Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate1

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate1 NeuroImage 15, 870–878 (2002) doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1037, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Thresholding of Statistical Maps in Functional Neuroimaging Using the False Discovery Rate1 Christopher R. Genovese,* Nicole A. Lazar,* and Thomas Nichols† *Department of Statistics, Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 15213; and †Department of Biostatistics, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109 Received February 23, 2001 When one uses theoretically motivated thresholds Finding objective and effective thresholds for voxel- for the individual tests, ignoring the fact that many wise statistics derived from neuroimaging data has tests are being performed, the probability that there been a long-standing problem. With at least one test will be false positives (voxels declared active when they performed for every voxel in an image, some correc- are really inactive) among all the tests becomes very tion of the thresholds is needed to control the error high. For example, for a one-sided t test with a 0.05 rates, but standard procedures for multiple hypothe- significance level, the threshold would be 1.645, which sis testing (e.g., Bonferroni) tend to not be sensitive would lead to approximately 1433 voxels declared ac- enough to be useful in this context. This paper intro- tive on average of the 28672 voxels in a 64 ϫ 64 ϫ 7 duces to the neuroscience literature statistical proce- image when there is no real activity. The 5% error rate dures for controlling the false discovery rate (FDR). thus leads to a very large number of false positives in Recent theoretical work in statistics suggests that FDR-controlling procedures will be effective for the absolute terms, especially relative to the typical num- analysis of neuroimaging data. These procedures op- ber of true positives. erate simultaneously on all voxelwise test statistics to The traditional way to deal with multiple testing is determine which tests should be considered statisti- to adjust thresholds such that Type I error is controlled cally significant. The innovation of the procedures is for all voxels in the brain, simultaneously. There are that they control the expected proportion of the re- two types of error control, weak and strong. Weak jected hypotheses that are falsely rejected. We demon- control requires that, when the null hypothesis is true strate this approach using both simulations and func- everywhere, the chance of rejecting one or more tests is tional magnetic resonance imaging data from two less than or equal to a specified level ␣. Strong control simple experiments. © 2002 Elsevier Science (USA) requires that, for any subset of voxels where the null Key Words: functional neuroimaging; false discovery hypothesis is true, the chance of rejecting one or more rate; multiple testing; Bonferroni correction. of the subset’s tests is less than or equal to ␣.As concisely stated by Holmes et al. (1996), “A test with strong control declares nonactivated voxels as acti- ␣ INTRODUCTION vated with probability at most . .” A significant result from a test procedure with weak control only A common approach to identifying active voxels in a implies there is an activation somewhere; a procedure neuroimaging data set is to perform voxelwise hypoth- with strong control allows individual voxels to be de- esis tests (after suitable preprocessing of the data) and clared active—it has localizing power. to threshold the resulting image of test statistics. At There is a variety of methods available for control- each voxel, a test statistic is computed from the data, ling the false-positive rate when performing multiple usually related to the null hypothesis of no difference tests. Among the methods, perhaps the most commonly between specified experimental conditions. The voxels used is the Bonferroni correction (see, for example, for which the test statistics exceed the threshold are Miller, 1981). If there are k tests being performed, the then classified as active, relative to the particular com- Bonferroni correction replaces the nominal significance parison being made. While this approach has proved level ␣ (e.g., 0.05) with the level ␣/k for each test. It can reasonably effective for a wide variety of testing meth- be shown that the Bonferroni correction has strong ods, a basic problem remains: choosing the threshold. control of Type I error. This is a conservative condition, and in practice with neuroimaging data, the Bonfer- roni correction has a tendency to wipe out both false 1 This work partially supported by NSF Grant SES 9866147. and true positives when applied to the entire data set. 1053-8119/02 $35.00 870 © 2002 Elsevier Science (USA) All rights reserved. CONTROLLING FDR IN NEUROIMAGING 871 To get useful results, it is necessary to use a more TABLE 1 complicated method or to reduce the number of tests Classifications of Voxels in V Simultaneous Tests considered simultaneously. For instance, one could identify regions of interest (ROI) and apply the correc- Declared active Declared inactive tion separately within each region. More involved (discovered) (not discovered) methods include random field approaches (such as Truly active V V T Worsley et al., 1996) or permutation based methods aa ai a Truly inactive Via Vii Ti (such as Holmes et al., 1996). The random field meth- Da Di V ods are suitable only for smoothed data and may re- quire assumptions that are very difficult to check; the permutation method makes few assumptions, but has an additional computational burden and does not ac- which controls the rate of false positives among all count for temporal autocorrelation easily. ROIs are tests whether or not the null is actually rejected. A labor intensive to create, and further, they