Primitive Man As Philosopher, by Paul Radin

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Primitive Man As Philosopher, by Paul Radin W j 64302 QSMANIA UNIVERSITY LIBRARY Arression No. Call Na 1 28/R1 2P 2??7? Author liarlir, Paul Title Primitive man as philosophy last inarK, 4 This book shoulJ be returned on or bctou: the date Primitive Man as Philosopher Dr. Radin here essays to describe the role of thought among primitive peoples the attitude of the thinkers among them towards life and society, the theories held of the soul, the human personality, the nature of the external world, etc. Dr, Radin knows how to let the aboriginal speak for himself, but illuminated always by the proximity of the highly sophisticated mind, so that the primitive appears always warmer, nearer to our time, and more accessible to our understanding. The material offered holds great fascination and is derived largely from the author's own first-hand investigations of the life of the Winnebago Indians. Primitive Man as Philosopher by Paul Radin, Ph.D. Research Fellow of Yale University and sometime Lecturer in Ethnology in Cambridge University; editor of "Crashing Thunder, the Autobiography of an American Indian" with a foreword by John Dewcy Professor of Philosophy in Columbia University New York and London D, Appleton and Company 1927 COPYRIGHT, 1927, D. APPLETON AND COMPANY PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA TO MY WIFE PREFACE When a modern historian desires to study the civilization of any people, he regards it as a necessary preliminary that he divest himself, so far as possible, of all prejudice and bias. He realizes that differences between cultures exist, but he does not feel that it is necessarily a sign of inferiority that a people differs in customs from his own. There seems, how- ever, to be a limit to what an historian treats as legitimate difference, a limit not always easy to determine. On the whole it may be said that he very naturally passes the same judgments that the majority of his fellow countrymen do. Hence, if some of the differences between admittedly civil- ized peoples often call forth unfavorable judgments or even provoke outbursts of horror, how much more must we expect this to be the case where the differences are of so funda- mental a nature as those separating us from people whom we have been accustomed to call uncivilized. The term "uncivilized" is a very vague one, and it is spread over a vast medley of peoples, some of whom have comparatively simple customs and others extremely com- plex ones. Indeed, there can be said to be but two charac- teristics possessed in common by all these peoples, the absence of a written language and the fact of original posses- sion of the soil when the various civilized European and Asiatic nations came into contact with them. But among all aboriginal races appeared a number of customs which undoubtedly seemed exceedingly strange to their European and Asiatic conquerors. Some of these customs they had never heard of; others they recognized as similar to observ- vli viii PREFACE ances and beliefs existing among the more backward mem- bers of their own communities. Yet the judgments civilized peoples have passed on the aborigines, we may be sure, were not initially based on any calm evaluation of facts. If the aborigines were regarded as innately inferior, this was due in part to the tremendous gulf in custom and belief separating them from the con- querors, in part to the apparent simplicity of their ways, and in no small degree to the fact that they were unable to offer any effective resistance. Romance soon threw its distorting screen over the whole primitive picture. Within one hundred years of the dis- covery of America it had already become an ineradicably established tradition that all the aborigines encountered by Europeans were simple, untutored savages from whom little more could be expected than from uncontrolled children, individuals who were at all times the slaves of their passions, of which the dominant one was hatred. Much of this tradi- tion, in various forms, disguised and otherwise, has persisted to the present day. The evolutionary theory, during its heyday in the iSyo's and 'So's, still further complicated and misrepresented the situation, and from the great classic that created modern ethnology Tylor's Primitive Culture, published in 1870 future ethnologists were to imbibe the cardinal and funda- mentally misleading doctrine that primitive peoples represent an early stage in the history of the evolution of culture. What was, perhaps, even more dangerous was the strange and uncritical manner in which all primitive peoples were lumped together in ethnological discussion simple Fuegians with the highly advanced Aztecs and Mayans, Bushmen with the peoples of the Nigerian coast, Australians with Poly- nesians, and so on. PREFACE ix For a number of years scholars were apparently content with the picture drawn by Tylor and