Of Sexual Dichromatism and Bright Plumage Coloration Has Been The

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Of Sexual Dichromatism and Bright Plumage Coloration Has Been The Vol. 148,No. 3 The AmericanNaturalist September1996 CAROTENOIDS AND SEXUAL DICHROMATISM IN NORTH AMERICAN PASSERINE BIRDS DAVID A. GRAY* Departmentof Biology,167 Castetter Hall, Universityof New Mexico,Albuquerque, New Mexico87131 SubmittedFebruary 3, 1995; Revised November 9, 1995; Accepted November 17, 1995 Abstract.-Sexualdichromatism and the extent of male carotenoid-derived plumage coloration in breedingseason NorthAmerican passerine birds was analyzedby phylogeneticstatistical methods.Passerines as a wholeand five passerine subclades were analyzed by both independent contrastsand simulationmethods. In passerinesas a whole,carotenoids and sexualdichroma- tismare positivelycorrelated irrespective of analysismethod. In threeof the subclades,the correlationsare significant by a clearmajority of analysis methods. Neither melanin nor structur- allyderived colors show similar significant increases with dichromatism. Carotenoids are ob- tainedby animals solely through the diet, whereas both melanins and structurally derived colors can be synthesized.The relationshipbetween sexual dichromatism and theuse of carotenoids in plumagesuggests that sexual selection may have promotedthe expression of a condition- dependenthonest indicator of phenotypic quality. In birdswith nuptial display feathers, carotenoids undoubtedly play a positive r6lewhich is possiblya truesexual function. (GOODWIN 1952, p. 267) The function(s)of sexual dichromatism and bright plumage coloration has been thefocus of much research and debate (Darwin 1871; Sibley 1957; Selander 1972; Bakerand Parker 1979; Hamilton and Zuk 1982).Largely missing from this debate has been a considerationof the biochemicalbasis of plumagecoloration. The majorityof avian plumagecoloration is producedby one, or a combinationof several,of fourmajor types of colordeterminants. Three of thesefour are pig- ments,namely carotenoids, melanins, and porphyrins;the fourth is due to the physicalscattering of lightby microscopicairspaces within the keratinof the featheritself and by reflectionfrom the featherbarbules-so-called structural coloration(Fox 1976;Brush 1978). In general,white, blue, and iridescent plumage colorationis producedstructurally (Fox 1976;Brush 1978); bright red, orange, and yellowplumage coloration is due to carotenoidpigments (Fox and Vevers 1960;Fox 1976;Brush 1978; Hudon and Brush1992); brown, black, gray, buff, and dullbrownish red, dull orange, and dullyellowish plumage coloration is due to melaninpigments (Fox 1976;Brush 1978). Porphyrins produce colors ranging frombrown to blue, green,and red. Althoughwidely distributed among avian taxa,porphyrins tend to be restrictedto feathersor partsof feathers not exposed * E-mail:[email protected]. Am. Nat. 1996. Vol. 148, pp. 453-480. ? 1996 by The Universityof Chicago. 0003-0147/96/4803-0002$02.00.All rightsreserved. This content downloaded from 130.166.34.126 on Thu, 10 Sep 2015 17:16:07 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions 454 THE AMERICANNATURALIST to directsunlight (Brush 1978),for example, feather shafts of youngpigeons (Fox 1976). Exceptionsare owls (Strigidae),bustards (Otididae), and turacos (Musophagidae).As thepresent analysis is restrictedto passerines,and porphy- rinstend to be restrictedto unexposedfeather areas, porphyrins are notconsid- eredfurther here. Carotenoidsare synthesizedonly in plants, algae, and somebacteria and fungi. Althoughmany animals can modifythe structureof ingestedcarotenoids, no animalshave been shownto synthesizethem (Fox and Vevers 1960;Fox 1976, 1979; Brush 1978); therefore,all carotenoidsfound within animals must have been obtainedthrough the diet. Because ofthis, a condition-dependentsignaling role forcarotenoids has been suggestedby severalauthors (Kodric-Brown and Brown1984; Andersson 1986; Hill 1990;Zuk 1992).Melanins are producedin animalsby the catabolism of the amino acids tyrosine, tryptophan, and phenylala- nine(Fox 1976;Brush 1978). Anabolic and catabolicpathways can interconvert proteinsand theseamino acids (Fox 1976).For thisreason, melanins are less likelythan carotenoids to be limited,honest indicators of