Using an Argument-based Approach to Validity for Selected Tests of Spatial Ability in Allied Medical Professions Students

DISSERTATION

Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University

By

Terri Ann Bruckner, M.A.

Graduate Program in Education

The Ohio State University

2013

Dissertation Committee:

Dorinda Gallant, Advisor

Ann O’Connell

Laura Harris

Copyrighted by

Terri Ann Bruckner

2013

Abstract

Spatial ability is a cognitive skill required for success in many professions. Those in the Allied Medical professions utilize this skill in the performance of many of their daily tasks. Understanding the nature of this ability in Allied Medical educational programs may allow educators to improve the delivery of material or develop training material for students who have low levels of spatial ability. In order to assess spatial ability in Allied Medical students, valid instruments are needed. This study used the argument-based approach to examine the validity evidence for six tests of spatial ability in a group of 128 Allied Medical students. Tests were chosen to assess spatial

(Cube Comparison Test and The Purdue Spatial Visualization Test Visualization of

Views), spatial visualization (Hidden Figures Test and The Purdue Spatial Visualization

Test Visualization of Developments), and mental rotation (Mental Rotations Test and The

Purdue Spatial Visualization Test Visualization of Rotations). Evidence is presented to support the assertions that some of these tests can be interpreted as spatial ability measures, but the assertions that the chosen tests measure spatial perception, spatial visualization, and mental rotation abilities was not satisfied.

ii

Dedication

To my husband, Mike, your love and support made it possible for me to get this far. To my son David and his family, thank you for your willingness to share me with my pursuit of a graduate degree. To Brian and Rebecca Welch—thanks for your encouragement and our Friday nights. Thanks to my parents, Mary and Roger Johnson, and to my sister Tammy Fisher, for all of your encouragement. Melinda, thanks for making graduate school fun.

iii

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Dr. Dorinda Gallant for her help and constant support.

Without your encouragement, this would never have been possible. I would also like to thank Drs. Ann O’Connell and Laura Harris for serving as my committee members and for their suggestions for improving this document. I want to express my appreciation to

Dr. Ayres D’Costa, who encouraged me to pursue my doctorate and helped me in refining my topic. I also want to sincerely thank Deb Zabloudil, who wouldn’t let me quit.

iv

Vita

June 1974 ...... West Liberty-Salem High School

1978 ...... B.S., Allied Medical Professions, The Ohio

State University

1988 ...... M.A., Education, The Ohio State University

1985 to present ...... Clinical Instructor, School of Health and

Rehabilitation Sciences, The Ohio State

University

Fields of Study

Major Field: Education

v

Table of Contents

Abstract ...... ii

Dedication ...... iii

Acknowledgments...... iv

Vita ...... v

Fields of Study ...... v

Table of Contents ...... vi

List of Tables ...... xi

List of Figures ...... xv

Chapter 1: Introduction ...... 1

Allied Medical Professionals and Their Associated Spatial Tasks ...... 4

Athletic Training...... 5

Health Information Management and Systems ...... 6

Medical Dietetics ...... 6

Occupational Therapy ...... 7

vi

Significance and Purpose of this Study ...... 16

Organization of the Dissertation ...... 22

Chapter 2: Review of the Pertinent Literature ...... 23

Validity ...... 23

Unified Theory of Validity ...... 28

The History of Research Related to Spatial Ability ...... 32

What is Spatial Ability? ...... 39

Individual Differences in Spatial Ability ...... 46

Biologic Factors Related to Individual Differences in Spatial Ability ...... 47

Environmental Factors Related to Individual Differences in Spatial Ability ...... 55

Tests of Spatial Ability ...... 58

The Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT) ...... 60

Hidden Figures Tests ...... 64

Mental Rotations Test (MRT) ...... 66

The Cube Comparison Test ...... 69

Studies of Spatial Ability in Healthcare ...... 70

Summary ...... 79

Chapter 3: Design and Methods ...... 84

Population and Sample ...... 86

vii

Population ...... 86

Sample ...... 88

Instrumentation...... 89

Tests to Measure Spatial Perception Ability ...... 90

Tests to Measure Spatial Visualization Ability ...... 95

Tests to Measure Mental Rotation Ability ...... 100

The Spatial Experience Questionnaire ...... 105

Data Collection ...... 105

Data Analysis ...... 107

Statistical Analysis Assumption Checking ...... 118

Chapter 4: Results ...... 120

Spatial Skills in Allied Medical Professions ...... 120

Validation Evidence ...... 127

Validation Evidence for the Cube Comparison Test Results ...... 129

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire ...... 139

Validation Evidence for the PSVT Visualization of Views Test Results...... 142

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire ...... 148

Validation Evidence for the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test ...... 150

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire ...... 161

viii

Validation Evidence for the Hidden Figures Test ...... 163

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire ...... 169

Validation Evidence for the PSVT Visualization of Rotations Test ...... 170

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire ...... 176

Validation Evidence for the Mental Rotations Test ...... 177

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire ...... 187

Additional Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire ...... 188

Chapter 5: Discussion and Conclusions ...... 190

Spatial Skills in Allied Medical Professions ...... 191

Validation Evidence for the Cube Comparison Test ...... 194

Validation Evidence for the Visualization of Views subtest of the Purdue Spatial

Visualization Test ...... 199

Validation Evidence for the Visualization of Developments Subtest of the Purdue

Spatial Visualization Test...... 203

Validation Evidence for the Hidden Figures Test ...... 207

Validation Evidence for the Visualization of Rotations Subtest of the Purdue Spatial

Visualization Test battery...... 211

Validation Evidence for the Mental Rotations Test ...... 215

Limitations ...... 219

ix

Conclusions and Recommendations for Future Research ...... 222

References ...... 228

Appendix A: Spatial Tasks for Athletic Training ...... 244

Appendix B: Spatial Tasks for Health Information Management and Systems ...... 255

Appendix C: Spatial Tasks for Medical Dietetics...... 261

Appendix D: Spatial Tasks in Occupational Therapy...... 265

Appendix E: Spatial Tasks in Radiologic Science ...... 273

x

List of Tables

Table 1. Spatial Tests Used in this Study ...... 11

Table 2. Descriptions of the Allied Medical Professions Included in this Study ...... 87

Table 3. Demographic Information for Sample ...... 89

Table 4. Tasks Requiring the Use of Spatial Perception ...... 122

Table 5. Tasks Requiring Spatial Visualization ...... 124

Table 6. Tasks Requiring Mental Rotation ...... 126

Table 7. Score Information for the Cube Comparison Test, Version A ...... 131

Table 8. Score Information for the Cube Comparison Test, Version B ...... 133

Table 9. Pearson Correlations between the Cube Comparison Test and the Mental

Rotations Test ...... 136

Table 10. Pearson Correlations between the Cube Comparison Test and the PSVT

Visualization of Views Test ...... 137

Table 11. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Cube Comparison Test Scores ...... 141

Table 12. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Spatial Test Scores ...... 142

Table 13. Score Information for the Visualization of Views Subtest of the PSVT ...... 144

xi

Table 14. Pearson Correlation between the PSVT Visualization of Views Test and the

Mental Rotations Test ...... 146

Table 15. Pearson Correlations between the Cube Comparison Test and the PSVT

Visualization of Views Test ...... 147

Table 16. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Views Test Scores...... 149

Table 17. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Views Test Scores...... 150

Table 18. Descriptive Statistics and Scoring Information for Version A of the PSVT

Visualization of Developments Subtest ...... 152

Table 19. Descriptive and Score Information for the Visualization of Developments

Subtest of the PSVT, Version B ...... 155

Table 20. Pearson Correlations between the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test and the Mental Rotations Test ...... 157

Table 21. Pearson Correlation between the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test ...... 158

Table 22. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Developments Tests Scores ...... 161

Table 23. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Developments Test Scores ...... 162

Table 24. Descriptive and Scoring Information for the Hidden Figures Test ...... 165

xii

Table 25. Pearson Correlation between the Hidden Figures Test and the Mental

Rotations Test ...... 167

Table 26. Pearson Correlation between the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test ...... 168

Table 27. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Hidden Figures Test Scores ...... 170

Table 28. Score Information for the Visualization of Rotations Subtest of the PSVT .. 172

Table 29. Pearson Correlations between the PSVT Visualization of Rotations Test and the Mental Rotations Test ...... 174

Table 30. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Rotations Test Score ...... 176

Table 31. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Rotations Test Score ...... 177

Table 32. Descriptive Statistics and Scoring Information for the Mental Rotations Test,

Version A ...... 180

Table 33. Descriptive Statistics and Score Information for the Mental Rotations Test,

Version B ...... 182

Table 34. Pearson Correlations Between all Tests of Spatial Ability in this Study...... 184

Table 35. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Scores on the Mental Rotations Test ...... 187

Table 36. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Scores on the Mental Rotations Test ...... 188

xiii

Table 37. Summary of Validity Evidence for Spatial Tests ...... 224

Table 38. Spatial Tasks in Athletic Training ...... 244

Table 39. Spatial Tasks in Health Information Management and Systems ...... 255

Table 40. Spatial Tasks for Medical Dietitians ...... 261

Table 41. Spatial Tasks in Occupational Therapy ...... 265

Table 42. Spatial Tasks in Radiologic Science ...... 273

xiv

List of Figures

Figure 1. Example Item from the Visualization of Views Subtest of the Purdue Spatial

Visualization Test ...... 92

Figure 2. Example Item from the Cube Comparison Test ...... 94

Figure 3. Example Item from the Hidden Figures Test ...... 97

Figure 4. Example Item from the Visualization of Developments Subtest of the Purdue

Spatial Visualization Test ...... 99

Figure 5. Example Item from the Visualization of Rotations Subtest of the Purdue

Spatial Visualization Test ...... 101

Figure 6. Example Item from the Mental Rotations Test ...... 103

Figure 7. Scores on Version A of the Cube Comparison Test ...... 130

Figure 8. Scores on Version B of the Cube Comparison Test ...... 132

Figure 9. Variations in Performance on the Cube Comparison Test, Version A...... 139

Figure 10. Variations in Performance on the Cube Comparison Test, Version B ...... 139

Figure 11. Score Distribution of the PSVT Visualization of Views Subtest ...... 143

Figure 12. Gender Differences on the Visualization of Views Test ...... 148

Figure 13. Score Distribution of the PSVT Visualization of Developments Subtest,

Version A ...... 151

xv

Figure 14. Score Distribution of the PSVT Visualization of Developments Subtest,

Version B ...... 154

Figure 15. Variations in Performance on the Visualization of Developments Test,

Version A ...... 160

Figure 16. Variations in Performance on the Visualization of Developments, Version B

...... 160

Figure 17. Score Distribution of the Hidden Figures Test ...... 164

Figure 18. Variations in Performance on the Hidden Figures Test ...... 169

Figure 19. Score Distribution of the PSVT Visualization of Rotations Test ...... 171

Figure 20. Variations in Performance on the Visualization of Rotations Test ...... 176

Figure 21. Score Distribution for the Mental Rotations Test, Version A ...... 179

Figure 22. Score Distribution for the Version B of the Mental Rotations Test ...... 181

Figure 23. Variations in Performance on the Mental Rotations Test, Version A ...... 186

Figure 24. Variations in Performance on the Mental Rotations Test, Version B ...... 186

xvi

Chapter 1: Introduction

Spatial ability is a broad term that relates to how individuals interact with and interpret their environment. Over the past 90 years, many researchers have attempted to define spatial ability. Unfortunately, there is still debate regarding whether this is a unique ability, or a combination of abilities. If it is a combination of abilities, there are still questions as to what factors are involved. In various studies, spatial ability is considered to be a group of related skills distinctly different from other cognitive skills such as verbal reasoning (Keehner, Cohen, Hegarty, & Montello, 2004; McGee, 1979).

Characteristics of this skill include the ability to generate visual images, perceive spatial arrangements, construct and deconstruct mental visual images, retain images in the mind, and transform, manipulate, or rotate images mentally (Barratt, 1953; Cohen & Hegary,

2007; Guilford, Fruchter, & Zimmerman, 1952; Guilford & Lacey, 1947; Hegarty &

Kozhevnikov, 1999; Kelley, 1928; Kesner & Linzey, 2005; Kyllonen, Lohman, & Snow,

1984; Michaelides, 2003; Thurstone, 1938). Eliot (2002) used a broader term for spatial ability, referring to it as a type of . He discussed the magnitude of this type of intelligence by stating:

I am convinced that spatial intelligence has been significantly underestimated in the role it plays in our everyday lives. Spatial intelligence is fundamental in the sense that it may operate at any given moment at several levels of human consciousness and, in 1 combination with other cognitive functions, it may also contribute both to how we learn to process different kinds of information and how we develop strategies for solving many types of everyday problems. It is pervasive in the sense that we use it whenever we act upon information about the positional, distance, and directional relationships of people and objects in our daily lives. In my opinion, we have only begun to appreciate the fundamental and pervasive role this intelligence plays in our everyday lives (Eliot, 2002, p. 479).

Numerous studies in the early part of the twentieth century served to establish the existence of a spatial ability factor (El Koussy, 1935; Guilford, Fruchter, & Zimmerman,

1952; Guilford & Lacey, 1947; Kelley, 1928; McFarlane, 1925; Thurstone, 1924). Over the past 70-80 years, hundreds of researchers have worked to further define, categorize, and explain this ability. Most authors agree that spatial ability, sometimes referred to as spatial intelligence, includes the ability to mentally visualize, manipulate, or transform objects in space as well as the ability to orient one’s self, or an object in relationship to one’s self (Eliot, 2000; Guilford, 1967; Guilford & Hoepfner, 1971; Hyde, Geiringer, &

Yen, 1975; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Lohman, 1989; McGee, 1979).

In addition, researchers have attempted to determine if spatial ability is a unitary concept, or if it is a variety of related abilities. Many categories of spatial ability have been proposed. Some of the most widely studied are spatial orientation, spatial perception, spatial visualization, and mental rotation. Linn and Petersen (1985), and

Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden (1995) proposed three categories of spatial ability: (a) spatial perception, which involves an ability to determine relationships with respect to one’s own

2 body; (b) spatial visualization, involving tasks that require complex multistep manipulations; and (c) the mental rotation of imagined figures.

Spatial ability appears to be an essential skill for those who are involved in science and math related professions. This skill has been studied in engineering students, chemistry students, and medical students (Berbaum, Smoker, & Smith, 1985; Bodner &

Guay, 1997; Carter, LaRussa, & Bodner, 1987; Garg, Norman, & Sperotable, 2001;

Hegarty, Keehner, Cohen, Montello, & Lippa, 2007; Martin-Dorta, Saorin, & Contero,

2008). It has been found that those who do better on tests of spatial skills tend to perform better in science and math related academic courses (Baker & Talley, 1972; Battista,

1990; Edens & Potter, 2007; Eisenberg & McGinty, 1977; Fennema & Sherman, 1977;

Guay & McDaniel, 1977).

Furthermore, studies have been done which allude to the relationship between spatial ability and ability in certain academic, professional, and cultural endeavors

(Lunneborg & Lunneborg, 1984; McDaniel, Guay, Ball, & Kolloff, 1978; Newcombe,

Bandura, & Taylor, 1983; Zimowski & Wothke, 1988). In studies of college students, men who enrolled in courses such as architecture, calculus, chemistry, music, art, engineering, and drafting did well on spatial tests (Bodner & Guay, 1997; Carter,

LaRussa, & Bodner, 1987; Edens & Potter, 2007; Guay, 1980; Hegarty & Kozhevnikov,

1999; Johnson & Bouchard, 2007). Women who enrolled in astronomy, physics, architecture, high level math, and drafting courses also scored well on spatial tests (Olson

& Eliot, 1986). Guay (1978) found that college students who scored highly on a spatial

3 questionnaire scored higher on a mental rotation test, but not on other tests of spatial ability.

Allied Medicine (e.g. Athletic Training, Circulation Technology [Perfusion],

Health Sciences, Health Information Management and Systems [HIMS], Medical

Dietetics, Medical Laboratory Science [sometimes referred to as Medical Technology],

Occupational Therapy, Physical Therapy, Radiologic Sciences, and Respiratory Therapy) is a term that encompasses the various professions involved in healthcare who support and consult with physicians and nurses. Students must be proficient in a variety of science courses to practice in their chosen fields. In addition, there are crucial spatial skills that must be mastered for both didactic and clinical courses.

Allied Medical Professionals and Their Associated Spatial Tasks

Many of the routine tasks performed by health care professionals seem to have a spatial component. Understanding the nature of the required spatial skills may inform educators as they develop curricula and ultimately allow those health professionals to do their jobs more efficiently. The roles of these medical professionals can be categorized and analyzed according to their spatial nature. The following section briefly describes the five professions to be investigated in this study.

4

Athletic Training

Athletic Trainers (AT) work with physicians, athletic personnel, patients and their families, and other medical professionals, to provide care in a variety of settings. These professionals are skilled in methods of risk management and injury prevention. When an athlete is injured the AT must be able to examine, diagnose, and recommend the appropriate care for the injury (Athletic Training, 2012). Examination of patients requires keen observational skills. The trainer must be able to recognize visual clues related to health and injury. These clues may be overt or covert and may require that visual information be mentally manipulated. Internal structures must be mentally visualized, and normal versus abnormal physical patterns evaluated and acted upon.

Gravitational or spatial orientation is a desirable skill for this group of professionals.

They must be able to use, instruct, and assist others in using exercise equipment, and they must be able to adjust their client’s and their own body mechanics (Athletic Trainers,

2010). Athletic Trainers generally complete courses in anatomy and physiology, biology, chemistry, human nutrition, and pharmacology in addition to math, humanities courses, and athletic training courses.

5

Health Information Management and Systems

This profession focuses on the business aspects of healthcare. A career in this field appeals to those with interests in healthcare, business, and information technology.

Health information management professionals work in a variety of healthcare or commercial settings. Major responsibilities include collection, storage, management, and retrieval of healthcare data. Communication is another key component of this occupation

(Health Information Management and Systems, 2012). Visualization of anatomy and physiology are helpful skills for these professionals. They must also be able to recognize patterns and disembed patterns from medical data, graphs, and charts (Medical Records and Health Information Technicians, 2010). Preparatory courses for this profession include biology or chemistry, anatomy and physiology, pharmacology, and computer information courses.

Medical Dietetics

These professionals practice medical and community nutrition therapy and work with patients in long term care. They have a heavy background in science courses such as anatomy and physiology, chemistry, biology, biochemistry, and microbiology to prepare for the occupation. Professionals in this occupation must be able to recognize patterns in laboratory specimens, interpret radiographic images, and interact with patients

6 and clients (Dietitians and Nutrionionists, 2010). Visualization of anatomic structures and physiologic processes are also key skills for these professionals. They may practice in medical facilities, governmental departments, or commercial enterprises.

Communication and consultation are essential skills (Medical Dietetics, 2012).

Occupational Therapy

Occupational Therapists (OT) work with other members of the healthcare team to assess and help patients/clients maintain or regain the ability to function independently.

Their clients may have physical impairments or diseases which restrict or limit their day- to-day activities (Occupational Therapy, 2012). Therapists must assess their clients’ health and well-being which frequently requires the ability to visualize internal anatomy as well as the ability to spatially orient their own bodies in relation to their client.

Visualization and spatial orientation are further required as they plan and develop adaptive devices for various types of disabilities or diseases (Occupational Therapists,

2010). Prerequisite science courses for students in this profession include biology, anatomy, and physiology.

Radiologic Sciences

Radiologic Sciences professions include those related to medical imaging and those related to the use of radiation to provide therapeutic treatment for disease.

7

Radiologic Technology and Medical Imaging are other terms used for those engaged in the science of using radiation (x-ray, magnetism, and ultrasound) to produce images of the tissues, organs, bones, and vessels of the body for both diagnostic and therapeutic purposes (Radiologic Sciences and Therapy, 2012). As a practicing educator in this field, the author knows that those who specialize in the various facets of medical imaging must be able to visualize normal as well as abnormal or diseased anatomy. Preparing patients for imaging procedures requires the ability to orient themselves and their patients in relation to the imaging equipment. Evaluation of images requires the ability to visualize depth, and with some types of images the ability to mentally rotate a visualized anatomic structure into a different imaging plane (Radiologic Technologists, 2010). Students in a radiologic sciences program require a background in physics, biology, anatomy, and physiology.

Validity Evidence for Instruments Used to Evaluate Spatial Ability

There are hundreds of tests of spatial ability that have been used over the past 100 years. A large sampling of these tests was compiled by Eliot and Smith (1983), and classified into ten task categories. Meta-analyses such as those by Linn and Petersen

(1985) and Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden (1995) have categorized spatial ability by the skills that tests seem to measure. These meta-analyses examined studies from children as young as four years old and adults as old as their mid-eighties. No specific information was included in either of these studies as to the disciplines that were studied. While no

8 specific validation evidence was offered by these two studies, they did categorize spatial tests as to the skills they were purported to measure based on reported gender differences.

Tests that measured mental rotation included the Space Relations subtest of the Primary

Mental Abilities Test by Thurstone, the Flags and Cards Rotation Tests, generic mental rotations tests, and the Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotations Test. Tests that measured skills in the category of spatial perception included the Rod-and-Frame Test and the

Water Level Test. Tests that measured skills related to spatial visualization included: paper folding tests, surface developments tests, the Spatial Relations subtest of the

Differential Aptitude Test (DAT-SR), the Embedded Figures Test, the Hidden Figures

Test, the children’s version of the Embedded Figures Test, the Identical Blocks Test, the

Paper Form Board Test, and the Block Design subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence

Scale.

Although hundreds of spatial tests are available, validity evidence is scarce.

Validity is defined in the Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing (1999) as

“the degree to which evidence and theory support the interpretation of test scores entailed by proposed uses of tests” (p. 9). Put another way, validation lends evidence that an instrument measures what it professes to measure (Huck, 2004; Knapp & Mueller, 2010;

Messick, 1989; Salkind, 2004). In any discussion of validity, it is critical to realize that it is not the instruments themselves that are being validated, but the uses and interpretations of the results of these instruments that are being examined (Babbie, 1995; Kane, 2006;

Knapp & Mueller, 2010; Messick, 1989). Thus, in order to interpret results from spatial tests for Allied Medical students, evidence of validity must be examined.

9

A search of the medical literature found validation studies with a few researcher- developed tests for teaching anatomy or physician skills (Friedman, Dev, Dafoe, Murphy,

& Felciano, 1993; Smoker, Berbaum, Luebke, & Jacoby, 1984). The following databases were searched to attempt to locate information on validation studies of spatial tests in

Allied Medical Professions: Academic OneFile, Academic Search Complete,

MasterFILE Complete, Newspaper Source Plus, OAIster, Oxford Scholarship Online,

WorldCat.org, and Medline. No validation studies in any of the Allied Medical professions were found for any published spatial test.

Tasks that seem especially pertinent to those in the Allied Medical Professions are spatial visualization, spatial perception and orientation, and mental rotation. The tests chosen to evaluate Allied Medical students in this study (see Table 1) are The Hidden

Figures Test for spatial visualization (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976), the Cube

Comparison Test for spatial perception and orientation (Ekstrom, French, & Harman,

1976), the three-part Purdue Spatial Visualization Test for spatial orientation, spatial visualization, and mental rotation (Guay, 1980), and the Mental Rotations Test for mental rotation (Peters, Laeng, Latham, Jackson, Zaiyouna, & Richardson, 1995; Vandenberg &

Kuse, 1978). These tests were chosen because some validity evidence was available in the literature, and because each has been used in previous studies of spatial ability in college students. A brief description of each test is provided below. Complete descriptions for each of these tests are presented in Chapter Three of this document.

10

Spatial Skill to be Evaluated Spatial Test

Spatial Visualization The Hidden Figures Test

Spatial Visualization The Visualization of Developments Subtest

of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test

Spatial Perception The Cube Comparison Test

Spatial Perception The Visualization of Views Subtest of the

Purdue Spatial Visualization Test

Mental Rotation The Mental Rotation Test

Mental Rotation The Visualization of Rotations subtest of

the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test

Table 1. Spatial Tests Used in this Study

The Hidden Figures Test and other embedded figures tests have been frequently used as tests of spatial visualization (Davis & Eliot, 1994; Hassler, Birbaumer, & Feil,

1985; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Lord, 1990; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). These tests have reported internal consistency and split-half reliabilities ranging from .8 to .95

(Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976; Hegarty, Montello, Richardson, Ishikawa, &

Lovelace, 2006; Hyde, Geiringer, & Yen, 1975). These types of tests have also been found to correlate significantly with other tests of spatial ability, both large and small scale demonstrating construct-related validity evidence (Hegarty, Montello, Richardson,

Ishikawa, & Lovelace, 2006). There are conflicting reports as to whether the Hidden

Figures test can demonstrate the expected gender differences in spatial ability (Linn &

11

Petersen, 1985; Massa, Mayer, & Bohon, 2005; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995), but this is not unexpected since spatial visualization has not demonstrated consistent significant gender differences.

The Cube Comparison Test has been designated as a test of spatial orientation by

Ekstrom, French, and Harman in the Kit of Factor Referenced Cognitive Tests (1976). In a group of 11th and 12th grade students, the reliability (type of reliability was not specified) was reported to be .77 for both males and females, and .84 in college students

(Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). This test has been used as part of a battery to determine high and low spatial ability in a group of undergraduate students taking a biology course (Lord, 1990) and as a measure of spatial orientation in a study of 83 adolescent pairs of twins to evaluate sex-linked gender differences in spatial orientation

(Jardine & Martin, 1984), however no reliability or validity evidence was included in this study.

The Mental Rotations Test (Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978) is a widely accepted test for spatial ability. Many authors have reported evidence related to the validity of interpretations and uses of this test. The Mental Rotations Test has been shown to correlate significantly with other tests of spatial ability such as the Spatial Relations subtest of the Primary Mental Abilities Test (Voyer, et al., 2006), the Cards Rotation Test

(Cherney, 2008), the Spatial Relations subtest of the Differential Aptitude Test

(Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978), and the Embedded and Hidden Figures Tests (Ekstrom,

French, & Harman, 1976). Vandenberg and Kuse (1978) reported test-retest reliability of

.83 and internal consistency reliability of .88 when this test was given to large samples of

12 adolescents and adults, and Vandenberg, Kuse, and Vogler (1985) reported split-half reliability of .64 in a study of 56 college undergraduates. Internal consistency reliability of .91for a redrawn version of the Mental Rotations Test was found in a group of 157 college undergraduates (Voyer, et al., 2006). Hegerty et al. (2006) reported internal reliability of .88 in a group of 221 adult volunteers.

Furthermore, spatial tests should correlate well with mathematics and science grades which The Mental Rotations Test has been shown to do. Peters et al. (1995) found that students from science-based academic programs scored significantly better on their redrawn Mental Rotations Test than students from arts and humanities programs. In a later study, similar results for academic program type were found in students from

Canada, Germany, and Japan (Peters, Lehmann, Takahira, Takeuchi, & Jordan, 2006).

This test also correlates highly with large scale and environmental tests of spatial ability

(Hegarty, Montello, Richardson, Ishikawa, & Lovelace, 2006). Spatial ability is seen to be a separate ability from verbal reasoning and spatial tests should exhibit small correlations with these types of tests as well as tests for English and Humanities courses.

As expected, low correlations are seen between this test and tests of verbal reasoning

(Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978). Additionally, spatial ability scores tend to favor males, and this test almost always demonstrates this gender difference in scores (Cherney, 2008;

Geiser, Lehmann, & Eid, 2008; Kaufman, 2007; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Peters, Laeng,

Latham, Jackson, Zaiyouna, & Richardson, 1995; Stumpf, 1993; Vandenberg & Kuse,

1978; Voyer, Rodgers, & McCormick, 2004; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). In fact,

13 the effect size for gender differences in performance on this test have been reported to range from .7 to .95 (Masters, 1998; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995).

The Purdue Spatial Visualization Test is a three-part test consisting of a

Visualization of Views subtest, a Visualization of Developments subtest, and a

Visualization of Rotations subtest. Composite reliability has been reported to be as high as .92, with a range of .56 to .87 for the individual subtests (Battista, 1990; Guay, 1978;

Guay, 1980). It has been found to correlate significantly with other spatial tests such as the Spatial Relations subtest of the Differential Aptitude Test and the Shepard and

Metzler mental rotations test (Kovac, 1989). It has also shown the expected gender differences favoring males, and to predict performance in college chemistry and engineering courses (Bodner & Guay, 1997; Guay, 1980; Sorby & Baartmans, 2000).

These characteristics tend to suggest the construct validity for the use of this test in measuring spatial ability.

The Spatial Experience Questionnaire (McDaniel, Guay, Ball, & Kolloff, 1978) contains 25 items and asks respondents to indicate their level of participation and their level of enjoyment for each of these items. It is described as being “developed to measure the extent of participation in and enjoyment of experiences that might be relevant to the development of spatial abilities” (p. 1). The authors of the questionnaire compiled this list of 25 activities as those which “young adults might have engaged at some time while growing up” (McDaniel, Guay, Ball, & Kolloff, 1978, p. 2). In a study of 242 college students, respondents were asked to classify each activity according to level of both participation and enjoyment. The authors found the survey resulted in

14 significant differences in participation scores between high and low spatial groups for both men and women, and in enjoyment scores for high and low spatial groups for men.

Kane’s Interpretive and Validity Argument within Validity and Validation

Validation and validity have been categorized in many ways. Kane (2006) described the historical evolution of validity theory from criterion to content to construct models over the last 90 years. He went on to discuss the melding of these methods in the

1970s and 1980s, in which Cronbach, and later Messick advocated a unified theory of validity. A unified theory of validity was described by Kane as emphasizing “the need for an overall evaluation of validity involving multiple kinds of evidence” (Kane, 2006, p. 20). Kane (2006) described the unified theory of validity as “quite appealing as a conceptual framework for validity” but noted that it “didn’t provide clear guidance for validation of a test-score interpretation or use” (p. 21). His argument-based framework for validation involved two steps: the specification of interpretive arguments and the uses of scores, and an evaluation of the plausibility of these proposed interpretations and uses

(Kane, 2011). He called these the interpretive argument and the validity argument.

According to Kane (2006), the interpretive argument “specifies the proposed interpretations and uses of test results by laying out the network of inferences and assumptions leading from the observed performances to the conclusions and decisions based on the performances” (p. 23). The validity argument then consists of an evaluation of the plausibility of the inferences and assumptions put forth in the interpretive

15 argument. This framework is similar to the approach adopted by the 1999 Standards for

Educational and Psychological Testing.

The interpretive argument commonly starts with an examination of scoring. This interpretation “claims relatively little and requires relatively little evidence for its support, mainly evidence that the scoring rule is reasonable and that it has been applied appropriately and consistently” (Kane, 2011, p. 8). A second inference may involve generalization. Evidence for this inference would involve reliability or generalizability studies to support claims for validity. Extending the interpretive argument further may provide evidence that information from the scores can be extrapolated to broader domains. Criterion-related studies or analyses of the commonalities between assessment performance and performance in wider domains would supply evidence to support this inference. Often, a final inference is related to decision-making based on the results of the responses on the measurement instruments (Kane, 2006; Kane, 2011). This framework will be used for this research study and is more fully described in Chapter

Two.

Significance and Purpose of this Study

Spatial ability has been established as a type of intellectual ability, distinct from other abilities. It has been shown to differ across genders, and be affected by life experiences. Many studies have been done with physicians and dental professionals to evaluate the need for, existence of, and levels of spatial ability in various healthcare

16 occupations. In the dental profession, tests of spatial ability are used as criteria for admission. Some physician training programs have also investigated the usefulness of including spatial tests as part of the admission process (Hegarty, Keehner, Cohen,

Montello, & Lippa, 2007). Other researchers have examined how spatial ability is related to skills such as surgery or interpreting radiologic images (Anastakis, Hamstra, &

Matsumoto, 2000; Berbaum, Smoker, & Smith, 1985; Garg, Norman, & Sperotable,

2001; Keehner, Tendick, Meng, Anwar, Stoller, & Duh, 2004; Nilsson, Hedman, &

Ahlqvist, 2007). While most physician programs have, to date, rejected using tests of spatial ability as a component of admission criteria, it has been established that this intellectual skill is highly useful in medical fields such as surgery and radiology

(Anastakis, Hamstra, & Matsumoto, 2000; Hegarty, Keehner, Cohen, Montello, & Lippa,

2007; Risucci, 2002; Smoker, Berbaum, Luebke, & Jacoby, 1984; Wantzel, Hamsta,

Anastakis, Matsumoto, & Cusimano, 2002).

Allied Medical professionals are also engaged in highly visual occupations, and many types of spatial skills are necessary in order to efficiently perform the necessary tasks required for these jobs. A search of the Medline and CINAHL databases using the keywords “spatial ability” with the keywords for the various Allied Medical Professions examined in this study revealed only one study examining this skill, a study examining the relationship between spatial perception and achievement in sonography students

(Clem, Anderson, Donaldson, & Hdeib, 2010).

