Eye-Movement Analysis in Reading Content Words and Function Words

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Eye-Movement Analysis in Reading Content Words and Function Words Eye-movement analysis in reading content words and function words Sophie Roussel1, Anne Rohr1, Eric Raufaste 2, Jean-Luc Nespoulous1 1Laboratoire Lordat – CEA.1941- IFR N°96 - Université Toulouse-II, France 2 Laboratoire Travail et Cognition – UMR5551 du CNRS - Université Toulouse-II, France Address for correspondence: Sophie Roussel - Laboratoire Lordat Université Toulouse-II Maison de la Recherche 5, Allées A. Machado, 31058 Toulouse Cedex France Email: [email protected] Tel: (+33) 561-503-594 Fax: (+33) 561-504-918 Running Head: EYE-MOVEMENTS in READING ROUSSEL EYE-MOVEMENTS in READING 1 ABSTRACT Detection task studies and studies of eye-movement in reading all pointed out a differential processing of content and function words. Several determinants were proposed: grammatical class, word configuration, word frequency, etc. Although function words are both “short” and “highly frequent”, interactions between those factors were left widely underdetermined. Hence, the present eye-movement study aimed at isolate the specific effect of grammatical class, controlling other factors, and specifically frequency and form properties (e.g. word length). Results confirm that grammatical class induces by itself different attentional patterns with more fixations on content words than on function words. When words were gazed, there were longer gaze durations and smaller pupil diameter on content words than on function words. Keywords: Reading - Eye-movements - content word – function words ROUSSEL EYE-MOVEMENTS in READING 2 When reading a text, all words are not read in a similar way. The hypothesis of a differential processing of content and function words in non-pathological persons comes from the observation of aphasic patients with agrammatism. This pathology is characterized by almost systematic omission and/or substitution of function words in production. Although these typical errors are present in agrammatic production (Kean, 1985; Menn & Obler, 1990), the important variability in performances (within task and within subject) exhibited by those patients suggests a role of factors other than genuinely linguistic or psycholinguistic ones. It has been observed that an agrammatic patient could produce function words provided that the experimenter had drawn explicitly the patient’s attention towards those function words. Hence, variability in performance might be explained by attentional strategies (Nespoulous & Dordain, 1991). In other terms, any function word that does not receive enough attention would be omitted or substituted by this patient. In non-pathological subjects, studies based on detection tasks also showed a differential processing of content and function words, the former giving fewer detection errors than the second (Corcoran, 1966; Inhoff, 1984; Healy, 1994; Moravcsik & Healy, 1995; Koriat & Greenberg, 1991, 1994; Saint-Aubin & Poirier, 1997). This effect is known as “the missing letter effect”. The processing time hypothesis, developed by Healy (1976, 1994; Moravcsik & Healy, 1995) attribute this effect to the higher frequency of use of function words. However, according to the structural account developed by Koriat and Greenberg (1991, 1994), the higher level of omission on function words would ROUSSEL EYE-MOVEMENTS in READING 3 be due to their grammatical role within the sentence. Function words would enable the reader to establish a temporary syntactic structure, which would enable accessing the meaning of text. This meaning is provided by content words. In order to demonstrate the influence of syntactic factors at the letter detection level, Saint-Aubin and Poirier (1997) compared omission rates when a word is employed either as a conjunction or as a noun. Even when the noun was as frequent as the conjunction, results showed more omissions on the conjunction. Authors interpreted these results as a specific effect of syntactic factors on the number of omissions in a letter detection task. For about twenty years, eye-movement recording enabled a better understanding of processes in subject’s reading. So, when reading a text, eye- movements may be considered as a spontaneous, direct, and measurable trace of changes in the attentional