Natural Selection. II. Developmental Variability and Evolutionary Rate

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Natural Selection. II. Developmental Variability and Evolutionary Rate Natural selection. II. Developmental variability and evolutionary rate Steven A. Frank∗ Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology, University of California, Irvine, CA 92697{2525 USA In classical evolutionary theory, genetic variation provides the source of heritable phenotypic vari- ation on which natural selection acts. Against this classical view, several theories have emphasized that developmental variability and learning enhance nonheritable phenotypic variation, which in turn can accelerate evolutionary response. In this paper, I show how developmental variability al- ters evolutionary dynamics by smoothing the landscape that relates genotype to fitness. In a fitness landscape with multiple peaks and valleys, developmental variability can smooth the landscape to provide a directly increasing path of fitness to the highest peak. Developmental variability also allows initial survival of a genotype in response to novel or extreme environmental challenge, pro- viding an opportunity for subsequent adaptation. This initial survival advantage arises from the way in which developmental variability smooths and broadens the fitness landscape. Ultimately, the synergism between developmental processes and genetic variation sets evolutionary rateab. In evolutionary biology, environmentally induced claim to support the theory. Refinements to the theory modifications come under unfinished business ::: develop. There have been repeated assertions of both their In the end, few compelling examples relate nonheri- importance and their triviality, a lot of discussion table phenotypic variability to evolutionary rate. The with no consensus. ::: Yet the debate has contin- literature is hard to read. Enthusiasts extend the con- ued over such concepts as genetic assimilation, cepts and keep the problem alive. Through the enthusi- the Baldwin effect, organic selection, morphoses, asts' promotions, many have heard of the theory. But, and somatic modifications. So much controversy in practice, few consider the role of nonheritable phe- over the span of a century suggests that a prob- notypic variability in their own analyses of evolutionary lem of major significance remains unsolved [1, rate. Almost everyone ignores the problem. p. 498]. In this article, I emphasize simple theory that re- lates nonheritable phenotypic variability to evolutionary rate. Understanding the paradoxical relation between I. INTRODUCTION nonheritable phenotypic variability and evolutionary rate is an essential step in reasoning about many evolutionary A single genotype produces different phenotypes. De- problems. velopmental programs match the phenotype to differ- This article is primarily a concise tutorial to the ba- ent environments. Intrinsic developmental fluctuations sic concepts (see Box 1). I briefly mention some of the spread the distribution of phenotypes. Extrinsic en- history (Box 2) and recent, more advanced literature vironmental fluctuations perturb developmental trajec- (Box 3). tory. These nonheritable types of phenotypic variation are common. Nonheritable phenotypic variation is not transmitted II. SMOOTHING THE EVOLUTIONARY PATH through time. Thus, nonheritable variation would seem to be irrelevant for evolutionary change, which instead The distribution of phenotypes for a given genotype depends on the genetic component of variation. However, is called the reaction norm. All theories come down to nonheritable phenotypic variation can, in principle, affect the fact that a broad reaction norm smooths the path of arXiv:1109.5633v1 [q-bio.PE] 26 Sep 2011 evolutionary rate. At first glance, that contribution of increasing fitness. Once one grasps the smoothing pro- nonheritable phenotypic variation to evolutionary rate cess, many apparently different theories become easy to appears to be a paradox. understand. Many different theories, commentaries, and controver- The next section gives the mathematical expression for sies turn on this paradox (Box 2). The literature has the smoothing of fitness by the reaction norm. Fig. 1 followed a consistent pattern. Detailed theories relate de- explains the mathematics with a simple example. velopmental variability to accelerated evolution. Coun- terarguments ensue. Listings of complicated examples A. The reaction norm smooths