Effects of Catecholamine Depletion on Alertness and Mood in Rested and Sleep Deprived Normal Volunteers Una D

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Effects of Catecholamine Depletion on Alertness and Mood in Rested and Sleep Deprived Normal Volunteers Una D NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1993-VOL. 8, NO.4 345 Effects of Catecholamine Depletion on Alertness and Mood in Rested and Sleep Deprived Normal Volunteers Una D. McCann, M.D., David M. Penetar, Ph.D., Yavin Shaham, Ph.D., DruidR. Thorne, Ph.D., Helen C. Sing, B.S., Maria L. Thomas, B.A., ,. Christian Gillin, M.D., and Gregory Belenky, M.D. Alpha·methyl-para-tyrosine (AMPT), a tyrosine In contrast, although combined treatment with AMPT kydroxylase inhibitor, was used to evaluate the and sleep deprivation led to large increases in negative ylrysiologic role of central nervous system catecholamines mood, neither treatment alone produced consistent mood • modulating alertness and mood. Forty healthy males changes. These findings are consistent with the view that 1m randomized to one of four conditions: AMPT in a sleep deprivation is associated with decreased functional sed condition; AMPT plus 40.5 hours of total sleep catecholamine neurotransmission. Furthermore, mood Itprivation; placebo plus sleep deprivation; or placebo in effects following sleep deprivation plus AMPT suggest • rrsted condition. Repeated measures of alertness and that catecholamines may be involved in mood changes mxl rroealed that treatment with AMPT or sleep during sleep deprivation. £Neuropsychopharmacology. Itprir1(ltion increased sleepiness, and combined treatment 8:345-356, 1993J "muced greater sleepiness than either treatment alone. DY WORDS: Sleep deprivation; Alertness; Mood; The restorative actions of amphetamine are thought to CIltcholamines; Monoamines be secondary to functional increases in catecholamine neurotransrnission (Glowinski et al. 1966). Conversely, Thereis considerable pharmacologic evidence that cen­ drugs that decrease catecholamine activity also influ­ Ira!n ervous system (CNS) catecholamines are impor­ ence arousal. In general, drugs that inhibit noradrener­ tant modulators of arousal in man. For example, gic or dopaminergic activity increase daytime sleepi­ llimulants such as amphetamine effectively reverse ness. For example, a variety of catecholamine receptor decrements in alertness and cognitive performance as­ blockers cause sedation, fatigue, and sleepiness (Betts IlCiatedwith sleep deprivation (Weiss and Laties 1962). 1981) and decrease sleep latency times (Nicholson and Pascoe 1990). From the Department of Behavioral Biology; Walter Reed Army Further evidence for catecholaminergic involve­ .atuteofResearch (UDM, DMP, DRT, HCS, MLT, GB), Washing­ ment in arousal stems from clinical observations made .... DC; Section on Anxiety and Affective Disorders, Biological Plydtiatry Branch (UDM), National Institute of Mental Health, in a variety of patient populations treated with alpha­ SiIionallnstitutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland; Department of methyl-para-tyrosine (AMPT); AMPTis a selective cat­ PIydlology, Concordia University (YS), Montreal, Quebec, Canada; echolamine synthesis inhibitor that can be utilized to alDepartmentof Psychiatry, University of California,and Psychiatry SInn,San Diego V.A. Medical Center (JCG), San Diego, California. determine the involvement of catecholamines in be­ Addresscorrespondence to: Una D. McCann, Section on Anxiety havior (Nagatsu et al. 1964; Spector et al. 1965). In pa­ alAffectiveDisorders, National Institute of Mental Health, Building tient populations, AMPT has been noted to produce II. Room 35·239, 9000 Rockville Pike, Bethesda, Maryland 20892. RtreivedJuly 1, 1992; revised October 28, 1992; accepted November sedation early in the course of treatment (Engelman et 111992. a1. 1968; Jonsson et a1. 