his successors. The rebellion against it came in large measure from the Ameri- can ethnologists, chiefly under the influence of Prof. Franz Boas. Through methods of work foreshadowed by some of Boas' immediate predecessors but more penetratingly and systematically developed by him, a large body of authentic information has been gathered on the American Indians. Add to this the great monographs produced by European ethnologists for other parts of the world, and we have, for the first time in the history of the science, a moderately favorable opportunity for examining anew the nature of primitive man's mentality. That it is important to the ethnologist, the historian, the sociologist, and the psychologist to understand the true na- ture of the mentality of primitive man is, of course, self-evi- dent. But practical questions of far more general import are also involved. In Asia, Africa, the islands of the Pacific, Mexico, and South America there exist to-day millions of so- called primitive peoples whose relation to their white and Mongolian conquerors is of vital importance both to the conquerors and to the conquered, or semi-conquered, aborig- ines. Up to the present all attempts that have been made to understand them, or to come to any reasonable adjust- ment with them, have met with signal failure, and this failure is in most instances due to the scientifically accredited theories of the innate inferiority of primitive man in men- tality and capacity for civilization quite as much as to preju- dice and bias. Some governments, notably those of Great Britain and France, have already begun to recognize this fact. But how can we expect public officials to take an unbiased view of primitive mentality when the dominant tradition among ethnologists, among those who ostensibly x PREFACE devote their lives to the subject, is still largely based on an unjustified, or at least undemonstrated, assumption? The most pressing need in ethnology, then, is to examine anew the older assumptions which, wittingly or unwittingly, ethnologists themselves have harbored for two generations and which threaten to become fixed traditions among psy- chologists, sociologists, and historians. Among the more important of these assumptions is the notion that there is a dead level of intelligence among primitive peoples, that the individual is completely swamped by and submerged in the group, that thinkers and philosophers as such do not exist in short, that there is nothing even remotely comparable to an intellectual class among them. These conceptions of primitive mentality the writer re- gards as wholly unjustified, and it is with the object of contraverting them that the following pages have been writ- ten. Since the contrary view still holds the field among most scholars, including not a few ethnologists, I have been at considerable pains to collect my material in as irreproachable a manner as possible. The first requisite of all proof is, of course, that data be accurate and subject to control. To meet this requirement I have, in almost all cases, used only data that have been obtained at first hand and have been published in the original with a translation that could be vouched for. This, perhaps, may appear unduly pedantic, and it may possibly entail the omission of valuable material; but it cannot be too strongly emphasized that if the ethnologist wishes to obtain credence among the more skeptically inclined public, he must be willing to adopt a method of presentation that commends itself to them as meeting every critical test. A good deal of the data presented in the following chap- ters were obtained by the author himself. Perhaps I should PREFACE xi ask indulgence for using my own published and unpublished material so extensively. This is not due to any over-evalua- tion of either its accuracy or its importance, but simply to the fact that I happened to be interested specifically in a question which other ethnologists had touched only in pass- ing. In the interests of greater accuracy, and in order to substantiate statements that unsupported might seem in- credible to the reader accustomed to obtaining his descrip- tions of primitive culture from secondary works, native sources have been voluminously quoted. If certain tribes have been stressed more than others, it is simply because we happen to have for them better specific data of the kind de- sired. Yet it is my firm conviction that the conclusions arrived at on the basis of the study of a necessarily limited number of tribes will hold for practically every tribe. Perhaps it is not necessary to emphasize the dangers beset- ting the path of anyone venturing to describe and character- ize the ideas and mental workings of others, particularly those of races so different ostensibly from ourselves as are primitive peoples. Added to the ordinary risk of misunder- standing, ethnologists often find it necessary to give what are simply their own impressions and interpretations.
