Class 5: Faithfulness and Alternations (Part 2)

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Class 5: Faithfulness and Alternations (Part 2) Class 5: Faithfulness and alternations (part 2) Adam Albright ([email protected]) LSA 2017 Phonology University of Kentucky Announcements I Two resources posted in the ‘background’ I If you are taking this class for credit… I Option 1: assigment 1 feedback posted on Canvas, assignment 2 due next Monday by PDF on Canvas I Option 2: finding a topic for the short squib I Today: continue discussion of faithfulness constraints and alternations Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 1/40 Picking up from last time Which consonant deletes in CC cluster simplification? I Structural privilege I Onset C’s, word-initial C’s I Positional faithfulness = less likely to delete I Perceptual privilege I C/ V > C/ # > C/ C I Further to the left = better cued, less likely to delete I Continuing the argument for perceptual privilege I Jóola: C1 deletion even when both consonants are codas I Jóola: Deletion when both consonants are onsets I More generally: explaining divergent final CC# outcomes across languages Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 2/40 Shizuoka Japanese emphatic adjectives Adjective Emphatic Gloss Adjective Emphatic Gloss a. hade hande ‘showy’ m. kitanai kitːanai ‘dirty’ b. ozoi onzoi ‘terrible’ n. kusai kusːai ‘stinky’ c. jowai joɴwai ‘weak’ o. ikai ikːai ‘big’ d. hajai haɴjai ‘fast’ p. zonzai zoːnzai ‘impolite’ e. kaɾai kaɴɾai ‘spicy’ q. kandaɾui kaːndaɾui ‘languid’ f. nagai naŋgai ‘long’ r. onzokutai oːnzokutai ‘ugly’ g. kanaʃiː kanːaʃiː ‘sad’ s. supːai suːpːai ‘sour’ h. amai amːai ‘sweet’ t. okːanai oːkːanai ‘scary’ i. katai katːai ‘hard’ u. o͡ɪʃiː o͡ːɪʃiː ‘delicious’ j. osoi osːoi ‘slow’ v. kiːɾoi kiːɴɾoi ‘yellow’ k. takai takːai ‘high’ w. toːtoi toːtːoi respectable l. atsui atːsui ‘hot’ Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 3/40 Shizuoka Japanese emphatic adjectives I Three strategies for emphatic adjective formation I Lengthen consonant: kitanai ! kitːanai I Lengthen vowel: supːai ! suːpːai I Insert nasal C: hade ! hande I Choice of strategy is partially determined by general phonological restrictions I No voiced geminates: hade 6! hadːe I No ‘overlong’ consonants: supːai 6! supːːai I However, phonological restrictions underdetermine the choice I Why kitanai ! kitːanai, not *kiːtanai? I Why hade ! hande, not *haːde? I Priority: certain changes favored over others Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 4/40 OT is good at this! 1. Lengthen the consonant I Unless it’s already a geminate or it’s not a possible geminate, in which case… 2. Insert a nasal I Unless there’s already NC or Cː, in which case… 3. Lengthen the vowel These preferences follow from the ranking of the relevant markedness and faithfulness constraints. (Working this out is the purpose of assignment 2.) Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 5/40 Maybe too good… Another process we could easily have described 1. Lengthen the vowel I Unless it’s already long, in which case… 2. Insert a nasal I Unless there’s already NC or Cː, in which case… 3. Lengthen the consonant (In actuality this never proves necessary in native words, because there are no VːN syllables) Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 6/40 Shizuoka emphatic adjectives I Generalization: lowest sonority change that’s consistent with phonological requirements I obstruent lengthening > nasal > vowel lengthening I ‘Least conspicuous change’ I Is there anything significant about this? Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 7/40 The general question I Why do languages prefer one repair over another? I A prior question: how do we know that they do? I Alternations: compare morphologically related outputs (allomorphs) I German voicing alternations Sg. Pl. Gloss Sg. Pl. Gloss diːp diːbə ‘thief’ pʁɪntsiːp pʁɪntsiːpjən ‘principle’ liːt liːdɐ ‘song’ gəbiːt ɡəbiːtə ‘area’ kriːk kriːɡə ‘war’ blɪk blɪkə ‘glance’ braːf braːvə ‘good’ (adj.) ʃaːf ʃaːfə ‘sheep’ kraɪs kraɪzə ‘circle’ ɡlaɪs ɡlaɪsə ‘track’ I Contrast: stem-final obstruent voicing is unpredictable before vowels (contrastive) I Contextual neutralization: only voiceless word-finally I Loanword adaptation: compare source and loan form I Cantonese (Silverman, 1992; Yip, 1993) English Cantonese adaptation Gloss sæləd saː55løt35 salad kʰaːd kʰaːt55 card Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 8/40 The P-Map hypothesis (Steriade, 2001) Conjecture: faithfulness favors perceptually indistinct changes I Ident(big change) ≫ Ident(smaller change) I Max(well cued consonant) ≫ Max(poorly cued consonant) I Dep(well cued consonant) ≫ Dep(poorly cued consonant) NB: it’s not actually clear whether we’d expect a change with morphological function, such as emphatic adjective formation, to favor less perceptually salient or more perceptually salient change (Löfstedt, 2010). We’ll side-step this question in the discussion that follows by focusing on phonologically motivated changes. Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 9/40 Epenthesis Recall the problem posed by epenthesis: I We were interested in epenthetic consonants under the idea that they might tell us something about the relative markedness of different place features, since Ident doesn’t differentiate /aɪ/ Ident(place) *#V *Dors *Cor *Glottal a. aɪ *! W L b. kaɪ *! W L c. taɪ *! W L + d. ʔaɪ * I Our hopes for a cross-linguistically valid ranking of general place markedness constraints were dashed by discrepancies in inventories and contextual neutralizations I However, epenthetic segments do show a greater degree of consistency Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 10/40 Initial epenthesis German (Indo-European, Germany) /aʊf/ [ʔaʊf] ‘on’ /aɪn/ [ʔaɪn] ‘one’ /ɛkə/ [ˈʔɛkə] ‘corner’ /oft/ [ʔoft] ‘often’ /ideː/ [ʔiˈdeː] ‘idea’ I Epenthesis applies to all V-initial roots, even in the middle of a word, which can make it hard to determine whether it’s underlying or epenthetic ([ˈʃnaps.ʔiˌdeː] ‘stupid idea that you have when you’ve drunk too much schnapps’) I Epenthesis can depend on speech rate, position in phrase, and vowel quality (see Pompino-Marschall and Żygis 2010) I German also has [ʔ] epenthesis in VV sequences when V2 is stressed ([ka.ˈʔo.tiʃ] ‘chaotic’), but not stressless ([ˈka.os] ‘chaos’) I Cross-linguistically Blevins (2008): C epenthesis at phrase edges is usually [ʔ], occasionally [h] (more often finally than initially) Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 11/40 Favoring [ʔ] I P-Map approach: ∆(V∼ʔV) < ∆(V∼pV, ∆(V∼tV)), ∆(V∼kV), ∆(V∼hV), ∆(V∼jV), etc. I Stops at oral places introduce CV formant transitions, ʔ does not I Glide introduces distinct formant target, large CV transitions to vowels other than [i] I Among glottals, ʔ has fewer internal cues than h (i.e., none), so most like silence of word edge I Faithfulness hierarchy I Dep(oral stop)/# V ≫ Dep(h)/# V ≫ Dep(ʔ)/# V I Faithfulness constraints must care about quality of the segment and context, since both determine perceptual distance to Æ Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 12/40 Favoring [ʔ] [pa] [ha] [ta] [ja] [ka] [ʔa] Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 13/40 Intervocalic epenthesis Lou (Eastern Admiralty Islands branch of Malayo-Polynesian, Papua New Guinea; data from Blust 1998, cited in Blevins, 2008) I “Rising sonority” VV sequences repaired with epenthetic glides I Rising sonority = higher V + lower V /tia-n/ [tijan] ‘his/her abdomen’ /kea/ [keja] ‘swim’ /moloa-n/ [molowan] ‘his/her shadow/spirit’ /suep/ [suwep] ‘digging stick’ I “Falling sonority” VV sequences do not I Falling sonority = lower V + higher V /wei-n golom/ [weiŋgolom] ‘your saliva’ /mween/ [mwεεn] ‘man, male’ /kapeun/ [kaβeun] ‘bitter’ Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 14/40 Dep/V V I Between identical V’s: no change in formants, [ʔ] is predicted to be favored I Higher V + lower V I Intuitively: iV, uV transitions very similar to jV, wV I All have rising F1 I iV, jV: falling F2 (to varying degrees) I uV, wV: rising F2, F3 I Rankings reflect relative perceptual similarity I ≫ Dep(j)/Vi Vi Dep(ʔ)/Vi Vi (except maybe i i, though *ji may block) I Dep(ʔ)/i V ≫ Dep(j)/i V I Height of V1 I ∆(aV∼ajV) > ∆(eV∼ejV) > ∆(iV∼ijV) I Large excursion > small excursion I Asymmetrical implication: glide epenthesis in /ea/ implies epenthesis in /ia/ Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 15/40 Epenthesis in a few other contexts I NC, e.g., Latin I /suːm-s-iː/ ! [suːmpsiː] ‘take-prt-1sg’ I /suːm-t-us/ ! [suːmptus] ‘take-ppl’ I NL, e.g., in English I Historically: thimble I Synchronic variation: family [fæmli] ∼ [fæmbli] I ‘Shielding’: /VNV/ ! VndV, VdnV, VdndV I ‘Excrescent stops’: epenthesis favors a stop at same place of articulation as preceding nasal, and same voicing as following segment I Voiced stops are not the least marked segments in any global sense (i.e., can’t posit *ʔ ≫ *voiced stop) I But they are the most similar to nasals Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 16/40 The upshot I Epenthesis in different contexts motivated by different markedness constraints I Epenthetic C depends on the contexts I In all(?) cases, the C that is favored is the one that is most similar to the surrounding segments I Consistent with the P-Map hypothesis: faithfulness constraints penalizing perceptually more salient changes ≫ those penalizing less salient changes Perceptual asymmetries: The P-Map hypothesis References 17/40 Inverted deletions ‘Intrusive consonants’ I [r]: some dialects of British English, New England American English,
Recommended publications
  • Phonological Domains Within Blackfoot Towards a Family-Wide Comparison
    Phonological domains within Blackfoot Towards a family-wide comparison Natalie Weber 52nd algonquian conference yale university October 23, 2020 Outline 1. Background 2. Two phonological domains in Blackfoot verbs 3. Preverbs are not a separate phonological domain 4. Parametric variation 2 / 59 Background 3 / 59 Consonant inventory Labial Coronal Dorsal Glottal Stops p pː t tː k kː ʔ <’> Assibilants ts tːs ks Pre-assibilants ˢt ˢtː Fricatives s sː x <h> Nasals m mː n nː Glides w j <y> (w) Long consonants written with doubled letters. (Derrick and Weber n.d.; Weber 2020) 4 / 59 Predictable mid vowels? (Frantz 2017) Many [ɛː] and [ɔː] arise from coalescence across boundaries ◦ /a+i/ ! [ɛː] ◦ /a+o/ ! [ɔː] Vowel inventory front central back high i iː o oː mid ɛː <ai> ɔː <ao> low a aː (Derrick and Weber n.d.; Weber 2020) 5 / 59 Vowel inventory front central back high i iː o oː mid ɛː <ai> ɔː <ao> low a aː Predictable mid vowels? (Frantz 2017) Many [ɛː] and [ɔː] arise from coalescence across boundaries ◦ /a+i/ ! [ɛː] ◦ ! /a+o/ [ɔː] (Derrick and Weber n.d.; Weber 2020) 5 / 59 Contrastive mid vowels Some [ɛː] and [ɔː] are morpheme-internal, in overlapping environments with other long vowels JɔːníːtK JaːníːtK aoníít aaníít [ao–n/i–i]–t–Ø [aan–ii]–t–Ø [hole–by.needle/ti–ti1]–2sg.imp–imp [say–ai]–2sg.imp–imp ‘pierce it!’ ‘say (s.t.)!’ (Weber 2020) 6 / 59 Syntax within the stem Intransitive (bi-morphemic) vs. syntactically transitive (trimorphemic). Transitive V is object agreement (Quinn 2006; Rhodes 1994) p [ root –v0 –V0 ] Stem type Gloss ikinn –ssi AI ‘he is warm’ ikinn –ii II ‘it is warm’ itap –ip/i –thm TA ‘take him there’ itap –ip/ht –oo TI ‘take it there’ itap –ip/ht –aki AI(+O) ‘take (s.t.) there’ (Déchaine and Weber 2015, 2018; Weber 2020) 7 / 59 Syntax within the verbal complex Template p [ person–(preverb)*– [ –(med)–v–V ] –I0–C0 ] CP vP root vP CP ◦ Minimal verbal complex: stem plus suffixes (I0,C0).
