Attitudes De Se and Logophoricity

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Attitudes De Se and Logophoricity View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Doctoral Dissertations Dissertations and Theses July 2018 ATTITUDES DE SE AND LOGOPHORICITY Yangsook Park Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_2 Part of the Semantics and Pragmatics Commons, and the Syntax Commons Recommended Citation Park, Yangsook, "ATTITUDES DE SE AND LOGOPHORICITY" (2018). Doctoral Dissertations. 1263. https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_2/1263 This Open Access Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Dissertations and Theses at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. ATTITUDES DE SE AND LOGOPHORICITY A Dissertation Presented by YANGSOOK PARK Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Massachusetts Amherst in partial fulllment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY May 2018 Linguistics © Copyright by Yangsook Park 2018 All Rights Reserved ATTITUDES DE SE AND LOGOPHORICITY A Dissertation Presented By YANGSOOK PARK Approved as to style and content by: Rajesh Bhatt, Co-Chair Vincent Homer, Co-Chair Seth Cable, Member Margaret Speas, Member Alejandro Pérez Carballo, Member Joe Pater, Department Head Department of Linguistics ACKNOWLEDGMENTS It has been a long journey. I would not have been able to complete this dissertation without the help and support of many people. Although words cannot express my appreciation, let me at least try. First of all, I wish to thank Rajesh Bhatt and Vincent Homer, my co-chairs, for their invaluable guidance, encouragement, and patience during this whole process. This work has greatly beneted from the stimulating and inspirational conversations with them in our countless joint meetings. I feel fortunate to have them as my advisors and mentors. I am also grateful to the other members of my dissertation committee, Seth Cable, Peggy Speas, and Alejandro Pérez Carballo. Seth deserves my special thanks for his detailed comments and helpful discussions at every stage of my work on this and previous projects, as well as for his semantics seminars with various interesting topics. I thank Peggy for sharing her insights and knowledge on the subject of this study that inspired me a lot. I thank Alejandro for agreeing to be on my committee and raising important questions. I would like to thank other faculty members of the Department of Linguistics at UMass who I learned from, who I worked with, and who encouraged me over the years: Brian Dillon, Lyn Frazier, Lisa Green, Kyle Johnson, John Kingston, Angelika Kratzer, Magda Oiry, Barbara Partee, Tom Roeper, and Kristine Yu. I really appreciate all their help. I owe a special thanks to Brian, who taught me experimental methodologies and quan- titative data analysis, which led me to my current position. I wish to thank to Chisato Kitagawa for his kind encouragement and all the interesting discussions, as well as for providing me his Japanese judgments. For their assistance with administrative matters, iv many thanks are due to Kathy Adamczyk, Michelle McBride, and especially to Tom Max- eld, who always gave me valuable advice and help. I will miss the pleasant and friendly conversations with Tom. I am also deeply grateful to my teachers in the Department of Chinese Language and Literature and the Department of Linguistics at Seoul National University, who helped me to grow professionally and personally: Seongdo Heo, Heejeong Ko, Chungmin Lee, Kangjae Lee, Seungho Nam. I would not have been able to come to UMass without their help and inuence. I have met wonderful people at UMass who made all these years truly memorable. One of the best things that happened to me in Amherst was to meet my brilliant and funny friends, who shared laughter, tears, and lots of drinks with me: Eunjin Cho, Suyoung Choi, Bohyeong Kim, Junghwan Ko, Misun Lim, and Hyungjeong Oh. I also owe particular thanks to Sang-Im Lee, who professionally inspired and motivated me a lot and who I have personally felt like a real sister. Special thanks also go to my cohort members: Tracy Conner, Yelena Fainleib, Nicholas LaCara, and Aleksei Nazarov. I also thank the following fellow linguists: Elizabeth Bogal- Allbritten, Sakshi Bhatia, Thuy Bui, Isabelle Charnavel, Mike Clauss, Chris Davis, Amy Rose Deal, Ilaria Frana, Hsin-Lun Huang, Masashi Hashimoto, Rodica Ivan, Jyoti Iyer, Ste- fan Keine, Jaieun Kim, Petr Kusliy, Suzi Lima, Jon Ander Mendia, Jason Overfelt, Jérémy Pasquereau, Hazel Pearson, Presley Pizzo, Ethan Poole, Shayne Slogget, Brian Smith, Megan Somerday Ekaterina Vostrikova, and Andrew Weir. Needless to say, who made all this possible is my parents. Thank