On the Prospects for Building a Working Model of the Visual Cortex

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

On the Prospects for Building a Working Model of the Visual Cortex On the Prospects for Building a Working Model of the Visual Cortex Thomas Dean Glenn Carroll and Richard Washington Brown University, Providence, RI Google Inc., Mountain View, CA [email protected] {gcarroll,rwashington}@google.com Abstract new or recycled? How important is the role of Moore’s law in our pursuit of human-level perception? The full story Human visual capability has remained largely beyond the delves into the role of time, hierarchy, abstraction, complex- reach of engineered systems despite intensive study and con- siderable progress in problem understanding, algorithms and ity, symbolic reasoning, and unsupervised learning. It owes computing power. We posit that significant progress can be much to insights that have been discovered and forgotten at made by combining existing technologies from computer vi- least once every other generation for many decades. sion, ideas from theoretical neuroscience and the availability Since J.J. Gibson (1950) presented his theory of ecologi- of large-scale computing power for experimentation. From cal optics, scientists have followed his lead by trying to ex- a theoretical standpoint, our primary point of departure from plain perception in terms of the invariants that organisms current practice is our reliance on exploiting time in order to learn. Peter Foldi¨ ak´ (1991) and more recently Wiskott and turn an otherwise intractable unsupervised problem into a lo- Sejnowski (2002) suggest that we learn invariances from cally semi-supervised, and plausibly tractable, learning prob- lem. From a pragmatic perspective, our system architecture temporal input sequences by exploiting the fact that sensory follows what we know of cortical neuroanatomy and provides input tends to vary quickly while the environment we wish a solid foundation for scalable hierarchical inference. This to model changes gradually. Gestalt psychologists and psy- combination of features promises to provide a range of ro- chophysicists have long studied spatial and temporal group- bust object-recognition capabilities. ing of visual objects and the way in which the two operations interact (Kubovy & Gepshtein 2003), and there is certainly In July of 2005, one of us (Dean) presented a paper at ample evidence to suggest concrete algorithms. AAAI entitled “A Computational Model of the Cerebral The idea of hierarchy plays a central role in so many dis- Cortex” (Dean 2005). The paper described a graphical ciplines it is fruitless to trace its origins. Even as Hubel and model of the visual cortex inspired by David Mumford’s Weisel unraveled the first layers of the visual cortex, they computational architecture (1991; 1992; 2003). At that same couldn’t help but posit a hierarchy of representations of in- meeting, Jeff Hawkins gave an invited talk entitled “From creasing complexity (1968). However, direct evidence for AI Winter to AI Spring: Can a New Theory of Neocortex such hierarchical organization is slim to this day and ma- Lead to Truly Intelligent Machines?” drawing on the con- chine vision has yet to actually learn hierarchies of more tent of his popular book On Intelligence (2004). A month than a couple layers despite compelling arguments for their later at IJCAI, Geoff Hinton gave his Research Excellence utility (Ullman & Soloviev 1999). Award lecture entitled “What kind of a graphical model is Horace Barlow (1961) pointed out more than forty years the brain?” ago that strategies for coding visual information should In all three cases, the visual cortex is cast in terms of take advantage of the statistical regularities in natural im- a generative model in which ensembles of neurons are ages. This idea is the foundation for work on sparse rep- modeled as latent variables. All three of the speakers resentations in machine learning and computational neuro- were optimistic regarding the prospects for realizing useful, science (Olshausen & Field 1996). biologically-inspired systems. In the intervening two years, By the time information reaches the primary visual cortex we have learned a great deal about both the provenance and (V1), it has already gone through several stages of process- the practical value of those ideas. The history of the most ing in the retina and lateral geniculate. Following the lead important of these is both interesting and helpful in under- of Hubel and Wiesel, many scientists believe that the out- standing whether these ideas are likely to yield progress on put of V1 can be modeled as a tuned band-pass filter bank. some of the most significant challenges facing AI. The component features or basis for this representation are Are we poised to make a significant leap forward in un- called Gabor filters — mathematically, a Gabor filter is a derstanding computer and biological vision? If so, what are two-dimensional Gaussian modulated by a complex sinu- the main ideas that will fuel this leap forward and are they