fitness ∗ email: [email protected]; homepage: http://stevefrank.org We need to track three quantities. First, fitness, f(x), a doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02373.x in J. Evol. Biol. varies according to the particular phenotype expressed, b Part of the Topics in Natural Selection series. See Box 1. x. 2 Box 1. Topics in the theory of natural selection Box 2. Historical overview This article is part of a series on natural selection. Al- Schlichting and Pigliucci [3] and West-Eberhard [1] thor- though the theory of natural selection is simple, it remains oughly review the subject. Here, I highlight a few key points endlessly contentious and difficult to apply. My goal is to in relation to this article. I treat learning and developmental make more accessible the concepts that are so important, yet plasticity as roughly the same with regard to potential con- either mostly unknown or widely misunderstood. I write in sequences for evolutionary rate, although one could certainly a nontechnical style, showing the key equations and results choose to focus on meaningful distinctions. rather than providing full derivations or discussions of math- In my own reading during the 1980s, I had found the re- ematical problems. Boxes list technical issues and brief sum- lation between learning and evolutionary rate intriguing but maries of the literature. confusing. Baldwin's [4] idea that learning can accelerate evo- lutionary rate seemed attractive. Mayr [5], in his monumen- tal review of biological thought, also discussed various ways in which behavior or flexible developmental programs might Second, the phenotype expressed varies according to alter evolutionary dynamics. Those ideas seemed potentially the reaction norm. Read p(xjx¯) as the probability of ex- important, but it was not easy to grasp the essence. The pressing the phenotype x given a genotype with average literature at that time was not helpful, with a lot of jargon phenotypex ¯. and sometimes almost mystical commentary mixed in with Third, we must calculate F (¯x), the expected fitness intriguing and creative ideas. for a genotype with average phenotypex ¯. We obtain It was clear that learning could slow evolutionary rate. Different genotypes could, through learning, end with the the expected fitness by summing up the probability, p, of same phenotype. Reducing the phenotypic distinction be- expressing each phenotype multiplied by the fitness, f, tween different genotypes would generally slow evolutionary of each phenotype. That sum is rate. The more intriguing problem concerns the origin of evo- lutionary novelty or the response to novel or extreme environ- X F (¯x) = p(xjx¯)f(x); (1) mental challenge. Environmental novelty and acceleration of evolutionary response were the primary concern of Baldwin [4], Waddington [6, 7], and West-Eberhard [1]. My article taken over all the different phenotypes, x. We often mea- also focuses on acceleration of evolutionary response. sure x as a continuous variable. The sum is then equiv- Hinton and Nowlan [8] clarified the subject with their sim- alently written as ple conclusion that: Z Learning alters the shape of the search space F (¯x) = p(xjx¯)f(x)dx: (2) in which evolution operates and thereby pro- vides good evolutionary paths towards sets of co- adapted alleles. We demonstrate that this effect This equation shows how one averages the fitness, f(x), allows learning organisms to evolve much faster for each phenotypic value, x, over the reaction norm, than their nonlearning equivalents, even though p(xjx¯), to obtain the expected fitness of a genotype, F (¯x). the characteristics acquired by the phenotype are We label each genotype by its average phenotype,x ¯. The not communicated to the genotype. expected fitness of a genotype, F (¯x), is what matters for During the past few decades, the fundamental role of evolutionary process [2]. smoothed fitness surfaces in biology has not always been rec- The averaging of expected fitness over the reaction ognized as fully as it should be, in spite of several fine pa- norm is the key to the entire subject. Averaging over the pers along that line (see Box 3). Interestingly, certain com- reaction norm, p, flattens and smooths the fitness func- puter optimization algorithms take advantage of the increased tion, f. This smoothing makes the curve for expected search speed provided by a process similar to smoothed fitness fitness, F , have lower peaks and shallower valleys than landscapes [9, 10]. the original fitness curve, f. The smoothing of F changes evolutionary dynamics. The whole problem comes down to understanding how reaction norms smooth fitness, and In Fig. 2, the reaction norm follows a normal distribu- the consequences of a smoother relation between geno- tion. In symbols, we write type and fitness. p(xjx¯) ∼ N (¯x; γ2); which we read as the probability, p, of a phenotype, x, B. Example of continuous smoothing for a reaction norm centered atx ¯, follows a normal dis- tribution with meanx ¯ and variance γ2. Fig. 1 shows an example of smoothing with discrete For fitness, we write in symbols distributions. It will often be convenient to consider f(x) ∼ N (0; σ2); smoothing of continuous variables. Fig. 2 shows an ex- ample. The following expressions describe the underlying which we read as the fitness,
Recommended publications
  • Transformations of Lamarckism Vienna Series in Theoretical Biology Gerd B
    Transformations of Lamarckism Vienna Series in Theoretical Biology Gerd B. M ü ller, G ü nter P. Wagner, and Werner Callebaut, editors The Evolution of Cognition , edited by Cecilia Heyes and Ludwig Huber, 2000 Origination of Organismal Form: Beyond the Gene in Development and Evolutionary Biology , edited by Gerd B. M ü ller and Stuart A. Newman, 2003 Environment, Development, and Evolution: Toward a Synthesis , edited by Brian K. Hall, Roy D. Pearson, and Gerd B. M ü ller, 2004 Evolution of Communication Systems: A Comparative Approach , edited by D. Kimbrough Oller and Ulrike Griebel, 2004 Modularity: Understanding the Development and Evolution of Natural Complex Systems , edited by Werner Callebaut and Diego Rasskin-Gutman, 2005 Compositional Evolution: The Impact of Sex, Symbiosis, and Modularity on the Gradualist Framework of Evolution , by Richard A. Watson, 2006 Biological Emergences: Evolution by Natural Experiment , by Robert G. B. Reid, 2007 Modeling Biology: Structure, Behaviors, Evolution , edited by Manfred D. Laubichler and Gerd B. M ü ller, 2007 Evolution of Communicative Flexibility: Complexity, Creativity, and Adaptability in Human and Animal Communication , edited by Kimbrough D. Oller and Ulrike Griebel, 2008 Functions in Biological and Artifi cial Worlds: Comparative Philosophical Perspectives , edited by Ulrich Krohs and Peter Kroes, 2009 Cognitive Biology: Evolutionary and Developmental Perspectives on Mind, Brain, and Behavior , edited by Luca Tommasi, Mary A. Peterson, and Lynn Nadel, 2009 Innovation in Cultural Systems: Contributions from Evolutionary Anthropology , edited by Michael J. O ’ Brien and Stephen J. Shennan, 2010 The Major Transitions in Evolution Revisited , edited by Brett Calcott and Kim Sterelny, 2011 Transformations of Lamarckism: From Subtle Fluids to Molecular Biology , edited by Snait B.
    [Show full text]
  • Reaction Norms for the Study of Genotype- Environment Interaction for Growth and Indicator Traits of Sexual Precocity in Nellore Cattle
    Reaction norms for the study of genotype- environment interaction for growth and indicator traits of sexual precocity in Nellore cattle M.V.A. Lemos, H.L.J. Chiaia, M.P. Berton, F.L.B. Feitosa, C. Aboujaoude, G.C. Venturini, H.N. Oliveira, L.G. Albuquerque and F. Baldi Departamento de Zootecnia, Faculdade de Ciências Agrárias e Veterinárias, Universidade Estadual Paulista, Jaboticabal, SP, Brasil Corresponding author: F. Baldi E-mail: [email protected] Genet. Mol. Res. 14 (2): 7151-7162 (2015) Received August 8, 2014 Accepted February 5, 2015 Published June 29. 2015 DOI http://dx.doi.org/10.4238/2015.June.29.9 ABSTRACT. The objective of this study was to quantify the magnitude of genotype-environment interaction (GxE) effects on age at first calving (AFC), scrotal circumference (SC), and yearling weight (YW) in Nellore cattle using reaction norms. For the study, 89,152 weight records of female and male Nellore animals obtained at yearling age were used. Genetic parameters were estimated with a single-trait random- regression model using Legendre polynomials as base functions. The heritability estimates were of low to medium magnitude for AFC (0.05 to 0.47) and of medium to high magnitude for SC (0.32 to 0.51) and YW (0.13 to 0.72), and increased as the environmental gradient became more favorable. The genetic correlation estimates ranged from 0.25 to 1.0 for AFC, from 0.71 to 1.0 for SC, and from 0.42 to 1.0 for YW. High Spearman correlation coefficients were obtained for the three traits, ranging from 0.97 to 0.99.
