Compound-V Formations in Shorebird Flocks

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Compound-V Formations in Shorebird Flocks bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/545301; this version posted February 8, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. Compound-V formations in shorebird flocks Aaron J. Corcoran1, Tyson L. Hedrick1* 1 University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill NC USA 27599 *author for correspondence Dr. Tyson Hedrick UNC Dept. of Biology Coker Hall CB #3280 120 South Road Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3280 Email: [email protected] Phone: (919) 962-0757 Keywords: bird, flocking, collective behavior, flight, biomechanics, migration 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/545301; this version posted February 8, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. 1 Abstract 2 Animal groups have emergent properties that result from simple interactions among individuals. However, we 3 know little about why animals adopt different interaction rules because of sparse sampling among species. Here, 4 we identify an interaction rule that holds across single and mixed-species flocks of four migratory shorebird 5 species spanning a seven-fold range of body masses. The rule, aligning with a 1-wingspan lateral distance to 6 nearest neighbors in the same horizontal plane, scales linearly with wingspan but is independent of nearest 7 neighbor distance and neighbor species. This rule propagates outward to create a global flock structure that we 8 term the compound-V formation. We propose that this formation represents an intermediary between the cluster 9 flocks of starlings and the simple-V formations of geese and other large migratory birds. Analysis of individual 10 wingbeat frequencies and airspeeds indicates that the compound-V formation may be an adaptation for 11 aerodynamic flocking. 12 13 Introduction 14 The collective movements of animals—from schooling fish to swarming insects and flocking birds—have long 15 excited intrigue among observers of nature. Collective motion arises as an emergent property of interactions 16 between individuals [reviewed by (Herbert-Read, 2016; Vicsek & Zafeiris, 2012)]. Thus, much attention has been 17 placed on identifying local interaction rules (Ballerini et al., 2008; Herbert-Read et al., 2011; Katz, Tunstrøm, 18 Ioannou, Huepe, & Couzin, 2011; Lukeman, Li, & Edelstein-Keshet, 2010) and how those rules affect group 19 structure and movement (Buhl et al., 2006; Hemelrijk & Hildenbrandt, 2012). However, comparative data across 20 species are still limited, preventing us from testing hypotheses regarding the evolution and diversity of collective 21 movement patterns. 22 For example, out of hundreds of bird species that fly in groups, most research has focused on starlings (Attanasi 23 et al., 2014; Ballerini et al., 2008; Cavagna et al., 2010), homing pigeons (Nagy et al., 2013; Nagy, Ákos, Biro, & 24 Vicsek, 2010; Pettit, Ákos, Vicsek, & Biro, 2015; Pettit, Perna, Biro, & Sumpter, 2013; Usherwood, Stavrou, Lowe, 25 Roskilly, & Wilson, 2011) and birds that fly in V-formations (Badgerow & Hainsworth, 1981; Cutts & Speakman, 26 1994; Hummel, 1983; Lissaman & Shollenberger, 1970; Maeng, Park, Jang, & Han, 2013; Portugal et al., 2014; 27 Weimerskirch, Martin, Clerquin, Alexandre, & Jiraskova, 2001). These data indicate that smaller birds fly in 28 relatively dense cluster flocks that facilitate group cohesion and information transfer (Attanasi et al., 2014; 29 Ballerini et al., 2008), whereas larger migratory birds fly in highly-structured V formations (also known as line or 30 echelon formations) that provide aerodynamic and energetic benefits (Lissaman & Shollenberger, 1970; Portugal 31 et al., 2014; Weimerskirch et al., 2001). The species whose flocking behavior have been studied in detail differ in 32 many ways that could be important for flocking including body size, ecology, the frequency of aggregation and its 33 behavioral context. Therefore, it is difficult to conclude based on the available data what factors contribute to 34 birds adopting specific group formations. 