Constraint, Natural Selection, and the Evolution of Human Body Form

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Constraint, Natural Selection, and the Evolution of Human Body Form Constraint, natural selection, and the evolution of human body form Kristen R. R. Savella,1, Benjamin M. Auerbacha, and Charles C. Rosemanb,c aDepartment of Anthropology, The University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996; bDepartment of Cell Biology and Anatomy, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4; and cProgram in Ecology, Evolution, and Conservation Biology, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61802 Edited by Richard G. Klein, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, and approved May 26, 2016 (received for review March 3, 2016) Variation in body form among human groups is structured by a blend natural selection, rather than having arisen due to a correlated re- of natural selection driven by local climatic conditions and random sponse to selection on other traits. genetic drift. However, attempts to test ecogeographic hypotheses The distinction between the action of selection on a trait (a have not distinguished between adaptive traits (i.e., those that trait’s influence on fitness) and the evolutionary response of a evolved as a result of selection) and those that evolved as a correlated trait to natural selection is a cornerstone of modern evolutionary response to selection on other traits (i.e., nonadaptive traits), compli- theory (20). When natural selection is acting on a set of geneti- cating our understanding of the relationship between climate and cally covarying characteristics, individual responses to selection morphological distinctions among populations. Here, we use evolu- are a function of the relationship between that characteristic and tionary quantitative methods to test if traits previously identified as fitness independent of all other traits, as well as the correlated supporting ecogeographic hypotheses were actually adaptive by responses imparted by directional selection acting on genetically estimating the force of selection on individual traits needed to drive covarying traits. Fig. 1 (adapted with permission from ref. 21) among-group differentiation. Our results show that not all associa- shows this contrast graphically: if traits are uncorrelated, then tions between trait means and latitude were caused by selection they will respond to selection in the same direction as the force of acting directly on each individual trait. Although radial and tibial selection irrespective of what direction in the morphospace se- length and biiliac and femoral head breadth show signs of responses lection is acting (Fig. 1A). However, genetic correlations among to directional selection matching ecogeographic hypotheses, the femur traits may cause the response to selection not to match the di- was subject to little or no directional selection despite having shorter rection of selection (β2 and Δz2;Fig.1B), unless the response to values by latitude. Additionally, in contradiction to ecogeographic selection closely parallels the main axis of variation (β and Δz ; hypotheses, the humerus was under directional selection for longer 1 1 Fig. 1B). Distinguishing between the action of selection (the es- values by latitude. Responses to directional selection in the tibia and timated selective force) and the response to selection (the evolu- radius induced a nonadaptive correlated response in the humerus that tionary change) is important because when there are strong genetic overwhelmed its own trait-specific response to selection. This result correlations among traits, as there are in those reflecting body form emphasizes that mean differences between groups are not good indicators of which traits are adaptations in the absence of informa- (3, 4), the magnitude of realized differences among groups may not tion about covariation among characteristics. reflect the strength and direction of natural selection. Adaptation will not be apparent from differences in means between groups. natural selection | ecogeographic variation | Bergmann’srule| Similar studies have demonstrated that this is the case in cranial – Allen’srule| evolutionary constraints (22, 23), pelvic (24 26), and hand and foot bones (27). The standard ecogeographic model assumes the mean body form of groups migrating away from the tropics evolved from mong-group variation in human body form is the result of a tropically adapted ancestral condition (10–16). We take the Agenetic drift, gene flow, and natural selection (1–4). Patterns of variation in human body shape appear to match ecogeographic expectations set out by Bergmann (5) and Allen (6). Comparisons Significance of average body form among groups of humans living at different latitudes reflect these rules. Distal limb segments decrease relative Human morphological variation is thought to have been partially to proximal limb segments, body mass increases, and the pelvis shaped by natural selection associated with environmental fac- widens as latitude increases (7–16). The prevailing explanation for tors like climate. Patterns of variation in body form correspond these patterns is that among-group variation in body proportions with latitude, but evolutionary processes that yielded this vari- was shaped by natural selection acting on thermoregulatory efficacy ation are not yet established. Examining the traits used in these (8,9,11). studies (e.g., limb lengths) independently ignores their genetic Two problems with this interpretation require further investiga- covariation, which affects their responses to evolutionary forces. tion. First, there is considerable overlap between the distribution of To address