Recommended publications
  • Multiple Levels of Religious Meaning in Culture: a New Look at Winnebago Sacred Texts
    MULTIPLE LEVELS OF RELIGIOUS MEANING IN CULTURE: A NEW LOOK AT WINNEBAGO SACRED TEXTS LAWRENCE E. SULLIVAN Department of Religious Studies, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri, USA, 65211. ABSTRACT/RESUME Paul Radin wrote prolifically of Winnebago culture over a period of nearly fifty years. He made a distinction between magico-religious practice and systematic reflection, but provided such a wealth of detail on Winnebago beliefs that analysis has generally been limited by the precise, narrowly-defined boundaries of rigid academic disciplines. The author attempts a much broader, "humanist" approach to the subject, using the Winnebago Trickster as a vehicle. Au cours d'une période de pros de 50 ans, Paul Radin a beaucoup écrit sur la culture winnebago. Il l'a fair en établissant la distinction entre les pratiques de magie religieuse et la réflexion systématique chez ce peuple, mais il a apporté un tel luxe de détails à propos de leurs croyances que l'analyse qu'on en a faite, a, en général. été réduite par les limites étroites de rigides disciplines académiques. L'auteur de cette étude a tenté d'aborder le sujet sur un plan humaniste beaucoup plus large, utilisant comme véhicule le "décepteur" winne- bago, le médium entre le monde des esprits et celui des hommes. THE CANADIAN JOURNAL OF NATIVE STUDIES II, 2 (1982): 221-247 222 LAWRENCE E. SULLIVAN "The fact is that every writer creates his own precursors. His work modifies our conception of the past, as it will modify the future." Borges, Labyrinths PAUL RADIN We know so much about Winnebago life and thought through the writings of Paul Radin.
    [Show full text]
  • Paul Radin Papers, 1933-2000(Bulk 1934-1935)
    http://oac.cdlib.org/findaid/ark:/13030/kt858037zt No online items Finding Aid to the Paul Radin Papers, 1933-2000(bulk 1934-1935) Finding aid prepared by Jason Baxter and Wendy Kramer. San Francisco History Center, San Francisco Public Library 100 Larkin Street San Francisco, CA, 94102 (415) 557-4567 [email protected] 2010; revised 2019. Finding Aid to the Paul Radin SFH 23 1 Papers, 1933-2000(bulk 1934-1935) Title: Paul Radin Papers Date (inclusive): 1933-2000 Date (bulk): 1934-1935 Collection Identifier: SFH 23 Creator: Radin, Paul, 1883-1959 Physical Description: 4.0 boxes(4 cu. ft.) Contributing Institution: San Francisco History Center, San Francisco Public Library 100 Larkin Street San Francisco, CA, 94102 (415) 557-4567 [email protected] Abstract: Chiefly surveys from Radin's supervision of over 200 workers who interviewed ethnic groups in the San Francisco Bay Area for the State Emergency Relief Administration of California (SERA) over a period of nine months in 1934-1935. Known as SERA project 2-F2-98 (3-F2-145), its abstract was published in September 1935 as The Survey of San Francisco's Minorities: Its Purpose and Results. In addition to records from the WPA project, there is one folder of later correspondence from Jon Lee, a graduate of Oakland Technical High School whom Radin hired to collect and translate Chinese folklore, as well as a small amount of Mary Wolf's research materials on Radin, which includes Wolf's academic papers, a few of Radin's files, and some biographical information. The collection includes a series of index cards containing survey data on Italians in San Francisco, which was received as a separate accession but appears to be from the same SERA survey.
