Conversation with Jeff Hawkins – on the Thalamus

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Conversation with Jeff Hawkins – on the Thalamus Episode 10: Conversation with Jeff Hawkins – On the Thalamus Jeff: 00:00 One of the things we knew is that sequences in the brain have to be able to do is you have to be able to speed them up, slow them down. Matt: 00:04 Right. Yeah. Jeff: 00:05 Like I can play a melody back faster. I can recognize it faster. I can speak faster and slower. So there's all these - Matt: 00:09 And it's not different patterns. It's the same pattern. Jeff: 00:11 Yeah. It's the same pattern. But you can speed up and slow it down and so, there had to be a mechanism for that in the brain and if you think about it, that mechanism has to apply not to one column in the cortex, it would apply to like all the columns that are doing, that are doing speech, something like that. Music playing Christy: 00:35 You’re listening to Numenta On Intelligence, our podcast series on how intelligence works in the brain and how to implement it in non-biological systems. In this episode, we’re going to take you back to where the podcast started – a conversation with our co-founder Jeff Hawkins. In fact, for the next few episodes, we’re going to try something a little different than the previous few episodes, which featured Interviews with Neuroscientists. Matt Taylor, our Open Source Community manager, will be bringing you conversations with Jeff Hawkins, or Subutai Ahmad, our VP of Research, on a variety of topics. These could include anything from Jeff’s latest research developments that haven’t yet been documented, to Subutai’s updates on applying our theories to today’s machine learning systems. These will be casual conversations, the kind that happen every day here at Numenta, and we want to share them with you, our listeners. Some of them may get pretty technical, but we’ll provide links to resources for further reading. I hope you enjoy these conversation from Numenta On Intelligence. Matt: 01:34 So I'm here with Jeff Hawkins in his office. It's early in the morning here at Numenta. There's just me and you and we're going to talk about some speculative thoughts and thinking you have about the thalamus, right? Jeff: 01:45 Yeah. Yeah. So, uh, we chosen, I've chosen, uh, to talk about the Thalamus now. I'll tell you what it is and why we think it's important. And then some new ideas we have about what it might be doing. Matt: 01:57 I think people will be interested cause we, we voted, we usually just talk about neocortex. So it's a little refreshing to go to somewhere else in the brain. Jeff: 02:04 Yeah. So let's just paint a picture in your mind. You know, the neocortex is, is like a big sheet, like a big napkin wrinkled up on top of your brain. And it's only a couple of millimeters thick. The thalamus, it's right in the center of the brain and it's two parts, like everything in the brain. And there's like maybe like the shape of two eggs, two small eggs. Matt: 02:22 Right. One on either end, either hemisphere. Right? Jeff: 02:24 So we, you know, that's everything in the brain has got two parts. We don't usually focus on that, but they're doing the same thing, just divided in half. So the thalamus is right in the middle and um, it has a very, uh, unique relationship to the neocortex. In fact, myself and some other people studying the thalamus really don't think you could separate them. Um, and the reason is, is that when information goes to the neocortex, from your eyes, your skin or your, or your ears, it always goes through the thalamus, always. Matt: 02:53 Sensory information... Jeff: 02:53 All sensory information, pretty much, pretty much almost any sensory information. And so the object nerve, it doesn't go from the back of the retina, it doesn't go straight to the cortex. It goes to the thalamus and the thalamus then goes to the cortex. Matt: 03:05 Right. Jeff: 03:06 And when two regions in the neocortex project to each other, you know, region A and region B or v1, v2, there's they primarily do that through the thalamus again. So v1 projects to the thalamus and then the thalamus predicts to v2 that there are direct connections between these cortical regions, but there's always one that goes through the thalamus. And since all information goes through the thalamus, it used to be thought that the thalamus is like the gateway to the cortex. Like when you get through this gateway, you're in. But now it's believed that everything that goes between cortical region, the cortical region goes through the Thalamus. And that way you could think of it as a part of the neocortex. I kind of think of it like an extra layer of cells or extra set of tissue that has been, that's been consolidated down the center of the brain. But it's almost, it's so intimately connected with the neocortex. You can't really separate them out. Matt: 03:58 But it is an older part of the brain, right? Jeff: 04:01 Ah, I don't know that's true actually, Matt. I, you know, it's in the sense that it's not neocortex. We often tend to think about everything that's not the neocortex is the