this relationship, we estimated the directional selec- climate across latitude and the migratory routes of recent range tion necessary to evolve correlated traits reflecting body shape expansions of humans from Africa into other parts of the world across latitudes and examined trait-specific responses. Although (2, 17–19). Thus, patterns of body form variation among groups most traits appear to be under directional selection, their response is constrained by between-trait covariance. This finding suggests could reflect random genetic drift and gene flow and mimic the that trait differences among human groups may not directly re- hypothesized effects of climate. Correspondingly, among-group flect the forces of selection that shaped them. differences in traits strongly associated with Bergmann’s and Allen’s — — rules distal limb segment lengths and body breadth best fit Author contributions: B.M.A. and C.C.R. designed research; K.R.R.S., B.M.A., and C.C.R. models that include both latitude and random genetic drift (4). In performed research; K.R.R.S. and C.C.R. analyzed data; and K.R.R.S., B.M.A., and C.C.R. contrast, the among-group variation in proximal limb segment wrote the paper. lengths and femoral head diameter (a proxy for body mass) was The authors declare no conflict of interest. described best by population structure alone (4). A second, pre- This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. viously unexplored issue is that all studies of ecogeographic varia- 1To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected]. tion in body form to date have worked under the assumption This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. that morphological differences among populations are the result of 1073/pnas.1603632113/-/DCSupplemental. 9492–9497 | PNAS | August 23, 2016 | vol. 113 | no. 34 www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1603632113 Downloaded by guest on September 28, 2021 Fig. 1. The differences between the force of di- rectional selection (β) and responses to selection ðΔzÞ for two traits that are independent (A) or that strongly covary (B). In both examples, we subject the trait means to two selection gradients (β1 and β2). The traits are uncorrelated in A, so responses to selection ðΔz1, Δz2Þ will closely match the vectors of the selection gradients (β1 and β2). Studies that use the mean differences in traits between groups to indicate how those traits responded to selection implicitly assume this framework for selection. However, in B, the strong covariance between traits affects the ability of the traits to respond to certain selection gradients. A selection gradient (β1) similar to the major axis of variation will result in responses to selection that closely parallel that axis and yield a larger response than in uncorrelated traits. In con- trast, a selection gradient that greatly departs from the major axis (β2), and which is positive for trait 1 but zero for trait 2, will nonetheless result in positive responses to selection ðΔz2Þ for both traits because of their covariance. Adapted with permission from ref. 21; see box 1 and figure 1 in ref. 21 for a more comprehensive exploration of the effect of trait covariance on responses to selection. average morphology of several groups from sub-Saharan Africa to pressures on individual traits. Positive selection gradients between represent the morphological range of variation for putatively groups at different latitudes indicate a net positive relationship with tropically adapted groups. We then used groups from regions of fitness (for long bones to lengthen or biiliac breadth and femoral higher latitude (including North Africa, temperate Europe, and head diameter to broaden). Alternatively, negative selection gradi- the circumpolar Arctic) to represent the range of variation in body ents between groups at different latitudes indicate a net negative form that resulted, in part, from climate mediated natural selec- relationship with fitness. We also decompose separate
Recommended publications
  • Biology of Human Variation Fall 2014
    Anthropology 2110 Biology of Human Variation Fall 2014 Professor: Dr. Tamara Varney Location: AT1010 Lecture time: Tues 7-10 Phone: 807-343-8204 Office: Braun Building 2001D Email: [email protected] Office Hours: Thurs 9-11am am until Oct 15 then by appointment**(if its more convenient feel free to set an appointment time) OR you can phone, email or drop in anytime to see if I am free – just be prepared to come at another time if I am busy with something I cannot interrupt I cannot guarantee that I will respond in a timely fashion if messages are left on my voice mail rather than email and students should NOT expect less than 24 hour turnaround time to email messages. Please DO NOT assume that your message, voice or email, has been received unless you receive an acknowledgement. Course Description: This course focuses on human microevolution. Topics include evolutionary theory, the genetic background of human variation, the distribution of human variation, human adaptability, and the role of disease and other influences on human evolution. Required Textbook: (available at the University Bookstore) Mielke JH, Konigsberg LW and Relethford JH. 2011. Human Biological Variation. 2nd Edition. Oxford University Press. ISBN 13: 978-0-19-538740-7 Also see the course Desire2Learn (D2L) site (look in MyCourseLinks on the LU website) for additional resources. Evaluation: Your final grade will be based on: Term Test 1 25% Oct 27 Term Test 2 30% Nov 18 Final Exam 45% Final examination period – Dec 9-19 **Note exam contingency date is Dec 20. Please note that Fri, November 6, 2015 is the last date for withdrawal without academic penalty from this course.