    [Show full text]
  • Genomic and Environmental Determinants and Their Interplay Underlying Phenotypic Plasticity
    Genomic and environmental determinants and their interplay underlying phenotypic plasticity Xin Lia,1, Tingting Guoa,1,QiMua, Xianran Lia,2, and Jianming Yua,2 aDepartment of Agronomy, Iowa State University, Ames, IA 50011 Edited by Magnus Nordborg, Gregor Mendel Institute, and accepted by Editorial Board Member Joseph R. Ecker May 11, 2018 (received for review October 19, 2017) Observed phenotypic variation in living organisms is shaped by To understand G × E, we initiated a focused study in 2010 on genomes, environment, and their interactions. Flowering time under sorghum, a model crop species (10). Flowering time is a typical natural conditions can showcase the diverse outcome of the gene– subject in G × E research (11–13) and has significant bearing on environment interplay. However, identifying hidden patterns and spe- evolution and adaptation of plants to different environments (14, cific factors underlying phenotypic plasticity under natural field con- 15). Following our initial observation of the altered flowering ditions remains challenging. With a genetic population showing times for two sorghum inbreds in summer and winter nurseries, dynamic changes in flowering time, here we show that the integrated extensive phenotyping and genetic analysis were conducted with analyses of genomic responses to diverse environments is powerful to a mapping population. Expanding the testing to two additional reveal the underlying genetic architecture. Specifically, the effect con- summer nurseries at a higher latitude site and a summer planting at the winter nursery site generated additional interesting ob- tinuum of individual genes (Ma1, Ma6, FT,andELF3) was found to vary in size and in direction along an environmental gradient that was servations.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Generalized Norms of Reaction for Ecological Developmental Biology
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Philsci-Archive Generalized Norms of Reaction for Ecological Developmental Biology Sahotra Sarkar* and Trevon Fuller Biodiversity and Biocultural Conservation Laboratory, Section of Integrative Biology and Department of Philosophy, University of Texas at Austin, 1 Texas Longhorns, #C3500, Austin, TX 78712 –1180. Abstract A standard norm of reaction (NoR) is a graphical depiction of the phenotypic value of some trait of an individual genotype in a population as a function an environmental parameter. NoRs thus depict the phenotypic plasticity of a trait. The topological properties of NoRs for sets of different genotypes can be used to infer the presence of (non-linear) genotype-environment interactions. While it is clear that many NoRs are adaptive, it is not yet settled whether their evolutionary etiology should be explained by selection on the mean phenotypic trait values in different environments or whether there are specific genes conferring plasticity. If the second alternative is true the NoR is itself an object of selection. Generalized NoRs depict plasticity at the level of populations or subspecies within a species, species within a genus, or taxa at higher levels. Historically, generalized NoRs have routinely been drawn though rarely explicitly recognized as such. Such generalized NoRs can be used to make evolutionary inferences at higher taxonomic levels in a way analogous to how standard NoRs are used for microevolutionary inferences. Running head: Generalized Norms of Reaction. Keywords: Norm of reaction, reaction norm, phenotypic plasticity, developmental evolution. * Corresponding author; e-mail: <[email protected]>; Phone: 1 512 232 7122; FAX: 1 512 471 4806.
    [Show full text]
  • Canalization and Robustness - Evolutionary Biology - Oxford Bibliog
    Canalization and Robustness - Evolutionary Biology - Oxford Bibliog... http://www.oxfordbibliographies.com/view/document/obo-978019994... Canalization and Robustness Thomas Flatt, Günter Wagner LAST MODIFIED: 27 JUNE 2018 DOI: 10.1093/OBO/9780199941728-0109 Introduction Canalization describes the phenomenon whereby particular genotypes exhibit reduced phenotypic sensitivity or variation (i.e., increased robustness) in response to mutations and/or to environmental changes relative to other genotypes. Canalization is a variational property of genotypes: it implies a reduced potential or propensity of the phenotype, produced by this genotype, to vary in response to genetic or environmental change. The terms “canalization,” “robustness” and “buffering” are typically used interchangeably; today, “robustness” is perhaps more commonly used than “canalization.” The concept of canalization was first introduced by Conrad Hal Waddington in the 1940s; around the same time, Ivan Ivanovich Schmalhausen came up with essentially the same concept (see Books and Early History of the Canalization Concept). Their main conjecture was the existence of a special kind of stabilizing selection, so-called canalizing selection, which favors genotypes that deviate least from the trait optimum (e.g., the fitness optimum), by selecting for genetic mechanisms that suppress phenotypic variation caused by mutations (genetic canalization) or by environmental perturbations or changes (environmental canalization). The concept of canalization is closely related to the phenomenon of genetic assimilation, that is, the idea that previously hidden, cryptic genetic variants can become phenotypically expressed following an environmental or genetic perturbation and increase in frequency by selection. General Overviews Early experimental evidence for the existence of canalization and genetic assimilation is reviewed in depth by Scharloo 1991, the first comprehensive review paper in the field.
