Meat Is Good to Taboo: Dietary Proscriptions As a Product of The

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Meat Is Good to Taboo: Dietary Proscriptions As a Product of The Meat Is Good to Taboo ¤ Dietary Proscriptions as aProduct ofthe Interaction of Psychological Mechanisms and Social Processes DANIEL M.T. FESSLER¤¤ and CARLOS DAVID NAVARRETE¤¤ ABSTRACT Comparingfood taboos across 78 cultures,this paperdemonstrates that meat,though aprizedfood, is also the principaltarget ofproscriptions. Reviewing existing explanations oftaboos, we nd that bothfunctionalist and symbolic approaches fail to account for meat’s cross-cultural centrality anddo not reect experience-near aspectsof food taboos, principalamong which isdisgust. Adopting an evolutionaryapproach to the mind,this paperpresents an alternative toexisting explanations of food taboos. Consistent with the attendant risk ofpathogentransmission, meat has special salience as a stimulusfor humans, asanimal products are stronger elicitorsof disgust and aversion than plant products.We identifythree psychosocialprocesses, socially-mediatedingestive conditioning, egocentric empathy , and normativemoralization ,each ofwhich likelyplays a rolein transforming individualdisgust responsesand conditioned food aversions into institutionalized food taboos. Introduction Culturalunderstandings concerning food, edibility, contamination, and re- latedtopics exhibit enormous variation across groups (Barer-Stein 1999; Rozin2000; Simoons 1994). However, despite such evident heterogeneity, investigators(e.g., Rozin 1987; Haidt et al.1997; Simoons 1994; Tambiah 1969)have offhandedlysuggested that animals and animal products seem especially likely tobe the focusof foodtaboos. The possibilityof uniformity ¤Harriet Whiteheadand Robert Aunger kindly suppliedus with draftsof their respective works.Paul Arguello assisted with research. FranciscoGil-White, RoyD’ Andrade,Robert Aunger,and Robert Sussman provided useful suggestions. We thank the many investigators whoshared taboo data and references. ¤¤Center for Behavior,Evolution, and Culture andDepartment ofAnthropology, UCLA, LosAngeles, CA 90095-1553. c KoninklijkeBrill NV, Leiden, 2003 Journal of Cognition and Culture 3.1 ° 2 DANIEL M.T.FESSLERANDCARLOSDAVIDNA VARRETE ina domainsubject to substantialvariation is ofgreatinterest, for such pat- ternscry out for explanation (Brown 1991). We have thereforeconducted the rstsystematic ethnological investigation of food prohibitions designed toevaluate the relative prominenceof animalproducts as a focusof taboos. Afterdescribing the methodsused in our investigation we present results demonstratingthat meat is indeed disproportionately represented in food taboos.Reviewing the twoprincipal existing explanationsof food taboos, wendthat neither functionalistnor symbolic approaches provides a com- pelling account.We thereforeconsider a variety ofpsychosocial processes thatmay contributeto taboo formation, highlighting the roleof emotions ineach process.Arguing that meat, while nutritious, is also potentially dangerous,we propose that natural selection has producedan ambiva- lence towardmeat such that, compared to other foods, meat is morelikely tobecome the targetof disgust. We then showhow this predisposition can articulatewith the psychosocialprocesses that generate taboos,thereby accountingfor the prominenceof meat infoodproscriptions. ACross-Cultural Studyof theTargets of Food Taboos The Sample ofFood Taboos and the Problem ofNon-independence Anthropologistsoften subsume proscriptions of markedly differingtypes underthe rubricof ‘ taboo’(Valeri 2000:43-6).While thispractice elimi- nates distinctionsthat are vital forunderstanding the detailsof any given culturalsystem, becauseour goal is toinvestigate patternsof human belief attheir broadest, we