must be procedure that controls the FDR bounds the expected created prior to data analysis and left unchanged rate of false positives among those tests that show a throughout, a rigid condition of which researchers are significant result. The procedures we describe operate understandably wary. simultaneously on all voxels in a specified part of the Variation across subjects has a critical impact on data (e.g., the entire data set) and identify in which of threshold selection in practice. It has frequently been those voxels the test is rejected. This implicitly corre- observed that, even with the same scanner and exper- sponds to a threshold selection method that adapts to imental paradigm, subjects vary in the degree of acti- the properties of the given data set. These methods vation they exhibit, in the sense of contrast-to-noise. work for any statistical testing procedure for which one Subjective selection of thresholds (set low enough that can generate a P value. FDR methods also offer an meaningful structure is observed, but high enough so objective way to select thresholds that is automatically that appreciable random structure is not evident) sug- adaptive across subjects. gests that different thresholds are appropriate for dif- An outline of the paper is as follows. In Section 2, we ferent subjects. Without an objective method for select- describe the FDR in more detail and present a family of ing these thresholds, however, the meaning of the FDR-controlling procedures that have been studied in statistical tests can be subverted by the researcher by the statistics literature. In Section 3, we present sim- adjusting the thresholds, implicitly or explicitly, to give ple simulations that illustrate the performance of the desirable results. Many researchers using neuroimag- FDR-controlling procedures. In Section 4, we apply the ing therefore tend to choose a single threshold consis- methods to two data sets, one describing a simple mo- tently for all data analyzed in an individual experi- tor task (Kinahan and Noll, 1999) and the other from a ment. This choice is usually based on what has “worked study of auditory stimulation. Finally, in Section 5, we well” in the past. For example, a t threshold of 6 and a discuss some of the practical issues in the use of FDR. P value of less than 0.001 are commonly used, though completely arbitrary, values for thresholding maps. THE FALSE DISCOVERY RATE This practice avoids biases from ad hoc threshold ad- justments, but its forced consistency can significantly In a typical functional magnetic resonance imaging reduce sensitivity (and waste data). (fMRI) data analysis, one computes, for each voxel of There have been a number of efforts to find an ob- interest, a test statistic that relates to the magnitude of jective and effective method for threshold determina- a particular contrast among experimental conditions. tion (Genovese et al., 1997; Worsley et al., 1996; A voxel is declared active if the corresponding test Holmes et al., 1996). While these methods are promis- statistic is sufficiently extreme with respect to the sta- ing, they all involve either extra computational effort tistic’s distribution under the null hypothesis. or extra data collection that may deter researchers Let V denote the total number of voxels being tested from using them. In this paper, we describe a recent in such an analysis. Each voxel can be classified into development in statistics that can be adapted to auto- one of four types, depending on whether or not the matic and implicit threshold selection in neuroimag- voxel is truly active and whether or not it is declared ing: procedures that control the false discovery rate active, as shown in Table 1. For example, Via denotes the number of false posi- (FDR) (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995; Benjamini and ϭ ϩ Liu, 1999; Benjamini and Yekutieli, 2001). tives and Da Vaa Via denotes the number of voxels Whenever one performs multiple tests, the FDR is declared active. In any data analysis, we only observe the proportion of false positives (incorrect rejections of Da, Di, and V; the remaining counts are unknown.
Recommended publications
  • Multiple Testing
    The p-value Familywise Error Rate Fisher's Method False Discovery Rate Chapter 8 Hypothesis Testing Multiple Testing 1 / 20 The p-value Familywise Error Rate Fisher's Method False Discovery Rate Outline The p-value Familywise Error Rate Fisher's Method Benjamini-Hochberg Procedure False Discovery Rate 2 / 20 The p-value Familywise Error Rate Fisher's Method False Discovery Rate The p-value In 2016, the American Statistical Association set for itself a task to make a statement on p-values. They note that it is all too easy to set a test, create a test statistic and compute a p-value. Proper statistical practice is much more than this and includes • appropriately chosen techniques based on a thorough understanding of the phenomena under study, • adequate visual and numerical summaries of the data, • properly conducted analyses whose logic and quantitative approaches are clearly explained, • correct interpretation of statistical results in context, and • reproducibility of results via a thorough reporting. Expressing a p-value is one of many approaches to summarize the results of a statistical investigation. 3 / 20 The p-value Familywise Error Rate Fisher's Method False Discovery Rate The p-value The statement's six principles, many of which address misconceptions and misuse of the p-value, are the following: 1. P-values can indicate how incompatible the data are with a specified statistical model. 2. P-values do not measure the probability that the studied hypothesis is true, or the probability that the data were produced by random chance alone. 3. Scientific conclusions and business or policy decisions should not be based only on whether a p-value passes a specific threshold.