phenotypicquality. Structuralcolors are producedby propertiesof thefeather, as previouslynoted. Featherstructure abnormalities are infrequentand usuallylethal in wild birds (Harrison1985). For thisreason, structural colors, in and ofthemselves, are less likely than carotenoid-derivedcolors to honestlyindicate phenotypic qual- ity,except perhaps extreme (i.e., usuallyfatal) nutritional, disease, or genetic stress. The basis ofavian plumage coloration suggests that carotenoid-derived colors, in and of themselves,have thepotential to honestlyreflect phenotypic quality, whereasmelanin and structurallyderived coloration may do so if coupledwith an additionalhonesty-enforcing mechanism. This is notto arguethat traits that are synthesizedendogenously are necessarilypoor indicators of phenotypic qual- ity.What is beingargued, however, is thatcarotenoid pigments by theirnature havegreater potential to be honestindicators of phenotypic quality than do either melaninsor structuralcolors in the absence of additional honesty-enforcing mech- anisms.Clearly many synthesized traits do reflectphenotypic quality, for exam- ple, the tail of the male peafowl,Pavo cristatus(Petrie 1994), the size of the combof redjungle fowl, Gallus gallus (Ligon et al. 1990;Zuk et al. 1990a),and the size and symmetryof severalfeather ornaments (M0ller and Pomiankowski 1993). Synthesizedtraits that do honestlyreflect phenotypic quality, however, mostlikely will require an additionalmechanism to enforcehonesty, for example, strongopposing natural selection, that is, handicaptraits (Zahavi 1975,1991), or testosterone-mediatedexpression (Folstad and Karter1992). For example,the badgeof the male house sparrow, Passer domesticus,is producedby melanin.It acts as a signalof social dominanceand status(M0ller 1987a). However,so far as anyoneknows, the honestyof thebadge as a signalis unrelatedto thefact thatit is producedby melanin.Instead, honesty is maintainedthrough social interaction(M0ller 1987b). Testosterone-mediated trait expression seems an un- likelyhonesty-enforcing mechanism for avian plumage, as male-typeplumage in most species of birdsdevelops from the absence of estrogenrather than the presenceof testosterone(see Owensand Short1995). This content downloaded from 130.166.34.126 on Thu, 10 Sep 2015 17:16:07 UTC All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions CAROTENOIDSAND SEXUALDICHROMATISM 455 If sexualselection, either by male-male competition or byfemale choice, oper- ates on the basis of condition-dependenthonest indicators of quality(Zahavi 1975,1991; Andersson 1982, 1986; Kodric-Brown and Brown1984; Grafen 1990), thencarotenoid, but notnecessarily melanin or structural,coloration would be expectedto increasein lineageswith increased sexual selection.To test this hypothesis,I lookedfor interspecific correlations between sexual dichromatism and theproportion of carotenoid, melanin, and structurallyderived plumage col- oration.Specifically, the hypothesis predicts positive correlations between carot- enoid-derivedplumage coloration and sexualdichromatism. This argumentassumes that sexual dichromatism reflects the accumulated ef- fectsof sexualselection rather than other selective pressures (Darwin 1871; Sel- ander1972; Baker and Parker 1979; M0ller and Birkhead 1994). It is also assumed thatthe biochemical origin of plumagecolors can reliablybe inferredfrom the color itselfwithout requiring direct biochemical evidence for all species. This assumptionmay seem tenuous at best;however, Hudon and Brush(1992, p. 319) state,"Carotenoids produce the majority of bright yellow, orange, and redhues foundin birds . plumage color is a good indicatorof the kinds of carotenoids deposited."That is, notonly can thepresence of carotenoids reliably be inferred fromplumage color, but the specifictypes of carotenoidsmay also be inferred. Known exceptionsinclude hummingbird (Trochilidae) gorgets, which are pro- duced structurally,parrot (Psittaciiformes) red and yellowfeathers, which have an unknown,but probably noncarotenoid, color origin (Hudon and Brush1992), and the greenand red pigmentsof turacos(Musophagidae), which derive from theporphyrins turacoverdin and turacin(Fox 1976). An importantdifference between this study and previousstudies of theevolu- tionarysignificance of avian colorationis the analysisof dichromatismand the