    [Show full text]
  • Phenotypic Plasticity/A
    Variance Reaction Norms • Yet another complication in studying • A genotype often doesn’t specify exactly what the variance is that some variance might result phenotype will be. from environment-gene interactions (e.g. • The genotype often determines a range of phenotypes that an organism will have under genes “turning on” only because of some different environments. environmental cue) • The fact that phenotypes vary with environment is known as phenotypic plasticity. • VP = VG + VE + VGxE • One example of this is the phenomenon of • The specific relationship between phenotype and environment, given a certain genotype, is called a reaction norms. reaction norm. The rotifer Brachionus calycoflorus develops spines when predators are present in its environment (right), but not in Water crowfoot, their absence (left). This is phenotypic plasticity. Ranunculus aquatilis, grows broad leaves that float on the surface of the water, and branching filamentous leaves below the surface of water—another example of phenotypic plasticity (Lamarck mentioned this). Clausen et al. (1948) grew cuttings from seven wild Two different fruit yarrow plants (Achillea) in the same garden at fly mutant alleles, Mather, California. Here’s what they got. called infrabar and ultrabar, both produce a phenotype with unusually small eyes (the y-axis shows “number of facets” in the eye) —but they differ in their reaction norms They all grew in the same environment, so differences in height must be genetic. When they grew cuttings from the same seven wild plants in a different garden at Stanford, California, Here’s the two they got this. compared directly. Remember that the Mather and Stanford plants are genetically identical—both grew from cuttings from the seven original wild plants.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Change in Continuous Reaction Norms Courtney J
    West Chester University Digital Commons @ West Chester University Biology Faculty Publications Biology 4-2014 Evolutionary change in continuous reaction norms Courtney J. Murren College of Charleston Heidi J. Maclean University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill Sarah E. Diamond North Carolina State University at Raleigh Ulrich K. Steiner University of Southern Denmark Mary A. Heskel Australian National University See next page for additional authors Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.wcupa.edu/bio_facpub Part of the Evolution Commons Recommended Citation Murren, C. J., Maclean, H. J., Diamond, S. E., Steiner, U. K., Heskel, M. A., Handelsman, C. A., Ghalambor, C. K., Auld, J. R., Callahan, H. S., Pfennig, D. W., Relyea, R. A., Schlichting, C. D., & Kingsolver, J. (2014). Evolutionary change in continuous reaction norms. American Naturalist, 183(4), 453-467. Retrieved from http://digitalcommons.wcupa.edu/bio_facpub/35 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Biology at Digital Commons @ West Chester University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Biology Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of Digital Commons @ West Chester University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Authors Courtney J. Murren, Heidi J. Maclean, Sarah E. Diamond, Ulrich K. Steiner, Mary A. Heskel, Corey A. Handelsman, Cameron K. Ghalambor, Josh R. Auld, Hilary S. Callahan, David W. Pfennig, Rick A. Relyea, Carl D. Schlichting, and Joel Kingsolver This article is available at Digital Commons @ West Chester University: http://digitalcommons.wcupa.edu/bio_facpub/35 vol. 183, no. 4 the american naturalist april 2014 Evolutionary Change in Continuous Reaction Norms Courtney J.
