Music Review, © 1987 Harwood Academic Publishers Gmbh 1987, Vol

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Music Review, © 1987 Harwood Academic Publishers Gmbh 1987, Vol Contemporary Music Review, © 1987 Harwood Academic Publishers GmbH 1987, Vol. 2 pp. 1-61 Printed in the United Kingdom Photocopying permitted by license only Music: A science of the mind? Stephen McAdams Institut de Recherche et Coordination Acoustique/Musique, Paris, France (psychologist) There is an increasing interest in psychological studies of music for the advancement of both musical and scientific thought. An historical perspective of psychological considera- tions of music reveals a trend leading from physical thought through theories of sensation and finally up to modern cognitive psychology. What might truly be called the field of music psychology can be shown to exist by an overview of the domains of research covered by contemporary theorists and researchers. Within the framework of cognitive psychology one can demonstrate the importance of mental representation of the various dimensions and structures of music and of organizational processes underlying music listening. The relation between a musical structure and the form that is "accumulated" by a listener depends to a large extent on his or her musical experience within a given culture. A difference in experience may be hypothesized to result from the nature of mental schemata acquired by listeners. A great deal of work is still needed, however, to approach more affective and aesthetic aspects of musical experience. KEY WORDS music psychology, cognition, internal representation, organizational processes, musical form, musical structure. There is one real, and graded, distinction between sciences like the biologies and the physical sciences. The former are unrestricted and their investigator must be prepared to follow their problems into any other science whatsoever. Downloaded By: [McGill University] At: 22:23 22 August 2008 C.F.A. Pantin (1968, p.24) Introduction Music is a fertile ground for the development of thought in the cognitive sciences, but why would a scientist of the mind dare attempt an analysis of the nature of human musical experience? Can there be any realm of collective human production and response more obscure, mysterious and, finally, so absolutely personal and individual? At the same time, can any human science ignore one of the species' most unique capacities? The answer to this last question being an obvious "No!," the question arises of how to,proceed. With current knowledge drawn from the various human sciences, from diverse theories of music and from the speculations of practising 2 Stephen McAdams composers, we must take stock of what we know and what we can formulate as a starring point for a psychology of music. A thorough scientific consideration of musical experience necessitates drawing from many fields including acoustics, psychology, brain sciences, artificial intelligence and music theory. Although, any single researcher or thinker must necessarily limit the scope of his investigations, much of what today calls itself the psychology of music runs the opposite risk of narrowness of focus and lack of appropriate musical culture. There is a need at this point in the development of cognitive explorations into music to have a plan of approach and a vision of the totality with which to judge the relevance of more specific considerations. Included in this global vision is an intimate knowledge of music itself. The study of music has a great deal to offer an understanding of the human mind. The starting assumption of the psychologist of music is that the structure and process of music can indicate the nature of certain mental structures and processes. The study of mental structures and processes is the domain of cognitive psychology, which seeks to understand internal (or mental) representations and the things that these representations allow one to do with music. The nature of the representations is inferred from the ways people listen to, memorize, perform, create and react to music. The psychologist of music seeks the neatest and most economical manner of describing music's structure in a way that most closely resembles the psychological processes by which music is created, reproduced and understood (Sloboda, 1985, p.ll). We may suppose, then, that since music is a uniquely human product, we can logically draw conclusions about the connection between the observed structure of music and the nature of the human mind that produces it to be heard by others. What might these conclusions be, and what do they contribute to a general understanding about human mental activity and about the possibilities of music itself? Another of the underlying beliefs of this endeavour is that psychologi- cal investigation and theory also have something to offer the musical disciplines. There are two main constraints, however: the work must Downloaded By: [McGill University] At: 22:23 22 August 2008 conform to the accepted procedures of scientific investigation and must, as well, succeed in being relevant to the experience of real music and not only limited to that of overly reduced pre- and proto-musical collections of sounds, though these lower-level investigations will always be important in analysing the contribution of various psychological processes to the comprehension of music. I will attempt to demonstrate to both psychologists and musicians the relevance of music psychology for contemporary musical and scientific thought. Starting with an historical perspective that traces psychological considerations of music from the ancient Greeks to modern cognitive psychology, I will then describe some of the main problem areas in