The Acquisition of English Dative Constructions

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Acquisition of English Dative Constructions Applied Psycholinguistics 22 (2001), 253–267 Printed in the United States of America The acquisition of English dative constructions AIMEE L. CAMPBELL Emory University MICHAEL TOMASELLO Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Michael Tomasello, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Inselstrasse 22, D-04103 Leipzig, Germany. E-mail: [email protected] ABSTRACT We analyzed the three main types of English dative constructions – the double-object dative, the to dative, and the for dative – in the spontaneous speech of seven children from the age of 1;6 to 5;0. The main findings were as follows. First, the double-object dative was acquired by most of the children before either of the prepositional datives; this was attributed to the greater frequency with which children heard this construction with individual verbs. Second, the verbs children used with these constructions were not only the adult prototypical ones, but also a number of the less prototypi- cal ones; again, this was very likely due to their frequency and saliency in the language children heard. Third, no support was found for Ninio’s (1999) analysis of the emergence of constructions in terms of a single “pathbreaking” verb; rather, children began using the double-object dative with many different verbs and did not follow the trajectory proposed by Ninio (i.e., a single verb is used for some months before an “explosion” of new verbs is introduced in the construction). Finally, most of the verbs initially used in the three dative constructions were first used in other constructions (e.g., a simple transitive); this was even true for some obligatory datives, such as give and show. The current results provide a starting point for determining the underlying representations for the different kinds of dative constructions and for explicating how children understand the interrelations among these and other constructions. All languages of the world have grammatical constructions for expressing the transfer of objects (and other things) between people (Newman, 1996). In English, a constellation of three related constructions is responsible for doing this: the to dative, the for dative (or benefactive), and the double-object dative (or ditransitive). These English constructions are interesting from a developmental point of view for several reasons: (a) each refers to a salient semantic situation for children and so is acquired relatively early; (b) each is relatively coherent semantically in that it is always used for some kind of trans- fer between people (either literal or metaphorical); and (c) each is cognitively 2001 Cambridge University Press 0142-7164/01 $9.50 Applied Psycholinguistics 22:2 254 Campbell & Tomasello: Acquisition of English dative constructions complex in that it involves three participants (donor, recipient, gift) (Dixon, 1991). Although virtually all accounts of children’s early acquisition of English note the existence of these constructions, few studies have directly examined them. One exception is a study by Snyder and Stomswold (1997) in the UG frame- work; the authors used children’s acquisition of the dative constructions as part of an argument about parameter setting in language acquisition. Specifically, they argued from UG analyses, presenting some suggestive evidence, that chil- dren should always acquire the double-object dative before they acquire the other two constructions. Another exception is a study by Osgood and Zehler (1981); here, the authors provided evidence that children understand prototypi- cal instantiations of these constructions (in which two human participants ex- change an inanimate entity) before they understand less prototypical uses. Fi- nally, Tomasello (1998) gave a descriptive account of one child’s use of expressions of possession; he found that dative constructions were among the earliest constructions to emerge. In addition, a number of proposals have been made about the relation between children’s grasp of verb meanings and constructions in general, often granting a prominent role to the dative constructions. First, Gropen, Pinker, Hollander, Goldberg, and Wilson (1989) studied children’s acquisition of the dative alterna- tion (i.e., their ability to identify verbs that do and do not participate in both double-object dative and to dative constructions). The basic question involved children’s identification of the verb classes that do and do not alternate; this was part of a larger set of questions about the relation between verbs and con- structions in general (e.g., Pinker, 1989). Second, Goldberg and Sethuraman (n.d.) argued that all of the most basic verb-argument constructions of English have one or more basic verbs – usually a “light” verb – as their central sense. In the case of the dative constructions, this verb is give. They provided some evidence that many children learn their first dative construction with give. Fi- nally, Ninio (1999) presented a related argument that children acquire their earli- est constructions on the basis of one or two “pathbreaking” verbs (again, usually a light verb), which pave the way for the acquisition of other verbs for use in that construction. Although Ninio did not address the dative constructions in particular, her proposals are relevant here, as these constructions form a very distinctive group. This article presents the results of a corpus study of seven children from the CHILDES database. The purpose of the study was to document the manner in which children acquired their earliest to dative, for dative, and double-object dative constructions. The following questions were posed. First, which of the