1971; Ahlenius et al. 1973; Pabbshed 1993 by Elsevier Science Publishing Co., Inc. 0893-133X/93/$0.00 346 U. D. McCann et a1. NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1993-VOL. 8, NO.4 Pozuelo 1976; Nasrallah et al. 1977}. In addition, it has tained, subjects completed a Beck Depression Inven· been noted that discontinuation of AMPT treatment is tory ([BOI] Beck et al. 1961) and underwent a thorough associated with insomnia and decreased total sleep physical and laboratory examination, including an elec· (Bunney et al. 1971, 1977). This observed rebound in­ trocardiogram, psychiatric screening, SMA-20, thyroid somnia has been postulated to be secondary to an in­ function tests, hepatitis screen, human immunoden· crease in catecholaminergic activity. Further evidence ciency virus testing, complete blood count, urinalysis, for catecholaminergic involvement in the sleep-wake and urine drug screen for drugs of abuse. Exclusion cycle has been reviewed elsewhere (Mendelson 1987). criteria included self-reported past or present major In addition to their importance in sleep, there iscon­ medical or psychiatric illness, positive urine drug siderable pharmacologic evidence that brain catechol­ screens, a score of 10 or greater on the BDI, regular amines are involved in the regulation of mood (Schild­ tobacco use, ingestion of more than three cups of coffee kraut 1965; Bunney and Davis 1965; Greenspan et al. (or the equivalent) per day, or atypical sleeping patterns 1969; Iqbal et al. 1989; Schildkraut et al. 1973a, 1973b; (including unusually delayed or advanced sleep phases, Goodwin and Post 1975), although the relationship be­ history of insomnia or hypersomnia, or habitual sleep tween sleep and mood is complex. For example, major hours not occurring between 2000and 1000hours). Fol· depression is associated with objective electroen­ lowing completion of screening procedures, subjects cephalographic sleep abnormalities (Kupfer and Thase were randomlyassigned to one of four treatment con· 1983; Gillin 1983), and manic patients typicallyhave de­ ditions: 1) treatment with AMPT in a rested state creased sleep, particularly during the period when they (AMPT/R); 2) treatment with AMPT plus sleep depri· switch into mania (Bunney et al. 1972). Further suggest­ vation (AMPTISD); 3) treatment with placebo plus sleep ing a link between the regulation of mood and arousal deprivation (P/SD); or 4) treatment with placebo ina is the observation that the majority of depressed pa­ rested state (P/R). A schematic time line of study proce­ tients exhibit symptomatic improvement following one dures is depicted in Figure 1. night of total sleep deprivation (Vogel et al. 1975). The mechanism by which sleep deprivation improves mood Drug Administration in depressed individuals remains unclear, but may in­ volve catecholamines (Siegel and Rogawski 1988; Hart­ Drugs were administered in a double-blind fashion, mann and Schildkraut 1973; Schildkraut et al. 1973b; with subjects from both drug conditions included in Post et al. 1976; Mogilnicka et al. 1986). each study run, beginning at 1430 hours on treatment Despite the growing clinical interest in the relation­ day 1 (TD1). Subsequent doses were administered at ship between sleep and mood, few studies have 1830 and 2330 hours on TD1, and at 0730, 1330, 1830, reported on the effects of one night of sleep depriva­ and 2330 hours on treatment day 2 (TD2). Each of the tion on mood in healthy populations (Gerneret al. 1979; seven doses contained 750 mg AMPT (or lactose pia· Gillberg and Akerstedt 1981), and of these, results have cebo), for a total of 5.25 g AMPT over a 33-hour period. varied. Moreover, no study that has investigated the Previous studies have shown that this regimen depletes effect of sleep deprivation on mood in normal volun­ cerebrospinal fluid catecholamines by 68% to 77%af· teers has used a rested control group. The purpose of ter 24 hours (Brodie et al. 1971). Subjects treated with this study was twofold: frrst, to systematically evalu­ AMPT in the present study had signiflcantelevations ate the effects of sleep deprivation on self-reported in serum prolactin concentrations on day 2, indicating mood in healthy volunteers; and second, to use AMPT depletion of eNS catecholamines at the level of the as a pharmacologic probe for assessing the role of cate­ cholamine neurotransmitters in changes of alertness Tlme Unefor AMPT study mood associated with sleep deprivation. The pres­ and 0700 ent study was performed as part of a larger research Sleep· Deprivation project investigating the role of catecholamines in alert­ ness, sleep, performance, and mood. Adaptation Baseline Treatment T,elltment Recovery Night Night Day t Day 2 Day METHODS Subjects Forty healthy men with a mean age of 25 years (range Figure 1. Generalized time line for study procedures. Fol· 21 to 36) were recruited through advertisements in lo­ lowing adaptation and baseline sleep nights, subjects were cal newspapers. After an initial telephone screen, in­ sleep deprived for 40.5 hours (treatment days 1 and 2). Our· terested volunteers came to the laboratory for a more ing the