35 We aimed to address this question by collecting three-dimensional (3D) trajectories of the birds in flocks of four 36 shorebird species that have similar ecologies (all forage in large groups in coastal habitats and migrate long 37 distances) but cover a seven-fold range of body mass and two-fold range of wingspan. Our study species include 38 dunlin (Calidris alpina, Linnaeus 1758; 56 g, 0.34 m wingspan), short-billed dowitcher (Limnodromus griseus, 39 Gmelin 1789; 110 g, 0.52 m wingspan), American avocet (Recurvirostra Americana, Gmelin 1789; 312 g, 0.72 m 40 wingspan), and marbled godwit (Limosa fedoa, Linnaeus, 1758; 370 g, 0.78 m). Molecular dating indicates that 41 these species diverged from their nearest common ancestor by approximately 50 Mya (Baker, Pereira, & Paton, 42 2007), providing time for evolutionary diversification of flocking behavior. By comparing group structure of birds 2 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/545301; this version posted February 8, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. 43 across a range of body sizes and comparing our data from that in the literature, we aimed to determine the extent 44 to which flock structure varies across species with different body sizes and ecologies. We employ three 45 approaches: 1) identify local interaction rules by quantifying the relative positions of birds and their nearest 46 neighbors; 2) quantify the degree of spatial structure within flocks; and 3) use measurements of individual speeds 47 and wingbeat frequencies to test whether local or global 48 position within the flock affects flight performance. a 49 Based on existing flock data, we hypothesized that flocks Humboldt bay 50 of larger shorebird species would be more structured High-tide roost 51 than smaller species (recapitulating the trend of larger Coastal habitat 52 birds flying in highly-structured V formations) and that 120 m Flock path 53 larger species would also more frequently exhibit Camera Camera view 54 aerodynamic formations. Because a previous study 270,000 m3 55 showed that flying in a cluster flock is energetically 56 costly in pigeons (Usherwood et al., 2011), we 57 hypothesized that birds flying in the middle and rear of b 58 flocks and birds flying closer to their nearest neighbor 40 59 would have reduced flight performance (lower speed 60 relative to their wingbeat frequency). Surprisingly, we 61 found that all four species studied here fly in a 30 62 previously-undescribed flock structure that we term the 63 compound-V formation. We propose that this structure Y (m) 20 64 is an adaptation for aerodynamic flocking in migratory 65 species, and that ecology is an underappreciated driver 66 of the evolution of avian flocking behavior. 10 67 Results 0 c 68 We reconstructed the 3D trajectories from 18 bird flocks 10 69 that ranged in size from 189 to 961 individuals and were 5 Z (m) 70 recorded for 2.4 – 13.2 s at 29.97 frames per second 0 0 10 20 30 40 50 71 (Figure 1, Table 1). This resulted in 1,598,169 3D position X (m) 72 measurements that were used for examining flock 73 structure. Sixteen of the 18 flocks were comprised Figure 1. Shorebird flock recording 74 entirely of a single species. The remaining two flocks (a) Multi-camera videography was used to reconstruct 75 were mixed-species flocks of marbled godwits and 3D trajectories of shorebirds flying near high-tide roosts 76 short-billed dowitchers. Computer vision techniques in Humboldt Bay, California. (b) Overhead and (c) profile 77 allowed species identification of individuals in mixed- views of an example flock. Symbol sizes reflect actual 78 species flocks based on differences in body size (See scales for birds with outstretched wings. 79 Materials and Methods). 3 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/545301; this version posted February 8, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. 80 Nearest neighbor alignment 81 We examined flock 82 structure by quantifying the Flock Species N N n.n. dist n.n. Ground Airspeed Wind Wind Z-speed Turn rate birds Frame s (m) (WS) power speed (m·s-1) speed direction (m·s-1) (° s-1) 83 position of each bird with (m·s-1) (m·s-1) (deg.) 1.30 1.67 5.43 9.36 1.12 42.6 84 respect to its nearest 0417-2 Godwit 286 147 0.361 5.73 5.9 0.79, 2.15 3.64, 8.66 8.04, 10.55 0.09, 1.97 18.5, 91.8 1.17 2.25 5.30 9.25 1.29 45.8 85 neighbor. We used modal 0417-2 Dowitcher 309 147 0.385 5.73 5.9 0.73, 1.89 3.56, 8.56 7.95, 10.48 0.23, 2.08 20.1, 97.7 1.81 2.32 3.33 7.94 -0.18 35.9 86 values to characterize 0417-3 Godwit 474 278 0.428 5.73 3.9 1.01, 2.94 1.72, 6.56 6.21, 9.78 -1.27, 0.76 0.04, 121.2 1.71 2.19 6.67 8.84 0.22 16.4 87 typical neighbor positions 0417-4 Godwit 802 397 0.391 5.73 -312.1 1.02, 2.57 3.29, 9.57 7.69, 10.06 -0.78, 1.09 5.2, 50.5 88 because position 1.57 2.25 4.49 8.69 0.41 22.5 0417-4 Dowitcher 74 397 0.382 5.73 -312.1 0.94, 2.37 2.76, 7.61 7.63, 9.75 -0.60, 1.26 7.2, 59.3 89 distributions were skewed 1.19 1.53 7.05 10.62 -0.26 24.5 0420-1 Godwit 628 177 0.409 4.06 5.5 0.60, 1.94 5.14, 9.85 8.57, 13.42 -1.83, 1.02 8.1, 95.4 90 as a result of values being 1.54 1.97 8.92 10.55 -0.07 13.1 0420-2 Godwit 304 147 0.431 4.06 54.8 91 cropped at zero.