adopt an extreme versionof this approach, treating all foodproscriptions as equivalentregardless of whether they applyto all orpart of society, all orpartof aspeciesor fooditem, or all orpartof the calendaror life span.To collectdata on tabooswe conductedan extended survey ofprint and electronic ethnographies and studies in related elds. We alsocontacted investigators known to have workedon taboos, and em- ployeda snowballstrategy to learn ofothers possessing relevant data.So asnot to weightthe studytoward heavily-investigated societies,we avoided usingmany sourceson the same culture.While oursearch was, to our knowledge,the mostextensive ever undertaken,we make noclaim that it wasexhaustive. However,although there aredoubtlessly numerous sources thatwe did not encounter, there isnoreason to believe thatour methods biasedthe natureof the datacollected. Our search produced information MEAT ISGOODTOTAB OO 3 onfood prohibitions (henceforth used interchangeably with ‘ taboos’) in seventy-eight cultures.For each culture,the rst ve taboosencountered inthe text(s) werenoted (if fewer than ve tabooswere listed in the source, all werenoted). Limiting the numberof taboos per culture provided an initialconstraint on the disproportionatein uence ofsocieties having a largenumber of taboos; we selected ve asthe cut-offbecause inspection suggestedthat this was slightly abovethe average numberof taboos re- ported.Because wehad previously demonstrated that taboos imposed on pregnantwomen focus primarily on meat(Fessler 2002),pregnancy taboos wereexcluded in orderto avoidprejudicing the results.Using foodgroups commonlyemployed in the nutritionliterature, the numberof taboos was talliedfor the followingcategories: meat, vegetables, fruit,sweets, dairy products,and starches. Because ourwork on pregnancy taboos suggested thatspicy foods have specialsalience asthe targetof taboos, we included thisas a seventh category.Results aredisplayed in the Appendix.For illustrativepurposes, Table 1 presentsthe average numberof taboos on meatversus taboos on all non-meatfoods. Note that, in each region,meat taboosare more frequent than non-meattaboos, and, with the exception ofSoutheast Asia, this difference is quitemarked. Ifall ethnographieswere independent sources of data,simple statistical tests couldbe performed on the Appendix.However, such an approach riskscommitting Galton’ s error,the assumptionof independence among societiesthat do not constitute independent data points. The possibilityof i) the diffusionof cultural traits between neighboringsocieties, and ii) the recentsplintering of once homogeneous groups makes any suchtest prob- lematic.Previous attempts to solve thisproblem employ autocorrelation techniquesto recalibrate in ated sample sizes (Naroll 1976) or contin- gency tablesthat collapse matrices by the amountof excess tobalance the numberof cells ineach independentcategory (Strauss & Orans1975). These techniquesunderestimate the varianceof data points within inde- pendentsamples and may skew condence intervals infavor of the null hypothesis.However, these problemscan be managedusing bootstrapping techniques.Standard bootstrapping involves the samplingof the original data(with replacement), and the executionof some formula of interest to createa samplingdistribution of the statisticof interest on which hypoth- esis tests canbe performed (Fox 1997).Although this method solves the 4 DANIEL M.T.FESSLERANDCARLOSDAVIDNA VARRETE Table 1 Average number ofmeat and non-meat taboosper societyby geographic region Region Meat non-Meat Australia 4.44 0.33 C. America 2 0 East Asia 3 1.33 Europe 5 0 Mid. East 5 0 N. Africa 2.4 1.4 N. America 3.2 0.1 Oceania 2 0.85 S. Africa 3.33 0.58 S. America 3.5 0.1 S. Asia 2.6 0.4 S. E. Asia 1.88 1.63 Totals 38.35 6.72 Total Mean 3.2 0.56 S.D. 1.59 0.13 problemof