    [Show full text]
  • Finding Aid to the Robert Harry Lowie Papers, 1872-1968
    http://oac.cdlib.org/findaid/ark:/13030/tf1k400237 No online items Finding Aid to the Robert Harry Lowie Papers, 1872-1968 Processed by Marie E. Byrne and Lauren L. Lassleben The Bancroft Library. University of California, Berkeley Berkeley, California, 94720-6000 Phone: (510) 642-6481 Fax: (510) 642-7589 Email: [email protected] URL: http://bancroft.berkeley.edu © 1997 The Regents of the University of California. All rights reserved. Note History --History, University of California --History, UC BerkeleyGeographical (By Place) --University of California --University of California BerkeleySocial Sciences --EducationSocial Sciences --LinguisticsSocial Sciences --Area and Interdisciplinary Studies --Native American StudiesSocial Sciences --Anthropology Finding Aid to the Robert Harry BANC MSS C-B 927 1 Lowie Papers, 1872-1968 Finding Aid to the Robert Harry Lowie Papers, 1872-1968 Collection number: BANC MSS C-B 927 The Bancroft Library University of California, Berkeley Berkeley, California Contact Information: The Bancroft Library. University of California, Berkeley Berkeley, California, 94720-6000 Phone: (510) 642-6481 Fax: (510) 642-7589 Email: [email protected] URL: http://bancroft.berkeley.edu Processed by: Marie E. Byrne and Lauren L. Lassleben Date Completed: February 1994 Encoded by: James Ryan © 1997. The Regents of the University of California. All rights reserved. Collection Summary Collection Title: Robert Harry Lowie Papers, Date (inclusive): 1872-1968 Collection Number: BANC MSS C-B 927 Origination: Lowie, Robert Harry, 1883-1957 Extent: Number of containers: 16 boxes, 7 cartons, 1 oversize folder, 4 card file boxes, and 1 volumeLinear feet: Approximately 17.5 Repository: The Bancroft Library. Berkeley, California 94720-6000 Physical Location: For current information on the location of these materials, please consult the Library's online catalog.
    [Show full text]
  • Views of Our People in Regard to the Negro Problem,” He Told Booker T
    Books Genius at Work: How Franz Boas Created the Field of Cultural Anthropology By Charles King | Winter 2019-20 Boas reenacts a Kwakiutl ceremonial dance to assist Smithsonian Institution sculptors building a diorama. (Courtesy of Smithsonian Institution Archives) A century ago, when people believed that intelligence, empathy, and human potential were determined by race and gender, Franz Boas looked at the data and decided everyone was wrong. In this excerpt from the new book Gods of the Upper Air, Charles King profiles the maverick Columbia professor. After his appointment at Columbia, Boas’s connections with the American Museum of Natural History began to fade. He had a habit of making himself more respected than liked. His time at the museum had produced new research and exhibitions but also disappointments, professional disagreements, and hurt feelings among his colleagues, who found him confident to a fault, officious, and given to pique. When he formally resigned his curatorship in 1905, no one begged him to stay. The move to full-time work at the university gave Boas the opportunity to build his own team of researchers. “Neither Berlin with its five anthropological professorships, nor Paris with its anthropological school, nor Holland with its colonial school, could give a proper training to the observers whom we need,” he wrote to a colleague in 1901. He reorganized the department’s coursework to include training in linguistics and ethnology, not just the traditional anthropometry. “With archaeology represented,” he told the university’s president, Nicholas Murray Butler, “we should be able to train anthropologists in all directions.” Boas had decamped with Marie and the children to a rambling house across the Hudson River in Grantwood, New Jersey.
    [Show full text]
  • In Paul Radin's Letters to Edward Sapir Grant Arndt
    History of Anthropology Newsletter Volume 25 Article 4 Issue 1 June 1998 1-1-1998 Burckhardtian Culture History and the "Durkheim- Mauss Bug" in Paul Radin's Letters to Edward Sapir Grant Arndt Paul Radin This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. http://repository.upenn.edu/han/vol25/iss1/4 For more information, please contact [email protected]. HAN ON THE WEB: With a view to the future, we are establishing a HAN website at http://anthro.spc. uchicago.edulhan/ As of press time, there is nothing to be found there save our logo, but we do plan to develop the site over the next several months. The format and content categories are not yet definite, and we welcome suggestions as to the sorts of material that might be included. Although we do not plan to include current numbers of HAN as such, the site will probably offer selected material from past volumes, as well as '"between numbers" bibliography. FOOTNOTES FOR THE IDSTORY OF ANTHROPOLOGY Burckhardtian Culture History and the "Durkheim-Mauss Bug" in Paul Radin's Letters to Edward Sapir Grant Arndt University of Chicago Paul Radin was one of the most heterodox anthropologists among the first generation of Franz Boas' students. His primary theoretical works, Primitive Man as a Philosopher (1927) and Method and Theory in Ethnology (1933), reflect a characteristic interest in the role of individuals in '"primitive" cultures (a description he used with a degree of irony), and in a humanistic, rather than scientific form of ethnology. Two letters from Radin's correspondence with Edward Sapir provide insight into the development of his idiosyncratic approach to anthropology.