older part of the brain. I don't know if the thalamus existed prior to the neocortex. I don't know that. Yeah. So, um, it probably did in some form, but really I think we, if you really want to understand how the neocortex works, ultimately you have to understand how the, what the thalamus is doing. Matt: 04:25 There are no mammals that we can find that just have a thalamus and no neocortex. Jeff: 04:29 No mammals, all mammals have a neocortex of the question is, or the non mammals that have a thalamus that don't have an neocortex. I don't know the answer that one, but we can, we can pretty much say right now the neocortex is intimate and required, and the thalamus, is required to have a neocortex. So you can't really separate the two out. Matt: 04:48 Well definitely the projections show that Jeff: 04:50 That's right. The projections show that, physically it's separated. But the projections are very intimate and um, and so we've always known that any theory of neocortex is going to have to explain what the thalamus is doing. Um, and the thalamus itself is not super complicated, but it's not super simple either. And I'm going to break it into two broad categories. There's one broad category we've just been talking about, which are called relay cells Matt: 05:16 Relay cells. Jeff: 05:17 Yeah, so like an axon comes from your eyeball and Matt: 05:20 Oh it's synapses in the thalamus... Jeff: 05:22 It makes a connection to one of these relay cells in the thalamus and the relay cells in the thalamus goes to the neocortex and it literally looks like one to one. Matt: 05:30 Wow. So just one, one stop and then Jeff: 05:32 That's right and not only one stop. It's, there's very little con-, there's no convergence in the thalamus. It's like it's literally like you took a long wire and you cut it and cut it in half and then connect it back together. Matt: 05:44 So why do it? Jeff: 05:44 That's right. Right. That's a good question. So the call of the relay is a little bit misleading because we know it must be, there's no reason you're not going to have this to do nothing. Right. But that's kind of what it looks like. They can say, this will show in many situations, it's one to one correspondence. A spike comes in and spike comes out and there's a, the topology of the arrangement of the cells in the retina are preserved in the Thalamus and they project to the cortex and they're preserved in the cortex. So, one thing is why are there relay cells? The second thing, there's a whole bunch of other cells and by the way it was a relay cells, a group of relay cells that go between the retina and V1 - that's called LGN lateral geniculate nucleus. That's just a bunch of cells in the thalamus and the separate ones that go between all the other regions. So these are dedicated cells for these units. Matt: 06:26 Right. They're localized to Jeff: 06:29 to whatever that projection is. There's another set of cells in the thalamus which are much more diffuse. They project to the cortex and they project very diffusely. So these cells, uh, they go by different names. I prefer the one that sometimes they're called matrix cells. I'm not referring to the movie, but referring to the, uh, the fact that they're sort of interstitial to these other, they're sort of in the matrix of the thalamus.
Recommended publications
  • Memory-Prediction Framework for Pattern
    Memory–Prediction Framework for Pattern Recognition: Performance and Suitability of the Bayesian Model of Visual Cortex Saulius J. Garalevicius Department of Computer and Information Sciences, Temple University Room 303, Wachman Hall, 1805 N. Broad St., Philadelphia, PA, 19122, USA [email protected] Abstract The neocortex learns sequences of patterns by storing This paper explores an inferential system for recognizing them in an invariant form in a hierarchical neural network. visual patterns. The system is inspired by a recent memory- It recalls the patterns auto-associatively when given only prediction theory and models the high-level architecture of partial or distorted inputs. The structure of stored invariant the human neocortex. The paper describes the hierarchical representations captures the important relationships in the architecture and recognition performance of this Bayesian world, independent of the details. The primary function of model. A number of possibilities are analyzed for bringing the neocortex is to make predictions by comparing the the model closer to the theory, making it uniform, scalable, knowledge of the invariant structure with the most recent less biased and able to learn a larger variety of images and observed details. their transformations. The effect of these modifications on The regions in the hierarchy are connected by multiple recognition accuracy is explored. We identify and discuss a number of both conceptual and practical challenges to the feedforward and feedback connections. Prediction requires Bayesian approach as well as missing details in the theory a comparison between what is happening (feedforward) that are needed to design a scalable and universal model. and what you expect to happen (feedback).