Recommended publications
  • Relatedness, Parentage, and Philopatry Within a Natterer's Bat
    Population Ecology (2018) 60:361–370 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10144-018-0632-7 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Relatedness, parentage, and philopatry within a Natterer’s bat (Myotis nattereri) maternity colony David Daniel Scott1 · Emma S. M. Boston2 · Mathieu G. Lundy1 · Daniel J. Buckley2 · Yann Gager1 · Callum J. Chaplain1 · Emma C. Teeling2 · William Ian Montgomery1 · Paulo A. Prodöhl1 Received: 20 April 2017 / Accepted: 26 September 2018 / Published online: 10 October 2018 © The Author(s) 2018 Abstract Given their cryptic behaviour, it is often difficult to establish kinship within microchiropteran maternity colonies. This limits understanding of group formation within this highly social group. Following a concerted effort to comprehensively sample a Natterer’s bat (Myotis nattereri) maternity colony over two consecutive summers, we employed microsatellite DNA profiling to examine genetic relatedness among individuals. Resulting data were used to ascertain female kinship, parentage, mating strategies, and philopatry. Overall, despite evidence of female philopatry, relatedness was low both for adult females and juveniles of both sexes. The majority of individuals within the colony were found to be unrelated or distantly related. How- ever, parentage analysis indicates the existence of a number of maternal lineages (e.g., grandmother, mother, or daughter). There was no evidence suggesting that males born within the colony are mating with females of the same colony. Thus, in this species, males appear to be the dispersive sex. In the Natterer’s bat, colony formation is likely to be based on the benefits of group living, rather than kin selection. Keywords Chiroptera · Kinship · Natal philopatry · Parentage assignment Introduction of the environment (Schober 1984).
    [Show full text]
  • Swarming (Bulletin #404) (PDF)
    Swarming Apiculture Bulletin #404 Updated: 10/15 Swarming is a natural method of honeybee colonies to reproduce, resulting in the creation of a new honeybee colony in addition to the established colony. Contributing factors to swarming • Crowding - too many bees, food stores and no cell space for the queen to lay eggs in. • Poor air circulation • April-May is swarming season and healthy colonies develop strong swarm impulse. • Inclement weather - crowded bees confined by cold, wet weather will build queen cells and swarm out on the first sunny, warm day. All colonies in similar condition will swarm as soon as weather becomes favorable. • Large amount of drone brood in early spring is a precursor to strong swarm impulse. Catching the swarm A swarm generally emerges from the hive between 11:00AM and 1:00PM and settles close to the apiary for several hours. Allow the swarm to cluster for at least 30 minutes before placing the swarm in a single super with frames, bottom board and hive cover. Leave the hive for the remainder of the day to allow all the bees to enter, before moving to a permanent location. Examine the new colony after a week and then every two weeks from mid May to late June. Look for disease, brood abundance, brood pattern and overall condition of the colony. Add room where necessary, cut queen cells, or divide, to prevent other swarms to develop. Hived swarms have no stored food reserves and may go hungry when there is little forage. When feeding sugar syrup, add antibiotics as a precautionary measure.