non-normally distributed error, it does not solve the problem ofnon-independence, as bootstrapping too assumes that the originalset iscomposed of independent data points. We thereforedevised a novel bootstrappingtechnique that corresponds to a z-test forproportions, uses all available data,does not underestimate variances by averaging out data pointsinto contingency tables, and minimizes Galton’ s problemof non- independence. Methods ofStatistical Analysis We organizedall ofthe societiesin our sample into twelve geo- graphic/culturalregions that we treat as roughly independent areas. This approachis conservative in that, by clustering societies in categorieslarger thancommonly-recognized culture areas, we decreasedthe likelihoodthat, ifwe compared two societies from different categories, they wouldshare substantivehistorical links. We wrotesoftware that randomly chose a geo- graphicalregion and then randomlyselected oneculture from within that MEAT ISGOODTOTAB OO 5 region.1 The programrepeated this process with replacement until the numberof samples equaled the numberof geographic regions ( n 12). D The frequenciesof taboos for each foodcategory (e.g. meat, fruit, etc.) werethen convertedto proportions that describe the relative occurrence oftaboos in the meat categoryversus the relative occurrenceof taboos ineach non-meatcategory, resulting in a totalof seven tests ofdiffer- ences ofproportions. The processwas repeated 10,000 times to create, foreach comparison,a distributionof differences. p-values wereestab- lishedby calculating the percentageof cases (outof 10,000) in which the value differencesfor each comparisonwas less thanor equal to zero. One mightargue that each non-meattaboo does not represent a categoryfor a specic tabooedfood, as some societies might de ne food taboossimply with regard to meat versus non-meat. A test ofdifferences inproportions was therefore also performed for the differencebetween meattaboos and all otherfood taboos combined. The argumentcould also bemade that multiple meat taboosshould not be treated as independent entities,as
Recommended publications
  • Fraud in Animal Origin Food Products: Advances in Emerging Spectroscopic Detection Methods Over the Past Five Years
    foods Review Fraud in Animal Origin Food Products: Advances in Emerging Spectroscopic Detection Methods over the Past Five Years Abdo Hassoun 1,* , Ingrid Måge 1 , Walter F. Schmidt 2, Havva Tümay Temiz 3, Li Li 4, Hae-Yeong Kim 5 , Heidi Nilsen 1, Alessandra Biancolillo 6 , Abderrahmane Aït-Kaddour 7 , Marek Sikorski 8 , Ewa Sikorska 9 , Silvia Grassi 10 and Daniel Cozzolino 11 1 Nofima AS, Norwegian Institute of Food, Fisheries, and Aquaculture Research, Muninbakken 9-13, 9291 Tromsø, Norway; ingrid.mage@Nofima.no (I.M.); heidi.nilsen@nofima.no (H.N.) 2 United States Department of Agriculture, Agricultural Research Service, 10300 Baltimore Avenue, Beltsville, MD 20705-2325, USA; [email protected] 3 Department of Food Engineering, Bingol University, 12000 Bingol, Turkey; [email protected] 4 Key Laboratory of Mariculture, Ministry of Education, Ocean University of China, Qingdao 266003, China; [email protected] 5 Department of Food Science and Biotechnology, Kyung Hee University, Yongin 17104, Korea; [email protected] 6 Department of Physical and Chemical Sciences, University of L’Aquila, 67100 Via Vetoio, Coppito, L’Aquila, Italy; [email protected] 7 Université Clermont-Auvergne, INRAE, VetAgro Sup, UMR F, 63370 Lempdes, France; [email protected] 8 Faculty of Chemistry, Adam Mickiewicz University in Poznan, Uniwersytetu Poznanskiego 8, 61-614 Poznan, Poland; [email protected] 9 Institute of Quality Science, Pozna´nUniversity of Economics and Business, al. Niepodległo´sci10, 61-875 Pozna´n,Poland;
    [Show full text]
  • Did Eating Human Poop Play a Role in the Evolution of Dogs?
