Sign Language
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
How Does Social Structure Shape Language Variation? a Case Study of the Kata Kolok Lexicon
HOW DOES SOCIAL STRUCTURE SHAPE LANGUAGE VARIATION? A CASE STUDY OF THE KATA KOLOK LEXICON KATIE MUDD*1, HANNAH LUTZENBERGER2,3, CONNIE DE VOS2, PAULA FIKKERT2, ONNO CRASBORN2, BART DE BOER1 *Corresponding Author: [email protected] 1Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium 2Center of Language Studies, Nijmegen, Netherlands 3International Max Planck Research School, Nijmegen, Netherlands Sign language emergence is an excellent source of data on how language varia- tion is conditioned. Based on the context of sign language emergence, sign lan- guages can be classified as Deaf community sign languages (DCSL), used by a large and dispersed group of mainly deaf individuals (Mitchell & Karchmer, 2004) or as shared sign languages (SSL), which typically emerge in tight-knit communities and are shared by deaf and hearing community members (Kisch, 2008)1. It has been suggested that, in small, tight-knit populations, a higher degree of variation is tolerated than in large, dispersed communities because individuals can remember others’ idiolects (de Vos, 2011; Thompson et al., 2019). Confirm- ing this, Washabaugh (1986) found more lexical variation in Providence Island Sign Language, a SSL, than in American Sign Language (ASL), a DCSL. DC- SLs frequently exhibit variation influenced by schooling patterns, for instance seen in the differences between ages in British Sign Language (Stamp et al., 2014), gender in Irish Sign Language (LeMaster, 2006) and race in ASL (Mc- Caskill et al., 2011). It remains unknown how variation is conditioned in SSLs. The present study of Kata Kolok (KK) is one of the first in-depth studies of how sociolinguistic factors shape lexical variation in a SSL. -
Assessing the Bimodal Bilingual Language Skills of Young Deaf Children
ANZCED/APCD Conference CHRISTCHURCH, NZ 7-10 July 2016 Assessing the bimodal bilingual language skills of young deaf children Elizabeth Levesque PhD What we’ll talk about today Bilingual First Language Acquisition Bimodal bilingualism Bimodal bilingual assessment Measuring parental input Assessment tools Bilingual First Language Acquisition Bilingual literature generally refers to children’s acquisition of two languages as simultaneous or sequential bilingualism (McLaughlin, 1978) Simultaneous: occurring when a child is exposed to both languages within the first three years of life (not be confused with simultaneous communication: speaking and signing at the same time) Sequential: occurs when the second language is acquired after the child’s first three years of life Routes to bilingualism for young children One parent-one language Mixed language use by each person One language used at home, the other at school Designated times, e.g. signing at bath and bed time Language mixing, blending (Lanza, 1992; Vihman & McLaughlin, 1982) Bimodal bilingualism Refers to the use of two language modalities: Vocal: speech Visual-gestural: sign, gesture, non-manual features (Emmorey, Borinstein, & Thompson, 2005) Equal proficiency in both languages across a range of contexts is uncommon Balanced bilingualism: attainment of reasonable competence in both languages to support effective communication with a range of interlocutors (Genesee & Nicoladis, 2006; Grosjean, 2008; Hakuta, 1990) Dispelling the myths….. Infants’ first signs are acquired earlier than first words No significant difference in the emergence of first signs and words - developmental milestones are met within similar timeframes (Johnston & Schembri, 2007) Slight sign language advantage at the one-word stage, perhaps due to features being more visible and contrastive than speech (Meier & Newport,1990) Another myth…. -
COMMUNICATION BASICS Section 5 Communication Basics |Alberta Hands & Voices Parent Toolkit