    [Show full text]
  • What Is a Social Spider? “Sub”-Social Spiders Eusociality Social Definitions Natural History
    What is a Social Spider? • Generally accepted as living in colonies while having generational overlap and exhibiting cooperative brood care and nest maintenance Scott Trageser • Can also have reproductive division of labor and exhibit swarming behavior (Achaearanea wau) • Social hierarchies arise • Species and population dependant • Arguable if certain species qualify as eusocial “Sub”-Social Spiders Eusociality • Sociality also classified by territoriality and • There is cooperative brood-care so it is permanence not each one caring for their own offspring • Can be social on a seasonal basis and • There is an overlapping of generations so have an obligate solitary phase that the colony will sustain for a while, • Individuals can have established allowing offspring to assist parents during territories within the nest or can move their life freely • That there is a reproductive division of • Can have discrete webs connected to labor, i.e. not every individual reproduces other webs in the colony (cooperative?) equally in the group Social Definitions Natural History • Solitary: Showing none of the three features mentioned in the previous slide (most insects) • 23 of over 39,000 spider species are • Sub-social: The adults care for their own young for “social” some period of time (cockroaches) • Many other species are classified as • Communal: Insects use the same composite nest without cooperation in brood care (digger bees) “sub”-social • Quasi-social: Use the same nest and also show cooperative brood care (Euglossine bees and social • Tropical origin. Latitudinal and elevational spiders) distribution constrictions due to prey • Semi-social: in addition to the features in type/abundance quasisocial, they also have a worker caste (Halictid bees) • Emigrate (swarm) after courtship and • Eusocial: In addition to the features of semisocial, copulation but prior to oviposition there is overlap in generations (Honey bees).
    [Show full text]
  • The Use of Swarming Unmanned Vehicles to Support Target
    Forthcoming in Proceedings of SPIE Defense & Security Conference, March 2008, Orlando, FL Distributed Pheromone-Based Swarming Control of Unmanned Air and Ground Vehicles for RSTA John A. Sauter, Robert S. Joshua S. Robinson, John Stephanie Riddle Mathews, Andrew Yinger* Moody† Naval Air Systems NewVectors Augusta Systems, Inc. Command (NAVAIR) 3520 Green Ct 3592 Collins Ferry Road 48150 Shaw Road Ann Arbor, MI 48105 Suite 200 Bldg. 2109 Suite S211-21 Morgantown, WV 26505 Patuxent River, MD 20670 Abstract The use of unmanned vehicles in Reconnaissance, Surveillance, and Target Acquisition (RSTA) applications has received considerable attention recently. Cooperating land and air vehicles can support multiple sensor modalities providing pervasive and ubiquitous broad area sensor coverage. However coordination of multiple air and land vehicles serving different mission objectives in a dynamic and complex environment is a challenging problem. Swarm intelligence algorithms, inspired by the mechanisms used in natural systems to coordinate the activities of many entities provide a promising alternative to traditional command and control approaches. This paper describes recent advances in a fully distributed digital pheromone algorithm that has demonstrated its effectiveness in managing the complexity of swarming unmanned systems. The results of a recent demonstration at NASA’s Wallops Island of multiple Aerosonde Unmanned Air Vehicles (UAVs) and Pioneer Unmanned Ground Vehicles (UGVs) cooperating in a coordinated RSTA application are discussed. The vehicles were autonomously controlled by the onboard digital pheromone responding to the needs of the automatic target recognition algorithms. UAVs and UGVs controlled by the same pheromone algorithm self-organized to perform total area surveillance, automatic target detection, sensor cueing, and automatic target recognition with no central processing or control and minimal operator input.
    [Show full text]
  • On Adaptive Self-Organization in Artificial Robot Organisms
    On adaptive self-organization in artificial robot organisms Serge Kernbach∗, Heiko Hamann†, Jurgen¨ Stradner†, Ronald Thenius†, Thomas Schmickl†, Karl Crailsheim†, A.C. van Rossum‡, Michele Sebag§, Nicolas Bredeche§, Yao Yao¶, Guy Baele¶, Yves Van de Peer¶, Jon Timmisk, Maizura Mohktark, Andy Tyrrellk, A.E. Eiben∗∗, S.P. McKibbin††, Wenguo Liu‡‡, Alan F.T. Winfield‡‡ ∗Institute of Parallel and Distributed Systems, University of Stuttgart, Germany, [email protected] †Artificial Life Lab, Karl-Franzens-University Graz, Universitatsplatz 2, A-8010 Graz, Austria, {heiko.hamann,juergen.stradner,ronald.thenius,thomas.schmickl,karl.crailsheim}@uni-graz.at ‡Almende B.V., 3016 DJ Rotterdam, Netherlands, [email protected] §TAO, LRI, Univ. Paris-Sud, CNRS, INRIA Saclay, France, {Michele.Sebag, Nicolas.Bredeche}@lri.fr ¶VIB Department Plant Systems Biology, Ghent University, Belgium, {Yao.Yao, Guy.Baele, Yves.VandePeer}@psb.vib-ugent.be kUniversity of York, York, United Kingdom, {mm520, jt517, amt}@ohm.york.ac.uk ∗∗Free University Amsterdam, [email protected] ††Materials and Engineering Research Institute (MERI), Sheffield Hallam University, [email protected] ‡‡Bristol Robotics Laboratory (BRL), UWE Bristol, {wenguo.liu, alan.winfield}@uwe.ac.uk Abstract—In Nature, self-organization demonstrates very reliable and scalable collective behavior in a distributed fash- ion. In collective robotic systems, self-organization makes it possible to address both the problem of adaptation to quickly changing environment and compliance with user-defined target objectives. This paper describes on-going work on artificial self-organization within artificial robot organisms, performed in the framework of the Symbrion and Replicator European projects. (a) (b) Keywords-adaptive system, self-adaptation, adaptive self- organization, collective robotics, artificial organisms I.