the realm of music psychology in order to situate the framework for a more detailed discussion of the two main problems in cognitive science as applied to an understanding of musical experience. These include the mental representation of the dimensions and structures of music, and Music: a science of the mind? 3 the organizational mechanisms underlying the mental processing of musical structures. The aim is to arrive at an understanding of the experience of musical form, as rich or poor as that may be depending on one's previous experience with the music of a given culture. The psychology of music clearly includes more areas of study than are treated in this article, but I have deliberately limited the scope of this volume to reception and processing of musical structure and the experience of musical form. Also, while this article is consciously biased towards the theory of psychological issues, it is balanced by the more specifically musical considerations of my musical colleagues in the other articles of this issue. The cognitive trajectory The history of psychological considerations of music reveals a trajectory from physical thought to sensation, perception and cognition (Figure I).1 The Greeks of the 6th Century BC attempted an integration of the laws of nature with theories of aesthetics. For Pythagoras, there was an important relation of ratios of small numbers between pitches (string lengths) to musical consonance. The apprehension of these ratios was a manifestation of a "higher" sense he called "pleasure-in-proportion." This sense was to be distinguished from the more base senses involved in ordinary perception. The idea was developed by Plato who made a psychological distinction between mere perception and an inbuilt response to proportion and the rhythms of the universe (the harmony of the spheres). This response naturally leads one, he mused, to a state of being where the flux of the base senses is resolved in a higher state of equilibrium. In contemporary times this classical notion of resolution as equilibrium or a state of quiescence has yielded to a more dynamic homeostatic model of perception which takes into account the necessity within an organism of fluctuations around an optimal level. At a physiological level, things must constantly be in a state of flux or the Downloaded By: [McGill University] At: 22:23 22 August 2008 sensory systems cease to respond. The best known example of this is the fact that the eyes constantly make small random movements to keep the image of the environment moving across the retina. If this image is held in tiie same place, by pressing one's finger against the eyeball, for example, it slowly fades away. Similar kinds of "adaptation" or "fatigue" to constant sound stimuli have been shown in auditory perception. What this may imply at a higher, more musical, level is that the tensing and releasing functions in the development of musical ideas may characterize an aesthetic response by first defining a certain "optimal" state, and then departing from and returning to this state as a way of modulating the experienced tension. Another assumption in Pythagorian thought that is implicitly psycho- logical is the notion that there is an identical correspondence between some specific external, or physical, property (string length) and the resulting internal, or perceptual, property (sensation of pitch). This 4 Stephen Me Adams Physicalists Downloaded By: [McGill University] At: 22:23 22 August 2008 "Natural" Psychoaesthetics Psychoacoustics Music Cognition Intonations and Microtonal Systems Figure 1 This diagram represents a possible reading of the historical trajectory of music psychology (with time progressing from top to bottom) from the physically based thought of the ancient Greeks to modern cognitive science. The arrows indicate transitions to subsequent schools of thought and the double bars indicate a rupture. At the bottom are some current areas of systematic or scientific investigation
Recommended publications
  • Statistical Techniques in Music Psychology: an Update
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Goldsmiths Research Online Statistical techniques in music psychology: An update Daniel M¨ullensiefen 1 Introduction Music psychology as a discipline has its origins at the end of the 19th century and ever since then, empirical methods have been a core part in this field of research. While its experimental and analytical methods have mainly been related to methodology employed in general psychology, several statistical techniques have emerged over the course of the past century being specific for empirical research in music psychology. These core methods have been described in a few didactic and summarising publications at several stages of the discipline’s history (see e.g. Wundt, 1882; B¨ottcher & Kerner, 1978; Windsor, 2001, or Beran, 2004 for a very technical overview), and these publications have been valuable resources to students and researchers alike. In contrast to these texts with a rather didactical focus, the objective of this chapter is to provide an overview of a range of novel statistical techniques that have been employed in recent years in music psychology research.1 This overview will give enough insight into each technique as such. The interested reader will then have to turn to the original publications, to obtain a more in-depth knowledge of the details related to maths and the field of application. Empirical research into auditory perception and the psychology of music might have its beginnings in the opening of the psychological laboratory by Wilhelm Wundt in Leipzig in 1879 where experiments on human perception were conducted, and standards for empirical research and analysis were developed.
