Constraint, natural selection, and the evolution of human body form Kristen R. R. Savella,1, Benjamin M. Auerbacha, and Charles C. Rosemanb,c aDepartment of Anthropology, The University of Tennessee, Knoxville, TN 37996; bDepartment of Cell Biology and Anatomy, Cumming School of Medicine, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada T2N 1N4; and cProgram in Ecology, Evolution, and Conservation Biology, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61802 Edited by Richard G. Klein, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, and approved May 26, 2016 (received for review March 3, 2016) Variation in body form among human groups is structured by a blend natural selection, rather than having arisen due to a correlated re- of natural selection driven by local climatic conditions and random sponse to selection on other traits. genetic drift. However, attempts to test ecogeographic hypotheses The distinction between the action of selection on a trait (a have not distinguished between adaptive traits (i.e., those that trait’s influence on fitness) and the evolutionary response of a evolved as a result of selection) and those that evolved as a correlated trait to natural selection is a cornerstone of modern evolutionary response to selection on other traits (i.e., nonadaptive traits), compli- theory (20). When natural selection is acting on a set of geneti- cating our understanding of the relationship between climate and cally covarying characteristics, individual responses to selection morphological distinctions among populations. Here, we use evolu- are a function of the relationship between that characteristic and tionary quantitative methods to test if traits previously identified as fitness independent of all other traits, as well as the correlated supporting ecogeographic hypotheses were actually adaptive by responses imparted by directional selection acting on genetically estimating the force of selection on individual traits needed to drive covarying traits. Fig. 1 (adapted with permission from ref. 21) among-group differentiation. Our results show that not all associa- shows this contrast graphically: if traits are uncorrelated, then tions between trait means and latitude were caused by selection they will respond to selection in the same direction as the force of acting directly on each individual trait. Although radial and tibial selection irrespective of what direction in the morphospace se- length and biiliac and femoral head breadth show signs of responses lection is acting (Fig. 1A). However, genetic correlations among to directional selection matching ecogeographic hypotheses, the femur traits may cause the response to selection not to match the di- was subject to little or no directional selection despite having shorter rection of selection (β2 and Δz2;Fig.1B), unless the response to values by latitude. Additionally, in contradiction to ecogeographic selection closely parallels the main axis of variation (β and Δz ; hypotheses, the humerus was under directional selection for longer 1 1 Fig. 1B). Distinguishing between the action of selection (the es- values by latitude. Responses to directional selection in the tibia and timated selective force) and the response to selection (the evolu- radius induced a nonadaptive correlated response in the humerus that tionary change) is important because when there are strong genetic overwhelmed its own trait-specific response to selection. This result correlations among traits, as there are in those reflecting body form emphasizes that mean differences between groups are not good indicators of which traits are adaptations in the absence of informa- (3, 4), the magnitude of realized differences among groups may not tion about covariation among characteristics. reflect the strength and direction of natural selection. Adaptation will not be apparent from differences in means between groups. natural selection | ecogeographic variation | Bergmann’srule| Similar studies have demonstrated that this is the case in cranial – Allen’srule| evolutionary constraints (22, 23), pelvic (24 26), and hand and foot bones (27). The standard ecogeographic model assumes the mean body form of groups migrating away from the tropics evolved from mong-group variation in human body form is the result of a tropically adapted ancestral condition (10–16). We take the Agenetic drift, gene flow, and natural selection (1–4). Patterns of variation in human body shape appear to match ecogeographic expectations set out by Bergmann (5) and Allen (6). Comparisons Significance of average body form among groups of humans living at different latitudes reflect these rules. Distal limb segments decrease relative Human morphological variation is thought to have been partially to proximal limb segments, body mass increases, and the pelvis shaped by natural selection associated with environmental fac- widens as latitude increases (7–16). The prevailing explanation for tors like climate. Patterns of variation in body form correspond these patterns is that among-group variation in body proportions with latitude, but evolutionary processes that yielded this vari- was shaped by natural selection acting on thermoregulatory efficacy ation are not yet established. Examining the traits used in these (8,9,11). studies (e.g., limb lengths) independently ignores their genetic Two problems with this interpretation require further investiga- covariation, which affects their responses to evolutionary forces. tion. First, there is considerable overlap between the distribution of To address this relationship, we estimated the directional selec- climate across latitude and the migratory routes of recent range tion necessary to evolve correlated traits reflecting body shape expansions of humans from Africa into other parts of the world across latitudes and examined trait-specific responses. Although (2, 17–19). Thus, patterns of body form variation among groups most traits appear to be under directional selection, their response is constrained by between-trait covariance. This finding suggests could reflect random genetic drift and gene flow and mimic the that trait differences among human groups may not directly re- hypothesized effects of climate. Correspondingly, among-group flect the forces of selection that shaped them. differences in traits strongly associated with Bergmann’s and Allen’s — — rules distal limb segment lengths and body breadth best fit Author contributions: B.M.A. and C.C.R. designed research; K.R.R.S., B.M.A., and C.C.R. models that include both latitude and random genetic drift (4). In performed research; K.R.R.S. and C.C.R. analyzed data; and K.R.R.S., B.M.A., and C.C.R. contrast, the among-group variation in proximal limb segment wrote the paper. lengths and femoral head diameter (a proxy for body mass) was The authors declare no conflict of interest. described best by population structure alone (4). A second, pre- This article is a PNAS Direct Submission. viously unexplored issue is that all studies of ecogeographic varia- 1To whom correspondence should be addressed. Email: [email protected]. tion in body form to date have worked under the assumption This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10. that morphological differences among populations are the result of 1073/pnas.1603632113/-/DCSupplemental. 9492–9497 | PNAS | August 23, 2016 | vol. 113 | no. 34 www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1603632113 Downloaded by guest on September 28, 2021 Fig. 1. The differences between the force of di- rectional selection (β) and responses to selection ðΔzÞ for two traits that are independent (A) or that strongly covary (B). In both examples, we subject the trait means to two selection gradients (β1 and β2). The traits are uncorrelated in A, so responses to selection ðΔz1, Δz2Þ will closely match the vectors of the selection gradients (β1 and β2). Studies that use the mean differences in traits between groups to indicate how those traits responded to selection implicitly assume this framework for selection. However, in B, the strong covariance between traits affects the ability of the traits to respond to certain selection gradients. A selection gradient (β1) similar to the major axis of variation will result in responses to selection that closely parallel that axis and yield a larger response than in uncorrelated traits. In con- trast, a selection gradient that greatly departs from the major axis (β2), and which is positive for trait 1 but zero for trait 2, will nonetheless result in positive responses to selection ðΔz2Þ for both traits because of their covariance. Adapted with permission from ref. 21; see box 1 and figure 1 in ref. 21 for a more comprehensive exploration of the effect of trait covariance on responses to selection. average morphology of several groups from sub-Saharan Africa to pressures on individual traits. Positive selection gradients between represent the morphological range of variation for putatively groups at different latitudes indicate a net positive relationship with tropically adapted groups. We then used groups from regions of fitness (for long bones to lengthen or biiliac breadth and femoral higher latitude (including North Africa, temperate Europe, and head diameter to broaden). Alternatively, negative selection gradi- the circumpolar Arctic) to represent the range of variation in body ents between groups at different latitudes indicate a net negative form that resulted, in part, from climate mediated natural selec- relationship with fitness. We also decompose separate
Details
-
File Typepdf
-
Upload Time-
-
Content LanguagesEnglish
-
Upload UserAnonymous/Not logged-in
-
File Pages6 Page
-
File Size-