    [Show full text]
  • Prosodic Identity in Copy Epenthesis Evidence for a Correspondence-Based Approach
    Nat Lang Linguist Theory DOI 10.1007/s11049-017-9385-9 Prosodic identity in copy epenthesis Evidence for a correspondence-based approach Juliet Stanton1 · Sam Zukoff2 Received: 31 October 2016 / Accepted: 23 June 2017 © Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2017 Abstract This paper focuses on languages that exhibit processes of copy epenthe- sis, specifically those where the similarity between a copy vowel and its host ex- tends to prosodic or suprasegmental resemblance. We argue that copy vowels and their hosts strive for identity in all prosodic properties, and show that this drive for prosodic identity can cause misapplication in the assignment of properties such as stress and length. To explain these effects, we argue that any successful analysis of copy epenthesis must involve a correspondence relation (following Kitto and de Lacy 1999). Our proposal successfully predicts the extant typology of prosodic identity ef- fects in copy epenthesis; alternative analyses of copy epenthesis relying solely on fea- tural spreading (e.g. Kawahara 2007) or gestural realignment (e.g. Hall 2003, 2006) do not naturally capture the effects discussed here. Keywords Copy epenthesis · Phonology · Correspondence · Misapplication · Prosody 1 Introduction The term copy epenthesis describes a class of patterns in which the quality of an epenthetic vowel depends on the quality of one of its vocalic neighbors. For exam- ple: in a language where underlying /pri/ is realized as [piri] but /pra/ as [para] (not *[pira]), the vowel that appears in the unexpected position (the copy) is featurally identical to the vowel that appears in the expected position (its host). B J.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Processes
    Phonological Processes Phonological processes are patterns of articulation that are developmentally appropriate in children learning to speak up until the ages listed below. PHONOLOGICAL PROCESS DESCRIPTION AGE ACQUIRED Initial Consonant Deletion Omitting first consonant (hat → at) Consonant Cluster Deletion Omitting both consonants of a consonant cluster (stop → op) 2 yrs. Reduplication Repeating syllables (water → wawa) Final Consonant Deletion Omitting a singleton consonant at the end of a word (nose → no) Unstressed Syllable Deletion Omitting a weak syllable (banana → nana) 3 yrs. Affrication Substituting an affricate for a nonaffricate (sheep → cheep) Stopping /f/ Substituting a stop for /f/ (fish → tish) Assimilation Changing a phoneme so it takes on a characteristic of another sound (bed → beb, yellow → lellow) 3 - 4 yrs. Velar Fronting Substituting a front sound for a back sound (cat → tat, gum → dum) Backing Substituting a back sound for a front sound (tap → cap) 4 - 5 yrs. Deaffrication Substituting an affricate with a continuant or stop (chip → sip) 4 yrs. Consonant Cluster Reduction (without /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (grape → gape) Depalatalization of Final Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the end of a word (dish → dit) 4 - 6 yrs. Stopping of /s/ Substituting a stop sound for /s/ (sap → tap) 3 ½ - 5 yrs. Depalatalization of Initial Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the beginning of a word (shy → ty) Consonant Cluster Reduction (with /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (step → tep) Alveolarization Substituting an alveolar for a nonalveolar sound (chew → too) 5 yrs.