    [Show full text]
  • Discussion: on Methods Controlling the False Discovery Rate
    Sankhy¯a: The Indian Journal of Statistics 2008, Volume 70, Part 2, pp. 169-176 c 2008, Indian Statistical Institute Discussion: On Methods Controlling the False Discovery Rate Joseph P. Romano Stanford University, USA Azeem M. Shaikh University of Chicago, USA Michael Wolf University of Zurich, Switzerland 1 Introduction It is a pleasure to acknowledge another insightful article by Sarkar. By developing clever expressions for the FDP, FDR, and FNR, he manages to prove fundamental properties of stepdown and stepup methods. It is particularly important that the theory is sufficiently developed so as to apply to what Sarkar calls adaptive BH methods. Here, the goal is to improve upon the Benjamini Hochberg procedure by incorporating a data-dependent estimate of the number of true null hypotheses. Theoretical justification of such methods is vital and Sarkar’s analysis is useful for this purpose. A perhaps more ambitious task is to develop methods which implicitly or explicitly estimate the joint dependence structure of the test statistics (or p-values). The focus of our discussion is to show how resampling methods can be used to construct stepdown procedures which control the FDR or other general measures of error control. The main benefit of using the boot- strap or subsampling is the ability to estimate the joint distribution of the test statistics, and thereby offer the potential of improving upon methods based on the marginal distributions of test statistics. The procedure below is a generalization of one we developed for FDR control, and the utility of the bootstrap is that it can apply to essentially arbitrary measures of error control, such as the pairwise FDR of Sarkar, the k-FWER, or the tail prob- abilities of the false discovery proportion.
    [Show full text]
  • Inference on the Limiting False Discovery Rate and the P-Value Threshold Parameter Assuming Weak Dependence Between Gene Expression Levels Within Subject
    Inference on the limiting false discovery rate and the p-value threshold parameter assuming weak dependence between gene expression levels within subject Glenn Heller1 and Jing Qin2 1Department of Epidemiology and Biostatistics Memorial Sloan-Kettering Cancer Center New York, NY 10021, USA 2Biostatistics Research Branch National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases Bethesda, Maryland 20892, USA Corresponding author: Glenn Heller email: [email protected] phone: 646 735 8112 fax: 646 735 0010 Running Head: FDR analysis with dependent data 1 Summary. An objective of microarray data analysis is to identify gene expressions that are associated with a disease related outcome. For each gene, a test statistic is computed to determine if an association exists, and this statistic generates a marginal p-value. In an effort to pool this information across genes, a p-value density function is derived. The p-value density is modeled as a mixture of a uniform (0,1) density and a scaled ratio of normal densities derived from the asymptotic normality of the test statistic. The p-values are assumed to be weakly dependent and a quasi-likelihood is used to estimate the parameters in the mixture density. The quasi-likelihood and the weak dependence assumption enables estimation and asymptotic inference on the false discovery rate for a given rejection region, and its inverse, the p-value threshold parameter for a fixed false discovery rate. A false discovery rate analysis on a local- ized prostate cancer data set is used to illustrate the methodology. Simulations are performed to assess the performance of this methodology. Keywords: Asymptotic normal test statistic, confidence interval, microarray, p-value mixture model, quasi-likelihood, weak dependence.