underlyingbiochemical nature of coloration. This approach has at leasttwo major advantages.First the conceptsof "conspicuousness"and "brightness"are avoided.This is a significantimprovement over previous analyses because con- spicuousnessand brightnessare subjectiveand dependenton ecologicallyrele- vantenvironmental conditions. In addition,as humansalmost universally per- ceive carotenoidcoloration as bright,correlating carotenoid coloration with "brightness"would be tautological.Second, the resultsare morelikely to be independentof differences between human and avian sensory systems, which are greaterthan previously appreciated (Bennett et al. 1994). METHODS Data Collection NorthAmerican passerines for which information was availablein a standard fieldguide (National Geographic Society 1987) and in a technicalbird-banding guide(Pyle et al. 1987)and forwhich phylogenetic information was availablein Sibleyand Ahlquist(1990) were used. Each of 12 plumageregions was scored forsexual dichromatism (i.e., dichromaticor notdichromatic) and probable color origin(i.e., carotenoid,melanin,
Recommended publications
  • Prevalence of Color Blindness Among Students of Four Basic Schools in Koya City
    Journal of Advanced Laboratory Research in Biology E-ISSN: 0976-7614 Volume 10, Issue 2, April 2019 PP 31-34 https://e-journal.sospublication.co.in Research Article Prevalence of Color Blindness among Students of four basic schools in Koya City Harem Othman Smail*, Evan Mustafa Ismael and Sayran Anwer Ali Department of Biology, Koya University, University Park, Danielle Mitterrand Boulevard, Koysinjaq, Kurdistan Region –Iraq. Abstract: Color blindness or color vision deficiency is X-linked recessive disorder that affects males more frequently than females. Abnormality in any one or all three cone photoreceptors caused Congenital disorders. Protanopia, deuteranopia results when long wavelength (L), photopigments (red), middle wavelength (M) and photopigments (green) are missing. This cross-sectional study was done to find out the prevalence of color vision deficiency among basic school students in Koya city with different ages and genders. The study was conducted in four basic schools that were present in Koya city (Zheen, Zanst, Nawroz and Najibaxan). All students screened by using Ishihara 24 plates. For the study (n=400, male=206, female=194, age=8-14) were selected & examined. The result revealed that the prevalence rate of the deficiency in four primary schools 3.39% (7) in males and 0% females. The study concluded that color blindness is different between students in each school, cannot find the prevalence of Color blindness in females in each school and affects males more than females because color blindness is an X-linked recessive disorder. The aim of the study was to determine the prevalence of color blindness in some basic schools in Koya city, Kurdistan Region/ Iraq.
    [Show full text]
  • Consequences of Color Vision Variation on Performance and Fitness in Capuchin Monkeys
    University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 2014 Consequences of color vision variation on performance and fitness in capuchin monkeys Andrea Theresa Green Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Green, Andrea Theresa, "Consequences of color vision variation on performance and fitness in capuchin monkeys" (2014). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 10766. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/10766 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected]. CONSEQUENCES OF COLOR VISION VARIATION ON PERFORMANCE AND FITNESS IN CAPUCHIN MONKEYS By ANDREA THERESA GREEN Masters of Arts, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, 2007 Bachelors of Science, Warren Wilson College, Asheville, NC, 1997 Dissertation Paper presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Organismal Biology and Ecology The University of Montana Missoula, MT May 2014 Approved by: Sandy Ross, Dean of The Graduate School Graduate School Charles H. Janson, Chair Division of Biological Sciences Erick Greene Division of Biological Sciences Doug J. Emlen Division of Biological Sciences Scott R. Miller Division of Biological Sciences Gerald H. Jacobs Psychological & Brain Sciences-UCSB UMI Number: 3628945 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted.
