The Symbol-Grounding Problem in Numerical Cognition: a Review of Theory, Evidence, and Outstanding Questions

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Symbol-Grounding Problem in Numerical Cognition: a Review of Theory, Evidence, and Outstanding Questions See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/295910425 The symbol-grounding problem in numerical cognition: A review of theory, evidence, and outstanding questions Article in Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology · February 2016 DOI: 10.1037/cep0000070 CITATIONS READS 8 534 2 authors, including: Daniel Ansari The University of Western Ontario 144 PUBLICATIONS 5,569 CITATIONS SEE PROFILE All content following this page was uploaded by Daniel Ansari on 01 March 2016. The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file. All in-text references underlined in blue are added to the original document and are linked to publications on ResearchGate, letting you access and read them immediately. APA NLM tapraid5/cae-cae/cae-cae/cae00415/cae0277d15z xppws Sϭ1 10/1/15 15:45 Art: 2015-1231 Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology / Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale © 2015 Canadian Psychological Association 2015, Vol. 69, No. 4, 000 1196-1961/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/cep0000070 The Symbol-Grounding Problem in Numerical Cognition: A Review of Theory, Evidence, and Outstanding Questions AQ: au Tali Leibovich and Daniel Ansari AQ: 1 The University of Western Ontario How do numerical symbols, such as number words, acquire semantic meaning? This question, also referred to as the “symbol-grounding problem,” is a central problem in the field of numerical cognition. Present theories suggest that symbols acquire their meaning by being mapped onto an approximate system for the nonsymbolic representation of number (Approximate Number System or ANS). In the present literature review, we first asked to which extent current behavioural and neuroimaging data support this theory, and second, to which extent the ANS, upon which symbolic numbers are assumed to be grounded, is numerical in nature. We conclude that (a) current evidence that has examined the association between the ANS and number symbols does not support the notion that number symbols are grounded in the ANS and (b) given the strong correlation between numerosity and continuous variables in nonsymbolic number processing tasks, it is next to impossible to measure the pure association between symbolic and nonsymbolic numerosity. Instead, it is clear that significant cognitive control resources are required to disambiguate numerical from continuous variables during nonsymbolic number processing. Thus, if there exists any mapping between the ANS and symbolic number, then this process of association must be mediated by cognitive control. Taken together, we suggest that studying the role of both cognitive control and continuous variables in numerosity comparison tasks will provide a more complete picture as to the symbol-grounding problem. Keywords: numerical cognition, symbol-grounding problem, approximate number system, numerical magnitudes, nonnumerical magnitudes Whether it is to estimate the distance to the nearest bus stop, to of number that we share with other species (the so-called “Approx- choose the shortest line in the supermarket or to calculate the imate Number System” or ANS). Against this background, the change given to you, we use numbers constantly in our everyday present literature review will examine (a) the extent to which the lives. How we represent and process numbers has been studied available behavioural and neuroscientific evidence supports extensively in humans at different developmental stages, as well as the theory that number symbols are grounded in the ANS and (b) in a variety of animal species. One of the key questions in the the extent to which the purported ANS is numerical in nature. The literature concerns how humans learn the meaning of numerical review will close by outlining questions for future theory and symbols (sometimes referred to as the “symbol-grounding prob- research. lem”), such as Arabic numerals, which are uniquely human cul- tural representation of numerosity that children need to learn over The Number Sense/ANS Account of the the course of development. The present article begins by reviewing Symbol-Grounding Problem the most prominent proposal for the resolution of the symbol- One of the most prominent theories in the field of numerical grounding problem in numerical cognition, which postulates that cognition was put forward by several researchers (e.g., Dehaene, symbols are grounded in a system for the approximate representation 1997; Gallistel & Gelman, 1992). According to this theory, the ability to process numerical quantities (i.e., the total number of items in a set or its numerosity) is