    WellBeing International WBI Studies Repository 4-24-2020 Did Eating Human Poop Play a Role in the Evolution of Dogs? Harold Herzog Animal Studies Repository Follow this and additional works at: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/sc_herzog_compiss Recommended Citation Herzog, Harold, Did eating Human Poop Play a Role in the Evolution of Dogs? (2020), 'Animals and Us' Blog Posts, Psychology Today, 24 August, 118. https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/animals-and- us/202008/did-eating-human-poop-play-role-in-the-evolution-dogs This material is brought to you for free and open access by WellBeing International. It has been accepted for inclusion by an authorized administrator of the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Did Eating Human Poop Play a Role in the Evolution of Dogs? The consumption of human feces may have influenced canine evolution. Posted Aug 24, 2020 Source: Photo by K. Thalhotter/123RF “Poop is central to the story of how dogs came into our lives," write Duke University dog researchers Brian Hare and Vanessa Woods in their wonderful new book, Survival of the Friendliest: Understanding Our Origins and Rediscovering Our Common Humanity. I think they may be right. Molly, our beloved Labrador retriever, certainly loved to eat poop. Sometimes she would chow down on her own feces, and at other times, she preferred excrement of the cows that lived behind our house. Mary Jean and I found this practice (coprophagia) disgusting. We were not alone. Ben Hart and his colleagues at the University of California at Davis School of Veterinary Medicine surveyed nearly 3,000 dog owners about their pet’s penchant for poop.
    [Show full text]
  • The Use of Species-Specific Primer Targeting on D-Loop Mitochondrial
    Journal of Advanced Veterinary and Animal Research ISSN 2311-7710 (Electronic) http://doi.org/10.5455/javar.2018.e275 September 2018 A periodical of the Network for the Veterinarians of Bangladesh (BDvetNET) Vol 5 No 3, Pages 361-368. Short Communication The use of species-specific primer targeting on D-loop mitochondrial for identification of wild boar meat in meatball formulation Rien Larasati Arini1, Dwiky Ramadhani1, Ni wayan Pebriyanti1, Sismindari 2 and Abdul Rohman 2,# • Received: June 4, 2018 • Revised: July 18, 2018 • Accepted: July 21, 2018 • Published Online: August 6, 2018 AFFILIATIONS ABSTRACT 1Faculty of Pharmacy, Gadjah Mada Objective: The study was intended to design the new specific primer targeting on University, Sekip Utara, Yogyakarta mitochondrial D-Loop gene (D-Loop 443 primer) combined with a real-time 55281, Indonesia. polymerase chain reaction (PCR) for the analysis of wild boar meat (WBM) in food products of meatball. 2Departement of Pharmaceutical Materials and methods: The primer was designed and subjected to primer-basic Chemistry, Gadjah Mada University, local alignment search tool using National Center for Biotechnology Information Sekip Utara, Yogyakarta 55281, software. Validation of real-time PCR using designed primer was performed by Indonesia. evaluation of several performance characteristics which included specificity, sensitivity, repeatability, linearity, and efficiency. Results: The results showed that the D-loop primer could be attached at 60.7°C and no amplification was detected against other species confirming the specificity of the primers. The limits of detection were found to be 4.68 ng and 2.34 ng using DNA extracted from WBM and that extracted from wild boar in meatball product.
    [Show full text]
  • Identification of Meat Species by Using Molecular and Spectroscopic Techniques
    Turkish Journal of Agriculture - Food Science and Technology, 5(5): 488-492, 2017 Turkish Journal of Agriculture - Food Science and Technology Available online, ISSN: 2148-127X www.agrifoodscience.com, Turkish Science and Technology Identification of Meat Species by Using Molecular and Spectroscopic Techniques Evrim Gunes Altuntas1*, Ebru Deniz2, Beycan Ayhan3, Kezban Candogan2, Duygu Ozel Demiralp4 1Biotechnology Institute, System Biotechnology Advanced Research Unit, Ankara University, 06100 Ankara, Turkey 2Department of Food Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, Ankara University, 06110 Ankara, Turkey 3Dışkapı Yıldırım Beyazıt Training and Research Hospital, Aziz Sancar Research Institute, 06110 Ankara, Turkey 4Department of Biomedical Engineering, Faculty of Engineering, Ankara University, 06110 Ankara, Turkey A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T Meat is one of the main nutrition source in the human diet with its excellent protein, Review Article vitamin and mineral contents. Despite its advantages, being high-priced makes meat products open to adulteration. Meat products are mixed food types which can contain Received 31 October 2016 different species of meat. However, mixing two or more types of meats is not always Accepted 20 December 2016 allowed by laws. On the other hand, replacement high quality meats with cheaper meat types are a cost lowering way for the producers. The commonly consumed meat types Keywords: differ from country to country, but generally economical, ethnic and religion concerns are Meat and meat products in the foreground. In this case, species identification techniques are gaining importance. Meat adulteration Although some techniques depending on DNA or spectroscopy have been developed for Identification of meat species many years, choosing the best method for species identification is still among the DNA-based methods controversial issues today.