COMMUNICATION BASICS Section 5 Communication Basics |Alberta Hands & Voices Parent Toolkit Communication Options: Speech? Sign? Both? What is Best for my Child? Making a Decision One of the most important decisions facing a family with a child who is Deaf or Hard of Hearing is choosing a communication method (also called a communication mode). Recently a parent told us she was terrified of making this “critical, life-long decision.” This parent also wanted to know what the current research says about the best method of communication for children who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing. Decisions about communication modes are not No one method has been irreversible. We encourage families to remain scientifically proven to be open-minded and flexible. The needs of your best for all children. child, and your family, change over time. Some families start with speech and sign language and later change to using only sign language as their child’s strengths and preferences become more obvious. Other families start with speech only, and then add sign language when they realize their child is not making enough progress. Still others decide to use speech only, and stay with that decision over time. Remember the Alberta Hands & Voices mantra: whatever choice is best for your child makes that the right choice. As you think about how your family communicates now with your child and how you would like to communicate with him or her in the future, the best way to decide is to: ➢ be open to all communication modes ➢ ask questions 2 Communication Basics |Alberta Hands & Voices Parent Toolkit ➢ talk to adults who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing ➢ talk to other families with children who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing; meet their children ➢ talk to professionals who know your child ➢ discuss, read, and obtain as much information as you can about the various methods What Current Research Does and Does Not Tell Us If you looked through scientific journals to try to determine what is the best communication method, you might soon find yourself feeling very confused. -
Summer Institute for Teachers of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students
Summer Institute for Teachers of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students Linda Cundy, Education Consultant Natalia Rohatyn, Doctoral Student, U of A Calgary, Alberta August 23, 2011 Objectives ¢ Understanding who the Deaf/Hard of Hearing (D/HH) student is ¢ Modifying Instructional Strategies ¢ Roles and Responsibilities ¢ Resources KWL Chart Guess the acronyms activity! Guess the Acronyms Activity ¢ ASL ¢ BAHA ¢ BICS ¢ CALP ¢ CASE ¢ FSCD ¢ ERECS ¢ HA ¢ REACH ¢ UDL Profile of a Deaf or HH Student ¢ Background – Family ¢ Communication ¢ Language Development ¢ Identity ¢ School experience BICS L1 CALP L1 BICS L2 CALP L2 Sample writing Me and Leah and Amy go swimming fun good Big swimming cool fun me like Swim finish go play fun 1,2,3 run fun finish me see Hello Leah and Amy go home Hello me go home cool. Me go Play Game fun. My own secret place is over by the beach in the dark, quiet cave. A little light coming out of the open door. I feel calm not too scared of the dark. I see many gulls walking with their webbed feet and many waves rushing through rocks. I hear nothing because I am deaf! ASL Proficiency and Reading achievement Native ASL Signers Not Native Signers Swiss Cheese Learning How a sentence is “heard” by a student with hearing loss A. spoken sentence by teacher B. sounds heard by student C. sounds speechread by student SPELLING TEST for you! Courtesy of communication in classrooms with D/HH students ¢ Hard of Hearing Students l Using the FM system l Sharing the microphone l Visual lines of communication ¢ Deaf Students l Using the Interpreter/Signing EA l Lag time l Visual lines of communication 15 MINUTE BREAK Universal Attitudes about Classroom Accommodations ¢ Multiple means of representation ¢ Multiple means of expression ¢ Multiple means of engagement Special Accommodations ¢ Sign Language Interpreter/ E.A. -
Access to Assessments
GUIDANCE FOR AWARDING BODIES Access to External Assessment for D/deaf Candidates Version 3 – December 2006 1 CONTENTS Page 1. Introduction 4 2. About the D/deaf Candidate 5 3. Making Reasonable Adjustments 7 4. Range of Reasonable Adjustments to the Assessment of D/deaf Candidates 8 A Changes to assessment conditions 8 A1 Extra time A2 Accommodation A3 Early opening of papers B Use of mechanical, electronic and technological aids 9 B1 Assistive technology B2 BSL/English dictionaries/glossaries C Modification to the presentation of assessment material 10 C1 Language modified assessment material a) Modified written paper b) Assessment material in British Sign Language C2 Modified or standard written paper with modification through ‘live’ presentation C3 Modified oral