    [Show full text]
  • PAUL RADIN ANTHROPOLOGUE AMERICAIN Ancien Professeur À L’Université De Californie
    PAUL RADIN ANTHROPOLOGUE AMERICAIN Ancien professeur à l’Université de Californie Biographie Paul Radin (2 avril 1883 à Lodz, Pologne1 – 21 février 1959) était un anthropologue américain. Étudiant de Franz Boas à Columbia, il y étudia avec Edward Sapir et Robert Lowie entre autres. Il commença ses études de terrain par les indiens Winnebago en 1908. Ses ouvrages son nombreux, mais le plus célèbre fut The Trickster (1956). Il coécrivit aussi en lien avec ce sujet, Le Fripon divin: un mythe indien, qui comprend des textes des précurseurs de la Mythologie grecque de l'universitaire Karl Kerényi et des analyses du psychanalyste C.G. Jung. Né à Lodz en 1883, il est le fils du rabbin Adolf M. et de Johanna Theodor Radin, sa famille s'installe l'année suivante à Elmira (New York). Il obtient l'équivalent du baccalauréat au College of the City of New York en 1902, puis un doctorat en anthropologie à la Columbia University en 1911. Il conduit des études de terrain sur les Winnebago (Ho-Chunk) dans le Nebraska et le Wisconsin entre 1908 et 1912; les Ojibwé et les Outaouais en Ontario, Canada, de 1912 à 1917; les Huave et Zapotèques, à Oaxaca, Mexique de 1912 à 1913 et de 1927 à 1929; Wiyot et Yurok en Californie, de 1917 à 1918 puis en 1930; et les Outaouais au Michigan en 1925. Il supervise ensuite les travaux de ses étudiants sur les afro-américains à Nashville (Tennessee), de 1927 à 1928; et sur divers groupes ethniques (Chinois, Grecques, Irlandais, Italiens, et Japonais) à San Francisco, de 1937 jusque dans les années 1940.
    [Show full text]
  • History of Anthropology Newsletter
    History of Anthropology Newsletter Volume 25 Issue 1 June 1998 Article 1 January 1998 Volume 25, Issue 1 Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/han Part of the Anthropology Commons, and the History of Science, Technology, and Medicine Commons Recommended Citation (1998) "Volume 25, Issue 1," History of Anthropology Newsletter: Vol. 25 : Iss. 1 , Article 1. Available at: https://repository.upenn.edu/han/vol25/iss1/1 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/han/vol25/iss1/1 For more information, please contact [email protected]. H istory of A' nthropology N ewsletter XXV:l 1998 History of Anthropology Newsletter VOLUME XXV, NUMBER 1 June 1998 TABLE OF CONTENTS FOOTNOTES FOR THE IDSTORY OF ANTHROPOLOGY Burckhardtian Culture History and the "Durkheim-Mauss Bug"......... 3 SOURCES FOR THE IDSTORY OF ANTHROPOLOGY ................... 7 RESEARCH IN PROGRESS . 8 BIBLIOGRAPHICA ARCANA I. History of Anthropology in the Netherlands . 8 II. Recent Dissertations ...........•........................ 13 III. Recent Work by Subscribers . 14 IV. Suggested by Our Readers . 15 GLEANINGS FROM ACADEMIC GATHERINGS ...................... 20 The Editorial Committee Robert Bieder Regna Darnell Indiana University University of Western Ontario Curtis Hinsley Dell Hymes Northern Arizona University University of Virginia George W. Stocking William Sturtevant University of Chicago Smithsonian Institution Subscription rates (Each volume contains two numbers: June and December) Individual subscribers (North America) $5.00 Student subscribers 3.00 Institutional subscribers 7.00 Subscribers outside North America 7.00 Checks for renewals, new subscriptions or back numbers should be made payable (in United States dollars only) to: History of Anthropology Newsletter (or to HAN).