    [Show full text]
  • Color Dichromatism in Female American Redstarts.-Male American Redstarts (Setophaga Ruticilla) Are Easily Categorized by Plumage Into Yearlings (Subadults) and Adults
    SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 257 HUDSON, R. H., R. K. TUCKER, AND M. A. HAEGELE. 1984. Handbook of toxicity of pesticides to wildlife. 3rd ed. U.S.D.I. Fish Wildlife Serv. Resource Publ. 153. MANNAN, R. W., M. L. MORRISON, AND E. C. MESLOW. 1984. Comment: The use of guilds in forest bird management. Wildl. Sot. Bull. 12:426-430. MCCLELLAND, B. R. AND S. S. FRISSEL. 1975. Identifying forest snags useful for hole-nesting birds. J. For. 73:404-417. MCCOMB, W. C. AND R. N. MULLER. 1983. Snag densities in old-growth and second- growth Appalachian forests. J. Wildl. Manage. 47:376-382. MCPEEK, G. A. 1985. Decay patterns and bird use of snags created with herbicide injections and tree topping. M.S. thesis. Univ. Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky. MORIARTY, J. J. 1982. Long-term effects of timber stand improvement on snag and natural cavity characteristics and cavity use by vertebrates in a mixed mesophytic forest. M.S. thesis. Univ. Kentucky, Lexington, Kentucky. SAS INSTITUTE. 1982. SAS users’ guide: statistics, 1982 ed. SAS Institute, Cary, North Carolina. SEDGWICK, J. A. AND F. L. KNOPF. 1986. Cavity-nesting birds and the cavity-tree resource in plains cottonwood bottomlands. J. Wildl. Manage. 50:247-252. THOMAS, J. W., R. G. ANDERSON, C. MASER, AND E. L. BULL. 1979. Snags. Pp. 60-77 in Wildlife habitats in managed forests, the Blue Mountains of Oregon and Washington (J. W. Thomas, ed.). U.S.D.A. For. Serv. Agric. Handb. 553. U.S. FORESTSERVICE. 1984. Pesticide background statements. Vol. IV, Herbicides. U.S.D.A.
    [Show full text]
  • And Interspecific Contex
    Light matters: testing the “Light Environment Hypothesis” under intra- and interspecific contexts Angelica Hernandez-Palma School of Renewable Natural Resources, Louisiana State University Agricultural Center, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, Louisiana Keywords Abstract Dichromatism, Furnariides, model of color discrimination, species recognition, The “Light Environment Hypothesis” (LEH) proposes that evolution of inter- tetrahedral color space model, visual specific variation in plumage color is driven by variation in light environments signaling. across habitats. If ambient light has the potential to drive interspecific variation, a similar influence should be expected for intraspecific recognition, as color sig- Correspondence nals are an adaptive response to the change in ambient light levels in different Angelica Hernandez-Palma, School of habitats. Using spectrometry, avian-appropriate models of vision, and phyloge- Renewable Natural Resources, Louisiana State netic comparative methods, I quantified dichromatism and tested the LEH in University Agricultural Center, Louisiana State University, Room 227 RNR Building, Baton both intra- and interspecific contexts in 33 Amazonian species from the infraor- Rouge, Louisiana 70803. der Furnariides living in environments with different light levels. Although Tel: 225 578 4228; these birds are sexually monochromatic to humans, 81.8% of the species had at Fax: 225-578-4227; least one dichromatic patch in their plumage, mostly from dorsal areas, which E-mail: [email protected] provides evidence for a role for dichromatism in sex recognition. Furthermore, birds from habitats with high levels of ambient light had higher dichromatism Funding Information levels, as well as brighter, more saturated, and more diverse plumages, suggest- National Science Foundation (Grant/Award Number: ‘0545491’, ‘1257340’) ing that visual communication is less constrained in these habitats.
    [Show full text]
  • Division 1: Vision and Colour