focus. According to Posner (1980), any saccade is preceded by a visual attention shift toward the desired gaze location. This hypothesis was then developed by Fisher (1986; Fisher & Breitmeyer, 1987), who suggested that gaze movements follow a series of steps: attentional disengagement from start location, displacement of attention toward the target location, and engagement of attention on the new target position. According to Morrison (1984), access to the meaning of the fixated word (n) permits an attentional shift toward the next word (n+1). For short function words, the author emphasizes that the lag between preparation of the saccade toward n+1 and its realization would suffice for the reader to access the n+1 word meaning. In such case, the saccade might be reprogrammed and n+1 skipped. ROUSSEL EYE-MOVEMENTS in READING 4 Some other works directed at the study of word recognition in peripheral vision showed that the proactive processing of a word might be primed in periphery, before gaze actually reaches it (O’Regan, 1975 ; Rayner, 1979). Finally, in an overview of twenty years of research on eye-movements in reading, Rayner concluded: "Although we can easily decouple the locus of attention and eye-location in simple discrimination tasks (Posner, 1980), in complex information processing tasks such as reading, the link between the two is probably quite tight" (1998, p. 375). A major finding of eye-movement studies is the fact that not all words are fixated. Particularly, analyses showed that non-fixated words are essentially function words (prepositions, conjunctions, articles). Even when function words are fixated, they receive fewer fixations than content words– nouns, verbs, adjectives (Rayner & McConkie, 1976 ; Rayner, 1977 ; Just & Carpenter, 1980 ; Rayner & Duffy, 1988). Thus, content words are fixated about 85% of the time, whereas function words are fixated about 35% of the time (Carpenter & Just, 1983 ; Rayner & Duffy, 1988). Besides, there are more multiple fixations (including forward and background fixations) on content words than on function words. With regard to fixation durations, function words receive shorter fixations than content words (Just & Carpenter, 1983; Rayner & Duffy, 1988). Studies that used eye-tracking data thus led their authors to postulate a differential processing of content and function words. ROUSSEL EYE-MOVEMENTS in READING 5 The very fact that a reader is able to understand a sentence without having fixated every word in it rises questions about criteria that determine fixations. Although these results emphasize a differential processing of content and function words during reading, variables other than grammatical class can affect the number and duration of fixations. Actually, variables like word length, frequency of use, expectancy, might also influence ocular patterns. Just and Carpenter (1980), who considered gaze durations (i.e., cumulated duration of all fixations made on the word, either forward or backward) observed a negative correlation between word frequency and fixation duration. More precisely, after translating word frequencies with a log function, they observed a 53ms increase in gaze duration for each log unit decrease in frequency. Inhoff (1984) reached a similar conclusion for gaze duration, but not for first fixation duration. With regard to first fixation duration, Inhoff found that frequent words receive longer fixations than less frequent words. According to the author, this would be due to the fact that lexical access and context-based word interpretation would occur simultaneously when processing high frequency words, that is, during first fixations. To the contrary, in low frequency words, first fixation durations would only reflect the time necessary for lexical access, word interpretation being made during subsequent fixations. Longer fixation durations on low frequency words would be due to returns (in order to complete processing) and then to new fixations. Low frequency words would then receive more multiple fixations than high frequency words. ROUSSEL EYE-MOVEMENTS in READING 6 Word length also influences the ocular pattern. Rayner and McConkie (1976) showed that short words (up to three letters) are less fixated, receive less multiple fixations, and shorter fixations durations than long words (six letters and more). In order to control for word form (and word