    [Show full text]
  • UC Berkeley Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society
    UC Berkeley Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society Title Perception of Illegal Contrasts: Japanese Adaptations of Korean Coda Obstruents Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6x34v499 Journal Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, 36(36) ISSN 2377-1666 Author Whang, James D. Y. Publication Date 2016 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California PROCEEDINGS OF THE THIRTY SIXTH ANNUAL MEETING OF THE BERKELEY LINGUISTICS SOCIETY February 6-7, 2010 General Session Special Session Language Isolates and Orphans Parasession Writing Systems and Orthography Editors Nicholas Rolle Jeremy Steffman John Sylak-Glassman Berkeley Linguistics Society Berkeley, CA, USA Berkeley Linguistics Society University of California, Berkeley Department of Linguistics 1203 Dwinelle Hall Berkeley, CA 94720-2650 USA All papers copyright c 2016 by the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Inc. All rights reserved. ISSN: 0363-2946 LCCN: 76-640143 Contents Acknowledgments v Foreword vii Basque Genitive Case and Multiple Checking Xabier Artiagoitia . 1 Language Isolates and Their History, or, What's Weird, Anyway? Lyle Campbell . 16 Putting and Taking Events in Mandarin Chinese Jidong Chen . 32 Orthography Shapes Semantic and Phonological Activation in Reading Hui-Wen Cheng and Catherine L. Caldwell-Harris . 46 Writing in the World and Linguistics Peter T. Daniels . 61 When is Orthography Not Just Orthography? The Case of the Novgorod Birchbark Letters Andrew Dombrowski . 91 Gesture-to-Speech Mismatch in the Construction of Problem Solving Insight J.T.E. Elms . 101 Semantically-Oriented Vowel Reduction in an Amazonian Language Caleb Everett . 116 Universals in the Visual-Kinesthetic Modality: Politeness Marking Features in Japanese Sign Language (JSL) Johnny George .