The overall goal of this study is two-fold: (a) to determine the extent to which validity evidence can be established for currently available instruments for use with

17

Allied Medical professionals and (b) to examine spatial abilities in Allied Medical professionals. Allied Medical professionals must be proficient in a variety of spatial tasks in the performance of their occupational duties. As a practitioner and educator in an

Allied Medical profession, the author knows that some tasks involve the ability to visualize information; internal anatomic structures cannot be seen, but must be mentally pictured to perform tasks such as patient assessment and prepare for radiographic imaging. Other tasks require that the practitioner be able to mentally orient themselves, their patients, or their equipment. Patients with disabilities or injuries cannot always be placed in textbook positions for therapeutic treatments. Professionals must be able to orient themselves and their equipment to function efficiently for patients in spite of these difficulties. This may require the construction of mental images that relate up/down and/or left/right visualization of machinery or persons. Still other tasks require that medical professionals be able to mentally rotate or transform verbal or figural information. One example involves cross-sectional images used in the diagnosis and evaluation of many types of diseases. Anatomy is learned in frontal, lateral (from the side), or three-dimensional space. Sectional images require that the information be rotated mentally in order to identify the anatomic structures as they are presented in this unique format.

A deeper understanding of the above skills may allow educators to enhance learning and develop alternative instructional opportunities for students in their professional programs. Additionally, remedial educational modules could be developed to potentially help students strengthen their spatial skills. If there are identifiable

18 differences in spatial skills in these various occupations, there may be implications for career counseling. Potential students with lower levels of spatial ability might need additional course work of a spatial nature to ensure success in their chosen career path, or future studies may investigate the need for spatial tests as part of admission criteria for some healthcare educational programs. To add to the limited research on spatial ability in this population, tests that accurately measure spatial abilities must be found, and identification of relevant spatial experiences must be examined. Interpretation of any measurement tool’s scores can only be initiated after evidence of validity has been established.

Moreover, understanding the nature of the spatial abilities required for each Allied

Medical profession could be important in other ways. Spatial ability could be used as a criterion for admission into some of these programs. Courses could be assessed for the degree and level of spatial ability needed for successful completion. Educators could evaluate their teaching methods to take spatial ability into account in their course planning. Students could be given special assignments to attempt to improve their spatial skills in didactic and clinical courses.

Research Questions

Research Question 1: What spatial skills are used by Allied Medical Professionals in the accomplishment of their occupational duties? Which of these skills are shared across the

19 professions? Do the professions differ in their reliance on spatial perception, spatial visualization, or mental rotation abilities?

Research Question 2: To what extent is validity evidence provided for the use of the

Mental Rotations Test, the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT), the Cube

Comparison Test, and The Hidden Figures Test to measure spatial ability in Allied

Medical students?

In order to examine this question, the following assumptions addressing the various aspects of the interpretive argument will be used:

1. Scores on the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental

Rotations Tests will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the

sample of Allied Medical students in this study.

2. Scores on the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental

Rotations Tests will demonstrate acceptable reliability.

3. The Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, and Visualization of Rotations Tests will

demonstrate criterion related validity evidence when compared to the gold

standard Mental Rotations Test.

4. The Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of Views Test will

exhibit concurrent validity evidence as measures of spatial perception.

The Hidden Figures Test and the Visualization of Developments Test will

20

exhibit concurrent validity evidence as measures of spatial visualization.

The Visualization of Rotations Test and the Mental Rotations Test will

exhibit concurrent validity evidence as measures of mental rotation.

5. If the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental

Rotations Tests measure the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the

male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and

will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire.

Research Question 3: To what extent is validity evidence provided for the use of the

Spatial Experience Questionnaire to measure spatial ability in Allied Medical students?

The major source of data for this study will come from a spatial experience questionnaire and four widely used spatial ability tests. To use these data to answer the research questions, the following assumptions are made:

1. The spatial tests used for the study are reasonable measures of the general

construct of spatial ability.

2. These spatial tests can differentiate types and levels of this ability.

3. The spatial experience questionnaire is a reasonable measure of types of

experiences in which students in this sample may have engaged.

4. Participants will quickly and accurately respond to as many items as possible

within the allotted time.

21

5. Participants will have adequate vision to complete the tests and questionnaire.

Technical Standards for the Allied Medical programs require normal or corrected

vision in order for students to function in didactic and clinical courses.

Organization of the Dissertation

This chapter introduced spatial ability, spatial ability tests, a spatial experience questionnaire, the various Allied Medical Professions, and the concept of validity. In addition, this chapter included the research questions and significance of the study.

Chapter Two will explore pertinent literature on the nature of spatial ability, the various models to explain its nature, spatial ability tests, spatial experience questionnaires, and pertinent studies in the medical field. Chapter Three discusses the population, sampling plan, the instruments, data collection procedure, and data analysis for this study. Chapter

Four will present the results of data analysis. Finally, Chapter Five will include a discussion of the results, describe limitations of the study, and suggest recommendations for further research.

22

Chapter 2: Review of the Pertinent Literature

Validity

Validity is a critical and fundamental issue when discussing any type of conclusions that are based on surveys or test instruments. Some authors describe validity and the process of validation as a process that helps to ensure that a measurement tool measures what it claims to measure, that it reflects the meaning of the concept that is under consideration (Babbie, 1995; Huck, 2004; Knapp & Mueller, 2010; Salkind, 2004).

Polgar and Thomas (2000) stated that validity is concerned with the accuracy of a measure, “in other words, the amount of measurement error” (p. 138). However it is important to note that many recent publications clarify this further by noting that validity is not a characteristic of the tool itself, but rather an indicator that the data and results are accurate. Anastasi and Urbina (1997) stated that the “validity of a test concerns what the test measures and how well it does so. It tells us what can be inferred from test scores”

(p.113). Dick and Hagerty (1971) are even more specific in stating that validity “gives some indication of how well a test measures a given area, under certain circumstances and with a given group” (p. 75). This definition is echoed in the Standards for

Educational and Psychological Testing (1999) which state that validity “refers to the degree to which evidence and theory support the interpretations of test scores entailed by 23 proposed uses of tests” (p. 9). The common thread on most of the current validity research is that it is not the instrument that is valid, but the uses and interpretations that must be put to the test.

Some authors describe various types of validity. These include content, construct, criterion, internal, external, face, ecological, curricular, differential, and others (Babbie,

1995; Carmines & Zeller, 1979; Dick & Hagerty, 1971; Polgar & Thomas, 2000; Salkind,

2004). The three the most commonly described types of validity are content, criterion, and construct.

Content validity refers to the extent to which a tool measures the domain of knowledge and skills of interest and the range of meanings associated with the concept of interest (Babbie, 1995; Bryant, 2000; Carmines & Zeller, 1979; Dick & Hagerty, 1971;

Huck, 2004; Knapp & Mueller, 2010; Polgar & Thomas, 2000; Salkind, 2004).

Validation of this sort is commonly used for tests of a particular skill and for achievement tests (Kane, 2006; Salkind, 2004). Evidence of content validity is commonly sought through the evaluation of expert judges who attempt to determine that items in the measurement tool reflect the universe of content being studied (Huck, 2004; Knapp &

Mueller, 2010; Salkind, 2004). Bryant (2000) noted that multivariate statistical methods such as principal components analysis, exploratory factor analysis, and confirmatory factor analysis can also be used to examine how well the tool measures the content domain. These types of analyses can provide information on the number of facets that are tapped by the test and whether any important content information is underrepresented

(Bryant, 2000; Kurpius & Stafford, 2006). Kurpius and Stafford (2006) noted that

24 comparison of the test items with specific job tasks could also provide evidence of content validity. Kane (2006) noted that this type of validation “works well if the relevant content domain has been defined with care, the tasks have been sampled in a way that makes them representative of the domain, the observations were made using procedures that would tend to control random and systematic errors, and the performances were evaluated appropriately” (p. 19). One fundamental limitation of content validation relates to the vagueness of certain concepts. If concepts are not described in enough detail and with sufficient exactness, it is nearly impossible to insure that a measurement tool adequately samples the intended domain (Carmines & Zeller,

1979). Messick (1989) stated that content validity provided evidence for how representative an instrument might be, but noted that this was a somewhat limited role in overall validation. Kane (2006) added that this type of validation evidence tends to be subjective, and while it might supply evidence for domain coverage, it does not involve test scores and so cannot justify conclusions related to the interpretation of these scores.

A second commonly described type of validity is criterion validity. A criterion is a standard, a rule, or a principle. Criterion validity examines test scores in relation to this standard. Evidence for this type of validity is commonly found by examining the extent of agreement between the test in question and a “gold standard” or benchmark validated measurement tool (Bryant, 2000; Knapp & Mueller, 2010; Salkind, 2004). This type of validity is further differentiated into concurrent and predictive validity. Concurrent validity correlates the tool being used to an external criterion or task at the same point in time to determine if the test measures current behavior. Predictive validation attempts to

25 correlate the tool with some future criterion or performance (Carmines & Zeller, 1979;

Kurpius & Stafford, 2006). This type of validity is commonly examined using correlation between the measure and the criterion variable. Correlation coefficients from these analyses are referred to as validity coefficients (Bryant, 2000; Carmines & Zeller,

1979). Carmines and Zeller (1979) noted that there may be many validity coefficients dependent upon the number of criteria for a particular trait. Criterion validity can also be examined using regression analysis and canonical correlation analysis (Bryant, 2000).

Kane (2006) stated that the major advantages to this type of validation include its objectivity and the relevance of the criterion to the proposed uses of tests and other measurement tools. He went on to note that the main limitation lies in the difficulty of finding a valid criterion (Kane, 2006; Kane, 2011).

The third type of validity that has commonly been described is construct validity.

Carmines and Zeller (1979) described this type of validity as being “concerned with the extent to which a particular measure relates to other measures consistent with theoretically derived hypotheses concerning the concepts (constructs) that are being measured” (p. 23). A construct is generally considered to be an explanation or a hypothesis about some type of behavior. Because these behaviors, attributes, or traits cannot be readily observed or measured their explanations tend to be based in theory.

Measurement tools must be compared to tools that reflect the theorized construct. The difficulty is in defining the construct sufficiently to determine that the test measures only the relevant construct and not a related trait or attribute. Evaluation of construct validity generally involves establishing evidence for convergent or discriminant (sometimes

26 called divergent) validity. Convergent validity provides evidence that tools meant to measure the same thing are related. That is, all tools measuring the same construct should correlate highly (Bryant, 2000; Huck, 2004). Discriminant validity verifies that tools that should have no relationship (that measure different constructs) are not related.

The first step in establishing construct validity is clearly formulating a definition of the underlying construct (Bryant, 2000; Carmines & Zeller, 1979). This involves “carefully specifying the necessary components or key ingredients that constitute the construct and what distinguishes it from related but separate constructs” (Bryant, 2000, p. 112;

Carmines & Zeller, 1979). Convergent and discriminant validation may be reported in terms of correlations between instruments. Factor analysis is another tool which helps to establish evidence for construct validity (Carmines & Zeller, 1979; Knapp & Mueller,

2010).

Kane (2006) reported that authors such as Cronbach had called for an overall evaluation of validity with multiple sources of evidence and that construct validation was

“widely accepted as the general approach to validity” (p. 21) in the early 1980s. In the mid to late 1980s, Messick described construct validation as a unifying framework for validity (Messick, 1989). Kane (2006) noted that there were three positive effects for using the construct validation as a unified framework for validity: 1) this model focused on issues related to interpretations and uses of test scores rather than simply correlations with criteria, 2) the role for, and need to check assumptions in score interpretations, and

3) allowance for alternative interpretations for tests and test uses.

27

Unified Theory of Validity

The unified theory of validity is a broad conceptual framework. Anastasi and

Urbina (1997) clarified that “almost any information gathered in the process of developing or using a test is relevant to its validity” (p. 138). Because so many types of evidence can be presented in the validation process, it can seem like an endless process

(Kane, 2006). For that reason, Kane (2006) characterized the unified theory of validation as “quite appealing as a conceptual framework for validity” but stated that “it didn’t provide clear guidance for validation of a test-score interpretation or use” (p. 21). He also stated that this method of validation tended to be “very open-ended”, and that “it is not clear where to begin or how to gauge progress” (Kane, 2006, p. 21; Kane, 2011, p. 8).

The basic questions regarding validity are: what is being claimed and are these claims warranted, given the evidence (Kane, 2011, p. 4). An argument-based approach to validity can be used to attempt to answer these questions. This approach is used to examine the interpretations and uses of test score by “generating a coherent analysis of all of the evidence for and against the proposed interpretation or use, and to the extent possible, the evidence relevant to plausible alternative interpretations and decision procedures” (Kane, 2006, p. 22). Kane (2006) went on to note that the main advantage in this approach “is the guidance it provides in allocating research effort and in gauging progress in the validation effort” (p. 23).

28

Validation has two major components in the argument-based approach: the interpretive arguments and the validity arguments (Kane, 2006; Kane, 2011). The interpretive arguments develop a network of assumptions leading from the interpretation of test performance or scores to the decisions based on these performances or scores.

Validation relies on the support provided by the assumptions and the plausibility of the interpretations. This type of validation “specifies the reasoning involved in getting from the test results to the conclusions and decisions based on these results” (Kane, 2006, p.

25). The validity argument is then used to evaluate the interpretive argument; to see if it makes sense, is plausible, and to determine if it is reasonable (Kane, 2006; Kane, 2011).

The interpretive argument commonly involves inferences related to scoring, generalizability, extrapolation to broader domains, and decision-making. Kane (2011) declared “Each inference in the interpretive argument extends the interpretation or adds a decision” (p. 8). The scoring inference generally claims that the “scoring rule is reasonable and has been applied appropriately and consistently” (Kane, 2011, p. 8). A generalization inference makes the argument that observed scores can be taken as an indication of performance over a broader domain of task performance. To support this inference, reliability and/or generalizability evidence is needed. Scores can be referenced to norms for other measures, groups, benchmark performances, etc. Another common inference relates to extrapolation of scores to a broader domain. These types of inferences require criterion related analyses. There are many other types of inferences that can be used. The key is that the inferences and assumptions provide the framework for validation.

29

Kane (2011) held that interpretive arguments are informal, presumptive arguments and three characteristics distinguish these arguments from more formal arguments:

1. Their assumptions are subject to challenge, and they can be evaluated in terms

of how well they stand up to such challenges.

2. Their assumptions and conclusions are tentative, or probable, rather than

certain.

3. They are defeasible in the sense that they can be overturned in a particular case

even if the argument is generally sound (Kane, 2011, p. 11)

Presumptive arguments favor conclusions, but are never certain; they just offer strong support for the claims that are being made. Kane (2006, 2011) cites Toulmin’s (1958) framework for analyzing informal arguments. According to this framework, each inference starts from a datum and ends with a claim. A warrant is used to justify the inference so that the claim can be reasonably inferred from the datum. Warrants are if- then rules and require justification or supportive evidence called backing. Kane (2011) stated “The interpretive argument specifies the inferences involved in getting from the observed performances to the conclusion to be drawn and the decisions to be made based on test scores. It would include a network or chain of inferences. The validity argument would provide a critical appraisal of the coherence of the interpretive argument and an evaluation of the warrants and their backing for the inferences in the interpretive argument” (Kane, 2011, p. 12).

30

The 1999 Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing also described to process of validation as a process of developing a “sound validity argument to support the intended interpretation of test scores and their relevance to the proposed use” (p.9). This document advocates the development of a set of propositions supporting any proposed interpretation of a testing process. Once developed, these propositions can be evaluated by things such as empirical studies and literature review in order to establish validity.

Twenty-four Standards offer guidance in developing validity evidence for proposed uses and interpretations of tests (American Educational Research Association,

1999). Potential sources of evidence in the validation process include an examination of the content domain covered by the test, establishment of reliability and generalizability, determination of relationships to criterion measures, and an investigation as to the consequences of the decisions resulting from score interpretations. These types of processes closely mirror the argument-based approach explicated by Kane (2006).

All available information is therefore helpful in establishing validity evidence for a test or measurement tool. The Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing

(1999) stated “Ultimately, the validity of an intended interpretation of test scores relies on all the available evidence relevant to the technical quality of a testing system. This includes evidence of careful test construction; adequate score reliability; appropriate test administration and scoring; accurate score scaling, equating, and standard setting; and careful attention to fairness for all examinees…” (p. 17). In order to begin the validation process proposed by this study, an examination of the relevant construct is essential. The

31 following sections examine spatial ability, in an attempt to accurately identify the relevant constructs associated with this trait.

The History of Research Related to Spatial Ability

Any historical discussion of spatial ability must begin with the studies of intellect conducted by Charles Spearman. In the early 1900s Spearman conducted several experiments with school-aged children in an attempt to understand human intelligence.

Through his pioneering work with factor analysis, he theorized that all tests of mental ability measured a general ability factor (g factor) and at least one specific factor (s1, s2, s3…). These specific factors were uncorrelated with the general factor, and may or may not be correlated with each other. When specific factors were correlated, abilities measured by scores from a test were thought to be related to a group factor (Jensen, 1998;

Spearman, 1904; Spearman, 1938). The g factor represented that which a test had in common with all other tests of ability; the s factors represented specific abilities (e.g. spatial) which were assumed to be peculiar to each test (Eliot & Smith, 1983). Over the past 100 years some authors have agreed and some have disagreed with Spearman’s research and/or methods, (Smith, 1964), but all spatial ability research has in some manner built on his framework in the two factor theory of intelligence.

During the early decades of the twentieth century researchers in psychology and education had a profound interest in understanding intelligence (Spearman, 1904;

Thurstone, 1924). Was mental ability a single concept, or were there multiple types of

32 ? How did children learn? Were there ways (e.g., tests) which could predict academic success? Was verbal reasoning synonymous with intelligence? How should those with poor verbal skills be tested? Non-verbal tests were being developed during this period, but the question remained: what were these tests measuring (El Koussy,

1935; McFarlane, 1925; Spearman, 1904; Spearman, 1938; Spearman, 1946)?

According to Smith (1964), the original impetus to develop mental tests arose from a perceived need to improve methods of predicting scholastic success. In addition to the work of Spearman, in the first decade of the 1900s Binet was developing mental tests which were designed to predict academic success. These tests were rooted in reading and writing, and these verbal skills were found to be the most successful predictors of school achievement. The assumption that verbal types of questions are able to measure intelligence, lead to these types of tests being called “intelligence tests”

(Smith, 1964). With the increased use of these verbal tests, some complained that the scales were unsatisfactory for testing students who had difficulty with language skills, and this gave rise to the development of non-language or performance types of tests

(Eliot & Smith, 1983). The non-language tests included activities such as jigsaw-like form boards, wooden construction activities, dress-pattern cutting, pegboards, etc.

(McFarlane, 1925; Smith, 1964). Non-language tests of intelligence were slow to be accepted since all of these tests were initially devised to predict academic success and academic success was closely associated with the ability to read and write (Eliot &

Smith, 1983), and because administering the tests was done individually, so it was a slow and cumbersome process (Smith, 1964).

33

In 1918, the United States Army initiated large-scale testing for both verbal skills

(Alpha Test) and non-verbal skills (Beta Test). The Alpha Test was given to educated or literate recruits, while the Beta Test was used for the uneducated or those who had difficulty with language. The Beta Tests were used to assess practical or mechanical ability, and according to Smith (1964), included paper versions of practical tests like form boards and maze tracing as well as peg-board, wire bending, tapping, and manipulation exercises. This was the first non-verbal battery of tests to be given to a large number of subjects, and included some of the first paper-and-pencil tests which we now think of as spatial in nature (Eliot & Smith, 1983; Smith, 1964).

During the 1920s, the literature on mechanical and practical ability tests seemed to point to a spatial factor of intelligence that was independent of g. These types of tests were thought to be the original spatial tests (Carter, LaRussa, & Bodner, 1987; Eliot &

Smith, 1983; Zimowski & Wothke, 1988). One of the first studies to demonstrate a spatial factor independent of g was completed by McFarlane in 1925 (Eliot & Smith,

1983; McGee, 1979; Smith, 1964). In an investigation of “practical” ability using construction type tests and puzzle boxes McFarlane (1925) found some evidence of the presence of a group factor additional to g with her sample of boys but not with girls. The group factor she isolated was found in boys to a higher degree than girls, was independent from general intelligence, and she suggested that those who have it can judge better about concrete spatial situations (McFarlane, 1925).

During the late 1920s, some researchers concentrated on developing practical and/or mechanical tests. The authors of some of these tests claimed that they were highly

34 successful in selecting prospective mechanical engineers and measuring other types of mechanical abilities such as those seen in shop classes (Smith, 1964). Unfortunately, early forms of these tests consisted of mechanical models which were very elaborate, expensive and time consuming to administer.

Other evidence of a unique spatial factor, separate from general intelligence, was emerging in studies conducted in the late 1920s and 1930s. Spatial factors were beginning to be studied in their own right by researchers such as Kelley, Alexander, El

Koussy, and Smith (Lohman, 1989; Smith, 1964). Factor studies of elementary and middle school children suggested evidence of a spatial factor that could be separated into two parts which were designated: ε—ability involving the sensing and retention of geometric forms and θ—a facility in the mental manipulation of spatial relationships

(Kelley, 1928). El Koussy (1935) administered a large battery of tests covering a wide range of abilities to groups of children, and compared them with reference tests for g. He concluded that some spatial tests involved a group factor over and above their g-content.

This group factor, called the k-factor, “receives a ready psychological explanation in terms of visual imagery” (El Koussy, 1935, p. 65). From subject feedback he concluded

“the explanation of the k-factor consists in the ability to obtain and the facility to utilize visual, spatial imagery” (El Koussy, 1935, p. 65). In the mid 1930s, Smith (1964) did research similar to El Koussy. He found evidence for the presence of a spatial group factor (similar to El Koussy). This group factor was present in tests that require an ability to form and retain an exact impression of shape or pattern. His battery of tests showed significant gender differences with boys doing better than girls. In addition, he found

35 significant correlations for test scores with art, geometry, and engineering drawing but not with handwork. He concluded that ability measured by these types of tests might be useful in certain occupations and an improved form of the test might be used as a diagnostic test of ability for the above occupations and/or school subjects (Smith, 1964).

Thurstone disagreed with Spearman’s Two Factor theory and reported that human intelligence was composed of many independent or primary factors rather than one general ability factor (Eliot & Smith, 1983; Smith, 1964; Thurstone, 1947). In the late

1930s, Thurstone evaluated results from 56 psychological tests using factor analysis. He identified seven primary ability factors, named for the “type of thinking that is involved in doing the task” (p. 1) which included: verbal (V), number (N), space (S), memory (M), and perception (P) (Thurstone, 1938). Eliot and Smith (1983) reported that Thurstone defined his space factor as “requiring a facility in spatial or visual imagery” (p. 3) where subjects taking tests needed to turn, rotate, or transform images mentally.

During the 1940s and 1950s, authors continued the debate over intellectual ability, how spatial factors differed from each other, and developed new measures of spatial ability (Eliot & Smith, 1983). Using various statistical techniques, they tried to solve the question of g versus multiple ability factors. Also during this period, many of the spatial tests in use today were developed (Zimowski & Wothke, 1988). Large scale ability testing was again undertaken by the military in order to classify personnel for pilot training or aircraft maintenance. Many new tests were developed for this project, and the battery was administered to thousands of military personnel. Factor analysis of the test results provided strong evidence for the existence of several spatial factors (Eliot &

36

Smith, 1983; Smith, 1964). Guilford and Lacey (1947) found that one cluster of tests seemed to measure spatial relations which they described as requiring the subject to determine the relationships between different spatially-arranged stimuli and responses, and the comprehension of the arrangement of elements within a visual stimulus. They also found a second cluster of tests that seemed to pertain to visualization which they describe as the ability to imagine the rotation of depicted objects, the folding and unfolding of flat patterns, and the relative changes of position of objects in space.

During the late 1940s and early 1950s, work continued in attempting to identify the nature of spatial intelligence. Thurstone developed tests for his Primary Abilities and continued testing large groups of adolescents and adults. Smith (1964) reported that in a reanalysis of his primary ability factors, Thurstone found three factors having to do with visual orientation in space (his space factor) which he called S1, S2, and S3. S1 seemed to be the ability to recognize the identity of an object when it is seen from different angles or as the ability to visualize a rigid configuration when it is moved into different positions, S2 appeared to be the ability to imagine the movement or internal displacement among the parts of a configuration, and S3 appeared to be the ability to think about those spatial relations in which the body orientation of the observer is an essential part of the problem.

Fruchter (1954) reported that during this period Burt (1949) had distinguished two sub-factors of practical ability: spatial and mechanical. Burt defined his space sub-factor as the ability to perceive, interpret, or mentally rearrange objects as spatially related. He said that at least two spatial factors are needed, a factor for static spatial relations and a

37 factor for kinetic spatial relations; he further subdivided the static sub-factor into two- and three-dimensional abilities (Fruchter, 1954).

During this period, several authors reanalyzed the Army Air Force Data and found evidence for at least three spatial factors (Eliot, 1980; Smith, 1964). Eliot and

Smith (1983) reported that French (1951) isolated three spatial factors: space or spatial factor, spatial orientation, and spatial visualization. Zimmerman (1954), using increasingly difficult versions of the Army Air force tests, was reported to have demonstrated that increasing the difficulty of items on the Army Air Force experimental test could be made to emphasize in succession a perceptual speed, a space, and a visualization factor, and claimed that the ability to visualize relationships required more intellectual effort than the ability to carry out tasks requiring spatial orientation alone

(Eliot & Smith, 1983; Smith, 1964).

During the 1940s and 1950s, Eliot and Smith (1983) reported that spatial ability terminology had become very confusing. Spatial factors were referred to in a number of different ways by various researchers, and sometimes the same term was used to identify entirely different abilities by different authors. Three spatial factors seemed to emerge from an analysis of the literature at that time. Eliot and Smith (1983) explained these factors and their relationships with other terms of the day as follows:

Spatial relations and orientation (SR-O) seemed to be a composite of Thurstone’s

S1 and S2 and a composite of French’s Space and Spatial Orientation. It is the

ability to comprehend the nature of an arrangement of elements within a visual

38

stimulus pattern primarily with respect to the examinee’s body as the frame of

reference.

Visualization (Vz) was found in results from tests that required mental

manipulation.

Kinestetic imagery (K) appeared to be the same as Thurstone’s K-factor, and

closely linked to El Koussy’s k.

From the 1960s and onward, research on spatial ability exploded. Some authors have conducted investigations attempting to further explain the complex nature of spatial ability, (Eliot, 2002; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Lohman, 1988; McGee, 1979), while others have attempted to create models that explain spatial ability (D'Costa, 2005; Guilford,

1967; Wattanawaha & Clements, 1982). One of the largest areas of research in the past

50 years has focused on individual differences in spatial ability. This research will be summarized in later sections of this chapter.

What is Spatial Ability?

From the factor studies regarding the nature of human intelligence described above, it has been found that one of the most consistently identified factors is spatial ability. This mental ability is a complex concept which researchers have struggled to define, describe, characterize, and assess for nearly 100 years. Understanding the fundamentals of this mental ability is complicated, and it is rendered even more difficult because authors aren’t sure whether it is a single ability or a mixture of various abilities. 39

Many of the definitions of spatial ability are based on the nature of the tests used in factor analysis studies, which may or may not relate to real-world abilities. To begin to understand spatial ability, some of the prevailing definitions and descriptions of this trait are discussed below.

In general, spatial ability has been defined as the cognitive skill or ability to reason or mentally represent spatial relationships, visual and nonvisual, and to anticipate the course and outcome of mental transformations applied to those relations (Berg,

Hertzog, & Hunt, 1982; Michaelides, 2003; Reio, Czarnolewski, & Eliot, 2004; Rilea,

2008). It has been described as entailing “visualization and mental transformation of two- and three-dimensional images” (Jardine & Martin, 1984, p. 345), and as the ability to generate, represent, retain, recall, and transform abstract visual symbolic images and information (Cherney, 2008; Kyllonen, Lohman, & Snow, 1984; Linn & Petersen, 1985;

Lohman, 1996). However, Guay (1980) argued that it is the mental manipulation involved, and not the perception or retention, which enables a task to measure spatial ability (p.5). Spatial ability has also been defined in terms factor analysis results. For example, groups of tests that load on a particular factor may all require one to mentally manipulate figures or images (Michaelides, 2003; Zimowski & Wothke, 1988). Other terms used for general spatial ability include spatial intelligence, visuospatial ability, and the Space factor.

Eliot (2000) described spatial intelligence as entailing “a very general capacity for seeing patterns and connections” and as “the capacity to put the relationships of the world together in one’s head such that the distance and the directional positioning of things

40 becomes part of an interconnected system of knowledge” (p.1). He noted that this ability is so pervasive that it is difficult to measure. It is involved in symbolic processing, it may be involved with body movement and orientation, it may operate on many levels at once, and it may interact with other cognitive functions to solve everyday problems (Eliot,

2000; Eliot, 2002). In their 1985 meta-analysis, Linn and Petersen noted that spatial ability “is an important component of intellectual ability, yet its nature remains to be clarified. Activities as disparate as perception of horizontality, mental rotation of objects, and location of simple figures within complex figures have all been referred to as measures of spatial ability” (p. 1479).

Guay and McDaniel (1977) divided spatial ability into low and high ability levels.

They described low level spatial abilities as “requiring the visualization of two- dimensional configurations, but no mental transformations”, and high level spatial abilities as being “characterized as requiring the visualization of three-dimensional configurations, and the mental manipulation of these visual images” (p. 211).

Based on factor analytic and other types of studies, authors have differentiated various subtypes of spatial ability. Two of the most commonly cited types of spatial ability are spatial visualization and spatial orientation (Carter, LaRussa, & Bodner, 1987;

Guilford & Lacey, 1947; Jardine & Martin, 1984; Kaufman, 2007; McGee, 1979;

Newcombe, Bandura, & Taylor, 1983). Visualization, visuospatial ability, and spatial visualization are occasionally discussed as separate entities, however in much of the literature these terms are used interchangeably. Other subtypes or facets of spatial ability discussed in the literature include spatial relations, spatial perception, closure speed,

41 closure flexibility, mental rotation, and spatial scanning, just to name a few (Guilford,

Fruchter, & Zimmerman, 1952; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Lohman, 1988; Lohman, 1989;

Lohman, 1996; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995).

At its simplest, spatial visualization involves the ability to mentally manipulate information of a spatial nature. This mental manipulation may be in the form of transformations, rotations, reflections, and/or inversions (Bishop, 1980; Bock &

Kolakowski, 1973; Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976; Guilford, Fruchter, &

Zimmerman, 1952; Guilford & Lacey, 1947; Lord, 1990; McGee, 1976). It is generally agreed that the information is complex, or the mental processes involved are difficult or multifaceted. Patterns may need to be rearranged, or internal configurations of the visualized object may need to be mentally altered (Linn & Petersen, 1985; Lohman,

1996; Martin-Dorta, Saorin, & Contero, 2008; McGee, 1979; Rilea, 2008; Voyer, Voyer,

& Bryden, 1995). Stimuli in tests used to measure this factor may be two-dimensional, but are more generally thought to be three dimensional (Martin-Dorta, Saorin, & Contero,

2008; McGee, 1979). Guay (1980) summed up the uniqueness of spatial visualization by saying that mental manipulation is the critical component that sets this factor apart from other spatial abilities. Some researchers differentiate visualization from other types of spatial ability because it seems to require complicated, multistep processing of spatial information which can be kept in mind even when there are distractions (Ben-Chaim,

Lappan, & Houang, 1988; Hassler, Birbaumer, & Feil, 1985; Linn & Petersen, 1985;

Rilea, 2008; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). Spatial visualization is commonly associated with tests that require folding or unfolding of patterns, displacement or

42 restructuring of internal components of a figure, and serial operations to complete a task

(Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976; Guilford & Lacey, 1947; Lohman, 1988; Martin-

Dorta, Saorin, & Contero, 2008; McGee, 1979).

Another component of spatial ability identified by multiple authors is spatial orientation. McGee (1979) summarized works from Thurstone, French, Guildford and

Lacey, and Ekstrom to develop the following definition for spatial orientation:

…the comprehension of the arrangement of elements within a visual stimulus

pattern and the aptitude to remain unconfused by the changing orientation in

which a spatial configuration may be presented, and the ability to determine

spatial orientation with respect to one’s body (p. 897).

Researchers tend to agree that what differentiates spatial orientation from spatial visualization is the concept that relationships are viewed and/or manipulated from the perspective of one’s own body (Bishop, 1980; Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976;

Guilford, 1967; Lohman, 1988; McGee, 1979). In addition, spatial orientation tends to involve moving or mentally viewing a configuration as a whole, or determining how an object or figure will appear from a new or different perspective (Ekstrom, French, &

Harman, 1976; Hassler, Birbaumer, & Feil, 1985; Lohman, 1988; Lord, 1990). Linn &

Petersen (1985) described a similar ability wherein subjects must determine spatial relationships with respect to the orientation of their own bodies, in spite of distracting information, and called it spatial perception (p. 1482). They found that this ability was also factored out in tests which required disembedding activities or the ability to overcome distracting cues.