    [Show full text]
  • Woltereck's Genotype
    Woltereck’s Role in the Genotype Press, MO Woltereck’s Genotype Maximilian Oliver Press [email protected] Summary The reaction norm, originally introduced by Richard Woltereck in 1909, describes the range of phenotypes available to a single genotype under different environments. It is a foundational concept in genetics and evolutionary thought. At its inception, it represented a counterpoint to a hereditarian Mendelism championed by Wilhelm Johannsen, though both authors ultimately agreed that the reaction norm was essentially identical to the “genotype” term introduced earlier the same year by Johannsen. Woltereck both literally and figuratively wrote “Genotypus = Reaktionsnorm”. However, some have argued that Woltereck’s interpretation of the reaction norm was incomplete up to the point of Johannsen’s commentary on it. Here, I demonstrate that the reaction norm constituted a direct and intentional challenge to Johannsen’s original notion of fixed differences between types, and present new translations of relevant texts from both Johannsen and Woltereck. I conclude that both authors’ acceptance of the equivalence between genotypes and reaction norms constituted a redefinition of the genotype by Woltereck that was ultimately accepted (with apparent poor grace) by Johannsen. Introduction All organisms respond to routine environmental variation with alterations of form, physiology, or behavior. These alterations can occur without altering the underlying heritable material, suggesting a separation between heritable and non-heritable biological variation. This separation appears no later than Goethe’s 1790 essay on metamorphosis (Goethe 1989), and ​ ​ probably earlier. Goethe made a distinction between typus (the implicate biological rules of an ​ ​ organism) and outward form (Steiner 1988). This distinction prefigures Weismann’s later ​ ​ categories of germ-plasm (material bearing heritable information) and soma (non-heritable form) (Weismann 1893).
    [Show full text]
  • Variation in Reaction Norms: Statistical Considerations and Biological
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by St Andrews Research Repository 1 Variation in reaction norms: statistical considerations and biological 2 interpretation 3 Michael B. Morrissey1;3, and Maartje Liefting2 June 15, 20164 1School of Biology, University of St Andrews5 2Department of Animal Ecology, VU University Amsterdam6 7 3contact email: [email protected] phone: +44 (0) 1334 463738 fax: +44 (0) 1334 463366 post: Dyers Brae House School of Biology, University of St Andrews St Andrews, Fife, UK, KY16 9TH 1 Analysis of reaction norms 2 8 Abstract 9 Analysis of reaction norms, the functions by which the phenotype produced by a given geno- 10 type depends on the environment, is critical to studying many aspects of phenotypic evo- 11 lution. Different techniques are available for quantifying different aspects of reaction norm 12 variation. We examine what biological inferences can be drawn from some of the more readily- 13 applicable analyses for studying reaction norms. We adopt a strongly biologically-motivated 14 view, but draw on statistical theory to highlight strengths and drawbacks of different tech- 15 niques. In particular, consideration of some formal statistical theory leads to revision of 16 some recently, and forcefully, advocated opinions on reaction norm analysis. We clarify what 17 simple analysis of the slope between mean phenotype in two environments can tell us about 18 reaction norms, explore the conditions under which polynomial regression can provide ro- 19 bust inferences about reaction norm shape, and explore how different existing approaches 20 may be used to draw inferences about variation in reaction norm shape.
    [Show full text]
  • The Selective Advantage of Reaction Norms for Environmental Tolerance
    J. evol. Bid. 5: 41-59 (1992) 1010 061X/92/01041 I9 $ 1.50+0.20/O Q 1992 Birkhiuser Verlag. Base1 The selective advantage of reaction norms for environmental tolerance Wilfried Gabriel’ and Michael Lynch’ ‘Department of Ph~~siological Ecology, Mn?s Planck Institute jbr Limnology, Postfxh 165, W-2320 Pliin, Germuny ‘Department qf Biology. University of Oregon, Eugene, Oregon 97403 USA Ke), words: breadth of adaptation; environmental tolerance; phenotypic plasticity, quantitative genetics; reaction norm. Abstract A tolerance curve defines the dependence of a genotype’s fitness on the state of an environmental gradient. It can be characterized by a mode (the genotype’s optimal environment) and a width (the breadth of adaptation). It seems possible that one or both of these characters can be modified in an adaptive manner, at least partially, during development. Thus, we extend the theory of environmental toler- ance to include reaction norms for the mode and the width of the tolerance curve. We demonstrate that the selective value of such reaction norms increases with increasing spatial heterogeneity and between-generation temporal variation in the environment and with decreasing within-generation temporal variation. Assuming that the maintenance of a high breadth of adaptation is costly, reaction norms are shown to induce correlated selection for a reduction in this character. Nevertheless, regardless of the magnitude of the reaction norm, there is a nearly one to one relationship between the optimal breadth of adaptation and the within-generation temporal variation perceived by the organism. This suggests that empirical esti- mates of the breadth of adaptation may provide a useful index of this type of environmental variation from the organism’s point of view.