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 5 Sound Change
    An articulatory theory of sound change An articulatory theory of sound change Hypothesis: Most common initial motivation for sound change is the automation of production. Tokens reduced online, are perceived as reduced and represented in the exemplar cluster as reduced. Therefore we expect sound changes to reflect a decrease in gestural magnitude and an increase in gestural overlap. What are some ways to test the articulatory model? The theory makes predictions about what is a possible sound change. These predictions could be tested on a cross-linguistic database. Sound changes that take place in the languages of the world are very similar (Blevins 2004, Bateman 2000, Hajek 1997, Greenberg et al. 1978). We should consider both common and rare changes and try to explain both. Common and rare changes might have different characteristics. Among the properties we could look for are types of phonetic motivation, types of lexical diffusion, gradualness, conditioning environment and resulting segments. Common vs. rare sound change? We need a database that allows us to test hypotheses concerning what types of changes are common and what types are not. A database of sound changes? Most sound changes have occurred in undocumented periods so that we have no record of them. Even in cases with written records, the phonetic interpretation may be unclear. Only a small number of languages have historic records. So any sample of known sound changes would be biased towards those languages. A database of sound changes? Sound changes are known only for some languages of the world: Languages with written histories. Sound changes can be reconstructed by comparing related languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Phonological Consonant Epenthesis Possible? a Series of Artificial Grammar Learning Experiments
    Is Phonological Consonant Epenthesis Possible? A Series of Artificial Grammar Learning Experiments Rebecca L. Morley Abstract Consonant epenthesis is typically assumed to be part of the basic repertoire of phonological gram- mars. This implies that there exists some set of linguistic data that entails epenthesis as the best analy- sis. However, a series of artificial grammar learning experiments found no evidence that learners ever selected an epenthesis analysis. Instead, phonetic and morphological biases were revealed, along with individual variation in how learners generalized and regularized their input. These results, in combi- nation with previous work, suggest that synchronic consonant epenthesis may only emerge very rarely, from a gradual accumulation of changes over time. It is argued that the theoretical status of epenthesis must be reconsidered in light of these results, and that investigation of the sufficient learning conditions, and the diachronic developments necessary to produce those conditions, are of central importance to synchronic theory generally. 1 Introduction Epenthesis is defined as insertion of a segment that has no correspondent in the relevant lexical, or un- derlying, form. There are various types of epenthesis that can be defined in terms of either the insertion environment, the features of the epenthesized segment, or both. The focus of this paper is on consonant epenthesis and, more specifically, default consonant epenthesis that results in markedness reduction (e.g. Prince and Smolensky (1993/2004)). Consonant
    [Show full text]
  • Epenthesis in Jerash Fallaahi Dialect: an Autosegmental Analysis
    International Journal of Liberal Arts and Social Science Vol. 7 No. 9 October 2019 Epenthesis in Jerash Fallaahi Dialect: An Autosegmental Analysis Dr. Muneera Mohammad Jaradat Jordan University of Science and Technology, Department of English Language and Linguistics Email: [email protected] Published: 26 October 2019 Copyright © Jaradat. Abstract: The study aims to investigate the phonological epenthesis processes which are manifested in Jerash Falla:ħi Dialect (JFD), a rural Jordanian dialect spoken in the north of Jordan by almost 130,000 people and has never been investigated before. The study uses a non-linear approach, namely, the autosegemental approach to analyze the phonological epenthesis processes. The data are collected by recording spontaneous conversations of twenty participants of Jerash Falla:ħi people who are native speakers of this dialect. The analysis shows that JFD displays two types of epenthesis, namely, vowel epenthesis and consonant epenthesis. Vowel epenthesis involves the insertion of the high short vowel /i/, or the high back vowel /u/, or the short low central vowel /a/ in certain cases within the word boundary or within an adjacent utterance; word-finally and word- medially. The second type is consonant epenthesis, which involves the insertion of the glottal stop /? / within the word boundary. In this type, the glottal stop can only occur word initially. Keywords: Jerash Fallahi Dialect, phonological processes, autosegmental analysis, vowel epenthesis, consonant epenthesis Cite this article: Jaradat, M.M. (2019). Epenthesis in Jerash Fallaahi Dialect: An Autosegmental Analysis. International Journal of Liberal Arts and Social Science, 7(9), 43-59. 43 International Journal of Liberal Arts and Social Science ISSN: 2307-924X www.ijlass.org 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Consonant Mutation and Reduplication in Blin Singulars and Plurals