length) in testing the effect of grammatical class, an efficient strategy consists in using homographic words such that at least one meaning attached to the form corresponds to a content word and one meaning corresponds to a function word. From now on, we will name "bi-class" such forms. We also name "single-class" the forms that are attached to only one class (whether function- or content-word class). An analysis of the crossed effect of grammatical class and word form led Perrier et al. (1991) to conclude to an effect of grammatical class on the number and duration of fixations. When confronting the function word and content word acceptations of a bi-class form, it was found that the content word was significantly more fixated, received more multiple fixations, and longer fixations than the corresponding function word. Despite the fact that those experiments showed an effect of frequency and length on the number and duration of fixations, none
Recommended publications
  • Eye-Movement Research: an Overview of Current and Past Developments
    Elsevier AMS Ch01-I044980 Job code: EMAW 14-2-2007 1:04p.m. Page:1 Trimsize:165×240MM Chapter 1 EYE-MOVEMENT RESEARCH: AN OVERVIEW OF CURRENT AND PAST DEVELOPMENTS ROGER P. G. VAN GOMPEL, MARTIN H. FISCHER AND WAYNE S. MURRAY University of Dundee, UK ROBIN L. HILL University of Edinburgh, UK Eye Movements: A Window on Mind and Brain Edited by R. P. G. van Gompel, M. H. Fischer, W. S. Murray and R. L. Hill Copyright © 2007 by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Basal Fonts:Times Margins:Top:4.6pc Gutter:4.6pc Font Size:10/12 Text Width:30pc Depth:43 Lines Elsevier AMS Ch01-I044980 Job code: EMAW 14-2-2007 1:04p.m. Page:2 Trimsize:165×240MM 2 R. P. G. van Gompel et al. Abstract This opening chapter begins with an overview of the issues and questions addressed in the remainder of this book, the contents of which reflect the wide diversity of eye-movement research. We then provide the reader with an up-to-date impression of the most significant developments in the area, based on findings from a survey of eye-movement researchers and database searches. We find that the most heavily cited publications are not necessarily those rated as most influential. It is clear that eye-movement research is published across a wide variety of journals and the number of articles is increasing, but the relative proportion of eye-movement articles has remained almost constant. The United States produces the largest number of these articles, but other countries actually produce more per capita.
    [Show full text]
  • Proquest Dissertations
    THE ATTENTIONAL DISENGAGEMENT MODEL OF THE MISSING-LETTER EFFECT: A TEST OF THE ATTENTIONAL BEAM by AMANDA LALANDE Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirement for the degree of Master of Arts in Psychology (M.A.) School of Graduate Studies Laurentian University Sudbury, Ontario © AMANDA LALANDE, 2011 Library and Archives Bibliotheque et 1*1 Canada Archives Canada Published Heritage Direction du Branch Patrimoine de I'edition 395 Wellington Street 395, rue Wellington OttawaONK1A0N4 Ottawa ON K1A0N4 Canada Canada Your file Votre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-82018-6 Our file Notre reference ISBN: 978-0-494-82018-6 NOTICE: AVIS: The author has granted a non­ L'auteur a accorde une licence non exclusive exclusive license allowing Library and permettant a la Bibliotheque et Archives Archives Canada to reproduce, Canada de reproduire, publier, archiver, publish, archive, preserve, conserve, sauvegarder, conserver, transmettre au public communicate to the public by par telecommunication ou par I'lnternet, preter, telecommunication or on the Internet, distribuer et vendre des theses partout dans le loan, distribute and sell theses monde, a des fins commerciales ou autres, sur worldwide, for commercial or non­ support microforme, papier, electronique et/ou commercial purposes, in microform, autres formats. paper, electronic and/or any other formats. The author retains copyright L'auteur conserve la propriete du droit d'auteur ownership and moral rights in this et des droits moraux qui protege cette these. Ni thesis. Neither the thesis nor la these ni des extraits substantiels de celle-ci substantial extracts from it may be ne doivent etre imprimes ou autrement printed or otherwise reproduced reproduits sans son autorisation.