    [Show full text]
  • Unsupervised Anomaly Detection in Time Series with Recurrent Neural Networks
    DEGREE PROJECT IN TECHNOLOGY, FIRST CYCLE, 15 CREDITS STOCKHOLM, SWEDEN 2019 Unsupervised anomaly detection in time series with recurrent neural networks JOSEF HADDAD CARL PIEHL KTH ROYAL INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY SCHOOL OF ELECTRICAL ENGINEERING AND COMPUTER SCIENCE Unsupervised anomaly detection in time series with recurrent neural networks JOSEF HADDAD, CARL PIEHL Bachelor in Computer Science Date: June 7, 2019 Supervisor: Pawel Herman Examiner: Örjan Ekeberg School of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science Swedish title: Oövervakad avvikelsedetektion i tidsserier med neurala nätverk iii Abstract Artificial neural networks (ANN) have been successfully applied to a wide range of problems. However, most of the ANN-based models do not attempt to model the brain in detail, but there are still some models that do. An example of a biologically constrained ANN is Hierarchical Temporal Memory (HTM). This study applies HTM and Long Short-Term Memory (LSTM) to anomaly detection problems in time series in order to compare their performance for this task. The shape of the anomalies are restricted to point anomalies and the time series are univariate. Pre-existing implementations that utilise these networks for unsupervised anomaly detection in time series are used in this study. We primarily use our own synthetic data sets in order to discover the networks’ robustness to noise and how they compare to each other regarding different characteristics in the time series. Our results shows that both networks can handle noisy time series and the difference in performance regarding noise robustness is not significant for the time series used in the study. LSTM out- performs HTM in detecting point anomalies on our synthetic time series with sine curve trend but a conclusion about the overall best performing network among these two remains inconclusive.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuromorphic Architecture for the Hierarchical Temporal Memory Abdullah M
    IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON EMERGING TOPICS IN COMPUTATIONAL INTELLIGENCE 1 Neuromorphic Architecture for the Hierarchical Temporal Memory Abdullah M. Zyarah, Student Member, IEEE, Dhireesha Kudithipudi, Senior Member, IEEE, Neuromorphic AI Laboratory, Rochester Institute of Technology Abstract—A biomimetic machine intelligence algorithm, that recognition and classification [3]–[5], prediction [6], natural holds promise in creating invariant representations of spatiotem- language processing, and anomaly detection [7], [8]. At a poral input streams is the hierarchical temporal memory (HTM). higher abstraction, HTM is basically a memory based system This unsupervised online algorithm has been demonstrated on several machine-learning tasks, including anomaly detection. that can be trained on a sequence of events that vary over time. Significant effort has been made in formalizing and applying the In the algorithmic model, this is achieved using two core units, HTM algorithm to different classes of problems. There are few spatial pooler (SP) and temporal memory (TM), called cortical early explorations of the HTM hardware architecture, especially learning algorithm (CLA). The SP is responsible for trans- for the earlier version of the spatial pooler of HTM algorithm. forming the input data into sparse distributed representation In this article, we present a full-scale HTM architecture for both spatial pooler and temporal memory. Synthetic synapse design is (SDR) with fixed sparsity, whereas the TM learns sequences proposed to address the potential and dynamic interconnections and makes predictions [9]. occurring during learning. The architecture is interweaved with A few research groups have implemented the first generation parallel cells and columns that enable high processing speed for Bayesian HTM. Kenneth et al., in 2007, implemented the the HTM.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory-Prediction Framework for Pattern Recognition: Performance
    Memory–Prediction Framework for Pattern Recognition: Performance and Suitability of the Bayesian Model of Visual Cortex Saulius J. Garalevicius Department of Computer and Information Sciences, Temple University Room 303, Wachman Hall, 1805 N. Broad St., Philadelphia, PA, 19122, USA [email protected] Abstract The neocortex learns sequences of patterns by storing This paper explores an inferential system for recognizing them in an invariant form in a hierarchical neural network. visual patterns. The system is inspired by a recent memory- It recalls the patterns auto-associatively when given only prediction theory and models the high-level architecture of partial or distorted inputs. The structure of stored invariant the human neocortex. The paper describes the hierarchical representations captures the important relationships in the architecture and recognition performance of this Bayesian world, independent of the details. The primary function of model. A number of possibilities are analyzed for bringing the neocortex is to make predictions by comparing the the model closer to the theory, making it uniform, scalable, knowledge of the invariant structure with the most recent less biased and able to learn a larger variety of images and observed details. their transformations. The effect of these modifications on The regions in the hierarchy are connected by multiple recognition accuracy is explored. We identify and discuss a number of both conceptual and practical challenges to the feedforward and feedback connections. Prediction requires Bayesian approach as well as missing details in the theory a comparison between what is happening (feedforward) that are needed to design a scalable and universal model. and what you expect to happen (feedback).