a basic, automatic, and innate ability that can be found across species. In other words, humans Tali Leibovich and Daniel Ansari, Numerical Cognition Laboratory, are born with a capacity to process nonsymbolic numerosities (e.g., Department of Psychology & Brain and Mind Institute, The University of dot arrays, groups of objects, number of sounds, etc.). Further- Western Ontario. more, according to this theory, symbolic representations of num- This work was supported by operating grants from the Natural Sciences ber, such as Arabic numerals and number words, which children and Engineering Council of Canada (NSERC), the Canadian Institutes of learn over the course of development, are thought to acquire their Health Research (CIHR), the Canada Research Chairs Program, an E.W.R meaning by being mapped onto the preexisting, nonsymbolic rep- Steacie Memorial Fellowship from the Natural Sciences and Engineering resentations of number. Council of Canada (NSERC) to Daniel Ansari, as well as a Brain & Mind Institute (Western University) Postdoctoral Scholarship to Tali Leibovich. This theory is supported by studies demonstrating that preverbal Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Daniel babies and animals are able to spontaneously discriminate between Ansari, Department of Psychology & Brain and Mind Institute, The Uni- numerosities (e.g., Libertus & Brannon, 2009; Nieder, 2005). For versity of Western Ontario, Westminster Hall, Room 325, London, ON example, in a habituation study (repeated presentation of a partic- N6A 3K7, Canada. E-mail: [email protected] ular stimuli followed by the presentation of a stimuli that differs in 1 APA NLM tapraid5/cae-cae/cae-cae/cae00415/cae0277d15z xppws Sϭ1 10/1/15 15:45 Art: 2015-1231 2 LEIBOVICH AND ANSARI the variable of interest) by Xu and Spelke (2000), infants were habituated to groups of dots in different sizes but that contained the same numerosity. In the test stage, the infants saw two displays— one display with the same numerosity as in the habituation stimuli and another display containing a novel number of dots. The results OC NO of this experiment revealed that infants looked longer toward the LL novel numerosity compared with the numerosity to which they had IO been habituated. Furthermore, it was found that 6-month-old in- NR fants only exhibited the ability to discriminate between the habit- E uated and novel numerosity when the ratio between the two stimuli was 0.5 (e.g., 8 vs. 16 dots), but not when the ratio was higher Figure 1. Basic effect in numerical cognition. (A) The numerical ratio (e.g., 8 vs. 12 dots). These findings have since been replicated in effect. The plot illustrates the relationship between numerical ratio and a number of different labouratories (Coubart, Izard, Spelke, Marie, response time, in a number comparison task. The x-axis describes the numerical ratio: smaller/larger numerosity. The y-axis represents the time & Streri, 2014; Izard, Sann, Spelke, & Streri, 2009). In addition to it takes to respond to the larger number. Task difficulty increases when the work on infants, it has also been demonstrated that a variety of numerical ratio is closer to 1 (i.e., with an increase in numerical ratio). animals are able to discriminate between different nonsymbolic Inside the plot are examples for symbolic and nonsymbolic stimuli: The numerosities (birds, Bogale, Kamata, Mioko, & Sugita, 2011; numerical ratio of 3 and 8 is ϳ0.37, and the numerical distance is 4; the Watanabe, 1998; fish, Agrillo, Dadda, Serena, & Bisazza, 2008, numerical ratio of 7 and 9 is ϳ0.77, and the numerical distance is 2. It is 2009; rodents, Meck & Church, 1983; lions, McComb, Packer, & also true, then, that task difficulty increases with the decrease in numerical Pusey, 1994; and primates, Cantlon & Brannon, 2006; Nieder, distance. (B) Approximate representation of numerosities. Representation Freedman, & Miller, 2002). of numbers is thought to be represented on a logarithmic scale. This Discrimination between different numerosities shows a similar representation is assumed to be approximate and noisy: Larger numbers are response pattern to that of discriminating between different mag- represented more approximately, and the representation of adjacent num- bers overlap. See the online article for the color version of this figure. nitudes, such as brightness, pitch of sound, physical size, and weight (Cantlon, Platt, & Brannon, 2009; but see also Leibovich, Ashkenazi, Rubinsten, & Henik, 2013; Leibovich, Diesendruck, The ANS and the Symbol-Grounding Problem Rubinsten, & Henik, 2013). In all these cases, the ability to detect change (or to understand that two presented magnitudes are dif- To this point, the discussion of number representations has ferent from each other) depends on the ratio between the to-be- focused on nonsymbolic representations of number. This raises compared magnitudes.