    [Show full text]
  • The Trickster Archetype: Tracing the Trickster Myths to Their Proto-Trickster Roots
    A Dissertation entitled The Trickster Archetype: Tracing the Trickster Myths to Their Proto-Trickster Roots by Gregory Keith Robinson Submitted to the Graduate Faculty as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy Degree in History ___________________________________ Dr. Alfred Cave, Committee Chair ___________________________________ Dr. Thomas Barden, Committee Member ___________________________________ Dr. Michael Jakobson, Committee Member ___________________________________ Dr. James Seelye, Committee Member ___________________________________ Dr. Patricia R. Komuniecki, Dean College of Graduate Studies The University of Toledo May 2011 Copyright 2011, Gregory K. Robinson This document is copyrighted material. Under copyright law, no parts of this document may be reproduced without the expressed permission of the author. An Abstract of The Trickster Archetype: Tracing the Trickster Myths to Their Proto-Trickster Roots by Gregory Keith Robinson Submitted to the Graduate Faculty as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctor of Philosophy Degree in History The University of Toledo May 2011 In modern Native American culture, a ―trickster‖ trope often appears in myths, oral traditions, recorded narratives, ceremonies, and historical accounts, although its purpose and role in society is ambiguous. To the First Peoples who owned the myths, these stories had educational, moral, and spiritual purposes that explained the natural phenomena of their environments, their histories and backgrounds, and their
    [Show full text]
  • Article RIP524.Pdf
    THE SOCIAL ORGANIZATION OF ETHNOLOGICAL THEORY by Leslie A. White Scicnce consists essentially of concepts and ways of dealing with concepts. These conccpts interact with onc another in the minds of individuals and in the social life of the scientific community. Science grows as new concepts are formcd in thc process of interaction between concepts and the external world, on the one hand, and interaction among concepts themselves, on the other. As concepts interact among themselves, new combinations and syn- theses-i.e., ncw concepts-are formcd. Scientific conccpts, as well as concepts in general, may be regarded as a distinct order of phenomena, resident in their human carriers. They diffuse from one people or rcgion to another, and thcy flow through time from one generation and age to another. The realm of scientific concepts may be considered as a process sui gcneris in which concepts beget concepts. Thus, concepts held by Galilco and laws formulated by Keplerl were synthesized in the nervous system of Newton to form laws of motion and gravitation. In October, 1838, Darwin tells us in his autobiographic sketch, concepts derived from Malthus' essay on population were integrated by him with other concepts that had come from many sources, past and contemporary. Up to that time Darwin had wrestled with the problem of new species with- out success. But whcn the Malthusian conccpt was integrated with others in his mind, "here then," he wrote, "I[ had at last got a theory by which to work" (Darwin, 1896, I, 68). Later, Mendelian concepts were added to Darwinism to form still further biological theory.
    [Show full text]
  • Robert Harry Lowie. a Biographical Memoir
    NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES R O B E R T H A R R Y L O W IE 1883—1957 A Biographical Memoir by J U L I A N H. S TE W ARD Any opinions expressed in this memoir are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Academy of Sciences. Biographical Memoir COPYRIGHT 1974 NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES WASHINGTON D.C. ROBERT HARRY LOWIE June 12, 1883-September 21, 1957 BY JULIAN H. STEWARD OBERT LOWIE was one of the key figures in the history of R anthropology. His professional years spanned the first five decades of the present century. He entered anthropology not long after Franz Boas had established it as an academic discipline and had removed it from the rather philosophical study of the nineteenth century and placed it on an empirical, scientific basis. Although Lowie was initially employed for a few years by the American Museum of Natural History, his true niche was as a university scholar where his influence reached an in- creasing number of students as well as those who read his large number of publications. Lowie's principal interests were in ethnological theory, in- cluding the history of such theory, and in social organization, especially kinship, marriage, the family, kinship terminology, men's and women's societies including age-grade societies, and political and social organization. He also made major contri- butions to the study of primitive religion and folklore. Lowie did not do original research on physical anthropology or archae- ology, which were little developed during his active years, and he did not have a major interest in language.
    [Show full text]