you, Mom and Dad, for your endless love and support and believing in me – I dedicate this work to you both. I also thank my brother and his lovely family for their encouragement. And nally, to Patryk, for everything. v ABSTRACT ATTITUDES DE SE AND LOGOPHORICITY MAY 2018 YANGSOOK PARK B.A., SEOUL NATIONAL UNIVERSITY M.A., SEOUL NATIONAL UNIVERSITY Ph.D., UNIVERSITY OF MASSACHUSETTS AMHERST Directed by: Professor Rajesh Bhatt and Professor Vincent Homer This dissertation investigates two related notions that represent attitudes and perspec- tives, de se attitudes and the notion of logophoricity, based on the case study of the long- distance reexive caki in Korean, which specically encodes attitudes towards oneself in attitude reports and co-refers with individuals whose point of view or perspective is represented in a sentence. The main claim of this thesis is that there are distinct but related semantic/syntactic mechanisms for the obligatory de se interpretation of the long-distance reexive caki on the one hand, and the long-distance binding of caki on the other. Specically, for the semantics of the de se, I provide a unied account of the obligatory de se interpretation of the long-distance reexive caki and (null and overt) controlled subjects, based upon Lewis (1979) and Chierchia (1989). For the long-distance binding of caki in both attitude vi and non-attitude environments, I take a local logophoric binding approach (Koopman and Sportiche 1989, Adesola 2005, Anand 2006, Nishigauchi and Kishida 2008, Sundare- san 2012, Nishigauchi 2014, Charnavel and Zlogar 2016, a.o.). Based on the idea that caki is uniformly bound by a local logophoric binder in both attitude and non-attitude envi- ronments, I further propose the link between de se and logophoric binding mechanisms. Our two-layered system of de se and logophoric binding ensures that the antecedent of caki must be the perspective holder of a clause containing caki, while caki denotes the de se counterpart of its antecedent under attitude verbs. vii TABLE OF CONTENTS Page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS . iv ABSTRACT . vi LIST OF TABLES . xiii LIST OF FIGURES . xiv LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS . xvi CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION . 1 1.1 Two main interests: de se ascription and logophoricity . 1 1.1.1 Attitude reports . 1 1.1.2 The notion of logophoricity . 5 1.1.3 The link between de se attitude reports and logophoricity . 7 1.2 A case study of the Korean long-distance reexive caki . 10 1.2.1 Novel observations . 10 1.2.2 Central claims . 14 1.3 Overview of the dissertation . 15 2 BACKGROUND ON LONG-DISTANCE ANAPHORS . 18 viii 2.1 Introduction . 18 2.2 Long-distance anaphors . 18 2.3 The long-distance reexive caki in Korean . 21 2.3.1 Basic properties of caki ......................... 21 2.3.2 Local binding of caki .......................... 25 2.3.3 Local vs. long-distance caki ...................... 29 2.4 Conclusion . 33 3 DE SE ATTITUDE REPORTS . 34 3.1 Introduction . 34 3.2 Background on de se attitude reports . 35 3.3 The long-distance reexive caki in attitude reports . 39 3.3.1 No locality restriction . 40 3.3.2 (Non-)Subject-orientation . 41 3.3.3 Obligatory coreference between multiple caki’s . 43 3.3.4 Sloppy readings under ellipsis . 49 3.4 Obligatory de se interpretations of caki .................... 50 3.4.1 Obligatory de se in CP complements of attitude verbs . 51 3.4.2 Obligatory de se in NP complements of attitude verbs . 59 3.4.3 Obligatory de se under negative attitude verbs . 60 3.5 Deriving obligatory de se interpretations of caki . 63 3.5.1 A dedicated de se LF: The Lewis-Chierchia’s property approach . 64 3.5.2 A proposal . 72 3.5.2.1 Basic assumptions . 72 3.5.2.2 Two licensing conditions for caki . 75 3.5.3 Consequences . 78 3.5.4 Summary . 82 3.6 Comparison with previous accounts . 83 ix 3.6.1 The indexical approach . 83 3.6.2 The de se-as-a-special-de re analysis . 90 3.6.2.1 The concept-generator analysis: Percus and Sauerland (2003) . 93 3.6.2.2 Landau (2015): De se-as-a-special-de re . 98 3.7 An extension: Caki in other attitude environments . 101 3.7.1 Attitudes without attitude verbs . 102 3.7.2 Backward binding . 103 3.7.3 Summary . 107 3.8 Conclusion . 108 4 INTERACTIONS BETWEEN DE SE ELEMENTS IN CONTROL CONSTRUC- TIONS . 110 4.1 Introduction . 110 4.2 Basic properties of obligatory control constructions in Korean . 111 4.2.1 Obligatory control . 111 4.2.2 Logophoric control . 115 4.3 Overt controlled subjects in Korean . 118 4.3.1 Pronouns and caki as controlled subjects . 118 4.3.2 Controlled vs. non-controlled pronouns/reexives . 121 4.3.3 Controlled caki vs. OC PRO . 122 4.3.4 Summary . 124 4.4 OC PRO vs.