43

One of the most widely studied aspects of spatial ability is the facility to mentally rotate an object. Although some authors see this as an embedded aspect of spatial visualization, Linn and Petersen (1985) viewed mental rotation as a unique type of spatial ability. Their meta-analysis of gender differences in spatial ability led them, and consequently many other researchers, to conclude that mental rotation involves the ability to retain a two- or three-dimensional object in the mind, mentally rotate it quickly and accurately in space, and then compare it to another object to determine similarity or differences (De Lisi & Cammarano, 1996; Guillot, Champely, Batier, Thiriet, & Collet,

2007; Heil & Jansen-Osmann, 2008; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Moe, Meneghetti, &

Cadinu, 2009; Monahan, Harke, & Shelley, 2008; Rilea, 2008). Tests that measure this ability have consistently demonstrated the largest gender differences, especially the mental rotation tests of Shepard and Metzler (1971), and Vandenburg and Kuse (1978).

These consistent gender differences are one of the things that differentiate this ability from spatial visualization and spatial orientation (Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer,

& Bryden, 1995).

Although less commonly discussed in the spatial literature, other sub-factors of this ability have been described. Closure speed (sometimes called Speed of Closure) is an ability to quickly identify ambiguous visual stimuli or visual patterns even if distorted or obscured in some way. It involves the ability to quickly identify an incomplete or distorted picture when the subject does not know what the pattern is in advance (Ekstrom,

French, & Harman, 1976; Lohman, 1988; Lohman, 1996). Closure flexibility (or

Flexibility of Closure) is an ability to quickly identify visual patterns even if distorted or

44 obscured in some way when the subject does know what the pattern is in advance

(Lohman, 1988; Lohman, 1996). It is also described as the ability to hold a given visual percept or configuration in mind so as to disembed it from other well defined perceptual material (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976, p. 19). Perceptual speed involves finding or accurately comparing figures and symbols, or quickly matching visual stimuli (Ekstrom,

French, & Harman, 1976; Lohman, 1988; Lohman, 1996). Perceptual speed has been called of visual figural units or evaluation of figural units by Guilford in his

Structure of Intellect model (Guilford, 1967), speed of perception by Thurstone, and

Gestalt perception by French (Lohman, 1988; Lohman, 1996). Serial Integration is described as an ability to integrate temporally spaced visual stimuli (Lohman, 1988).

Spatial scanning (sometimes called Planning Speed) involves activities like scanning for a correct answer prior to marking a test item (Lohman, 1988), or quickly exploring wide or complicated visual fields (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). It has been described when maze or path-finding tests are included in spatial batteries (Lohman, 1988).

As can be seen for the above summary of research findings, spatial ability is a highly complex concept. Researchers still do not completely agree on the nature of this ability. While some authors conduct studies as if spatial ability is it a solitary trait, others view it as a complex set of multiple abilities. Definitions are also varied, and frequently conflict with each other. In some cases spatial factors are only defined in terms of the tests that load on a particular factor (Guilford, 1967; Guilford, Fruchter, & Zimmerman,

1952; Guilford & Lacey, 1947).

45

Much of the current literature on spatial ability is concerned with individual differences in this type of intelligence. This literature can roughly be grouped into biological causes and environmental causes of individual differences in ability. Relevant studies in these areas are summarized below.

Individual Differences in Spatial Ability

Research has shown that individuals vary in their ability to visualize mental concepts and to manipulate spatial relationships. The reason for these differences is not clearly understood, and comprehending the underlying causes of these differences has been a focus of hundreds of research studies. One intriguing question is: Are we born with a certain spatial ability, or can we develop and refine it? Biologic factors have been proposed by numerous authors as the cause for spatial ability differences. Gender, chronological age, brain organization, processing strategy employed, the effects of hormones, genetic influences, and personality characteristics have all been proposed as biologic reasons for spatial ability differences. Another school of thought relates to environmental causes for differences in spatial ability. The ways children are raised, the toys they play with, and the activities they engage in may all be involved in developing a sense of spatial awareness. The types of tests and the mechanisms for testing have also been implicated in determining whether differences are seen in spatial abilities. The following sections will discuss some of the potential biological, environmental, and mechanical causes for variations in spatial ability.

46

Biologic Factors Related to Individual Differences in Spatial Ability

One biologic factor, gender, has been widely researched in studies of individual differences in spatial ability. Gender differences in certain intellectual abilities have been established, and are widely accepted for verbal and qualitative reasoning (Maccoby &

Jacklin, 1974). There is also a widely held belief that men tend to perform better than women on tests of spatial ability (Guay, 1978; Hyde, Geiringer, & Yen, 1975; McGee,

1979). Researchers have been trying to understand this phenomenon since the 1930s.

In 1974, Maccoby and Jacklin performed an extensive review of the available literature on sex differences in intelligence, achievement, and social behavior. Sex differences in spatial ability favoring males were clearly established. In an extensive review of the literature, McGee (1979) stated that male superiority on spatial visualization and orientation tasks is among the most persistent of individual differences in all of the abilities literature (p. 895). Hyde (1981) reanalyzed the studies in the

Maccoby and Jacklin research and found that the gender differences were consistent and replicable, although the differences were not large. Two other meta-analyses corroborated the findings on gender differences in spatial ability, but found that the differences varied by the groups of tests used and the ability measured (Linn & Petersen,

1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995).

Linn and Petersen (1985) conducted a meta-analysis of spatial ability literature from 1974 through 1982. Their goal was to further clarify the magnitude, nature, and age

47 of first occurrence of gender differences. Their study was informed by four research perspectives: comparisons of different populations, factor studies comparing different spatial tasks, identification of the cognitive processes used to solve spatial tasks, and identification of the various strategies used to solve spatial tasks. Their analyses of 172 effect sizes enabled them to name and describe three categories of spatial ability: spatial perception, mental rotation, and spatial visualization.

Spatial perception tasks involve determining spatial relationships with respect to one’s own body orientation in spite of distraction. Tests that require the examinee to determine horizontality such as the Rod and Frame Test and the Water Level Test are examples of measures for this ability. Gender effect sizes favoring males for this category of spatial tasks were significant for those over 18 years of age and amounted to about two thirds of a standard deviation.

The second ability category described by Linn and Petersen was spatial visualization, which they noted is an ability commonly associated with performing complicated multistep spatial tasks. Common tests associated with this ability include paper folding, surface development, and hidden figures tasks. These tasks generally tend to require more analytical cognitive processing strategies. Gender differences on tests of spatial visualization were homogenous over the life span, but small, with an average effect size of .13 which failed to reach significance. They concluded that this type of spatial ability was equally difficult for both males and females.

The third category of spatial ability described by Linn and Petersen was mental rotation. In earlier spatial ability studies mental rotation was considered an aspect of

48 spatial visualization. However, research concerned with the Vandenberg and Kuse

(1978) Mental Rotations Test had shown it to have a low relationship to other factors in test batteries and, unlike other tests of spatial visualization, to have low correlations with verbal tests. Linn and Petersen suggest that this type of test may identify a spatial ability that is separate from visualization.

Tests that fall into this category include those mental rotation tasks developed by

Shepard and Metzler (1971) and Vandenberg and Kuse (1978). Researchers have claimed that these types of tasks involve holistic, or Gestalt types of mental processing to successfully solve (Guay, 1978; McGee, 1979; Shepard & Metzler, 1971), although this is still questioned by others (Just & Carpenter, 1985) because of the complexity of the items and the speeded nature of mental rotation tests. For mental rotation tasks analyzed in the Linn and Petersen (1985) study, sex differences favoring males were seen as soon as mental rotation could be measured in subjects, were homogenous over all of the ages included in the study, and were largest in studies which had used the Vandenberg and

Kuse test. In fact, the overall estimator for effect size for gender differences on this test was .94, almost an entire standard deviation (Linn & Petersen, 1985).

Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden (1995) also sought to determine the magnitude of sex differences in spatial ability. Using meta-analysis techniques, they examined studies in the spatial literature from 1974 through 1993 which yielded a total of 286 effect sizes. In attempting to categorize spatial abilities, their results replicated the work of Linn and

Petersen (1985) and the same three categories were used: spatial perception, mental rotation, and spatial visualization. Results of the overall analysis of the 286 studies

49 revealed an effect size of .37 for gender which demonstrated that “sex differences in spatial abilities favoring males are highly significant” (p. 253). The sex difference in spatial perception was found to have an overall effect size of .44, with a small and gradual increase between the ages of 13 and adulthood. Mental rotation tasks again showed the highest overall effect of .56, but this study found that the magnitude of gender differences increased from adolescence to adulthood. Finally, small gender differences were found in spatial visualization, increasing from .02 at age 13 to .23 for adults with an average effect size of .13.

In addition to the above large studies, various other authors have reported gender differences in spatial abilities favoring males. Guay (1978) found that males performed significantly better than females on tests designed to measure surface development, object rotation, and coordination of viewpoint tasks. Using the Heinrich Spatial

Visualization Test, Chen (1995) found significant gender differences in spatial ability with male Taiwanese college students outperforming female students. Dunn and Eliot

(1999) found significant correlations between gender and performance on the CTY spatial battery which included seven subtests. Studies using various versions of the

Mental Rotations and other tests have demonstrated fairly large and reliable male superiority on test scores, approaching a Cohen’s d value of 1.5 (Castelli, Corazzini, &

Geminiani, 2008; Colom, Contreras, Arend, Leal, & Santacreu, 2004; Geiser, Lehmann,

& Eid, 2008; Johnson & Bouchard, 2007; Kaufman, 2007; Keith, Reynolds, Patel, &

Ridley, 2008; Parsons, et al., 2004; Peters, Laeng, Latham, Jackson, Zaiyouna, &

Richardson, 1995; Peters, Lehmann, Takahira, Takeuchi, & Jordan, 2006). Although the

50 age at which sex differences in spatial ability emerges is still questionable, it has been claimed that male superiority in spatial tasks can be seen as early as three to four months of age (Moore & Johnson, 2008; Quinn & Liben, 2008).

Male superiority in spatial ability is not always supported by research. Olson and

Eliot (1986) found no correlation between gender and the composite scores on the Spatial

Dimensionality Test (a group of 6 spatial ability tests). Michaelides (2003) found no significant gender differences in children performing a block rotation task. It has also been reported that gender differences on some tests disappear when subjects are given instructions that indicate the task is a measure of empathy or general intellect (Caplan,

MacPherson, & Tobin, 1985; Michaelides, 2003), or when tests are scored in non- standard ways (Goldstein, Haldane, & Mitchell, 1990; Stumpf, 1993). Some claim that gender differences are disappearing altogether (Feingold, 1988; Stumpf, 1989), but these claims are in dispute (Halpern, 1989; Hedges & Nowell, 1995).

Some studies of individual differences in spatial ability have advocated that brain function and organization is the biologic factor responsible. It is believed that male and female brains perform neural tasks differently. Studies of the brains, utilizing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), have demonstrated that different areas of the brain, mainly the parietal lobe, are activated when performing either a mental rotation or an orientation type of spatial task (Keehner, Guerin, Miller, Turk, & Hegarty, 2006;

Wantzel, Hamsta, Anastakis, Matsumoto, & Cusimano, 2002; Zacks, 2008). Other studies have focused on differential organization of the brain’s hemispheres, termed lateralization. Male brains tend to be more asymmetrically organized and it is believed

51 that language processing is predominant in the left hemisphere while perception and non- verbal processes like spatial ability are mainly processed in the right hemisphere which may partially explain gender differences in spatial ability (Levy, 1976; Newcombe,

Dubas, & Baenninger, 1989; Rilea, 2008; Rilea, Roskos-Ewoldsen, & Boles, 2004;

Zacks, 2008).

A related avenue of research has examined handedness in relation to individual differences in spatial ability. Studies of the relationship between handedness and spatial ability are conflicting. If spatial processing in centered in the right hemisphere and asymmetrical hemispheric processing improves spatial ability as laterality theories predict, then it would be expected that left-handed females would have the poorest spatial performance while left-handed males should have the best performance on spatial tasks

(McGee, 1976). Right-handed subjects were found to perform better than left-handed subjects on spatial tasks by some researchers, but not on all tasks or in all situations

(Guay, 1978; Levy, 1976; McGee, 1976; Yen, 1975). No significant effects for handedness were reported by various authors (Ecuyer-Dab, Tremblay, Joanette, &

Passini, 2004; Li, Zhu, & Nuttall, 2003; Peters, Laeng, Latham, Jackson, Zaiyouna, &

Richardson, 1995); however, Reio, Czarnolewski, and Eliot (2004) found that three- dimensional rotation/visualization and maze tracing tasks showed a small but significant relationship with tendency to left-handedness.

Another potential biologic source of variance in spatial ability scores may be related to the influence of sex hormones. If differences in spatial ability emerge early in life, prenatal hormones may play a role by having some effect on brain organization and

52 function (Delgado & Prieto, 1996; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Moe, Meneghetti, & Cadinu,

2009; Moe & Pazzaglia, 2006; Moore & Johnson, 2008; Quinn & Liben, 2008). Moore

(2008) cited studies in which positive associations are found between spatial ability in seven to twelve year olds and their androgen levels during gestation, postulating that higher androgen levels in utero influence the development and organization of the nervous system which in turn contributes to spatial ability.

On the other hand, sex hormone levels may influence brain organization as a child goes through puberty, and if this is so, ability differences would emerge in adolescence.

Since males typically outperform females on tests of spatial ability, and since males are typically later in maturing than females, it has been suggested that there is a relationship between the brain’s exposure to sex hormones and the neural processing needed for spatial tasks (Linn & Petersen, 1985; McGee, 1979; Sanders & Soares, 1986). Studies of spatial ability in relation to the timing of puberty have found that both males and females who mature later tend to have better scores on measures of mental rotation, but differences tend to be relatively small (McGee, 1979; Newcombe & Bandura, 1983;

Sanders & Soares, 1986).

Because men tend to perform better on spatial tests, some believe there is a genetic cause for women’s poorer performance on these measures (Hyde, Geiringer, &

Yen, 1975). This type of biologic cause could explain differences that emerge in young children (Linn & Petersen, 1985). Traits related to the transmission of a gene on the X- chromosome are said to be X-linked; if the trait is recessive, more males than females would be affected (Goldstein, Haldane, & Mitchell, 1990; McGee, 1976). Bock and

53

Kilakowski (1973) provided evidence which supports the notion that spatial ability is influenced by an X-linked genetic factor, determined by a recessive gene on the X- chromosome. McGee (1979) stated that different assessments of spatial abilities may have different genetic structures. Other authors have explored genetic factors as a cause of ability differences and found little or no evidence for an X-chromosome linkage

(Guttman, 1974; Jardine & Martin, 1984; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Vandenberg & Kuse,

1978). Studies related to practice and training in spatial abilities show that both genders can improve their performance significantly after even short periods of training and this casts doubt on the theory that there is a genetic reason behind differences in spatial ability

(Conner, Schackman, & Serbin, 1978; Connor, Serbin, & Schackman, 1977).

Another avenue of study in the biologic causes of individual differences in spatial ability assessed the influence of personality characteristics. Smith (1964) concluded that those with higher levels of spatial ability tended to be more emotionally stable, possess more self-confidence, have more initiative, and were less neurotic. He also compiled literature that showed that high spatial ability on some types of tests tended to be associated with introversion, aggressiveness, and schizothymic, desurgent, delinquent or asocial traits. Dunn and Eliot (1993) attempted to replicate Smith’s conclusions but found no significant relationship between introversion and results from their battery of spatial tests.

54

Environmental Factors Related to Individual Differences in Spatial Ability

Many studies have been done which allude to the relationship between spatial ability and participation in certain leisure, academic, professional, and cultural endeavors

(Baenninger & Newcombe, 1989; Lunneborg & Lunneborg, 1984; McDaniel, Guay, Ball,

& Kolloff, 1978; Newcombe & Bandura, 1983; Signorella, Krupa, & Jamison, 1986;

Zimowski & Wothke, 1988). Environmental factors which might influence spatial abilities have been widely studied. In their investigation with infants, Moore and

Johnson (2008) suggested that the factors which influence mental rotation abilities in infants are unknown, but that “by 3 months after birth…male and female infants have already experienced a social world that treats them differently” (p. 1065). Children typically play with toys and engage in activities that are somewhat gender-specific.

Many of the toys and activities (e.g., construction toys, model building, certain computer games, and some sports), especially those associated with males tend to be more spatial in nature and may be somewhat responsible for individual differences in spatial ability

(Voyer, Nolon, & Voyer, 2000).

Numerous questionnaires have been developed to assess patterns of participation in spatial and nonspatial activities, and how these activities compare to measures of spatial ability (Baenninger & Newcombe, 1989; Eliot & Czarnolewski, 2007; McDaniel,

Guay, Ball, & Kolloff, 1978; Newcombe & Bandura, 1983; Olson, Eliot, & Hardy, 1988;

Quaiser-Pohl & Lehmann, 2002; Signorella, Krupa, & Jamison, 1986). Using spatial

55 activities questionnaires, significant, but not always large, correlations have been found between some activities and composite scores on various spatial tests. Women who engaged in activities such as tackle football, high jump, running hurdles, mechanical drawing, car repair, ice hockey, figure skating, dance, gymnastics, building models, carpentry, photography, using compasses, arranging things, and solving math riddles scored well overall on some spatial tests. Men who engaged in playing horseshoes, using hand tools, using a compass, building models, working on cars, furniture repair, and mechanical drawing did well on the some spatial tests (Baenninger & Newcombe, 1989;

Lunneborg & Lunneborg, 1984; Newcombe, Bandura, & Taylor, 1983; Olson & Eliot,

1986; Olson, Eliot, & Hardy, 1988; Signorella, Krupa, & Jamison, 1986; Vandenberg,

Kuse, & Vogler, 1985; Voyer, Nolon, & Voyer, 2000). In general, preference for masculine activities is associated with better spatial performance in women but not in men (Quaiser-Pohl & Lehmann, 2002; Signorella, Krupa, & Jamison, 1986).

Some authors believe that experience with video games is related positively with spatial ability (De Lisi & Cammarano, 1996; Green & Bavelier, 2006). Playing computer games that require spatial knowledge has been related to better performance on mental rotation and other types of spatial tests, but not always for both genders (Cherney, 2008;

De Lisi & Cammarano, 1996; Lunneborg & Lunneborg, 1984; Quaiser-Pohl & Lehmann,

2002).

Because males tend to do better on spatial tests, and because males and females tend to engage in different activities from a young age, McDaniel et al. (1978) developed the Spatial Experience Questionnaire to obtain data about life experiences and compared

56 results to spatial ability scores. The questionnaire consisted of 25 activities which were classified by a panel of judges as either masculine, feminine, or gender neutral. These included activities such as playing checkers, skiing, sketching clothes or automobile designs, and arranging furniture. Subjects who completed the questionnaire were asked to identify their “Extent of Participation” and their “Extent of Enjoyment” for each of the

25 items. The second part of the questionnaire included four spatial-type questions about constructing mental maps, visualizing directions, picturing mathematical equations, and visualizing the rotation of a cube and asked the subject to estimate the difficulty of each.

The authors determined that the questionnaire was able to significantly differentiate men into high and low spatial ability groups based on the total score and both the participation and the enjoyment scores. For women, the total score and the participation sub-score were both able to differentiate between high and low spatial ability females. There was no significant difference between the high and low spatial women for the enjoyment score. Further analysis of the questionnaire results revealed that for men, participation in four activities had the most influence in discriminating between high and low spatial ability: making or repairing furniture, playing pool, reading a map or using a compass, and using machine tools. For women, participation in the following had the most influence in discriminating between high and low spatial abilities: sketching house plans, using hand tools, skiing, putting together jigsaw puzzles, building models, weaving or macramé, and reading a map or using a compass (McDaniel, Guay, Ball, & Kolloff,

1978). Results of this study also suggested that girls who possess superior spatial skills

57 tend to be involved in activities that are deemed to be more masculine in nature (for example using hand tools or building models).

Tests of Spatial Ability

Some of the earliest tests of spatial ability include the Maze Test by Porteus

(1915), paper form board (1918), and the Spatial Relations and Hands Tests by Thurstone

(1918) (Eliot & Smith, 1983; Smith, 1964). Other commonly cited tests in spatial research include the Progressive Matrices by Raven (Caplan, MacPherson, & Tobin,

1985; Guay, 1980; Guttman, 1974; Hegarty & Kozhevnikov, 1999; Johnson & Bouchard,

2007; Zimowski & Wothke, 1988), the Water Level and Rod and Frame Tests by Witkin

(Allen & Hogeland, 1978; Caplan, MacPherson, & Tobin, 1985; Hyde, Geiringer, & Yen,

1975; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995), and the Spatial Relations subtest of the Differential Aptitude Test (Anastakis, Hamstra, & Matsumoto, 2000;

Barratt, 1953; Cochran & Wheatley, 1989; Fennema & Sherman, 1977; Fennema &

Sherman, 1978; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995; Wang, 1993).

These tests and many others that claim to be spatial measures have been found to load on factors other than spatial ability (Caplan, MacPherson, & Tobin, 1985; Eliot, 1980; Guay,

1980; Hyde, Geiringer, & Yen, 1975).

In their meta-analysis of sex differences in spatial ability, Linn and Petersen

(1985) classified spatial ability into three categories: spatial perception, mental rotation, and spatial visualization. Spatial tests were analyzed as they related to these categories.

58

Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden (1995) suggested that this classification, although somewhat vague and in need of further refinement, was reasonably successful in categorizing spatial ability. Because this model of spatial ability has been supported in these meta-analytic studies, it will serve as the model for this research.

Spatial perception (also sometimes referred to as spatial orientation) requires determination of relationships with respect to one’s own body (Linn & Petersen, 1985;

Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). Tests that were found to load on this category of spatial ability in Linn and Petersen’s study included the Rod and Frame Test, the Water Level

Test, and some tests that focused on disembedding or overcoming distracting cues. Other processes that characterize spatial perception tests include the use of gravitational vertical or kinesthetic cues.

Spatial visualization was found to involve complicated, multistep processes when dealing with spatial information. Tests that were found to measure this ability included paper folding and form board tests, surface development, hidden or embedded figures tests, and the Spatial Relations subtest of the Differential Aptitude Test. Tests found in this category tend to need more analytic-type strategies for solution, or an ability to switch solution strategies as needed (Linn & Petersen, 1985).

The third category of spatial ability in this theoretical model was described as mental rotation. Linn and Petersen (1985) found this to be a dimension of ability separate from spatial visualization. It is generally believed that tasks in this category are solved by Gestalt or holistic processing strategies (Guay, 1980; Lohman, 1996; Shepard &

Metzler, 1971). Mental rotations tests developed by Shepard and Metzler, and

59

Vandenberg and Kuse, the Primary Mental Abilities Space Rotations Test by Thurstone, and the Flags and Cards Tests by French are commonly used to test this ability. The three categories differentiated by Linn and Petersen will serve as the model for spatial ability in this study. With that in mind, tests that are purported to measure these abilities will be discussed. The following four tests include items meant to measure spatial abilities in each of these categories.

The Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT)

The Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT) was developed by Roland Guay and published in 1976. There are two versions of the test: one with 36 total items, and one with 90 total items. Both versions of the test are divided into three sections or subtests: Developments, Rotations, and Views. Depending on the version, each of the sections has either twelve or thirty questions. The three subtests were developed to measure a different aspect of spatial ability according to the author. Guay (1976) states that the test can be administered either individually or in a group setting, and that it is appropriate for ages 13 and older. Reliability and validity information for the composite test and each of the subtests is discussed in Chapter Three of this document.

In a study of 217 college undergraduates, Guay (1978) found that males scored significantly higher than females overall, and on all three sections of the 36-item version of the PSVT. The Rotations and Views subtests were found to correlate significantly with the spatial relations subtest of the Differential Aptitude Test which suggests they

60 measure the same mental ability (Kovac, 1989). However, in Kovac’s study with middle school children, the test was found to be gender neutral which caused him to question if this is truly a spatial test (Kovac, 1989).

The Visualization of Developments subtest is comprised of either twelve or thirty two-dimensional developments (patterns) which can be mentally folded into a three- dimensional figure. Each development represents the inside of a three-dimensional figure with a shaded portion representing the bottom surface of the figure (Guay, 1978). Each question in this section consists of the development and five answer options, with only one correct option per item (Guay, 1976). The test has been reported to demonstrate significant male-favored gender differences in elementary school and college students, and a similar significant gender difference in math achievement in the study of younger children (Guay, 1978; Guay & McDaniel, 1977). Guay (1978) reported that this test relies on analytic processing strategies, which he described as involving “explicit trial- and-error checking of relationships between different parts of a figure” (p. 5).

The Rotations subtests consist of twelve or thirty questions which examine how a subject is able to mentally rotate a three-dimensional object. The test was developed as a research tool to measure spatial visualization (McGee’s definition). It has been reported that this test was developed on the basis of two assumptions: “(1) that all existing spatial ability tests vary in the degree to which they evoke the use of analytic versus gestalt processing, and (2) that tests that maximize gestalt processing while minimizing analytic processing are the best measures of spatial ability” (Bodner & Guay, 1997, p. 1436).

Previous studies with this test by the author are noted as evidence of construct validity

61 based on reported holistic processing strategies used to solve items (Guay, 1980). Each question contains a prompt and five answer options with only one correct choice.

Subjects are shown a three dimensional sample object rotated from one position to a different position. They are then shown the question prompt figure and asked to mentally picture this item when it is rotated the same way as the sample, then choose the option that represents its appearance if rotated in this way (Guay, 1976). The thirty item version of the test is reported appropriate for those aged 13 and older. A time limit of 20 minutes is recommended (Guay, 1980). Guay (1978, 1980) reported that this test was designed to minimize analytic processing, and that it appears to require a gestalt or holistic processing strategy where subjects solve items by mentally rotating figures as wholes rather than as parts. The test is arranged with four levels of difficulty in increasing order: 90 degree rotation on one axis, 180 degrees on one axis, 90 degrees on two axes, and 90 degrees on one axis with 180 degrees on another axis. Questions are scored as either right or wrong with a maximum score of 30 and no correction for guessing (Guay, 1980). Internal consistency reliability based on previous studies by Guay and others was reported to be between .82 and .92 (K-R 20) (Bodner & Guay, 1997; Carter, LaRussa, & Bodner, 1987;

Cochran & Wheatley, 1989; Guay, 1980). This test was found to correlate reasonably well with the Shepard-Metzler mental rotation test (r = .61) and with the DAT-SR (r =

.72) which suggests construct validity (Cochran & Wheatley, 1989; Guay, 1980).

Reported significant male-favored gender differences also add to the construct validity

(Guay, 1980). This test has been found to show a significant relationship with participation in spatial activities (Guay, 1978).

62

Although this test shares some characteristics with the Vandenberg modifications of the Shepard-Metzler rotations test, Bodner (1997) notes several differences: the

Purdue Rotations test items are rotated around their natural axis rather than a strictly vertical axis, the Purdue test items may have hidden parts, and test items may be rotated around more than one axis.

Kovacs (1989) administered this test along with the Visualization of Views subtest of the PSRT and the Differential Aptitude Test to middle-school aged children to evaluate the validity of spatial ability measurement instruments. Statistical analysis of the scores from the three tests showed significant correlations for all three suggesting convergent validity (all tests measure the same construct). Differences between the tests were found when scores were compared to course grades for the students. The Rotations test was correlated positively with “Practical Arts, Science, Social Studies, and Math” (p.

32), the Views test with Science, Math, and Vocabulary, and the DAT with Math only.

None of the tests showed significant gender differences in scores which is at odds with the literature showing that tests of spatial skills favor males. While a majority of those interviewed reported a more holistic strategy in solving items, others reported distinctly analytical strategies for the Rotations test. Based on these criteria, Kovac questioned the validity of this test and whether it was truly measuring spatial ability. On the other hand, this finding could be seen as support for Linn & Petersen’s (1985) contention that gender differences don’t reliably emerge until around the age of 13.

The third subtest of the PSVT is the Views test. Again, there is a twelve item version and a thirty item version. The test presents the subject with a three-dimensional

63 object, drawn within an imaginary cube. One corner of the cube is designated as the viewpoint and the examinee must imagine how the three-dimensional object will appear from that viewpoint. There are five answer options with one correct choice (Guay,

1976). Males have been found to score significantly higher on this test, although no significant relationship was found between this test and birth order, handedness, or spatial experience (Guay, 1978). Gender differences are not always found on this test, and items may be solved more by analytical processes than by spatial processes (Kovac, 1989).

Hidden Figures Tests

There are multiple variations of this type of test, variously called the Hidden

Figures Test, Hidden Patterns Test, Embedded Figures Test, Embedded Pattern Test, or the Group Embedded Figures Test. There are versions for both adults and children.

While these types of tests are commonly described as measures of field dependence/independence (Anastakis, Hamstra, & Matsumoto, 2000; Linn & Petersen,

1985; Lord, 1990; Witkin, Moore, Goodenough, & Cox, 1977), they have also been used to evaluate spatial ability. Ekstrom, French, and Harman (1976) described this type of test as a measure for Flexibility of Closure. It has also been described as a marker measure of figure-ground perception (Davis & Eliot, 1994, p. 399). Linn and Petersen

(1985) classified these types of tests as measures of spatial visualization.

The ETS Hidden Figures Test (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976) is an adaptation of the Embedded Figures Test described by Witkin (1977). The difficulty

64 level for this test is described as high, and it is described as appropriate for adolescents and adults. For this test, the subject is presented with five geometric figures labeled A through E at the top of the page. Below the figures are eight complex patterns in which one of the simple patterns has been embedded. The subject must determine which of the simple patterns is hidden within the complex pattern. The test is given in two parts.

There are 16 complex patterns in each part of the test and subjects are given 12 minutes to complete these 16 items. Scoring for the test is the number of correct answers “minus the number marked incorrectly” (p. 21) to control for guessing (Ekstrom, French, &

Harman, 1976), and subjects should be cautioned that guessing is not to their advantage.

In a 1995 meta-analysis by Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden, 34 studies were included which either used the Embedded Figures Test (30) or the Hidden Figures Test (2) as a measure of sex differences in Spatial Visualization. Some of the studies were done with groups as young as six years old, and others used ages ranging up to 82 years old, however most of the studies were done with young adults (college undergraduate students). Fourteen of the studies reported significant gender differences favoring males,

19 reported no significant gender differences in scores, and one study reported females scored significantly better. The gender difference effect sizes were larger when these types of tests were administered in individual settings than when given in group settings leading the authors to suggest that there may be differences in the ways that men and women respond to testing situations. These authors (Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995) reported that gender differences were seen as early as 14 years on these tests, but overall the tests in this group are inconsistent in demonstrating reliable gender differences in

65 spatial ability. If this is true, it reflects the findings of Linn and Petersen (1985) who noted that there was little evidence of gender differences in the category of spatial ability which they labeled as spatial visualization.

Mental Rotations Test (MRT)

In 1971, Shepard and Metzler described a spatial test which required “mental rotation in three-dimensional space” (Shepard & Metzler, 1971, p. 703). Their test stimuli consisted of figures comprised of 10 contiguous blocks pictured in a way so as to appear three dimensionally. Subjects were presented with a pair of these figures, one of which was rotated in respect to the other one. The figures could be the same or could be mirror images of each other. The task was to determine quickly and accurately whether the figures were the same or different (mirror images). In 1978, Vandenberg and Kuse described a paper-and-pencil adaptation of the mental rotation test developed by Shepard and Metzler. The Vandenberg and Kuse test was described as a three-dimensional test of spatial visualization which was suitable for group administration. In their adaptation, the stimulus figure must be compared to four options rather than just one as in the original test. In the original 20-item version (Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978), each item consists of a criterion figure, two correct alternatives, and two incorrect distracters. Correct alternatives are always identical to the criterion in structure but are shown in a rotated position; the two incorrect alternatives cannot be matched to the criterion figure. For half the items in the test, the distracters are rotated mirror-images of the criterion, while

66 distracters in the other 10 items show a segment or two of the criterion figure in a rotated position (Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978, p. 599). Subjects are given 10 minutes to complete the test, and the authors recommended that items are scored as correct only when both correct alternatives are chosen, which they explained eliminated the need to correct for guessing.

It has frequently been reported that spatial tests should correlate well with mathematical and science grades, but not with grades in English and courses in humanities. In a study of college students, those enrolled in science-based programs did significantly better on the Mental Rotations Test than those enrolled in arts and humanities type programs (Peters, et al., 1995). This was found to be true, not just with

American students, but also with German and Japanese high school and college students

(Peters, 2005). The Mental Rotations Test has also been shown to correlate significantly with surgical skills and anatomy knowledge (Cohen & Hegary, 2007; Wantzel, Hamsta,

Anastakis, Matsumoto, & Cusimano, 2002).