    [Show full text]
  • Sequence Variations in the Public Human Genome Data Reflect a Bottlenecked Population History
    Sequence variations in the public human genome data reflect a bottlenecked population history Gabor Marth*†, Greg Schuler*, Raymond Yeh‡, Ruth Davenport§, Richa Agarwala*, Deanna Church*, Sarah Wheelan*¶, Jonathan Baker*, Ming Ward*, Michael Kholodov*, Lon Phan*, Eva Czabarka*, Janos Murvai*, David Cutlerʈ, Stephen Wooding**, Alan Rogers**, Aravinda Chakravartiʈ, Henry C. Harpending**, Pui-Yan Kwok†,††, and Stephen T. Sherry*† *National Center for Biotechnology Information, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD 20894; ‡Department of Genetics, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63130; §Division of Internal Medicine, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO 63130; ¶Department of Molecular Biology and Genetics and ʈMcKusick–Nathans Institute of Genetic Medicine, The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21205; **Department of Anthropology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84112; and ††Cardiovascular Research Institute and Department of Dermatology, University of California, San Francisco, CA 94143 Contributed by Henry C. Harpending, November 5, 2002 Single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) constitute the great ma- (density) distribution of genomic sequence variations. To this jority of variations in the human genome, and as heritable variable end, we built a set of reagents (pairwise sequence alignments landmarks they are useful markers for disease mapping and re- and their corresponding sets of variation) by analyzing the solving population structure. Redundant coverage in overlaps of overlapping regions of large-insert clones sequenced as part of large-insert genomic clones, sequenced as part of the Human the human genome project. These data provided marker Genome Project, comprises a quarter of the genome, and it is density observations grouped by overlap fragment length.
    [Show full text]
  • Methods to Detect Selection in Populations with Applications to the Human
    P1: FQP/VEN July 6, 2000 12:24 Annual Reviews AR104-19 Annu. Rev. Genomics Hum. Genet. 2000. 01:539–59 Copyright c 2000 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved METHODS TO DETECT SELECTION IN POPULATIONS WITH APPLICATIONS TO THE HUMAN Martin Kreitman Department of Ecology and Evolution, University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois 60637; e-mail: [email protected] Key Words natural selection, genetic drift, polymorphism, molecular evolution ■ Abstract The development of statistical tests of natural selection at the DNA level in population samples has been ongoing for the past 13 years. The current state of the field is reviewed, and the available tests of selection are described. All tests use predictions from the theory of neutrally evolving sites as a null hypothesis. Departures from equilibrium-neutral expectations can indicate the presence of natural selection acting either at one or more of the sites under investigation or at a sufficiently tightly linked site. Complications can arise in the interpretation of departures from neutrality if populations are not at equilibrium for mutation and genetic drift or if populations are subdivided, both of which are likely scenarios for humans. Attempts to understand the nonequilibrium configuration of silent polymorphism in human mitochondrial DNA illustrate the difficulty of distinguishing between selection and alternative demographic hypotheses. The range of plausible alternatives to selection will become better defined, however, as additional population genetic data sets become available, allowing better null models to be constructed. INTRODUCTION Kimura’s immensely influential formulation of the neutral theory of molecular evolution, which came in 1968, was based primarily on an argument about the magnitude of the genetic load that would be imposed by positive selection if it were the only driving force in protein evolution (56).
    [Show full text]
  • Human DNA Sequences: More Variation and Less Race
    AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 139:23–34 (2009) Human DNA Sequences: More Variation and Less Race Jeffrey C. Long,1* Jie Li,1 and Meghan E. Healy2 1Department of Human Genetics, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-5618 2Department of Anthropology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131 KEY WORDS race; DNA sequence; short tandem repeat; diversity; hierachical models ABSTRACT Interest in genetic diversity within and sity is one of nested subsets, such that the diversity in between human populations as a way to answer questions non-Sub-Saharan African populations is essentially a sub- about race has intensified in light of recent advances in set of the diversity found in Sub-Saharan African popula- genome technology. The purpose of this article is to apply tions. The actual pattern of DNA diversity creates some a method of generalized hierarchical modeling to two unsettling problems for using race as meaningful genetic DNA data sets. The first data set consists of a small sam- categories. For example, the pattern of DNA diversity ple of individuals (n 5 32 total, from eight populations) implies that some populations belong to more than one who have been fully resequenced for 63 loci that encode a race (e.g., Europeans), whereas other populations do not total of 38,534 base pairs. The second data set consists of belong to any race at all (e.g., Sub-Saharan Africans). As a large sample of individuals (n 5 928 total, from 46 popu- Frank Livingstone noted long ago, the Linnean classifica- lations) who have been genotyped at 580 loci that encode tion system cannot accommodate this pattern because short tandem repeats.