Recommended publications
  • Logophoricity in Finnish
    Open Linguistics 2018; 4: 630–656 Research Article Elsi Kaiser* Effects of perspective-taking on pronominal reference to humans and animals: Logophoricity in Finnish https://doi.org/10.1515/opli-2018-0031 Received December 19, 2017; accepted August 28, 2018 Abstract: This paper investigates the logophoric pronoun system of Finnish, with a focus on reference to animals, to further our understanding of the linguistic representation of non-human animals, how perspective-taking is signaled linguistically, and how this relates to features such as [+/-HUMAN]. In contexts where animals are grammatically [-HUMAN] but conceptualized as the perspectival center (whose thoughts, speech or mental state is being reported), can they be referred to with logophoric pronouns? Colloquial Finnish is claimed to have a logophoric pronoun which has the same form as the human-referring pronoun of standard Finnish, hän (she/he). This allows us to test whether a pronoun that may at first blush seem featurally specified to seek [+HUMAN] referents can be used for [-HUMAN] referents when they are logophoric. I used corpus data to compare the claim that hän is logophoric in both standard and colloquial Finnish vs. the claim that the two registers have different logophoric systems. I argue for a unified system where hän is logophoric in both registers, and moreover can be used for logophoric [-HUMAN] referents in both colloquial and standard Finnish. Thus, on its logophoric use, hän does not require its referent to be [+HUMAN]. Keywords: Finnish, logophoric pronouns, logophoricity, anti-logophoricity, animacy, non-human animals, perspective-taking, corpus 1 Introduction A key aspect of being human is our ability to think and reason about our own mental states as well as those of others, and to recognize that others’ perspectives, knowledge or mental states are distinct from our own, an ability known as Theory of Mind (term due to Premack & Woodruff 1978).
    [Show full text]
  • Minimal Pronouns, Logophoricity and Long-Distance Reflexivisation in Avar
    Minimal pronouns, logophoricity and long-distance reflexivisation in Avar* Pavel Rudnev Revised version; 28th January 2015 Abstract This paper discusses two morphologically related anaphoric pronouns inAvar (Avar-Andic, Nakh-Daghestanian) and proposes that one of them should be treated as a minimal pronoun that receives its interpretation from a λ-operator situated on a phasal head whereas the other is a logophoric pro- noun denoting the author of the reported event. Keywords: reflexivity, logophoricity, binding, syntax, semantics, Avar 1 Introduction This paper has two aims. One is to make a descriptive contribution to the crosslin- guistic study of long-distance anaphoric dependencies by presenting an overview of the properties of two kinds of reflexive pronoun in Avar, a Nakh-Daghestanian language spoken natively by about 700,000 people mostly living in the North East Caucasian republic of Daghestan in the Russian Federation. The other goal is to highlight the relevance of the newly introduced data from an understudied lan- guage to the theoretical debate on the nature of reflexivity, long-distance anaphora and logophoricity. The issue at the heart of this paper is the unusual character of theanaphoric system in Avar, which is tripartite. (1) is intended as just a preview with more *The present material was presented at the Utrecht workshop The World of Reflexives in August 2011. I am grateful to the workshop’s audience and participants for their questions and comments. I am indebted to Eric Reuland and an anonymous reviewer for providing valuable feedback on the first draft, as well as to Yakov Testelets for numerous discussions of anaphora-related issues inAvar spanning several years.