    DIVISION 1: VISION AND COLOUR MINUTES of the 1st Meeting of the Luo Term COMMISSION INTERNATIONALE DE L'ECLAIRAGE Sunday 15 June 2008 INTERNATIONAL COMMISSION ON ILLUMINATION INTERNATIONALE BELEUCHTUNGSKOMMISSION Stockholm, Sweden ATTENDANCE Officers Ronnier Luo GB DD – Director Miyoshi Ayama JP AD Vision Ellen Carter US AD Colour Mike Pointer GB DS – Secretary Country Peter Hanselaer Belgium Representatives Juliana de Freitas Gomes Brazil Marjukka Eloholma Finland Françoise Viénot France Gerhard Rössler Germany Michael Pointer Great Britain Janos Schanda* Hungary Miyoshi Ayama* Japan Chang Kim Soon Korea (Republic) Peter van der Burgt Netherlands Thorstein Seim* Norway Elsie Coetzee South Africa Siv Lindberg Sweden Rengin Ünver Turkey Paula Alessi USA *Nominated representative Technical Paula Alessi US TC1-27 Committee Françoise Viénot FR TC1-36 Chairs Ken Sagawa JP TC1-37, TC1-54 Miyoshi Ayama JP TC1-42 Robert Hirshler HU TC1-44 Taiichiro Ishida JP TC1-61 Klaus Richter DE TC1-63 Janos Schanda HU TC1-66 Hiroyasu Ujike JP TC1-67 Peter Bodrogi HU TC1-68 Wendy Davis US TC1-69 Changjun Li CN TC1-71 Mike Pointer GB TC1-72 Reporters Mike Pointer GB R1-39 Boris Oicherman IS R1-41, R1-43 Changjun Li CN R1-42 Guests In addition there were approximately 8 guests present. Apologies Mike Brill TC1-56 Andrew Chalmers NZ Vibeke Clausen DK Gunilla Derefeldt R1-32 Marta K Gunde SI Jack Holm R1-40, L1-5 Eugenio Martinez-Uriegas TC1-60 Sharon McFadden TC1-64 Manuel Melgosa ES, TC1-55 Inna Nissenbaum IS Danny Rich L1-6 Alan Robertson TC1-57 Michael Stock L1-2 Ian Tutt L1-8 Klara Wenzel HU Joanne Zwinkels L1-3 Total attendance: Approximately 32 persons 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Color Vision
    Color Vision Ebony, and ivoryyyy March 30, 2020 PSYCH 4041 / 6014 Overview ØColor vision in nature ØColor mixture ØEffects ØTheories ØDefective color vision March 30, 2020 PSYCH 4041 / 6014 Questions Ø Why do we perceive blue dots when a yellow flash bulb goes off? Ø What does someone who is color-blind see? Ø What colors does a honeybee perceive? March 30, 2020 PSYCH 4041 / 6014 Color vision in nature ØUtility v Evolutionary advantages ØPrevalence v No clear phylogenetic trends v Primates -- good v Birds -- better v Fish -- better v Dogs -- worse March 30, 2020 PSYCH 4041 / 6014 Camouflage ØNatural March 30, 2020 PSYCH 4041 / 6014 Camouflage ØMan-made v Often imitate natural (but not always) March 30, 2020 PSYCH 4041 / 6014 Describing Color Ø Hue v Perception of wavelength • Why is the sky blue, anyway? • Why is the sky reddish at dusk? Note Martian sunset is blue • Rainbow, ROYGBIV & Newton Ø Brightness v Perception of intensity v Brightness/intensity relationship depends on hue (wavelength) v Similar to loudness/intensity depends on frequency v Bezold-Brucke shift: change in hue with intensity Ø Saturation v Perception of purity (like timbre) v A pure light is monochromatic March 30, 2020 PSYCH 4041 / 6014 Colors Ø Primary colors v Red, green, blue • Are these 3 colors special because of something in our visual system? • Why 3 primary colors? Why not 4, 5? Ø Secondary colors v Mixture of primary colors v Yellow, cyan, magenta (between two rainbows) v Brighter (two sets of cones stimulated) v Key for mixes, paints, printing (CYMK, not
    [Show full text]
  • Color Perceptions of Deuteranopic and Protanopic Observers by Deane B
    U. S. Department of Commerce Research Paper RP1922 National Bureau of Standards Volume 41# October 1948 Part of the Journal of Research of the National Bureau of Standards Color Perceptions of Deuteranopic and Protanopic Observers By Deane B. Judd It is well established that about 2 percent of otherwise normal human males are con- fusers of red and green from birth. There is considerable interest in the question: What do red-green confusers see? From a knowledge of the normal color perceptions corresponding to deuteranopic and protanopic red and green, we may not only understand better why color-blindness tests sometimes fail, and so be in a position to develop improved tests, but also the color-deficient observer may understand better the nature of his color-confusions and be aided to avoid their consequences. If an observer has trichromatic vision over a portion of his total retinal area, and dichromatic vision over another portion, he may give valid testimony regarding the color perceptions characteristic of the particular form of dichromatic vision possessed by him. Preeminent among such observers are those born with one normal eye and one dichromatic eye. A review of the rather considerable literature on this subject shows that the color perceptions of both protanopic and deuteranopic observers are confined to two hues, yellow and blue, closely like those perceived under usual conditions in the spectrum at 575 and 470 m/x, respectively, by normal observers. By combining this result with standard response functions recently derived (Bureau Research Paper RP1618) for protanopic and deuteranopic vision, it has been possible to give quantitative estimates of the color perceptions typical of these observers for the whole range of colors in the Munsell Book of Color.