    [Show full text]
  • The Impact of Music on Studying Ability in College Students
    College of Saint Benedict and Saint John's University DigitalCommons@CSB/SJU Celebrating Scholarship and Creativity Day Undergraduate Research 4-26-2018 The Impact of Music on Studying Ability in College Students Nathaniel T. Lutmer College of Saint Benedict/Saint John's University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.csbsju.edu/ur_cscday Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Lutmer, Nathaniel T., "The Impact of Music on Studying Ability in College Students" (2018). Celebrating Scholarship and Creativity Day. 39. https://digitalcommons.csbsju.edu/ur_cscday/39 This Presentation is brought to you for free and open access by DigitalCommons@CSB/SJU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Celebrating Scholarship and Creativity Day by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@CSB/SJU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. 1 RUNNING HEAD: MUSIC AND STUDYING ABILITY The Impact of Music on Studying Ability in College Students Nathaniel T. Lutmer College of St. Benedict and Saint John’s University Author Note Nathaniel T. Lutmer, Department of Psychology, College of Saint Benedict and Saint John’s University Corresponding concerns regarding this article should be addressed to Nathaniel Lutmer, Department of Psychology, College of St. Benedict and Saint John’s University, Collegeville, MN 56321 Contact: [email protected] 2 RUNNING HEAD: MUSIC AND STUDYING ABILITY Abstract This study investigates the relationship between listening to music and studying ability in college students. This study was conducted by utilizing a convenience sampling technique to have participants partake in the study. Each participant was randomly assigned to either a control or one of two experimental groups based on block-random assignment.
    [Show full text]
  • Music Analysis: an Annotated Bibliography. INSTITUTION Southwest Regional Library for Educational Research and Development, Los Alamitos, Calif
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 067 359 SO 004 665 AUTHOR Fink, Michael TITLE Music Analysis: An Annotated Bibliography. INSTITUTION Southwest Regional Library for Educational Research and Development, Los Alamitos, Calif. REPORT NO TR-43 BUREAU NO BR- 6-2865 PUB DATE 1 Aug 72 NOTE 25p. EDRSPRICE MF-$0.65 HC-$3.29 DESCRIPTORS *Annotated Bibliographies; *Applied Music; Elementary Education; Higher Education; Music; Musical Composition; Music Appreciation; *Music Education; Music Techniques; *Music Theory; Secondary Education ABSTRACT One hundred and forty citations comprise this annotated bibliography of books, articles, and selected dissertations that encompass trends in music theory and k-16 music education since the late 19th century. Special emphasis is upon writings since the 19501s. During earlier development, music analysts concentrated upon the elements of music (i.e., melody, harmony, rhythm, and form). Since 1950, varying viewpoints on the teaching of music analysis have emerged, producing a surge of various analytical trends and philosophies derived from other than musical contexts. Information theory, phenomenology, and the application of computers have made the strongest impact upon music theory in recent years. Classified headings in the listing cover:1) general discussions of music analysis, 2)the analysis of specific elements of music (melody, harmony, etc.),3)principal trends and approaches to the subject, and 4)samples of analytic models. The headings reflect the evolution of trends within the subject. Author entries are alphabetically arranged under headings. Whief descriptive annotations are provided. (Author/SJM) ibie ks!) 1 I5A) SOUTHWEST REGIONAL LABORATORY FOR EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH & DEVELOPMENT Music Analysis: An AnnotatedBibliography Tit 431 August 1972 I U.S.
    [Show full text]
  • Concepts As Correlates of Lexical Labels. a Cognitivist Perspective
    This is a submitted manuscript version. The publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form. Final published version, copyright Peter Lang: https://doi.org/10.3726/978-3-653-05287-9 Sławomir Wacewicz Concepts as Correlates of Lexical Labels. A Cognitivist Perspective. This is a submitted manuscript version. The publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form. Final published version, copyright Peter Lang: https://doi.org/10.3726/978-3-653-05287-9 CONTENTS Introduction………………………………………………………………... 6 PART I INTERNALISTIC PERSPECTIVE ON LANGUAGE IN COGNITIVE SCIENCE Preliminary remarks………………………………………………………… 17 1. History and profile of Cognitive Science……………………………….. 18 1.1. Introduction…………………………………………………………. 18 1.2. Cognitive Science: definitions and basic assumptions ……………. 19 1.3. Basic tenets of Cognitive 22 Science…………………………………… 1.3.1. Cognition……………………………………………………... 23 1.3.2. Representationism and presentationism…………………….... 25 1.3.3. Naturalism and physical character of mind…………………... 28 1.3.4. Levels of description…………………………………………. 30 1.3.5. Internalism (Individualism) ………………………………….. 31 1.4. History……………………………………………………………... 34 1.4.1. Prehistory…………………………………………………….. 35 1.4.2. Germination…………………………………………………... 36 1.4.3. Beginnings……………………………………………………. 37 1.4.4. Early and classical Cognitive Science………………………… 40 1.4.5. Contemporary Cognitive Science……………………………... 42 1.4.6. Methodological notes on interdisciplinarity………………….. 52 1.5. Summary…………………………………………………………. 59 2. Intrasystemic and extrasystemic principles of concept individuation 60 2.1. Existential status of concepts ……………………………………… 60 2 This is a submitted manuscript version. The publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form. Final published version, copyright Peter Lang: https://doi.org/10.3726/978-3-653-05287-9 2.1.1.