    [Show full text]
  • RDT-6050H-25.Pdf
    Related Items Instruction Manual No. RDT-6050H-25 Catalog# ProdDescription 6020-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT Chicken IgG Rapid Test cards,25/pk 6050-RDT-50 TruStrip RDT Chicken Egg Ovalbumin (Ova/Gal d 2) Rapid Test cards, 25/pk TruStrip RDT 5-minute Horse meat detection/adulteration rapid test cards, 25 tests 6320-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT Mouse IgG Rapid Test cards,25/pk 6420-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT Rat IgG Rapid Test cards,25/pk 6520-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT Rabbit IgG Rapid Test cards,25/pk 7000-30-RDT TruStrip RDT Rabbit Albumin Rapid test cards, 25/pk 7000-40-RDT TruStrip RDT Cat Albumin Rapid test cards, 25/pk 7000-50-RDT TruStrip RDT Dog Albumin Rapid test cards, 25/pk 7000-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT Dog Serum Albumin Rapid Test cards, 25/pk 7050-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT Monkey IgG Rapid Test cards,25/pk 7420-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT G. pig IgG Rapid Test cards,25/pk 7520-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT Goat IgG Rapid Test cards,25/pk 7620-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT Sheep IgG Rapid Test cards,25/pk Cat# RDT-6050H-25 7730-RDT-10 TruStrip RDT Horse/Foal IgG (Failure of passive transfer, FPT) Rapid test cards, 10/pk 5 minutes Horse Meat identification or adulteration detection in raw, uncooked meat or grounded meat 7740-RDT-10 TruStrip RDT Horse IgM (immunodeficiency syndrome) Rapid test cards, 10/pk 7820-RDT-25 TruStrip RDT Camel IgG Rapid test cards, 25/pk RDT-0400-100 TruStrip RDT Pregnancy Test, rapid tests for human serum or urine (HCG Combo card) RDT-3010-GA TruStrip RDT Goat Albumin Rapid Test cards,25/pk RDT-4050D-10 TruStrip RDT 5-minute Dog meat detection/adulteration rapid test,
    [Show full text]
  • Species Image of Mandible Dietary Ecology
    Species Image of Mandible Dietary Ecology Acomys cahirinus Omnivore – Seeds, fruits, (Northeast African insects, food scavenged from humans, shrubs (green leaves), spiny mouse) molluscs, carrion. Omnivore - (Nowak, 1999) Aplodontia rufa Herbivore – forbs, grasses, ferns. (mountain beaver) Specialised Herbivore – (Samuels, 2009). Bathyergus suillus Herbivore – grass, sedge, roots, (Cape dune mole- bulbs, tubers. rat) Specialised Herbivore – (Samuels, 2009). Cannomys badius Herbivore – roots, bamboo, (Lesser bamboo rat) shoots, grasses. Occasional seeds and fruits. Specialised Herbivore – (Samuels, 2009). Capromys pilorides Omnivore – Bark leaves, fruits, (Desmarest’s