Recommended publications
  • Systems Neuroscience of Mathematical Cognition and Learning
    CHAPTER 15 Systems Neuroscience of Mathematical Cognition and Learning: Basic Organization and Neural Sources of Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development Teresa Iuculano, Aarthi Padmanabhan, Vinod Menon Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States OUTLINE Introduction 288 Ventral and Dorsal Visual Streams: Neural Building Blocks of Mathematical Cognition 292 Basic Organization 292 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 295 Parietal-Frontal Systems: Short-Term and Working Memory 296 Basic Organization 296 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 299 Lateral Frontotemporal Cortices: Language-Mediated Systems 302 Basic Organization 302 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 302 Heterogeneity of Function in Numerical Cognition 287 https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-811529-9.00015-7 © 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 288 15. SYSTEMS NEUROSCIENCE OF MATHEMATICAL COGNITION AND LEARNING The Medial Temporal Lobe: Declarative Memory 306 Basic Organization 306 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 306 The Circuit View: Attention and Control Processes and Dynamic Circuits Orchestrating Mathematical Learning 310 Basic Organization 310 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 312 Plasticity in Multiple Brain Systems: Relation to Learning 314 Basic Organization 314 Heterogeneity in Typical and Atypical Development 315 Conclusions and Future Directions 320 References 324 INTRODUCTION Mathematical skill acquisition is hierarchical in nature, and each iteration of increased proficiency builds on knowledge of a lower-level primitive. For example, learning to solve arithmetical operations such as “3 + 4” requires first an understanding of what numbers mean and rep- resent (e.g., the symbol “3” refers to the quantity of three items, which derives from the ability to attend to discrete items in the environment).
    [Show full text]
  • True Numerical Cognition in the Wild Research-Article6868622017
    PSSXXX10.1177/0956797616686862Piantadosi, CantlonTrue Numerical Cognition in the Wild 686862research-article2017 Research Article Psychological Science 2017, Vol. 28(4) 462 –469 True Numerical Cognition in the Wild © The Author(s) 2017 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI:https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616686862 10.1177/0956797616686862 www.psychologicalscience.org/PS Steven T. Piantadosi and Jessica F. Cantlon Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester Abstract Cognitive and neural research over the past few decades has produced sophisticated models of the representations and algorithms underlying numerical reasoning in humans and other animals. These models make precise predictions for how humans and other animals should behave when faced with quantitative decisions, yet primarily have been tested only in laboratory tasks. We used data from wild baboons’ troop movements recently reported by Strandburg-Peshkin, Farine, Couzin, and Crofoot (2015) to compare a variety of models of quantitative decision making. We found that the decisions made by these naturally behaving wild animals rely specifically on numerical representations that have key homologies with the psychophysics of human number representations. These findings provide important new data on the types of problems human numerical cognition was designed to solve and constitute the first robust evidence of true numerical reasoning in wild animals. Keywords numerosity, evolution, numerical cognition, animal cognition Received 1/11/16; Revision accepted 12/8/16 Over the past half century, laboratory work on numerical who are able to follow a consensus group vote rather processing has advanced considerably in psychology, than an experienced leader or despot will face fewer fit- neuroscience, and comparative cognition.