    [Show full text]
  • Pronouns, Logical Variables, and Logophoricity in Abe Author(S): Hilda Koopman and Dominique Sportiche Source: Linguistic Inquiry, Vol
    MIT Press Pronouns, Logical Variables, and Logophoricity in Abe Author(s): Hilda Koopman and Dominique Sportiche Source: Linguistic Inquiry, Vol. 20, No. 4 (Autumn, 1989), pp. 555-588 Published by: MIT Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/4178645 Accessed: 22-10-2015 18:32 UTC Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/ info/about/policies/terms.jsp JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. MIT Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Linguistic Inquiry. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 128.97.27.20 on Thu, 22 Oct 2015 18:32:27 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions Hilda Koopman Pronouns, Logical Variables, Dominique Sportiche and Logophoricity in Abe 1. Introduction 1.1. Preliminaries In this article we describe and analyze the propertiesof the pronominalsystem of Abe, a Kwa language spoken in the Ivory Coast, which we view as part of the study of pronominalentities (that is, of possible pronominaltypes) and of pronominalsystems (that is, of the cooccurrence restrictionson pronominaltypes in a particulargrammar). Abe has two series of thirdperson pronouns. One type of pronoun(0-pronoun) has basically the same propertiesas pronouns in languageslike English. The other type of pronoun(n-pronoun) very roughly corresponds to what has been called the referential use of pronounsin English(see Evans (1980)).It is also used as what is called a logophoric pronoun-that is, a particularpronoun that occurs in special embedded contexts (the logophoric contexts) to indicate reference to "the person whose speech, thought or perceptions are reported" (Clements (1975)).
    [Show full text]
  • Toward a Shared Syntax for Shifted Indexicals and Logophoric Pronouns
    Toward a Shared Syntax for Shifted Indexicals and Logophoric Pronouns Mark Baker Rutgers University April 2018 Abstract: I argue that indexical shift is more like logophoricity and complementizer agreement than most previous semantic accounts would have it. In particular, there is evidence of a syntactic requirement at work, such that the antecedent of a shifted “I” must be a superordinate subject, just as the antecedent of a logophoric pronoun or the goal of complementizer agreement must be. I take this to be evidence that the antecedent enters into a syntactic control relationship with a null operator in all three constructions. Comparative data comes from Magahi and Sakha (for indexical shift), Yoruba (for logophoric pronouns), and Lubukusu (for complementizer agreement). 1. Introduction Having had an office next to Lisa Travis’s for 12 formative years, I learned many things from her that still influence my thinking. One is her example of taking semantic notions, such as aspect and event roles, and finding ways to implement them in syntactic structure, so as to advance the study of less familiar languages and topics.1 In that spirit, I offer here some thoughts about how logophoricity and indexical shift, topics often discussed from a more or less semantic point of view, might have syntactic underpinnings—and indeed, the same syntactic underpinnings. On an impressionistic level, it would not seem too surprising for logophoricity and indexical shift to have a common syntactic infrastructure. Canonical logophoricity as it is found in various West African languages involves using a special pronoun inside the finite CP complement of a verb to refer to the subject of that verb.