In an effort to establish construct validity for this test, it has been compared with other commonly used spatial tests as well as verbal and reasoning tests. The test was found to correlate reasonably well with other tests of spatial ability such as the Spatial

Relations Subtest of the Differential Aptitude Test (DAT), Primary Mental Abilities-

Spatial Relations Test, Card Rotations, hidden patterns, paper form board, and Identical

Blocks; it correlated especially well with tests of spatial visualization (Cherney, 2008;

Guillot, Champely, Batier, Thiriet, & Collet, 2007; Jansen-Osmann & Heil, 2007;

Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978; Voyer, et al., 2006). A significant correlation of .51 was

67 found with the ETS Paper Folding Test (Blajenkova, Kozhevnikov, & Motes, 2006).

Cohen and Hegarty (2007) reported a nonsignificant correlation of .32 between the

Mental Rotations Test and the PSVT Visualization of Views subtest. There was a significant correlation with the DAT-SR of .61. (Kaufman, 2007).

Another common characteristic of spatial tests is a low correlation with verbal reasoning tasks. Vandenberg and Kuse (1978) found very low correlations between the

Mental Rotations Test and tests for vocabulary and verbal reasoning. Hegarty et al.

(2006) found correlations of -.01 to .05 between the MRT and verbal ability tests. These low correlations supply evidence that this test measures a trait that is separate from verbal ability, and that it can be differentiated from measures for spatial visualization (Linn &

Petersen, 1985).

One of the most reliable characteristics of this test is the differences in scores between men and women. This test consistently shows a clear gender difference in spatial ability favoring males. This difference has been seen in a variety of population samples and over a wide range of ages in the general population (De Lisi & Cammarano,

1996; Geiser, Lehmann, & Eid, 2008; Guillot, Champely, Batier, Thiriet, & Collet, 2007;

Kaufman, 2007; Li, Zhu, & Nuttall, 2003; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Peters, Laeng, Latham,

Jackson, Zaiyouna, & Richardson, 1995; Voyer, Rodgers, & McCormick, 2004). Peters et al. (1995) found significant gender differences favoring males, and that the effect of gender accounted for almost 18% of the variance in scores. In fact, in the 1985 meta- analysis by Linn and Petersen, the Mental Rotations Test yielded the largest gender difference effect sizes for all ages. Gender differences have been reported to be from .6

68 to almost an entire standard deviation unit with this and other tests of mental rotation

(Linn & Petersen, 1985; Stumpf, 1993; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). Effect sizes for the gender differences with this test have been reported as high as d = 1.01 (Cherney,

2008; Kaufman, 2007; Voyer, et al., 2006). These gender differences remain, even when scoring method, instructions, or timing differences are introduced into the administration process (Delgado & Prieto, 1996; Massa, Mayer, & Bohon, 2005; Masters, 1998; Moe &

Pazzaglia, 2006; Peters, Laeng, Latham, Jackson, Zaiyouna, & Richardson, 1995;

Stumpf, 1993; Voyer, Rodgers, & McCormick, 2004).

The Cube Comparison Test

The Cube Comparison Test is one of the two tests categorized as a measure of spatial orientation in the Educational Testing Services Kit of Factor Referenced Tests

(Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). This test is composed of representations of six- sided cubes, similar to children’s blocks. Each side is identified by a unique number, letter, or symbol. Subjects are presented with two cubes for each question, and must determine if they are the rotated images of the same cube or if the two images represent different cubes. The test is divided into two parts, each with 21 questions. Three minutes per part is recommended. It is suggested that scores be calculated as the number correct minus the number incorrect to correct for guessing.

This test was used in conjunction with other spatial ability tests to differentiate college students into low and high spatial ability groups in a study on learning in a

69 science course (Lord, 1990). It was found to load on a spatial rotation factor when included in a battery of tests administered to a large sample of adults (Johnson &

Bouchard, 2007). Gender and generational differences have been reported with this test.

In a study of twins and their parents, males performed better than females, and children performed better than their parents (Jardine & Martin, 1984).

Studies of Spatial Ability in Healthcare

Many studies of spatial ability have been done to examine the role this trait plays in fields such as engineering. In recent years, researchers have become interested in examining the nature and role of spatial ability in healthcare fields. The majority of the investigations have related to spatial ability as it relates to the study of anatomy (Dev,

Friedman, Dafoe, & Felciano, 1992; Friedman, Dev, Dafoe, Murphy, & Felciano, 1993;

Garg, 2002; Garg, Norman, & Sperotable, 2001; Guillot, Champely, Batier, Thiriet, &

Collet, 2007; Kesner & Linzey, 2005). The role of spatial ability in radiology and surgery has also been explored (Berbaum, Smoker, & Smith, 1985; Hegarty, Keehner,

Khooshabeh, & Montello, 2009; Keehner, et al., 2004b; Nilsson, Hedman, & Ahlqvist,

2007; Risucci, 2002; Sidhu, et al., 2004; Smoker, Berbaum, Luebke, & Jacoby, 1984;

Wantzel, Hamsta, Anastakis, Matsumoto, & Cusimano, 2002). Another avenue of research in spatial ability in healthcare relates to its uses as an admissions gate-keeping tool (Hegarty, Keehner, Khooshabeh, & Montello, 2009; Keehner, et al., 2004b). Despite the wealth of research, there are still many questions about spatial learning and spatial

70 ability as it relates to the medical field. Understanding how spatial ability relates to learning, skill acquisition, and job performance are still areas in need of further research

(Anastakis, Hamstra, & Matsumoto, 2000; Garg, Norman, & Sperotable, 2001; Levinson,

Weaver, Garside, McGinn, & Norman, 2007; Rochford, 1985).

In healthcare, many of the clinical tasks performed by practitioners require a spatial understanding of anatomy. Over the past 25 years, multiple studies have investigated methods and concepts involved in understanding anatomy, both basic and cross-sectional (Garg, Norman, Eva, Spero, & Sharan, 2002; Garg, Norman, &

Sperotable, 2001; Guillot, Champely, Batier, Thiriet, & Collet, 2007; Kesner & Linzey,

2005; Khalil, Payer, & Johnson, 2005; Miller, 2000; Pandey & Zimitat, 2007; Rizzolo &

Stewart, 2006; Rochford, 1985). Rosse (1995) stated “…the ultimate purpose of anatomy education is to assist the student in developing an implicit and fully internalized understanding of the three-dimensional dynamic structure of the living human body so that he or she can apply the appropriate cognitive skills when clinical problems call for anatomical reasoning” (p. 499). Learning anatomy is difficult. It requires memorization of many verbal concepts, but more importantly, it requires that one learn the spatial relationships between the various tissues and organs within the body (Friedman, Dev,

Dafoe, Murphy, & Felciano, 1993; Miller, 2000). Verbal knowledge includes such activities as classification; spatial knowledge would involve location and orientation of three-dimensional objects; the ability to recognize and name a displayed object would exhibit characteristics of both verbal and spatial knowledge (Friedman, Dev, Dafoe,

Murphy, & Felciano, 1993; Rosse, 1995). Proficiency in one area does not necessarily

71 translate into proficiency in another area. A student may be able to discuss information about a structure, but not necessarily be able to recognize or locate the object.

Understanding anatomic relationships becomes even more complex when faced with cross-sectional images. Recognizing an anatomic representation in one plane does not necessarily mean that the student will be able to recognize or locate a particular structure in an alternative visual imaging plane.

Studies on the relationship between spatial skills and anatomy achievement have shown that students with higher level spatial skills tend to do better on both spatially- related test questions and on practical anatomy examinations (Garg, 2002; Garg, Norman,

& Sperotable, 2001; Guillot, Louis, Thiriet, & Collet, 2007; Luursema, Verwey,

Kommers, & Annema, 2008; Rochford, 1985; Smoker, Berbaum, Luebke, & Jacoby,

1984). Spatial ability does not however seem to relate to non-spatial types of anatomy exam questions (Rochford, 1985).

Guillot, Champely, Batier, Thiriet, and Collet (2007) conducted a study to look at various aspects of spatial ability and their relationship with scores on anatomy examinations. They were interested specifically in field dependence/independence, mental imagery, and mental rotation. Based on the literature they reviewed, they expected students who were more field-independent, those who could more easily form mental images, and those with the ability to mentally rotate objects to do better on the examinations given. To test their hypotheses, 184 students (130 men and 54 women) enrolled in an anatomy course completed spatial and visual imagery tests. On the final anatomy examination they found that men scored significantly better than women and

72 that there was a strong relationship between the anatomy examination scores and the spatial test scores, especially the test for mental rotation. They concluded that students with higher spatial abilities, especially those related to mental rotation, would appear to be more successful in learning anatomy. This information could potentially be used to identify students who might need supplemental instruction, or to develop teaching materials to aid those students who lack this ability.

Radiology is a medical specialty that requires a high degree of spatial ability. In the mid 1980s, physicians began to rely heavily on cross-sectional imaging techniques.

This type of imaging was thought by many to require an even higher level of spatial ability since those interpreting the images were required to mentally rotate anatomical information in order to understand what they were seeing (Berbaum, Smoker, & Smith,

1985; Nilsson, Hedman, & Ahlqvist, 2007; Smoker, Berbaum, Luebke, & Jacoby, 1984).

A study undertaken to determine if those training in radiology had an aptitude for perceiving three-dimensional relations was conducted using a surface development spatial test and an author developed Visual Form Reconstruction Test (Smoker, Berbaum,

Luebke, & Jacoby, 1984). Residents’ scores on the two tests were compared with semiannual faculty ratings to determine if the Form Test was predictive of acquisition of radiologic expertise. In this study, the Visual Form Reconstruction test was found to be highly correlated with faculty ratings of resident image interpretation performance. High positive correlations were found with evaluations from neuroradiology and rotations that involved computed tomography (CT) and sonography. This supported the authors’ hypothesis that spatial ability is important in subspecialties involving cross-sectional

73 imaging methods. Although the test was not highly correlated with initial faculty ratings of residents, it was highly correlated with scores for residents in the later periods of training. These rating scores in the later periods of training tend to be more predictive of interpretive ability in radiology (Berbaum, Smoker, & Smith, 1985).

Several authors have proposed a link between spatial ability and surgical skill, but results have been inconclusive. In a short summary article, Anastakis, Hamstra, and

Matsumoto (2000) cited the following reasons for some of the conflict: there has been a lack of theoretical justification for the particular visual-spatial tests used; objective assessments of a specific surgical skill have not been used; and samples have not been homogenous when using general ratings of surgical ability. They suggested that future research should first attempt to determine the nature of individual surgical skills to determine which might require visual-spatial skills and how those skills are involved in these individual processes. They stated “this implies that surgical ability be examined within subspecialties to determine those tasks that involve constructs that lend themselves to psychomotor or visual-spatial abilities assessed” (p. 470). Using subspecialties in surgical skills could also solve an additional problem, by using more homogenous groups to evaluate visual-spatial skills. When these authors consolidated the available research, they were able to establish a hierarchy of visual-spatial skills: perceptual recognition of objects, visual imagery involving 2D representations of the reconstruction of objects from their parts, and visual imagery involving 2D and 3D whole object rotations and translations. They suggested that skills identified through a task inventory of surgical abilities be compared with this hierarchy of visual-spatial skills, and then appropriate

74 spatial tests can be chosen. They also suggested that results of research based on these principles would then have both face validity and construct validity, and ultimately would supply a more theoretical framework on which to build.

In a 2002 study by Wanzel, Hamsta, Anastakis, Matsumoto, and Cusimano surgical residents were given six spatial tests (in order of complexity: snowy-pictures, gestalt completion, shape memory, cube comparison, form board, and mental rotation) and then their ability to learn a spatially complex surgical task was evaluated. This was done to test the authors’ hypothesis that visual-spatial ability is important in development of competency for complex surgical procedures. While there was no relationship between scores on the less complex tests of spatial ability, the authors found a significant relationship between the score on the more complex mental rotation test and ability to successfully complete the complex surgical task. Residents who scored low on the complex spatial tests tended to do poorly on the surgical skill, although their performance improved after a teaching intervention. The authors suggested that this need for supplementary education for students with low scores on mental rotation tests be taken into account by developing additional educational tasks and practice situations.

In a related study, spatial ability was studied as a predictor of surgical skills in more experienced surgeons (Keehner, et al., 2004b). Spatial ability was described here as

“…referring to a set of separable but related cognitive functions concerned with representing and processing spatial information, including visualization, spatial orientation, and speeded mental rotation” (p. 71). Previous studies showed a positive correlation between spatial ability and surgical skills in residents, and the authors were

75 interested in determining if this relationship held with more experienced surgeons. The study sample was composed of 98 physicians who attended one of two videoscopic surgery courses; one group highly experienced in videoscopic surgery, and one group less experienced in this particular skill. A paper folding test to measure spatial ability, a self- completed questionnaire to estimate surgical experience, and instructor evaluations to measure competence were the tools used in the study. The authors found that spatial ability for the entire group was slightly below the published norms for the test (although they noted that the norms were developed with college aged students). While spatial ability did not differ significantly between the two groups, it was a significant predictor of the operative skill in less experienced physicians, but not in the more experienced group. They concluded that spatial ability is less of a factor with skilled surgeons because with experience even complex skills become somewhat proceduralized.

Sidhu et al. (2004) were interested in spatial abilities of endovascular surgeons.

This group of surgeons needs to perform vascular surgery, a three-dimensional task, guided by two-dimensional image displays of the affected anatomy. The authors sought to determine whether innate visuospatial ability could predict novice vascular surgeon performance, whether performance could be improved in novice surgeons after an educational intervention, and whether there was a difference between three-dimensional perception between novice and experienced vascular surgeons. To test their hypotheses a computer-based depth map was developed and tested with both expert and novice vascular surgeons. All novices were given the Surface Development Test and the Mental

Rotations Test to evaluate spatial ability. All completed an exercise using the depth map,

76 and then novices were randomized into a control and a treatment group. The treatment group received an instructional session, and then all (novices and experienced surgeons) completed another exercise with the depth map program. While they found a statistically significant difference between novices and experts prior to the educational intervention, after treatment the educated group performed more similar to the experts. Contrary to other studies, these authors found no statistical relationship between spatial ability and performance on this type of test.

In a study of dental students and dental education, Hegarty, Keehner,

Khooshabeh, and Montello (2009) examined spatial ability and its relationship with anatomy grades and dental laboratory grades. As with other professions in the health care field, dentists must be proficient in anatomy and must be able to interpret cross- sectional images of the head and mouth. As outlined above, many studies have related spatial ability with the mental rotation requirements needed to interpret these cross- sectional types of images. Dental admissions tests currently include a spatial ability component, although there is controversy over whether these types of tests are appropriate to use as part of professional program selection criteria. On one hand, some assume that performance depends on pre-existing abilities. On the other hand, others assume that a particular skill, such as spatial ability, can be acquired.

In the above Hegarty et al. 2009 study, undergraduate psychology students, and dental students in their first and fourth years of training were given multiple spatial ability tests (both available and self-developed). These tests included the Perceptual

Ability Test (PAT) which is part of the Dental Admission Test, and a test of spatial

77 ability developed by these authors (an explanation of this test can be found in Cohen and

Hegarty, 2007). The psychology students were included in the sample to determine if there were differences in spatial abilities between dental students and other groups. The spatial test scores were compared with anatomy and dental restorative laboratory grades for the dental students. Their goals with these comparisons were to determine if spatial ability enhanced success in the dental program and/or if spatial ability improved over the course of the training. Statistical analysis of the results of the tests and comparison with the selected grades revealed no significant relationship between spatial ability and anatomy grades and only a small positive significance between spatial ability and the dental laboratory grades. The PAT was most highly correlated with the dental lab grades.

They found no evidence that dental education enhances spatial ability in general; however, the ability to identify cross-sectional information in teeth was superior in fourth year students compared to first year students.

Interpretation of three-dimensional information from two-dimensional radiographic images is also a critical skill for dental professionals. Nilsson, Hedman, and

Ahlqvist (2007) considered this interpretive ability along with spatial ability in a study of dental students. When interpreting radiographic images, depth can be interpreted using cross-sectional imaging, or by using a method called parallax imaging. Parallax imaging uses an x-ray tube shift between exposures of images to simulate depth. The authors theorized that those with higher spatial ability will be able to more accurately interpret depth-related information using the parallax method than those with less spatial ability.

Spatial ability was assessed using a modified version of the Vandenberg and Kuse (1978)

78

Mental Rotations Test. The students were assessed in their interpretation skills, given training in depth localization, and then assessed in interpretation again. As expected, men scored significantly better than women on the spatial test. Final radiography test results as well as measures of improvement were both found to have a significant positive correlation with spatial ability scores. Regression models used for the analysis however, explained no more than 28% of the variance which may indicate that there are other factors of importance when dental students are learning this skill.

Summary

Spatial ability has been studied for over 100 years. Many of the tests we use today have evolved from early mechanical and performance tests. These tests were originally developed to evaluate intelligence in those who were illiterate or had difficulty with language skills. Factor analytic studies on large batteries of these tests seemed to indicate that many of these instruments were measuring abilities of a spatial nature.

Various terms have been used for this cognitive skill by researchers over the years. Spatial ability has been seen as a solitary skill by some (Berg, Hertzog, & Hunt,

1982; Zimowski & Wothke, 1988). Factor analytic studies have identified two, (McGee,

1979), three (Barratt, 1953; Guilford, Fruchter, & Zimmerman, 1952), or as many as nine spatial factors (Lohman, 1988). The terms to describe spatial skills are often confusing and sometimes conflicting. For example, spatial visualization has been referred to as S2,

V1, Vz, visualization, general visualization, visuospatial ability, or simply as spatial

79 ability (McGee, 1979). Spatial orientation has variously been referred to as the S factor, spatial relations, spatial perception, and cognition of figural systems (Guilford, Fruchter,

& Zimmerman, 1952; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Lohman, 1988; McGee, 1979).

One of the most reasonable explanations of this ability classifies spatial skills into three categories: spatial perception, spatial visualization, and mental rotation (Linn &

Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). Spatial perception and spatial orientation seem to involve tasks that rely on gravitational cues, and subjects solve these tasks by determining relationships with respect to their own body. Spatial visualization involves tasks that require mental manipulation of objects in complicated multistep processes. Mental rotation tasks are those that require rotation, inversion, or other manipulation of two- or three-dimensional figures (Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer,

& Bryden, 1995).

Studies related to individual differences in spatial ability have followed two main pathways: biologic and environmental. The major question underlying this research is:

Are we born with a certain level of spatial ability or can it be developed and/or improved? Biologic reasons for differences in spatial ability may be related to gender, genetics, brain organization, sex hormone influences, or the functional way that the brain processes visual information. Unfortunately, none of the studies have definitively shown that biologic reasons underlie one’s spatial ability. Individual differences in spatial ability between men and women have received the most attention. Gender differences are generally large for mental rotation tasks, smaller for spatial perception tasks, and inconclusive for spatial visualization tasks.

80

Environmental causes for differences in spatial ability have also been studied extensively. One of the core beliefs underlying many of these studies in this area is that the activities in which we engage and the environment in which we live strongly influences our level of spatial ability. Many of the toys and activities enjoyed by children are somewhat spatial in nature. Building blocks, model building, certain computer games, and some sports are highly spatial. Males tend to engage in these activities more often than females, and this is hypothesized to underlie the gender differences in spatial ability. Practice effects related to spatial skills have also been seen in several research studies, and this also contributes to beliefs that this skill is not just an inborn ability, but can be enhanced. But again, the evidence is not definitive.

A major problem with much of the spatial research relates to the tests that are used to measure this skill. In spite of years of research, there are no clear guidelines for what many spatial tests actually measure. Many tests claim to be measuring spatial ability, but if spatial ability is a highly complex cognitive skill, no test currently available can legitimately make the assertion that it measures the breadth of this ability.

Spatial ability is commonly believed to be positively correlated with mathematical and science abilities. Studies of engineering students have shown that these students tend to excel in spatial ability tests when compared to other students. Healthcare professionals need a strong background in math and sciences courses, and the skills needed in these professionals are often highly spatial in nature. Research in this area is somewhat limited. Anatomy knowledge and skill has been found to be positively related to spatial ability. Studies of surgeons, radiologists, and dentists have also shown that their

81 professional skills are strongly related to scores on spatial ability tests. Other healthcare professionals, such as occupational therapists, radiographers, and medical laboratory scientists rely on spatial skills to perform their required occupational job tasks. If spatial ability is a critical skill for these professionals, understanding the nature of this cognitive ability could be critical in developing teaching and testing material.

Spatial skills in healthcare professions are critical in estimating lengths and distances, in orienting one’s own body in relation to a patient, in manipulation of equipment, in mentally picturing and rotating representations of anatomy and pathology, and in examining and extracting patterns. In short, spatial skills in healthcare tend to align with the theoretical model of spatial ability proposed by Linn and Petersen (1985) and Voyer, Voyer and Bryden (1995). With that in mind, tests for this study have been chosen to represent the three categories of spatial ability proposed in these meta-analyses.

Their spatial perception term mirrored the definition that others have named spatial orientation. Tests that purport to measure this ability include the ETS Cube Comparison

Test and the Visualization of Views subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test.

Both of these tests tend to rely on an ability to examine spatial problems with relation to orientation of one’s body. Spatial visualization requires complex, multistep manipulation of spatial information and the ability to disembed spatial patterns. To examine both the complex processes and the disembedding processes, the ETS Hidden Figures Test and the

Visualization of Developments subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test will be used. The third category of spatial skills involves the mental rotation of two- and three- dimensional figures in the . The best measure of this skill is commonly

82 accepted to be the Mental Rotations Test described by Vandenberg and Kuse (1978).

The Visualization of Rotations subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test has also been shown to measure mental rotation skills. Because these tests have not been used in this population, examination of the validity and reliability for these measures must be examined before any inferences about their results can be made.

83

Chapter 3: Design and Methods

This chapter explains the methods, sampling and statistical analysis used in the study. The study is primarily quantitative in nature, supported by qualitative analysis of professional documents and task analyses. This chapter includes a description of the methods employed to investigate three research questions. The population and sample will be described, followed by the instrumentation, data collection, and statistical analysis procedures.

There are many tools available to evaluate spatial abilities. Hundreds of tests have been published in the past 100 years. However, none of these tests have been studied in allied health professional students. Prior to drawing any conclusions about spatial abilities in this population, these tools need to be evaluated for validity.

Therefore, this study has three main purposes: to evaluate the nature of spatial abilities in various healthcare professional students, to evaluate the validity evidence for tests used in measuring spatial ability in this population, and to examine the validity evidence of the

Spatial Experience Questionnaire.

Specifically, the research questions for this study are:

Research Question 1: What spatial skills are used by Allied Medical Professionals in the accomplishment of their occupational duties? Which of these skills are shared across the

84 professions? Do the professions differ in their reliance on spatial perception, spatial visualization, or mental rotation abilities?

Research Question 2: To what extent is validity evidence provided for the use of the

Mental Rotations Test, the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT), the Cube

Comparison Test, and The Hidden Figures Test to measure spatial ability in Allied

Medical students?

In order to examine this question, the following assumptions addressing the various aspects of the interpretive argument will be used:

1. Scores on the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental Rotations

Tests will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical

students in this sample.

2. Scores on the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental Rotations

Tests will demonstrate acceptable reliability.

3. The Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of Developments,

Hidden Figures, and Visualization of Rotations Tests will demonstrate criterion

related validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations

Test.

4. The Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of Views Test will exhibit

concurrent validity evidence as measures of spatial perception. The Hidden

Figures Test and the Visualization of Developments Test will exhibit concurrent

85

validity evidence as measures of spatial visualization. The Visualization of

Rotations Test and the Mental Rotations Test will exhibit concurrent validity

evidence as measures of mental rotation.

5. If the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of Developments,

Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental Rotations Tests measure

the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences

commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities

examined with the Spatial Experience Questionnaire.

Research Question 3: To what extent is validity evidence provided for the use of the

Spatial Experience Questionnaire to measure spatial ability in Allied Medical students?

Population and Sample

Population

This study was designed to evaluate the spatial abilities needed and used in Allied

Medical professions. There are many occupations that can fall under this banner, but not all have the same need of spatial skills. For that reason, the occupations studied include

Athletic Training (AT), Health Information Management and Systems (HIMS), Medical

Dietetics (MD), Occupational Therapy (OT), and Radiologic Sciences (RS). Each of these professions requires a strong background in mathematics and science, especially anatomy, physiology, and pathophysiology. The duties associated with each of these healthcare providers are diverse and extensive (Table 2).

86

Professions Description of Professional Duties and Job Tasks

Athletic Athletic Trainers (AT) work with physicians, athletic personnel, Training patients and their families, and other medical professionals to provide care in a variety of settings. These professionals are skilled in methods of risk management and injury prevention, and must be able to examine, diagnose, and recommend the appropriate care for an injury (Athletic Training, 2012).

Health Health information management (HIMS) professionals work in a Information variety of settings, and focus on business-related aspects of healthcare. Management Major responsibilities include collection, storage, communication, and and Systems retrieval of healthcare data. They must be knowledgeable about healthcare as well as business and information systems (Health Information Management and Systems, 2012).

Medical Registered Dietitians are food and nutrition experts. They must be Dietetics able to monitor patients, teach and advise, and understand anatomy, pathology, and physiology. They may be involved in activities such as administering nutritional therapy, product development, food and nutrition marketing, or pharmacological research (The Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, 2012).

Occupational Occupational Therapists (OT) work to enable people to achieve and Therapy maintain the ability to carry out day-to-day activities and live independently. As part of their professional duties, the OT must be able to assess and provide interventions, evaluate the nature and extent of disease and disability, identify environmental barriers to daily activities, and provide and/or develop assistive devices (Description of Occupational Therapy, 2012).

Radiologic Radiologic Science (RS) is the science of using various types of Science radiation (x-ray, magnetism, and ultrasound) to produce images of the tissues, organs, bones, and vessels of the body. Members of this profession implement imaging procedures, manipulate various types of imaging and procedure-related equipment, educate patients and other members of the healthcare team, and provide patient care during diagnostic and therapeutic imaging procedures (Radiologic Sciences and Therapy, 2012).

Table 2. Descriptions of the Allied Medical Professions Included in this Study

87

Sample

Data were gathered from 128 students in their final academic year in Allied

Medical Professional programs in the School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences at a large Midwestern university. The sample included students in the following Allied

Medical programs: Athletic Training (AT), Health Information Management and

Systems (HIMS), Medical Dietetics (MD), Occupational Therapy (OT), and Radiologic

Sciences (RS). Faculty members in each of the programs were contacted, and one class period was allocated to administering the test and survey instruments. Students in the sample must have completed at least one year in their respective program to be more representative of professionals in each of these fields. The sample therefore is a convenience sample that is representative of the various spatially oriented health professions. Demographics of the study sample as well as the entire student population in the School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences (HRS) is found in Table 3.

88

Ethnicity Total in Sample GPA Gender

M F Asian Black Caucasian Hispanic Other

AT 2 18 3.5 0 0 20 0 0 20

HIMS 13 10 3.45 1 1 20 1 0 23

MD 2 15 3.70 1 0 16 0 0 17

OT 6 33 3.82 1 0 37 0 0 39

RS 10 19 3.63 0 1 26 1 1 29

Sample 33 95 3.64 3 2 119 2 1 128

HRS 308 73 3.28 63 51 886 17 10 1046 Total 8

Table 3. Demographic Information for Sample M = male; F = female

Instrumentation

For this study, four tests have been chosen to correspond to the three categories of spatial ability proposed by Linn and Petersen (1985). This battery of tests includes the following spatial tests: the Mental Rotations Test (Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978), The three- part Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT; Guay, 1976), The Hidden Figures Test

(Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976), and the Cube Comparison Test (Ekstrom, French, &

Harman, 1976). The Mental Rotations Test and the Visualization of Rotations subtest of

89 the PSVT have been used many times to evaluate mental rotation ability. These tests both require that the subject mentally rotate a three dimensional figure. The Hidden

Figures Test and the Visualization of Developments subtest of the PSVT have been used to measure spatial visualization, and the Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of

Views subtest of the PSVT have been used to evaluate spatial perception ability.

Research has shown that certain types of experiences and activities are related to spatial ability (Baenninger & Newcombe, 1989; Eliot & Czarnolewski, 2007; Lunneborg

& Lunneborg, 1984; McDaniel, Guay, Ball, & Kolloff, 1978; Newcombe, Bandura, &

Taylor, 1983; Newcombe, Dubas, & Baenninger, 1989; Olson & Eliot, 1986). In order to evaluate the relationship between these activities and spatial ability, students will also complete the Spatial Experience Questionnaire (McDaniel, Guay, Ball, & Kolloff, 1978).

Tests to Measure Spatial Perception Ability

Linn and Petersen (1985) describe spatial perception as an ability to orient visual stimuli in relation to oneself or to gravitational fields. Their definition of this term is similar to what other researchers would call spatial orientation (Guilford, 1967; Jardine &

Martin, 1984; Lohman, 1988; Lord, 1990; McGee, 1979). Two of the most commonly used tests to measure this ability are the Water Level Test and the Rod and Frame Test.

Administration and scoring methodology make these tests impractical for large group administration. Other tests that have been used to measure spatial orientation ability include the Spatial Relations subtest of the Primary Mental Abilities Test (Hassler,

90

Birbaumer, & Feil, 1985), the Cube Comparison Test (Jardine & Martin, 1984; Lord,

1990), Card Rotations (Olson & Eliot, 1986), the Visualization of Views subtest of the

PSVT (Guay, 1980), as well as navigational and landmark tests (Hegarty, Montello,

Richardson, Ishikawa, & Lovelace, 2006). For this study, the tests chosen to measure spatial perception and orientation are the Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of

Views subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test. Both of these tests rely strongly on the subject’s ability to orient visual images with respect to gravity, or to imagine a gravitational change in one’s own position.

The Visualization of Views Subtest of the PSVT. The Visualization of Views subtest of the PSVT chosen for this study is a 24 item instrument which is designed to evaluate how subjects can visualize a three-dimensional figure from various perspectives

(Guay, 1976). Guay and McDaniel (1977) describe this test as a measure of high-level spatial ability. They also state that this tool can test “the ability to perceive a three- dimensional object and conceptualize that object sufficiently well to describe portions of it not immediately visible” (McDaniel & Guay, 1976, p. 5). As Figure 1 illustrates, subjects are presented with a drawing of a three-dimensional object enclosed within an imaginary cube which can be thought of as a “glass box”. One corner of the imaginary cube is identified with a black dot. The subject must mentally reorient his or her position so that the black dot lies between them and the central three-dimensional object, and then imagine how it will look from that viewpoint. Each question offers five options from which to choose the correct answer. Students were given 4 minutes to complete the 12 items on the test. The score on the test is simply the number correct.

91

Figure 1. Example Item from the Visualization of Views Subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test

Internal consistency reliability estimates for this test or a similar variation have been reported as .56 (K-R 20) in second through seventh grade school children (Guay &

McDaniel, 1977). Guay (1978) reports internal consistency reliability for the composite

PSVT at .92 and for the three subtests as ranging between .65 and .87 (K-R 20) in a group of undergraduate students, although the specific reliability coefficient for this subtest is not specified.

92

Some evidence of validity includes significant male-favored gender differences which have been reported with the Visualization of Views Test (Guay, 1978; Guay &

McDaniel, 1977). However Kovac (1989) did not find gender differences on this test when examining eighth grade students. The correlation between this test and the Spatial

Relations subtest of the DAT was found to be highly significant at .51 (Kovac, 1989).

Out of the 58 students who took this test, 35 used either object-reorientation or user- reorientation processing strategies (Kovac, 1989). Elementary school children who were high math achievers also scored significantly better than lower achievers on this test

(Guay & McDaniel, 1977). Kovac (1989) found this test to be significantly correlated with grades in science, math, and vocabulary.

The Cube Comparison Test. The Cube Comparison Test is designated as a test of spatial orientation by Ekstrom, French, and Harman in the Kit of Factor Referenced

Cognitive Tests (1976). As illustrated in Figure 2 the examinee is presented with pictures of two three-dimensional cubes. Each of the six sides of the cubes is labeled with a unique letter or symbol. The task is to determine which of the drawings represent drawings of the same cube, and which present drawings that could not be the same cube.

Two example drawings are presented to the examinee and explained; and then three practice problems are presented and explained. The test is composed of two sets of 21 items and three minutes are allowed for each set. The test is scored as the number correct minus the number incorrect to correct for guessing. The authors noted that the test was suitable for grades 8 through 16.

93

Figure 2. Example Item from the Cube Comparison Test

In a group of 11th and 12th grade students, reliability was reported to be .77 for both males and females, and .84 for college students, although the specific type of reliability was not specified (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). Correlation between the two parts of the test was reported to be .90 to a group of subjects aged 12 and over

(Smalley, Thompson, Spence, Judd, & Sparkes, 1989). Limited validity data is available 94 for this test. It was used in a battery to determine high and low spatial ability in a group of undergraduate students taking a biology course (Lord, 1990). The test was used as a measure of spatial orientation in a study of 83 adolescent pairs of twins to evaluate sex- linked gender differences in spatial orientation (Jardine & Martin, 1984).