    [Show full text]
  • The Missing-Letter Effect for Common Function Words Depends on Their Linguistic Function in the Phrase Seth N
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 0278-7393/91/$3.00 1991, Vol. 17, No. 6, 1051-1061 The Missing-Letter Effect for Common Function Words Depends on Their Linguistic Function in the Phrase Seth N. Greenberg Asher Koriat Union College University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel It has been proposed that function words such as for and on conceal their letters because then- higher familiarity allows fast access to their unitized representations. However, in this study we show that letter detection in function words varies with their linguistic role in text. When such words were embedded in a phrase where they were forced into a content role by the surrounding context (t.$.,for or against or on switch), letter detection improved markedly and did not differ from that of matched content words. The result was replicated when the context preceding the function word and the overall sentential meaning were equated for both function and content usages. The results support a late-stage structural account of the function-disadvantage effect, where the syntactic units that support the structural frame of a sentence are lost in the transition from structure to meaning. Healy and her associates (e.g., Healy, 1976; Healy & Drew- Clearly, letters are at least as familiar as the words in which nowski, 1983; Proctor & Healy, 1985) claimed that reading they appear. So, if the perceptual system is sensitive to stim- of text is carried out simultaneously at several levels of analy- ulus frequency, we might ask, why do the higher order familiar sis.
    [Show full text]
  • Age Changes in the Missing-Letter Effect Reflect the Reader's Growing Ability to Extract the Structure from Text
    JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL CHILD PSYCHOLOGY 69, 175–198 (1998) ARTICLE NO. CH982441 Age Changes in the Missing-Letter Effect Reflect the Reader’s Growing Ability to Extract the Structure from Text Seth N. Greenberg Union College Asher Koriat University of Haifa, Israel and Frank R. Vellutino State University of New York at Albany Readers searching for a target letter in text are more likely to miss it in frequent function words than in less frequent content words, and the magnitude of this effect increases with age. While this increase has been taken to indicate that proficient readers process familiar words in terms of larger orthographic units, we propose that it reflects the reader’s growing ability to extract the structure of text, resulting in a reduced emphasis on function than on content words. Indeed, comparing 2nd graders (7 to 7 1/2 years) and college students (Experiment 1) this increase was found even when function and content words were equated for frequency. Scrambling words within a sentence (Experiment 2) im- proved letter detection in function compared to content words among 7th graders (12 to 13 years) and college students, but not among 3rd graders (8 to 9 years). Although letter detection was also affected by word frequency, the age differences noted above are possibly due not to the increasing familiarity of words, but rather to the growing sensitivity to their structural role in text. © 1998 Academic Press This research was supported by Grant 88-00395 to Koriat and Greenberg from the United States–Israel Binational Science Foundation (BSF), Jerusalem, Israel.
    [Show full text]
  • Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Visual Search in Reading
    Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Visual Search in Reading Ralph S. Redden Dalhousie University Kaylee Eady Université de Moncton Raymond M. Klein Dalhousie University Jean Saint-Aubin Université de Moncton Authors note: Jean Saint-Aubin and Kaylee Eady, École de psychologie, Université de Moncton; Ralph S. Redden and Raymond M. Klein, Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, Dalhousie University. This research was supported by Discovery grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to Jean Saint-Aubin and to Raymond M. Klein. Correspondence: Jean Saint-Aubin, École de psychologie Université de Moncton, New Brunswick, E1A 3E9, Canada Tel. (506) 858-4766 Email: [email protected] Abstract Individual differences in working memory capacity (WMC) are related to variations in a wide range of cognitive tasks. Surprisingly, effects of individual differences in working memory capacity are somewhat limited in visual search tasks. Here we tested the hypothesis that such an effect would be robust when search was one component of a dual task. Participants were presented strings of letters using rapid serial visual presentation and were required to detect all instances of a particular target letter. In Experiment 1, participants performed the letter search task in three contexts, while: a) reading a prose passage, b) processing a stream of random words, or c) processing a random stream of non-words. In the absence of the dual task of reading prose, and in line with much of the literature on individual differences in WMC and visual search, search performance was unaffected by WMC. As hypothesized, however, higher working memory capacity participants detected more target letters than lower capacity participants in the “true” dual task (searching while reading prose).