    [Show full text]
  • A Syntactic Analysis of Rhetorical Questions
    A Syntactic Analysis of Rhetorical Questions Asier Alcázar 1. Introduction* A rhetorical question (RQ: 1b) is viewed as a pragmatic reading of a genuine question (GQ: 1a). (1) a. Who helped John? [Mary, Peter, his mother, his brother, a neighbor, the police…] b. Who helped John? [Speaker follows up: “Well, me, of course…who else?”] Rohde (2006) and Caponigro & Sprouse (2007) view RQs as interrogatives biased in their answer set (1a = unbiased interrogative; 1b = biased interrogative). In unbiased interrogatives, the probability distribution of the answers is even.1 The answer in RQs is biased because it belongs in the Common Ground (CG, Stalnaker 1978), a set of mutual or common beliefs shared by speaker and addressee. The difference between RQs and GQs is thus pragmatic. Let us refer to these approaches as Option A. Sadock (1971, 1974) and Han (2002), by contrast, analyze RQs as no longer interrogative, but rather as assertions of opposite polarity (2a = 2b). Accordingly, RQs are not defined in terms of the properties of their answer set. The CG is not invoked. Let us call these analyses Option B. (2) a. Speaker says: “I did not lie.” b. Speaker says: “Did I lie?” [Speaker follows up: “No! I didn’t!”] For Han (2002: 222) RQs point to a pragmatic interface: “The representation at this level is not LF, which is the output of syntax, but more abstract than that. It is the output of further post-LF derivation via interaction with at least a sub part of the interpretational component, namely pragmatics.” RQs are not seen as syntactic objects, but rather as a post-syntactic interface phenomenon.
    [Show full text]
  • Intensifiers, Reflexivity and Logophoricity in Axaxdərə Akhvakh
    Conference on the Languages of the Caucasus Leipzig, 07 – 09 December 2007 Intensifiers, reflexivity and logophoricity in Axaxdərə Akhvakh Denis CREISSELS Université Lumière (Lyon2) e-mail: [email protected] 1. Introduction Akhvakh is a Nakh-Daghestanian language belonging to the Andic branch of the Avar-ADDic-Tsezic family, spoken in the western part of Daghestan and in the village of Axaxdərə near Zaqatala (Azerbaijan). The variety of Akhvakh spoken in Axaxdərə (henceforth AD Akhvakh) is very close to the Northern Akhvakh varieties spoken in the Axvaxskij Rajon of Daghestan (henceforth AR Akhvakh), presented in Magomedbekova 1967 and Magomedova & Abdullaeva 2007. AD Akhvakh shows no particular affinity with any of the Southern Akhvakh dialects spoken in three villages (Cegob, Ratlub and Tljanub) of the Šamil’kij Rajon (formerly Sovetskij Rajon). The analysis of Akhvakh intensifiers, reflexives and logophorics proposed in this paper is entirely based on a corpus of narrative texts I collected in Axaxdərə between June 2005 and June 2007.1 I will be concerned here by the uses of the pronoun ži-CL (CL = class marker) in its simple form and in the form enlarged by the addition of the intensifying particle -da. The use of identical or related forms in intensifying, reflexive, and logophoric functions is attested in many languages of the world, and pronouns cognate with Akhvakh ži-CL fulfilling similar functions are found in the other Andic languages, but in some details of its use, Akhvakh ži-CL shows features which deserve to be examined. The paper is organized as follows. Section 2 summarizes basic information about Akhvakh morphosyntax.
    [Show full text]
  • A Morphosyntactic Investigation of Nominal Possession in Tigrinya
    University of Calgary PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository Graduate Studies The Vault: Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2013-05-28 Projecting possessors: A Morphosyntactic Investigation of Nominal Possession in Tigrinya Gebregziabher, Keffyalew Gebregziabher, K. (2013). Projecting possessors: A Morphosyntactic Investigation of Nominal Possession in Tigrinya (Unpublished doctoral thesis). University of Calgary, Calgary, AB. doi:10.11575/PRISM/27453 http://hdl.handle.net/11023/741 doctoral thesis University of Calgary graduate students retain copyright ownership and moral rights for their thesis. You may use this material in any way that is permitted by the Copyright Act or through licensing that has been assigned to the document. For uses that are not allowable under copyright legislation or licensing, you are required to seek permission. Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY Projecting possessors: A Morphosyntactic Investigation of Nominal Possession in Tigrinya By Keffyalew Gebregziabher A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY DEPARTMENT OF LINGUISTICS CALGARY, ALBERTA May 2013 © Keffyalew Gebregziabher 2013 Abstract In this dissertation, I examine the grammatical expression of possession in Tigrinya, a lesser-studied Semitic language of Ethiopia and Eritrea. I show that possession in Tigrinya is encoded by two strategies, which differ in both structure and function: (i) PREDICATIONAL STRATEGY has the particle nay and is used for alienable possession; (ii) ARGUMENTAL STRATEGY or BARE POSSESSION has no nay and is used for inalienable possession. To account for such differences, I propose different treatments for both types of possession.