    [Show full text]
  • Heterocedasticity-Adjusted Ranking and Thresholding for Large-Scale Multiple Testing
    Heterocedasticity-Adjusted Ranking and Thresholding for Large-Scale Multiple Testing Luella Fu1, Bowen Gang2, Gareth M. James3, and Wenguang Sun3 Abstract Standardization has been a widely adopted practice in multiple testing, for it takes into account the variability in sampling and makes the test statistics comparable across different study units. However, despite conventional wisdom to the contrary, we show that there can be a significant loss in information from basing hypothesis tests on stan- dardized statistics rather than the full data. We develop a new class of heteroscedasticity{ adjusted ranking and thresholding (HART) rules that aim to improve existing methods by simultaneously exploiting commonalities and adjusting heterogeneities among the study units. The main idea of HART is to bypass standardization by directly incor- porating both the summary statistic and its variance into the testing procedure. A key message is that the variance structure of the alternative distribution, which is subsumed under standardized statistics, is highly informative and can be exploited to achieve higher power. The proposed HART procedure is shown to be asymptotically valid and optimal for false discovery rate (FDR) control. Our simulation results demon- strate that HART achieves substantial power gain over existing methods at the same FDR level. We illustrate the implementation through a microarray analysis of myeloma. Keywords: covariate-assisted inference; data processing and information loss; false dis- covery rate; heteroscedasticity; multiple testing with side information; structured multiple testing 1Department of Mathematics, San Francisco State University. 2Department of Statistics, Fudan University. 3Department of Data Sciences and Operations, University of Southern California. The research of Wen- guang Sun was supported in part by NSF grant DMS-1712983.
    [Show full text]
  • Comprehensive Comparative Analysis of Local False Discovery Rate Control Methods
    H OH metabolites OH Article Comprehensive Comparative Analysis of Local False Discovery Rate Control Methods Shin June Kim, Youngjae Oh and Jaesik Jeong * Department of Mathematics and Statistics, Chonnam National University, Gwangju 61186, Korea; [email protected] (S.J.K.); [email protected] (Y.O.) * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +82-062-530-3442 Abstract: Due to the advance in technology, the type of data is getting more complicated and large-scale. To analyze such complex data, more advanced technique is required. In case of omics data from two different groups, it is interesting to find significant biomarkers between two groups while controlling error rate such as false discovery rate (FDR). Over the last few decades, a lot of methods that control local false discovery rate have been developed, ranging from one-dimensional to k-dimensional FDR procedure. For comparison study, we select three of them, which have unique and significant properties: Efron’s approach, Ploner’s approach, and Kim’s approach in chronological order. The first approach is one-dimensional approach while the other two are two-dimensional ones. Furthermore, we consider two more variants of Ploner’s approach. We compare the performance of those methods on both simulated and real data. Keywords: biomarker; familywise error rate; false discovery rate; large scale inference 1. Introduction Due to the advent of advanced high-throughput technologies, a large amount of raw data have been produced and various methods that can appropriately preprocess Citation: Kim, S.J.; Oh, Y.; Jeong, J. such data have been developed. After various preprocessing steps, statistical methods Comprehensive Comparative are applied to the preprocessed, yet large-scale data.
    [Show full text]
  • Asymptotic Results on Adaptive False Discovery Rate Controlling Procedures Based on Kernel Estimators
    JournalofMachineLearningResearch14(2013)1423-1459 Submitted 4/08; Revised 8/12; Published 5/13 Asymptotic Results on Adaptive False Discovery Rate Controlling Procedures Based on Kernel Estimators Pierre Neuvial∗ [email protected] Laboratoire Statistique et Génome Université d’Évry Val d’Essonne UMR CNRS 8071 – USC INRA 23 boulevard de France 91 037 Évry, France Editor: Olivier Teytaud Abstract The False Discovery Rate (FDR) is a commonly used type I error rate in multiple testing problems. It is defined as the expected False Discovery Proportion (FDP), that is, the expected fraction of false positives among rejected hypotheses. When the hypotheses are independent, the Benjamini- Hochberg procedure achieves FDR control at any pre-specified level. By construction, FDR control offers no guarantee in terms of power, or type II error. A number of alternative procedures have been developed, including plug-in procedures that aim at gaining power by incorporating an estimate of the proportion of true null hypotheses. In this paper, we study the asymptotic behavior of a class of plug-in procedures based on kernel estimators of the density of the p-values, as the number m of tested hypotheses grows to infinity. In a setting where the hypotheses tested are independent, we prove that these procedures are asymptotically more powerful in two respects: (i) a tighter asymptotic FDR control for any target FDR level and (ii) a broader range of target levels yielding positive asymptotic power. We also show that this increased asymptotic power comes at the price of slower, non-parametric convergence rates k/(2k+1) for the FDP.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Why Is Multiple Testing a Problem? 2 the Bonferroni Correction
    Spring 2008 - Stat C141/ Bioeng C141 - Statistics for Bioinformatics Course Website: http://www.stat.berkeley.edu/users/hhuang/141C-2008.html Section Website: http://www.stat.berkeley.edu/users/mgoldman GSI Contact Info: Megan Goldman [email protected] Office Hours: 342 Evans M 10-11, Th 3-4, and by appointment 1 Why is multiple testing a problem? Say you have a set of hypotheses that you wish to test simultaneously. The first idea that might come to mind is to test each hypothesis separately, using some level of significance α. At first blush, this doesn't seem like a bad idea. However, consider a case where you have 20 hypotheses to test, and a significance level of 0.05. What's the probability of observing at least one significant result just due to chance? P(at least one significant result) = 1 − P(no significant results) = 1 − (1 − 0:05)20 ≈ 0:64 So, with 20 tests being considered, we have a 64% chance of observing at least one sig- nificant result, even if all of the tests are actually not significant. In genomics and other biology-related fields, it's not unusual for the number of simultaneous tests to be quite a bit larger than 20... and the probability of getting a significant result simply due to chance keeps going up. Methods for dealing with multiple testing frequently call for adjusting α in some way, so that the probability of observing at least one significant result due to chance remains below your desired significance level. 2 The Bonferroni correction The Bonferroni correction sets the significance cut-off at α=n.