soid — and are tuned to respond to oriented dark bars against Copyright c 2007, Association for the Advancement of Artificial a light background (or, alternatively, light bars against a dark Intelligence (www.aaai.org). All rights reserved. background). The story of why scientists came to this con- 1597 clusion is fascinating, but the conclusion may have been pre- functions. Kearns and Valiant showed that the problem of mature; more recent work suggests Gabor filters account for learning polynomial-size circuits (in Valiant’s probably ap- only about 20% of the variance observed in the output of proximately correct learning model) is infeasible given plau- V1 (Olshausen & Field 2005). sible cryptographic limitations (1989). We’re sure that temporal and spatial invariants, hierarchy, However, if we are provided access to the inputs and out- levels of increasingly abstract features, unsupervised learn- puts of the circuit’s internal subconcepts, then the problem ing of image statistics and other core ideas that have been becomes tractable (Rivest & Sloan 1994). This implies that floating around for decades must be part of the answer, but if we had the “circuit diagram” for the visual cortex and machine vision still falls far short of human capability in could obtain labeled data, inputs and outputs, for each com- most respects. Are we on the threshold of a breakthrough ponent feature, robust machine vision might become feasi- and if so what will push us through the final barriers? ble. Instead of labels, we have knowledge — based on mil- lennia of experience summarized in our genes — enabling Temporal and Hierarchical Structure us to transform an otherwise intractable unsupervised learn- The primate cortex serves many functions and most scien- ing problem (one in which the training data is unlabeled) tists would agree that we’ve discovered only a small fraction into a tractable semi-supervised problem (one in which we of its secrets. Let’s suppose that our goal is to build a com- can assume that consecutive samples in time series are more putational model of the ventral visual pathway, the neural likely than not to have the same label). circuitry that appears to be largely responsible for recogniz- This property called temporal coherence is the basis for ing what objects are present in our visual field. A successful the optimism of several researchers that they can succeed model would, among other things, allow us to create a video where others have failed.1 If we’re going to make progress, search platform with the same quality and scope that Google temporal coherence has to provide some significant lever- and Yahoo! provide for web pages. Do we have the pieces age. It is important to realize, however, that exploiting tem- in place to succeed in the next two to five years? poral coherence does not completely eliminate complexity In many areas of science and engineering, time is so in- in learning hierarchical representations. Knowing that con- tegral to the description of the central problem that it can’t secutive samples are likely to have the same label is help- be ignored. Certainly this is the case in speech understand- ful, but we are still left with the task of segmenting the time ing, automated control, and most areas of signal process- series into subsequences having the same label, a problem ing. By contrast, in most areas of machine vision time has related to learning HMMs (Freund & Ron 1995). been considered a distraction, a complicating factor that we can safely ignore until we’ve figured out how to interpret Learning Invariant Features static images. The prevailing wisdom is that time will only make the problem of recognizing objects and understand- It’s hard to come up with a trick that nature hasn’t already ing scenes more difficult. A similar assumption has influ- discovered. And, while nature is reluctant to reveal its tricks, enced much of the work in computational neuroscience, but decades of machine vision researchers have come up with now that assumption is being challenged. There are a num- their own. Whether or not we have found neural analogs ber of proposals that suggest time is an essential ingredient for the most powerful of these, a pragmatic attitude dic- in explaining human perception (Foldi¨ ak´ 1991; Dean 2006; tates adapting and adopting them, where possible. David Hawkins & George 2006; Wiskott & Sejnowski 2002). The Lowe (Lowe 2004) has developed an effective algorithm common theme uniting these proposals is that the percep- for extracting image features called SIFT (for scale invari- tual sequences we experience provide essential cues that we ant feature transform). The algorithm involves searching in exploit to make critical discriminations. Consecutive sam- scale space for features in the form of small image patches ples in an audio recording or frames in a video sequence that can be reliably recovered in novel images. These fea- are likely to be examples of the same pattern undergoing tures are used like words in a dictionary to categorize im- changes in illumination, position, orientation, etc.