sleep-deprivation period, subjects received seven detailed evaluation. After informed consent was ob- doses of AMPT (750 mg/dose) or placebo. �OPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 1993-VOL. 8, NO.4 AMPT and Sleep Deprivation in Normal Volunteers 347 Ipothalamus(McCann et al. 1992). Subjects received Alertness and Mood Measures 6eirla st dose of AMPT immediately before recovery Simultaneous measures of alertness and mood were *ep.at the end of the sleep-deprivation period. taken during the two sleep-deprivation and drug­ treatment days (TD1 and TD2) and the day following recovery sleep (recovery day). Measurements included IerpDeprivation a pencil and paper VAS designed to measure global PI!viouslydescribed methods for sleep-deprivation and vigor and affect (Monk 1989), and a computerized ver­ paformance assessment were used, with minor mod­ sion of the Prohle
Recommended publications
  • About Emotions There Are 8 Primary Emotions. You Are Born with These
    About Emotions There are 8 primary emotions. You are born with these emotions wired into your brain. That wiring causes your body to react in certain ways and for you to have certain urges when the emotion arises. Here is a list of primary emotions: Eight Primary Emotions Anger: fury, outrage, wrath, irritability, hostility, resentment and violence. Sadness: grief, sorrow, gloom, melancholy, despair, loneliness, and depression. Fear: anxiety, apprehension, nervousness, dread, fright, and panic. Joy: enjoyment, happiness, relief, bliss, delight, pride, thrill, and ecstasy. Interest: acceptance, friendliness, trust, kindness, affection, love, and devotion. Surprise: shock, astonishment, amazement, astound, and wonder. Disgust: contempt, disdain, scorn, aversion, distaste, and revulsion. Shame: guilt, embarrassment, chagrin, remorse, regret, and contrition. All other emotions are made up by combining these basic 8 emotions. Sometimes we have secondary emotions, an emotional reaction to an emotion. We learn these. Some examples of these are: o Feeling shame when you get angry. o Feeling angry when you have a shame response (e.g., hurt feelings). o Feeling fear when you get angry (maybe you’ve been punished for anger). There are many more. These are NOT wired into our bodies and brains, but are learned from our families, our culture, and others. When you have a secondary emotion, the key is to figure out what the primary emotion, the feeling at the root of your reaction is, so that you can take an action that is most helpful. .
    [Show full text]
  • 1 December 19, 2019 the Psychology of Online Political Hostility
    The Psychology of Online Political Hostility: A Comprehensive, Cross-National Test of the Mismatch Hypothesis Alexander Bor* & Michael Bang Petersen Department of Political Science Aarhus University August 30, 2021 Please cite the final version of this paper published in the American Political Science Review at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055421000885. Abstract Why are online discussions about politics more hostile than offline discussions? A popular answer argues that human psychology is tailored for face-to-face interaction and people’s behavior therefore changes for the worse in impersonal online discussions. We provide a theoretical formalization and empirical test of this explanation: the mismatch hypothesis. We argue that mismatches between human psychology and novel features of online environments could (a) change people’s behavior, (b) create adverse selection effects and (c) bias people’s perceptions. Across eight studies, leveraging cross-national surveys and behavioral experiments (total N=8,434), we test the mismatch hypothesis but only find evidence for limited selection effects. Instead, hostile political discussions are the result of status-driven individuals who are drawn to politics and are equally hostile both online and offline. Finally, we offer initial evidence that online discussions feel more hostile, in part, because the behavior of such individuals is more visible than offline. Acknowledgements This research has benefitted from discussions with Vin Arceneaux, Matt Levendusky, Mark Van Vugt, John Tooby, and members of the Research on Online Political Hostility (ROHP) group, among many others. We are grateful for constructive comments to workshop attendees at the Political Behavior Section of Aarhus University, at the NYU-SMAPP Lab, at the NYU Social Justice Lab, at the Hertie School, and to conference audiences at APSA 2019, HBES 2019, and ROPH 2020.