    Consonant Mutation and Reduplication in Blin Singulars and Plurals Paul D. Fallon Howard University 1. Introduction Blin1, a Central Cushitic (Agaw) language of Eritrea, displays a complex series of consonant mutations between plural and singular forms, with several unusual properties. This paper describes several such properties, focusing on consonant mutation (or apophony), especially in relation to reduplication, using Correspondence Theory (McCarthy and Prince 1995) within the overall framework of Optimality Theory, drawing on data based on both published sources (e.g. Lamberti and Tonelli 1997) and the author's fieldwork in Eritrea. This paper describes the rare interaction between mutation and reduplication, and provides additional support for Mc Laughlin's (2000) analysis of mutation as the result of featural affixation (Akinlabi 1996, Zoll 1998) to the root node of a consonant. However, unlike Mc Laughlin's analysis, where mutation was stem-initial, in Blin such mutation is stem-final. One of the more unusual properties of Blin is that for phonological purposes, it is often easier to take the plural as the underlying form, since singulars and singulatives are morphologically marked. For example, /kr/ 'stones' has a singular /kr-a/. One common mutation is the lenition of velar stops: /lk/ 'fires', /lx-a/ (sg.). The loss of continuancy also results in the loss of ejection (/ak'/ → /ax-a/ 'cave (pl./sg.)', but not labialization /kin/ → /xin-a/ 'woman (pl./sg.)', /sak’/ → /sax-a/ 'fat (n.)(pl./sg.)'. In addition, velar lenition may occur word-medially, e.g. [bkl] → [bxl-a] 'mule (pl./sg.)'. More than one mutation process may also occur within the word: /dk’l/ → /dxar-a/ 'donkey (pl./sg.)'.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter XIII: After the French Invasion
    Chapter XIII: After the French Invasion • Introduction • 1066 Battle of Hastings (William the Conqueror) • The Norman Invasion established French as the language of England. • The Normans were originally Vikings (Norman= North + Man) • William established himself as King William I. • Redistributed the lands among his supporters. Ling 110 Chapter XIII: After the 1 French Partition Introduction • Consequently, Norman French became the prestige language in England (government, justice, & education). • English was spoken by only second class citizens. • This social situation had significant consequences for the English language- penetrated the lexicon and the grammar. • English lost the inflectional morphology characteristic of the Germanic languages. • The word order of English came to resemble French word order more than Germanic. Ling 110 Chapter XIII: After the 2 French Partition Introduction con’t • Social stratification can be observed when comparing native words and French borrowings from the same semantic domain. Example: • English French • pig pork • chicken poultry • cow beef • sheep mutton • calf veal The animal is English; the food is French. Illustrating who was tending the animals and who was eating them. Ling 110 Chapter XIII: After the 3 French Partition Introduction con’t • English has borrowed continuously from Latin. • Beginning in the Middle English period, from French. • This pattern of borrowing establishes the opportunity for English to borrow the same word at different points in its history. • Example: English borrowed humility from Latin. The word developed in French and was borrowed again into English as humble. • Two changes have occurred: – The i of humil has been deleted. – A b has been inserted between m and l.
    [Show full text]
  • Slouching Toward Optimality: Coda Reduction in OT-CC John J
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series Linguistics January 2007 Slouching toward optimality: Coda reduction in OT-CC John J. McCarthy University of Massachusetts, Amherst, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs Part of the Morphology Commons, Near Eastern Languages and Societies Commons, and the Phonetics and Phonology Commons Recommended Citation McCarthy, John J., "Slouching toward optimality: Coda reduction in OT-CC" (2007). Phonological Studies (Journal of the Phonological Society of Japan). 74. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.umass.edu/linguist_faculty_pubs/74 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Linguistics at ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Linguistics Department Faculty Publication Series by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Slouching Towards Optimality: Coda Reduction in OT-CC* John J. McCarthy University of Massachusetts Amherst Abstract. There is a well-established asymmetry in the behavior of medial consonant clusters: the first consonant in the cluster can undergo assimilation or deletion, but the second consonant in the cluster cannot. This article presents an explanation for that asymmetry based on a version of Optimality Theory with candidate chains (McCarthy (2006a)). The key idea is that a consonant can only assimilate or delete if it first loses its place features by debuccalizing, and debuccalization is only possible in coda position. Keywords: OT, coda, assimilation, candidate chain. 1. Introduction Medial consonant clusters are often simplified or assimilated: /patka/ → [paka], /pamka/ → [paŋka]. There is an interesting asymmetry in these processes: they always target the first member of the cluster for deletion or assimilation.