Tests to Measure Spatial Visualization Ability

Spatial visualization tasks generally are more complex than those for spatial perception. Linn and Petersen (1985) describe tests that measure this ability as requiring

“complicated, multistep manipulations of spatially presented information” (p. 1484).

These tests may or may not rely on analytic strategies, but do require that the examinee work quickly and draw on a repertoire of cognitive strategies. Gender differences with these types of tests were generally small. Tests that have been found to measure this ability include embedded/hidden figures tests, paper folding tests, surface development tests, paper form board tests, and block design tests (Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer,

Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). To measure spatial visualization, this study used the Hidden

Figures Test (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976), and the Visualization of Developments subtest of the PSVT (Guay, 1976).

The Hidden Figures Test. Although this test is sometimes described as a measure for flexibility of closure, and sometimes as a measure for field dependence—field independence, the nature of the items are complex and contain distracting information.

This type of test is frequently used as a test of spatial visualization (Davis & Eliot, 1994;

95

Hassler, Birbaumer, & Feil, 1985; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Lord, 1990; Voyer, Voyer, &

Bryden, 1995). The test is considered to have a high level of difficulty and is suitable for grades 8 through 16 (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). This test consists of 32 questions divided into two 16 question halves. Figure 3 shows an example of a problem from this test. Examinees are presented with five geometric figures at the top of each page of the exam booklet. Below that are complex figures in which one of the reference figures is embedded. Letters corresponding to the reference figures are below the test figures, and the examinee must choose the letter corresponding to the embedded figure within the complex test figure. In the test booklet two example items are presented, and then each 16-item section of the test is allotted 12 minutes. Scoring consists of the number correct minus a fraction of the number marked incorrectly to discourage guessing on the test, according to the ETS Manual; but since the fraction was not specified in the manual, for this study, the test was scored as the number correct.

96

Figure 3. Example Item from the Hidden Figures Test

Reliability for samples of high school males and females are reported to be .82 and .80, and for college males to be .83 (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). Davis and

Eliot (1994) report reliabilities in the .80-.83 range. Coefficient alpha of .82 in a sample of college students was reported by Eliot (1984). Although little specific reliability data has been reported for this specific test, other hidden or embedded figures tests report

97 reliabilities in the range of .82 to .95 (Hegarty, Montello, Richardson, Ishikawa, &

Lovelace, 2006; Hyde, Geiringer, & Yen, 1975).

Some evidence of construct validity is evidenced by studies on gender differences with this test. While gender differences in favor of males were found by Davis and Eliot

(1994), no gender differences were found by Hassler, Birbaumer, & Feil, (1985). Gender differences were mixed in the Voyer et al. (1995) meta-analysis with about half of the analyzed studies showing male advantage on this test and about half showing no gender difference. This is consistent with Linn and Petersen’s conclusion that gender differences are small and somewhat inconsistent on tests of spatial visualization (Linn & Petersen,

1985).

The Visualization of Developments Subtest of the PSVT. The Visualization of

Developments subtest is a type of surface development test. In this test, subjects are presented with an unfolded two-dimensional representation (development) of the inside of a three-dimensional object as seen in Figure 4. One surface of the development is shaded to represent the bottom surface of the object. Below each development are five three-dimensional objects, one of which represents the development folded into its three- dimensional shape. The examinee chooses the letter corresponding to the object that would be formed by the folded development. The test is divided into two 12-item sections. Four minutes are allowed for each section. The exam is scored as the number of items marked correctly.

98

Figure 4. Example Item from the Visualization of Developments Subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test

Internal consistency reliability for this test has been reported to be .66 (K-R 20) in children grades 2-7 (Guay & McDaniel, 1977) and somewhere between .65 and .87 (K-R

20) in a group of undergraduate students, although the specific reliability coefficient for this subtest is not specified (Guay, 1978). Evidence of validity included significant male gender advantage in a study of college students and in elementary students (Guay, 1978;

99

Guay & McDaniel, 1977), and significant correlations between this test and math achievement (Guay & McDaniel, 1977). This test also tends to rely on more analytic processing strategies which are found more often in spatial visualization tasks (Guay,

1978; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Lohman, 1989; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995).

Tests to Measure Mental Rotation Ability

Mental rotation is the third category of spatial ability in Linn and Petersen’s model. This ability requires that a mental picture of an object be held in the mind and rotated in some direction about its axis. Many tests have been developed to measure this ability including the Card Rotations Test, the Spatial Relations subtest of Thurstone’s

Primary Mental Abilities Test, and various adaptations of the Shepard and Metzler test.

The two tests chosen for this study are the Visualization of Rotations subtest of the PSVT and the Mental Rotation Test developed by Vandenberg and Kuse (1978).

The Visualization of Rotations Subtest of the PSVT. It is commonly believed that mental rotation ability relies heavily on Gestalt or holistic mental processing abilities and this test was developed to maximize this type of processing while minimizing analytic processing (Bodner & Guay, 1997). For this test, subjects are presented with a three- dimensional geometric figure and its image as it is rotated into a different position (see

Figure 5). A second geometric figure is presented and the subject must mentally rotate this figure in the same way as the prompt. Five options are offered with one matching the appropriate rotation of the question prompt. This test contains 24 or 30 questions in two

100

12 or15 question sections. Each section is arranged in four levels of difficulty in increasing order: 90 degree rotation on one axis, 180 degree rotation on one axis, 90 degree rotation on two axes, 90 degree rotation on one axis and 180 degree rotation on another axis. One 12 question section was used for this study; a time limit of 4 minutes per section is recommended. Questions are scored as either right or wrong with no correction for guessing (Guay, 1978; Guay, 1980). This test differs somewhat from the

Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotations Test. In this test items are rotated around their natural axis rather than a strictly vertical axis, parts of the prompt or test items may be hidden from view, and test items may be rotated around more than one plane.

Figure 5. Example Item from the Visualization of Rotations Subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test

101

Reliability information on this test has been reported with high school and college students. Cronbach’s alpha was reported to be .77 for the 20-item version given to a group of high school geometry students (Battista, 1990). In various studies of college students, reliability has been reported between .78 and .92 (Bodner & Guay, 1997; Carter,

LaRussa, & Bodner, 1987; Cochran & Wheatley, 1989; Guay, 1980).

Evidence of construct validity has been found in studies where respondents report the use of holistic processing strategies (Carter, LaRussa, & Bodner, 1987; Guay, 1980;

Kovac, 1989). Construct and convergent validity are also supported by studies (Bodner

& Guay, 1997; Guay, 1980) which demonstrate reasonably strong correlations with the

Shepard and Metzler mental rotation items (r = .61) and with DAT-SR (r = .72), especially with the more difficult items (Cochran & Wheatley, 1989; Kovac, 1989).

Significant male-favored gender differences have been reported (Bodner & Guay,

1997; Cochran & Wheatley, 1989; Guay, 1980). Results from this test have been significantly and positively correlated with spatial activities (Guay, 1978), and with tests of mechanical and quantitative reasoning (Bodner & Guay, 1997; Guay, 1980), spatial problems in college chemistry (Bodner & Guay, 1997; Carter, LaRussa, & Bodner,

1987), and with science, math, geometry, and social studies grades (Battista, 1990;

Kovac, 1989). In addition, this test was found to have a low correlation with the

Minnesota Paper Form Board Test which has been shown to rely heavily on analytic processing (Bodner & Guay, 1997).

102

The Mental Rotations Test. In 1978, Vandenberg and Kuse modified a three- dimensional test of mental rotation so as to be more practical for group administration.

This paper-and-pencil version of the Shepard and Metzler test has become the gold standard in testing metal rotation abilities. As seen in Figure 6, each question is composed of a criterion figure, two correct responses, and two distracters. The criterion figure is composed of ten three-dimensional blocks. Correct alternatives are rotated versions of the criterion figure. There are always two correct alternatives. The distracters are either rotated mirror images of the criterion object or rotation of some part of the criterion object. The task is to determine quickly and accurately whether the figures are the same or different. The original test contained 20 items divided into two 10 question sections. Subjects are given 3 minutes for each section of the test. The authors recommend that items are scored correct only if both correct alternatives are chosen, which they explain eliminates the need to correct for guessing (Vandenberg & Kuse,

1978).

Figure 6. Example Item from the Mental Rotations Test

103

Reliability for this test is generally high and has been reported for subjects from ages 12through adulthood. With normal timing and scoring procedures, reported reliabilities are generally between .83 and .91 (Hegarty, Montello, Richardson, Ishikawa,

& Lovelace, 2006; Moe, Meneghetti, & Cadinu, 2009; Stumpf, 1993; Vandenberg &

Kuse, 1978; Voyer, et al., 2006).

The Mental Rotations Test has consistently shown significant male-favored gender differences (Geiser, Lehmann, & Eid, 2008; Heil & Jansen-Osmann, 2008;

Kaufman, 2007; Masters, 1998; Moe, Meneghetti, & Cadinu, 2009; Peters, Lehmann,

Takahira, Takeuchi, & Jordan, 2006; Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978). The effect size for this difference has been reported to be as large as .95 to 1.03 (Cherney, 2008; Linn &

Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995), and has been seen to hold across several cultural groups (Peters, Laeng, Latham, Jackson, Zaiyouna, & Richardson, 1995; Peters,

Lehmann, Takahira, Takeuchi, & Jordan, 2006). Scores on this test correlate highly with other tests of spatial ability such as the PMA-SR (r = .61), the DAT-SR, Card Rotations

Test, and the Identical Blocks Test (Kaufman, 2007; Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978; Voyer, et al., 2006). Low correlations have been found between this test and tests of verbal reasoning (Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978). Reports of processing strategies place this test toward the holistic end of the continuum (Li, Zhu, & Nuttall, 2003; Moe, Meneghetti, &

Cadinu, 2009; Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978).

104

The Spatial Experience Questionnaire

The Spatial Experience Questionnaire is comprised of a list of 25 activities and experiences identified by a panel of judges to be important in the development of spatial ability (McDaniel E. , Guay, Ball, & Kolloff, 1978). The activities include games such as checkers and chess, sports such as skiing and golf, and activities such as sculpting and reading a map. The subject is instructed to examine each of the activities and rate his/her level of participation on a four-point scale using: Very often, Often, Occasionally, and

Never. Using the same set of 25 activities, each is then rated on a five-point scale for extent of enjoyment using: Very much, Much, Little, None, and Never Participated in this

Activity. Fifteen minutes is given to complete the survey. The survey has been found to discriminate between high and low spatial ability in a group of college students, and to identify activities which may be associated to these groups (McDaniel, Guay, Ball, &

Kolloff, 1978). The questionnaire has been found to correlate significantly with the

PSVT-Visualization of Rotations subtest (Guay, 1978), however no other test comparisons are reported in the literature.

Data Collection

In order to answer Research Question 1 (What spatial skills are used by Allied

Medical Professionals in the accomplishment of their job duties? Which of these skills are shared across the professions? Do the professions differ in their reliance on spatial

105 perception, spatial visualization, or mental rotation abilities?), documentation regarding significant job tasks, occupational activities, and critical knowledge was obtained. This information was garnered from professional organization websites, professional certification agencies, and a U. S. Department of Labor occupational website.

To address research questions 2 and 3, after obtaining IRB approval, data were collected through the administration of several spatial tests and a spatial experience questionnaire. In addition to data collected on the tests and questionnaire, the following demographic information was collected for each participant: degree program enrolled in, date of birth, dominant hand (for writing), ethnicity, gender, and GPA.

Instructors in each of the Allied Medical professional programs were contacted by phone to obtain permission to administer the instruments. During one regularly scheduled 90 minute class period the nature of the research and testing procedure were explained to the students. Consent was obtained and the four spatial tests were then administered. Students were informed that participation was completely voluntary and that there would be no grade penalty for not participating in the study. The study instruments were administered in the following order for each group: Cube Comparison

Test version A, Visualization of Views, Visualization of Developments version A,

Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, Mental Rotations Test version A, Spatial

Experience Questionnaire, Cube Comparison Test version B, Visualization of

Developments version B, Mental Rotations Test version B. The first and second versions of the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Developments, and Mental Rotations Tests were separated to reduce practice and memory effects. Written instructions were read for

106 each test to ensure consistency, and a one to two-minute rest break was given between each of the tests. Test and questionnaire answers were recorded on a digital scoring sheet and scored by hand. All scores were rechecked to ensure accuracy.

Data Analysis

Prior to statistical analysis, the accuracy of data entry was examined and missing variables were evaluated. Because the tests of spatial ability used in this study were timed, it is expected that some subjects would not be able to complete all questions for some or all of the tests. The score for the Cube Comparison Test was calculated as the number of correct items minus the number of incorrect items out of a possible 42 in compliance with the test directions (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). The maximum scores for each of the other tests were the total number correct: 12 for the Visualization of

Views, 16 for the Hidden Figures Test, 24 for the Visualization of Developments Test, 12 for the Visualization of Rotations Test, and 20 for the Mental Rotations Test (Ekstrom,

French, & Harman, 1976; Guay, 1976; Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978).

Prior to the analyses for the specific research questions, descriptive statistics were analyzed for the sample. Frequencies, means, standard deviations, and ranges were calculated from the demographic data. Student counts, means, and standard deviations for each of the spatial abilities tests were calculated for each program as well as for the entire sample. Means and standard deviations for test scores were also calculated by gender. Significance levels for all statistical tests were set at an alpha level of .05 unless

107 otherwise noted. Specific statistical analysis procedures used for each of the research questions are explained below.

Research Question 1: What spatial skills are used by Allied Medical Professionals in the accomplishment of their job duties? Which of these skills are shared across the professions? Do the professions differ in their reliance on spatial perception, spatial visualization, or mental rotation abilities?

It is expected that Allied Medical professionals will share much in the way of spatial ability, but some differences in the specific subcategories of spatial ability will emerge. Examination of task analyses and critical skills will reveal the numerous skills in each profession that have a spatial nature. This analysis may also show clear overlaps or divisions in the nature of spatial abilities needed for each of the various professions.

Investigation of the job analyses, task inventories, certification requirements, and other documents was done using the NVivo® qualitative research tool (QRS

International, 2012). Each item from the above documents was examined by the author for its use of spatial skills. Items were then categorized for each Allied Medical profession into spatial perception tasks, spatial visualization tasks, and/or mental rotation tasks. Job tasks were also examined to determine which were unique to the professions and which were shared by each of the Allied Medical professions studied. These data were used to determine meaningful patterns of spatial ability. Because all of the students in the sample are in medically-related professional programs, it is logical to expect that there will be tasks that are concurrent throughout the professions. On the other hand, the

108 data should also show areas of divergence among the spatial tasks needed for the various professions.

Research Question 2: To what extent is validity evidence provided for the use of the Mental Rotations Test, the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT), the Cube

Comparison Test, and The Hidden Figures Test to measure spatial ability in Allied

Medical students?

In order to examine this question, the following assumptions addressing the various aspects of the interpretive argument will be used:

1. Scores on the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental

Rotations Tests will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the

Allied Medical students in this sample.

2. Scores on the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental

Rotations Tests will demonstrate acceptable reliability.

3. The Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, and Visualization of Rotations Tests will

demonstrate criterion related validity evidence when compared to the gold

standard Mental Rotations Test.

4. The Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of Views Test will

exhibit concurrent validity evidence as measures of spatial perception.

109

The Hidden Figures Test and the Visualization of Developments Test will

exhibit concurrent validity evidence as measures of spatial visualization.

The Visualization of Rotations Test and the Mental Rotations Test will

exhibit concurrent validity evidence as measures of mental rotation.

5. If the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental

Rotations Tests measure the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the

male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and

will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire.

Assumption 1: Scores on the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views,

Visualization of Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental

Rotations Tests will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. Descriptive statistics and frequencies were calculated for each test of spatial ability, both for the entire sample and for each Allied Medical profession group individually. High spatial ability groups were calculated as those scoring at least one standard deviation above the mean, and low spatial ability groups were designated as those scoring at least one standard deviation below the mean. High and low ability groups were described for the entire sample and for each individual Allied Medical group.

Assumption 2: Scores on the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views,

Visualization of Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental

110

Rotations Tests will demonstrate acceptable reliability. Reliability refers to the trustworthiness, consistency, or reproducibility of test results (Carmines & Zeller, 1979;

Dick & Hagerty, 1971; Kurpius & Stafford, 2006; Polgar & Thomas, 2000; Salkind,

2004). Although reliability does not ensure accuracy, it is critical that tests are consistent and stable in what they purport to measure. Reliability coefficients can be calculated in a number of ways, but are always represented numerically by a value between 0 and +1.

Ideally the number is as large as possible (Carmines & Zeller, 1979).

There are several ways to estimate the reliability of a measure: the test-retest method, alternate or parallel forms method, split-half method, or calculation of internal consistency. The test-retest method is used to determine reliability of a measure over time, and compares tests scores given at time one to test scores from the same instrument from a second administration at a later time to the same group of subjects. Test-retest reliability is computed by calculating the Pearson product moment correlation and is sometimes referred to as the coefficient of stability (Huck, 2004; Kurpius & Stafford,

2006). The correlation between these two administrations of the test equals the reliability coefficient: ρx = ρx1x2. There is almost always instability of measures taken at two points in time which may be due to personal reasons, testing conditions, guessing, or changes in ability. In fact, the longer the time between test administrations, the more likely it is that the trait has changed (Carmines & Zeller, 1979). It is expensive and not always practical to administer tests at multiple times. A second problem with test-retest correlations is that the initial test may influence answers on the second administration. This may be due

111 to practice effects, memory effects, or subjects falsely striving for consistency. Each of these can lead to inflated reliability estimates (Carmines & Zeller, 1979).

Parallel or alternative forms reliability requires that two equal versions of a test be given, and then the scores from these versions are compared. The two forms should not differ in any significant way. Timing between administrations of the instruments is still a consideration, but less so than with the test-retest method because subjects cannot just repeat the same answers (Knapp & Mueller, 2010). Again, quantification of the relationship between these two tests is through computation of the Pearson product moment correlation, and is sometimes called the coefficient of equivalence (Huck, 2004;

Salkind, 2004). This method reduces the inflation of reliability due to memory. Like the test-retest method, this method only uses two administrations of an instrument, so does not allow one to distinguish true change from unreliability of the measure. A major difficulty is constructing two forms of a test that are truly equivalent. Major disadvantages for both test-retest and parallel forms methods are increased costs, time lag between measurements, and possible missing data if subjects don’t participate in the second measurement session (Knapp & Mueller, 2010). The advantages of this method are that it helps control for errors due to variability in content and time.

Estimation of internal consistency reliability only requires a single administration of the instrument. In general, this type of reliability measures consistency across parts of an instrument rather than across time or across equivalent versions of a test (Huck, 2004).

The split-halves method of reliability assessment is one method for evaluating internal consistency of an instrument. The total items of the test are divided into halves and the

112 scores on the halves are correlated to obtain the reliability estimate. The halves can be considered approximations to the alternative forms. Although it is common to compare the first half of the test to the second half, or to compare odd numbered questions to even numbered questions, there are many ways in which to group the questions. Therefore, it is probable that reliability estimates will differ depending on the grouping used to split the test. Reliability estimates can be calculated for both halves of the test, but to determine the reliability for the entire test, one must calculate the reliability coefficient for the halves, and then use the Spearman-Brown formula as a statistical correction

(Carmines & Zeller, 1979). This formula can be written as ρxx’ = (nρxx’) / (1+ρxx’) or as

Spearman-Brown formula rtt = (n r11) / (1 + (n-1) r11).

Another way to evaluate internal consistency of a test is a correlational method that compares each item in a test with every other item and with the total test. If this reliability coefficient is high it indicates that all items on the test measure the same trait or construct. One of the most common of the internal consistency reliability estimates is

2 2 2 Cronbach’s alpha which is expressed as: α = (k / k - 1) x ((sy - ∑si ) / sy ), where k = #

2 2 of items; sy = variance of observed scores; ∑si = the sum of the variances for each item

(Salkind, 2004).

From the formula above, it can be seen that this estimate relies on inter-item correlations. As the number of items increase and the inter-item correlations increase, then the estimate of reliability increases. Carmines & Zeller (1979) cite Novick and

Lewis (1967) who proved that coefficient alpha is a lower bound to the reliability of an

113 unweighted scale of N items. So Cronbach’s alpha can be provided as a conservative estimate for the reliability of a test (Carmines & Zeller, 1979, p. 45).

Another method for quantifying internal consistence was developed by Kuder and

Richardson in 1937 and is referred to as formula 20. Using this method, reliability is

2 2 calculated as Rtt = (k / k - 1) ((σo - ∑piqi) / σo ), where k = number of items in test; pi = proportion of students responding correctly to item i; qi = 1 - p (or proportion of students

2 responding incorrectly); σo = test variance; ∑piqi = sum of p times q for all items; k/k-1 is a correction factor which permits rtt to equal 1; p is item difficulty (Dick & Hagerty,

1971, p. 32). This method is used for dichotomous variables, but has the same interpretation as Cronbach’s alpha (Carmines & Zeller, 1979).

When speeded tests are used, it is common for students to not complete the entire test. This precludes several methods for determining reliability. The ideal way to calculate reliability coefficients for speeded tests is to use the parallel or alternate forms method. Two equal versions of a test are given as separate timed tests and then an estimate for the total test can be calculated from these halves using the Spearman-Brown correction formula (Carmines & Zeller, 1979; Dick & Hagerty, 1971).

Because each spatial test in the battery used for this study consists of two equal parts, reliability will be determined by using the parallel forms method with the

Spearman-Brown correction rtt = (n r11) / (1 + (n-1) r11). Internal consistency reliability of the questionnaire will be reported using Cronbach’s alpha.

Assumption 3: The Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, and Visualization of Rotations Tests will demonstrate

114 criterion related validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations

Test. Criterion validity assesses whether a test reflects certain abilities. There are two subtypes of criterion validity evidence: concurrent and predictive. The concurrent type of criterion validity evidence evaluates the relationship between two measures of a trait when they are administered at the same time. The Mental Rotations Test is found most often in the literature as a measure of spatial ability in general and as a measure of mental rotation in particular. Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated to determine if the various tests of spatial ability were significantly related to the Mental Rotations Test.

Strong correlations would indicate these tests measure spatial ability and would offer evidence of criterion-related validity.

Assumption 4: The Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of Views Test will exhibit concurrent validity evidence as measures of spatial perception. The Hidden

Figures Test and the Visualization of Developments Test will exhibit concurrent validity evidence as measures of spatial visualization. The Visualization of Rotations Test and the Mental Rotations Test will exhibit concurrent validity as measures of mental rotation.

For this study, the theoretical model that proposes three categories of spatial ability (Linn

& Petersen, 1985) was used. Two tests were chosen to represent each category of spatial ability according to their model. Since all of the tests propose to measure spatial ability, they should correlate significantly with each other. The spatial test battery used in this study was designed to measure the three categories of spatial ability proposed by Linn and Peterson. It is hypothesized that the Mental Rotations Test and the Visualization of

Rotations subtest of the PSVT will measure mental rotation, that the Visualization of

115

Views subtest of the PSVT and the Cube Comparison Test will measure spatial perception, and that the Visualization of Developments subtest of the PSVT and the

Hidden Figures Test will measure spatial visualization.

The tests chosen for this study have all been used numerous times to measure various aspects of spatial ability. For this study, tests were chosen to correspond to each of the three categories of spatial ability proposed by Linn & Petersen (1985) and Voyer,

Voyer, & Bryden (1995). Content and face validity can be assumed for these tests based on previous research in spatial ability. Evidence for concurrent validity can be obtained by examining the relationship between the two tests that were chosen for each of the three categories of spatial ability. Pearson correlation coefficients were calculated between the two tests in each category to examine the assertion that they are measuring the same category of spatial ability.

Assumption 5: If the Cube Comparison, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, and Mental Rotations Tests measure the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial Experience Questionnaire. Most agree that spatial intelligence generally exhibits gender differences in ability, although the strength of these differences varies depending on the type of ability tapped by tests (Hyde, Geiringer, & Yen, 1975; Linn &

Petersen, 1985; McGee, 1979; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). It is widely accepted that the trait of spatial ability demonstrates gender differences favoring males, and this is most clearly evident in tasks related to mental rotation.

116

Tasks related to spatial perception should also demonstrate gender differences, but it is expected that these will be smaller than those related to mental rotation. Spatial visualization tasks generally do not demonstrate significant gender differences. Evidence for construct validity will be found in evaluation of gender differences on each of the above tests. Further evidence of construct validity may be obtained by comparing means of males and females in the sample. Scores on the test battery should display gender differences over all of the students in the sample. In addition, following the Linn and

Petersen (1985) model, gender differences should be the largest for the mental rotation type tests, moderate for the spatial perception or orientation tests, and smallest for the visualization tests. Mean scores for males and females were compared using independent samples t-tests to examine differences in the genders.

It is widely accepted that the trait of spatial ability is correlated with various types of experiences and activities (Baenninger & Newcombe, 1989; Eliot & Czarnolewski,

2007; Guay, 1978; Lunneborg & Lunneborg, 1984; McDaniel, Guay, Ball, & Kolloff,

1978; Newcombe, Bandura, & Taylor, 1983). Evidence that the test results in this study are indicators of spatial ability will be examined by comparing the relations between the spatial test results and the Spatial Experience Questionnaire (McDaniel, Guay, Ball, &

Kolloff, 1978). These comparisons were done by examining Spearman correlations between test scores and the two scales of the Spatial Experience Questionnaire.

Research Question 3: To what extent is validity evidence provided for the use of the Spatial Experience Questionnaire to measure spatial ability in Allied Medical students? The questionnaire included 25 spatial activities and asked participants to report

117 their extent of participation and their extent of enjoyment in those activities. Inter-item correlations were examined to ensure that the activities were examining sufficiently different activities. Reliability for both the participation and the enjoyment scales were examined. Additionally, the results of the participation scale and the enjoyment scale were compared with the results of the spatial tests to further examine validity evidence for the questionnaire.

Statistical Analysis Assumption Checking

Prior to analysis of any data, assumptions for the various statistical methods used were checked. These assumptions include independence of each observations, continuous variables, identical distribution of error within and between groups, homogeneity of variance, normal distribution, and linearity (Afifi, Clark, & May, 2004;

Fan & Konold, 2010; Grimm & Yarnold, 1995; Keppel & Wickens, 2004; Nunnally &

Bernstein, 1994). In this study, the scores from the various spatial tests and the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire were treated as continuous variables. Although students in the study were all from the same university, they can be assumed to be typical students in their respective programs. The students were observed by the researcher during completion of the spatial tests and questionnaire, and were required to finish each independently. In this way the independence assumption was not violated. Frequency distributions, skewness, and kurtosis of data were inspected in order to examine

118 univariate normality. Examination of outliers was conducted since these can contribute to non-normality of univariate statistical procedures. Results related to the statistical methods for assumption checking will be discussed in Chapter 4.

119

Chapter 4: Results

This chapter examines the nature of spatial skills in the following Allied Medical

Professions: Athletic Training (AT), Health Information Management and Systems

(HIMS), Medical Dietetics (MD), Occupational Therapy (OT), and Radiologic Sciences

(RS). The chapter also discusses the results of analyses conducted to examine evidence for validity in the use and score interpretations of the Cube Comparison Test, the subtests of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test, the Hidden Figures Test, and the Mental

Rotations Test as measures of spatial ability in Allied Medical students in these five professions.

Spatial Skills in Allied Medical Professions

Research Question 1: What spatial skills are used by Allied Medical Professionals in the accomplishment of their occupational duties? Which of these skills are shared across the professions? Do the professions differ in their reliance on spatial perception, spatial visualization, or mental rotation abilities?

Information about the occupational tasks in the following Allied Medical

Professions was gathered from professional websites, accrediting agency websites, and the website for the Bureau of Labor Statistics: Athletic Training (AT), Health 120

Information Management and Systems (HIMS), Medical Dietetics (MD), Occupational

Therapy (OT), and Radiologic Sciences (RS). The documents for each profession were analyzed qualitatively by the author to inspect the required skills that might be of a spatial nature in each, and to evaluate spatial skills that appear to be shared between the five professions studied here. Complete listings of the occupational tasks with the proposed spatial components are found in Appendices A-E.

Table 4 summarizes tasks from the five professions which seem to require the use of spatial perception. This type of spatial ability requires the orientation of one’s body or another person in space. It is used in determining one’s relationship to the gravitational forces of the earth or in relation to another person or object. This ability is also involved in skills which require the perception of up and down or right and left. Spatial perception is a skill used in each of the five Allied Medical Professions studied here, but more of these types of tasks were found for AT, OT, and RS. These are professionals that spend much of their time working directly with patients. Many of the duties of these professions require the manipulation of a body part or a piece of equipment into a particular orientation. Additionally, Athletic Trainers and Occupational Therapists must often create and fit custom equipment to care for or assist their patients. While there are some tasks requiring spatial perception designated for HIMS and MD professions, this ability does not appear to be involved in as many of their daily job tasks.

121

Spatial Perception Tasks AT HIMS MD OT RS Application or fitting of equipment to patient X X X Development or modification of treatment or X X therapeutic devices Operate equipment safely in relation to self, co- X X X X X workers, or patients Move, immobilize, or position patient for an X X X X assessment, procedure, or intervention Perform or monitor vital signs X X X Perform CPR X X X X Palpate anatomic landmarks for assessment or for X X X performance of a diagnostic or therapeutic procedure Instruct, demonstrate, evaluate equipment or X X X procedures with a patient in an environment outside of a healthcare facility Evaluate environmental and ergonomic risks X X X X X Inventory maintenance X X X X X Arrange physical environment for security of medical X X X X X information or safety of patients and staff Test products and equipment X X X X X Use appropriate body mechanics in the performance of X X X X X the profession Table 4. Tasks Requiring the Use of Spatial Perception

Table 5 summarizes tasks in the five Allied Medical Professions that seem to require a proficiency in spatial visualization. This type of skill is generally considered to be a complex, multistep process in mentally picturing, manipulating, transforming, or rearranging objects (Linn & Petersen, 1985; McGee, 1979; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden,

1995). It includes tasks which require recognition of visual clues; the ability to recognize, create, or recreate patterns; the ability to disembed information from complex backgrounds; and the ability to assimilate visual cues into a whole picture. One of the major visualization skills for each of the Allied Medical Professions studied here is the ability to visualize internal structures and relate these to the relevant occupational tasks of

122 their respective jobs. In order to assess a patient, plan a treatment, or appropriately enter the required medical coding, members of each of these professions must be able to picture relevant anatomy, physiologic processes, and pathology. Other important visualization skills involve recognition of patterns. This type of skill is relevant in the preparation and evaluation of charts and graphs, educational materials, and computer software. Spatial visualization is required in measuring or estimating quantities such as in the preparation of medications or the appropriate quantities of food or fluid in the treatment of a medical condition. Radiologic Science professionals must be proficient in estimating whether portable x-ray equipment will fit through a door into a patient room.

As Table 5 illustrates, spatial visualization is used to some extent by each of the five professions studied here. For example, each group needs to be able to visualize normal anatomy and physiologic processes to successfully complete their jobs. However, a spatial visualization task such as creating custom software is unique to the Health

Information Management profession. Overall, the greatest number of tasks requiring spatial visualization on the job seems to apply to the AT, OT, and RS professions.