Recommended publications
  • Memory-Prediction Framework for Pattern
    Memory–Prediction Framework for Pattern Recognition: Performance and Suitability of the Bayesian Model of Visual Cortex Saulius J. Garalevicius Department of Computer and Information Sciences, Temple University Room 303, Wachman Hall, 1805 N. Broad St., Philadelphia, PA, 19122, USA [email protected] Abstract The neocortex learns sequences of patterns by storing This paper explores an inferential system for recognizing them in an invariant form in a hierarchical neural network. visual patterns. The system is inspired by a recent memory- It recalls the patterns auto-associatively when given only prediction theory and models the high-level architecture of partial or distorted inputs. The structure of stored invariant the human neocortex. The paper describes the hierarchical representations captures the important relationships in the architecture and recognition performance of this Bayesian world, independent of the details. The primary function of model. A number of possibilities are analyzed for bringing the neocortex is to make predictions by comparing the the model closer to the theory, making it uniform, scalable, knowledge of the invariant structure with the most recent less biased and able to learn a larger variety of images and observed details. their transformations. The effect of these modifications on The regions in the hierarchy are connected by multiple recognition accuracy is explored. We identify and discuss a number of both conceptual and practical challenges to the feedforward and feedback connections. Prediction requires Bayesian approach as well as missing details in the theory a comparison between what is happening (feedforward) that are needed to design a scalable and universal model. and what you expect to happen (feedback).
    [Show full text]
  • Unsupervised Anomaly Detection in Time Series with Recurrent Neural Networks
    DEGREE PROJECT IN TECHNOLOGY, FIRST CYCLE, 15 CREDITS STOCKHOLM, SWEDEN 2019 Unsupervised anomaly detection in time series with recurrent neural networks JOSEF HADDAD CARL PIEHL KTH ROYAL INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY SCHOOL OF ELECTRICAL ENGINEERING AND COMPUTER SCIENCE Unsupervised anomaly detection in time series with recurrent neural networks JOSEF HADDAD, CARL PIEHL Bachelor in Computer Science Date: June 7, 2019 Supervisor: Pawel Herman Examiner: Örjan Ekeberg School of Electrical Engineering and Computer Science Swedish title: Oövervakad avvikelsedetektion i tidsserier med neurala nätverk iii Abstract Artificial neural networks (ANN) have been successfully applied to a wide range of problems. However, most of the ANN-based models do not attempt to model the brain in detail, but there are still some models that do. An example of a biologically constrained ANN is Hierarchical Temporal Memory (HTM). This study applies HTM and Long Short-Term Memory (LSTM) to anomaly detection problems in time series in order to compare their performance for this task. The shape of the anomalies are restricted to point anomalies and the time series are univariate. Pre-existing implementations that utilise these networks for unsupervised anomaly detection in time series are used in this study. We primarily use our own synthetic data sets in order to discover the networks’ robustness to noise and how they compare to each other regarding different characteristics in the time series. Our results shows that both networks can handle noisy time series and the difference in performance regarding noise robustness is not significant for the time series used in the study. LSTM out- performs HTM in detecting point anomalies on our synthetic time series with sine curve trend but a conclusion about the overall best performing network among these two remains inconclusive.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuromorphic Architecture for the Hierarchical Temporal Memory Abdullah M
    IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON EMERGING TOPICS IN COMPUTATIONAL INTELLIGENCE 1 Neuromorphic Architecture for the Hierarchical Temporal Memory Abdullah M. Zyarah, Student Member, IEEE, Dhireesha Kudithipudi, Senior Member, IEEE, Neuromorphic AI Laboratory, Rochester Institute of Technology Abstract—A biomimetic machine intelligence algorithm, that recognition and classification [3]–[5], prediction [6], natural holds promise in creating invariant representations of spatiotem- language processing, and anomaly detection [7], [8]. At a poral input streams is the hierarchical temporal memory (HTM). higher abstraction, HTM is basically a memory based system This unsupervised online algorithm has been demonstrated on several machine-learning tasks, including anomaly detection. that can be trained on a sequence of events that vary over time. Significant effort has been made in formalizing and applying the In the algorithmic model, this is achieved using two core units, HTM algorithm to different classes of problems. There are few spatial pooler (SP) and temporal memory (TM), called cortical early explorations of the HTM hardware architecture, especially learning algorithm (CLA). The SP is responsible for trans- for the earlier version of the spatial pooler of HTM algorithm. forming the input data into sparse distributed representation In this article, we present a full-scale HTM architecture for both spatial pooler and temporal memory. Synthetic synapse design is (SDR) with fixed sparsity, whereas the TM learns sequences proposed to address the potential and dynamic interconnections and makes predictions [9]. occurring during learning. The architecture is interweaved with A few research groups have implemented the first generation parallel cells and columns that enable high processing speed for Bayesian HTM. Kenneth et al., in 2007, implemented the the HTM.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory-Prediction Framework for Pattern Recognition: Performance