    [Show full text]
  • The Neuroscience of Human Intelligence Differences
    Edinburgh Research Explorer The neuroscience of human intelligence differences Citation for published version: Deary, IJ, Penke, L & Johnson, W 2010, 'The neuroscience of human intelligence differences', Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 11, pp. 201-211. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2793 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.1038/nrn2793 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Nature Reviews Neuroscience Publisher Rights Statement: This is an author's accepted manuscript of the following article: Deary, I. J., Penke, L. & Johnson, W. (2010), "The neuroscience of human intelligence differences", in Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11, p. 201-211. The final publication is available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2793 General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 02. Oct. 2021 Nature Reviews Neuroscience in press The neuroscience of human intelligence differences Ian J. Deary*, Lars Penke* and Wendy Johnson* *Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH4 2EE, Scotland, UK. All authors contributed equally to the work.
    [Show full text]
  • On Intelligence As Memory
    Artificial Intelligence 169 (2005) 181–183 www.elsevier.com/locate/artint Book review Jeff Hawkins and Sandra Blakeslee, On Intelligence, Times Books, 2004. On intelligence as memory Jerome A. Feldman International Computer Science Institute, Berkeley, CA 94704-1198, USA Available online 3 November 2005 On Intelligence by Jeff Hawkins with Sandra Blakeslee has been inspirational for non- scientists as well as some of our most distinguished biologists, as can be seen from the web site (http://www.onintelligence.org). The book is engagingly written as a first person memoir of one computer engineer’s search for enlightenment on how human intelligence is computed by our brains. The central insight is important—much of our intelligence comes from the ability to recognize complex situations and to predict their possible outcomes. There is something fundamental about the brain and neural computation that makes us intelligent and AI should be studying it. Hawkins actually understates the power of human associative memory. Because of the massive parallelism and connectivity, the brain essentially reconfigures itself to be constantly sensitive to the current context and goals [1]. For example, when you are planning to buy some kind of car, you start noticing them. The book is surely right that better AI systems would follow if we could develop programs that were more like human memory. For whatever reason, memory as such is no longer studied much in AI—the Russell and Norvig [3] text has one index item for memory and that refers to semantics. From a scientific AI/Cognitive Science perspective, the book fails to tackle most of the questions of interest.
