ASPECTS, the Mismeasure of Stroke: a Metrological Investigation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

ASPECTS, the Mismeasure of Stroke: a Metrological Investigation ASPECTS, The Mismeasure of Stroke: A Metrological Investigation Richard A. Suss, M.D. ([email protected]) Neuroradiology Section, Department of Radiology The University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center 5323 Harry Hines Boulevard, Dallas, TX 75390-9178 OSF Preprints: https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/c4tkp or https://osf.io/c4tkp/ Versions: 2019: 1 (Dec. 1), 2 (Dec. 29), 3 (Dec. 31); 2020: 4 (May 15), 5 (Dec 31). Abstract—ASPECTS (Alberta Stroke Program Early CT Score) is an irregular scoring system that reads out as 10 to 0 points. It was originally conceived to reflect acute MCA (middle cerebral artery) territory infarct volume on (plain) CT, but it is equally applicable to and testable by MRI. Background material prepares the reader by explaining pertinent measurement principles and ASPECTS’s genesis, definitions, claims, and quirks. This report investigates ASPECTS as a volume surrogate without independently advocating for or against the therapies it might help plan. Method: The original authors of four publications provided their unpublished primary numerical data for further analysis. A CT study expands on the ACCESS database, which compares ASPECTS with two other subjective infarct scales: the legacy 1/3 MCA estimate and the multivariate IST-3 scale. An MRI study pools three diffusion-weighted (DWI) series as a patient-level meta-analysis unifying their comparisons of ASPECTS to the volumes that were found by semiautomated measurement. CT results: ASPECTS is unreliable, showing wide interrater variation with three quantifiable effects. It delivers little more than half as much entropy reduction as the IST-3 scale shows CT can support. Its large standard deviation (SD) takes up much of the scale width. Converting SD to a loss function with respect to a reference standard neuroradiologist gives an alarming weighted measure of error in ratings. MRI results: There are many-fold ranges of volume per ASPECTS and of ASPECTS per volume, causing sometimes large misclassifications one or both ways by any ASPECTS dichotomization. Looking past the ranges to averages, an estimate of 1/3 of the MCA territory corresponds best to ASPECTS cutting between 6 and 5 (6//5) and a volume of 65 cm3 (<1/4 of the average MCA territory). Discussion: There is already a simple, quick, and reliable manual volume measure (ABC/2, 2Sh/3). An attempt to salvage ASPECTS by reinterpreting its purpose does not hold up under scrutiny. ASPECTS can be replaced in stroke guidelines: the guideline cutting at ASPECTS 6//5 is consistent on average with the increasingly commonly stated, and better defined, 70 cm3 threshold. Arguments defending ASPECTS are rebutted by the evidence herein and by literature citations. Conclusion: ASPECTS subtracts value from the more objective direct volume measurements that are universally available by manual calculation and are becoming available by automatic software. ASPECTS inherently risks clinically significant misclassification (harm) for many patients. Page 1 Page 2 ASPECTS, The Mismeasure of Stroke: A Metrological Investigation Acknowledgments My local colleague and friend, Marco Pinho, recruited me for this project. Thank you, Marco! Ke Lin, Constance de Margerie-Mellon, Julian Schröder, J. M. Wardlaw and Francesca Chappell shared data from their studies. J. M. Wardlaw sent me an otherwise unavailable article. Ke Lin, J. M. Wardlaw, Tudor Jovin, Keith Muir, Greg Albers, and Francesca Chappell corresponded with me on various topics. Marco Pinho, Ke Lin, and J. M. Wardlaw commented on full drafts, each sharing insights that I incorporated. Several members of my Neuroradiology Section and Radiology Department, whose residents’ misgivings about ASPECTS amplified my original interest, expressed support for this investigation. In particular, Merissa Harris, Will Moore, Michael Achilleos, John Barr, Amit Agarwal, Joseph Maldjian, and Devan Moody made helpful remarks or offered valuable suggestions, Kelly Tornow based a neuroradiology journal club presentation on two of the references, Lee Pride provided §7’s critical case in point and contributed to its description and Lauren