    [Show full text]
  • A Model of Discourse Predictions in Human Sentence Processing
    A Model of Discourse Predictions in Human Sentence Processing Amit Dubey and Frank Keller and Patrick Sturt Human Communication Research Centre, University of Edinburgh 10 Crichton Street, Edinburgh EH8 9AB, UK {amit.dubey,frank.keller,patrick.sturt}@ed.ac.uk Abstract prediction has been demonstrated by studies that show anticipation based on selectional restrictions: This paper introduces a psycholinguistic listeners are able to launch eye-movements to the model of sentence processing which combines predicted argument of a verb before having encoun- a Hidden Markov Model noun phrase chun- tered it, e.g., they will fixate an edible object as soon ker with a co-reference classifier. Both mod- els are fully incremental and generative, giv- as they hear the word eat (Altmann and Kamide, ing probabilities of lexical elements condi- 1999). Semantic prediction has also been shown in tional upon linguistic structure. This allows the context of semantic priming: a word that is pre- us to compute the information theoretic mea- ceded by a semantically related prime or by a seman- sure of surprisal, which is known to correlate tically congruous sentence fragment is processed with human processing effort. We evaluate faster (Stanovich and West, 1981; Clifton et al., our surprisal predictions on the Dundee corpus 2007). An example for syntactic prediction can be of eye-movement data show that our model achieve a better fit with human reading times found in coordinate structures: readers predict that than a syntax-only model which does not have the second conjunct in a coordination will have the access to co-reference information.
    [Show full text]
  • Saint-Aubin, J., Klein, RM, Babineau, M., Christie, J., & Gow, D
    Retrieved from DalSpace, the institutional repository of Dalhousie University https://dalspace.library.dal.ca/ handle/10222/71822 Version: Post-print Publisher’s version: Saint-Aubin, J., Klein, R. M., Babineau, M., Christie, J., & Gow, D. W. (2016). The Missing-Phoneme Effect in Aural Prose Comprehension. Psychological science, doi:0956797616645096. Running head: MISSING-PHONEME EFFECT 1 There is a Missing-Phoneme Effect in Aural Prose Comprehension Jean Saint-Aubin École de psychologie, Université de Moncton Raymond M. Klein Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Dalhousie University Mireille Babineau Department of Psychology, Université du Québec à Montréal John Christie Department of Psychology & Neuroscience, Dalhousie University David W. Gow Cognitive/Behavioral Neurology Group, Massachusetts General Hospital Correspondence to Jean Saint-Aubin: École de psychologie Universté de Moncton 18, avenue Antonine-Maillet Moncton, New Brunswick E1A 3E9, CANADA E-mail: [email protected] 2 MISSING-PHONEME EFFECT Abstract When participants search for a target letter while reading, they make more omissions if the target letter is embedded in frequent function words than in less frequent content words. This phenomenon, called the missing-letter effect, has been considered a window on the cognitive mechanisms involved in the visual processing of written language. In the present study, one group of participants read two texts for comprehension while searching for a target letter and another group listened to the narration of the same two texts while listening for the corresponding target letter's phoneme. The ubiquitous missing-letter effect was replicated and extended to a "missing-phoneme effect". Item-based correlations between the letter and the phoneme detection tasks were high leading us to conclude that both procedures reflect cognitive processes that reading and listening have in common which are rooted in psycholinguistically driven allocation of attention.