    [Show full text]
  • Coatings for Toys Transport
    GLOSSARY OF ABBREVIATIONS 831 Coatings for toys Under a proposed new EEC directive [122], apart from physical, mechan­ ical and flammability constraints, toys will be subject to control of their chemical composition by limiting the bioavailability of a range of heavy metals (on a daily intake basis). The numerical relationship between the acceptable daily intake and the corresponding concentration of the rele­ vant components in the coating, is currently under discussion as a topic for a revision of European Standard EN 7l. This will have a knock-on effect on European national legislation once it achieves an adoptable form. Transport There are separate international recommendations for the transport of dangerous goods multimodally, by sea, by air, by road and by rail. These continue to be developed but there is evidence of increasing harmonisa­ tion with UN recommendations [65]. Mostyn has compiled a series of guides [123], bringing together the various route requirements for the main UN classes of dangerous goods. Light gauge metal packaging, made of steel or tin plate (thickness less than 0.5 mm) is extensively used in the conveyance of printing inks. From 1989 such packaging will be subject to regulations for sea, air, and international road/rail transport and will be required to pass performance tests and bear an official mark as evidence of compliance 1124]. GLOSSARY OF ABBREVIATIONS ACGIH American Conference of Governmental Industrial Hygienists ACOIN Australian Core Inventory ACTS Advisory Committee on Toxic Substances (to HSE) ADR Accord europeen relatif au transport international des merchandises Dangereuses par Route CEFIC European Council of Chemical Manufacturers' Federations CEPE European Committee of Paint, Printing Ink and Artist's Colour Manufacturers Associations CIA Chemical Industries Association CIMAH Control of Industrial Major Accident Hazards COSHH Control of Substances Hazardous to Health (proposals) CPL Classification, Packaging and Labelling of dangerous sub­ stances regulations (SI 1984, No.
    [Show full text]
  • The Bright Incubate at Night: Sexual Dichromatism and Adaptive
    The bright incubate at night: sexual rspb.royalsocietypublishing.org dichromatism and adaptive incubation division in an open-nesting shorebird Kasun B. Ekanayake1, Michael A. Weston1, Dale G. Nimmo1, Research Grainne S. Maguire2, John A. Endler3 and Clemens Ku¨pper4,† Cite this article: Ekanayake KB, Weston MA, 1Centre for Integrative Ecology, School of Life and Environmental Sciences, Faculty of Science, Engineering and Nimmo DG, Maguire GS, Endler JA, Ku¨pper C. Built Environment, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood, Victoria 3125, Australia 2015 The bright incubate at night: sexual 2BirdLife Australia, Suite 2-05, 60 Leicester Street, Carlton, Victoria 3053, Australia 3Centre for Integrative Ecology, School of Life and Environmental Sciences, Faculty of Science, Engineering and dichromatism and adaptive incubation division Built Environment, Deakin University, 75 Pigdons Road, Waurn Ponds, Victoria 3216, Australia in an open-nesting shorebird. Proc. R. Soc. B 4Department of Animal and Plant Sciences, University of Sheffield, Sheffield S10 2TN, UK 282: 20143026. KBE, 0000-0002-8777-8784; JAE, 0000-0002-7557-7627 http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.3026 Ornamentation of parents poses a high risk for offspring because it reduces cryptic nest defence. Over a century ago, Wallace proposed that sexual dichro- matism enhances crypsis of open-nesting females although subsequent studies Received: 12 December 2014 found that dichromatism per se is not necessarily adaptive. We tested whether Accepted: 13 March 2015 reduced female ornamentation in a sexually dichromatic species reduces the risk of clutch depredation and leads to adaptive parental roles in the red- capped plover Charadrius ruficapillus, a species with biparental incubation.