    [Show full text]
  • Bio-Skin-Color-Transcript.Pdf
    [crickets chirp] [cymbal plays] [chime plays] [music plays] [JABLONSKI (narrated):] Human brains are gray. Human blood is red. Our bones are off-white. Doesn’t matter where you’re born or to whom. But human skin is different. [music plays] Some of us have rich dark brown skin; some of us have pinkish white skin. Most of us are somewhere in between. For the longest time, why this variation exists was a real scientific mystery … that opened the door for some to invest this biological trait with moral value, and then use that to justify the suffering of others. [elephant trumpets] But biological traits aren’t good or bad. They’re features that have evolved because they enhance an organism’s odds of surviving and passing on its genes. [JABLONSKI:] Like other animal traits, the sepia rainbow of human skin color evolved through natural selection. Now, thanks to advances in anthropology and genetics, exactly how and why it did is no longer a mystery. [music plays] [background discussion] [JABLONSKI:] Biological anthropologists like myself spend our lives studying how humans evolved, and why we differ from one another physically. [music plays] [JABLONSKI (narrated):] Our skin provides one of the most visible markers of human variability. It’s something that sets us apart from our closest animal relatives. Under their dark fur, chimpanzees have pale skin, and millions of years ago that was probably also the case for the primates that were our common ancestors. So where did humanity’s range of skin colors come from? From physics we know that the color of any object comes from the wavelengths of light that it reflects back to an observer’s eye.
    [Show full text]
  • Evidence for Positive Selection and Population Structure at the Human MAO-A Gene
    Evidence for positive selection and population structure at the human MAO-A gene Yoav Gilad*†, Shai Rosenberg‡, Molly Przeworski§, Doron Lancet*, and Karl Skorecki†‡ *Department of Molecular Genetics and the Crown Human Genome Center, The Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 76100, Israel; ‡Rappaport Faculty of Medicine and Research Institute, Technion–Israel Institute of Technology, and Rambam Medical Center, Haifa 31096, Israel; and §Department of Statistics, University of Oxford, 1 South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3TG, United Kingdom Communicated by Eviatar Nevo, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel, November 19, 2001 (received for review May 15, 2001) We report the analysis of human nucleotide diversity at a genetic locus known to be involved in a behavioral phenotype, the mono- amine oxidase A gene. Sequencing of five regions totaling 18.8 kb and spanning 90 kb of the monoamine oxidase A gene was carried out in 56 male individuals from seven different ethnogeographic groups. We uncovered 41 segregating sites, which formed 46 distinct haplotypes. A permutation test detected substantial pop- ulation structure in these samples. Consistent with differentiation between populations, linkage disequilibrium is higher than ex- pected under panmixia, with no evidence of a decay with distance. The extent of linkage disequilibrium is not typical of nuclear loci and suggests that the underlying population structure may have been accentuated by a selective sweep that fixed different hap- lotypes in different populations, or by local adaptation. In support of this suggestion, we find both a reduction in levels of diversity Fig. 1. Overall genomic structure and sequencing strategy for the MAO-A (as measured by a Hudson–Kreitman–Aguade test with the DMD44 gene.