    [Show full text]
  • Long-Distance Reflexivization and Logophoricity in the Dargin Language Muminat Kerimova Florida International University
    Florida International University FIU Digital Commons MA in Linguistics Final Projects College of Arts, Sciences & Education 2017 Long-Distance Reflexivization and Logophoricity in the Dargin Language Muminat Kerimova Florida International University Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/linguistics_ma Part of the Linguistics Commons Recommended Citation Kerimova, Muminat, "Long-Distance Reflexivization and Logophoricity in the Dargin Language" (2017). MA in Linguistics Final Projects. 3. https://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/linguistics_ma/3 This work is brought to you for free and open access by the College of Arts, Sciences & Education at FIU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in MA in Linguistics Final Projects by an authorized administrator of FIU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. FLORIDA INTERNATIONAL UNIVERSITY Miami, Florida LONG-DISTANCE REFLEXIVIZATION AND LOGOPHORICITY IN THE DARGIN LANGUAGE A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in LINGUISTICS by Muminat Kerimova 2017 ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS LONG-DISTANCE REFLEXIVIZATION AND LOGOPHORICITY IN THE DARGIN LANGUAGE by Muminat Kerimova Florida International University, 2017 Miami, Florida Professor Ellen Thompson, Major Professor The study of anaphora challenges us to determine the conditions under which the pronouns of a language are associated with possible antecedents. One of the theoretical questions is whether the distribution of pronominal forms is best explained by a syntactic, semantic or discourse level analysis. A more practical question is how we distinguish between anaphoric elements, e.g. what are the borders between the notions of pronouns, locally bound reflexives and long-distance reflexives? The study analyzes the anaphora device saj in Dargin that is traditionally considered to be a long-distance reflexivization language.
    [Show full text]
  • UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations
    UCLA UCLA Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Heterogeneity and uniformity in the evidential domain Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/40m5f2f1 Author Korotkova, Natalia Publication Date 2016 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UNIVERSITYOF CALIFORNIA Los Angeles Heterogeneity and uniformity in the evidential domain A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics by Natalia Korotkova 2016 © Copyright by Natalia Korotkova 2016 ABSTRACTOFTHE DISSERTATION Heterogeneity and uniformity in the evidential domain by Natalia Korotkova Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Los Angeles, 2016 Professor Dominique L. Sportiche, Co-chair Professor Yael Sharvit, Co-chair The dissertation is devoted to the formal mechanisms that govern the use of evidentials, expressions of natural language that denote the source of information for the proposition conveyed by a sentence. Specifically, I am concerned with putative cases of semantic variation in evidentiality and with its previously unnoticed semantic uniformity. An ongoing debate in this area concerns the relation between evidentiality and epistemic modality. According to one line of research, all evidentials are garden variety epistemic modals. According to another, evidentials across languages fall into two semantic classes: (i) modal evidentials; and (ii) illocutionary evidentials, which deal with the structure of speech acts. The dissertation provides a long-overdue discussion of analytical options proposed for evidentials, and shows that the debate is lacking formally-explicit tools that would differenti- ate between the two classes. Current theories, even though motivated by superficially different data, make in fact very similar predictions.