    [Show full text]
  • Dysphoria As a Complex Emotional State and Its Role in Psychopathology
    Dysphoria as a complex emotional state and its role in psychopathology Vladan Starcevic A/Professor, University of Sydney Faculty of Medicine and Health Sydney, Australia Objectives • Review conceptualisations of dysphoria • Present dysphoria as a transdiagnostic complex emotional state and assessment of dysphoria based on this conceptualisation What is dysphoria? • The term is derived from Greek (δύσφορος) and denotes distress that is hard to bear Dysphoria: associated with externalisation? • “Mixed affect” leading to an “affect of suspicion”1,2 1 Sandberg: Allgemeine Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie und Psychisch-Gerichtl Medizin 1896; 52:619-654 2 Specht G: Über den pathologischen Affekt in der chronischen Paranoia. Festschrift der Erlanger Universität, 1901 • A syndrome that always includes irritability and at least two of the following: internal tension, suspiciousness, hostility and aggressive or destructive behaviour3 3 Dayer et al: Bipolar Disord 2000; 2: 316-324 Dysphoria: associated with internalisation? • Six “dysphoric symptoms”: depressed mood, anhedonia, guilt, suicide, fatigue and anxiety1 1 Cassidy et al: Psychol Med 2000; 30:403-411 Dysphoria: a nonspecific state? • Dysphoria is a “nonspecific syndrome” and has “no particular place in a categorical diagnostic system”1; it is neglected and treated like an “orphan”1 1 Musalek et al: Psychopathol 2000; 33:209-214 • Dysphoria “can refer to many ways of feeling bad”2 2 Swann: Bipolar Disord 2000; 2:325-327 Textbook definitions: dysphoria nonspecific, mainly internalising? • “Feeling
    [Show full text]
  • Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Departmental Papers (ASC) Annenberg School for Communication 11-2017 Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm Emile Bruneau University of Pennsylvania, [email protected] Mina Cikara Rebecca Saxe Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/asc_papers Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons, Cognitive Psychology Commons, Communication Commons, Community Psychology Commons, Experimental Analysis of Behavior Commons, Personality and Social Contexts Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Bruneau, E., Cikara, M., & Saxe, R. (2017). Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8 (8), 934-942. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617693064 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/asc_papers/554 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Parochial Empathy Predicts Reduced Altruism and the Endorsement of Passive Harm Abstract Empathic failures are common in hostile intergroup contexts; repairing empathy is therefore a major focus of peacebuilding efforts. However, it is unclear which aspect of empathy is most relevant to intergroup conflict. Although trait empathic concern predicts prosociality in interpersonal settings, we hypothesized that the best predictor of meaningful intergroup attitudes and behaviors might not be the general capacity for empathy (i.e., trait empathy), but the difference in empathy felt for the in-group versus the out-group, or “parochial empathy.” Specifically, we predicted that out-group empathy would inhibit intergroup harm and promote intergroup helping, whereas in-group empathy would have the opposite effect. In three intergroup contexts—Americans regarding Arabs, Hungarians regarding refugees, Greeks regarding Germans—we found support for this hypothesis.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychosexual Characteristics of Vestibulodynia Couples: Partner Solicitousness and Hostility Are Associated with Pain
    418 ORIGINAL RESEARCH—PAIN Psychosexual Characteristics of Vestibulodynia Couples: Partner Solicitousness and Hostility are Associated with Pain Mylène Desrosiers, MA,* Sophie Bergeron, PhD,* Marta Meana, PhD,† Bianca Leclerc, B.Sc.,* Yitzchak M. Binik, PhD,‡ and Samir Khalifé, MD§ *Department of Sexology, Université du Québec à Montréal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; †Department of Psychology, University of Nevada Las Vegas, Las Vegas, NV, USA; ‡Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; §Jewish General Hospital—Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Montreal, Quebec, Canada DOI: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2007.00705.x ABSTRACT Introduction. Provoked vestibulodynia is a prevalent yet misunderstood women’s sexual health issue. In particular, data concerning relationship characteristics and psychosexual functioning of partners of these women are scarce. Moreover, no research to date has examined the role of the partner in vestibulodynia. Aims. This study aimed to characterize and compare the psychosexual profiles of women with vestibulodynia and their partners, in addition to exploring whether partner-related variables correlated with women’s pain and associ- ated psychosexual functioning. Methods. Forty-three couples in which the woman suffered from vestibulodynia completed self-report question- naires focusing on their sexual functioning, dyadic adjustment, and psychological adjustment. Women were diagnosed using the cotton-swab test during a standardized gynecological examination. They also took part in a structured interview during which they were asked about their pain during intercourse and frequency of intercourse. They also completed a questionnaire about their perceptions of their partners’ responses to the pain. Main Outcome Measures. Dependent measures for both members of the couple included the Sexual History Form, the Locke-Wallace Marital Adjustment Scale and the Brief Symptom Inventory.