    [Show full text]
  • The Development of Numerical Cognition in Children and Artificial
    The development of numerical cognition in children and artificial systems: a review of the current knowledge and proposals for multi-disciplinary research DI NUOVO, Alessandro <http://orcid.org/0000-0003-2677-2650> and JAY, Tim Available from Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive (SHURA) at: http://shura.shu.ac.uk/23767/ This document is the author deposited version. You are advised to consult the publisher's version if you wish to cite from it. Published version DI NUOVO, Alessandro and JAY, Tim (2019). The development of numerical cognition in children and artificial systems: a review of the current knowledge and proposals for multi-disciplinary research. Cognitive Computation and Systems. Copyright and re-use policy See http://shura.shu.ac.uk/information.html Sheffield Hallam University Research Archive http://shura.shu.ac.uk Cognitive Computation and Systems Review Article eISSN 2517-7567 Development of numerical cognition in Received on 28th September 2018 Revised 10th December 2018 children and artificial systems: a review of the Accepted on 2nd January 2019 doi: 10.1049/ccs.2018.0004 current knowledge and proposals for multi- www.ietdl.org disciplinary research Alessandro Di Nuovo1 , Tim Jay2 1Sheffield Robotics, Department of Computing, Sheffield Hallam University, Howard Street, Sheffield, UK 2Sheffield Institute of Education, Sheffield Hallam University, Howard Street, Sheffield, UK E-mail: [email protected] Abstract: Numerical cognition is a distinctive component of human intelligence such that the observation of its practice provides a window in high-level brain function. The modelling of numerical abilities in artificial cognitive systems can help to confirm existing child development hypotheses and define new ones by means of computational simulations.
    [Show full text]
  • Language-Specific Numerical Estimation in Bilingual Children
    BILINGUAL ESTIMATION 1 Language-specific numerical estimation in bilingual children Elisabeth Marchand1, Shirlene Wade2,3, Jessica Sullivan4, & David Barner1 1University of California, San Diego 2University of Rochester 3University of California, Berkeley 4Skidmore College CONTACT: Elisabeth Marchand Department of Psychology, UCSD 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla CA 92093 [email protected] BILINGUAL ESTIMATION 2 Abstract We tested 5- to 7-year-old bilingual learners of French and English (N = 91) to investigate how language-specific knowledge of verbal numerals affects numerical estimation. Participants made verbal estimates for rapidly presented random dot arrays in each of their two languages. Estimation accuracy differed across children’s two languages, an effect which remained when controlling for children’s familiarity with number words across their two languages. Also, children’s estimates were equivalently well ordered in their two languages, suggesting that differences in accuracy were due to how children represented the relative distance between number words in each language. Overall, these results suggest that bilingual children have different mappings between their verbal and non-verbal counting systems across their two languages and that those differences in mappings are likely driven by an asymmetry in their knowledge of the structure of the count list across their languages. Implications for bilingual math education are discussed. Keywords: Numerical cognition, number estimation, bilingualism, cognitive development BILINGUAL
    [Show full text]
  • A Look Behind Perceptual Performance in Numerical Cognition
    A look behind perceptual performance in numerical cognition Thesis by Dennis Reike Submitted to the Human Sciences Faculty for the Degree of Dr. phil. Submission: November 2017 University of Potsdam Supervisor: Prof. Dr. rer. nat. Wolfgang Schwarz Cosupervisor: Dr. habil. Eberhard Schröder Defense: 14. February 2018 1st reviewer: Prof. Dr. Wolfgang Schwarz 2nd reviewer: Prof. Dr. Anja Ischebeck Published online at the Institutional Repository of the University of Potsdam: URN urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus4-407821 http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:kobv:517-opus4-407821 iii This is to my beloved children Timo and Lucy. If you ever have the idea to read this you should know that the only reason to do this is that you are really interested in numerical cognition. iv Publication overview This thesis is based on following publications: Reike, D., & Schwarz, W. (2016). One model fits all: Explaining many aspects of number comparison within a single coherent model – A random walk account. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 1957- 1971. DOI: 10.1037/xlm0000287 Reike, D., & Schwarz, W. (2017). Exploring the origin of the number size con- gruency effect: Sensitivity or response bias? Attention, Perception, & Psy- chophysics, 79, 383-388. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-016-1267-4 Schwarz, W., & Reike, D. (2017). Local probability effects of repeating irrel- evant attributes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79, 230-242. DOI: 10.3758/s13414-016-1200-x v Contents 1 Introduction: Specific aspects of number processing 1 Number Comparison .............................. 5 The mental number line ............................ 7 A theory of magnitude ............................. 9 Numbers and the brain ............................
