Mate Choice and Learning

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Mate Choice and Learning University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Eileen Hebets Publications Papers in the Biological Sciences 2010 Mate Choice and Learning Eileen Hebets University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Laura Sullivan-Beckers University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets Part of the Behavior and Ethology Commons Hebets, Eileen and Sullivan-Beckers, Laura, "Mate Choice and Learning" (2010). Eileen Hebets Publications. 46. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets/46 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Papers in the Biological Sciences at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Eileen Hebets Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published in Encyclopedia of Animal Behavior, edited by Michael D. Breed and Janice Moore. Amsterdam: Elsevier B.V., 2010, vol. 2, pp. 389-393. Copyright © 2010 Elsevier Ltd. Used by permission. Mate Choice and Learning E. A. Hebets and L. Sullivan-Beckers, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, USA Introduction choice learning. For example, the use of public informa- tion relieves an individual from personally gathering While an individual’s genetic framework is a major con- information and could minimize costs typically associ- tributor in determining its eventual mate choice, the role ated with mate assessment such as exposure to preda- of the environment in further influencing mating deci- tors or decreased time devoted to other important ac- sions has long been recognized. Animals gather informa- tivities such as foraging. Mate-choice learning more tion from the environment throughout life, and in some generally permits flexibility in mate choice, which could cases, may apply this information to increase their odds be extremely important in a changing environment. In of obtaining a high-quality mate. In short, these individu- the following text, examples of different forms of mate- als learn. Moreover, such learning can have a social com- choice learning will be provided and the state of re- ponent. “Social learning” is a general term that describes search in this area summarized. any learning based on observing, interacting with, and/ or imitating others in a social context. Social learning can Private (Personal) Information transmit information vertically, generation to generation (e.g., parent to offspring) and/or horizontally, within a Juvenile Experience: Mate-Choice Imprinting generation (as individual to individual). This form of in- formation transfer is generally referred to as “cultural “Mate-choice imprinting” refers to the learning process, transmission.” This entry will focus on social learning or processes, by which young individuals acquire sex- that relates to mate choice – mate-choice learning. ual preferences based on their observation of adults. Mate-choice learning can be separated into two broad Several specific forms of imprinting exist and the gen- categories: learning based on personal experiences with eral tenet was first described by Douglas Spalding in others (referred to as “private” or “personal informa- the nineteenth century as he recounted his observations tion”) or learning that results from the observation of of newly hatched chicks following random moving ob- others (referred to as “public information”). Learning jects. Despite its early description, however, the notion from private experiences can occur at the juvenile or of imprinting was not popularized until the 1930s by adult stage and may include encounters with conspe- the pioneering work of the Nobel Prize winning Aus- cifics or heterospecifics, same sex or opposite-sex indi- trian ethologist, Konrad Lorenz. Similar to other forms viduals (Figure 1). Mate-choice imprinting, for exam- of imprinting (e.g., filial imprinting), sexual imprinting, ple, demonstrates how an early experience based on or mate-choice imprinting, typically takes place during private information shapes subsequent mate choice. a sensitive period early in life. Historically, it has most Conversely, public information refers to any informa- frequently been observed in species with parental care, tion gained through the observations of other individ- where the young use the parent of the opposite sex as ual’s experiences. An example of the use of public in- the model upon which they base their future mating formation is mate-choice copying, for example, when a preferences. This kind of early mate-choice imprinting female mimics the mating decision of another female in is thought to function to ensure conspecific matings, en- the population. Mate choice that is influenced by private abling individuals to avoid presumably costly hetero- information is sometimes termed “independent mate specific matings. Nonetheless, it is now clear that mate- choice,” whereas mate choice based on public informa- choice imprinting is not always restricted to an early tion is “nonindependent mate choice.” sensitive period and that preferences often continue to Mate-choice learning, whether it is through the acqui- be modified throughout development. sition of private or public information, balances various Crossfostering experiments are one of the primary costs and benefits. For example, the process of learning means by which scientists study early mate-choice im- itself can be costly, a