    [Show full text]
  • Logophoric Speech Is Not Indirect: Towards a Syntactic Approach To
    Linguistics 2021; 59(3): 609–633 Tatiana Nikitina* and Anna Bugaeva Logophoric speech is not indirect: towards a syntactic approach to reported speech constructions https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2021-0067 Received February 13, 2019; accepted April 8, 2020; published online April 21, 2021 Abstract: The distinction between direct and indirect speech has long been known not to reflect the crosslinguistic diversity of speech reporting strategies. Yet prominent typological approaches remain firmly grounded in that traditional distinction and look to place language-specific strategies on a universal contin- uum, treating them as deviations from the “direct” and “indirect” ideals.We argue that despite their methodological attractiveness, continuum approaches do not provide a solid basis for crosslinguistic comparison. We aim to present an alternative by exploring the syntax of logophoric speech, which has been commonly treated in the literature as representative of “semi-direct” discourse. Based on data from two unrelated languages, Wan (Mande) and Ainu (isolate), we show that certain varieties of logophoric speech share a number of syntactic properties with direct speech, and none with indirect speech. Many of the properties of indirect speech that are traditionally described in terms of perspective follow from its syntactically subordinate status. Constructions involving direct and logophoric speech, on the other hand, belong to a separate, universal type of structure. Our findings suggest that the alleged direct/indirect continuum conflates
    [Show full text]
  • Publ 23523 Issue 5142 Page
    Logophoricity and Locality: A View from French Anaphors Isabelle Charnavel In many unrelated languages, the same anaphor is either subject to Condition A of the binding theory, or exempt from it but with specific interpretive properties. On the basis of French data and crosslinguistic comparisons, I first show that such exempt anaphors must be anteceded by logophoric centers. Elaborating on but modifying Sells 1987, I provide specific tests to argue that these logophoric antecedents can be classified into two kinds of perspective centers, attitude holders and empathy loci, thus reducing logophoricity to mental perspective. Next, I propose to derive the logophoricity of exempt anaphors from the following hypothesis: seemingly exempt anaphors are in fact bound by silent logophoric pronouns introduced by syntactically represented logophoric operators within their local domain. Crucially, this hypoth- esis, which is independently supported by exhaustive coreference con- straints on locally cooccurring exempt anaphors, also accounts for their apparent exemption from Condition A, reanalyzed here as local binding by a silent logophoric binder. Keywords: binding theory, Condition A, anaphor, exemption, logo- phoricity, perspective In a wide range of languages, the very same element displays two distinct behaviors: sometimes, it is subject to the locality conditions imposed by Condition A of the binding theory (e.g., Chomsky 1986); sometimes, it is exempt from them. In its former guise, I will call such an element a plain anaphor (as in Charnavel and Sportiche 2016) and in its latter guise, an exempt anaphor (as in Pollard and Sag 1992). Examples of this type of element are English himself (e.g., Pollard and Sag 1992), French lui-meˆme and son propre (e.g., Charnavel and Sportiche 2016), Icelandic sig (e.g., Maling 1984), Mandarin ziji (e.g., Huang and Liu 2001), Japanese zibun (e.g., Kuroda 1973), and Turkish kendi si and Uyghur o¨z (Major and O¨ zkan 2018), among many others.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 16 Logophoricity in Ibibio Lydia Newkirk Rutgers University
    Chapter 16 Logophoricity in Ibibio Lydia Newkirk Rutgers University This paper presents a description and analysis of logophoric pronouns in Ibibio. I show that Ibibio logophors, although they behave in most respects like typical logophoric pronouns in West African languages, obey Shift Together like shifted indexicals. In order to explain this data I propose that Ibibio logophors are sensitive to two operators in the left periphery of the embedded CP: a shifting operator and a logophoric binding operator. Ibibio indexicals (which do not shift) differ in that they are defined to be insensitive to shifting operators. Thus indexical shift requires cooperation between the semantics of the indexical and of the shifting operator. This proposal in turn expands the predicted typology of possible de se pronouns cross-linguistically. 1 Ibibio logophors Ibibio (Cross-River, Nigeria) logophors are distinct from both ordinary pronouns and reflexives (1), and can only occur in embedded clauses (2).1 (1) a. Ekpe푖 a-bo ke (imọ)푖 ì-ma í-to Udo logophor Ekpe 3sg-say C log log-pst log-hit Udo ‘Ekpe푖 says that he푖 hit Udo.’ b. Ekpe푖 a-bo ke anye푖/푘 a-diyọn̄ọ ikwo ikwo mfọnmfọn pronoun Ekpe 3sg-say C 3sg 3sg-know sing song well ‘Ekpe푖 says that he푖/푘 sings well.’ c. Ekpe푖 a-bo ke ì-ma í-tọ idem푖 reflexive Ekpe 3sg-say C log-pst log-hit self ‘Ekpe푖 says that he푖 hit himself푖’ 1With the exception of the logophoric markers, on which I annotated the tone for clarity, I use the Ibibio orthography developed in Essien (1990), which does not mark tone.
    [Show full text]