    [Show full text]
  • Two Systems of Non-Symbolic Numerical Cognition
    HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY ARTICLE published: 29 November 2011 HUMAN NEUROSCIENCE doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00150 Two systems of non-symbolic numerical cognition Daniel C. Hyde* Laboratory for Developmental Studies, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA Edited by: Studies of human adults, infants, and non-human animals demonstrate that non-symbolic Daniel Ansari, The University of numerical cognition is supported by at least two distinct cognitive systems: a “paral- Western Ontario, Canada lel individuation system” that encodes the numerical identity of individual items and an Reviewed by: Ian Mark Lyons, University of “approximate number system” that encodes the approximate numerical magnitude, or Chicago, USA numerosity, of a set. The exact nature and role of these systems, however, have been Angela Heine, Freie Universität Berlin, debated for over a 100-years. Some argue that the non-symbolic representation of small Germany numbers (<4) is carried out solely by the parallel individuation system and the non-symbolic *Correspondence: representation of large numbers (>4) is carried out solely by the approximate number sys- Daniel C. Hyde, Laboratory for Developmental Studies, Department tem. Others argue that all numbers are represented by the approximate number system. of Psychology, Harvard University, This debate has been fueled largely by some studies showing dissociations between small 1118 WJH, 33 Kirkland Street, and large number processing and other studies showing similar processing of small and Cambridge, MA 02138, USA. large numbers. Recent work has addressed this debate by showing that the two systems e-mail: [email protected] are present and distinct from early infancy, persist despite the acquisition of a symbolic number system, activate distinct cortical networks, and engage differentially based atten- tional constraints.
    [Show full text]
  • Title Effects of Attentional Shifts Along the Vertical Axis on Number
    Title Effects of attentional shifts along the vertical axis on number processing: an eye-tracking study with optokinetic stimulation Authors Arianna Felisatti1*, Mariagrazia Ranzini2*, Elvio Blini3, Matteo Lisi4, & Marco Zorzi3,5 *These authors contributed equally to this work and are listed in alphabetical order Affiliation 1. Department of Psychology, University of Potsdam, Germany 2. Department of Neuroscience, University of Padua, Italy 3. Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Italy 4. Department of Psychology, University of Essex, UK 5. IRCCS San Camillo Hospital, Venice-Lido, Italy Corresponding Author Prof. Marco Zorzi Department of General Psychology – University of Padua Via Venezia 8 35131 Padova (Italy) Email: [email protected] Word Count: 6415 (excluding Title, Tables titles, Abstract, Figures legends and References) Tables: 3 Figures: 4 Abstract (Word count = 252) Previous studies suggest that associations between numbers and space are mediated by shifts of visuospatial attention along the horizontal axis. In this study, we investigated the effect of vertical shifts of overt attention, induced by optokinetic stimulation (OKS) and monitored through eye-tracking, in two tasks requiring explicit (number comparison) or implicit (parity judgment) processing of number magnitude. Participants were exposed to black-and-white stripes (OKS) that moved vertically (upward or downward) or remained static (control condition). During the OKS, participants were asked to verbally classify auditory one-digit numbers as larger/smaller than 5 (comparison task; Exp. 1) or as odd/even (parity task; Exp. 2). OKS modulated response times in both experiments. In Exp.1, downward attentional displacement increased the Magnitude effect (slower responses for large numbers) and reduced the Distance effect (slower responses for numbers close to the reference).