topic covered in depth elsewhere. printing and such studies are most easily, and frequently, Additionally, costs can come in the form of imprinting conducted with birds. In crossfostering experiments, off- on the wrong species (which could lead to reduced fit- spring are raised by parents of either another phenotype ness), or from copying another individual that has cho- (e.g., a different color morph) or another species and, sen poorly itself. Nonetheless, the prevalence of mate- subsequently, their adult mate choice is examined. Us- choice learning across taxonomic groups suggests that ing crossfostering experiments, mate-choice imprinting there are significant benefits associated with mate- has been demonstrated in numerous bird species includ- 389 390 HEBETS & SULLIVAN-BECKERS IN ENCYCLOPEDIA OF ANIMAL BEHAVIOR (2010) Figure 1. Mate-choice learning as influenced by social environment. This diagram depicts the various sources of social information that can impact mate-choice learning; the various life stages during which learning might be important; and some of the documented outcomes of mate-choice learning. Sections marked with “***” indicate topics for which research is lacking or nonexistent, and we suggest that these might be potentially fruitful areas for future focus. ing, but not confined to snow geese, zebra finches, Ben- mathematical models have been constructed to examine galese finches, great tits, blue tits, and red jungle fowl. In the various aspects of mate-choice imprinting. For ex- mammals, reciprocal crossfostering of sheep and goats ample, population genetic models have been used to ex- has demonstrated a role of maternal imprinting on sub- plore the evolution of different forms of imprinting. In sequent sexual preferences. Similarly, in Lake Victoria these models, Tramm and Servedio compared the evo- fishes, females of some species appear to imprint on the lution of paternal, maternal, and oblique imprinting and phenotype of their mother. Crossfostering experiments found that paternal imprinting was the most likely to supporting a process of mate-choice imprinting are prev- evolve. Their results suggest that the success of a partic- alent, yet studies do exist for which such early experi- ular imprinting strategy is most influenced by the group ences have not influenced adult mate choice—raising in- of individuals that are imprinted upon (termed the “im- teresting questions about species-level differences in the printing set”). potential for, and importance of, mate-choice learning. Traditional examples of mate-choice imprinting, as Juvenile Experience: Mate Selectivity outlined earlier, are often restricted to species in which young spend significant time with their parents, thus, Mate-choice imprinting involves juvenile individuals enabling parental imprinting (either paternal or mater- imprinting on, or learning, various characteristics of nal). However, mate-choice learning may also be preva- an adult model, whether the model is their mother, fa- lent in species that lack parental care, yet still have sig- ther, or another nonparental adult. Subsequently, these nificant exposure to other conspecifics. For example, learned characteristics are incorporated into the indi- female wolf spiders are known to choose to mate with vidual’s mate-choice criteria, and mating partners with mature males of a phenotype with which they had expe- similar characteristics are preferred. However, experi- rience as a subadult. This type of imprinting is referred ence with conspecific adults may not always lead to a to as “oblique imprinting”—imprinting on a nonpa- preference for individuals resembling a model. Some- rental adult. In another example of oblique imprinting, times, early experience may simply increase choosiness. damselfly males alter their preference of female morphs Such effects of early experience have been documented based upon prior experience – males raised in the ab- in various animal taxa. For example, in both field crick- sence of females show no preference, while those raised ets and
Recommended publications
  • Female Mate Choice in Mammals Author(S): by Tim Clutton-Brock and and Katherine Mcauliffe Reviewed Work(S): Source: the Quarterly Review of Biology, Vol
    Female Mate Choice in Mammals Author(s): By Tim Clutton-Brock and and Katherine McAuliffe Reviewed work(s): Source: The Quarterly Review of Biology, Vol. 84, No. 1 (March 2009), pp. 3-27 Published by: The University of Chicago Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/596461 . Accessed: 07/09/2012 01:00 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. The University of Chicago Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Quarterly Review of Biology. http://www.jstor.org Volume 84, No. 1 March 2009 THE QUARTERLY REVIEW of Biology FEMALE MATE CHOICE IN MAMMALS Tim Clutton-Brock Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, United Kingdom e-mail: [email protected] Katherine McAuliffe Department of Anthropology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138, USA e-mail: [email protected] keywords sexual selection, mate choice, mammals, mating systems, genetic benefits abstract Studies of mate choice in vertebrates have focused principally on birds, in which male ornaments are often highly developed, and have shown that females commonly select mates on the basis of particular phenotypic characteristics that may reflect their genetic quality.