    [Show full text]
  • A Method to Analyze Similarities of Eye Movements: a Giscience Based Spatiotemporal Approach
    University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School November 2019 A Method to Analyze Similarities of Eye Movements: a GIScience Based Spatiotemporal Approach Mehrdad Vaziri University of South Florida Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd Part of the Geographic Information Sciences Commons Scholar Commons Citation Vaziri, Mehrdad, "A Method to Analyze Similarities of Eye Movements: a GIScience Based Spatiotemporal Approach" (2019). Graduate Theses and Dissertations. https://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/8691 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. A Method to Analyze Similarities of Eye Movements: a GIScience Based Spatiotemporal Approach by Mehrdad Vaziri A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirement for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Geography and Environmental Science and Policy School of Geosciences College of Arts and Sciences University of South Florida Major Professor: Joni Firat, Ph.D. Steven Reader, Ph.D. Philip Van Beynen, Ph.D. Elizabeth Schotter, Ph.D. Date of Approval: November 19, 2019 Keywords: Similarity Analysis, GIScience, Eye Tracking, Spatiotemporal Copyright © 2019, Mehrdad Vaziri DEDICATION Dedicated to my wife Tina and my Uncle Reza, my Mother and Father, My Brothers and Sisters and all their beautiful children, Mahshid, Sina, Kian, Parnyan ACKNOWLEDGMENT I would like to thank my major professor Dr. Joni Firat for her neverending support and mentorship throughout my time at USF.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effects of Word Length and Suffixation on Eye Movement Control in Turkish Reading
    THE EFFECTS OF WORD LENGTH AND SUFFIXATION ON EYE MOVEMENT CONTROL IN TURKISH READING A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF INFORMATICS OF THE MIDDLE EAST TECHNICAL UNIVERSITY BY TUĞÇE NUR BOZKURT IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE IN THE DEPARTMENT OF COGNITIVE SCIENCE SEPTEMBER 2017 THE EFFECTS OF WORD LENGTH AND SUFFIXATION ON EYE MOVEMENT CONTROL IN TURKISH READING Submitted by TUĞÇE NUR BOZKURT in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Cognitive Science Department, Middle East Technical University by, Prof. Dr. Deniz Zeyrek Bozşahin Dean, Graduate School of Informatics Prof. Dr. Cem Bozşahin Head of Department, Cognitive Science Asst. Prof. Dr. Cengiz Acartürk Supervisor, Cognitive Science Dept., METU Examining Committee Members: Prof. Dr. Cem Bozşahin Cognitive Science Dept., METU Asst. Prof. Dr. Cengiz Acartürk Cognitive Science Dept., METU Prof. Dr. Deniz Zeyrek Bozşahin Cognitive Science Dept., METU Asst. Prof. Dr. Burcu Can Buğlalılar Computer Engineering Dept., Hacettepe University Assoc. Prof. Dr. Bilal Kırkıcı Foreign Language Education Dept., METU Date: _September 7, 2017 I hereby declare that all information in this document has been obtained and presented in accordance with academic rules and ethical conduct. I also declare that, as required by these rules and conduct, I have fully cited and referenced all material and results that are not original to this work. Name, Last name : Tuğçe Nur Bozkurt Signature : iii ABSTRACT THE EFFECTS OF WORD LENGTH AND SUFFIXATION ON EYE MOVEMENT CONTROL IN TURKISH READING Bozkurt, Tuğçe Nur MSc., Department of Cognitive Science Supervisor: Asst.
    [Show full text]
  • Asher Koriat Date
    1 October 2015 Asher Koriat Curriculum Vitae and Publications A. Curriculum Vitae 1. Personal Details Date of birth: January 8, 1939 Citizenship: Israeli Marital Status: Married Home Address: 101/9 Yefe Nof, Haifa 34454, Israel Tel. 972-4-8360049 Office: Institute of Information Processing and Decision Making University of Haifa, Haifa 3498838, Israel Tel. 972-4-8249746; Fax: 972-4-8249431; E-mail: [email protected] 2. Details of Higher Education Hebrew University of Jerusalem, 1960-1965 B.A. in Psychology and Philosophy in 1964 M.A. studies in psychology, 1963-1965 University of California, Berkeley 1965-1969: Ph.D. in Psychology Main Subjects: Personality and Cognition Ph.D. degree, 1970 3. Academic rank and tenure in institutions of higher learning Dates Name of Institution Rank 1969-1970 Univ. California, Berkeley Teaching Associate 1970-1970 Univ. California, Berkeley Postdoc. Research Fellow 1970-1976 Hebrew University Lecturer 1976-1977 University of Haifa Senior Lecturer 1977-1978 University of Oregon Visiting Associate Professor Decision Research Visiting Research Associate 1978-1985 University of Haifa Senior Lecturer 1985-1986 Institute of Advanced Studies, Hebrew University Fellow 1986-1990 University of Haifa Associate Professor 1990- University of Haifa Professor 1992-1992 Rotman Research Institute Irv Acenberg Visiting Professor Baycrest Centre, Toronto & University of Toronto 1998 Visiting Professor, Max-Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich 1999, 2000 Humboldt Research Award, Max-Planck Institute for Psychological Research, Munich 2003-2004 Centre for Advanced Study, Norwegian Academy of Science, Oslo & 2 Max-Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Munich 1993- 2014 Head, Institute of Information Processing and Decision Making (IIPDM), University of Haifa 1996- 2013 Director, The Max Wertheimer Minerva Center for Cognitive Processes and Human Performance, University of Haifa & Technion 4.