    [Show full text]
  • Homo Sapiens
    Global landscape of recent inferred Darwinian selection for Homo sapiens Eric T. Wang*†, Greg Kodama‡, Pierre Baldi*†‡, and Robert K. Moyzis*†§ *Department of Biological Chemistry, College of Medicine, ‡Donald Bren School of Information and Computer Sciences, and †Institute of Genomics and Bioinformatics, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697 Communicated by Douglas C. Wallace, University of California, Irvine, CA, November 9, 2005 (received for review June 8, 2005) By using the 1.6 million single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) F test statistics, positive scores are indicative of unusually high genotype data set from Perlegen Sciences [Hinds, D. A., Stuve, L. L., heterozygosity within the data set. Additionally, these tests Nilsen, G. B., Halperin, E., Eskin, E., Ballinger, D. G., Frazer, K. A. & usually do not take distance between variable sites into consid- Cox, D. R. (2005) Science 307, 1072–1079], a probabilistic search for eration and rely heavily on statistics obtained from rare muta- the landscape exhibited by positive Darwinian selection was con- tional events. The selection criterion for the Perlegen and ducted. By sorting each high-frequency allele by homozygosity, we HapMap genotyping efforts, however, was the high heterozy- search for the expected decay of adjacent SNP linkage disequilib- gosity and equal spacing of SNPs (1, 2). Hence, these data sets rium (LD) at recently selected alleles, eliminating the need for have high ascertainment bias. Using tests that rely on heterozy- inferring haplotype. We designate this approach the LD decay gosity and frequency of rare mutations to infer selection on such (LDD) test. By these criteria, 1.6% of Perlegen SNPs were found to biased data sets should be largely meaningless.
    [Show full text]
  • NIEHS and EPA Children's Environmental Health and Disease
    SESSION 1: Plenary Session Couple Based Approach for Assessing Environmental Pollutants and Human Reproduction and Development – Recent Findings from the LIFE Study Germaine Buck Louis, National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) The Longitudinal Investigation of Fertility and the Environment (LIFE) Study was designed to assess environmental pollutants and lifestyle in relation to a spectrum of reproductive and developmental outcomes. This talk will briefly address the LIFE Study’s experience in overcoming key methodologic challenges underlying the design and conduct of population based research with preconception enrollment of couples planning pregnancies. In addition, emerging results regarding couples’ exposures to persistent environmental chemicals and human fecundity as measured by time-to-pregnancy will be presented. Environmental Epigenetics – Results and Opportunities for Children's Environmental Health and Disease Prevention Research Andrea Baccarelli, Harvard School of Public Health Epigenetics investigates heritable changes in gene expression that occur without changes in DNA sequence. Several epigenetic mechanisms, including DNA methylation and histone modifications, can change genome function under exogenous influence. Results obtained from animal models indicate that in utero or early-life environmental exposures produce effects that can be inherited transgenerationally and are accompanied by epigenetic alterations. The search for human equivalents of the epigenetic mechanisms identified in animal models
    [Show full text]
  • Notes: Introduction to Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (Snps)
    VCU Bioinformatics and Bioengineering Summer Institute The Origin of Human Genetic Variability from Cross-species Comparisons Notes: Introduction to Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) I. Overview The Scenario begins with a grand idea of discovering the source of human variability. By the end, it gets bogged down in a technical problem, but let's leave that aside for the moment and go back to the big picture. Human variability – at least its genetic component – is the result of differences in only a tiny fraction of the human genome. In contrast, two bacteria, Escherichia coli and Salmonella typhimurium, that you would be hard pressed to tell apart by their looks or metabolic capabilities differ in as much as x%. One lesson we might take away, of course, is that we are all genetic siblings under the skin, but the question remains: Why is it that so little difference in DNA sequence makes so large a difference in at least appearance? The most abundant genetic variants in human genomes are single basepair differences, also called single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). Understanding how SNPs arise from might therefore help us understand the major source of human variability. II. Technical notes on sequences SNPs by definition have positions where more than one nucleotide appear in the population. This poses a problem in representation. How do you convey the idea that the nucleotide at a given position in a sequence might be either an A or a G? A system of nomenclature has grown up to deal with this problem: Nuc Symbol Nuc Symbol Nuc Symbol A A A or C M (aMino) A or C or G V (not T) C C A or G R (puRine) A or C or T H (not G) G G A or T W (Weak) A or G or T D (not C) T T C or G S (Strong) C or G or T B (not A) C or T Y (pYrimidine) A or C or G or T N (aNy) G or T K (Keto) Thus the Scenario's first SNP sequence, GGGCGGGGARCCTGGCCACCA, represents a sequence that permits either an A or a G in the 10th position.