    [Show full text]
  • Eleanor J. Gibson from Wikipedia, the Free Encyclopedia
    Eleanor J. Gibson From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia Eleanor Jack Gibson (7 December 1910 – 30 December 2002) was an American psychologist who focused on reading Eleanor Jack Gibson development and perceptual learning in infants and toddlers. In the 1960s and 1970s Gibson, with her husband James J. Gibson, created the Gibsonian ecological theory of development which emphasized how important perception was because it allows humans to adapt to their environments. Perhaps her most well- known contribution to psychology was the "visual cliff", which studied depth perception and visual or motor impairments in both human and animal species. This led to a new understanding of perceptual development in infants. The environment provides information for the sensory system to develop with increased Eleanor Gibson - Keynote Address - 1993 APS stimuli, so perceptual development corresponds with Convention environmental stimuli. Infants develop from adapting to the Born December 7, 1910 environment. Gibson was elected to the National Academy of Peoria, Illinois Sciences in 1971 and as a fellow of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1977. In 1992 she was awarded the Died December 30, 2002 (aged 92) National Medal of Science, which is the highest scientific honor Columbia, South Carolina in the United States, and only five of which have been awarded Fields psychology to psychologists. Institutions Cornell University Alma mater Smith College (B.A., 1931) (M.S., 1933) Contents Yale University (Ph.D., 1938) Doctoral Clark L. Hull 1 Biography advisor 2 Legacy timeline Known for Visual Cliff, Differentiation and 3 Representative research 3.1 Perceptual learning Enrichment of Embedded Structures 3.2 Visual cliff Influences James J.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive and Neuroscience Research on Learning And
    The 11th International Conference on Education Research New Educational Paradigm for Learning and Instruction September 29 – October 1, 2010 Cognitive and Neuroscience Research on Learning and Instruction: Recent insights about the impact of non-conscious knowledge on problem solving, higher order thinking skills 12 and interactive cyber-learning environments. Richard E. Clark University of Southern California, USA Abstract There are as least three powerful insights from recent studies of the brain that support cognitive science research findings: First, our brains learn and process two very different types of knowledge; non conscious, automated knowledge and conscious, controllable, declarative knowledge. Evidence also suggests that we believe we control our own learning by conscious choice when in fact nearly all mental operations are highly automated, including learning and problem solving; Second, human beings have a very limited capacity to think during learning and problem solving and when that capacity is exceeded, thinking and learning stop without us being aware. Thus instruction and self managed learning must strive to avoid cognitive overload; and Third, nearly all of our instructional design and cyber learning theories and models fail to account for the influence of non-conscious cognitive processes and therefore are inadequate to deal with complex learning and performance. Evidence for these points will be described and their implications for instruction and the learning of problem solving and higher order thinking skills will be discussed. Models of learning and instruction that appear to help overcome some of these biological and cognitive barriers will be described. In addition, suggestions for new research questions on interactive learning environments that take account of the three insights will also be described.