    [Show full text]
  • Evidence from the Missing Letter Effect Psicológica, Vol
    Psicológica ISSN: 0211-2159 [email protected] Universitat de València España Saady, Amany; Ibrahim, Raphiq; Eviatar, Zohar Language-specific and language-general factors in text reading in Arabic: Evidence from the Missing Letter Effect Psicológica, vol. 36, núm. 1, enero-junio, 2015, pp. 123-142 Universitat de València Valencia, España Available in: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=16932960006 How to cite Complete issue Scientific Information System More information about this article Network of Scientific Journals from Latin America, the Caribbean, Spain and Portugal Journal's homepage in redalyc.org Non-profit academic project, developed under the open access initiative Psicológica (2015), 36, 123-142. Language-specific and language-general factors in text reading in Arabic: Evidence from the Missing Letter Effect Amany Saady, Raphiq Ibrahim* and Zohar Eviatar University of Haifa, Israel The goal of the present study was to extend the models explaining the missing-letter effect (MLE) to an additional language and orthography, and to test the role of phonology in silent reading in Arabic. We also examined orthographic effects such as letter position and letter shape, morphological effects such as pseudo-prefixes, and phonological effects such as pronounceability. The results showed that readers miss letters more often in function words and prefixes than in content words, more in second position than in first position, more often when the letters are silent than pronounced, and less often when the letter shape is more symmetric and stable. The results show that these aspects of the missing letter effect can be generalized over writing systems that are not alphabetic, suggesting that the models proposed to explain the MLE in all the orthographies tested may reflect a universal aspect of reading.
    [Show full text]
  • The Extraction of Phrase Structure During Reading: Evidence from Letter Detection Errors
    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 1994, 1 (3), 345-356 The extraction of phrase structure during reading: Evidence from letter detection errors ASHER KORIAT University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel and SETH N. GREENBERG Union College, Schenectady, New York In light of recent suggestions regarding the prominence of structure in speech production and com- prehension, it has been postulated that structural processing might also play a similarly important role in reading. Some evidence in support of this contention can be gleaned from eye-movement re- search. However, more systematic support comes from recent work on letter detection during read- ing, which has shown that the rate of omission errors is inordinately high for morphemes that dis- close phrase structure. The results of three lines of research suggest that, early in text processing, readers attempt to extract a structural frame for the sentence to help the on-line integration of ac- cessed representations, and that structure-supporting units recede to the background as the mean- ing of the sentence evolves. "Twas brillig, and the slithy toves did gyre and gimble example, "someone is doing something to someone in the wabe." These first two lines of Lewis Carroll's somewhere (or at some time)." It strikes us that, in read- poem "Jabberwocky" leave the reader with the uncom- ing normal text, the elements that impart information fortable feeling of comprehending pure nonsense. Al- about the abstract schema of the sentence—functor mor- though there appears to be precious little to comprehend, phemes—generally receive little attention, and hence an invisible structure seems to carve a rhythm and fa- their contribution to text processing is unappreciated by miliarity out of this gibberish.
    [Show full text]