    [Show full text]
  • Mapping Mind-Brain Development: Towards a Comprehensive Theory
    Journal of Intelligence Review Mapping Mind-Brain Development: Towards a Comprehensive Theory George Spanoudis 1,* and Andreas Demetriou 2,3 1 Psychology Department, University of Cyprus, 1678 Nicosia, Cyprus 2 Department of Psychology, University of Nicosia, 1700 Nicosia, Cyprus; [email protected] 3 Cyprus Academy of Science, Letters, and Arts, 1700 Nicosia, Cyprus * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +357-22892969 Received: 20 January 2020; Accepted: 20 April 2020; Published: 26 April 2020 Abstract: The relations between the developing mind and developing brain are explored. We outline a theory of intellectual development postulating that the mind comprises four systems of processes (domain-specific, attention and working memory, reasoning, and cognizance) developing in four cycles (episodic, realistic, rule-based, and principle-based representations, emerging at birth, 2, 6, and 11 years, respectively), with two phases in each. Changes in reasoning relate to processing efficiency in the first phase and working memory in the second phase. Awareness of mental processes is recycled with the changes in each cycle and drives their integration into the representational unit of the next cycle. Brain research shows that each type of processes is served by specialized brain networks. Domain-specific processes are rooted in sensory cortices; working memory processes are mainly rooted in hippocampal, parietal, and prefrontal cortices; abstraction and alignment processes are rooted in parietal, frontal, and prefrontal and medial cortices. Information entering these networks is available to awareness processes. Brain networks change along the four cycles, in precision, connectivity, and brain rhythms. Principles of mind-brain interaction are discussed. Keywords: brain; mind; development; intelligence; mental and brain changes; brain networks 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Intelligence As a Developing Function: a Neuroconstructivist Approach
    Journal of Intelligence Article Intelligence as a Developing Function: A Neuroconstructivist Approach Luca Rinaldi 1,2,* and Annette Karmiloff-Smith 3 1 Department of Brain and Behavioural Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia 27100, Italy 2 Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milano 20126, Italy 3 Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, London WC1E 7HX, UK; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +39-02-6448-3775 Academic Editors: Andreas Demetriou and George Spanoudis Received: 23 December 2016; Accepted: 27 April 2017; Published: 29 April 2017 Abstract: The concept of intelligence encompasses the mental abilities necessary to survival and advancement in any environmental context. Attempts to grasp this multifaceted concept through a relatively simple operationalization have fostered the notion that individual differences in intelligence can often be expressed by a single score. This predominant position has contributed to expect intelligence profiles to remain substantially stable over the course of ontogenetic development and, more generally, across the life-span. These tendencies, however, are biased by the still limited number of empirical reports taking a developmental perspective on intelligence. Viewing intelligence as a dynamic concept, indeed, implies the need to identify full developmental trajectories, to assess how genes, brain, cognition, and environment interact with each other. In the present paper, we describe how a neuroconstructivist approach better explains why intelligence can rise or fall over development, as a result of a fluctuating interaction between the developing system itself and the environmental factors involved at different times across ontogenesis. Keywords: intelligence; individual differences; development; neuroconstructivism; emergent structure; developmental trajectory 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Conversation with Jeff Hawkins – on Defining Intelligence
    Episode 12: Conversation with Jeff Hawkins – On Defining Intelligence Matt: 00:00 Today on the Numenta On Intelligence podcast. Jeff: 00:04 You see something to believe the earth is flat and you give them all these facts that don't fit that model. Now, the Earth is flat predicts certain things. Right. And, and, and they will do their damnedest to try to fit these new facts into their model, no matter how, that's what they're going to, otherwise you have to throw the whole thing away and start over again, which is a really disconcerting thing for them. Matt: 00:26 It is. It is uncomfortable, to have to throw away a whole frame of reference. Jeff: 00:30 Basically, the models you build in the world are your reality. Matt: 00:34 It's your belief system. Jeff: 00:34 It's your belief system. It's your reality. This is what you know, that's the, oh, that's what it boils down to. There is no other reality in terms of your head. This is it. Instead of, if you say, well, you know what? Everything you believed these references, and you have to start over again. Um, then it's kind of like, oh, you're going back to square one. Matt: 00:49 Jeff Hawkins on defining intelligence. What is it? What makes us intelligent? How was our intelligence different from a mouse'? We're narrowing down the answers to some pretty existential questions. I'm Numenta community manager, Matt Taylor.