    [Show full text]
  • Assessment and Treatment of Posttraumatic Anger and Aggression: a Review
    Volume 49, Number 5, 2012 JRRDJRRD Pages 777–788 Assessment and treatment of posttraumatic anger and aggression: A review Casey T. Taft, PhD;1–2* Suzannah K. Creech, PhD;1,3 Lorig Kachadourian, PhD1–2 1National Center for PTSD, Department of Veterans Affairs (VA) Boston Healthcare System, Boston, MA; 2Boston Uni- versity School of Medicine, Boston, MA; 3Providence VA Medical Center, Providence, RI; and Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, RI Abstract—The Department of Veterans Affairs (VA) and hyperarousal and are increasingly being viewed as a Department of Defense’s (DOD) recently published and updated defining feature of patients’ clinical presentations. Department of Veterans Affairs/Department of Defense VA/ DOD Clinical Practice Guideline for Management of Post- Traumatic Stress includes irritability, severe agitation, and METHODS anger as specific symptoms that frequently co-occur with PTSD. For the first time, the guideline includes nine specific We begin by reviewing literature on the associations recommendations for the assessment and treatment of PTSD- related anger, irritability, and agitation. This article will review between PTSD and both anger and aggression. The the literature on PTSD and its association with anger and review that occurred for the update in the 2010 VA/DOD aggression. We highlight explanatory models for these associa- PTSD clinical practice guideline and additional relevant tions, factors that contribute to the occurrence of anger and literature published since that guideline review is aggression in PTSD, assessment of anger and aggression, and included. We then discuss theoretical models that have effective anger management interventions and strategies. been proposed to explain these associations.
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:2011.03588V1 [Cs.CL] 6 Nov 2020
    Hostility Detection Dataset in Hindi Mohit Bhardwajy, Md Shad Akhtary, Asif Ekbalz, Amitava Das?, Tanmoy Chakrabortyy yIIIT Delhi, India. zIIT Patna, India. ?Wipro Research, India. fmohit19014,tanmoy,[email protected], [email protected], [email protected] Abstract Despite Hindi being the third most spoken language in the world, and a significant presence of Hindi content on social In this paper, we present a novel hostility detection dataset in Hindi language. We collect and manually annotate ∼ 8200 media platforms, to our surprise, we were not able to find online posts. The annotated dataset covers four hostility di- any significant dataset on fake news or hate speech detec- mensions: fake news, hate speech, offensive, and defamation tion in Hindi. A survey of the literature suggest a few works posts, along with a non-hostile label. The hostile posts are related to hostile post detection in Hindi, such as (Kar et al. also considered for multi-label tags due to a significant over- 2020; Jha et al. 2020; Safi Samghabadi et al. 2020); however, lap among the hostile classes. We release this dataset as part there are two basic issues with these works - either the num- of the CONSTRAINT-2021 shared task on hostile post detec- ber of samples in the dataset are not adequate or they cater tion. to a specific dimension of the hostility only. In this paper, we present our manually annotated dataset for hostile posts 1 Introduction detection in Hindi. We collect more than ∼ 8200 online so- The COVID-19 pandemic has changed our lives forever, cial media posts and annotate them as hostile and non-hostile both online and offline.