hutia) small vertebrates, ground and tree level vegetation. Omnivore - (Nowak, 1999). Castor canadensis Herbivore – Leaves, bark, bud (North American and roots, cambium (softer tissue of trees beneath bark). Beaver) Specialised Herbivore – (Samuels, 2009). Cavia porcellus Herbivore – Leaves, roots and (Domestic guinea tubers, fruits, flowers, lettuce etc. (rely on humans). pig) Specialised Herbivore (Cavia aperea) - (Samuels, 2009). Cricetomys Omnivore – Fruits, vegetables, gambianus nuts, insects, molluscs, roots (sweet potatoes etc.). (Northern giant pouched rat) Omnivore – (Nowak, 1999). Ctenomys opimus Diet for this species has not (Highland tuco-tuco) been extensively documented. Assuming that it is like other tuco-tuco, it is a herbivore – Grasses and roots primarily. Specialised Herbivore (Ctenomys conoveri) - (Samuels, 2009). Dasyprocta (Agouti - Species unknown. Assuming
    [Show full text]
  • Public Workshop, a Lot of It Is Stemming from C
    FOOD AND DRUG ADMINISTRATION CENTER FOR BIOLOGICS EVALUATION AND RESEARCH CENTER FOR DRUG EVALUATION AND RESEARCH FECAL MICROBIOTA FOR TRANSPLANTATION: SCIENTIFIC AND REGULATORY ISSUES CENTER FOR BIOLOGICS, EVALUATION AND RESEARCH (FDA) AND THE NATIONAL INSTITUTE FOR ALLERGY AND INFECTIOUS DISEASES (NIH) [This transcript has not been edited or corrected, but appears as received from the commercial transcribing service. Accordingly, the Food and Drug Administration makes no representation as to its accuracy.] Bethesda, Maryland Thursday, May 2, 2013 A G E N D A Welcome and Opening Remarks: KAREN MIDTHUN, MD Director, CBER/FDA FRED CASSELS, PhD Branch Chief of Enteric and Hepatic Diseases,DMID/NIAID Session I: The Microbiome in Health and Disease Part I: Moderator: MELODY MILLS, PhD NIAID/NIH Panelists: LITA PROCTOR, PhD National Human Genome Research Institute PHILLIP TARR, MD Washington University, School of Medicine in St. Louis YASMINE BELKAID, PhD National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases ERIC G. PAMER, MD Sloan-Kettering Institute VINCENT B. YOUNG, MD, PhD University of Michigan Session II: The Microbiome in Health and Disease Part II Moderator: DAVID RELMAN, MD Panelists: ROBERT BRITTON, PhD Michigan State University LINDA S. MANSFIELD, MS, VMD, PhD Michigan State University EMMA ALLEN-VERCOE, PhD University of Guelph * * * * * P R O C E E D I N G S (8:43 a.m.) MS. MIDTHUN: Good morning, can you hear me? Okay, very good. Well, first off I'd like to welcome all of you. Thank you so much for coming today. I'm Karen Midthun, the Director of the Center for Biologics Evaluation and Research which is one of the Centers within the Food and Drug Administration.