    Memory–Prediction Framework for Pattern Recognition: Performance and Suitability of the Bayesian Model of Visual Cortex Saulius J. Garalevicius Department of Computer and Information Sciences, Temple University Room 303, Wachman Hall, 1805 N. Broad St., Philadelphia, PA, 19122, USA [email protected] Abstract The neocortex learns sequences of patterns by storing This paper explores an inferential system for recognizing them in an invariant form in a hierarchical neural network. visual patterns. The system is inspired by a recent memory- It recalls the patterns auto-associatively when given only prediction theory and models the high-level architecture of partial or distorted inputs. The structure of stored invariant the human neocortex. The paper describes the hierarchical representations captures the important relationships in the architecture and recognition performance of this Bayesian world, independent of the details. The primary function of model. A number of possibilities are analyzed for bringing the neocortex is to make predictions by comparing the the model closer to the theory, making it uniform, scalable, knowledge of the invariant structure with the most recent less biased and able to learn a larger variety of images and observed details. their transformations. The effect of these modifications on The regions in the hierarchy are connected by multiple recognition accuracy is explored. We identify and discuss a number of both conceptual and practical challenges to the feedforward and feedback connections. Prediction requires Bayesian approach as well as missing details in the theory a comparison between what is happening (feedforward) that are needed to design a scalable and universal model. and what you expect to happen (feedback).
    [Show full text]
  • The Neuroscience of Human Intelligence Differences
    Edinburgh Research Explorer The neuroscience of human intelligence differences Citation for published version: Deary, IJ, Penke, L & Johnson, W 2010, 'The neuroscience of human intelligence differences', Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol. 11, pp. 201-211. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2793 Digital Object Identifier (DOI): 10.1038/nrn2793 Link: Link to publication record in Edinburgh Research Explorer Document Version: Peer reviewed version Published In: Nature Reviews Neuroscience Publisher Rights Statement: This is an author's accepted manuscript of the following article: Deary, I. J., Penke, L. & Johnson, W. (2010), "The neuroscience of human intelligence differences", in Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11, p. 201-211. The final publication is available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2793 General rights Copyright for the publications made accessible via the Edinburgh Research Explorer is retained by the author(s) and / or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing these publications that users recognise and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. Take down policy The University of Edinburgh has made every reasonable effort to ensure that Edinburgh Research Explorer content complies with UK legislation. If you believe that the public display of this file breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Download date: 02. Oct. 2021 Nature Reviews Neuroscience in press The neuroscience of human intelligence differences Ian J. Deary*, Lars Penke* and Wendy Johnson* *Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh EH4 2EE, Scotland, UK. All authors contributed equally to the work.
    [Show full text]
  • On Intelligence As Memory
    Artificial Intelligence 169 (2005) 181–183 www.elsevier.com/locate/artint Book review Jeff Hawkins and Sandra Blakeslee, On Intelligence, Times Books, 2004. On intelligence as memory Jerome A. Feldman International Computer Science Institute, Berkeley, CA 94704-1198, USA Available online 3 November 2005 On Intelligence by Jeff Hawkins with Sandra Blakeslee has been inspirational for non- scientists as well as some of our most distinguished biologists, as can be seen from the web site (http://www.onintelligence.org). The book is engagingly written as a first person memoir of one computer engineer’s search for enlightenment on how human intelligence is computed by our brains. The central insight is important—much of our intelligence comes from the ability to recognize complex situations and to predict their possible outcomes. There is something fundamental about the brain and neural computation that makes us intelligent and AI should be studying it. Hawkins actually understates the power of human associative memory. Because of the massive parallelism and connectivity, the brain essentially reconfigures itself to be constantly sensitive to the current context and goals [1]. For example, when you are planning to buy some kind of car, you start noticing them. The book is surely right that better AI systems would follow if we could develop programs that were more like human memory. For whatever reason, memory as such is no longer studied much in AI—the Russell and Norvig [3] text has one index item for memory and that refers to semantics. From a scientific AI/Cognitive Science perspective, the book fails to tackle most of the questions of interest.