three constructions did children use first? Second, which verbs were used (and which of these were used first) in the three constructions? Third, did the verbs used first in these constructions have a prior history of use in other construc- tions? Finally, what was the role of child-directed speech in the acquisition of dative constructions? In answering these questions we hoped to provide insight not only into children’s acquisition of these particular constructions, but also into the general processes of language acquisition – especially with regard to the relation between verbs and constructions. Applied Psycholinguistics 22:2 255 Campbell & Tomasello: Acquisition of English dative constructions METHOD Data Participants consisted of seven children from the CHILDES database (Mac- Whinney, 1995). These children were chosen to represent a large span of early language development during the preschool years. Included in the study were all the transcripts for Eve, from ages 1;6 to 2;3 (Brown, 1973); Adam, from ages 2;3.4 to 4;10.23 (Brown, 1973); Sarah, from ages 2;3.5 to 5;1.6 (Brown, 1973); Peter, from ages 1;9.7 to 3;1.21 (Bloom, 1970); Abe, from ages 2;4.24 to 5;0.11 (Kuczaj, 1976); Naomi, from ages 1;2.29 to 4;9.3 (Sachs, 1983); and Nina, from ages 1;11.16 to 3;3.21 (Suppes, 1974). Analytic procedure All transcripts were initially searched by hand, as a part of a larger study of the development of early verb-argument constructions. As a part of this process, all utterances identified as to datives, for datives, or double-object datives were collated. In all cases, the utterance in question had to have two postverbal argu- ments and had to convey some kind of transfer of objects or information (e.g., telling people secrets, reading books to children, and making things for people). In double-object datives, the recipient appeared postverbally before the gift (as in X gives Y the Z). In to datives, the gift appeared before the recipient, which was marked by either to (as in X gives the Z to Y) or a schwa. In for datives, the gift also appeared before the recipient, which was marked by for (as in X makes the Z for Y). For purposes of the present study, the authors analyzed and tabulated these utterances as needed for each of the specific research questions. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The results address three main questions. First, what was the order of first uses of the three different dative construction types? Second, which verbs were used in these constructions? Third, when were the very earliest uses of these construc- tions and verbs? First uses of the constructions Snyder and Stomswold (1997) claimed that all of the children in their sample (some of whom overlap with the current sample) learned double-object datives before to datives. This generalization has some truth to it, but the situation is actually more complex than this if we look at both individual children and their individual verbs. First of all, there was a large range in the age of acquisition for the constructions. Eve produced her first construction when she was only 18 months of age, whereas Sarah was 33 months of age when she initially used a dative. The other five children began producing constructions somewhere in between these two ages. Although the age of initial production showed large variance among the children, patterns in the order of acquisition were apparent. As can be seen in Table 1, five of the seven children produced double-object Applied Psycholinguistics 22:2 256 Campbell & Tomasello: Acquisition of English dative constructions Table 1. Age of first use of the three dative constructions for the seven children Double-object datives To datives For datives Eve 1;6a 1;10 1;11 Nina 1;11.29a 2;0.17 2;1.15 Peter 2;1.21 2;0.7a 2;1.21 Naomi 2;1.7a 2;5.3 2;3.19 Adam 2;3.4a 2;11.13 2;10.30 Abe 2;6.14 2;6.18 2;5.20a Sarah 2;9.29a 3;2.23 3;0.18 aIndicates the first construction type used by the child. datives before either to datives or for datives.
Recommended publications
  • Diachrony of Ergative Future
    • THE EVOLUTION OF THE TENSE-ASPECT SYSTEM IN HINDI/URDU: THE STATUS OF THE ERGATIVE ALGNMENT Annie Montaut INALCO, Paris Proceedings of the LFG06 Conference Universität Konstanz Miriam Butt and Tracy Holloway King (Editors) 2006 CSLI Publications http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/ Abstract The paper deals with the diachrony of the past and perfect system in Indo-Aryan with special reference to Hindi/Urdu. Starting from the acknowledgement of ergativity as a typologically atypical feature among the family of Indo-European languages and as specific to the Western group of Indo-Aryan dialects, I first show that such an evolution has been central to the Romance languages too and that non ergative Indo-Aryan languages have not ignored the structure but at a certain point went further along the same historical logic as have Roman languages. I will then propose an analysis of the structure as a predication of localization similar to other stative predications (mainly with “dative” subjects) in Indo-Aryan, supporting this claim by an attempt of etymologic inquiry into the markers for “ergative” case in Indo- Aryan. Introduction When George Grierson, in the full rise of language classification at the turn of the last century, 1 classified the languages of India, he defined for Indo-Aryan an inner circle supposedly closer to the original Aryan stock, characterized by the lack of conjugation in the past. This inner circle included Hindi/Urdu and Eastern Panjabi, which indeed exhibit no personal endings in the definite past, but only gender-number agreement, therefore pertaining more to the adjectival/nominal class for their morphology (calâ, go-MSG “went”, kiyâ, do- MSG “did”, bola, speak-MSG “spoke”).