    [Show full text]
  • Seroprevalence of Anti-Toxoplasma Gondii Antibodies in Wild Boars (Sus Scrofa), Hunting Dogs, and Hunters of Brazil
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Seroprevalence of anti-Toxoplasma gondii antibodies in wild boars (Sus scrofa), hunting dogs, and hunters of Brazil 1☯ 2☯ 1 Fernanda Pistori Machado , Louise Bach Kmetiuk , Pedro Irineu Teider-JuniorID , Maysa Pellizzaro3, Ana Carolina Yamakawa4, Camila Marinelli Martins5, Renato van Wilpe Bach6, VõÂvien Midori Morikawa7, Ivan Roque de Barros-Filho1, HeÂlio Langoni4, 8 1 Andrea Pires dos SantosID , Alexander Welker BiondoID * 1 Graduate College of Veterinary Science, Federal University of Parana (UFPR), Curitiba, ParanaÂ, Brazil, 2 Graduate College of Cellular and Molecular Biology, Federal University of Parana (UFPR), Curitiba, a1111111111 ParanaÂ, Brazil, 3 Public Health Institute (PHI), Federal University of Bahia (UFBA), Salvador, Bahia, Brazil, a1111111111 4 Department of Veterinary Hygiene and Public Health, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Botucatu, São a1111111111 Paulo, Brazil, 5 Department of Nursing and Public Health, State University of Ponta Grossa, Ponta Grossa, a1111111111 ParanaÂ, Brazil, 6 Department of Medicine, State University of Ponta Grossa, Ponta Grossa, ParanaÂ, Brazil, a1111111111 7 Department of Collective Health, Federal University of Parana, Curitiba, ParanaÂ, Brazil, 8 Department of Comparative Pathobiology, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, United States of America ☯ These authors contributed equally to this work. * [email protected] OPEN ACCESS Citation: Machado FP, Kmetiuk LB, Teider-Junior Abstract PI, Pellizzaro M, Yamakawa AC, Martins CM, et al. (2019) Seroprevalence of anti-Toxoplasma gondii Seroprevalence of Toxoplasma gondii has been extensively studied in wild boars worldwide antibodies in wild boars (Sus scrofa), hunting dogs, and hunters of Brazil. PLoS ONE 14(10): due to the emerging risk for human infection through meat consumption. However, this is e0223474.
    [Show full text]
  • Coprophagia” Is Defined As the Ingestion of Any Type of Faeces, and Is a Common Complaint of Dog Owners
    Homeward Bound Golden Retriever Rescue Golden Rule Training Canine Corprophagia "Coprophagia” is defined as the ingestion of any type of faeces, and is a common complaint of dog owners. For humans it can seem disgusting and embarrassing for our dogs to engage in this behavior. It is somewhat a natural act for dogs, but as we domesticate our pets, we expect these behaviors to disappear; however, they are still animals with natural instincts! A dog may eat its own faeces, those of another dog or the faeces of another species. Dogs commonly eat the faeces of cats with which they share a household (or farm animals). The only circumstance in which coprophagia is considered a normal behavior is when a mother dog eats the faeces of her pups from the time of their birth until about 3 weeks of age. It is felt that the mother does this to keep the area clean until the pups are able to move away from it to defecate. A clean area may be less likely to attract predators in the wild. What behaviors are linked with corprophagia? Coprophagia may be a behavioral or a medical problem. It may be a type of behavioral coping mechanism for an animal that is caught in a stressful situation, such as a sudden environmental change. It can also be a tactic to avoid punishment for having a bowel movement in an inappropriate area. Coprophagia can inadvertently be taught to a puppy or adult during housetraining; if the puppy or dog is scolded or he sees the owner 'cleaning up' the bowel movement in the house, the puppy may learn to 'hide' the bowel movement by eating it.
    [Show full text]
  • Book of Abstracts
    XIIIXIII th SLOVAKSLOVAK ANDAND CZECHCZECH PARASITOLOGICALPARASITOLOGICAL DAYSDAYS XIII. SLOVENSKÉ A ČESKÉ PARAZITOLOGICKÉ DNI ParasitesParasites inin thethe HeartHeart ofof EuropeEurope 2 BOOK OF ABSTRACTS Košice, Slovakia,Sl ki Congress C g Hotel H t l Centrum C May 21 – 25, 2018 The editors hold no responsibility for any content, inaccuracy or language errors in the abstracts. EDITORS MARTINA MITERPÁKOVÁ, ZUZANA VASILKOVÁ GRAPHIC DESIGN ZUZANA VASILKOVÁ ISBN 978 – 80 - 968473 – 9 – 6 ©SLOVAK SOCIETY FOR PARASITOLOGY AT SAS KOŠICE, MAY 2018 OORRGAANINIZZEED BY TTHHE Sllovakovak Soocietyciety fforor Paarasitologyrasitology Innstitutestitute ooff Paarasitology,rasitology, Sllovakovak