    [Show full text]
  • Mapping Mind-Brain Development: Towards a Comprehensive Theory
    Journal of Intelligence Review Mapping Mind-Brain Development: Towards a Comprehensive Theory George Spanoudis 1,* and Andreas Demetriou 2,3 1 Psychology Department, University of Cyprus, 1678 Nicosia, Cyprus 2 Department of Psychology, University of Nicosia, 1700 Nicosia, Cyprus; [email protected] 3 Cyprus Academy of Science, Letters, and Arts, 1700 Nicosia, Cyprus * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +357-22892969 Received: 20 January 2020; Accepted: 20 April 2020; Published: 26 April 2020 Abstract: The relations between the developing mind and developing brain are explored. We outline a theory of intellectual development postulating that the mind comprises four systems of processes (domain-specific, attention and working memory, reasoning, and cognizance) developing in four cycles (episodic, realistic, rule-based, and principle-based representations, emerging at birth, 2, 6, and 11 years, respectively), with two phases in each. Changes in reasoning relate to processing efficiency in the first phase and working memory in the second phase. Awareness of mental processes is recycled with the changes in each cycle and drives their integration into the representational unit of the next cycle. Brain research shows that each type of processes is served by specialized brain networks. Domain-specific processes are rooted in sensory cortices; working memory processes are mainly rooted in hippocampal, parietal, and prefrontal cortices; abstraction and alignment processes are rooted in parietal, frontal, and prefrontal and medial cortices. Information entering these networks is available to awareness processes. Brain networks change along the four cycles, in precision, connectivity, and brain rhythms. Principles of mind-brain interaction are discussed. Keywords: brain; mind; development; intelligence; mental and brain changes; brain networks 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Intelligence As a Developing Function: a Neuroconstructivist Approach
    Journal of Intelligence Article Intelligence as a Developing Function: A Neuroconstructivist Approach Luca Rinaldi 1,2,* and Annette Karmiloff-Smith 3 1 Department of Brain and Behavioural Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia 27100, Italy 2 Milan Center for Neuroscience, Milano 20126, Italy 3 Centre for Brain and Cognitive Development, Birkbeck, London WC1E 7HX, UK; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +39-02-6448-3775 Academic Editors: Andreas Demetriou and George Spanoudis Received: 23 December 2016; Accepted: 27 April 2017; Published: 29 April 2017 Abstract: The concept of intelligence encompasses the mental abilities necessary to survival and advancement in any environmental context. Attempts to grasp this multifaceted concept through a relatively simple operationalization have fostered the notion that individual differences in intelligence can often be expressed by a single score. This predominant position has contributed to expect intelligence profiles to remain substantially stable over the course of ontogenetic development and, more generally, across the life-span. These tendencies, however, are biased by the still limited number of empirical reports taking a developmental perspective on intelligence. Viewing intelligence as a dynamic concept, indeed, implies the need to identify full developmental trajectories, to assess how genes, brain, cognition, and environment interact with each other. In the present paper, we describe how a neuroconstructivist approach better explains why intelligence can rise or fall over development, as a result of a fluctuating interaction between the developing system itself and the environmental factors involved at different times across ontogenesis. Keywords: intelligence; individual differences; development; neuroconstructivism; emergent structure; developmental trajectory 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Conversation with Jeff Hawkins – on Defining Intelligence
    Episode 12: Conversation with Jeff Hawkins – On Defining Intelligence Matt: 00:00 Today on the Numenta On Intelligence podcast. Jeff: 00:04 You see something to believe the earth is flat and you give them all these facts that don't fit that model. Now, the Earth is flat predicts certain things. Right. And, and, and they will do their damnedest to try to fit these new facts into their model, no matter how, that's what they're going to, otherwise you have to throw the whole thing away and start over again, which is a really disconcerting thing for them. Matt: 00:26 It is. It is uncomfortable, to have to throw away a whole frame of reference. Jeff: 00:30 Basically, the models you build in the world are your reality. Matt: 00:34 It's your belief system. Jeff: 00:34 It's your belief system. It's your reality. This is what you know, that's the, oh, that's what it boils down to. There is no other reality in terms of your head. This is it. Instead of, if you say, well, you know what? Everything you believed these references, and you have to start over again. Um, then it's kind of like, oh, you're going back to square one. Matt: 00:49 Jeff Hawkins on defining intelligence. What is it? What makes us intelligent? How was our intelligence different from a mouse'? We're narrowing down the answers to some pretty existential questions. I'm Numenta community manager, Matt Taylor.