assessments a) Listening tests (language exam) and assessment material in audio format b) Listening tests (subjects other than languages) D Alternative ways of presenting responses 18 D1 Responses in British Sign Language to written questions D2 Responses in spoken English to written questions D3 Responses to oral assessments a) Speaking tests (language exam) b) Speaking/oral tests (subject exams other than languages) 2 E Use of access facilitators 20 E1 Reader E2 Scribe E3 Oral Language Modifier E4 BSL/English Interpreter E5 Transcriber E6 Lipspeakers, Note-takers, Speech to Text Reporters, Cued Speech Transliterators APPENDIX Summary of Reasonable Adjustments for D/deaf Candidates 31 Signature would like to thank everyone who contributed to this guidance, including members of the British Association of Teachers of the Deaf (BATOD), teachers of D/deaf students and other specialist staff at specific schools, colleges and universities. -
Variation and Change in English Varieties of British Sign Languagei
Variation and change in English varieties of BSL 1 Variation and change in English varieties of British Sign Languagei Adam Schembri, Rose Stamp, Jordan Fenlon and Kearsy Cormier British Sign Language (BSL) is the language used by the deaf community in the United Kingdom. In this chapter, we describe sociolinguistic variation and change in BSL varieties in England. This will show how factors that drive sociolinguistic variation and change in both spoken and signed language communities are broadly similar. Social factors include, for example, a signer’s age group, region of origin, gender, ethnicity, and socio-economic status (e.g., Lucas, Valli & Bayley 2001). Linguistic factors include assimilation and co-articulation effects (e.g., Schembri et al. 2009; Fenlon et al. 2013). It should be noted, however, some factors involved in sociolinguistic variation in sign languages are distinctive. For example, phonological variation includes features, such as whether a sign is produced with one or two hands, which have no direct parallel in spoken language phonology. In addition, deaf signing communities are invariably minority communities embedded within larger majority communities whose languages are in another entirely different modality and which may have written systems, unlike sign languages. Some of the linguistic outcomes of this contact situation (such as the use of individual signs for letters to spell out written words on the hands, known as fingerspelling) are unique to such communities (Lucas & Valli 1992). This picture is further complicated by patterns of language transmission which see many deaf individuals acquiring sign languages as first languages at a much later age than hearing individuals (e.g., Cormier et al. -
Chapter 2 Sign Language Types
Chapter 2 Sign language types This chapter defines four different sign language types, based on the infor- mation available in the respective sources. Before introducing the types of sign languages, I first report on the diachronic developments in the field of typological sign language research that gave rise to the distinction of the various sign language types. Sign language research started about five decades ago in the United States of America mainly due to the pioneering work of Stokoe (2005 [1960]), Klima and Bellugi (1979), and Poizner, Klima and Bellugi (1987) on American Sign Language (ASL). Gradually linguists in other countries, mainly in Europe, became interested in sign language research and started analyzing European sign languages e.g. British Sign Language (BSL), Swedish Sign Language (SSL), Sign Language of the Netherlands (NGT) and German Sign Language (DGS). Most of the in-depth linguistic descrip- tions have been based on Western sign languages. Therefore, it has long been assumed that some fundamental levels of linguistic structure, such as spatial morphology and syntax, operate identically in all sign languages. Recent studies, however, have discovered some important variations in spatial organization in some previously unknown sign languages (Washabaugh, 1986; Nyst, 2007; Marsaja, 2008; Padden, Meir, Aronoff, & Sandler, 2010). In the context of growing interest in non-Western sign languages towards the end of the 1990s and more recently, there have been efforts towards developing a typology of sign languages (Zeshan, 2004ab, 2008, 2011b; Schuit, Baker, & Pfau, 2011). Although it has been repeatedly emphasized in the literature that the sign language research still has too little data on sign languages other than those of national deaf communities, based in Western or Asian cultures (Zeshan, 2008). -