    [Show full text]
  • Mate Choice | Principles of Biology from Nature Education
    contents Principles of Biology 171 Mate Choice Reproduction underlies many animal behaviors. The greater sage grouse (Centrocercus urophasianus). Female sage grouse evaluate males as sexual partners on the basis of the feather ornaments and the males' elaborate displays. Stephen J. Krasemann/Science Source. Topics Covered in this Module Mating as a Risky Behavior Major Objectives of this Module Describe factors associated with specific patterns of mating and life history strategies of specific mating patterns. Describe how genetics contributes to behavioral phenotypes such as mating. Describe the selection factors influencing behaviors like mate choice. page 882 of 989 3 pages left in this module contents Principles of Biology 171 Mate Choice Mating as a Risky Behavior Different species have different mating patterns. Different species have evolved a range of mating behaviors that vary in the number of individuals involved and the length of time over which their relationships last. The most open type of relationship is promiscuity, in which all members of a community can mate with each other. Within a promiscuous species, an animal of either gender may mate with any other male or female. No permanent relationships develop between mates, and offspring cannot be certain of the identity of their fathers. Promiscuous behavior is common in bonobos (Pan paniscus), as well as their close relatives, the chimpanzee (P. troglodytes). Bonobos also engage in sexual activity for activities other than reproduction: to greet other members of the community, to release social tensions, and to resolve conflicts. Test Yourself How might promiscuous behavior provide an evolutionary advantage for males? Submit Some animals demonstrate polygamous relationships, in which a single individual of one gender mates with multiple individuals of the opposite gender.
    [Show full text]
  • Estimating Genetic Benefits of Polyandry from Experimental Studies
    Biol. Rev. (2011), pp. 000–000. 1 doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2011.00182.x Estimating genetic benefits of polyandry from experimental studies: a meta-analysis Rachel A. Slatyer†, Brian S. Mautz†, Patricia R.Y. Backwell and Michael D. Jennions∗ Evolution, Ecology & Genetics, Research School of Biology, The Australian National University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia ABSTRACT The consequences of polyandry for female fitness are controversial. Sexual conflict studies and a meta-analysis of mating rates in insects suggest that there is a longevity cost when females mate repeatedly. Even so, compensatory material benefits can elevate egg production and fertility, partly because polyandry ensures an adequate sperm supply. Polyandry can therefore confer direct benefits. The main controversy surrounds genetic benefits. The argument is analogous to that surrounding the evolution of conventional female mate choice, except that with polyandry it is post-copulatory mechanisms that might bias paternity towards males with higher breeding values for fitness. Recent meta-analyses of extra-pair copulations in birds have cast doubt on whether detectable genetic benefits exist. By contrast, another meta-analysis showed that polyandry elevates egg hatching success (possibly due to a fertilization bias towards sperm with paternal genes that elevate embryo survival) in insects. A detailed summary of whether polyandry elevates other components of offspring performance is lacking. Here we present a comprehensive meta-analysis of 232 effect sizes from 46 experimental studies. These experiments were specifically designed to try to quantify the potential genetic benefits of polyandry by controlling fully for the number of matings by females assigned to monandry and polyandry treatments.
    [Show full text]
  • Female Mate Choice Based Upon Male Motor Performance
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Eileen Hebets Publications Papers in the Biological Sciences 4-2010 Female mate choice based upon male motor performance John Byers University of Idaho, [email protected] Eileen Hebets University of Nebraska - Lincoln, [email protected] Jeffrey Podos University of Massachusetts, Amherst, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets Part of the Behavior and Ethology Commons Byers, John; Hebets, Eileen; and Podos, Jeffrey, "Female mate choice based upon male motor performance" (2010). Eileen Hebets Publications. 45. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/bioscihebets/45 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Papers in the Biological Sciences at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Eileen Hebets Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published in Animal Behaviour 79:4 (April 2010), pp. 771–778; doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.009 Copyright © 2010 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour; published by Elsevier Ltd. Used by permission. Submitted October 12, 2009; revised November 19, 2009; accepted January 11, 2010; published online February 19, 2010. Female mate choice based upon male motor performance John Byers,1 Eileen Hebets,2 and Jeffrey Podos 3 1. Department of Biological Sciences, University of Idaho, Moscow, ID 83844-3051, USA 2. School of Biological Sciences, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588, USA 3. Department of Biology, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA 01003, USA Corresponding author — J. Byers, email [email protected] Abstract Our goal in this essay is to review the hypothesis that females choose mates by the evaluation of male motor performance.