    [Show full text]
  • Hierarchical Temporal Memory (HTM)
    Hierarchical Temporal Memory (HTM) Computational Cognitive Neuroscience COSC 521, Spring 2019 Corey Johnson Resources for information on HTM Numenta.org HTM school NuPIC Jeff Hawkins 2004 book: On Intelligence: How a New Understanding of the Brain will Lead to the Creation of Truly Intelligent Machines (Note: some slide graphics are from Numenta.org, HTM school) 2 Hierarchical ● Higher levels → abstraction & permanence Temporal ● Change over time: patterns Memory ● Sparse Distributed Representation (SDR) 3 HTMs ● Biologically plausible model for intelligence ● Based on pyramidal neurons ● Neocortex micro-columns as building blocks "The neocortex comprises about 75% of the volume of the human brain and it is the seat of most of what we think of as intelligence." -Jeff Hawkins 4 HTMs continued ● Can learn / recall / infer high-order sequences ● Local learning rules, no global supervisor (HW) ● Relies on sparse distributed representation (SDR) ○ Fault tolerance ○ High capacity ● Learns by modeling the growth of new synapses 5 ANN Pyramidal HTM 6 HTM Layers (Lect. 2 slides) 7 Sparse Distributed Representation Fundamental to HTM systems SDR: 'Language of intelligence' Sparse as opposed to dense binary code, many bits are needed SDR bit similarity → similar semantic meaning Semantic error is key to generalization Cell only needs a few connections to neighbor to match pattern 8 Applications Good for: ● Data streams that change over time: text, GPS, dates, numbers ● Data with inherent structure ● System where many models are required rather than
    [Show full text]
  • From Neural Networks to Deep Learning: Zeroing in on the Human Brain
    From Neural Networks to Deep Learning: Zeroing in on the Human Brain Pondering the brain with the help of machine learning expert Andrew Ng and researcher- turned-author-turned-entrepreneur Jeff Hawkins. By Jonathan Laserson DOI: 10.1145/2000775.2000787 n the spring of 1958, David Hubble and Torsten Wiesel stood helpless in their lab at John Hopkins University. In front of them lay an anesthetized cat. In the cat’s skull, just above its visual cortex (an area in the back of the brain), there was a 3mm-wide hole through which electrodes recorded the response of several neurons. Hubble and Wiesel had Iconjectured that these neurons would fire when shown the right stimulus on the retina. If only they had any idea what that stimulus could be. For days they had shown the poor cat different shapes and shiny spots of light, testing different areas of the retina, but nothing elicited a response. In their book, Brain and the Visual Perception, Hubble and Wiesel give a personal account of this experience: The break came one long day in which example of what is now known as “orien- using the following experiment. They we held onto one cell for hour after hour. tation selective cells,” a type of cell which collected a bunch of natural images— To find a region of retina from which our is actually prevalent in the visual cortex. trees, lakes, leaves, and so on—and spots gave any hint of responses took These cells fire when they detect an edge extracted thousands of small image many hours, but we finally found a place oriented at a particular angle, in a specif- patches, 16x16 pixels each, from ran- that gave vague hints of responses.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Prospects for Building a Working Model of the Visual Cortex
    On the Prospects for Building a Working Model of the Visual Cortex Thomas Dean Glenn Carroll and Richard Washington Brown University, Providence, RI Google Inc., Mountain View, CA [email protected] {gcarroll,rwashington}@google.com Abstract new or recycled? How important is the role of Moore’s law in our pursuit of human-level perception? The full story Human visual capability has remained largely beyond the delves into the role of time, hierarchy, abstraction, complex- reach of engineered systems despite intensive study and con- siderable progress in problem understanding, algorithms and ity, symbolic reasoning, and unsupervised learning. It owes computing power. We posit that significant progress can be much to insights that have been discovered and forgotten at made by combining existing technologies from computer vi- least once every other generation for many decades. sion, ideas from theoretical neuroscience and the availability Since J.J. Gibson (1950) presented his theory of ecologi- of large-scale computing power for experimentation. From cal optics, scientists have followed his lead by trying to ex- a theoretical standpoint, our primary point of departure from plain perception in terms of the invariants that organisms current practice is our reliance on exploiting time in order to learn. Peter Foldi¨ ak´ (1991) and more recently Wiskott and turn an otherwise intractable unsupervised problem into a lo- Sejnowski (2002) suggest that we learn invariances from cally semi-supervised, and plausibly tractable, learning prob- lem. From a pragmatic perspective, our system architecture temporal input sequences by exploiting the fact that sensory follows what we know of cortical neuroanatomy and provides input tends to vary quickly while the environment we wish a solid foundation for scalable hierarchical inference.
    [Show full text]