    [Show full text]
  • Asynchronous Evolution: Emergence of Signal-Based Swarming
    Asynchronous Evolution: Emergence of Signal-Based Swarming Olaf Witkowski and Takashi Ikegami University of Tokyo, Japan [email protected] Abstract towards a common direction. The movement itself may differ from species to species. Since Reynolds boids, swarming behavior has often been re- For example, fish and insects swarm in three dimensions, produced in artificial models, but the conditions leading to whereas herds of sheep move only in two dimensions. More- its emergence are still subject to research, with candidates over, the collective motion can have quite diverse dynamics. ranging from obstacle avoidance to virtual leaders. In this While birds migrate in relatively ordered formations with paper, we present a multi-agent model in which individuals develop swarming using only their ability to listen to each constant velocity, fish schools change directions by align- others signals. Our model uses an original asynchronous ge- ing rapidly and keeping their distances, and insects swarms netic algorithm to evolve a population of agents controlled by move in a messy and random-looking way (Budrene et al. artificial neural networks, looking for an invisible resource 1991, Czirok´ et al. 1997, Shimoyama et al. 1996). in a 3D environment. The results demonstrate that agents Numerous evolutionary hypotheses have been proposed use the information exchanged between them via signaling to form temporary leader-follower relations allowing them to to explain swarming behavior across species. These include flock together. more efficient mating, good environment for learning, com- bined search for food resources, and reducing risks of pre- dation (Zaera et al., 1996). Partridge and Pitcher (1979) also Introduction mention energy saving in fish schools by reducing drag.
    [Show full text]
  • Expert Assessment of Stigmergy: a Report for the Department of National Defence
    Expert Assessment of Stigmergy: A Report for the Department of National Defence Contract No. W7714-040899/003/SV File No. 011 sv.W7714-040899 Client Reference No.: W7714-4-0899 Requisition No. W7714-040899 Contact Info. Tony White Associate Professor School of Computer Science Room 5302 Herzberg Building Carleton University 1125 Colonel By Drive Ottawa, Ontario K1S 5B6 (Office) 613-520-2600 x2208 (Cell) 613-612-2708 [email protected] http://www.scs.carleton.ca/~arpwhite Expert Assessment of Stigmergy Abstract This report describes the current state of research in the area known as Swarm Intelligence. Swarm Intelligence relies upon stigmergic principles in order to solve complex problems using only simple agents. Swarm Intelligence has been receiving increasing attention over the last 10 years as a result of the acknowledgement of the success of social insect systems in solving complex problems without the need for central control or global information. In swarm- based problem solving, a solution emerges as a result of the collective action of the members of the swarm, often using principles of communication known as stigmergy. The individual behaviours of swarm members do not indicate the nature of the emergent collective behaviour and the solution process is generally very robust to the loss of individual swarm members. This report describes the general principles for swarm-based problem solving, the way in which stigmergy is employed, and presents a number of high level algorithms that have proven utility in solving hard optimization and control problems. Useful tools for the modelling and investigation of swarm-based systems are then briefly described.