    [Show full text]
  • False Discovery Rate 1 the Pairwise Multiple
    False Discovery Rate 1 The Pairwise Multiple Comparison Multiplicity Problem: An Alternative Approach to Familywise/Comparisonwise Type I Error Control by H. J. Keselman, Robert Cribbie University of Manitoba and Burt Holland Temple University False Discovery Rate 2 Abstract When simultaneously undertaking many tests of significance researchers are faced with the problem of how best to control the probability of committing a Type I error. The familywise approach deals directly with multiplicity problems by setting a level of significance for an entire set (family) of related hypotheses, while the comparison approach ignores the multiplicity issue by setting the rate of error on each individual contrast/test/hypothesis. A new formulation of control presented by Benjamini and Hochberg, their false discovery rate, does not provide as stringent control as the familywise rate, but concomitant with this relaxing in stringency is an increase in sensitivity to detect effects, compared to familywise control. Type I error and power rates for four relatively powerful and easy-to-compute pairwise multiple comparison procedures were compared to the Benjamni and Hochberg technique for various one-way layouts using test statistics that do not assume variance homogeneity. False Discovery Rate 3 The Pairwise Multiple Comparison Multiplicity Problem: An Alternative Approach to Familywise/Comparisonwise Type I Error Control The multiplicity problem in statistical inference refers to selecting the statistically significant findings from a large set of findings (tests) to support one's research hypotheses. Selecting the statistically significant findings from a larger pool of results that also contain nonsignificant findings is problematic since when multiple tests of significance are computed the probability that at least one will be significant by chance alone increases with the number of tests examined.
    [Show full text]
  • Size, Power and False Discovery Rates
    The Annals of Statistics 2007, Vol. 35, No. 4, 1351–1377 DOI: 10.1214/009053606000001460 c Institute of Mathematical Statistics, 2007 SIZE, POWER AND FALSE DISCOVERY RATES By Bradley Efron Stanford University Modern scientific technology has provided a new class of large- scale simultaneous inference problems, with thousands of hypothe- sis tests to consider at the same time. Microarrays epitomize this type of technology, but similar situations arise in proteomics, spec- troscopy, imaging, and social science surveys. This paper uses false discovery rate methods to carry out both size and power calculations on large-scale problems. A simple empirical Bayes approach allows the false discovery rate (fdr) analysis to proceed with a minimum of frequentist or Bayesian modeling assumptions. Closed-form accuracy formulas are derived for estimated false discovery rates, and used to compare different methodologies: local or tail-area fdr’s, theoretical, permutation, or empirical null hypothesis estimates. Two microarray data sets as well as simulations are used to evaluate the methodology, the power diagnostics showing why nonnull cases might easily fail to appear on a list of “significant” discoveries. 1. Introduction. Large-scale simultaneous hypothesis testing problems, with hundreds or thousands of cases considered together, have become a familiar feature of current-day statistical practice. Microarray methodology spearheaded the production of large-scale data sets, but other “high through- put” technologies are emerging, including time of flight spectroscopy, pro- teomic devices, flow cytometry and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Benjamini and Hochberg’s seminal paper [3] introduced false discovery rates (Fdr), a particularly useful new approach to simultaneous testing.