    [Show full text]
  • The Science of Psychology 1
    PSY_C01.qxd 1/2/05 3:17 pm Page 2 The Science of Psychology 1 CHAPTER OUTLINE LEARNING OBJECTIVES INTRODUCTION PINNING DOWN PSYCHOLOGY PSYCHOLOGY AND COMMON SENSE: THE GRANDMOTHER CHALLENGE Putting common sense to the test Explaining human behaviour THE BEGINNINGS OF MODERN PSYCHOLOGY Philosophical influences Physiological influences PSYCHOLOGY TODAY Structuralism: mental chemistry Functionalism: mental accomplishment Behaviourism: a totally objective psychology Gestalt psychology: making connections Out of school: the independents The cognitive revolution FINAL THOUGHTS SUMMARY REVISION QUESTIONS FURTHER READING PSY_C01.qxd 1/2/05 3:17 pm Page 3 Learning Objectives By the end of this chapter you should appreciate that: n psychology is much more than ‘common sense’; n psychological knowledge can be usefully applied in many different professions and walks of life; n psychology emerged as a distinct discipline around 150 years ago, from its roots in physiology, physics and philosophy; n there are fundamental differences between different schools of thought in psychology; n psychology is the science of mental life and behaviour, and different schools of thought within psychology place differing degrees of emphasis on understanding these different elements of psychology; n most academic departments in the English-speaking world focus on the teaching of experimental psychology, in which scientific evidence about the structure and function of the mind and behaviour accumulates through the execution of empirical investigations; n in the history of psychology many different metaphors have been used for thinking about the workings of the human mind, and since the Second World War the most influential of these metaphors has been another complex information-processing device – the computer.
    [Show full text]
  • The Elements of Music
    KSKS35 The elements of music Anna Gower was by Anna Gower a secondary music teacher and head of music for 18 years. She now works as a consultant for Trinity College London INTRODUCTION and Musical Futures International, as well The elements of music. DR P SMITH or MAD T SHIRT; posters on the walls of your classroom; a handy checklist as with various MEH for writing a model answer to a GCSE question; or a guide to development in an A level composition. and organisations in the UK and overseas. The elements of music have long been an important component of KS3 music, whether they’re taught as a stand- alone unit or topic, or threaded through performing, composing or listening activities in a topic-based curriculum. Often described as the ‘building blocks’ of music, the However, the elements of music are really just a set of labels and concepts under which sits a wealth of musical elements of music understanding, and a heap of vocabulary and musical language that can help to hone student responses to may have slightly music that they hear, play, sing or compose from the very start of their music education. different labels that vary from school to school. But they It’s that further layer beneath the label and concept that’s much harder to measure, and which weaves through each come with all good music learning and teaching. It’s about crafting an understanding of what makes music a medium a label or name (pitch, rhythm, through which to be expressive, to make sense of sound, and to arrange it into meaningful musical experiences etc), a concept that allow students to demonstrate their learning in ways other than a listening test or performance assessment.
    [Show full text]
  • The Rise of Cognitive Science in the 20Th Century
    The Rise of Cognitive Science in the 20th Century Carrie Figdor (forthcoming in Kind, A., ed., Philosophy of Mind in the 20th and 21st Centuries: Routledge) Penultimate Draft Abstract. This chapter describes the conceptual foundations of cognitive science during its establishment as a science in the 20th century. It is organized around the core ideas of individual agency as its basic explanans and information-processing as its basic explanandum. The latter consists of a package of ideas that provide a mathematico- engineering framework for the philosophical theory of materialism. Keywords: cognitive science; psychology; linguistics; computer science; neuroscience; behaviorism; artificial intelligence; computationalism; connectionism Part I. Introduction. Cognitive science is the study of individual agency: its nature, scope, mechanisms, and patterns. It studies what agents are and how they function. This definition is modified from one provided by Bechtel, Abrahamson, and Graham (1998), where cognitive science is defined as “the multidisciplinary scientific study of cognition and its role in intelligent agency.” Several points motivate the modification. First (and least consequential), the multidisciplinarity of cognitive science is an accident of academic history, not a fact about its subject matter (a point also pressed in Gardner 1985). Second, the label “intelligent” is often used as a term of normative assessment, when cognitive science is concerned with behavior by entities (including possibly groups, as individual or collective agents) that are not considered intelligent, as well as unintelligent behavior of intelligent agents, for any intuitive definition of “intelligent”.1 Third and most importantly, the term “cognition” is omitted from the definiens to help emphasize a position of neutrality on a number of contemporary debates.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Music Studies Pdf, Epub, Ebook
    AN INTRODUCTION TO MUSIC STUDIES PDF, EPUB, EBOOK Jim Samson,J. P. E. Harper-Scott | 310 pages | 31 Jan 2009 | CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS | 9780521603805 | English | Cambridge, United Kingdom An Introduction to Music Studies PDF Book To see what your friends thought of this book, please sign up. An analysis of sociomusicology, its issues; and the music and society in Hong Kong. Critical Entertainments: Music Old and New. Other Editions 6. The examination measures knowledge of facts and terminology, an understanding of concepts and forms related to music theory for example: pitch, dynamics, rhythm, melody , types of voices, instruments, and ensembles, characteristics, forms, and representative composers from the Middle Ages to the present, elements of contemporary and non-Western music, and the ability to apply this knowledge and understanding in audio excerpts from musical compositions. An Introduction to Music Studies by J. She has been described by the Harvard Gazette as "one of the world's most accomplished and admired music historians". The job market for tenure track professor positions is very competitive. You should have a passion for music and a strong interest in developing your understanding of music and ability to create it. D is the standard minimum credential for tenure track professor positions. Historical studies of music are for example concerned with a composer's life and works, the developments of styles and genres, e. Mus or a B. For other uses, see Musicology disambiguation. More Details Refresh and try again. Goodreads helps you keep track of books you want to read. These models were established not only in the field of physical anthropology , but also cultural anthropology.