Kolski and Jacob Fleming assisted in its preparation. My wife, Shelley, graciously shared me with this project and critiqued parts of several drafts. I am indebted to all of these individuals for their generosity. Through it all, I retain sole responsibility for my errors of commission and omission and would appreciate feedback for possible future versions. Prospective readers—from students, residents and fellows, to radiologists, neurologists and other clinicians, and to research scientists and administrators—are also acknowledged for their time, interest, and labor, in the arrangement of the material. Depending on the desired topics and depth, each might read just the Synopsis, or selected sections such as the central data section (§6), or the full main text alone, or perhaps also explore the footnotes which supplement the arguments and list page numbers for the convenience of verifying or exploring the references. Version Changes Version 2 makes clarifications and additions on pages 2, 19-21, 24, 27-28, 45, 48-49, 60-63, 70, 74, and 76-78, and adds 20 new references (now 537 and with digital object identifiers if available), to reinforce the point that the use of ASPECTS is a suboptimal and superfluous practice. Version 3 adds to Page 1 this article’s OSF (Open Science Framework) Preprints digital object identifier (doi). Version 4 corrects minor technical errors; tweaks wording, typography, and graphics; credits Cavalieri’s principle; augments page 67’s last paragraph; discriminates the suitabilities of “satisficing”; notes a 207-word summary at https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A6485; and adds references: one for “data theory” on page 7, three for “QIB” on page 49, two more for “ABC/2” on page 52, as well as Yoo et al. 2012, Goyal et al. 2016b, Sheth et al. 2019, and Qiu et al. 2020. Text pagination and most footnote numbers are unchanged. Version 5 cites, on page 49, a head-to-head comparison of auto-ASPECTS and auto-volume applying the same software package to baseline CTs (Brugnara et al. 2020). Footnote numbers and pagination are otherwise unchanged. ASPECTS, The Mismeasure of Stroke: A Metrological Investigation Contents Abstract . 1 Acknowledgments . 2 Version Changes . 2 Introductory Material and Essential Background §1. Preface . 4 Synopsis . 5 §2. Ground Rules of Measurement . 7 §2.1 Scales . 7 §2.2 Purpose . 8 §2.3 Precision and Accuracy . 9 §2.4 Getting It Wrong—The Loss Function . 10 §2.5 Correlation and Surrogacy . 12 §3. The Development of ASPECTS . 13 §3.1 ASPECTS Invented. 13 §3.2 ASPECTS Taught . 16 §4. The MCA Territory and Its “1/3” Rule . 22 §5. Kappa Agreement . 29 Reanalysis of Published Data §6. How Not to Measure Volume: ASPECTS . 34 §6.1 The Real Anatomy of ASPECTS . 34 §6.2 A Further Analysis of Some CT-ASPECTS Data . 38 §6.3 A Patient-Level Pooling of DWI-ASPECTS Data . 45 Discussion Topics §7. How To Measure Volume: ABC/2, 2Sh/3 . 51 §8. ASPECTS Repurposed . 60 §9. Whither ASPECTS? . 64 Summation and Conclusion §10. Looking Back At and Forward From ASPECTS . 69 References . 79 Special Symbols and Abbreviations . 99 Page 3 Page 4 ASPECTS, The Mismeasure of Stroke: A Metrological Investigation §1. Preface ASPECTS is the Alberta Stroke Program Early CT Score, a surrogate for infarct volume (§3, §6). CT is x-ray computed tomography, but the Score applies also to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). In this report, §6.2 reanalyzes data from a CT series comparing 3 scales using 32 scans and 259 raters, and §6.3 assembles a patient-level meta-analysis of 3 MRI series (949 patients: ASPECTS vs. volume). They both show ASPECTS to be an erratic measure. Background sections address foundational issues in measurement (§2) and ASPECTS’s motive and method (§3), and review its primary claims (§4, §5). The innovative §6 assesses ASPECTS as a measurement for its target, infarct volume. §7 describes an efficient alternative. §8 rebuts a revisionist rebuttal. §9 looks at guidelines using ASPECTS. §10 sums up pros and cons and looks ahead. Flanking §6, §§1-5 collect essential introductory context, and §§7-10 elaborate on §6’s methodological implications and potential clinical significance. Accordingly, while the broad setting is early acute brain infarct therapy by human recombinant tissue plasminogen activator (tPA,1 alteplase) or embolectomy, this