology, USA Color Appearance Models Wiley–IS&T Series in Imaging Science and Technology Series Editor: Michael A. Kriss Formerly of the Eastman Kodak Research Laboratories and the University of Rochester The Reproduction of Colour (6th Edition) R. W. G. Hunt Color Appearance Models (2nd Edition) Mark D. Fairchild Published in Association with the Society for Imaging Science and Technology Color Appearance Models Second Edition Mark D. Fairchild Munsell Color Science Laboratory Rochester Institute of Technology, USA Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons Ltd, The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex PO19 8SQ, England Telephone (+44) 1243 779777 This book was previously publisher by Pearson Education, Inc Email (for orders and customer service enquiries): [email protected] Visit our Home Page on www.wileyeurope.com or www.wiley.com All Rights Reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning or otherwise, except under the terms of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988 or under the terms of a licence issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency Ltd, 90 Tottenham Court Road, London W1T 4LP, UK, without the permission in writing of the Publisher. Requests to the Publisher should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons Ltd, The Atrium, Southern Gate, Chichester, West Sussex PO19 8SQ, England, or emailed to [email protected], or faxed to (+44) 1243 770571. This publication is designed to offer Authors the opportunity to publish accurate and authoritative information in regard to the subject matter covered.
    [Show full text]
  • PRECISE COLOR COMMUNICATION COLOR CONTROL from PERCEPTION to INSTRUMENTATION Knowing Color
    PRECISE COLOR COMMUNICATION COLOR CONTROL FROM PERCEPTION TO INSTRUMENTATION Knowing color. Knowing by color. In any environment, color attracts attention. An infinite number of colors surround us in our everyday lives. We all take color pretty much for granted, but it has a wide range of roles in our daily lives: not only does it influence our tastes in food and other purchases, the color of a person’s face can also tell us about that person’s health. Even though colors affect us so much and their importance continues to grow, our knowledge of color and its control is often insufficient, leading to a variety of problems in deciding product color or in business transactions involving color. Since judgement is often performed according to a person’s impression or experience, it is impossible for everyone to visually control color accurately using common, uniform standards. Is there a way in which we can express a given color* accurately, describe that color to another person, and have that person correctly reproduce the color we perceive? How can color communication between all fields of industry and study be performed smoothly? Clearly, we need more information and knowledge about color. *In this booklet, color will be used as referring to the color of an object. Contents PART I Why does an apple look red? ········································································································4 Human beings can perceive specific wavelengths as colors. ························································6 What color is this apple ? ··············································································································8 Two red balls. How would you describe the differences between their colors to someone? ·······0 Hue. Lightness. Saturation. The world of color is a mixture of these three attributes.
    [Show full text]
  • X3 Sensor Characteristics
    57 J. Soc. Photogr. Sci. Technol. Japan. (2003) Vol. 66 No. 1: 57-60 Special Topic: Leading Edge of Digital Image Systems 2003 Exposition X3 Sensor Characteristics Allen RUSH* and Paul HUBEL* Abstract The X3 sensor technology has been introduced as the first semiconductor image sensor technology to measure and report 3 dis- tinct colors per pixel location. This technology provides a key solution to the challenge of capturing full color images using semi- conductor sensor technology, usually CCD's, with increasing use of CMOS. X3 sensors capture light by using three detectors embedded in silicon. By selecting the depths of the detectors, three separate color bands can be captured and subsequently read and reported as color values from the sensor. This is accomplished by taking advantage of a natural characteristic of silicon, whereby light is absorbed in silicon at depths depending on the wavelength (color) of the light. Key words: CMOS image sensor, Bayer pattern, photodiode, spatial sampling, color reconstruction, color error 1. X3 Sensor Operation ing has a beneficial perceptual effect. The net result is a series of cause-effect-remedy that adds considerable complexity and The X3 sensor is similar in design to other CMOS sensors-a cost to the capture system, while the best quality remains un- photodiode is used to collect electrons as a result of light energy achievable. entering the aperture. The charge from the photodiode is convert- By contrast, the X3 sensor reports three color values for each ed and amplified by a source follower, and subsequently read out pixel location or spatial sampling site.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is Metamerism?
    TECHNICAL BULLETIN What is Metamerism? What is Metamerism? pigment stronger, giving more of a red “color” to the red/ Metamerism is a visual phenomenon that is most often yellow mix paint. Conversely, viewing the combination in seen between two colors made with different pigments. more of a blue/yellow light will bring out the yellow in These colors appear to match under one lighting one sample, making it look less red than in another. condition (i.e. daylight) but not another lighting Metamerism can be measured with a spectrophotometer condition (i.e. shop LED lighting). and given a “DIN 6172 metamerism” reading between two samples to measure how two color samples will vary in The Science Behind Metamerism different light sources. This visual phenomenon is caused by the light source reflecting off an object. Without light, there is no color. How to Minimize Metamerism Different light sources like sunlight or fluorescent lighting The best way to eliminate or minimize metamerism is to have different amounts of red, yellow, or blue shade light use the same pigmentation within all color samples. to reflect off an object. Differing pigments can reflect Ensuring that there is no variation between pigments these amounts of light back to us as “color.” For example, used in the color samples is crucial for matching the in Figure 1 an orange can be made with orange and white coatings in multiple light sources. It is also helpful to pigments, or it can be made with yellow and red. Any establish the conditions of color evaluation as well as a light source that has a significant amount of red available, range of tolerances within the color.