    [Show full text]
  • P and Q Values in RNA Seq the Q-Value Is an Adjusted P-Value, Taking in to Account the False Discovery Rate (FDR)
    P and q values in RNA Seq The q-value is an adjusted p-value, taking in to account the false discovery rate (FDR). Applying a FDR becomes necessary when we're measuring thousands of variables (e.g. gene expression levels) from a small sample set (e.g. a couple of individuals). A p-value of 0.05 implies that we are willing to accept that 5% of all tests will be false positives. An FDR-adjusted p-value (aka a q-value) of 0.05 implies that we are willing to accept that 5% of the tests found to be statistically significant (e.g. by p-value) will be false positives. Such an adjustment is necessary when we're making multiple tests on the same sample. See, for example, http://www.totallab.com/products/samespots/support/faq/pq-values.aspx. -HomeBrew- What are p-values? The object of differential 2D expression analysis is to find those spots which show expression difference between groups, thereby signifying that they may be involved in some biological process of interest to the researcher. Due to chance, there will always be some difference in expression between groups. However, it is the size of this difference in comparison to the variance (i.e. the range over which expression values fall) that will tell us if this expression difference is significant or not. Thus, if the difference is large but the variance is also large, then the difference may not be significant. On the other hand, a small difference coupled with a very small variance could be significant.
    [Show full text]
  • From P-Value to FDR
    From P-value To FDR Jie Yang, Ph.D. Associate Professor Department of Family, Population and Preventive Medicine Director Biostatistical Consulting Core In collaboration with Clinical Translational Science Center (CTSC) and the Biostatistics and Bioinformatics Shared Resource (BB-SR), Stony Brook Cancer Center (SBCC). OUTLINE P-values - What is p-value? - How important is a p-value? - Misinterpretation of p-values Multiple Testing Adjustment - Why, How, When? - Bonferroni: What and How? - FDR: What and How? STATISTICAL MODELS Statistical model is a mathematical representation of data variability, ideally catching all sources of such variability. All methods of statistical inference have assumptions about • How data were collected • How data were analyzed • How the analysis results were selected for presentation Assumptions are often simple to express mathematically, but difficult to satisfy and verify in practice. Hypothesis test is the predominant approach to statistical inference on effect sizes which describe the magnitude of a quantitative relationship between variables (such as standardized differences in means, odds ratios, correlations etc). BASIC STEPS IN HYPOTHESIS TEST 1. State null (H0) and alternative (H1) hypotheses 2. Choose a significance level, α (usually 0.05) 3. Based on the sample, calculate the test statistic and calculate p-value based on a theoretical distribution of the test statistic 4. Compare p-value with the significance level α 5. Make a decision, and state the conclusion HISTORY OF P-VALUES P-values have been in use for nearly a century. The p-value was first formally introduced by Karl Pearson, in his Pearson's chi-squared test and popularized by Ronald Fisher.
    [Show full text]
  • False Discovery Rate Control with Unknown Null Distribution: Is It Possible to Mimic the Oracle?
    False discovery rate control with unknown null distribution: is it possible to mimic the oracle? Etienne Roquain and Nicolas Verzelen Abstract: Classical multiple testing theory prescribes the null distribution, which is of- ten a too stringent assumption for nowadays large scale experiments. This paper presents theoretical foundations to understand the limitations caused by ignoring the null distri- bution, and how it can be properly learned from the (same) data-set, when possible. We explore this issue in the case where the null distributions are Gaussian with an unknown rescaling parameters (mean and variance) and the alternative distribution is let arbitrary. While an oracle procedure in that case is the Benjamini Hochberg procedure applied with the true (unknown) null distribution, we pursue the aim of building a procedure that asymptotically mimics the performance of the oracle (AMO in short). Our main result states that an AMO procedure exists if and only if the sparsity parameter k (number of false nulls) is of order less than n= log(n), where n is the total number of tests. Further sparsity boundaries are derived for general location models where the shape of the null distribution is not necessarily Gaussian. Given our impossibility results, we also pursue a weaker objective, which is to find a confidence region for the oracle. To this end, we develop a distribution-dependent confidence region for the null distribution. As practical by-products, this provides a goodness of fit test for the null distribution, as well as a visual method assessing the reliability of empirical null multiple testing methods. Our results are illustrated with numerical experiments and a companion vignette Roquain and Verzelen(2020).
    [Show full text]