    [Show full text]
  • The Association of Strategy Use and Concrete-Operational Thinking in Primary School
    ORIGINAL RESEARCH published: 23 May 2018 doi: 10.3389/feduc.2018.00038 The Association of Strategy Use and Concrete-Operational Thinking in Primary School Moritz Börnert-Ringleb* and Jürgen Wilbert Department of Inclusive Education, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany Concrete-operational thinking depicts an important aspect of cognitive development. A promising approach in promoting these skills is the instruction of strategies. The construction of such instructional programs requires insights into the mental operations involved in problem-solving. In the present paper, we address the question to which extent variations of the effect of isolated and combined mental operations (strategies) on correct solution of concrete-operational concepts can be observed. Therefore, a cross-sectional design was applied. The use of mental operations was measured by thinking-aloud reports from 80 first- and second-graders (N = 80) while solving tasks depicting concrete-operational thinking. Concrete-operational thinking was assessed Edited by: using the subscales conservation of numbers, classification and sequences of the TEKO. Michael S. Dempsey, Boston University, United States The verbal reports were transcribed and coded with regard to the mental operations Reviewed by: applied per task. Data analyses focused on tasks level, resulting in the analyses of Sharinaz Hassan, N = 240 tasks per subscale. Differences regarding the contribution of isolated and Curtin University, Australia Niels A. Taatgen, combined mental operations (strategies) to correct solution were observed. Thereby, University of Groningen, Netherlands the results indicate the necessity of selection and integration of appropriate mental *Correspondence: operations as strategies. The results offer insights in involved mental operations while Moritz Börnert-Ringleb solving concrete-operational tasks and depict a contribution to the construction of [email protected] instructional programs.
    [Show full text]
  • Glossary for Music the Glossary for Music Includes Terms Commonly Found in Music Education and for Performance Techniques
    Glossary for Music The glossary for Music includes terms commonly found in music education and for performance techniques. The intent of the glossary is to promote consistent terminology when creating curriculum and assessment documents as well as communicating with stakeholders. Ability: natural aptitude in specific skills and processes; what the student is apt to do, without formal instruction. Analog tools: category of musical instruments and tools that are non-digital (i.e., do not transfer sound in or convert sound into binary code), such as acoustic instruments, microphones, monitors, and speakers. Analyze: examine in detail the structure and context of the music. Arrangement: setting or adaptation of an existing musical composition Arranger: person who creates alternative settings or adaptations of existing music. Articulation: characteristic way in which musical tones are connected, separated, or accented; types of articulation include legato (smooth, connected tones) and staccato (short, detached tones). Artistic literacy: knowledge and understanding required to participate authentically in the arts Atonality: music in which no tonic or key center is apparent. Artistic Processes: Organizational principles of the 2014 National Core Standards for the Arts: Creating, Performing, Responding, and Connecting. Audiate: hear and comprehend sounds in one’s head (inner hearing), even when no sound is present. Audience etiquette: social behavior observed by those attending musical performances and which can vary depending upon the type of music performed. Benchmark: pre-established definition of an achievement level, designed to help measure student progress toward a goal or standard, expressed either in writing or as an example of scored student work (aka, anchor set).
    [Show full text]