    [Show full text]
  • Social and Physiological Drivers of Rapid Colour Change in a Tropical Toad T ⁎ Katrina M
    General and Comparative Endocrinology 285 (2020) 113292 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect General and Comparative Endocrinology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ygcen Social and physiological drivers of rapid colour change in a tropical toad T ⁎ Katrina M. Gardnera, , Daniel J. Mennilla, Amy E.M. Newmanb, Stéphanie M. Douceta a Department of Integrative Biology, University of Windsor, Windsor, ON N9B 3P4, Canada b Department of Integrative Biology, University of Guelph, Guelph, ON N1G 2W1, Canada ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: Dynamic sexual dichromatism occurs when males and females differ in colouration for a limited time. Although Bufo luetkenii this trait has been primarily studied in cephalopods, chameleons, and fishes, recent analyses suggest that dy- Colour change namic dichromatism is prevalent among anurans and may be mediated through sexual selection and sex re- Corticosterone cognition. Yellow toads, Incilius luetkenii, exhibit dynamic dichromatism during explosive breeding events at the Dynamic sexual dichromatism onset of the rainy season: males change from a cryptic brown to a bright yellow and back again during the brief Incilius luetkenii mating event. We tested the hypothesis that dynamic dichromatism in yellow toads is influenced by conspecific Reflectance spectrometry Sexual selection interactions and mediated through sex hormones and stress hormones. We placed male toads into one of four Testosterone social treatments (with three other males, one male, one female, or no other toads). Immediately before and after Yellow toad each one-hour treatment, we quantified male colour with a reflectance spectrometer and we collected a blood sample to assess plasma concentrations of both testosterone and corticosterone. We found that males held with conspecific animals showed the brightest yellow colour and showed little or no change in their corticosterone levels.
    [Show full text]
  • Assessing Sexual Dicromatism: the Importance of Proper Parameterization in Tetrachromatic Visual Models
    University of Windsor Scholarship at UWindsor Biological Sciences Publications Department of Biological Sciences 1-11-2017 Assessing Sexual Dicromatism: The Importance of Proper Parameterization in Tetrachromatic Visual Models Pierre-Paul Bitton University of Windsor Kevyn Janisse Stéphanie M. Doucet Biological Sciences, University of Windsor Follow this and additional works at: https://scholar.uwindsor.ca/biologypub Part of the Biology Commons Recommended Citation Bitton, Pierre-Paul; Janisse, Kevyn; and Doucet, Stéphanie M., "Assessing Sexual Dicromatism: The Importance of Proper Parameterization in Tetrachromatic Visual Models" (2017). PLoS One, 12, 1, e0169810. https://scholar.uwindsor.ca/biologypub/126 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Biological Sciences at Scholarship at UWindsor. It has been accepted for inclusion in Biological Sciences Publications by an authorized administrator of Scholarship at UWindsor. For more information, please contact [email protected]. RESEARCH ARTICLE Assessing Sexual Dicromatism: The Importance of Proper Parameterization in Tetrachromatic Visual Models Pierre-Paul Bitton¤*, Kevyn Janisse, SteÂphanie M. Doucet Department of Biological Sciences, University of Windsor, Windsor, Ontario, Canada ¤ Current address: Institute of Evolution and Ecology, Eberhard Karls UniversitaÈt TuÈbingen, TuÈbingen, Germany * [email protected] a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 Abstract a1111111111 Perceptual models of animal vision have greatly contributed to our understanding of animal- animal and plant-animal communication. The receptor-noise model of color contrasts has been central to this research as it quantifies the difference between two colors for any visual system of interest. However, if the properties of the visual system are unknown, assump- OPEN ACCESS tions regarding parameter values must be made, generally with unknown consequences.
    [Show full text]
  • The Misuse of Colour in Science Communication ✉ Fabio Crameri 1 , Grace E
    PERSPECTIVE https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 OPEN The misuse of colour in science communication ✉ Fabio Crameri 1 , Grace E. Shephard 1 & Philip J. Heron2 The accurate representation of data is essential in science communication. However, colour maps that visually distort data through uneven colour gradients or are unreadable to those with colour-vision deficiency remain prevalent in science. These include, but are not limited to, rainbow-like and red–green colour maps. Here, we present a simple guide for the scientific use of colour. We show how scientifically derived colour maps report true data variations, 1234567890():,; reduce complexity, and are accessible for people with colour-vision deficiencies. We highlight ways for the scientific community to identify and prevent the misuse of colour in science, and call for a proactive step away from colour misuse among the community, publishers, and the press. ision is one of the most fundamental means of communication. It is (or should be) in Vevery scientist’s best intention to make figures and their content as accurate and easily understandable as possible. One of the most powerful aspects of images is colour, which in turn transforms information into meaning. The visual evaluation of a colour gradient is important to a variety of different fields such as the first direct impression of a black hole1, the mapping of votes cast in political elections2,3, the planning of an expensive rover route on Martian topography4, the essential communication of climate change5,6, or the critical diagnosis of heart disease7. However, when colours are used incorrectly, this can lead to the effective manipulation of data (e.g., by highlighting some data over others), the oversight of the needs of those with colour vision deficiencies, and the removal of meaning when printed in black and white (Supplementary Note 1).
    [Show full text]