report is a logic- and data-driven examination of measurement technique taking no position on the therapeutic procedures themselves. While not the sole determinant of therapy, volume or whatever ASPECTS represents must be taken seriously as a quantity, subject to measurement skills, common sense, testing, and criticism. “Metrology is the science of measurement.”2 Without becoming professional metrologists (minders of the measures), we still depend on concepts and tools that refer back to their discipline. I often say that when you can measure what you are speaking about, and express it in numbers, you know something about it; but when you cannot measure it, when you cannot express it in numbers, your knowledge is of a meagre and unsatisfactory kind; it may be the beginning of knowledge, but you have scarcely, in your thoughts, advanced to the stage of science....3 This opinion by William Thomson (since 1892, Lord Kelvin), that expression-by-numbers is necessary for science, might carelessly be converted to a feeling that it suffices. The feeling might be reinforced by statistical correlation, statistical
Recommended publications
  • Stevens, Stanley Smith (1906–73) Banaji M R, Hardin C D 1996 Automatic Stereotyping
    Stereotypes, Social Psychology of counter-stereotypic, interventions that highlight Jost J T, Banaji M R 1994 The role of stereotyping in system- this association can produce a lowering of the default justification and the production of false consciousness. British stereotype of female l weak. The possibility of such Journal of Social Psychology 33: 1–27 strategies for inducing a shift in automatic stereotypes Katz D, Braly K 1933 Racial stereotypes in one hundred college students. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 28: and the potential to track stereotypes through both 280–90 behavioral and brain activation measures has the Lippmann W 1922 Public Opinion. Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovich, potential, in the future, to inform about stereotype New York representation, process, content, and mechanisms for Nosek B, Banaji M R, Greenwald A G in press Harvesting social change. intergroup attitudes and stereotypes from a demonstration website. Group Dynamics See also: Mental Representation of Persons, Psy- Phelps E A, O’Connor K J, Cunningham W A, Funayama S, chology of; Prejudice in Society; Schemas, Frames, and Gatenby J C, Gore J C, Banaji M R 2000 Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala acti- Scripts in Cognitive Psychology; Schemas, Social vation. Journal of Cognitie Neuroscience 12: 729–38 Psychology of; Small-group Interaction and Gender; Tajfel H 1969 Cognitive aspects of prejudice. Journal of Social Stigma, Social Psychology of Issues 25: 79–97 Tajfel H, Turner J C 1986 The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In: Worchel S, Austin W A (eds.) Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Nelson-Hall, Chicago, Bibliography pp.
    [Show full text]
  • Vol 1 Ross A. Mcfarland Papers
    Ross A. McFarland Collection in Aerospace Medicine and Human Factors Engineering 1 Catalog of the Library Mary Ann Hoffman Fordham Health Sciences Library Wright State University School of Medicine Dayton, Ohio 1987 Fordham Library Publication No. 2 ©1987 Ross A. McFarland 1901-1976 CONTENTS Preface vi Introduction vii Acknowledgements ix Catalog 1 Vidéocassettes ИЗ Journals 114 Technical Reports Series 117 Name Index 119 Subject Index 146 PREFACE The Ross A. McFarland Collection in Aerospace Medicine and Human Factors Engineering at the Fordham Health Sciences Library, Wright State University School of Medicine, provides an unparalleled scientific resource and data base for physicians, life scientists, engineers and others working at the leading edge of human progress, especially those in the areas of aviation, space and advanced ground transportation. The Collection is regularly consulted by those currently pioneering these fields and is an invaluable source of information constituting the base upon which future progress is being constructed. I met Dr. McFarland in 1958 and came to know him -well. I observed first-hand his pioneering concepts in human factors, enhanced immeasurably by his articulate communications. Starting in the 1930's, he almost singlehandedly launched the human factors effort in aviation, directly