    [Show full text]
  • TWO FORMS of "BE" in MALAYALAM1 Tara Mohanan and K.P
    TWO FORMS OF "BE" IN MALAYALAM1 Tara Mohanan and K.P. Mohanan National University of Singapore 1. INTRODUCTION Malayalam has two verbs, uNTE and aaNE, recognized in the literature as copulas (Asher 1968, Variar 1979, Asher and Kumari 1997, among others). There is among speakers of Malayalam a clear, intuitively perceived meaning difference between the verbs. One strong intuition is that uNTE and aaNE correspond to the English verbs "have" and "be"; another is that they should be viewed as the "existential" and "equative" copulas respectively.2 However, in a large number of contexts, these verbs appear to be interchangeable. This has thwarted the efforts of a clear characterization of the meanings of the two verbs. In this paper, we will explore a variety of syntactic and semantic environments that shed light on the differences between the constructions with aaNE and uNTE. On the basis of the asymmetries we lay out, we will re-affirm the intuition that aaNE and uNTE are indeed equative and existential copulas respectively, with aaNE signaling the meaning of “x is an element/subset of y" and uNTE signaling the meaning of existence of an abstract or concrete entity in the fields of location or possession. We will show that when aaNE is interchangable with uNTE in existential clauses, its function is that of a cleft marker, with the existential meaning expressed independently by the case markers on the nouns. Central to our exploration of the two copulas is the discovery of four types of existential clauses in Malayalam, namely: Neutral: with the existential verb uNTE.
    [Show full text]
  • MR Harley Miyagawa Syntax of Ditransitives
    Syntax of Ditransitives Heidi Harley and Shigeru Miyagawa (in press, Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics) July 2016 Summary Keywords 1. Structure for the Two Internal Arguments 2. Underlying Order 3. Meaning Differences 4. Case, Clitic 5. The Structure of Ditransitives 6. Nominalization Asymmetries 6.1. A Morphological Account of the Nominalization Asymmetry 6.2. –kata Nominalization in Japanese and Myer’s Generalization 6.3 Selectional Accounts of the Nominalization Asymmetry 6.4 Applicative vs Small Clause Approaches to the DOC. 7. Constraints on the Dative/DOC Alternation 7.1 Morphological Constraints 7.2 Lexical Semantic Constraints 7.3 Information-Structural and Sentential Prosody Constraints 8. Overview and Prospects Further Reading References Summary Ditransitive predicates select for two internal arguments, and hence minimally entail the participation of three entities in the event described by the verb. Canonical ditranstive verbs include give, show and teach; in each case, the verb requires an Agent (a giver, shower or teacher, respectively), a Theme (the thing given, shown or taught) and a Goal (the recipient, viewer, or student). The property of requiring two internal arguments makes ditransitive verbs syntactically unique. Selection in generative grammar is often modelled as syntactic sisterhood, so ditranstive verbs immediately raise the question of whether a verb might have two sisters, requiring a ternary-branching structure, or whether one of the two internal arguments is not in a sisterhood relation with the verb. Another important property of English ditransitive constructions is the two syntactic structures associated with them. In the so-called “Double Object Construction”, or DOC, the Goal and Theme both are simple NPs and appear following the verb in the order V-Goal-Theme.
    [Show full text]
  • The Special Datives
    The Special Datives To this point, the functions of the Dative Case have been 1. Indirect Object of give, tell, show verbs 2. Dative with Special Adjectives friendly to, unfriendly to, similar to, dissimilar to, equal to, suitable for, near to, dear to, pleasing to, etc. 3. Dative with Special Intransitive Verbs parco, mando, impero, noceo, resisto, studeo, etc. 4. Dative with Certain Compound Verbs praesum, praeficio, occurro, etc. (often verbs with prefixes of ob- and prae-) Two new Dative Case functions, sometimes called Special Datives, are explained in Unit XIII. These are the Dative of Purpose and the Dative of Reference. 1. Dative of Purpose. Sometimes, the idea of purpose can be stated in a single noun. In such a case, the Dative Case form of the noun is used. It answers the question, “For what purpose does something exist?” Note how we often ask, “What is that for?” The consul donated money for a reward. Consul pecuniam praemio donavit. Seven common Latin nouns are often used as (for?!) the Dative of Purpose: Principal Parts Dative Singular Form Meaning cura, -ae, f. curae for a concern auxilium, -I, n. auxilio for a help impedimentum, -I, n. impedimento for an obstacle praemium, -I, n. praemio for a reward praesidium, -I, n. praesidio for a guard subsidium, -I, n. subsidio for a support usus,, -us, m. usui for a use NOTA BENE: These are not the only nouns which may be used for purpose; they are only the most common. Sometimes a plural Dative Case form is used for purpose. Catullus poeta scripsit puellas esse curis.