    [Show full text]
  • Leveraging Models of Cell Regulation and GWAS Data in Integrative Network-Based Association Studies
    PERSPECTIVE Leveraging models of cell regulation and GWAS data in integrative network-based association studies Andrea Califano1–3,11, Atul J Butte4,5, Stephen Friend6, Trey Ideker7–9 & Eric Schadt10,11 Over the last decade, the genome-wide study of both heritable and straightforward: within the space of all possible genetic and epigenetic somatic human variability has gone from a theoretical concept to a variants, those contributing to a specific trait or disease likely have broadly implemented, practical reality, covering the entire spectrum some coalescent properties, allowing their effect to be functionally of human disease. Although several findings have emerged from these canalized via the cell communication and cell regulatory machin- studies1, the results of genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have ery that allows distinct cells to interact and regulates their behavior. been mostly sobering. For instance, although several genes showing Notably, contrary to random networks, whose output is essentially medium-to-high penetrance within heritable traits were identified by unconstrained, regulatory networks produced by adaptation to spe- these approaches, the majority of heritable genetic risk factors for most cific fitness landscapes are optimized to produce only a finite number common diseases remain elusive2–7. Additionally, due to impractical of well-defined outcomes as a function of a very large number of exog- requirements for cohort size8 and lack of methodologies to maximize enous and endogenous signals. Thus, if a comprehensive and accurate power for such detections, few epistatic interactions and low-pen- map of all intra- and intercellular molecular interactions were avail- etrance variants have been identified9. At the opposite end of the able, then genetic and epigenetic events implicated in a specific trait or germline versus somatic event spectrum, considering that tumor cells disease should cluster in subnetworks of closely interacting genes.
    [Show full text]
  • Addressing Human Variability in Next-Generation Human Health Risk Assessments of Environmental Chemicals Lauren Zeise, Frédéric Y
    Addressing human variability in next-generation human health risk assessments of environmental chemicals Lauren Zeise, Frédéric Y. Bois, Weihsueh A. Chiu, Dale B. Hattis, Ivan Rusyn, Kathryn Z. Guyton To cite this version: Lauren Zeise, Frédéric Y. Bois, Weihsueh A. Chiu, Dale B. Hattis, Ivan Rusyn, et al.. Addressing human variability in next-generation human health risk assessments of environmental chemicals. En- vironmental Health Perspectives, National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences, 2013, 121 (1), pp.23-31. 10.1289/ehp.1205687. ineris-00961796 HAL Id: ineris-00961796 https://hal-ineris.archives-ouvertes.fr/ineris-00961796 Submitted on 20 Mar 2014 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Review Addressing Human Variability in Next-Generation Human Health Risk Assessments of Environmental Chemicals Lauren Zeise,1 Frederic Y. Bois,2 Weihsueh A. Chiu,3 Dale Hattis,4 Ivan Rusyn,5 and Kathryn Z. Guyton3 1Office of Environmental Health Hazard Assessment, California Environmental Protection Agency, Oakland, California, USA; 2Institut
    [Show full text]
  • The Human Genome Project: Biology, Computers, and Privacy. INSTITUTION Biological Sciences Curriculum Study, Colorado Springs
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 461 499 SE 061 488 AUTHOR Cutter, Mary Ann G.; Drexler, Edward; Gottesman, Kay S.; Goulding, Philip G.; McCullough, Laurence B.; McInerney, Joseph D.; Micikas, Lynda B.; Mural, Richard J.; Murray, Jeffrey C.; Zola, John TITLE The Human Genome Project: Biology, Computers, and Privacy. INSTITUTION Biological Sciences Curriculum Study, Colorado Springs. SPONS AGENCY Department of Energy, Washington, DC. PUB DATE 1996-00-00 NOTE 212p.; Distribution of material supported the U.S. Department of Energy and the University of Iowa Genome Center. Accompanying software not available from ERIC. CONTRACT DE-FG03-93ER61584 AVAILABLE FROM Biological Science and Curriculum Study, Colorado Springs, CO 80918-3842. Tel: 719-531-5550; Fax: 719-531-9104; e-mail: [email protected]; Web site: http://www.bscs.org. For full text: http://www.bscs.org/pdf/projects/HGN2/HGN-II.pdf. PUB TYPE Guides Classroom Learner (051) Guides Classroom Teacher (052) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC09 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Biology; *Ethics; Evolution; Genetic Engineering; *Genetics; High Schools; *Public Policy; Science Activities; *Science and Society; Science Curriculum; Science Education; Technology IDENTIFIERS Biological Sciences Curriculum Study; *Human Genome Project ABSTRACT This module, for high school teachers, is the second of two modules about the Human Genome Project (HGP) produced by the Biological Sciences Curriculum Study (BSCS). The first section of this module provides background information for teachers about the structure and objectives of the HGP, aspects of the science and technology that underlie the HGP, information science as it relates to the HGP, and ethics and public policy, especially as they concern information science and the HGP.
    [Show full text]