collecting data on airline pilot fatigue and other major operational flight safety aspects. Folio-wing Dr. McFarland's untimely death in 1976, an event -widely recognized as taking from us the father of aerospace human factors, his wife, Mrs. Emily McFarland, decided to deed his library and scientific papers to Wright State University School of Medicine, Fordham Health Sciences Library. This gift consisted of more than 6,000 print items and approximately 400 linear feet of scientific manuscripts, unpublished reports, research data and correspondence, covering 50 years of professional work and research by Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Stanley Smith Stevens
    PSYCHOMETRICA—VOL. 39, NO. 1 MARCH, 1974 Stanley Smith Stevens 1906-1973 S. S. Stevens, professor of Psychophysics in Harvard University was born in Ogden, Utah on 4 November 1906, and died in Vail, Colorado on 18 January 1973. He was, without question, the strongest voice in psychophysics since G. T. Fechner. Indeed, Stevens’ Law and Fechner’s Law represent the most widely known quantifications of stimulus-response relations in all of experimental psychology. “Smitty” Stevens’ scientific contributions bridged the Second World War. Before the war, his major work divides into three components: the analysis of methods and theory in psychology; empirical scaling studies to relate human physique to temperment; and experiments in hearing. The earliest work resulted in several classical papers on operationism and meta-psychology. His second interest culminated in a two volume collaboration with W. H. Sheldon on physique and temperment. His work on hearing led to the book of the same name in collaboration with Hallowell Davis which, even today represents a first rate overview of this complex area. During the Second World War, the Psychoacoustics Laboratory was established at Harvard in the basement of Memorial Hall, a warren in which the greatest part of Smitty’s intellectual career was spent. Under Smitty’s sponsorship, those heavy-walled basement rooms hosted a corps of scientists who helped shape the major conceptual forms of experimental psychology at mid-century. Many of these ideas entered as chapters in the monumental Handbook of Experimental Psychology , edited by Smitty, and published in 1950. By 1955, Smitty Stevens had developed methods for assessing sensory magnitudes that he believed were sufficient to be given the name “sensation scales”.
    [Show full text]
  • Networking Western Psychology's Elite: a Digital Analysis Of
    Networking Western Psychology’s Elite: a Digital Analysis of “A History of Psychology in Autobiography” Shayna Hope Anne Fox Lee THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS GRADUATE PROGRAM IN THE HISTORY & THEORY OF PSYCHOLOGY YORK UNIVERSITY TORONTO, ONTARIO DECEMBER 2014 © Shayna Hope Anne Fox Lee 2014 ii Abstract This thesis analyzes digital social networks for the institutional affiliations of the one hundred and twenty authors in the A History of Psychology in Autobiography (AHPA) book series. The introductory section contextualizes the analyses for the nine volumes in terms of the series’ historiographic foundations, socio-historical influences, and a history of the production of the first volume. It asserts that the series editors’ privileged disciplinary positions and the series’ unusual historiographic features render it an unusually precise internalist historical record of elite perspectives. The analytical chapter forwards the position that the AHPA networks illustrate the accuracy of Kurt Danziger’s (2006) historical premise of intellectual ‘centers’ and ‘peripheries’ in Western psychology’s disciplinary geography. The conclusion includes an assessment of the digital methods used, consideration of future directions, and a critical discussion of the AHPA series and how this thesis fits into a larger framework of ethical historiography. Keywords: digital history, A History of Psychology in Autobiography, social networks, disciplinary historiography, disciplinary geography, center and periphery, eminence, elitism iii Acknowledgements For their contributions and encouragement in the production of this work, I am indebted to Michael Pettit, Alexandra Rutherford, Christopher Green, Jacy Young, Raymond Fancher, Michael Sokal, Judith Fox Lee, Ali Baker, and Luke Bracegirdle.