    [Show full text]
  • RPI LRC Capturing the Lighting Edge New Color Metrics Mark Fairchild
    Color Appearance of Displays, etc. RGC scheduled September 28, 2012 from 6:45 AM to 9:45 AM RPI LRC Capturing the Lighting Edge New Color Metrics Oct. 3, 2012 Mark Fairchild Rochester Institute of Technology, College of Science 1 Some Adaptation Demos 2 Color 3 4 5 6 Blur (Sharpness) 7 8 9 10 Noise 11 12 13 14 Chromatic, Blur, and Noise Adaptation 15 Outline •Color Appearance Phenomena •Chromatic Adaptation •Metamerism •Color Appearance Models •HDR 16 Color Appearance Phenomena 17 Color Appearance Phenomena If two stimuli do not match in color appearance when (XYZ)1 = (XYZ)2, then some aspect of the viewing conditions differs. Various color-appearance phenomena describe relationships between changes in viewing conditions and changes in appearance. Bezold-Brücke Hue Shift Abney Effect Helmholtz-Kohlrausch Effect Hunt Effect Simultaneous Contrast Crispening Helson-Judd Effect Stevens Effect Bartleson-Breneman Equations Chromatic Adaptation Color Constancy Memory Color Object Recognition 18 Simultaneous Contrast The background in which a stimulus is presented influences the apparent color of the stimulus. Stimulus Indicates lateral interactions and adaptation. Stimulus Color- Background Background Change Appearance Change Darker Lighter Lighter Darker Red Green Green Red Yellow Blue Blue Yellow 19 Simultaneous Contrast Example (a) (b) 20 Josef Albers 21 Complex Spatial Interactions 22 Hunt Effect Corresponding chromaticities across indicated relative changes in luminance (Hypothetical Data) For a constant chromaticity, perceived 0.6 colorfulness increases with luminance. 0.5 As luminance increases, stimuli of lower colorimetric purity are required to match 1 10 a given reference stimulus. y 0.4 100 1000 10000 10000 1000 100 10 1 0.3 Indicates nonlinearities in visual processing.
    [Show full text]
  • Individual Differences in Color Matching and Adaptation: Theory and Practice
    Individual Differences in Color Matching and Adaptation: Theory and Practice Mark D. Fairchild, Program of Color Science / Munsell Color Science Laboratory, Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY USA Abstract their own color matching functions and in allowing the prediction Individual differences in color matching functions are well of the spread of observer matches for metameric stimuli. However, known and have recently been well modeled and quantified. The the availability of individual color matching functions leads one to phenomenon even carries a unique name, observer metamerism. pose the next-level question: should chromatic adaptation However, to date, no research has explored the effects of observer transforms (CATs) also be tailored to individuals? Such tailoring metamerism (or other individual differences in physiological would include optimizing the adaptation transform matrix to mechanisms) on chromatic adaptation and color appearance. This perform von Kries scaling on the individual’s cone fundamentals paper presents a computational study of the effects of observer as a minimum. At the more extreme end one might consider the metamerism on predicted corresponding colors, the result of need for individual chromatic adaptation transforms that are more chromatic adaptation. The ranges of predicted corresponding complicated than von Kries scaling, or even that differ across colors are computed, analyzed and explored. The differences in individuals. The computational exercise described in this paper predicted chromatic adaptation (using a von Kries model) are very begins to explore these issues. significant and could have practical importance. Additionally, a computation of the required precision in psychophysical Theory of the Appearance Problem experiments on chromatic adaptation indicates that the precision The theory of the observer metamerism and individual CATs required to adequately model individual differences (well less than problem is illustrated conceptually in Fig.
    [Show full text]
  • COLOUR ACCURACY WHEN TINTING DIFFERENT PAINT TYPES PROPRIETY COLOUR SYSTEMS Major Paint Manufacturers and Suppliers Market Their Own Standard Colour Range
    COLOUR ACCURACY WHEN TINTING DIFFERENT PAINT TYPES PROPRIETY COLOUR SYSTEMS Major paint manufacturers and suppliers market their own standard colour range. They present their colour range in the form of various colour charts, fan decks, folders or in-store colour chip displays. Each standard colour is formulated for specific products, tinted according to a unique tint formula. For example, Dulux has the “Dulux Colour Specifier,” which is quite specific to the Dulux Premium decorative brands such as Wash & Wear 101 (gloss, semi gloss low sheen and flat), Aquanamel (gloss and semi gloss) and Weathershield (gloss, semi gloss and low sheen). All these products are “tint strength aligned” which means that the colour will be a Class 1 match if tinted accurately according to the Colour Specifier Formula Book. The Colour Specifier formulae only refer to the Decorama decorative paint tint system, and only for Dulux Premium decorative brands. There are also standard colour systems, such as British Standard BS 4800 Colours, British Standard BS381C Colours, the European RAL Colours, and the Standards Australia AS 2700 colours. The AS2700 colour range consists of around 200 standard colours designed for Specifiers to select colours for pipeline identification, line marking, safety demarcation and other engineering purposes. [The AS2700 colour standards are the intellectual property of SAI Global and available from their on-line shop.] DOES COLOUR VARY WITH GLOSS LEVEL? Yes. There is a perceived variation in colour between flat, low sheen, semi gloss and gloss surfaces; the glossier a product is, the darker the colour appears. This is because at lower gloss levels (i.e.
    [Show full text]