    [Show full text]
  • Hierarchical Temporal Memory (HTM)
    Hierarchical Temporal Memory (HTM) Computational Cognitive Neuroscience COSC 521, Spring 2019 Corey Johnson Resources for information on HTM Numenta.org HTM school NuPIC Jeff Hawkins 2004 book: On Intelligence: How a New Understanding of the Brain will Lead to the Creation of Truly Intelligent Machines (Note: some slide graphics are from Numenta.org, HTM school) 2 Hierarchical ● Higher levels → abstraction & permanence Temporal ● Change over time: patterns Memory ● Sparse Distributed Representation (SDR) 3 HTMs ● Biologically plausible model for intelligence ● Based on pyramidal neurons ● Neocortex micro-columns as building blocks "The neocortex comprises about 75% of the volume of the human brain and it is the seat of most of what we think of as intelligence." -Jeff Hawkins 4 HTMs continued ● Can learn / recall / infer high-order sequences ● Local learning rules, no global supervisor (HW) ● Relies on sparse distributed representation (SDR) ○ Fault tolerance ○ High capacity ● Learns by modeling the growth of new synapses 5 ANN Pyramidal HTM 6 HTM Layers (Lect. 2 slides) 7 Sparse Distributed Representation Fundamental to HTM systems SDR: 'Language of intelligence' Sparse as opposed to dense binary code, many bits are needed SDR bit similarity → similar semantic meaning Semantic error is key to generalization Cell only needs a few connections to neighbor to match pattern 8 Applications Good for: ● Data streams that change over time: text, GPS, dates, numbers ● Data with inherent structure ● System where many models are required rather than
    [Show full text]
  • From Neural Networks to Deep Learning: Zeroing in on the Human Brain
    From Neural Networks to Deep Learning: Zeroing in on the Human Brain Pondering the brain with the help of machine learning expert Andrew Ng and researcher- turned-author-turned-entrepreneur Jeff Hawkins. By Jonathan Laserson DOI: 10.1145/2000775.2000787 n the spring of 1958, David Hubble and Torsten Wiesel stood helpless in their lab at John Hopkins University. In front of them lay an anesthetized cat. In the cat’s skull, just above its visual cortex (an area in the back of the brain), there was a 3mm-wide hole through which electrodes recorded the response of several neurons. Hubble and Wiesel had Iconjectured that these neurons would fire when shown the right stimulus on the retina. If only they had any idea what that stimulus could be. For days they had shown the poor cat different shapes and shiny spots of light, testing different areas of the retina, but nothing elicited a response. In their book, Brain and the Visual Perception, Hubble and Wiesel give a personal account of this experience: The break came one long day in which example of what is now known as “orien- using the following experiment. They we held onto one cell for hour after hour. tation selective cells,” a type of cell which collected a bunch of natural images— To find a region of retina from which our is actually prevalent in the visual cortex. trees, lakes, leaves, and so on—and spots gave any hint of responses took These cells fire when they detect an edge extracted thousands of small image many hours, but we finally found a place oriented at a particular angle, in a specif- patches, 16x16 pixels each, from ran- that gave vague hints of responses.
    [Show full text]
  • Fluid and Flexible Minds: Intelligence Reflects Synchrony in the Brain's Intrinsic Network Architecture
    RESEARCH Fluid and flexible minds: Intelligence reflects synchrony in the brain’s intrinsic network architecture ∗ ∗ Michael A. Ferguson1,2 , Jeffrey S. Anderson2, and R. Nathan Spreng1 1Laboratory of Brain and Cognition, Human Neuroscience Institute, Department of Human Development, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, 14853 2Departments of Bioengineering and Neuroradiology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, 84132 Keywords: intelligence, fMRI, resting state functional connectivity, machine learning, cognition an open access journal ABSTRACT Human intelligence has been conceptualized as a complex system of dissociable cognitive processes, yet studies investigating the neural basis of intelligence have typically emphasized the contributions of discrete brain regions or, more recently, of specific networks of functionally connected regions. Here we take a broader, systems perspective in order to investigate whether intelligence is an emergent property of synchrony within the brain’s intrinsic network architecture. Using a large sample of resting-state fMRI and cognitive data (n = 830), we report that the synchrony of functional interactions within and across distributed brain networks reliably predicts fluid and flexible intellectual functioning. By adopting a whole-brain, systems-level approach, we were able to reliably predict individual differences in human intelligence by characterizing features of the brain’s intrinsic network architecture. These findings hold promise for the eventual development of neural markers to Citation: Ferguson,
    [Show full text]