Accademyademy ooff Scciencesiences Czzechech Societyociety fforor Paarasitologyrasitology ORGANIZING COMMITTEE CHAIR: MARTINA MITERPÁKOVÁ MEMBERS: DANIELA ANTOLOVÁ ZUZANA HURNÍKOVÁ EVA NOVÁKOVÁ VERONIKA TARAGEĽOVÁ ZUZANA VASILKOVÁ JUDGING PANEL FOR STUDENT COMPETITION CHAIR: IVICA HROMADOVÁ Institute of Parasitology SAS, Košice, SK MEMBERS: DAVID BRUCE CONN Harvard University and Berry College, US LIBOR MIKEŠ Charles University, Faculty of Science, Prague, CZ DANIEL MŁOCICKI Medical University of Warsaw, PL MARIÁN VÁRADY Institute of Parasitology SAS, Košice, SK JAN VOTÝPKA Charles University, Faculty of Science, Prague, CZ GRZEGORZ ZALEŚNY Wroclaw University of Environmental and Life Sciences, PL TABLE OF CONTENTS Session I – Helminths: Diversity, Taxonomy and Ultrastructure……….......................……………1 Session II – Parasitology in Genomic, Immunology
    [Show full text]
  • An Exploration Into the Psychotherapeutic Needs of Males Who Have Been
    An Exploration into The Psychotherapeutic Needs of Males Who Have Been Sexually Abused by Their Biological Mother in Australia: A Qualitative Description Study Lucetta Eva Thomas A thesis submitted for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Faculty of Health The University of Canberra 2019 iii Abstract This thesis explores the experiences of males who have sought psychotherapeutic support for sexual abuse perpetrated by their biological mother in Australia, using a qualitative description research design. The research’s findings fill a gap in the existing body of sexual abuse knowledge, specifically regarding the requirements and needs of males who have been sexually abused by their mothers. The information in this thesis establishes recommendations for practitioners—whether sexual assault support workers, mental health nurses, relationship psychologists, medical doctors or psychiatrists—to use in an environment of limited resources for providing effective and appropriate support for maternally sexually abused males accessing their services. The sexual abuse experienced by these men when they were boys was often highly traumatic and, at times, extremely violent. The maternal sexual abuse has not only adversely affected their childhood, but their lives as adults. The research shines a light on gender stereotypes and myths of mothers as only gentle and caring nurturers and protectors of their children, and of males as only perpetrators of child sexual abuse. Important research outcomes include acknowledging the sexual abuse of boys by their biological mother and the therapeutic inclusion and comprehensive integration of this type of abuse into child abuse prevention—to protect boys from maternal sexual abuse in the future. vii Acknowledgements First, I acknowledge Greg, whose heartbreaking experience of abuse by his biological mother compelled me to undertake this necessary research.
    [Show full text]
  • Stop Your Dog from Eating Feces
    Stop Your Dog from Eating Feces Coprophagia is a nasty dog problem that dog owners hate. It does not make sense, as we feed them the best meals possible, and they chose to eat poop. Who figured? Eating feces issues are most common in puppies. However, it can be seen at any stage throughout a dog's life. For such a wide spread problem there hasn't been much research conducted into how to stop our dogs from eating poop. The good news is that there are many ways available to correct this nasty habit. Whichever method you try below, be consistent. You must enforce your strategy every time to be successful. This will soon be the new habit. So, why do dogs eat poop (dog or cat poop)? Let’s break them into two simple areas: 1. Behavioral – It’s either a habitual behavioral problem, or 2. Medical – There is an underlying medical issue. You can easily discount the medical issues by asking your vet to examine your dog. They have a battery of tests that will easily tell them if your dog has a deficiency that is causing them to need to eat feces. Keep your dog well vaccinated, as coprophagia will indeed cause other medical issues as expected from eating parasites resident in feces. Dogs eat their own poop because of the following reasons: - If a dog punished for defecating inside the house, he may on occasions eat his poop to "hide the evidence". - It tastes good to your dog - Sometimes anxiety causes them to do it – stress - Sometimes dogs develop this feces eating habit because they are copying the behavior of other dogs.
    [Show full text]