Linguistic Access to Education for Deaf Pupils and Students in Scotland
BRITISH SIGN LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTIC ACCESS WORKING GROUP SCOPING STUDY: LINGUISTIC ACCESS TO EDUCATION FOR DEAF PUPILS AND STUDENTS IN SCOTLAND © Crown copyright 2009 ISBN: 978-0-7559-5849-8 (Web only) This document is also available on the Scottish Government website: www.scotland.gov.uk RR Donnelley B56660 02/09 www.scotland.gov.uk BRITISH SIGN LANGUAGE AND LINGUISTIC ACCESS WORKING GROUP SCOPING STUDY: LINGUISTIC ACCESS TO EDUCATION FOR DEAF PUPILS AND STUDENTS IN SCOTLAND The Scottish Government, Edinburgh 2008 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We are very grateful to the many individuals who have contributed their time and commitment to this study. Appendix 2 lists the main organisations who have taken part. The views expressed in this report are those of the researcher and do not necessarily represent those of the Scottish Government or Scottish officials. © Crown copyright 2009 ISBN: 978-0-7559-5849-8 (Web only) The Scottish Government St Andrew’s House Edinburgh EH1 3DG Produced for the Scottish Government by RR Donnelley B56660 02/09 Published by the Scottish Government, February, 2009 CONTENTS 1. INTRODUCTION (including glossary of abbreviations) 1 2. METHODS 4 I Statistical information I Qualitative information I Documentary information THE LINGUISTIC ACCESS CONTEXT FOR DEAF PUPILS AND STUDENTS IN SCOTLAND 3. LANGUAGE APPROACHES USED WITH DEAF PUPILS IN SCOTLAND 7 I Monolingual approaches I Bilingual approaches I ‘No specific policy’ I Regional variation 4. LINGUISTIC ACCESS FOR DEAF STUDENTS IN FURTHER AND HIGHER EDUCATION 11 THE SCHOOL SECTOR: LINGUISTIC ACCESS TO EDUCATION FOR DEAF PUPILS NUMBERS OF PUPILS AND PROFESSIONALS 5. THE NUMBER OF DEAF PUPILS 13 I Scottish Government Statistics I ADPS statistics I Presenting statistics on the population of deaf pupils I Statistics relating to educational attainment I Recommendations 6. -
American Sign Language
U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH AND HUMAN SERVICES ∙ National Institutes of Health NIDCD Fact Sheet | Hearing and Balance American Sign Language What is American Sign Language? American Sign Language (ASL) is a complete, natural language that has the same linguistic properties as spoken languages, with grammar that differs from English. ASL is expressed by movements of the hands and face. It is the primary language of many North Americans who are deaf and hard of hearing, and is used by many hearing people as well. Is sign language the same in other countries? There is no universal sign language. Different sign languages are used in different countries or regions. For example, British Sign Language (BSL) is a different A young boy signs “I love you.” language from ASL, and Americans who know ASL may not understand BSL. Some countries adopt features of ASL in their sign languages. LSF are distinct languages. While they still contain some Where did ASL originate? similar signs, they can no longer be understood by each other’s users. No person or committee invented ASL. The exact beginnings of ASL are not clear, but some suggest that it How does ASL compare with spoken arose more than 200 years ago from the intermixing of language? local sign languages and French Sign Language (LSF, or Langue des Signes Française). Today’s ASL includes some ASL is a language completely separate and distinct elements of LSF plus the original local sign languages; from English. It contains all the fundamental features over time, these have melded and changed into a rich, of language, with its own rules for pronunciation, word complex, and mature language. -
Ed 304 810 Author Title Institution Spons Agency