    [Show full text]
  • Math, Monkeys, and the Developing Brain
    Math, monkeys, and the developing brain Jessica F. Cantlon1 Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627 Edited by Francisco J. Ayala, University of California, Irvine, CA, and approved May 10, 2012 (received for review February 28, 2012) Thirty thousand years ago, humans kept track of numerical quan- regions involved in analog numerical processing are related to the tities by carving slashes on fragments of bone. It took approximately development of math IQ (18). Taken together, current findings 25,000 y for the first iconic written numerals to emerge among implicate continuity in the primitive numerical abilities that are human cultures (e.g., Sumerian cuneiform). Now, children acquire shared by humans and nonhumans, as well as a degree of continuity the meanings of verbal counting words, Arabic numerals, written in human numerical abilities ranging from primitive approximation number words, and the procedures of basic arithmetic operations, to complex and sophisticated math. such as addition and subtraction, in just 6 y (between ages 2 and 8). What cognitive abilities enabled our ancestors to record tallies in the Oldest Numbers in the World first place? Additionally, what cognitive abilities allow children to The fact that humans have been recording tallies with sticks and rapidly acquire the formal mathematics knowledge that took our bones for 30,000 y is impressive, but the critical issue is this: what ancestors many millennia to invent? Current research aims to cognitive abilities enabled them to encode quantities in the first discover the origins and organization of numerical information in place? To identify the inherent constraints on humans’ ability to humans using clues from child development, the organization of the process numerical information, it is helpful to consider the evo- human brain, and animal cognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Arxiv:2108.13800V1 [Cs.SI] 31 Aug 2021 Cognitive Networks of Semantic/Emotional Associations (E.G
    Network psychometrics and cognitive network science open new ways for detecting, understanding and tackling the complexity of math anxiety: A review M. Stella CogNosco Lab, Department of Computer Science, University of Exeter, UK [email protected]. September 1, 2021 Abstract Math anxiety is a clinical pathology impairing cognitive processing in math-related contexts. Originally thought to affect only inexperienced, low-achieving students, recent investigations show how math anxiety is vastly diffused even among high-performing learners. This review of data-informed studies outlines math anxiety as a complex system that: (i) cripples well-being, self-confidence and information processing on both conscious and subconscious levels, (ii) can be transmitted by social interactions, like a pathogen, and worsened by distorted perceptions, (iii) affects roughly 20% of students in 63 out of 64 worldwide educational systems but correlates weakly with academic performance, and (iv) poses a concrete threat to students' well-being, computational literacy and career prospects in science. These patterns underline the crucial need to go beyond performance for estimating math anxiety. Recent advances with network psychometrics and cognitive network science provide ideal frameworks for detecting, interpreting and intervening upon such clinical condition. Merging education research, psychology and data science, the approaches reviewed here reconstruct psychological constructs as complex systems, represented either as multivariate correlation models (e.g. graph exploratory analysis) or as arXiv:2108.13800v1 [cs.SI] 31 Aug 2021 cognitive networks of semantic/emotional associations (e.g. free association networks or forma mentis networks). Not only can these interconnected networks detect otherwise hidden levels of math anxiety but - more crucially - they can unveil the specific layout of interacting factors, e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • The Complexity of Mental Integer Addition
    Journal of Numerical Cognition jnc.psychopen.eu | 2363-8761 Theoretical Contributions The Complexity of Mental Integer Addition Stefan Buijsman a, Markus Pantsar* b [a] Institutet för Framtidsstudier, Stockholm, Sweden. [b] Department of Philosophy, History and Art, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland. Abstract An important paradigm in modeling the complexity of mathematical tasks relies on computational complexity theory, in which complexity is measured through the resources (time, space) taken by a Turing machine to carry out the task. These complexity measures, however, are asymptotic and as such potentially a problematic fit when descriptively modeling mathematical tasks that involve small inputs. In this paper, we argue that empirical data on human arithmetical cognition implies that a more fine-grained complexity measure is needed to accurately study mental arithmetic tasks. We propose a computational model of mental integer addition that is sensitive to the relevant aspects of human arithmetical ability. We show that this model necessitates a two-part complexity measure, since the addition tasks consists of two qualitatively different stages: retrieval of addition facts and the (de)composition of multidigit numbers. Finally, we argue that the two-part complexity measure can be developed into a single response-time measure with the help of empirical study of the two stages. Keywords: integer addition, complexity, mental arithmetic, cognitive modeling Journal of Numerical Cognition, 2020, Vol. 6(1), 148–163, https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.v6i1.218 Received: 2019-04-05. Accepted: 2019-07-17. Published (VoR): 2020-06-15. *Corresponding author at: Department of Philosophy, History and Art, University of Helsinki, P.O.