    [Show full text]
  • © 2009 Sinauer Associates, Inc. This Material Cannot Be Copied
    © 2009 Sinauer Associates, Inc. This material cannot be copied, reproduced, manufactured, or disseminated in any form without express written permission from the publisher. AALCOCK_CH06.inddLCOCK_CH06.indd 118282 11/9/09/9/09 111:11:011:11:01 AAMM 6 Behavioral Adaptations for Survival t is hard to pass on your genes when you are dead. Not sur- prisingly, therefore, most animals do their best to stay I alive long enough to reproduce. But surviving can be a challenge in most environments, which are swarming with deadly predators. You may remember that bats armed with sophisticated sonar systems roam the night sky hunt- ing for moths, katydids, lacewings, and praying mantises. During the day, these same insects are at grave risk of be- ing found, captured, and eaten by keen-eyed birds. Life is short for most moths, lacewings, and the like. Because predators are so good at fi nding food, they place their prey under intense selection pressure, favor- ing those individuals with attributes that postpone death until they have reproduced at least once. The hereditary life-lengthening traits of these survivors can then spread through their populations by natural selection—an out- come that creates reciprocal pressure on predators, favor- ing those individuals who manage to overcome their prey’s improved defenses. For example, the hearing abilities of certain moths and other night-fl ying insects may have led ▲ Canyon treefrogs rely on camoufl age to protect to the evolution of bats with unusually high- themselves from predators, frequency sonar that their prey cannot detect which means they must pick the right rocks to which they as easily.500, 1266 But now some night-fl ying cling tightly wthout moving.
    [Show full text]
  • Development of a Swarming Algorithm Based on Reynolds Rules to Control a Group of Multi-Rotor Uavs Using ROS
    Development of a Swarming Algorithm Based on Reynolds Rules to control a group of multi-rotor UAVs using ROS Rafael G. Bragaa1, Roberto C. da Silva a and and Alexandre C. B. Ramosa aInstitute of Mathematics and Computer Science, University of Itajub´a,Itajub´a, MG, Brazil Received on September 30, 2016 / accepted on October 20, 2016 Abstract The objective of this article is to present a swarming algorithm based on Reynolds flocking rules to drive a group of Unmanned Aerial Vehicles (UAV) together as a coherent swarm. We used quadrotors controlled at the low level by a Pixhawk autopilot board and carry- ing an embedded Linux board (Raspberry Pi) to execute the control algorithms. Each robot is also equipped with distance sensors used to detect other robots next to itself. The swarming algorithm runs on the ROS platform and was implemented in C++. A simulating en- vironment based on the Gazebo Simulator was prepared to test and evaluate the algorithm in a way as close to the reality as possible. Keywords: Robot swarm, Unmanned Aerial Vehicles, Reynolds rules, Pixhawk, ROS. 1. Introduction In recent years UAVs have become very popular because they are easy to build and suitable for many different applications, both civil and military, such as aerial photography, crop dusting and surveillance operations. As the technology to build these small vehicles becomes cheaper it becomes possible to apply a group of UAVs to perform a mission instead of only one unit. This aproach has a number of advantages: a group of robots is able to acquire more data from sensors and cover a bigger area than a single one; the group becomes more aware of its surroundings and is able to more effectively defend itself from threats; and even if some of the robots are lost the mission can be carried on by the remaining ones, thus the system becomes more robust.
    [Show full text]
  • Swarm Aggregation Algorithms for Multi-Robot Systems
    Swarm Aggregation Algorithms for Multi-Robot Systems Andrea Gasparri [email protected] Engineering Department University “Roma Tre” ROMA TRE UNIVERSITÀ DEGLI STUDI Ming Hsieh Department of Electrical Engineering University of Southern California (USC) Los Angeles, USA - June 2013 Ming Hsieh Institute (USC) – Andrea Gasparri Swarm Aggregation Algorithms for Multi-Robot Systems 1 / 63 Outline 1 Introduction 2 Swarm Robotics 3 Swarming Algorithms 4 Algorithm Validation Ming Hsieh Institute (USC) – Andrea Gasparri Swarm Aggregation Algorithms for Multi-Robot Systems 2 / 63 Introduction Biological Swarming Introduction Biological Swarming Ming Hsieh Institute (USC) – Andrea Gasparri Swarm Aggregation Algorithms for Multi-Robot Systems 3 / 63 Introduction Biological Swarming In Nature I “Swarm behavior, or swarming, is a collective behavior exhibited by animals of similar size which aggregate together, perhaps milling about the same spot or perhaps moving en masse or migrating in some direction.” Ming Hsieh Institute (USC) – Andrea Gasparri Swarm Aggregation Algorithms for Multi-Robot Systems 4 / 63 Introduction Biological Swarming In Nature II Examples of biological swarming are found in bird flocks, fish schools, insect swarms, bacteria swarms, quadruped herds Several studies have been made to explain the social behavior which different groups of animals exhibit1,2. A common understanding is that in nature there are attraction and repulsion forces between individuals that lead to swarming behavior 1K Warburton . and J. Lazarus. “Tendency-distance models of social cohesion in animal groups.” In: Journal of theoretical biology 150.4 (1991), pp. 473–488. 2D. Grunbaum, A. Okubo, and S. A. Levin. “Modelling social animal aggregations”. In: Frontiers in theoretcial Biology: Lecture notes in biomathematics.