123

Spatial Visualization Tasks AT HIMS MD OT RS Prepare educational materials X X X X Assess patients for injuries, illness, pathology, and X X X X normal conditions in preparation for a diagnostic or therapeutic procedure Identify environmental and ergonomic risks for patient, X X X X X co-workers and self Select appropriate equipment for procedure, therapy, or X X X X intervention Apply treatments, therapeutic materials, equipment and X X X devices Fit standard equipment and devices to patient X X X Identify injuries, illnesses, and conditions that warrant X X application of custom made equipment and devices Create custom equipment and devices for procedures, X X therapy, or activities of daily living Recognize malfunction or disrepair of therapeutic, X X X X X diagnostic, or rehabilitation equipment or furnishings in clinical, treatment, or work areas Assess appropriateness of patient movement or X X X participation in activities, and correct or modify inappropriate, unsafe, or dangerous activities Recognize and visualize signs and symptoms of illness, X X X X X injury, pathology, or abnormalities Evaluate and interpret records relating to signs and X X X X X symptoms of illness, injury, pathology, or abnormalities Maintain a safe and sanitary work environment X X X X X Identify, locate, and palpate bony surface landmarks and X X X soft tissue abnormalities of specific injuries, illnesses, and health related conditions Identify location, function, action, and normal anatomic X X X X X presentation of bones, muscles, joints, and organs Interpret charts and graphs X X X X X Apply topical products to the appropriate anatomic area X X X Use computers and computer software X X X X X Perform data mining techniques to prepare reports X X Establish and manage information security and X authentication systems continued Table 5. Tasks Requiring Spatial Visualization

124

Table 5 Continued

Spatial Visualization Tasks AT HIMS MD OT RS Create software programs X Identify injuries, illnesses, and conditions that warrant X X application of custom made equipment and devices Create custom equipment and devices for procedures, X X therapy, or activities of daily living Recognize malfunction or disrepair of therapeutic, X X X X X diagnostic, or rehabilitation equipment or furnishings in clinical, treatment, or work areas Assess appropriateness of patient movement or X X X participation in activities, and correct or modify inappropriate, unsafe, or dangerous activities Recognize and visualize signs and symptoms of illness, X X X X X injury, pathology, or abnormalities Evaluate and interpret records relating to signs and X X X X X symptoms of illness, injury, pathology, or abnormalities Maintain a safe and sanitary work environment X X X X X Identify, locate, and palpate bony surface landmarks and X X X soft tissue abnormalities of specific injuries, illnesses, and health related conditions Identify location, function, action, and normal anatomic X X X X X presentation of bones, muscles, joints, and organs Interpret charts and graphs X X X X X Apply topical products to the appropriate anatomic area X X X Use computers and computer software X X X X X Perform data mining techniques to prepare reports X X Establish and manage information security and X authentication systems Create software programs X Plan and prepare meals X X Perform venipuncture X Prepare patient for radiographic procedures X Evaluate and interpret radiographic images X X X Navigation within the work environment X X X X X Preparation medication; measure foods and fluids X X X

125

Table 6 summarizes tasks that appear to require mental rotation for the five Allied

Medical Professions in this study. Mental rotation includes tasks requiring the ability to mentally rotate all or a portion of an object into a different or unique orientation. This skill is also involved in the ability to transform the of a figure or object. As this table demonstrates, professionals in AT, OT, and RS require the majority of tasks in this category of spatial ability in order to perform their jobs.

Mental Rotation Tasks AT HIMS MD OT RS Application or fitting of standard and custom X X X equipment to patient Development, fabrication, or modification of treatment X X or therapeutic devices Adapt instructions for the application, operation, or X X X X X modification of equipment Safely manage patient emergency situations or X X conditions Identify the relationship and severity of pathologic X X X X signs of injuries, illnesses, physiologic changes, and health-related conditions Participate in planning for construction and remodeling X of facilities Examine, evaluate, and propose adaptations to physical X X environments Manipulate medical equipment attached to patient X X X Prepare for and follow appropriate protocols for X patients in isolation Evaluate and interpret cross-sectional radiographic X X X images Table 6. Tasks Requiring Mental Rotation

126

Validation Evidence

Research Question 2: To what extent is validity evidence provided for the use of the

Mental Rotations Test, the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT), the Cube

Comparison Test, and The Hidden Figures Test to measure spatial ability in Allied

Medical students?

In order to examine this question, the following assumptions addressing the various aspects of the interpretive argument will be used:

Assumption 1: Scores on the Cube Comparison Test (CC), the subtests of the

Purdue Spatial Visualization Test (PSVT), the Hidden Figures Test (HF), and the Mental

Rotations Test (MRT) will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied

Medical students in this sample.

Assumption 2: The Cube Comparison Test, subtests of the Purdue Spatial

Visualization Test, the Hidden Figures Test, and the Mental Rotations Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability and may be generalizable to other Allied Medical students in the chosen population.

Assumption 3: The Cube Comparison Test, Hidden Figures Test, PSVT

Visualization of Views (VoV), PSVT Visualization of Developments (VoD), and PSVT

Visualization of Rotations (VoR) Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test.

Assumption 4: The Cube Comparison Test and the PSVT Visualization of Views

Test will exhibit concurrent validity as measures of spatial perception. The Hidden

127

Figures Test and the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test will exhibit concurrent validity as measures of spatial visualization. The Mental Rotations Test and the PSVT

Visualization of Rotations Test will exhibit concurrent validity as measures of mental rotation.

Assumption 5: Scores on the spatial tests given in this battery will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial Experience Questionnaire.

Six tests of spatial ability were given to each group of Allied Medical students in the following order: Cube Comparison Test version A (CCa), Visualization of Views

(VoV) subtest of the PSVT, Visualization of Developments subtest of the PSVT version

A (VoDa), Hidden Figures Test(HF), Mental Rotations Test version A (MRTa), and

Visualization of Rotations subtest of the PSVT (VoR). Following completion of the

Spatial Experience Questionnaire, parallel versions of three tests were then administered:

Cube Comparison Test version B (CCb), Visualization of Developments subtest of the

PSVT version B (VoDb), and Mental Rotations Test version B (MRTb). Keys for each test were provided by the authors and publishers. All answers were recorded on electronic answer sheets and were scored by one rater and checked by a second to ensure accuracy.

128

Validation Evidence for the Cube Comparison Test Results

Assumption 1: Scores on the Cube Comparison Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. The Cube

Comparison Test was chosen as one of the tests of spatial perception. This test is supplied with two versions; each version of the test consists of 21 items. According to recommendations (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976), the total score was calculated as the number of correct responses minus the number of incorrect responses in order to correct for guessing. Because this scoring could result in negative values, any negative value score was recorded as a zero.

Results for version A of this test for all students in the sample (n = 128) were slightly positively skewed with a mean of 7.27 items (34.64%) correct and a median score of 7 items (33.33%) correct out of a total of 21(see Figure 7). Average ability was considered to be the mean score plus or minus one standard deviation (7.27 +/- 3.89).

Based on this definition 82 students were considered to be of average ability. Overall there were 24 people (18.7%) who scored one or more standard deviations above the mean and were considered to be the high spatial ability group. These 24 students were distributed by program as follows: 4 (20%) AT, 3 (13%) HIMS, 3 (18%) MD, 8 (21%)

OT, and 6 (21%) RS. The low spatial ability group consisted of 22 students (17.2%) who scored one or more standard deviations below the mean: 3 (15%) AT, 3 (18%) HIMS, 2

(12%) MD, 8 (21%) OT, and 6 (21%) RS. Thirty-five (27.3%) of the entire group scored at or above the 75th percentile and 33 (25.8%) scored at or below the 25th percentile, with

129

60 scoring between the 25th and 75th percentile. The highest score was 19, obtained by one person in AT. Three people got no items correct.

Figure 7. Scores on Version A of the Cube Comparison Test

Descriptive statistics and scores for the entire sample as well as scores by Allied

Medical profession student groups are summarized in Table 7. Students in AT, MD, and

OT scored above the group mean on this test while those in HIMS and RS scored below the group mean. When all five sample groups were compared, there were no significant differences in means for this test, F (4, 127) = .41, p = .80.

130

All AT HIMS MD OT RS Groups (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 17) (n = 39) (n = 29) (n = 128) Mean (percent 7.27 7.55 6.43 7.65 7.59 7.10 correct) (34.64%) (35.95%) (30.64%) (36.41%) (36.14%) (33.83%) Std. Error of .34 .96 .74 .98 .62 .73 Mean Standard 3.89 4.27 3.54 4.03 3.86 3.93 Deviation Min. Score 0 1 0 0 1 1 Max. Score 19 19 15 16 16 15 Median 7 7 6 7 8 6 Mode (# of 8 5 4 7 8 6 correct items) Skewness .34 .95 .27 .20 .08 .27 Std. Error of .21 .51 .48 .55 .38 .43 Skewness # (%) of 24 4 (20%) 3 (13%) 3 (18%) 8 (21%) 6 (21%) sample 1+ SD (18.7%) above mean # (%) of 22 3 (15%) 3 (13%) 2 (12%) 8 (21%) 6 (21%) sample 1+ SD (17.2%) below mean # (%) of 35 5 (25%) 3 (13%) 5 13 9 (30.8%) sample (27.3%) (29.5%) (33.4%) scoring at/above 75th percentile # (%) of 33 4 (20%) 8 4 9 8 (27.6%) sample (25.8%) (34.8%) (23.5%) (23.1%) scoring at/below 25th percentile Table 7. Score Information for the Cube Comparison Test, Version A

Scores for the second version of the Cube Comparison Test (CCb) were slightly negatively skewed and are graphically depicted in Figure 8. On the second version of this test the mean was 8.64 items (41.15%) correct, and the median score was 9 (42.86%) out of a total of 21 items. Seventy-two students scored within one standard deviation 131 from the mean and were considered to have average spatial ability for this sample.

Twenty-four people (18.7%) scored one or more standard deviations above the mean: 3

(15%) AT, 3 (13%) HIMS, 2 (12%) MD, 9 (23%) OT, and 7 (24%) RS. Twenty-one students (16.4%) scored one or more standard deviations below the mean: 6 (30%) AT, 4

(17%) HIMS, 3 (18%) MD, 4 (10%) OT, and 4 (14%) RS. Of the entire group, 32 scored in the bottom 25th percentile and 48 scored at or above the 75th percentile. Three people got no items correct and three obtained the highest score of 19.

Figure 8. Scores on Version B of the Cube Comparison Test

Descriptive statistics and scores for the entire sample as well as scores by Allied

Medical profession student groups for version B of the Cube Comparison Test are 132 summarized in Table 8. Students in OT and RS had the highest mean scores, while MD and HIMS had the lowest means. Score distributions for all of the groups were close to normal in their distribution, with AT, HIMS, and RS being slightly negatively skewed and MD and OT being slightly positively skewed. The groups with the highest percentage of students scoring one or more standard deviations above the mean were OT and RS, however HIMS and MD had the highest percentages of students scoring at or above the 75th percentile. Athletic Training and OT had the greatest percentage of students scoring at or below the 25th percentile. When score means were compared for this test, no significant differences were found between any of the groups, F (4,127) =

.40, p = .81.

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (N = 128) (N = 20) (N = 23) (N = 17) (N = 39) (N = 29) Mean (percent 8.64 8.45 8.04 7.83 9.26 8.90 correct) (41.15%) (40.24%) (38.3%) (37.25%) (44.08%) (42.36%) Standard Error .24 1.23 .96 1.12 .74 .91 of Mean Standard 4.78 5.51 4.59 4.63 4.64 4.89 Deviation Minimum 0 0 0 0 2 1 Score 19 19 17 16 19 17 Maximum Score Median 9 9 9 7 8 9

continued Table 8. Score Information for the Cube Comparison Test, Version B

133

Table 8 Continued

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (n = 128) (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 17) (n = 39) (n = 29) Mode (# of 11 9 9 11 8 4 correct items) Skewness .18 .25 .13 .20 .37 -.04 Standard Error .21 .51 .48 .55 .38 .43 of Skewness # (%) of 24 3 4 3 9 7 sample 1+ SD (18.7%) (15%) (17.3%) (17.7%) (23.2%) (24.1%) above mean # (%) of 21 6 4 3 8 8 sample 1+ SD (16.4%) (30%) (17.4%) (17.7%) (20.5%) (27.6%) below mean # (%) of 31 5 7 7 11 7 sample scoring (23.5%) (25%) (30.3%) (41.2%) (28.3%) (24.1%) at/above 75th percentile # (%) of 32 6 6 4 12 8 sample scoring (25%) (30%) (26.1%) (23.5%) (30.8%) (27.6%) at/below 25th percentile

Assumption 2: The Cube Comparison Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability. Reliability relates to stability, consistency, or trustworthiness of a measure

(Dick & Hagerty, 1971; Huck, 2004; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Salkind, 2004;

Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). There are several methods for analyzing reliability depending on the type of instrument used and whether the test is speeded. The Kuder-

Richardson formula 20 (K-R 20) is a method used to measure the internal consistency when dichotomous variables are used. Two equivalent forms of the Cube Comparison

Test were administered and both were speeded. Because of the speeded nature of the 134 test, reliability was analyzed by the parallel forms method. The K-R 20 reliability coefficient for version A of the Cube Comparison Test was .68, for version B was .79, and the correlation between the forms was r = .64. The parallel forms reliability coefficient for the entire test was found to be .84. Reliability estimates range from 0 to

1.0, and values ranging from 0.7 to 0.95 are generally considered acceptable (Anastasi &

Urbina, 1997; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). Therefore, as a whole test the Cube Comparison Test demonstrates acceptable reliability. Version B is also well within the acceptable range for reliability, however, the reliability coefficient for version A puts this portion of the test in the questionable range. Therefore results from version A of the Cube Comparison Test would not necessarily be a consistent and trustworthy measure of spatial ability for the sample of students who took the test in this study.

Assumption 3: The Cube Comparison Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. The Mental

Rotations Test has been used in numerous studies and is considered to be a definitive test of spatial ability. If this is true, the Cube Comparison Test should demonstrate a moderate to high correlation with this test.

For this portion of the analysis, the percentage of correct answers for each test was used in order to have a common scale of measurement between the tests. Table 9 shows the correlations found between the two versions of the Mental Rotations Test and the two versions of the Cube Comparison Test. The combined versions of the Cube

Comparison Test are positively and significantly correlated with the combined Mental

135

Rotations Tests (r = .47, p < .001, see Table 9). When compared individually, version A of the Cube Comparison Test correlates with either the individual versions or the combined version of the MRT are smaller than those for version B of the Cube

Comparison Test. However, all correlations would be considered to be of moderate effect size. This is somewhat weak evidence to demonstrate that both tests are measuring similar traits. These moderate correlation coefficients provide some evidence to suggest that both of these tests measure the overall trait of spatial ability. Based on the work of

Jacob Cohen, correlations of .10 are considered small, correlations of approximately .30 are considered moderate, and correlations of .50 are considered to be large effects (Huck,

2004; Murphy, 2010). Very high or perfect correlations would not be expected if in fact these tests measure different categories of spatial ability.

Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Test, Version A Test, Version B Test, Combined A and B Cube Comparison, .29 .30 .33 Version A (.001) (.001) (< .001) Cube Comparison, .41 .39 .47 Version B (< .001) (< .001) (< .001) Cube Comparison, .41 .40 .47 Combined A & B (< .001) (< .001) (< .001) Table 9. Pearson Correlations between the Cube Comparison Test and the Mental Rotations Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values

136

Assumption 4: The Cube Comparison Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the PSVT Visualization of Views Test. If the selected tests of spatial ability measure the three categories of spatial ability proposed by Linn and

Peterson (1985) and Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden (1995), the two tests that were chosen to measure each category should be highly correlated. The tests chosen to measure spatial perception, the Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of Views Test should be highly correlated if measuring the same trait. Pearson correlation coefficients are positive and significant between the combined versions of the Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of Views Test (r = .30, p = .001, see Table 10). The correlations between the individual versions of the Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of

Views Test are slightly smaller. These correlations would be considered to have a low to moderate effect size (Huck, 2004; Murphy, 2010), and are smaller than the correlations between the Cube Comparison Test and the Mental Rotations Test (which should be measuring a different category of spatial ability). This evidence casts doubt on the assumption that these tests measure the same category of spatial ability.

Visualization of Views Test Cube Comparison, Version A .27 (.003) Cube Comparison, Version B .25 (.004) Cube Comparison, .30 Combined A & B (.001) Table 10. Pearson Correlations between the Cube Comparison Test and the PSVT Visualization of Views Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

137

Assumption 5: If the Cube Comparison Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores should reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire. Hundreds of studies have examined the gender differences in spatial ability. Males are almost always found to do better on tests that measure spatial ability. Gender differences were analyzed for the entire group of students taking the

Cube Comparison Test. Thirty-three males and 95 females completed the test. The mean for males taking the test was 7.03 out of 21 for version A, 8.82 out of 21 for version B, and 15.85 out of 42 for the combined versions. Means for the females were 7.36 out of

21 for version A, 8.58 out of 21 for version B, and 15.86 out of 42 for the combined versions of the test. No significant gender differences were found for version A (t = -.42, df = 126, p = .68), version B (t = .25, df = 126, p = .81), or the combined test (t = -.01, df

= 126, p = .99). Figures 9 and 10 illustrate the gender differences in each program for the individual versions of the Cube Comparison Test. Females outperformed males in three of the Allied Medical groups for version A of the test and in two of the Allied Medical groups on version B of the test, although the number of participants makes it difficult to reach statistically significant interpretations.

138

Figure 9. Variations in Performance on the Cube Comparison Test, Version A

Figure 10. Variations in Performance on the Cube Comparison Test, Version B

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire

Research has found positive correlations between various spatial tasks and spatial ability. Results from the Spatial Experience Questionnaire and the two versions of the

139

Cube Comparison Test were compared using a Spearman correlation analysis. Spatial experiences responses were collected for extent of participation and for extent of enjoyment. Extent of participation was coded as: 4 = very often, 3 = often, 2 = occasionally, 1 = never. Extent of enjoyment in spatial activities was coded as: 5 = very much, 4 = much, 3 = little, 2 = none, and 1 = never participated in this activity.

Therefore, higher correlations would indicate a higher level of participation in or enjoyment of the activities would be related to higher test scores. Total number of items correct was used as the value for each of the spatial tests. Table 11 details the pertinent significant correlations between participation in spatial activities and scores on the Cube

Comparison Test. Only one activity, Taking Pictures, had a significant correlation with version A of the Cube Comparison Test. This correlation was negative (rs = -.19, p =

.04) which would indicate that less participation in this activity is related to a higher spatial ability score on this test. Participation in three spatial activities was related to the scores on version B of the Cube Comparison Test: sketching house plans, skiing or snowboarding, and solving mathematical riddles. Each of these correlations was small.

The correlation between skiing or snowboarding participation was negative, indicating that less participation in this activity seemed to be correlated with a higher spatial ability score as measured by version B of the Cube Comparison Test. When the two versions of the test were combined, only participation in one activity was significantly correlated with test scores: sketching house plans.

140

Activity Cube Comparison, Cube Comparison, Cube Comparison, Version A Version B Combined Taking Pictures -.19 (.04) Sketching house .18 .18 plans (.05) (.04) Skiing or -.20 Snowboarding (.02) Solving 18 mathematical (.05) riddles Table 11. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Cube Comparison Test Scores Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values

Spearman correlations between the two versions of the Cube Comparison Test and the Extent of Enjoyment items from the Spatial Experience Questionnaire were also calculated. The results of this analysis revealed small, significant, positive correlations between version A of the test and degree of enjoyment of building models and sketching house plans. Version B of the Cube Comparison Test was positively correlated with enjoying solving math riddles, but negatively correlated with enjoying sketching auto designs. So if one likes solving math riddles their performance on version B of this test tended to be better; however if one likes sketching car designs, their performance tended to be worse. When the two versions of this test were combined, higher scores were only significantly correlated with enjoyment in solving math riddles.

141

It has been reported in the literature that spatial ability is positively related to spatial experiences. This analysis did not offer strong evidence to support these findings.

All effect sizes would be considered small. The assertion that spatial experiences are significantly related to scores on the Cube Comparison Test is weakly supported.

Activity Cube Comparison, Cube Comparison, Cube Comparison, Version A Version B Combined Sketching house .20 plans (.02) Building models .18 (.04) Solving .19 .19 mathematical riddles (.04) (.03) Sketching auto -.19 design (.03) Table 12. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Spatial Test Scores Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

Validation Evidence for the PSVT Visualization of Views Test Results

Assumption 1: Scores on the Visualization of Views Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. The

Visualization of Views subtest of the PSVT was chosen as the second test of spatial perception. This test included 12 items and the author recommended that one point be awarded for each correct item (Guay, 1976). As Figure 11 shows, scores for this test were slightly positively skewed with a mean of 5.72 (47.66%). The median score for this test was five items correct (41.67%). Twenty-one people (16.5%) scored one or more 142 standard deviations above the mean. For the individual groups, this includes 1 (5%) AT,

3 (13%) HIMS, 3 (17.7%) MD, 6 (15.5%) OT, and 8 (28%) RS. Twenty-six students

(20.3%) scored one or more standard deviations below the mean: 4 (20%) AT, 4 (17.3%)

HIMS, 3 (17.7%) MD, 9 (23.1%) OT, and 6 (20.7%) RS. Forty-seven students (36.7%) scored at or below the 25th percentile and 33 (25.9%) scored at or above the 75th percentile. Two people got no items correct and two achieved the maximum score of 12.

Figure 11. Score Distribution of the PSVT Visualization of Views Subtest

Descriptive statistics and scoring information for the PSVT Visualization of

Views subtest are found in Table 13. Radiologic Sciences students earned the highest mean score on this test while the HIMS student group earned the lowest mean score.

Most scores approached a normal distribution, however scores for the HIMS students were positively skewed and scores for MD, OT, and RS are slightly negatively skewed.

143

The RS group had the highest number of students (28% of all RS students), and the AT group had the lowest number of students (5% of all AT students) scoring one or more standard deviations above the mean. No significant difference was found between the means for the five groups of students, F (4,127) = .54, p = .70.

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (n = 128) (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 17) (n = 39) (n = 29) Mean (percent 5.72 5.35 5.17 5.82 5.82 6.21 correct) (47.66%) (44.58%) (43.12%) (48.53%) (48.51%) (51.72%) Standard Error .25 .43 .51 .76 .42 .65 of Mean Standard 2.78 1.93 2.46 3.13 2.62 3.49 Deviation Minimum 0 2 2 0 2 0 Score 12 9 11 11 12 12 Maximum Score Median 5 5.5 4 6 6 7 Mode 4 5 4 4 5 4 Skewness .33 -.16 1.83 .10 .27 -.01 Standard Error .21 .51 .48 .55 .38 .43 of Skewness # (%) of 21 1 3 3 6 8 sample 1+ SD (16.5%) (5%) (13%) (17.7%) (15.5%) (28%) above mean # (%) of 26 4 4 3 9 6 sample 1+ SD (20.3%) (20%) (17.3) (17.7%) (23.1%) (20.7%) below mean # (%) of 32 2 4 5 10 12 sample scoring (25.9%) (10%) (17.3%) (29.5%) (25.8%) (41.4%) at/above 75th percentile # (%) of 47 6 12 6 11 12 sample scoring (36.7%) (30%) (52.2%) (35.3%) (28.2%) (41.4%) at/below 25th percentile Table 13. Score Information for the Visualization of Views Subtest of the PSVT

144

Assumption 2: The Visualization of Views Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability. Reliability relates to stability, consistency, or trustworthiness of a measure

(Dick & Hagerty, 1971; Huck, 2004; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Salkind, 2004;

Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). There are several methods for analyzing reliability depending on the type of instrument used and whether the test is speeded. The Kuder-

Richardson formula 20 (K-R 20) is a method used to measure the internal consistency when dichotomous variables are used. The K-R 20 reliability coefficient for this test was found to be .38. Reliability coefficients from .7 to .95 are generally considered to be acceptable (Anastasi & Urbina, 1997; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Tavakol & Dennick,

2011). This test was speeded, so it was expected that not all items would be completed.

This decreases inter-item correlations and therefore reliability. Because no parallel version of this test was given the K-R 20 reliability coefficient cannot be used as evidence for or against the validity assumption for this test.

Assumption 3: The PSVT Visualization of Views Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. The

Mental Rotations Test has been used in numerous studies and is considered to be a definitive test of spatial ability. If this is true, the PSVT Visualization of Views Test should demonstrate moderate to high correlations with this test.

For this portion of the analysis, the percentage of items correct was used in order to have a common scale of measurement between the tests. Table 14 details the correlations found between the two versions of the Mental Rotations Test and the PSVT

Visualization of Views Test. Correlations between these tests are positive and

145 significant. Correlation coefficients are consistent between the Views test and the two versions of the Mental Rotations Test, varying from r = .41 to r = .43. The correlation between the Visualization of Views test and the combined versions of the Mental

Rotations test was found to be r = .42, p < .001. These correlations would be considered in the range of a moderate effect size (Huck, 2004; Murphy, 2010). This may provide some evidence to demonstrate that both tests are measuring similar traits. While supporting the assumption of criterion-related validity, this evidence does not show an exceptionally strong relationship between these two tests. However, the strength of the relationship might be somewhat affected because the PSVT Visualization of Views Test was chosen to evaluate the spatial perception category of spatial ability rather than the mental rotation category.

Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Test, Test, Version A Test, Version B Combined Visualization .43 .41 .42 of Views (< .001) (<.001) (< .001) Table 14. Pearson Correlation between the PSVT Visualization of Views Test and the Mental Rotations Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values

Assumption 4: The PSVT Visualization of Views Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the Cube Comparison Test. If the selected tests of spatial ability measure the three categories of spatial ability proposed by Linn and

Peterson (1985) and Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden (1995), the two tests that were chosen to measure each category should be highly correlated. The tests chosen to measure spatial 146 perception, the Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of Views Test should be highly correlated if measuring the same trait (Table 15). Pearson correlation coefficients are positive and significant between the combined versions of the Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of Views Test (r = .30, p = .001). The correlations between the individual versions of the Cube Comparison Test and the Views Test are slightly smaller.

These correlations would be considered to have a low to moderate effect size (Huck,

2004; Murphy, 2010), and are smaller than the correlations between the Cube

Comparison Test and the Mental Rotations Test (which should be measuring a different category of spatial ability). This evidence casts doubt on the assumption that these tests measure the same category of spatial ability.

Visualization of Views Test Cube Comparison, Version A .27 (.003) Cube Comparison, Version B .25 (.004) Cube Comparison, .30 Combined (.001) Table 15. Pearson Correlations between the Cube Comparison Test and the PSVT Visualization of Views Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values

Assumption 5: If the Visualization of Views Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire. Gender differences were analyzed for the entire group of

147 students taking the Visualization of Views Test. Thirty-three males and 95 females completed the test. The mean for males taking this test was 7.68 items correct and for women, the mean was 5.11 out of 12 total items. A multivariate overall main effect for gender was found, F (1, 118) = 14.70, p < .001. This male-favored gender difference (t =

3.58, p < .001) is illustrated in Figure 12.

Figure 12. Gender Differences on the Visualization of Views Test

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire

Spearman correlations were calculated between scores on the Visualization of

Views Test scores and items from the participation scale of the Spatial Experience

Questionnaire (see Table 16). These correlations indicated a positive and significant relationship between scores on this test and participation in six types of spatial activities:

148 sketching house plans, playing chess, using hand tools, playing golf, and reading a map or using a compass. The correlation coefficients ranged from .19 to .32. Most of the effects would be considered small; the effect size for reading a map or using a compass would be considered moderate (Huck, 2004; Murphy, 2010).

Spatial activities Visualization of Views Test Sketching house plans .24 (.01) Playing chess .19 (.03) Using hand tools .25 (.01) Playing pool .29 (.001) Playing golf .23 (.01) Reading a map, using .32 a compass (.00) Table 16. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Views Test Scores Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values

Spearman correlations between the enjoyment scale of spatial activities and scores on the Visualization of Views Test were positive and significant for six spatial activities: sketching house plans, playing chess, using hand tools, making or repairing furniture, playing pool, and reading a map or using a compass (Table 17). The correlations for enjoyment were slightly higher than for participation, ranging from .22 to .33. As with participation, most of the effects would be considered small, while playing pool and

149 reading a map or using a compass would be considered moderate effects (Huck, 2004;

Murphy, 2010).

Spatial Activities Visualization of Views Test Sketching house plans .22 (.01) Playing chess .24 (.01) Using hand tools .22 (.01) Making or repairing .27 furniture (.002) Playing pool .33 (.000) Reading a map, using a .31 compass (.000) Table 17. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Views Test Scores Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values

Validation Evidence for the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test

Assumption 1: Scores on the Visualization of Developments Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. The

Visualization of Developments subtest of the PSVT involves complex, multistep manipulations of visual information and was chosen as one of the tests of spatial visualization. Parallel forms of the test were given, each form consisting of 12 similar but not identical question items. The author recommends that each right answer be awarded one point with no correction for guessing (Guay, 1976).

150

Figure 13 demonstrates the score distribution for the first version of this test.

Scores for this test were slightly positively skewed with a mean of 4.24 items (35.35%) correct and a median score of 4 items (33.33%) correct out of a total of 12 items. The standard deviation for the group was 2.08. Eighteen people (14.2%) scored at least 7 items correct which was a score of one or more standard deviations above the mean. Of these eighteen people, 3 were in AT, 5 in HIMS, 2 in MD, 2 in OT, and 6 in RS.

Twenty-seven students (21.1%) scored two or fewer items correct which was one or more standard deviations below the mean. Two of these students were in AT, 2 in HIMS, 6 in

MD, 12 in OT, and 5 in RS. Forty-seven students (36.7%) scored at or below the 25th percentile and 32 (25.1%) scored at or above the 75th percentile. Two people got no items correct, one person achieved a score of 11 out of a possible 12, and no one earned the maximum score of 12.

Figure 13. Score Distribution of the PSVT Visualization of Developments Subtest, Version A 151

Descriptive statistics and scoring information for version A of the PSVT

Visualization of Developments subtest are found in Table 18. Scores were fairly similar, with the highest mean for HIMS students and the lowest for OT students. In fact, the maximum score for both AT and OT was only 7 items correct out of a total of 12 items.

The HIMS group had the highest group percentage scoring at or above the 75th percentile.

Both MD and OT had nearly 50% of their groups scoring and at or below the 25th percentile. However, no significant differences were found for the means between the five sample groups, F (4,123) = 2.11, p = .08.

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (n = 128) (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 17) (n = 39) (n = 29) Mean (percent 4.24 4.6 4.78 3.88 3.56 4.69 correct) (35.35%) (38.33%) (39.86%) (32.35%) (29.70%) (39.1%) Standard Error .18 .37 .39 .59 .30 .43 of Mean Standard 2.08 1.67 1.88 2.45 1.89 2.33 Deviation Minimum 0 1 1 1 0 1 Score 11 7 9 11 7 10 Maximum Score Median 4 5 4 3 4 4 Mode 4 5 4 2 3 4 Skewness .54 -.35 .21 1.66 -.04 .82 Standard Error .21 .51 .48 .55 .38 .43 of Skewness # (% of total or 18 3 5 2 2 6 program (14.2%) (15%) (21.7%) (11.8%) (5.1%) (20.6%) group) of sample 1+ SD above mean continued Table 18. Descriptive Statistics and Scoring Information for Version A of the PSVT Visualization of Developments Subtest 152

Table 18 Continued

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (n = 128) (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 17) (n = 39) (n = 29) # (%) of 27 2 2 6 12 5 sample 1+ SD (21.1%) (10%) (8.7%) (35.3%) (30.8%) (17.2%) below mean # (%) of 32 6 8 3 7 8 sample scoring (25.1%) (30%) (34.7%) (17.6%) (17.9%) (27.5%) at/above 75th percentile # (%) of 47 5 5 9 19 9 sample scoring (36.7%) (25%) (21.7%) (52.9%) (48.7%) (31%) at/below 25th percentile

Figure 14 demonstrates the score distribution for version B of the PSVT

Visualization of Developments Test. Scores for this test were slightly negatively skewed with a mean of 4.91 items (40.99%) correct and a median score of 5 items (41.67%) correct out of a total of 12 items. Nine people got no items correct, and one earned the maximum score of 12. Nineteen people (14.8%) had at least eight items correct which is at least one standard deviation above the mean. Of these 19 students, one was in the AT group, 5 were in the HIMS group, 3 were in the MD group, 5 were in the OT group, and

5 were in the RS group. Twenty-five students (19.5%) had scores of 2 or less, which was one or more standard deviations below the mean. Forty-one students (32%) scored at or below the 25th percentile (3 or fewer items correct) and 32 (25%) scored at or above the 75th percentile (7 or more items correct). Of those in the high scoring groups, 21.7% of the HIMS students scored eight of more items correct. Thirty percent of the AT students were in the group that scored two or fewer items correct. 153

Figure 14. Score Distribution of the PSVT Visualization of Developments Subtest, Version B

Descriptive and scoring information about the PSVT Visualization of

Developments subtest, Version B are found in Table 19. Radiologic Sciences students had the highest mean score (5.52 out of 12 items correct) and AT students had the lowest

(4.2 out of 12 items correct). Almost 22% of the HIMS students scored 8 or more items correct while 30% of the AT students got 2 or fewer items correct. Distributions for each of the groups were negatively skewed except for RS which was positively skewed. No significant differences in means were found for any of the five groups, F (4,127) = .74, p

= .57.

154

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (n = 128) (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 17) (n = 39) (n = 29) Mean 4.91 4.20 4.83 5 4.85 5.52 (40.99%) (35.25%) (40.22%) (41.67%) (40.39%) (45.98%) Standard Error .24 .69 .57 .68 .36 .52 of Mean Standard 2.67 3.07 2.72 2.81 2.27 2.81 Deviation Min. Score 0 0 0 1 0 0 Max. Score 12 10 10 12 10 11 Median 5 4.5 5 5 5 5 Mode 5, 6 0 5 3 5 6 Skewness .16 .51 .11 .83 -.11 .40 Standard Error .21 -1.06 .48 .55 .38 .43 of Skewness # (%) of sample 19 1 5 3 5 5 1+ SD above (14.8%) (5%) (21.7%) (17.6%) (12.9%) (17.2%) mean # (%) of sample 25 6 5 3 7 4 1+ SD below (19.5%) (30%) (21.7%) (17.6%) (17.9%) (13.8%) mean # (%) of sample 32 6 5 4 8 9 scoring at/above (25.00) (30%) (21.7%) (23.6%) (20.5%) (31%) 75th percentile # (%) of sample 41 8 7 7 12 7 scoring at/below (32%) (40%) (30.4%) (41.2%) (30.8%) (24.1%) 25th percentile Table 19. Descriptive and Score Information for the Visualization of Developments Subtest of the PSVT, Version B

Assumption 2: The PSVT Visualization of Developments Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability. As with the Cube Comparison Test, two equivalent forms of the

PSVT Visualization of Developments Test were administered and both were speeded.