    [Show full text]
  • Human Sexual Selection
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Human sexual selection David Puts Sexual selection favors traits that aid in competition over Here, I review evidence, focusing on recent findings, mates. Widespread monogamous mating, biparental care, regarding the strength and forms of sexual selection moderate body size sexual dimorphism, and low canine tooth operating over human evolution and consider how sexual dimorphism suggest modest sexual selection operating over selection has shaped human psychology, including psy- human evolution, but other evidence indicates that sexual chological sex differences. selection has actually been comparatively strong. Ancestral men probably competed for mates mainly by excluding The strength of human sexual selection competitors by force or threat, and women probably competed Some evidence suggests that sexual selection has been primarily by attracting mates. These and other forms of sexual relatively weak in humans. Although sexual dimorphisms selection shaped human anatomy and psychology, including in anatomy and behavior may arise from other selective some psychological sex differences. forces, the presence of sexually dimorphic ornamentation, Address weaponry, courtship displays, or intrasexual competition Department of Anthropology and Center for Brain, Behavior and indicates a history of sexual selection [3]. However, men’s Cognition, Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA 16802, 15–20% greater body mass than women’s is comparable to USA primate species with a modest degree of mating competi- tion among males, and humans lack the canine tooth Corresponding author: Puts, David ([email protected]) dimorphism characteristic of many primates with intense male competition for mates [4]. Moreover, humans exhibit Current Opinion in Psychology 2015, 7:28–32 biparental care and social monogamy, which tend to occur This review comes from a themed issue on Evolutionary psychology in species with low levels of male mating competition [5].
    [Show full text]
  • Sex Recognition and Mate Choice in Male <I>Rana Kukunoris</I>
    HERPETOLOGICAL JOURNAL 21: 141–144, 2011 day, 1983; Andersson, 1994). Such studies have mainly Short notes focused on bufonids, whereas knowledge about mate choice in ranids is less complete. Sex recognition and mate Rana kukunoris is endemic to the eastern Tibetan plateau (29–41ºN, 93–104ºE) and is distributed at high choice in male Rana kukunoris elevations (Zhang, 1999; Xie et al., 2000). Rana kuku- noris is an explosive breeder (Wells, 1977) with a short Wei Chen & Xin Lu breeding period (one week; pers. obs.). Female fecundity is positively related to body size (Lu et al., 2008). Males Department of Zoology, College of Life Sciences, lack vocal sacs and do not produce advertisement calls Wuhan University, China during the short breeding season. Therefore, males acti- vely search for and approach females for mating during Mate recognition is important for successful reproduction, daytime. Amplexus takes place in water and lasts three and consequently species have evolved various ways to or four days. Preliminary information on the distribution recognize potential mates. Little is known about mate and habitat of R. kukunoris exists (Xie et al., 2000; Fei & recognition in male Rana kukunoris, a temperate frog Ye, 2001), but little is known about the mating behaviour endemic to China. In the present study, we investigated of this Chinese endemic. In this study, we investigated experimentally whether male R. kukunoris can mating patterns and used three choice experiments to discriminate between conspecifics of different sexes, test whether male frogs have the ability to discriminate between gravid and non-gravid females of similar size, and between 1) females and males, 2) gravid and non-gravid between gravid females of different sizes.
    [Show full text]
  • Serial Monogamy Benefits Both Sexes in the Biparental Convict Cichlid
    Serial monogamy benefits both sexes in the biparental convict cichlid Jennifer L. Snekser1 and Murray Itzkowitz2 1 Department of Biology, LIU Post, Brookville, NY, USA 2 Department of Biological Sciences, Lehigh University, Bethlehem, PA, USA ABSTRACT Monogamy can be either long-term or serial, with new pairs formed with each breeding bout. Costs and benefits are associated with each strategy. Because biparental convict cichlids (Amatitlania nigrofasciata) typically switch mates, exhibiting serial monogamy, we tested for the costs associated with forcing individuals to remain with the same mate. Convict cichlids were observed over two successive breeding bouts, either with the same or a new, equally experienced, mate. Parental behavior did not differ between breeding bouts, nor did brood size. Surprisingly, fish that remained with their original partner for a second bout took significantly longer to produce a brood compared to fish that paired with new partners. New partners were also more likely to successfully produce a second brood than re-mated partners. This is in contrast to the majority of bird studies that show many benefits to staying with the same partner for multiple broods. In convict cichlids, there seems to be no benefit associated with remaining with the same partner and switching mates reduces duration between broods for both males and females, potentially increasing overall reproductive success. Subjects Animal Behavior, Aquaculture, Fisheries and Fish Science, Evolutionary Studies Keywords Biparental, Cichlids, Reproductive success, Brood success, Retrieval, Parental aggression, Parental care, Sex differences Submitted 16 October 2018 INTRODUCTION Accepted 28 January 2019 Published 5 March 2019 Monogamy is observed in a wide variety of animal species, including a limited number fi Corresponding author of invertebrates, teleost shes, mammals, and the vast majority of avian species Jennifer L.