    [Show full text]
  • 87Th LSA Meeting Extended Abstract 2013 1 Learnability of Complex
    87th LSA Meeting Extended Abstract 2013 Learnability of complex phonological interactions: an artificial language learning experiment K. Michael Brooks*, Bozena Pajak†, Eric Baković* *University of California, San Diego; †University of Rochester 1. Introduction. Natural languages exhibit evidence that phonological processes interact with one another in complex ways: the application or applicability of one process has the potential to impact that of anoth- er. These interactions come in various forms, including simple interactions such as feeding, in which the application of one process enhances the applicability of another, and bleeding, in which the application of one process diminishes the applicability of another. A more complex example comes from Baković’s (2005) analysis of the alternations in the English past tense, as shown in (1). In this analysis, a vowel is epenthesized to avoid a sequence of identical or near-identical consonants, as in (1c) and (1d). Crucially, vowel epenthesis in the case of near-identical consonants in (1d) is driven by an interaction between voicing assimilation and avoidance of identical consonants: with no epenthesis in a form such as /weɪt/+/d/, the independent voicing assimilation process in (1b) would otherwise produce an illegal sequence of identical consonants (*[weɪtt]). (1) a. Basic form /wæɡ/ + /d/ → [wæɡd] ‘wagged’ b. Voicing assimilation /wɑk/ + /d/ → [wɑkt] ‘walked’ c. Epenthesis between identical Cs /weɪd/ + /d/ → [weɪdəd] ‘waded’ d. Interaction preventing identity /weɪt/ + /d/ → [weɪtəd] (*[weɪtt]) ‘waited’ As with any other aspect of a language, these interactions can only be learned from their manifesta- tions in the surface forms of the language. However, the way that they are learned remains a question.
    [Show full text]
  • Unification Phonology: Another Look at 'Synthesis-By-Rule'
    Unification Phonology: Another look at "synthesis-by-rule" John Coleman Experimental Phonetics Laboratory Department of Langm~ge and Linguistic Science University of York Heslington YORK YOI 5DD United Kingdom e-maih JANET%UK.AC.YORK.VAX::J SC 1 (1) p --, p-/s__ else p --, ph/__V Transformational grammars and "synthesis- (where V is any vowel symbol) by-rule" Most current text-to-speech systems else p --~ p' (e.g. Allen et al. 1987; Hertz 1981~ 1982, forthcoming; Hertz et aL 1985) are, at heart, unconstrained string- Often, of course, grammars made with rules of this based transformational grammars. Generally, text- type may be (contingently) quite restricted. For in.- to-speech programs are implemented as the compo- stance, if the rules apply in a fixed order without sition of three non-invertible mappings: cyclicity, they may be compiled into a finite-state transducer (Johnson 1972). But in general there is no guarantee that a program which implements 1. grapheme to phoneme mapping (inverse spelling such a grammar will halt. This would be pretty r ales + exceptions dictionary) disastrous in speech recognition, and is undesirable even in generation-based applications, such as text- 2. phoneme to allophone mapping (pronunciation to-speech. However, this has not prevented the at> rules) pearance of a number of "linguistic rule compilers" 3. allophone to parameter mapping (interpolation such as Van Leenwen's (1987, 1989) and Hertz's sys.- rules) gems. Tile basic operations of a transformational gram° ])'or example: mar -- deletion, insertion, permutation, and copying -- are apparently empirically instantiated by such ph] %, pit well-established phonological phenemona as elision, [p'] ~-- /p/ ~-- sip epenthesis, metathesis, assimilation and coarticula- [p-] e/ spit tion.
    [Show full text]