    [Show full text]
  • Novice Versus Expert Algebraic Problem-Solving Strategies: an Eye Tracking Approach
    UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones 12-15-2018 Novice versus Expert Algebraic Problem-Solving Strategies: An Eye Tracking Approach Krystal Kamekona-Mendoza Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalscholarship.unlv.edu/thesesdissertations Part of the Cognitive Psychology Commons Repository Citation Kamekona-Mendoza, Krystal, "Novice versus Expert Algebraic Problem-Solving Strategies: An Eye Tracking Approach" (2018). UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones. 3501. http://dx.doi.org/10.34917/14279645 This Thesis is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It has been brought to you by Digital Scholarship@UNLV with permission from the rights-holder(s). You are free to use this Thesis in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/ or on the work itself. This Thesis has been accepted for inclusion in UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Digital Scholarship@UNLV. For more information, please contact [email protected]. NOVICE VERSUS EXPERT ALGEBRAIC PROBLEM-SOLVING STRATEGIES: AN EYE TRACKING APPROACH By Krystal Kamekona-Mendoza Bachelor of Arts – Psychology University of Nevada, Las Vegas 2012 A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of Arts – Psychology Department of Psychology College of Liberal Arts The Graduate College University of Nevada, Las Vegas December 2018 Thesis Approval The Graduate College The University of Nevada, Las Vegas November 9, 2018 This thesis prepared by Krystal Kamekona-Mendoza entitled Novice Versus Expert Algebraic Problem-Solving Strategies: An Eye Tracking Approach is approved in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts – Psychology Department of Psychology Mark Ashcraft, Ph.D.
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: EFFECTS of OBSERVED HEAD ORIENTATION on NUMERICAL
    Running head: EFFECTS OF OBSERVED HEAD ORIENTATION ON NUMERICAL COGNITION Low numbers from a low head? Effects of observed head orientation on numerical cognition Felix J. Götz, Anne Böckler, & Andreas B. Eder Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg, Germany Author Note The first author was supported by a scholarship funded by the Hanns Seidel Foundation and the Federal Ministry of Education and Research of Germany. We thank Morris Fischer, Nina Moll, and Paul M. Heineck for creating the photographs, Robin Willardt, Paul M. Heineck, and Martin Liskowsky, for their help with data collection, and Lisa Schenk for her help with creating the study materials of Experiment 2. Corresponding author: Felix J. Götz Department of Psychology Julius-Maximilians-Universität Würzburg Röntgenring 10 97070 Würzburg, Germany Phone: +49 (0) 931 3188603 E-mail: [email protected] EFFECTS OF OBSERVED HEAD ORIENTATION ON NUMERICAL COGNITION 2 Abstract The present research shows effects of observed vertical head orientation of another person on numerical cognition in the observer. Participants saw portrait-like photographs of persons from a frontal view with gaze being directed at the camera and the head being tilted up or down (vs. not tilted). The photograph appeared immediately before each trial in different numerical cognition tasks. In Experiment 1, participants produced smaller numbers in a Random Number Generation (RNG) task after having viewed persons with a down-tilted head orientation relative to up-tilted and non-tilted head orientations. In Experiment 2, numerical estimates in an anchoring-like trivia question task were smaller following presentations of persons with a down-tilted head orientation relative to a non-tilted head orientation.
    [Show full text]