    [Show full text]
  • Science Outside the Laboratory: Measurement in Field Science And
    Science Outside the Laboratory Science Outside the Laboratory Measurement in Field Science and Economics MARCEL BOUMANS Associate Professor History and Methodology of Economics at Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Amsterdam and at Faculty of Philosophy, Erasmus University Rotterdam 3 3 Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University’s objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide. Oxford New York Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto With offices in Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam Oxford is a registered trade mark of Oxford University Press in the UK and certain other countries. Published in the United States of America by Oxford University Press 198 Madison Avenue, New York, NY 10016 © Oxford University Press 2015 All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, without the prior permission in writing of Oxford University Press, or as expressly permitted by law, by license, or under terms agreed with the appropriate reproduction rights organization. Inquiries concerning reproduction outside the scope of the above should be sent to the Rights Department, Oxford University Press, at the address above. You must not circulate this work in any other form and you must impose this same condition on any acquirer. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Boumans, Marcel.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuroscience
    CHAPTER 16 Neuroscience Learning Objectives Timeline After reading this chapter, you should be able to: 1950 • Evaluate the contributions of behavioral neuroscience to our understanding of the brain 1954 James Olds and Peter Milner discover the “pleasure center” in the processes that underlie motivation rat brain. and learning. • Appraise the merits of cognitive 1960 neuroscience in revealing associations between localized distributebrain activity and cognitive 1966 Richard Lazarus publishes Psychological Stress and the Coping processes such as memory and Process. 1969 The Society for Neuroscience is attention. established. or 1970 • Assess the potential for affective neuroscience to contribute to the fields of clinical and health psychology. 1978 Michael Gazzaniga and Joseph • Critique the various scenarios LeDoux publish The Integrated Mind. post, that philosophers of science 1980 have envisioned for the future of neuroscience and psychology. 1983 Endel Tulving publishes Elements 1984 Richard Lazarus and Susan of Episodic Memory. Folkman publish Stress, Appraisal, and Coping. 1986 Richard Thompson publishes “The Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.” copy,1990 1994 Michael Posner and Marcus Raichle publish Images of Mind; Antonio Damasio publishes Descartes’ 1996 Joseph LeDoux publishes notError. The Emotional Brain. 1998 Jaak Panksepp publishes Affective Neuroscience; E. O. Wilson 2000 publishes Consilience: The Unity of Do Knowledge. 2010 2011 Michael Gazzaniga publishes Who’s in Charge: Free Will and the Science of the Brain. 375 Copyright ©2021 by SAGE Publications, Inc. This work may not be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means without express written permission of the publisher. Looking Back The scientific study of brain functioning goes back at least to the middle of the nineteenth cen- tury (Borck, 2016).
    [Show full text]
  • Psychologypsychology PSY A01.Qxd 2/2/05 4:30 Pm Page Ii
    PSY_A01.qxd 2/2/05 4:30 pm Page i PsychologyPsychology PSY_A01.qxd 2/2/05 4:30 pm Page ii To our children Rebecca and William Hewstone & Alex, Camilla and Jess Fincham & George and Hallam Foster PSY_A01.qxd 2/2/05 4:31 pm Page iii PsychologyPsychology Miles Hewstone, Frank D. Fincham and Jonathan Foster PSY_A01.qxd 2/2/05 4:31 pm Page iv © 2005 by The British Psychological Society and Blackwell Publishing Ltd except for editorial material and organization © 2005 by Miles Hewstone, Frank D. Fincham and Jonathan Foster BLACKWELL PUBLISHING 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148-5020, USA 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK 550 Swanston Street, Carlton, Victoria 3053, Australia The right of Miles Hewstone, Frank D. Fincham and Jonathan Foster to be identified as the Authors of the Editorial Material in this Work has been asserted in accordance with the UK Copyright, Designs, and Patents Act 1988. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise, except as permitted by the UK Copyright, Designs, and Patents Act 1988, without the prior permission of the publisher. First published 2005 by The British Psychological Society and Blackwell Publishing Ltd 1 2005 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Hewstone, Miles. Psychology / Miles Hewstone, Frank D. Fincham and Jonathan Foster. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN-13: 978-0-631-20678-1 (paperback) ISBN-10: 0-631-20678-7 (paperback) 1. Psychology—Textbooks.