    [Show full text]
  • The Old English To-Dative Construction1 Ghent
    1 The Old English to-dative construction1 LUDOVIC DE CUYPERE Ghent University 1 Acknowledgements: I wish to acknowledge helpful comments and suggestions by Cynthia Allen and Harold Koch. I am grateful to Daan Van den Nest for assisting me with the retrieval of the corpus data I also thank Joan Bresnan for providing me with an R script to perform the cross- validation test. Finally, I am grateful to the editor and the referees for several comments and suggestions that helped to improve the paper. 2 Abstract In Present Day English (PDE), the to-dative construction refers to clauses like John told/offered/mentioned/gave the books to Mary, in which a ditransitive verb takes a Recipient that is expressed as a to-Prepositional Phrase (to-PP). This study examines the to-dative construction in Old English (OE). I show, first of all, that this construction was not rare in OE, in contrast to what has been suggested in the literature. Second, I report on two corpus studies in which I examined the ordering behaviour of the NP and the to-PP. The results of the first study suggest that the same ordering tendencies already existed in OE as in PDE: both the NP-to-PP and the to- PP-NP orders were grammatical, but the NP-to-PP was the most frequently used one. However, in OE, the to-PP-NP was more common than in PDE, where its use is heavily restricted. My second corpus study is informed by the multifactorial approach to the English dative alternation and uses a mixed-effects logistic regression analysis to evaluate the effects of various linguistic (verbal semantics, pronominality, animacy, definiteness, number, person and length) and extra- linguistic variables (translation status, time of completion/manuscript) on the ordering of NP and to-PP.
    [Show full text]
  • First Objects and Datives: Two of a Kind?
    BLS 32 February 2006 First Objects and Datives: Two of a Kind? Beth Levin Stanford University ([email protected]) 1 The big question There are two options for expressing recipients in English with dative verbs—verbs taking agent, recipient, and theme arguments, such as give and other verbs of causation of possession. (1) a. Terry gave Sam an apple. (double object construction) b. Terry gave an apple to Sam. (to construction) Many languages which lack a double object construction still have a core (i.e., nonadjunct) grammatical relation, distinct from subject and object, used to express recipient. Specifically, many languages have a dative case and use the dative (case marked) NP as the basic realization of possessors, including recipients of verbs of causation of possession. (2) Ja dal Ivanu knigu. I.NOM give.PST Ivan.DAT book.ACC ‘I gave Ivan a book.’ (RUSSIAN; dative construction) A PERENNIAL QUESTION: What is the status of the first object in the double object construction? — Is it comparable to the object of a transitive verb? AN ANSWER: YES is implicit in the label “first/primary/inner object” and is supported by its passivizability and postverbal position; cf. Dryer’s (1986) “primary object languages”. (3) Sam was given an apple. — Is it comparable to the dative NP of languages with a dative case? AN ANSWER: In the context of the answer to the previous question, NO is the usual answer given; it is the NP in the to phrase in the to construction that is equated with the dative NP. THE COMPLICATION: Repeated observations that despite surface similarities with direct objects, recipients in the double object construction do not show all direct object properties (e.g., Baker 1997; Hudson 1992; Maling 2001; Marantz 1993; Polinsky 1996; Ziv & Sheintuch 1979).