    [Show full text]
  • Sexual Selection and Mate Choice
    Review TRENDS in Ecology and Evolution Vol.21 No.6 June 2006 Sexual selection and mate choice Malte Andersson1 and Leigh W. Simmons2 1Department of Zoology, University of Gothenburg, SE 405 30 Gothenburg, Sweden 2Centre for Evolutionary Biology, School of Animal Biology (M092), The University of Western Australia, Crawley 6009, WA, Australia The past two decades have seen extensive growth of characterization of genes and their effects, from DNA sexual selection research. Theoretical and empirical sequences via protein to phenotypic expression at the level work has clarified many components of pre- and of the individual, with possible consequences at the postcopulatory sexual selection, such as aggressive population level and above. competition, mate choice, sperm utilization and sexual conflict. Genetic mechanisms of mate choice evolution Evolution of mate choice have been less amenable to empirical testing, but Although mate choice occurs in males and females [4], for molecular genetic analyses can now be used for incisive convenience we refer here to female choice of male traits. experimentation. Here, we highlight some of the As experimental evidence accumulated, mate choice currently debated areas in pre- and postcopulatory became widely recognized, but the genetic mechanisms sexual selection. We identify where new techniques underlying its evolution remain the subject of debate can help estimate the relative roles of the various (Box 1). Showing how mating preferences evolve geneti- selection mechanisms that might work together in the cally is harder than showing that they exist, and the evolution of mating preferences and attractive traits, problem is aggravated by the possibility that several and in sperm–egg interactions.
    [Show full text]
  • Sexual Cannibalism and Mate Choice Decisions in Wolf Spiders: Influence of Male Size and Secondary Sexual Characters
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2005, 69, 83–94 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.12.030 Sexual cannibalism and mate choice decisions in wolf spiders: influence of male size and secondary sexual characters MATTHEW H. PERSONS & GEORGE W. UETZ Department of Biological Sciences, University of Cincinnati (Received 29 January 2003; initial acceptance 21 May 2003; final acceptance 19 December 2003; published online 11 November 2004; MS. number: A9539R) Sexual cannibalism may influence expression of elaborate male traits by either reinforcing or opposing sexual selection. Male Schizocosa ocreata (Hentz) wolf spiders (Lycosidae) have tufts of bristles on the first pair of legs that may function as a condition-indicating signal trait. We paired virgin and previously mated adult females with males under seminatural conditions (laboratory containers with leaf litter). For males encountering virgin females, probability of attempted premating cannibalism varied with male size, body condition, tuft size, fluctuating asymmetry of tufts (FA) and female size (larger females attacked smaller males and males in poor condition with smaller, more asymmetrical tufts). Probability of successful cannibalism varied with the relative size of both sexes and female body condition, but not male tuft size (smaller males were cannibalized by larger females in better condition). Males with larger tufts (relative to body size) were more likely to mate, but no other traits (male or female) were associated with mating success. Postmating cannibalism risk was associated with female size and age, and male size, body condition and tuft size (larger, older females cannibalized smaller males in poor condition with smaller tufts). For males paired with previously mated females, probability of cannibalism was influenced by size of both sexes and male tuft size (larger females cannibalized smaller males with smaller tufts).
    [Show full text]
  • Mate Choice and Sexual Selection: What Have We Learned Since Darwin?