DOCUMENT RESUME ED 304 810 EC 212 300 AUTHOR Stewart, David A. TITLE A Model Communication and Language Policy for Total Communication Programs for the Hearing Impaired. Occasional Paper No. 125. INSTITUTION Michigan State Univ., East Lansing. Inst. for Research on Teaching. SPONS AGENCY Special Education Programs (ED/OSERS), Wa3hington, DC. PUB DATE Sep 88 GRANT G008730145 NOTE 25p. AVAILABLE FROM Institute for Research on Teaching, College of Education, Michigan State University, 252 Erickson Hall, East Lansing, MI 48825 ($3.00). PUB TYPE Viewpoints (120) -- Reports - Descriptive (141) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC01 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS American Sign Language; *Educational Policy; Elementary Secondary Education; *Hearing Impairments; Interpersonal Communication; Intervention; Manual Communication; Models; School Policy; *Sign Language; *Teacher Behavior; *Teaching Methods; *Total Communication ABSTRACT This paper argues that current practices in total communication classrooms have basically assigned the responsibility of communication to hearing-impaired students wIlo must adapt to the variation in communication behaviors displayed by each of their teachers. The paper advocates use of a model communication and language policy designed to implement consistent linguistic input in the instruction of hearing-impaired students in total communication programs. The consistent use of a modified form of Signed English as the primary sign system and the use of American Sign Language as an intervention tool form the basis for establishing consistent linguistic input in the classroom. Following a rationale for adoption of such a policy, an appendix contains the communication and language policy that was developed for the Lansing (Michigan) School District's hearing-impaired programs. Two other appendixes describe the characteristics of modified signed English and provide a brief perspective of pidgin signs. -
Typological Aspects of Inuit Sign Language
UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Signs of the arctic: Typological aspects of Inuit Sign Language Schuit, J.M. Publication date 2014 Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): Schuit, J. M. (2014). Signs of the arctic: Typological aspects of Inuit Sign Language. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (https://dare.uva.nl) Download date:01 Oct 2021 INTRODUCTION | 1 1. Introduction “I am particularly interested in the unusual characteristics of Providence Island signing. These unusual characteristics […] tell us that, contrary to the nostrums of current linguistic and Sign language theory, Sign languages […] arise from human social life and not just out of human brains.” (Washabaugh 1986:xiii-xiv) Inuit Sign Language, or in Inuktitut Inuit Uukturausingit (henceforth, IUR1), is a language of the Inuit. -
Enactment in British Sign Language Conversations
SHOWING AND SEEING: ENACTMENT IN BRITISH SIGN LANGUAGE CONVERSATIONS GABRIELLE HODGE*, SANNAH GULAMANI, and KEARSY CORMIER *Corresponding Author: [email protected] Deafness Cognition & Language Research Centre, University College London, UK It is widely accepted that investigations of enactment (non-conventional, improvised, bodily depictions of events) are integral for understanding the origins and evolution of language (see e.g. Żywiczyński, Wacewicz & Sibierska, 2018). However, there is significant disconnect in how enactment in spoken and signed languages is understood and analysed, which inhibits cross-modal comparability and investigation of the role of deaf signed languages in evolutionary theory. Here we take the position that both signers and speakers use non-conventional bodily enactment with and without more conventionalised semiotic strategies to mimetically depict the actions, utterances, thoughts and feelings of themselves, other people, animals and things (Tannen, 1989; Metzger 1995). Proficient use of enactment in deaf signed language ecologies is vital for understanding others and making oneself understood (see e.g. Cormier, Smith & Zwets, 2013; Ferrara & Johnston, 2014). Indeed, enactment is just one of several strategies for depicting in face-to-face communication, which are tightly integrated with strategies for describing and indicating (Clark, 1996; see also Ferrara & Hodge, 2018). However, unlike with spoken languages (e.g. Hakulinen & Selting, 2005), little is known about signed conversations, and the role of non-conventional semiotics during these interactions. One question is how signers use bodily enactment to visibly depict a referent while indexing other ‘invisible’ referents in the signing space around them. This enables signers to ‘show’ one referent with their body while simultaneously ‘seeing’ another (Winston, 1991; Engberg-Pedersen, 1993; Liddell, 2003).