    [Show full text]
  • Shame and Guilt/Self-Blame As Predictors of Expressed Emotion in Family Members of Patients with Schizophrenia
    Psychiatry Research 196 (2012) 27–31 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Psychiatry Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres Shame and guilt/self-blame as predictors of expressed emotion in family members of patients with schizophrenia Stephanie Wasserman, Amy Weisman de Mamani ⁎, Giulia Suro University of Miami, United States article info abstract Article history: Expressed emotion (EE) is a measure of the family environment reflecting the amount of criticism and emo- Received 3 November 2010 tional over-involvement expressed by a key relative towards a family member with a disorder or impairment. Received in revised form 15 August 2011 Patients from high EE homes have a poorer illness prognosis than do patients from low EE homes. Despite Accepted 18 August 2011 EE's well-established predictive validity, questions remain regarding why some family members express high levels of EE attitudes while others do not. Based on indirect evidence from previous research, the current Keywords: study tested whether shame and guilt/self-blame about having a relative with schizophrenia serve as predic- Criticism Emotional over-involvement tors of EE. A sample of 72 family members of patients with schizophrenia completed the Five Minute Speech Self directed emotions Sample to measure EE, along with questionnaires assessing self-directed emotions. In line with the hypoth- Five Minute Speech Sample eses, higher levels of both shame and guilt/self-blame about having a relative with schizophrenia predicted Self-directed Emotions for Schizophrenia high EE. Results of the current study elucidate the EE construct and have implications for working with fam- Scale ilies of patients with schizophrenia.
    [Show full text]
  • Panic Disorder and Exercise Avoidance Ricardo W
    Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria. 2014;36:68–75 ß 2014 Associac¸a˜ o Brasileira de Psiquiatria doi:10.1590/1516-4446-2012-1012 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Panic disorder and exercise avoidance Ricardo W. Muotri, Ma´rcio A. Bernik Anxiety Disorders Program, Institute of Psychiatry and Department of Psychiatry, Universidade de Sa˜o Paulo (USP), Sa˜o Paulo, SP, Brazil. Objectives: 1) To identify whether patients with panic disorder in general and those with the respiratory subtype in particular actively avoid exercise; 2) to investigate physiological differences in cardiopulmonary function parameters in patients with panic disorder in general, patients with the respiratory subtype of panic disorder, and healthy controls upon exercise challenge. Methods: Patients with panic disorder were classified as having either the respiratory or the non- respiratory subtype. Both groups were compared to controls in terms of exercise avoidance patterns and performance on cardiopulmonary exercise testing. Results: Patients with panic disorder exhibited higher exercise avoidance scores and worse performance on cardiopulmonary exercise testing as compared with controls. No differences were found between patients with the respiratory and non-respiratory subtypes. Conclusions: Exercise avoidance is present in panic disorder and is associated with poorer performance on cardiopulmonary exercise testing. These findings are not limited to patients with the respiratory subtype of the disorder. Keywords: Anxiety disorders; exercise test; cardiopulmonary exercise testing; panic attack; physical exercise Introduction with PD and depression is an autonomic dysfunction, in which a dominance of sympathetic over vagal control of Fear of a heart attack is a major concern among patients heart rate might lead to arrhythmia and sudden death.6 with panic disorder (PD).