    [Show full text]
  • Title Serial Verb Construction in Cantonese-Speaking
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by HKU Scholars Hub Serial verb construction in Cantonese-speaking preschool Title children with and without language impairment Author(s) Chan, Ka-yee; 陳嘉儀 Chan, K. [陳嘉儀]. (2014). Serial verb construction in Cantonese- speaking preschool children with and without language Citation impairment. (Thesis). University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam, Hong Kong SAR. Issued Date 2014 URL http://hdl.handle.net/10722/238921 The author retains all proprietary rights, (such as patent rights) and the right to use in future works.; This work is licensed under Rights a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTION IN CANTONESE PRESCHOOLER 1 Serial Verb Construction in Cantonese-Speaking Preschool Children With and Without Language Impairment CHAN Ka Yee A dissertation submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the Bachelor of Science (Speech and Hearing Sciences), The University of Hong Kong, June 30, 2014. SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTION IN CANTONESE PRESCHOOLER 2 List of Abbreviations CL Noun Classifier LP Linking Particle PERT Perfective Aspect Marker V-PRT Verbal Particle SERIAL VERB CONSTRUCTION IN CANTONESE PRESCHOOLER 3 Serial Verb Construction in Cantonese-Speaking Preschool Children With and Without Language Impairment CHAN Ka Yee Abstract Serial verb construction (SVC) is very productive in Cantonese and it develops actively in the preschool years. Little is known about the development of SVC of children with language impairment (LI). Forty-four kindergarten children with and without LI, aged between 4 to 6 years, participated in the study. This study made use of a video description task to examine the developmental and error patterns of five subtypes of SVC, which were directional, instrumental, benefactive, purpose and dative SVC.
    [Show full text]
  • Plural Addressee Marker and Grammaticalization in Barayin
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by SOAS Research Online Published as: Lovestrand, Joseph. 2018. Plural addressee marker and Pre-publication version grammaticalization in Barayin. Journal of Afroasiatic Languages and Linguistics 10(1) https://doi.org/10.1163/18776930-01001004 Accepted version downloaded from SOAS Research Online: http:// eprints.soas.ac.uk/32651 Plural addressee marker and grammaticalization in Barayin Joseph Lovestrand, University of Oxford Abstract This article describes two distinct but related grammaticalization paths in Barayin, an East Chadic lan- guage. One path is from a first-person plural pronoun to a first-person dual pronoun. Synchronically, the pronominal forms in Barayin with first-person dual number must now be combined with a plural addressee enclitic, nà, to create a first-person plural pronoun. This path is identical to what has been documented in Philippine-type languages. The other path is from a first-person dative suffix to a suffix dedicated to first-person hortative. This path of grammaticalization has not been discussed in the literature. It occurred in several related languages, and each in case results in a hortative form with a dual subject. Hortative forms with a plural subject are created by adding a plural addressee marker to the dual form. The plural addressee marker in Chadic languages is derived from a second-person pronominal. Keywords Chadic, Barayin, hortative, dual, pronouns, diachronic1 1 Introduction This paper describes two distinct but related grammaticalization paths in Barayin [bva] and other lan- guages of the Guera subbranch of East Chadic languages (East Chadic B).
    [Show full text]
  • The Biabsolutive Construction in Lak and Tsez
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Lingua 150 (2014) 137--170 www.elsevier.com/locate/lingua The biabsolutive construction in Lak and Tsez Annie Gagliardi a, Michael Goncalves a, Maria Polinsky a,*, Nina Radkevich b a Harvard University, USA b University of York, United Kingdom Received 12 November 2013; received in revised form 4 July 2014; accepted 8 July 2014 Available online Abstract In ergative constructions, the agent of a transitive verb is in the ergative case and the theme is in the absolutive case. By contrast, in biabsolutive constructions, both the agent and theme of a transitive verb appear in the absolutive case. This paper presents and analyzes the biabsolutive construction in two Nakh-Dagestanian languages, Lak and Tsez. Despite many surface similarities, the biabsolutive constructions in Lak and Tsez call for different syntactic analyses. We argue that the biabsolutive construction in Lak is an instance of restructuring in the presence of an aspectual head bearing a progressive (imperfective) feature. Tsez biabsolutive constructions, on the other hand, are biclausal; we argue that the theme and the lexical verb are contained in a PP complement selected by a light verb. Related languages may be classified as ‘‘Lak-type’’ or ‘‘Tsez-type’’ based on the behavior of their biabsolutives. The existence of two underlying structures for one surface pattern in Nakh-Dagestanian poses a learnability problem for a child acquiring a language with biabsolutive constructions. We outline a set of strategies used by a learner who must compare the available input data with a set of structural hypotheses. © 2014 Elsevier B.V.