Because of the speeded nature of the test, reliability was analyzed individually for each version of the test using the K-R 20 method to determine internal consistency, and then both versions of the test were analyzed together by the parallel forms method. The

155 reliability coefficient for version A of this test was .62, for version B was .74, and the correlation between the forms was r = .53. The parallel reliability coefficient for the entire test was found to be .62. Reliability estimates range from 0 to 1.0, and values ranging from 0.7 to 0.95 are generally considered acceptable (Anastasi & Urbina, 1997;

Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). The correlation between the two forms of the test can be considered a large effect (Huck, 2004; Murphy, 2010), indicating that the two forms of the test are similar. Although version B demonstrates an acceptable reliability coefficient, neither version A nor the combined form of the test fall in the acceptable range. Interpretations of this test as a reliable measure of spatial ability in the Allied Medical students included in this study are therefore somewhat questionable.

Assumption 3: The PSVT Visualization of Developments Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test.

The Mental Rotations Test has been used in numerous studies and is considered to be a definitive test of spatial ability. If this is true, the Visualization of Developments Test should demonstrate a moderate to high correlation with this test.

For this portion of the analysis, the percentage correct scores were used in order to have a common scale of measurement between the tests. Table 20 details the correlations found between the two versions of the Mental Rotations Test and the two versions of the Visualization of Developments Test. Correlations between each of these tests are positive and significant. With correlation coefficients varying from r = .22 to r

= .46, these correlations would be considered in the range of small to moderate effect

156 sizes (Huck, 2004; Murphy, 2010). This provides some evidence to demonstrate that both tests are measuring similar traits. While somewhat supporting the assumption of criterion-related validity, this evidence does not show an exceptionally strong relationship between these two tests. However, the strength of the relationship might be somewhat affected because the Visualization of Developments Test evaluates a spatial ability that is different from mental rotation.

Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Test, Test, Version A Test, Version B Combined Visualization of .27 .22 .28 Developments, (.002) (.012) (.001) Version A Visualization of .33 .46 .43 Developments, (< .001) (< .001) (< .001) Version B Visualization of .34 .41 .41 Developments, (< .001) (< .001) (< .001) Combined Table 20. Pearson Correlations between the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test and the Mental Rotations Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values

Assumption 4: The PSVT Visualization of Developments Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the Hidden Figures Test. The

Visualization of Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test were chosen as measures of spatial visualization. Results from the correlation analysis (Table 21) seem to contradict that they measure the same trait. A small positive correlation is seen between the Hidden Figures Test and Version A of the Visualization of Developments

157

Test (r = .23, p = .01), but there is no significant correlation between Version B of the

Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test (r = .15, p = .08). In fact, the highest correlation coefficients for either version of the Developments Test is with the

Visualization of Rotations Test (r = .49, p < .001). There is also a moderate, positive correlation between version A of the Developments Test (r = .42, p < .001) and version B of the Cube Comparison Test (r = .40, p < .001). The assumption of concurrent validity between the Visualization of Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test as measures of the same construct is therefore not supported.

Hidden Figures Test Visualization of Developments, .23 Version A (.008) Visualization of Developments, .15 Version B (.08) Visualization of Developments, .22 Combined (.01) Table 21. Pearson Correlation between the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

Assumption 5: If the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire. Spatial tests normally demonstrate a male-favored gender difference from at least adolescence through adulthood. For version A of the

Visualization of Developments Test, males had an average score of 4.76 out of 12 items

158 and females averaged 4.06 out of 12. For version B of the test, males scored an average of 5.24 and females scored an average of 4.8 out of 12 items. An independent samples t- test for total score and individual version scores revealed no significant differences in performance between the genders. Results of this test were: combined, t = 1.35, p = .18; version A, t = 1.66, p = .10; version B, t = .82, p = .42. Figures 15 and 16 illustrate the gender differences in each program for the individual versions of the Visualization of

Developments Test. Females outperformed males in AT and MD for both version A and version B. In the HIMS group, males did slightly better on version A, but males and females had nearly the same mean scores on version B. Gender differences in tests of spatial visualization normally demonstrate smaller gender differences than spatial perception and mental rotation (Linn & Petersen, 1985; McGee, 1979; Voyer, Voyer, &

Bryden, 1995), however the lack of significant, even if small, gender differences cast some doubt of the use of this test for measuring spatial visualization in this group of

Allied Medical students.

159

Figure 15. Variations in Performance on the Visualization of Developments Test, Version A

Figure 16. Variations in Performance on the Visualization of Developments, Version B

160

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire

Spearman correlations (Table 22) indicated a positive and significant relationship between combined scores on the Visualization of Developments subtests and participation in sketching house plans and making or repairing furniture. Scores on version A of the Visualization of Developments Test had positive and significant correlations with participation in three types of spatial activities: sketching house plans, making and repairing furniture, and solving math riddles. Version B of the

Developments Test was only significantly correlated with participation in sketching house plans. A significant negative correlation was found between version A of the test and playing chess, which would seem to indicating that participation in this activity was related to lower scores on the test. Effect sizes for each of these significant correlations ranged from -.22 to .25, and were small (Huck, 2004; Murphy, 2010).

Spatial activities Visualization of Visualization of Visualization of Developments, Developments, Developments, Version A Version B Combined Sketching house plans .25 .20 .26 (.01) (.02) (.01) Playing chess -.22 (.02) Making or repairing .19 .18 furniture (.03) (.05) Solving mathematics .18 riddles (.04) Table 22. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Developments Tests Scores Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

161

Spearman correlations between reported enjoyment of spatial activities and scores on the combined versions of the Visualization of Developments Test (Table 23) were positive and significant for two spatial activities: sketching house plans and building models. Two activities from the enjoyment scale were also positively and significantly correlated with the individual versions of the Developments test. Version B was also positively correlated with playing checkers and reading maps. A significant negative correlation was found between enjoyment of playing tennis and the combined test scores on the two versions of the Developments test. The correlation coefficients for enjoyment were similar to those for participation, and would again be considered in the range of small effects.

Spatial activities Visualization of Visualization of Visualization of Developments, Developments, Developments, Version A Version B Combined Playing checkers .19 (.03) Sketching house plans .33 .20 .28 (.000) (.02) (.001) Playing tennis -.18 (.04) Building models .26 .20 .23 (.004) (.02) (.01) Reading a map, using a .19 compass (.03) Table 23. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Developments Test Scores Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

162

Validation Evidence for the Hidden Figures Test

Assumption 1: Scores on the Hidden Figures Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. The Hidden Figures

Test requires multistep mental manipulation of information in determining which geometric figure is embedded within a more complex figure, and so was chosen as the second test of spatial visualization. Students are given 12 minutes to complete the 16 items on the test. The scoring is recommended to be the calculated on the number correct minus a fraction of the number incorrect, however the “fraction” is not specified

(Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). In this study, no correction for guessing was made in scoring the test.

Figure 17 demonstrates the score distribution for the Hidden Figures test. Scores for this test were positively skewed with a mean of 6.57 items (41.06%) correct and a median score of 6 items (37.5%) correct. Twenty-five people (19.5%) scored 10 or more items correct, which was at least one standard deviation (3.58) above the mean and were classified as the high spatial ability group for this sample. Of the high ability group, there were 4 in AT, 2 in HIMS, 4 in MD, 12 in OT, and 3 in RS. Twenty-seven students in the entire group (21.1%) had three or fewer items correct out of 16 which was at least one standard deviation below the mean. The remaining 76 students had scores within one standard deviation of the mean. Forty-four students (34.4%) scored at or below the 25th percentile and 36 (28.1%) scored at or above the 75th percentile, with the remaining 48 scoring between the 25th and 75th percentile. One person got no items correct, and two

163 earned the maximum score of 16. No significant differences in means were found for any of the five groups, F (4,127) = .73, p = .57.

Figure 17. Score Distribution of the Hidden Figures Test

Descriptive statistics and scores for the entire sample as well as scores by Allied

Medical profession student group are summarized in Table 24. Students in the OT and

MD groups had the highest mean score (7.08 and 6.94 respectively), and those in HIMS had the lowest mean score (5.52). Almost 31% of the OT students scored one or more standard deviations above the mean, while approximately 30% of the HIMS students had scores at least one standard deviation below the mean. Scores for AT were normally distributed, scores for MD were slightly negatively skewed and scores for HIMS, OT, and RS were slightly positively skewed.

164

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (n = 128) (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 17) (n = 39) (n = 29) Mean (percent 6.57 6.5 5.52 6.94 7.08 6.55 correct) (41.06%) (40.63%) (34.51%) (43.38%) 44.25%) (40.91%) Standard Error .32 .78 .60 1.01 .65 .58 of Mean Standard 3.58 3.47 2.86 4.15 4.08 3.10 Deviation Minimum Score 0 1 1 1 0 2 Maximum 16 14 10 16 16 14 Score Median 6 6.5 5 7 7 6 Mode 4 4 8 4 7 9 Skewness .52 .37 .12 17.18 .37 .56 Standard Error .21 .51 .48 .67 .38 .43 of Skewness # (%) of sample 25 4 2 4 12 3 1+ SD above (19.5%) (20%) (8.7%) (23.6%) (30.9%) (10.2%) mean # (%) of sample 27 3 7 3 8 6 1+ SD below (21.1%) (15%) (30.4%) (17.6%) (20.5%) (20.7%) mean # (%) of sample 36 6 4 4 14 8 scoring at/above (28.1%) (30%) (17.4%) (23.6%) (36%) (27.4%) 75th percentile # (%) of sample 44 (34.4%) 7 (35%) 10 7 12 8 scoring (43.5%) (41.2%) (30.9%) (27.4%) at/below 25th percentile Table 24. Descriptive and Scoring Information for the Hidden Figures Test

Assumption 2: The Hidden Figures Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability.

Reliability relates to stability, consistency, or trustworthiness of a measure (Dick &

Hagerty, 1971; Huck, 2004; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Salkind, 2004; Tavakol &

Dennick, 2011). There are several methods for analyzing reliability depending on the type of instrument used and whether the test is speeded. The Kuder-Richardson formula 165

20 (K-R 20) is a method used to measure the internal consistency when dichotomous variables are used. The K-R 20 reliability coefficient for this test was found to be .78.

Reliability coefficients from .7 to .95 are generally considered to be acceptable (Anastasi

& Urbina, 1997; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). This test was speeded, so it was expected that not all items would be completed. This decreases inter- item correlations and therefore reliability. Because no parallel version of this test was given the K-R 20 reliability coefficient cannot be used as evidence for or against the validity assumption for this test.

Assumption 3: The Hidden Figures Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. The Mental

Rotations Test has been used in numerous studies and is considered to be a definitive test of spatial ability. If this is true, the Hidden Figures Test should demonstrate a moderate to high correlation with this test.

For this portion of the analysis, percentage scores were used in order to have a common scale of measurement between the tests. Table 25 details the correlations found between the two versions of the Mental Rotations Test and the Hidden Figures Test. No significant relationship was found between the Hidden Figures Test and either version of the Mental Rotations Test. This indicates that interpreting scores for this test as a measure of spatial ability in Allied Medical students is highly questionable if in fact the

Mental Rotations Test is truly a measure of spatial ability.

166

Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Test, Version A Test, Version B Test, Combined Hidden Figures .12 .07 .13 Test ( .18) (.45) ( .15) Table 25. Pearson Correlation between the Hidden Figures Test and the Mental Rotations Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

Assumption 4: The Hidden Figures Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test. The

Visualization of Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test were chosen as measures of spatial visualization. Results from the correlation analysis (Table 26) seem to contradict that they measure the same trait. A small positive correlation is seen between the Hidden Figures Test and Version A of the Visualization of Developments test (r = .23, p = .01), but there is no significant correlation between Version B of the

Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test (r = .15, p = .08). On the other hand, the

Hidden Figures Test was positively correlated with the Cube Comparison Test version A

(r = .28, p = .001), and the Cube Comparison Test version B (r = .34, p < .001). Effects for these correlations would be considered small to moderate (Huck, 2004; Murphy,

2010). The assumption of concurrent validity between the Visualization of

Developments test and the Hidden Figures test as measures of the same construct is therefore not strongly supported.

167

Hidden Figures Test Visualization of Developments, .23 Version A (.008) Visualization of Developments, .15 Version B (.08) Visualization of Developments, .22 Combined (.01) Table 26. Pearson Correlation between the PSVT Visualization of Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

Assumption 5: If the Hidden Figures Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial Experience

Questionnaire. Spatial tests normally demonstrate a male-favored gender difference from at least adolescence through adulthood. Males had an average score of 5.42 out of

16 items and females averaged 6.97 out of 16 on the Hidden Figures Test. Gender differences in these mean scores were compared using a factorial ANOVA For this test, a significant gender difference was found F (1, 118) = 3.96, p .05, with females performing significantly better than males, t = -2.168, p = .03. Figure 18 illustrates the gender differences in each program for the Hidden Figures Test. Females outperformed males in each Allied Medical student group except for HIMS. Tests of spatial visualization normally demonstrate smaller gender differences than spatial perception and mental rotation (Linn & Petersen, 1985; McGee, 1979; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995); however the higher mean scores for females cast some doubt of the use of this test for measuring spatial visualization in this group of Allied Medical students.

168

Figure 18. Variations in Performance on the Hidden Figures Test

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire

Spearman correlations indicated only one significant relationship between scores on the Hidden Figures test and participation in spatial activities. This was a negative correlation with playing tennis (rs = -.18, p = .05). This which would seem to indicate that participation in this activity was related to lower scores on the test. The effect size for this correlation was small (Huck, 2004; Murphy, 2010).

Spearman correlations between reported enjoyment of spatial activities and scores on the Hidden Figures Test were positive and significant for two spatial activities: arranging furniture and interior decorating (Table 27). Both of these tasks might be considered female type tasks. There was a significant negative correlation between enjoyment of playing golf and scores on the Hidden Figures test (rs = -.20, p = .02). The

169 correlation coefficients for enjoyment would again be considered in the range of small effects (Huck, 2004; Murphy, 2010).

Spatial activities Hidden Figures Test Arranging furniture .17 (.05) Interior decorating .19 (.03) Playing golf -.20 (.02) Table 27. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Hidden Figures Test Scores Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

Validation Evidence for the PSVT Visualization of Rotations Test

Assumption 1: Scores on the Visualization of Rotations Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. The

Visualization of Rotations subtest of the PSVT was chosen as one of the tests of mental rotation. This test included 12 items and the author recommends that one point be awarded for each correct item (Guay, 1976). Students are given four minutes to complete the test. No correction for guessing is recommended by Guay (1976).

Figure 19 demonstrates the score distribution for the PSVT Visualizations of

Rotations Test. Scores for this test were slightly negatively skewed with a mean of 4.28 items (35.68%) correct and a median score of 4 items (33.33%) correct out of a total of

12 items. Twenty-four people (18.8%) had seven or more items correct which was at

170 least one standard deviation above the mean. Of these, 3 were in AT, 5 were in HIMS, 5 were in MD, 4 were in OT, and 7 were in RS. Thirty students (23.4%) had scores of two or below which was at least one standard deviation below the mean. The low spatial ability group consisted of 3 AT students, 2 HIMS students, 2 MD students, 14 OT students, and 9 RS students. Seventy-four students scored within one standard deviation from the mean. Fifty-two students (40.6%) scored at or below the 25th percentile and 42

(32.9%) scored at or above the 75th percentile. Three people got no items correct, one person got 11 of the items correct, and no one earned the maximum score of 12.

Figure 19. Score Distribution of the PSVT Visualization of Rotations Test

Descriptive statistics for this test are found in Table 28. Students in the HIMS and MD groups had the highest mean scores, 4.96 and 4.94 respectively. Students in OT had the lowest mean scores with an average of 3.51 items correct out of 12. Over 29% of

171

MD and 24.1% of RS students scored more than one standard deviation above the mean.

Almost 36% of OT students and 31% of RS students scored one or more standard deviations below the mean. Scores for AT and HIMS were negatively skewed and those for the remaining three groups were positively skewed. No significant differences in means were found for any of the five groups, F (4,127) = 2.26, p = .07.

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (n = 128) (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 16) (n = 39) (n = 29) Mean (percent 4.28 4.75 4.96 4.94 3.51 4.07 correct) (35.68%) (39.58%) (41.30%) (41.18%) (29.27%) (33.91%) Standard .20 .48 .41 .58 .34 .50 Error of Mean Standard 2.32 2.15 1.99 2.38 2.11 2.67 Deviation Min. Score 0 1 0 1 0 0 Max. Score 11 9 8 11 8 9 Median 4 5 5 4 3 3 Mode 3 6 6 4 1 3 Skewness .25 -.06 .48 .80 .32 .52 Standard .21 .51 .21 .55 .38 .43 Error of Skewness # (%) of 24 3 5 5 4 7 sample 1+ SD (18.8%) (15%) (21.7%) (29.4%) (10.3%) (24.1%) above mean # (%) of 30 3 2 2 14 9 sample 1+ SD (23.4%) (15%) (8.7%) (11.8%) (35.9%) (31%) below mean continued Table 28. Score Information for the Visualization of Rotations Subtest of the PSVT

172

Table 28 Continued

# (%) of 42 8 11 6 8 9 sample (32.9%) (40%) (47.8%) (35.3%) (20.6%) (31%) scoring at/above 75th percentile # (%) of 52 6 5 4 21 16 sample (40.6%) (30%) (21.7%) (23.5%) (53.8%) (55.2%) scoring at/below 25th percentile

Assumption 2: The Visualization of Rotations Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability. Reliability relates to stability, consistency, or trustworthiness of a measure

(Dick & Hagerty, 1971; Huck, 2004; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Salkind, 2004;

Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). There are several methods for analyzing reliability depending on the type of instrument used and whether the test is speeded. The Kuder-

Richardson formula 20 (K-R 20) is a method used to measure the internal consistency when dichotomous variables are used. The K-R 20 reliability coefficient for this test was found to be .70. Reliability coefficients from .7 to .95 are generally considered to be acceptable (Anastasi & Urbina, 1997; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Tavakol & Dennick,

2011). This test was speeded, so it was expected that not all items would be completed.

This decreases inter-item correlations and therefore reliability. Because no parallel version of this test was given the K-R 20 reliability coefficient, although within the acceptable range, cannot be used as evidence for or against the validity assumption for this test.

173

Assumption 3: The PSVT Visualization of Rotations Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test.

The Mental Rotations Test has been used in numerous studies and is considered to be a definitive test of spatial ability. Because the Visualization of Rotations Test was developed to be a test of mental rotation, it should strongly correlate with the Mental

Rotations Test.

For this portion of the analysis, percentage scores were used in order to have a common scale of measurement between the tests. Table 29 details the correlations found between the two versions of the Mental Rotations Test and the Visualization of Rotations

Test. The relationship between these tests is moderately strong, positive, and highly significant. These correlation coefficients would be considered to be in the moderate to high range for effect sizes (Huck, 2004; Murphy, 2010). However, since these tests should measure the same spatial skill, mental rotation, higher correlation coefficients would be expected.

Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Test, Version A Test, Version B Test, Combined Visualization of .43 .32 .42 Rotations Test (< .001) (< .001) (< .001) Table 29. Pearson Correlations between the PSVT Visualization of Rotations Test and the Mental Rotations Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

Assumption 4: The Visualization of Rotations Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the Mental Rotations Test. As noted above, the 174

Visualization of Rotations Test has moderately strong correlations with the Mental

Rotations Test. However, the Visualization of Rotations Test is positively correlated with all of the spatial tests in this battery, and has higher correlations with version A (r =

.49, p < .001) and version B (r = .46, p < .001) of the Visualization of Developments Test than it does with the Mental Rotations Test. So while results of the scores on this test may be interpreted as an indication of spatial ability, it is not necessarily measuring mental rotation in this group of subjects.

Assumption 5: If the PSVT Visualization of Rotations Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire. Thirty-three males and 95 females completed the

Visualization of Rotations Test. Males had a mean score of 5 and females a mean score of 4.03 out of a possible 12 items. The initial comparison of means showed a difference in scores between the genders was significant (t = 2.10, p = .04), although multivariate analysis failed to reach significance, F (1, 118) = 3.05, p = .08. Figure 20 illustrates the gender differences in each group for the Visualization of Rotations Test. Males outperformed females in each Allied Medical student group, although the number of participants makes it difficult to reach statistically significant interpretations.

175

Figure 20. Variations in Performance on the Visualization of Rotations Test

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire

Spearman correlation analysis (Table 30) revealed two activities that were positively and significantly related to scores on the Visualization of Rotations test: sketching house plans and playing golf. One activity had a significant and negative correlation with the test scores: taking pictures. Higher scores on the test were significantly related to less participation in taking pictures.

Spatial activities Visualization of Rotations Test Taking pictures -.21 (.02) Sketching house plans .18 (.04) Playing golf .20 (.02) Table 30. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Rotations Test Score Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values. 176

Four items from the Extent of enjoyment in spatial activities scale were positively and significantly related to scores on the Visualization of Rotations Test (Table 31): sketching house plans, building models, playing pool, and reading a map and using a compass. A significant negative correlation was found between how much one enjoyed taking pictures and scores on this test. It is interesting to note, that while participation in playing golf was significantly related to scores on this test, enjoying this activity was not.

Spatial activities Visualization of Rotations Test Taking pictures -.20 (.02) Sketching house plans .24 (.01) Building models .31 (.000) Playing pool .23 (.01) Reading a map, using a .22 compass (.01) Table 31. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Visualization of Rotations Test Score Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

Validation Evidence for the Mental Rotations Test

Assumption 1: Scores on the Mental Rotations Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. The second test for the mental rotations category of spatial ability was the Mental Rotations Test. The

177 version of the test used here consisted of two parts, each with 10 items. The items were similar in each section but not identical. This test requires that the subject choose the two options that match the target item, and both options must be chosen in order for the answer to be deemed correct. The author recommends the score be based on the number of correct test items without any correction for guessing (Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978).

Figure 21 demonstrates the score distribution for the first part of the Mental

Rotations Test. Scores for this test were slightly positively skewed with a mean of 4.27 items (42.66%) correct and a median score of 4 items (40%) correct out of a possible 10 items. Twenty-eight people (21.9%) had at least seven items correct which was a score that was at least one standard deviation above the mean. Of these 28, 5 were AT students, 5 were HIMS students, 3 were MD students, 7 were OT students, and 8 were RS students. Thirty-one students (24.2%) had two or fewer items correct which gave them scores at least one standard deviation below the mean. Fifty-four students (42.2%) scored at or below the 25th percentile and 38 (29.7%) scored at or above the 75th percentile. Four people got no items correct, five people got 9 of the items correct, and no one earned the maximum score of 10. No significant differences in means were found for any of the five groups, F (4,127) = .80, p = .53.

178

Figure 21. Score Distribution for the Mental Rotations Test, Version A

On version A of the Mental Rotations Test, RS and AT student groups had the highest mean scores, 4.9 and 4.3 respectively (Table 32). Students in the HIMS group had the lowest mean scores: 3.78. Proportionally, 27.6% of the RS students scored at least one standard deviation above the mean while nearly 30% of students in the MD group had scores at least one standard deviation below the mean. Scores were slightly positively skewed for AT, OT and RS and were slightly negatively skewed for HIMS and

MD.

179

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (n = 128) (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 17) (n = 39) (n = 29) Mean 4.27 4.3 3.78 4.12 4.13 4.90 (42.66%) (43%) (37.83%) (41.18%) (41.28%) (48.97%) Standard Error .21 .50 .47 .51 .38 .50 of Mean Standard 2.37 2.23 2.26 2.12 2.39 2.69 Deviation Min. Score 0 1 0 1 0 1 Max. Score 9 9 8 7 8 9 Median 4 4 4 5 4 4 Mode 3 3 4 5 4 3 Skewness .26 .69 .32 -.04 .01 .22 Standard Error .21 .51 .48 .55 .38 .43 of Skewness # (%) of 28 5 5 3 7 8 sample 1+ SD (21.9%) (25%) (21.7%) (17.6%) (17.9%) (27.6%) above mean # (%) of 31 4 4 5 7 6 sample 1+ SD (24.2%) (20%) (17.4%) (29.4%) (17.9%) (20.7%) below mean # (%) of 38 5 7 5 11 8 sample scoring (29.7%) (25%) (30.4%) (29.4%) (28.2%) (27.6%) at/above 75th percentile # (%) of 54 9 7 5 15 11 sample scoring (42.2%) (45%) (30.4%) (29.4%) (38.5%) (37.9%) at/below 25th percentile Table 32. Descriptive Statistics and Scoring Information for the Mental Rotations Test, Version A

Scores were lower overall for the second portion of the Mental Rotations Test, and slightly positively skewed (see Figure 22). On this part of the test the mean score was 3.22 (32.19%) correct out of a possible 10 items. There were 20 (15.7%) who had at least 6 items correct which was at least one standard deviation (2.02) above the mean. Of these, 2 were AT students, 1 was a HIMS student, 2 were MD students, 7 were OT

180 students, and 8 were RS students. Twenty-seven students (21.1%) had zero or one item correct which was at least one standard deviation below the mean. Eight-one students had scores within one standard deviation from the mean. Fifty-six people (43.8%) scored at or below the 25th percentile and 32 (25.1%) who scored at or above the 75th percentile.

Forty students scored between the 25th and 75th percentile. Eight people got no items correct, one person achieved a score of 9, and no one got all 10 items correct. No significant differences in means were found for any of the five groups, F (4,127) = 1.21, p = .31.

Figure 22. Score Distribution for the Version B of the Mental Rotations Test

Over 27% of the RS students had scores at least one standard deviation above the mean, while only 4.3% of HIMS students were in this group (Table 33). Thirty percent of the AT students had scores at least one standard deviation below the mean. All but the

OT student group had more students scoring at or below the 25th percentile than above

181 the 75th percentile. Scores for the AT, HIMS, and MD groups were slightly negatively skewed; those for OT and RS were slightly positively skewed.

All Groups AT HIMS MD OT RS (n = 128) (n = 20) (n = 23) (n = 17) (n = 39) (n = 29) Mean 3.22 2.7 2.91 2.82 3.67 3.45 (32.19%) (27%) (29.13%) (28.24%) (36.67%) (34.48%) Standard Error .18 .46 .30 .39 .33 .50 of Mean Standard 2.02 2.05 1.44 1.59 2.04 2.47 Deviation Min. Score 0 0 1 1 1 0 Max. Score 9 6 7 6 8 9 Median 3 3 3 3 4 3 Mode 2 0 2 1 2 2 Skewness .48 .01 .96 .75 .32 .48 Standard Error .21 .51 .48 .55 .38 .43 of Skewness # (%) of 20 2 1 2 7 8 sample 1+ SD (15.7%) (10%) (4.3%) (11.8%) (18%) (27.5%) above mean # (%) of 27 6 3 4 7 7 sample 1+ SD (21.1%) (30%) (13%) (23.5%) (18%) (24.1%) below mean # (%) of 32 4 2 2 15 9 sample scoring (25.1%) (20%) (8.6%) (11.8%) (38.5%) (30.9%) at/above 75th percentile # (%) of 56 9 11 8 15 13 sample scoring (43.8%) (45%) (47.8%) (47.1%) (38.5%) (44.8%) at/below 25th percentile Table 33. Descriptive Statistics and Score Information for the Mental Rotations Test, Version B

Assumption 2: The Mental Rotations Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability. As with the Cube Comparison Test and the PSVT Visualization of

182

Developments Test, two equivalent forms of the Mental Rotations Test were administered and both were speeded. Because of the speeded nature of the test, reliability was analyzed individually for each version of the test using the K-R 20 method, and then both versions of the test were analyzed together by the parallel forms method. The K-R

20 reliability coefficient for version A of this test was .76, for version B was .64, and the correlation between the forms was r = .62. The reliability coefficient for the entire test was found to be .82. Reliability estimates range from 0 to 1.0, and values ranging from

0.7 to 0.95 are generally considered acceptable (Anastasi & Urbina, 1997; Nunnally &

Bernstein, 1994; Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). The correlation between the two forms of the test can be considered a large effect, indicating that the two forms of the test are measuring similar traits. Although version A demonstrates an acceptable reliability coefficient, version B does not. Version B of this test was the final instrument given in a

90 minute testing period, so subject fatigue may introduce a bias into the reliability coefficients. The reasonable reliability coefficient for the entire test provides evidence for this validity assertion. However, this assertion may be questioned due to the lower reliability coefficient for version B of the test.

Assumption 3: The other tests of spatial ability will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. This assumption has been addressed individually earlier in this chapter with each of the other spatial tests used in this study.

Assumption 4: The Mental Rotations Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the Visualization of Rotations Test. The Mental Rotations

183

Test and the Visualization of Rotations subtest of the PSVT were chosen as measures of mental rotation ability. Correlations between these tests are significant and positive but are considered moderate effect sizes (r = 43 and .32 respectively). Table 34 details correlations between all tests used in this study. The Rotations Test correlates more strongly with the Developments test than with either version of the Mental Rotations Test

(r = .49 and r = .46 respectively). The highest significant correlation for version A of the

Mental Rotations Test was with the Visualization of Views Test (r = .43). The highest correlation for version B of the Mental Rotations Test was with version B of the

Developments Test.

CCa VoV VoDa HF MRTa VoR CCb VoDb MRTb CCa 1 .265 .422 .284 293 .340 .574 .320 .299 (.003) (.000) (.001) (.001) (.000) (.000) (.000) (.001) VoV 1 .222 -.049 .434 .390 .252 .328 .412 (.012) (.583) (.000) (.000) (.004) (.000) (.000) VoDa 1 .233 .272 .489 .402 .513 .222 (.008) (.002) (.000) (.000) (.000) (.012) HF 1 120 .295 .341 .153 .068 (.176) (.001) (.000) (.084) (.448) MRTa 1 .429 .405 .325 .607 (.000) (.000) (.000) (.000) VoR 1 .342 .459 .319 (.000) (.000) (.000) CCb 1 .369 .390 (.000) (.000) VoDb 1 .463 (.000) MRTb 1 Table 34. Pearson Correlations Between all Tests of Spatial Ability in this Study Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

184

Assumption 5: If the Mental Rotations Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial Experience

Questionnaire. Thirty-three men and 95 women completed the two versions of the

Mental Rotations Test. On version A, men had a mean score of 5.58 and women had a mean of 3.81. On version B, the mean for males was 4.09 and for females, 2.92. These male-favored gender differences were both significant (t = 3.88, p < .000; t = 2.93, p =

.004 respectively), however only version A reached significance on multivariate analysis: version A, F (1,118) = 17.11, p <.001; version B, F (1,118) = 3.58, p = .06. Figures 23 and 24 illustrate the gender differences in each program for the individual versions of the

Mental Rotations Test. Males outperformed females in each Allied Medical student group on version A of the test; however, females did better than males in the AT and MD groups on version B of the test. This must be interpreted cautiously because of the small number of students in each group.

185

Figure 23. Variations in Performance on the Mental Rotations Test, Version A

Figure 24. Variations in Performance on the Mental Rotations Test, Version B

186

Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire

Participation in building models was the only positive and significant activity correlated with version A of the Mental Rotations Test (Table 35). A significant negative correlation was found between reported participation in gymnastics and this test. Version

B of the Mental Rotations Test was significantly correlated with reported participation in playing chess, making or repairing furniture, and building models. When scores for the two versions of the test were combined, positive and significant correlations were found with participation in making or repairing furniture and participation in building models.

Spatial Activity Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Test, Test, Version A Test, Version B Combined Playing Chess .19 (.04) Making or repairing .22 .18 furniture (.01) (.04) Building models .24 .24 .27 (.01) (.01) (.002) Performing -.18 gymnastics (.05) Table 35. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Participation in Spatial Activities and Scores on the Mental Rotations Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

Spearman correlations between version A scores on the MRT and reported enjoyment of spatial activities were positive and significant for three activities: playing chess, building models, and playing pool (Table 36). Scores on version B of the MRT were only positively correlated with playing chess. On the other hand, scores on version 187

B of the MRT were negatively correlated with sewing or embroidery and with weaving or macramé. Combined scores for the two versions of the test were positively related to playing chess, building models, and playing pool. All of the correlation coefficients would be considered to be in the range of small to moderate effects. It is interesting to note that the positive correlations all tend to be activities more commonly engaged in by males while the significant negative correlations for both participation and enjoyment tend to be considered female activities. The limited number of significant correlations found between the Mental Rotations Test and spatial activities would not be strong evidence for validity of either the test or the spatial survey.