    [Show full text]
  • Children's Mental Health Disorder Fact Sheet for the Classroom
    1 Children’s Mental Health Disorder Fact Sheet for the Classroom1 Disorder Symptoms or Behaviors About the Disorder Educational Implications Instructional Strategies and Classroom Accommodations Anxiety Frequent Absences All children feel anxious at times. Many feel stress, for example, when Students are easily frustrated and may Allow students to contract a flexible deadline for Refusal to join in social activities separated from parents; others fear the dark. Some though suffer enough have difficulty completing work. They worrisome assignments. Isolating behavior to interfere with their daily activities. Anxious students may lose friends may suffer from perfectionism and take Have the student check with the teacher or have the teacher Many physical complaints and be left out of social activities. Because they are quiet and compliant, much longer to complete work. Or they check with the student to make sure that assignments have Excessive worry about homework/grades the signs are often missed. They commonly experience academic failure may simply refuse to begin out of fear been written down correctly. Many teachers will choose to Frequent bouts of tears and low self-esteem. that they won’t be able to do anything initial an assignment notebook to indicate that information Fear of new situations right. Their fears of being embarrassed, is correct. Drug or alcohol abuse As many as 1 in 10 young people suffer from an AD. About 50% with humiliated, or failing may result in Consider modifying or adapting the curriculum to better AD also have a second AD or other behavioral disorder (e.g. school avoidance. Getting behind in their suit the student’s learning style-this may lessen his/her depression).
    [Show full text]
  • Redalyc.Emotions and the Emotional Disorders: a Quantitative
    International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology ISSN: 1697-2600 [email protected] Asociación Española de Psicología Conductual España Watson, David; Clark, Lee Anna; Stasik, Sara M. Emotions and the emotional disorders: A quantitative hierarchical perspective International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, vol. 11, núm. 3, 2011, pp. 429-442 Asociación Española de Psicología Conductual Granada, España Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=33719289001 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative © International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology ISSN 1697-2600 print ISSN 2174-0852 online 2011, Vol. 11, Nº 3, pp. 429-442 Emotions and the emotional disorders: A quantitative hierarchical perspective David Watson1, Lee Anna Clark, and Sara M. Stasik (University of Notre Dame, USA) ABSTRACT. Previous evidence has established that general negative affect represents a non-specific factor common to both anxiety and depression, whereas low positive affect is more specifically related to the latter. Little is known, however, about how specific, lower order affects relate to these constructs. We investigated how six emotional disorders—major depression, generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), panic disorder, social phobia, and obsessive compulsive disorder — are linked to both general and specific types of affect in two samples (Ns = 331 and 253), using the Expanded Form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS- X). Replicating previous results, the General Negative Affect scale was nonspecifically related to the emotional disorders, whereas General Positive Affect had a specific (inverse) association with major depression.
    [Show full text]
  • Direct and Indirect Relationships Among Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, Depression, Hostility, Anger, and Verbal and Physical Aggression in Returning Veterans
    Received: 29 October 2018 | Revised: 4 January 2019 | Accepted: 21 February 2019 DOI: 10.1002/ab.21827 RESEARCH ARTICLE Direct and indirect relationships among posttraumatic stress disorder, depression, hostility, anger, and verbal and physical aggression in returning veterans Vinnu Bhardwaj1,5 | Abigail C. Angkaw2,4 | Massimo Franceschetti1,5 | Ramesh Rao1,5 | Dewleen G. Baker2,3,4 1Qualcomm Institute, University of California, San Diego, CA Abstract 2Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare Hostility, anger, and aggression are conceptually related but unique constructs found System, San Diego, CA to occur more often among veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) than 3VA Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health, San Diego, CA among civilians or veterans without PTSD. However, the pathways between PTSD, 4Department of Psychiatry, University of depression, hostility, anger, and aggression have not been comprehensively California, San Diego, CA characterized. Therefore, drawing on a sample of returning Operation Enduring 5Department of ECE, University of California, San Diego, CA Freedom/Operation Iraqi Freedom combat veterans (N = 175; 95% male; mean age 30 years), this study sought to examine the direct and indirect relationships among Correspondence Dewleen G. Baker, VA San Diego Healthcare PTSD, depression, hostility, anger, and four types of aggression: verbal, and physical System, 3350 La Jolla Village Drive, MC 116A, toward self, others, and objects. Functional modeling of direct effects was done using San Diego, CA 92161. ‐ Email: [email protected] multiple least squares regression and bootstrapped mediation analyses were carried out to test indirect effects. Results indicate that PTSD is not the overall direct Funding information VA Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental contributor to different forms of aggression, supporting the mediating role of Health; Qualcomm Institute; VA CSR&D depression and trait anger.
    [Show full text]