    [Show full text]
  • Library of the History of Psychology Theories
    Library of the History of Psychology Theories Series Editor Robert W. Rieber Fordham University New York, NY USA For further volumes: http://www.springer.com/series/6927 Eugene Taylor The Mystery of Personality A History of Psychodynamic Theories 123 Eugene Taylor Saybrook Graduate School and Research Center 747 Front St San Francisco, CA 94111 USA [email protected] ISBN 978-0-387-98103-1 e-ISBN 978-0-387-98104-8 DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-98104-8 Springer Dordrecht Heidelberg London New York Library of Congress Control Number: 2009927014 © Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009 All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 Spring Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if they are not identified as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are subject to proprietary rights. Printed on acid-free paper Springer is part of Springer Science+Business Media (www.springer.com) “Every man is... like all other men, like some other men, like no other man.” Henry A. Murray, MD, PhD (1893–1988) Acknowledgments Readers, I hope, will forgive me at the outset for any inordinate focus on materi- als in the English language and particularly my focus on dynamic theories of per- sonality in the history of American psychology, although I have also referred to British and European sources and even touched lightly on the classical psycholo- gies of Asia.
    [Show full text]
  • A History of Psychology in Autobiography”
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by YorkSpace Networking Western Psychology’s Elite: a Digital Analysis of “A History of Psychology in Autobiography” Shayna Hope Anne Fox Lee THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS GRADUATE PROGRAM IN THE HISTORY & THEORY OF PSYCHOLOGY YORK UNIVERSITY TORONTO, ONTARIO DECEMBER 2014 © Shayna Hope Anne Fox Lee 2014 ii Abstract This thesis analyzes digital social networks for the institutional affiliations of the one hundred and twenty authors in the A History of Psychology in Autobiography (AHPA) book series. The introductory section contextualizes the analyses for the nine volumes in terms of the series’ historiographic foundations, socio-historical influences, and a history of the production of the first volume. It asserts that the series editors’ privileged disciplinary positions and the series’ unusual historiographic features render it an unusually precise internalist historical record of elite perspectives. The analytical chapter forwards the position that the AHPA networks illustrate the accuracy of Kurt Danziger’s (2006) historical premise of intellectual ‘centers’ and ‘peripheries’ in Western psychology’s disciplinary geography. The conclusion includes an assessment of the digital methods used, consideration of future directions, and a critical discussion of the AHPA series and how this thesis fits into a larger framework of ethical historiography. Keywords: digital history, A History of Psychology in Autobiography, social networks, disciplinary historiography, disciplinary geography, center and periphery, eminence, elitism iii Acknowledgements For their contributions and encouragement in the production of this work, I am indebted to Michael Pettit, Alexandra Rutherford, Christopher Green, Jacy Young, Raymond Fancher, Michael Sokal, Judith Fox Lee, Ali Baker, and Luke Bracegirdle.
    [Show full text]
  • Bibliography
    Chapter 2 BIBLIOGRAPHY This annotated bibliography of Frederick Mosteller's writings has four sec­ tions: books, numbered Bl through B56; papers, numbered PI through P182; miscellaneous writings, numbered Ml through M36; and reviews, numbered Rl through R25. Ordinarily we list publications chronologically within each of these sections. We deviate from that ordering to bring to­ gether closely related books. For example, B19 (1984) is an expanded edi­ tion of B18 (1964). We list Fred's name as it appeared on the publication, usually "Frederick Mosteller." For works with other authors, we list the names in the order that they appeared on the publication. For publications by a committee or panel, we list the members. If, in addition to being one of the editors of a book, Fred is author or coauthor of a chapter, we list that chapter under the book but not among the papers. For example, B47 was edited by Hoaglin, Mosteller, and Tukey, and they also coauthored Chapter 9. We therefore list Chapter 9 under B47. We give any additional information that we have, such as new editions, different printings, and translations. We realize that our information about translations is not complete, nor do we always know of reprinting of papers or chapters. When a paper is an abridgment of a chapter or another paper, we indicate that. BOOKS BI Hadley Cantril and Research Associates in the Office of Public Opinion Re­ search, Princeton University. Gauging Public Opinion. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1944. Second printing, 1947. • Chapter IV. Frederick Mosteller and Hadley Cantril. "The use and value of a battery of questions." pp.
    [Show full text]