    [Show full text]
  • Adaptable Platform for Interactive Swarm Robotics (APIS): a Human-Swarm Interaction Research Testbed
    2019 19th International Conference on Advanced Robotics (ICAR) Belo Horizonte, Brasil, December 02-06, 2019 Adaptable Platform for Interactive Swarm Robotics (APIS): A Human-Swarm Interaction Research Testbed Neel Dhanaraj2*† Nathan Hewitt3* Casey Edmonds-Estes4 Rachel Jarman5 Jeongwoo Seo6 Henry Gunner7 Alexandra Hatfield8 Tucker Johnson1 Lunet Yifru1 Julietta Maffeo9 Guilherme Pereira1 Jason Gross1 Yu Gu1 Abstract— This paper describes the Adaptable Platform for although the robotic swarm research community has experi- Interactive Swarm robotics (APIS) — a testbed designed to enced a recent boom in the development of low-cost systems accelerate development in human-swarm interaction (HSI) [3], [4], no existing system addresses all the considerations research. Specifically, this paper presents the design of a swarm robot platform composed of fifty low cost robots coupled to provide a testbench for HSI research. with a testing field and a software architecture that allows In our opinion, an ideal HSI platform should be: (1) low- for modular and versatile development of swarm algorithms. cost and low-maintenance, to allow for ease of manufac- The motivation behind developing this platform is that the turing and operation; (2) physically embodied, to account emergence of a swarm’s collective behavior can be difficult to for real-world constraints; (3) capable of running agents predict and control. However, human-swarm interaction can measurably increase a swarm’s performance as the human of various complexities with user-defined rulesets so that operator may have intuition or knowledge unavailable to the HSI with various algorithms may be tested; (4) versatile in swarm. The development of APIS allows researchers to focus its presentation of information to explore these effects on on HSI research, without being constrained to a fixed ruleset operator cognition; and (5) capable of accepting many types or interface.
    [Show full text]
  • Colony Fissioning in Honey Bees: How Is Swarm Departure Triggered and What Determines Who Leaves?
    COLONY FISSIONING IN HONEY BEES: HOW IS SWARM DEPARTURE TRIGGERED AND WHAT DETERMINES WHO LEAVES? A Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Cornell University In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy by Juliana Rangel Posada February 2010 © 2010 Juliana Rangel Posada SWARMING IN HONEY BEES: HOW IS A SWARM’S DEPARTURE TRIGGERED AND WHAT DETERMINES WHICH BEES LEAVE? Juliana Rangel Posada, Ph. D. Cornell University 2010 Honey bees (Apis mellifera) live in colonies that reproduce by fissioning. When a colony divides itself, approximately two thirds of the workers along with the (old) mother queen, leave the hive as a swarm to found a new colony else where. The rest of the workers, and a (new) daughter queen, stay behind and inherit the old nest. In cold, temperate regions, this ephemeral process of colony multiplication typically occurs only once per year and takes less than 20 minutes, making it a hard-to-study phenomenon. The purpose of this dissertation, which is divided into four chapters, was to uncover the mechanisms and functional organization of the colony fissioning process in honey bees. The first chapter explored the signals used by honey bee colonies to initiate the departure of a swarm from its nest, finding that the piping signal, and the buzz-run signal, are the key signals used to initiate the swarm’s departure. The second chapter searched for the identity of the individuals that performed the signals that trigger the swarm’s exodus. We now know that knowledgeable nest-site scouts are the producers of the signals that trigger this sudden departure.
    [Show full text]