    [Show full text]
  • Epigenetic Control of Phenotypic Plasticity in the Filamentous Fungus
    G3: Genes|Genomes|Genetics Early Online, published on September 30, 2016 as doi:10.1534/g3.116.033860 Epigenetic control of phenotypic plasticity in the filamentous fungus Neurospora crassa Ilkka Kronholm1, Hanna Johannesson2, Tarmo Ketola1 1Centre of Excellence in Biological Interactions, Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Jyväskylä, FI-40014 Jyväskylä, Finland 2Department of Organismal Biology, University of Uppsala, 752 36 Uppsala, Sweden 1 © The Author(s) 2013. Published by the Genetics Society of America. Running Head: Phenotypic plasticity in Neurospora Keywords: Reaction norm, DNA methylation, Histone methylation, Histone deacetylation, RNA interference, fungi Corresponding author: Ilkka Kronholm Centre of Excellence in Biological Interactions, Department of Biological and Environmental Sciences, University of Jyväskylä, P.O. Box 35, FI-40014 Jyväskylä, Finland Fax +358 14 617 239 Email: ilkka.kronholm@jyu.fi 2 Abstract 2 Phenotypic plasticity is the ability of a genotype to produce different phenotypes under different envi- ronmental or developmental conditions. Phenotypic plasticity is an ubiquitous feature of living organisms 4 and is typically based on variable patterns of gene expression. However, the mechanisms by which gene expression is influenced and regulated during plastic responses are poorly understood in most organisms. 6 While modifications to DNA and histone proteins have been implicated as likely candidates for generating and regulating phenotypic plasticity, specific details of each modification and its mode of operation have 8 remained largely unknown. In this study, we investigated how epigenetic mechanisms affect phenotypic plasticity in the filamentous fungus Neurospora crassa. By measuring reaction norms of strains that are 10 deficient in one of several key physiological processes, we show that epigenetic mechanisms play a role in homeostasis and phenotypic plasticity of the fungus across a range of controlled environments.
    [Show full text]
  • Bridging the Gap Between Developmental Systems Theory and Evolutionary Developmental Biology
    Problems and paradigms Bridging the gap between developmental systems theory and evolutionary developmental biology Jason Scott Robert,1* Brian K. Hall,2 and Wendy M. Olson2 Summary omous views of development relying on the division of Many scientists and philosophers of science are troubled ontogenetic causes into genetic causes and generic (every- by the relative isolation of developmental from evolu- thing else, but usually mainly environmental) causes. For tionary biology. Reconciling the science of development with the science of heredity preoccupied a minority of Oyama, as for other adherents to DST, developmental biologists for much of the twentieth century, but these information resides neither in the genes nor in the environ- efforts were not corporately successful. Mainly in the past ment, but rather emerges from the interactions of disparate, fifteen years, however, these previously dispersed inte- dispersed developmental resources Ð hence, the ontogeny of grating programmes have been themselves synthesized and so reinvigorated. Two of these more recent synthe- information. As against the usual interpretation of evolution as sizing endeavours are evolutionary developmental biol- the transmission of genetic information between successive ogy (EDB, or ``evo-devo'') and developmental systems generations, DST underscores the ontogenetic construction of theory (DST). While the former is a bourgeoning and developmental information in each generation from both scientifically well-respected biological discipline, the genetic and generic sources. Accordingly, ontogenetic pro- same cannot be said of DST, which is virtually unknown among biologists. In this review, we provide overviews cesses are responsible for both the relatively reliable reprod- of DST and EDB, summarize their key tenets, examine uction of type and the introduction of potentially evolutionarily how they relate to one another and to the study of significant variation.
    [Show full text]