    Mate choice and sexual selection: What have we learned since Darwin? Adam G. Jones1 and Nicholas L. Ratterman Department of Biology, Texas A&M University, 3258 TAMU, College Station, TX 77843 Charles Darwin laid the foundation for all modern work on sexual concerns sexual selection, but many of Darwin’s insights regard- selection in his seminal book The Descent of Man, and Selection in ing sexual selection appear in his chapters on humans. Relation to Sex. In this work, Darwin fleshed out the mechanism of Darwin’s most lasting achievement with respect to sexual sexual selection, a hypothesis that he had proposed in The Origin of selection must be his definition of the term, as it is essentially the Species. He went well beyond a simple description of the phenom- same as the one still in use today. It is difficult to find a quote enon by providing extensive evidence and considering the far-reach- from Darwin that captures the full essence of his concept of ing implications of the idea. Here we consider the contributions of sexual selection, but he provides the following definition (ref. 2; Darwin to sexual selection with a particular eye on how far we have Part I, pp 254–255): progressed in the last 150 years. We focus on 2 key questions in sexual selection. First, why does mate choice evolve at all? And second, what ‘‘We are, however, here concerned only with that kind factors determine the strength of mate choice (or intensity of sexual of selection, which I have called sexual selection. This selection) in each sex? Darwin provided partial answers to these depends on the advantage which certain individuals questions, and the progress that has been made on both of these have over other individuals of the same sex and species, topics since his time should be seen as one of the great triumphs of in exclusive relation to reproduction.’’ modern evolutionary biology.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolution of Human Mate Choice
    Evolution of Human Mate Choice David C. Geary, Jacob Vigil, and Jennifer Byrd-Craven University of Missouri – Columbia This article provides a review of evolutionary theory and empirical research on mate choices in non-human species and used as a frame for understanding the how and why of human mate choices. The basic principle is that the preferred mate choices and attendant social cognitions and behaviors of both women and men, and those of other species, have evolved to focus on and exploit the reproductive potential and reproductive investment of members of the opposite sex. Reproductive potential is defined as the genetic, material, and (or) social resources an individual can invest in offspring, and reproductive investment is the actual use of these resources to enhance the physical and social well being of offspring. Similarities and differences in the mate preferences and choices of women and men are reviewed, and can be understood in terms of simi- larities and differences in the form reproductive potential that women and men have to offer and their tendency to use this potential for the well- being of children. The study of human sexual behavior and human sex dif- mechanisms that operate within species and are principle ferences has been approached from many vantage points factors in the evolution of sex differences (Darwin, 1871). (Davidson & Moore, 2001; McGillicuddy-De Lisi & De These mechanisms are called sexual selection and involve Lisi, 2002) and in recent years has been viewed through the competition with members of the same sex over mates lens of evolutionary theory (Buss, 1994; Campbell, 2002; (intrasexual competition) and discriminative choice of mat- Geary, 1998; Low, 2000; Symons, 1979).
    [Show full text]
  • Opportunity for Female Mate Choice Improves Reproductive Outcomes in the Conservation Breeding Program of the Eastern Barred
    Applied Animal Behaviour Science 199 (2018) 67–74 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Applied Animal Behaviour Science journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/applanim Research Paper Opportunity for female mate choice improves reproductive outcomes in the T conservation breeding program of the eastern barred bandicoot (Perameles gunnii) ⁎ Christine M. Hartnetta,b, , Marissa L. Parrottb, Raoul A. Muldera, Graeme Coulsona, Michael J.L. Magrathb a School of Biosciences, The University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia b Department of Wildlife Conservation and Science, Zoos Victoria, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Captive breeding is fundamental to recovery programs for many endangered species. Most programs seek to Mate choice maximise retention of genetic variation and minimise inbreeding risk by assigning individuals to pre-determined Mate preference pairings, but the reproductive outcomes of such pairings are often poor. There is evidence that pairing in- Olfactory communication dividuals in captivity that are instead genetically and behaviourally compatible can result in improved re- Genetic compatibility productive success, and that the resulting offspring perform better when released into the wild. Evidence is also Behavioural compatibility mounting that such compatible outcomes can be achieved by allowing female mate choice. We investigated whether the reproductive success of captive endangered eastern barred bandicoots (Perameles gunnii) improved when females were permitted to choose their mate. Over a 21 day period, we allowed females to simultaneously evaluate two males that were unrelated to the female via interaction through perforated clear acrylic barriers, which permitted visual, auditory and olfactory, but not physical, contact. We determined female preferences by measuring how much time they spent interacting with each male, and whether they showed behavioural signs of receptivity.
    [Show full text]