    [Show full text]
  • A Case Study in Language Change
    Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Honors Theses Lee Honors College 4-17-2013 Glottopoeia: A Case Study in Language Change Ian Hollenbaugh Western Michigan University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/honors_theses Part of the Other English Language and Literature Commons Recommended Citation Hollenbaugh, Ian, "Glottopoeia: A Case Study in Language Change" (2013). Honors Theses. 2243. https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/honors_theses/2243 This Honors Thesis-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Lee Honors College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Honors Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. An Elementary Ghau Aethauic Grammar By Ian Hollenbaugh 1 i. Foreword This is an essential grammar for any serious student of Ghau Aethau. Mr. Hollenbaugh has done an excellent job in cataloguing and explaining the many grammatical features of one of the most complex language systems ever spoken. Now published for the first time with an introduction by my former colleague and premier Ghau Aethauic scholar, Philip Logos, who has worked closely with young Hollenbaugh as both mentor and editor, this is sure to be the definitive grammar for students and teachers alike in the field of New Classics for many years to come. John Townsend, Ph.D Professor Emeritus University of Nunavut 2 ii. Author’s Preface This grammar, though as yet incomplete, serves as my confession to what J.R.R. Tolkien once called “a secret vice.” History has proven Professor Tolkien right in thinking that this is not a bizarre or freak occurrence, undergone by only the very whimsical, but rather a common “hobby,” one which many partake in, and have partaken in since at least the time of Hildegard of Bingen in the twelfth century C.E.
    [Show full text]
  • Ditransitive Constructions with Differentially Marked Direct Objects in Romanian Alexandra Cornilescu University of Bucharest
    Chapter 5 Ditransitive constructions with differentially marked direct objects in Romanian Alexandra Cornilescu University of Bucharest The paper discusses Romanian data that had gone unnoticed so far and investi- gates the differences of grammaticality triggered by differentially marked direct objects in ditransitive constructions, in binding configurations. Specifically, while a bare direct object (DO) may bind a possessor contained in the indirect object (IO), whether or not the IO is clitic doubled, a differentially marked DO may bind into an undoubled IO, but cannot bind into an IO if the latter is clitic doubled. Gram- maticality is restored if the DO is clitic doubled in its turn. The focus of the paper is to offer a derivational account of ditransitive constructions, which accounts for these differences. The claim is that the grammaticality contrasts mentioned above result from the different feature structures of bare DOs compared with differen- tially marked ones, as well as from the fact that differentially marked DOs and IO have common features. Differentially marked DOs interfere with IOs since both are sensitive to the animacy hierarchy, and include a syntactic [Person] feature in their featural make-up. The derivational valuation of this feature by both objects may create locality problems. 1 Problem and aim In this paper, I turn to data not discussed for Romanian so far and consider the differences of grammaticality triggered by differentially marked direct objects (i.e. DOs with Differential Object Marking, from now one, DOM-ed DOs) indi- transitive constructions, in binding configurations. Alexandra Cornilescu. 2020. Ditransitive constructions with differentially marked direct objects in Romanian.
    [Show full text]
  • Dative by Genitive Replacement in the Greek Language of the Papyri: a Diachronic Account of Case Semantics*
    Journal of Greek Linguistics 15 (2015) 91–121 brill.com/jgl Dative by Genitive Replacement in the Greek Language of the Papyri: A Diachronic Account of Case Semantics* Joanne Vera Stolk Department of Philosophy, Classics, History of Art and Ideas, University of Oslo, Norway [email protected] Abstract Semantic analysis of the prenominal first person singular genitive pronoun (μου) in the Greek of the documentary papyri shows that the pronoun is typically found in the posi- tion between a verbal form and an alienable possessum which functions as the patient of the predicate. When the event expressed by the predicate is patient-affecting, the possessor is indirectly also affected. Hence the semantic role of this affected alienable possessor might be interpreted as a benefactive or malefactive in genitive possession constructions. By semantic extension the meaning of the genitive case in this posi- tion is extended into goal-oriented roles, such as addressee and recipient, which are commonly denoted by the dative case in Ancient Greek. The semantic similarity of the genitive and dative cases in these constructions might have provided the basis for the merger of the cases in the Greek language. Keywords Greek papyrology – cognitive linguistics – dative – genitive – pronouns – possession construction – case syncretism – affectedness – semantic extension * This article was presented at the 21st International Conference on Historical Linguistics, Oslo, August 5th–9th 2013. I would like to thank the participants for their contribution to the discussion as well as Anastasia Maravela, Mark Janse, Trevor Evans, Eirik Welo, Dag Haug and two anonymous reviewers for their useful comments on earlier drafts of this article.
    [Show full text]