Spatial Activity Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Mental Rotations Test, Version A Test, Version B Test, Combined Playing chess .26 .20 .27 (.003) (.02) (.002) Sewing or -.18 Embroidering (.04) Building models .31 .23 (.000) (.01) Playing pool .20 .20 (.02) (.02) Weaving or macrame -.20 (.02) Table 36. Statistically Significant Spearman Correlations between Extent of Enjoyment in Spatial Activities and Scores on the Mental Rotations Test Note. Numbers in parentheses are two-tailed p-values.

Additional Validation Evidence for the Spatial Experience Questionnaire

Research Question 3: To what extent is validity evidence provided for the use of the

Spatial Experience Questionnaire to measure spatial ability in Allied Medical students? 188

Reliability of the Spatial Experience Questionnaire was calculated for both the participation section and the enjoyment section. The coefficient alpha for the participation scale is .757 (n = 126), and that for the enjoyment scale is .779 (n = 125).

Both values are considered acceptable indications that the instruments are consistent measures of participation in spatial activities and enjoyment of spatial activities in this group of subjects. Inter-item correlations are small indicating that the individual items are not measuring the same constructs. Additional validation evidence is included with each of the spatial tests above.

189

Chapter 5: Discussion and Conclusions

The Allied Medical Professions encompass a group of occupations which provide therapy and/or treatment for patients, consult with physicians, and maintain the records for a variety of health conditions. Each of these professionals relies on spatial ability in the performance of their duties to some extent.

Studies with physicians and dentists have attempted to examine the importance of spatial ability in these professions. Dental schools use a spatial ability test as part of their admission testing. Some physician groups have also examined the possibility that spatial ability is a requisite skill for medical school. Others have evaluated levels of spatial skill in relation to specific job skills such as performing certain types of surgery. Also, studies have examined whether medical students self-select their specializations based on spatial skill, or whether spatial ability is enhanced by practice.

If spatial skill is necessary in successful performance of Allied Medical job skills, students in these professions could be evaluated for entry level skills. Educators could develop tools to assist students who have lower levels of spatial ability.

To examine spatial skills in Allied Medical Professional students, valid measures of this trait must be chosen. This study examined validity evidence for six tests of spatial ability in a sample of five Allied Medical Professions student groups: Athletic Training,

190

Health Information Management and Systems, Medical Dietetics, Occupational Therapy, and Radiologic Sciences.

Spatial Skills in Allied Medical Professions

To determine if these five groups of Allied Medical Professions differed in need or reliance on spatial ability, various documents detailing job duties were examined.

These documents came from professional websites, certification agencies, and the

Department of Labor website. A complete listing of the tasks associated with each profession is included in Appendices A through E. These tasks were grouped using the model of spatial ability proposed by Linn and Petersen (1985) and Voyer, Voyer, and

Bryden (1995) which divides spatial ability into three categories: spatial perception, spatial visualization, and mental rotation.

This analysis would seem to indicate that there are differences in the need for spatial ability in the performance of job tasks in this group of Allied Medical Professions.

The majority of spatially related tasks were found for Athletic Training, Occupational

Therapy, and Radiologic Science professions.

Spatial perception is a type of spatial ability which requires orientation of one’s own body in space, or in relation to another person or thing. It is believed that this ability is involved in activities that require the ability to perceive up and down or right and left.

Athletic Trainers, Occupational Therapists, and Radiologic Science professionals have many duties which require that they work directly with patients and with various types of

191 equipment. These types of duties require spatial perception in order to function efficiently. Laboratory courses and clinical internships during training programs provide opportunities for students to interact with equipment, and with patients (either real or simulated). Students with low levels of spatial perception ability might struggle more in these types of courses than students with high spatial perception ability.

Spatial visualization has been described as involving multistep processes in mentally picturing, manipulating, transforming, or rearranging objects (Linn & Petersen,

1985; McGee, 1979; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). Other tasks that require spatial visualization include recognition of patterns, locating and disembedding information from complex backgrounds, and recognition of visual cues. The majority of job tasks examined in each profession fell into this category. Allied Medical professionals must be able to mentally picture anatomic structures and physiologic processes. Each group of professionals must be able to examine their environment and identify environmental and ergonomic risks for themselves, their co-workers, and their patients. Other spatial visualization tasks shared by these professions include the preparation, examination, and interpretation of charts, graphs, and other medical information. Some activities, such as performing venipuncture, palpation of surface landmarks, and interpretation of radiographic images are unique to Athletic Training, Occupational Therapy, and

Radiologic Science. These three groups had the greatest proportion of job tasks in the spatial visualization category.

Mental rotation involves an ability to mentally rotate all or a portion of an object into a different or unique orientation. One task that exemplifies this type of ability is

192 interpretation of cross-sectional radiographic images. Professionals in Athletic Training,

Occupational Therapy, and Radiologic Science must be able to mentally manipulate anatomic images to determine their appearance in a different orientation.

Spatial ability has been linked to better performance in academic programs such as art, geometry, chemistry, anatomy, and engineering (Bodner & Guay, 1997; Carter,

LaRussa, & Bodner, 1987; Smith, 1964; Garg, Norman, & Sperotable, 2001; Guillot,

Champely, Batier, Thiriet, & Collet, 2007). In chemistry, spatial ability was linked to better performance on test items that required problem solving skills (Carter, LaRussa, &

Bodner, 1987). In anatomy, students with better spatial ability, especially mental rotation ability, performed better on examinations (Garg, Norman, & Sperotable, 2001; Guillot,

Champely, Batier, Thiriet, & Collet, 2007; Luursema, Verwey, Kommers, & Annema,

2008; Rochford, 1985; Smoker, Berbaum, Luebke, & Jacoby, 1984). If this is true, this information could be valuable to educators in Allied Medical student programs.

Understanding the nature of spatial ability could provide educators with a diagnostic tool for assisting low performing students in both didactic and clinical courses. Practice in spatial problem-solving, training in cognitive strategies, and formal training in additional math, science, and technical courses have all been found to improve spatial ability (De

Lisi & Cammarano, 1996; Kyllonen, Lohman, & Snow, 1984; Lord, 1990; Lunneborg &

Lunneborg, 1984) and could be implemented in Allied Medical professional education.

Training in spatial ability has proven to be effective in programs such as engineering and anatomy, and may merit consideration in Allied Medical student educational programs. Reports on courses developed to provide spatial practice for

193 engineering students have shown significant gains in course grades and retention (Martin-

Dorta, Saorin, & Contero, 2008; Sorby & Baartmans, 2000). As with engineering, remedial coursework may be helpful in those students with poor spatial ability.

To examine spatial ability in Allied Medical professionals, appropriate tests must be found. Using the theoretical model of spatial ability proposed by Linn and Petersen, four tests were analyzed to examine spatial ability in Allied Medical students. Since no studies have examined the interpretation of results of these tests in Allied Medical students, Kane’s theoretical framework for validation was employed. Validity evidence for each test used in this study was based on four assertions. Evidence supporting or refuting the assertions follows below.

Validation Evidence for the Cube Comparison Test

The Cube Comparison Test is classified as a measure of spatial orientation by the

Educational Testing Service in the Manual for Kit of Factor-Referenced Cognitive Tests

(Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). This manual defines spatial orientation as “The ability to perceive spatial patterns or to maintain orientation with respect to objects in space” (p. 149). It has been suggested that body orientation is an essential part of the problem (Guilford, 1967; Lohman, 1988; McGee, 1979). In a meta-analysis by Linn and

Petersen (1985), an ability where subjects must determine spatial relationships with respect to the orientation of their own bodies, in spite of distracting information, is called spatial perception. Because this test is classified as a test of spatial orientation, and

194 because the definitions of spatial orientation and spatial perception are nearly the same, this test was chosen as a measure of spatial perception for this study.

Assumption 1: Scores on the Cube Comparison Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. The Cube

Comparison Test consists of 42 items with two equivalent versions of 21 items each.

Average scores on the combined versions of this test have been reported to be from 10.9 to 23.5 (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976; Jardine & Martin, 1984). Combined scores for the Allied Medical students in this study averaged 15.86. High ability was defined as a score of one standard deviation above the mean, a combined score of at least 24 items.

Twenty-six students, 20.38% of the group, had scores in the high ability range. One person received a high score of 37 out of 42, but no students had all items correct. Low ability was defined as at least one standard deviation below the mean, or a score of seven or fewer items correct. Seventeen students, 13.3% of the group had scores in the low ability range. These groupings are similar to those reported in the literature (Ekstrom,

French, & Harman, 1976; Jardine & Martin, 1984; Lord, 1990), supporting this assertion for validity.

Assertion 2: The Cube Comparison Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability.

Reliability estimates for a test can be affected by many factors: length of a test, item difficulty, composition of the group, nature of the testing situation, and whether the test is speeded. Longer tests are generally found to be more reliable. Item difficulty must vary; that is, the test can’t be so easy that all items are answered correctly or so hard that all items are answered incorrectly. If the group is too homogenous, generally reliability

195 estimates are lowered. Differences in testing situations will lower reliability. Speeded tests result in some students not completing every item which tends to decrease reliability estimates.

Reliability estimates for the Cube Comparison Test have been reported to be anywhere from .47 to .84 (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976; Lord, 1990). Two equivalent versions of the Cube Test were administered. Each version consisted of 21 items, and each version was speeded. Internal consistency and split-half reliability resulted in lower estimates for reliability. The Kuder-Richardson formula 20 is a method used to estimate internal consistency for dichotomous variables. The K-R 20 reliability coefficient for version A of the Cube Comparison Test was .68 and for version B was .79.

Although the coefficient for version B is considered acceptable, the coefficient for version A would be considered below the acceptable level. This low reliability coefficient for version A would most likely be attributed to the use of a measure of internal consistency on a speeded test. Parallel forms reliability was calculated as an answer to this problem. The two versions of the test were highly correlated (r = .64), and the parallel forms reliability coefficient was .84. This reliability estimate is well within the accepted levels (Anastasi & Urbina, 1997; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994; Tavakol &

Dennick, 2011), and supports the assertion that the Cube Comparison Test is a reliable measure of spatial ability in this group of Allied Medical students.

Assumption 3: The Cube Comparison Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. The Mental

Rotations Test (Vandenberg & Kuse, 1978) has been used by numerous studies as a

196 measure of spatial ability in general and as a measure of mental rotation ability in particular (Linn & Petersen, 1985; McGee, 1979; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). If a test measures spatial ability it should show a positive correlation with the Mental

Rotations Test. The Cube Comparison Test is purported to be a measure of spatial orientation which is a category of spatial ability, but which may be a different skill than mental rotation (Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). This difference in category of spatial ability may be responsible for lowering the correlation between these two tests somewhat. However, the Cube Comparison Test was found to have a reasonably strong correlation with the Mental Rotations Test (r = .47, p < .001). This lends evidence that the Cube Comparison Test results can be interpreted as an indication of spatial ability for the sample in this study.

Assertion 4: The Cube Comparison Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the PSVT Visualization of Views Test. Tests of spatial ability have been categorized as measuring three classes of skills. One group of skills, spatial perception, involves activities in which spatial relationships are determined in relation to one’s own body (Linn & Petersen, 1985). This skill also involves manipulation from the perspective of one’s own body and determination of how an object will appear from a new or different perspective (Bishop, 1980; Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976; Guilford,

1967; Lohman, 1988; McGee, 1979). Two tests were selected to measure this ability: the

Cube Comparison Test and the Visualization of Views subtest of the Purdue Spatial

Visualization Test battery. If these tests measure the same category of spatial ability, they should be highly and positively correlated. In the sample of students studied, results

197 from these two tests had a Pearson correlation of .30 (p = .001). While this is considered to be a moderate effect size, it is lower than would be expected if these tests measure the same ability. This correlation is also smaller than the correlation between the Cube Test and the MRT. Evidence for concurrent validity for the tests measuring spatial perception is therefore only weakly supported.

Assumption 5: If the Cube Comparison Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores should reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire. No significant gender differences were found for the students taking this test. In fact, in the AT, HIMS, and MD groups females did better than males on version A of the test and females in HIMS and MD did better than males on version B.

Few significant correlations were found between the Cube Comparison Test and either participation in or enjoyment of spatial activities. Of those significant correlations found, all are small (.20 or less). Only participation in sketching house plans, and reported enjoyment of solving mathematical riddles were significantly correlated with the combined versions of the test. Two other activity participation scores were significantly correlated with results on the Cube Test: taking pictures, and skiing and snowboarding.

Both of these correlations were negative, the first with version A and the second with version B. Enjoyment scores for building models were positively correlated, and enjoyment scores for sketching auto design were negatively correlated for version A and version B respectively. These correlations are contrary to what would be expected, especially for skiing and snowboarding which relies heavily on the ability to orient one’s

198 self in space. The lack of gender differences and small number and small magnitude of the correlations do not support this assertion.

For this sample of students, differentiation of spatial ability similar to that reported in the literature was achieved by this test. Reliability evidence was satisfactory, and a moderately high correlation was found between the Cube Comparison Test and the

Mental Rotations Test. These satisfy the first three assertions proposed in the validation argument. The Cube Comparison Test was moderately correlated with the Visualization of Views Test, which does not support the assertion that these tests are both measures of spatial perception. No gender differences were found in scores on the Cube Comparison

Test, and there were few significant correlations with spatial activities. Therefore, there is some evidence that this test can be interpreted as a valid measure of spatial ability in

Allied Medical students, but not that it is able to measure spatial perception.

Validation Evidence for the Visualization of Views subtest of the Purdue Spatial

Visualization Test

Visualization of Views is one subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test battery. This test presents a figure enclosed within an imaginary box, and asks the subject to imagine the shape of the figure from a different viewing position. There are two equivalent versions of this test, although only the first version was used in this study.

The test includes 12 items and is scored as the number correct with no correction for

199 guessing. This test was selected, because of the need to mentally orient one’s perspective in relation to the test items, as a second test of spatial perception.

Assumption 1: Scores on the Visualization of Views Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. Scores on the Visualization of Views Test have been reported between 44% and 58% (Cohen &

Hegary, 2007). Guay (1978) reported an average score of 6.62 for females and 9.26 for males in a group of 217 university undergraduates.

The mean score for the students in this study was 5.72 or 48% correct out of a maximum 12 items. This average is in line with mean scores reported in the literature.

High spatial ability was defined as a score of 9 which is at least one standard deviation above the mean. Twenty-one students (16.5% of the sample) would comprise the high ability group. Twenty-six students (20.3% of the sample) had scores of 3 or less which is at least one standard deviation below the mean; this group was defined as the low ability group. Students in Radiologic Sciences had the highest mean score for this test (6.21) and the largest proportion in the high achievement group. Students in HIMS had the lowest mean score (5.17), and OT had the largest proportion of students in the low ability group. No significant differences in mean scores were found for any of the groups.

Assumption 2: The Visualization of Views Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability. This test contains 12 items and is speeded. Kuder-Richarson and Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities have been reported anywhere from .65 to .80 for this test (Guay, 1978;

Hegarty, Keehner, Khooshabeh, & Montello, 2009). For this study, answers were coded as correct or incorrect. Because of the dichotomous variables, internal consistency

200 reliability was estimated using the Kuder-Richardson formula 20. For this test, the K-R

20 reliability coefficient was .74. This is within the acceptable level of reliability; however the speeded nature of this test would affect the value of the coefficients.

Reliability evidence would be stronger if a parallel version of the test were given and used to compute reliability estimates.

Assumption 3: The Visualization of Views Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. Pearson correlations between the Visualization of Views Test were positive and significant for both the individual versions and the combined version of the Mental Rotations Test. The correlation between the combined versions of the Mental Rotations Test and the

Visualization of Views Test was .42 (p < .001), which is considered a medium to large effect. While this offers evidence of criterion validity in interpretation of scores for this test, the moderate size of the correlation coefficient may be related to this test measuring spatial perception rather than mental rotation.

Assumption 4: The Visualization of Views Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the Cube Comparison Test. As discussed previously, if the Visualization of Views Test and the Cube Comparison Test are both measures of spatial perception, correlation coefficients should be high and positive. However, the correlation coefficient between these two tests (r = .30) is smaller than the correlation between the Mental Rotations Test and either of these tests. This offers little evidence to support this assumption for validity.

201

Assumption 5: If the Visualization of Views Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire. A significant male-favored gender difference was found on this test, with males in all five student groups scoring better than females. This provides some support for this assumption of validity.

The scores on the Visualization of Views Test were significantly related to participation in five spatial activities: sketching house plans, playing chess, using hand tools, playing pool, playing golf, and reading a map/using a compass. Test scores were also positively related to reported enjoyment in sketching house plans, playing chess, using hand tools, making or repairing furniture, playing pool, and reading a map/using a compass. It is interesting that playing golf is positively related to scores on this test, but enjoying golf is not. It is also noteworthy that this test, of the six used in this study, had the highest number of correlated spatial activities. Most of the correlations are between

.22 and .33 putting them in the range of moderate effects. This provides supporting evidence for this validity assertion.

For this sample of students, differentiation of spatial ability similar to that reported in the literature was achieved by the Visualization of Views Test. Reliability evidence was satisfactory, although administration and analysis of a parallel form of the test would provide stronger evidence. A moderately high correlation was found between the Visualization of Views Test and the Mental Rotations Test. These satisfy the first three assertions proposed in the validation argument. The Cube Comparison Test was

202 moderately correlated with the Visualization of Views Test, which does not support the assertion that these tests are both measures of spatial perception. Significant male- favored gender differences were found in scores on the Visualization of Views Test, and there were significant correlations with multiple spatial activities from the Spatial

Experiences Questionnaire. Therefore, there is fairly strong evidence that this test can be interpreted as a valid measure of spatial ability in Allied Medical students. However, the magnitude of the correlation coefficients between this test and the Cube Comparison Test does not support the assertion that it is able to measure spatial perception.

Validation Evidence for the Visualization of Developments Subtest of the Purdue Spatial

Visualization Test

Surface development tests are commonly used as tests of spatial visualization

(Baker & Talley, 1972; Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976; Gilligan, Welsh, Watts, &

Treasure, 1999; Guay & McDaniel, 1977). Spatial visualization involves the ability to

“manipulate or transform the image of spatial patterns into other arrangements”

(Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976, p. 173). Surface development tests involve a two- dimensional drawing of a figure (a development), and the task is to mentally fold this into a three-dimensional object. Each of the 12 items on the Visualization of Developments

Test includes a development with one shaded region indicating the bottom surface and five options which might correspond to what the development would resemble when folded into a three-dimensional object. For each test item there is one correct answer, for

203 a maximum score of 12. Two equivalent versions of this test are available and both were used as part of this study to establish reliability.

Assumption 1: Scores on the Visualization of Developments Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. Guay

(1978) reported means of 7.88 for females and 9.67 for males on a 12 item version of this test. In this study, the overall mean for the combined test was 9.16 (standard deviation =

4.14) out of a possible 24. The average score for version A was 4.24 out of a possible 12, and for version B the average was 4.91 out of a possible 12. Eighteen students (14.1% of the sample) had at least 14 items correct out of a possible 24, which was at least one standard deviation above the mean, and were considered high ability. Twenty-six students (20.3%) had five or fewer items correct out of 24, which was at least one standard deviation below the mean, and were considered low ability. Students in HIMS had the highest mean score for version A, and those in OT had the lowest. Students in

RS had the highest mean score on version B of the test and those in AT had the lowest.

There were no significant differences between the mean scores for any of the student groups.

This test proved to be very difficult for most of the students in the sample. No one answered all of the items correctly, and only four of the 128 sampled were able to score 75% or better on this test. No clear performance trends emerged for any of the groups completing this test. These findings provide little support for this assumption for validity.

204

Assumption 2: The Visualization of Developments Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability. The K-R 20 reliability for this test has been reported to be somewhere between .65 and .87 by Guay (1978). In this study, K-R 20 reliability coefficients were found to be .62 for version A, and .74 for version B. The correlation between the two forms of the test was .53. Parallel forms estimates were used to obtain overall reliability coefficients for this test due to its speeded nature. This reliability coefficient was found to be .62.

The correlation between the two versions of this test is lower than would be expected if they are measuring the same construct. Also, the parallel forms reliability for the two versions of the test is below what is normally considered acceptable (Anastasi &

Urbina, 1997; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). This evidence is not adequate to say that this assumption for validity has been met.

Assumption 3: The Visualization of Developments Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. Pearson correlations between the individual versions of the Visualization of Developments Test and the individual versions of the Mental Rotations Test are all positive and significant, and range from .22 to .46. The correlation between the combined versions of each test is

.41 (p < .001). This moderate correlation is evidence that the two tests are measuring a similar construct, but may be somewhat low because the Visualization of Developments

Test is a measure of spatial visualization while the Mental Rotations Test is a measure of the mental rotations category of spatial ability.

205

Assumption 4: The Visualization of Developments Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the Hidden Figures Test. Both the

Visualization of Developments Test and the Hidden Figures Test require the complex, multistep processes of spatial visualization. If these two tests measure the same construct, they should be highly correlated. For the sample in this study, scores on version A of the Developments Test were significantly related to the scores on the

Hidden Figures Test, but those on version B of the Developments Test were not. When scores for version A and B were combined, a small but significant correlation was found between the two tests. This offers very limited support for the assumption that these tests are measuring the same construct.

Assumption 5: If the Visualization of Developments Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire. No significant gender differences were found for version A, version B, or the combined versions of the Developments Test. In general, the smallest gender differences have been found on tests of spatial visualization (Linn & Petersen,

1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). The lack of even small gender differences on this test casts doubt on its usefulness as a measure of spatial ability in this group of students.

Participation in sketching house plans was significantly related to both versions of the Developments test as well as to the overall score. Participation in making or repairing furniture was related to version A scores and overall scores. Participation in playing chess was negatively correlated with version A of the test. There were positive and

206 significant correlations between both versions and combined scores on the Developments

Test and reported enjoyment level for two activities: sketching house plans and building models. Enjoyment scores for playing checkers and for reading maps were also positively correlated, but only with version B of the test. Enjoyment for playing tennis was negatively correlated with the combined score on the Developments Test. The correlations between activities and test scores ranged from -.22 to .33 and would generally be considered small effects. These correlations do not provide adequate evidence to support this assertion for validity.

Little evidence for validity was found for the Visualization of Developments test for Allied Medical students. Overall scores were lower than those reported in the literature, and reliability coefficients were below the acceptable range. While the

Visualization of Developments Test did correlate moderately with the Mental Rotations

Test, no significant gender differences were found. There is little evidence that this test can be interpreted as a measure of spatial visualization based on the small correlation with the Hidden Figures Test, or as a measure of spatial ability in general with Allied

Medical students.

Validation Evidence for the Hidden Figures Test

The Hidden Figures Test has been presented as a test of flexibility of closure which is defined as “the ability to hold a given visual percept or configuration in mind so as to disembed it from other well defined perceptual material” (Ekstrom, French, &

207

Harman, 1976, p. 19). This test has been used as a measure of field independence and of figure-ground perception (Davis & Eliot, 1994). The test has been described as having a high level of difficulty, and sharing some variance on spatial visualization in factor studies (Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976). Hidden and embedded figures tests have also been used as a measure of general spatial ability and as a measure of spatial visualization (Hassler, Birbaumer, & Feil, 1985; Hegarty, Montello, Richardson,

Ishikawa, & Lovelace, 2006; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995).

The Hidden Figures Test used in this study presents subjects with five simple geometric figures. One of these figures is hidden within each of the complex test items. The test requires multistep mental manipulation of information in determining which geometric figure is embedded within the more complex test items. Because of the mental processes involved, this test was chosen as the second test of spatial visualization.

Assumption 1: Scores on the Hidden Figures Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. Two equivalent versions of the test are available, each with 16 items. Only the first version of the test was used for this study due to time constraints. Average scores for the 32 item versions of the test range from 8.4 to 15.8 (Davis & Eliot, 1994; Dunn & Eliot, 1993; Ekstrom,

French, & Harman, 1976; Stumpf, Performance factors and gender-related differences in spatial ability: Another assessment, 1993).

In this study, scores for the Hidden Figures Test were positively skewed with a mean of 6.57 out of a total of 16 items. This is consistent with the means reported in the literature. High ability students were classified as those scoring at least 11 correct

208 responses out of 16 which was one standard deviation above the mean. Twenty-five students (19.5% of the sample) had scores in the high ability range. Low ability students were classified as those having scores of 3 or below which was one standard deviation below the mean. Twenty-seven students (21.1% of the sample) were in the low ability group. Occupational Therapy students had the highest mean (7.08), and HIMS students the lowest (5.52). Thirty percent of the 23 HIMS students had scores classifying them as low ability. Two students earned the maximum score of 16, one in MD and one in OT.

In spite of these differences, no significant differences were found between the groups.

Assumption2: The Hidden Figures Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability.

Reliability estimates for the Hidden Figures Tests have been reported between .71 and

.85 (Davis & Eliot, 1994; Dunn & Eliot, 1993; Ekstrom, French, & Harman, 1976).

Because of the dichotomous variables, internal consistency reliability was calculated using K-R 20 and was found to be .77. While this reliability coefficient is within the acceptable range (Anastasi & Urbina, 1997; Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994), giving a parallel form of the test and using that in the computation of reliability estimates would strengthen the evidence for this assumption.

Assumption 3: The Hidden Figures Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. Pearson correlations were examined to evaluate the relationship between the Hidden Figures Test and the Mental Rotations Test. No significant correlations were found between the scores on the Hidden Figures Test and either version of the Mental Rotations Test.

Although this test was chosen as a measure of spatial visualization, there should be a

209 significant positive correlation with the Mental Rotations Test. This lack of correlation calls into question the interpretation of this test as a measure of spatial ability.

Assumption 4: The Hidden Figures Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the Visualization of Developments Test. Both the Hidden

Figures Test and the Visualization of Developments Test were chosen as measures of spatial visualization. If these two tests measure the same trait, high positive correlations would be expected. A small positive correlation (r = .23, p = .01) was found between the

Hidden Figures Test and version A of the Developments Test only. This assumption for concurrent validity evidence is therefore not supported.

Assertion 5: If the Hidden Figures Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial Experience

Questionnaire. Scores on the Hidden Figures Test showed that females had an average of 6.97 items correct out of 16 and males had an average 5.42 items correct out of 16.

These scores were significantly different. On almost all tests of spatial ability, males do better than females. Linn and Petersen (1985) found that tests of spatial visualization had the smallest gender differences, but males still did significantly better. This lack of the normal male-favored gender difference casts doubt on the validity of these test scores as measures of spatial ability in this group of Allied Medical students.

Only one spatial activity participation score reached significance when correlated with Hidden Figures Test scores. This was a negative correlation with participation in playing tennis. Two spatial activities from the enjoyment scale had small positive

210 correlations: arranging furniture and interior decorating. One activity from the enjoyment scale had a small, negative correlation with Hidden Figures Test scores: playing golf.

Little evidence for validity was found for the Hidden Figures Test for Allied

Medical students. Overall scores were similar to those reported in the literature, and reliability coefficients were within the acceptable range. However, the scores on the

Hidden Figures Test did not significantly correlate with the Mental Rotations Test.

Significant gender differences were found, but favoring women instead of men. There is little evidence that this test can be interpreted as a measure of spatial visualization based on the small correlation with the Visualization of Developments Test, or as a measure of spatial ability in general with Allied Medical students.

Validation Evidence for the Visualization of Rotations Subtest of the Purdue Spatial

Visualization Test battery

The Visualization of Rotations Test is one portion of the Purdue Spatial

Visualization Test battery. This test was described as being “designed to measure a specific type of spatial visualizing ability that requires imagining the movement of three- dimensional figures according to explicit directions” (Guay, 1980, p. 9). The directions depict two three-dimensional objects, one a rotated version of the other. Subjects must visualize the rotation, examine a second three-dimensional object and picture it rotated in the same way as the example, and then choose the image that matches the rotated object

211 from five options. Because of the rotational nature of the question prompts, this test was chosen as one of the measures of mental rotation ability.

Assumption 1: Scores on the Visualization of Rotations Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in the sample. Means for the 12 item version of the Visualization of Rotations Test have been reported to range from 5.96 for females to 8.55 for males (Guay, 1978). Bodner and Guay (1997) reported means ranging from 10.45 to 15.14 on a twenty item version of the test and Sorby (2000) reported average scores of 68.1% and 79.6% respectively for female and male college freshmen.

The mean score for the study sample on this test was 4.28 items correct out of a total of 12 items (35.68% correct). The maximum score earned on the test for 99% of the sample was 9 or fewer items, with one student scoring 11 correct items. Scores for the five groups of Allied Medical students in this sample were slightly negatively skewed, due to this one student’s high score. Students in HIMS and MD had the highest mean scores while those in OT had the lowest mean score. Twenty-four students obtained scores of 7 or more which was at least one standard deviation above the mean; however, only11 students would have earned a “passing” score on this test. Thirty students were classified as low ability because they had scores of 2 or fewer correct items, which was at least one standard deviation below the group mean. Almost 36% of OT students were in the low ability group, while only 8.7% of the HIMS students were in the low ability grouping. This seemed to be a difficult test for the majority of students tested. No significant differences in means were found between any of the student groups.

212

Assumption 2: The Visualization of Rotations Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability. Reliability for this test was calculated using the K-R 20 formula due to the dichotomous nature of the variables. The K-R 20 reliability coefficient was .70. This reliability estimate is on the low end of the acceptable range (Anastasi & Urbina, 1997;

Nunnally & Bernstein, 1994). This is understandable since the test proved very difficult for the students in this group with 67% answering less than half of the questions correctly. Speeded tests also decrease the reliability estimates. Because no parallel form of this test was given, it is difficult to draw conclusions regarding this assertion for reliability.

Assumption 3: The Visualization of Rotations Test will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. Because the Visualization of Rotations Test is purported to be a test of spatial ability, it should demonstrate a strong correlation with the Mental Rotations Test. For this part of the analysis, Pearson correlations were calculated between the Visualization of Rotations

Test and the individual and combined versions of the Mental Rotations Test. Correlation coefficients were larger between the Visualization of Rotations Test and version A of the

MRT than for version B. The overall correlation coefficient was .42 (p < .001). This would be considered a moderate to high effect, and is similar to the other between-test correlations found in this study providing evidence of criterion-related validity.

Assumption 4: The Visualization of Rotations Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the Mental Rotations Test. The Visualization of

Rotations Test was chosen as a test of mental rotation, and as such, should have a high

213 correlation with the Mental Rotations Test if these two tests measure the same trait.

However, the correlation coefficient between these two tests is only .42. In fact, the

Visualization of Rotations Test has a stronger correlation with the two versions of the

Visualization of Developments Test (r = .49, p < .001 for version A, r = .46, p < .001 for version B) than with the Mental Rotations Test. The findings in this study therefore, do not support the assertion that the Visualization of Rotations and the Mental Rotations

Tests are measuring the same spatial trait.

Assertion 5: If the Visualization of Rotations Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial

Experience Questionnaire. Tests of mental rotation ability typically show the greatest differences in spatial ability between men and women. This male-favored gender difference was replicated in this study. In fact males did better than females in each of the five Allied Medical student groups, although these findings must be tempered in light of the increased likelihood of Type 1 error.

Two activities from the participation scale of the Spatial Activities Questionnaire were positively related to scores on the Visualization of Rotations Test: sketching house plans and playing golf. Four activities from the enjoyment scale were positively correlated with this test: sketching house plans, building models, playing pool, and reading a map/using a compass. On the other hand, both participation in and enjoyment of taking pictures were negatively correlated with scores on the Visualization of

Rotations Test. Correlations for each of these activities ranged from .18 to .31, which

214 would be considered small to moderate effects. These correlations provide some evidence that the Visualization of Rotations Test is measuring the trait of spatial ability.

For this sample of students, differentiation of spatial ability similar to that reported in the literature was achieved by the Visualization of Rotations Test. Reliability evidence was satisfactory, although administration and analysis of a parallel form of the test would provide stronger evidence. A moderately high correlation was found between the Visualization of Rotations Test and the Mental Rotations Test. These satisfy the first three assertions proposed in the validation argument. Significant male-favored gender differences were found in scores on the Visualization of Rotations Test, and there were significant correlations with multiple spatial activities from the Spatial Experiences

Questionnaire. Therefore, there is fairly strong evidence that this test can be interpreted as a valid measure of spatial ability in Allied Medical students. However, the magnitude of the correlation coefficients between this test and the Mental Rotations Test does not support the assertion that both tests are valid measures of mental rotation ability.

Validation Evidence for the Mental Rotations Test

The Mental Rotations Test was adapted from the Shepard and Metzler test for group administration in testing three-dimensional spatial visualization ability (Vandenberg &

Kuse, 1978). Meta-analyses conducted by Linn and Petersen (1985) and Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden (1995) found this test showed the largest gender differences and led them to differentiate mental rotation as a separate ability from spatial visualization. The test is

215 comprised of two equivalent 10 item versions. For each item on the test, the subject is presented with a prompt drawing of a three-dimensional figure and four options, two of which are rotated versions of the prompt and two of which are rotated mirror images of the prompt. The developer recommends that one point be awarded only if both correct options are chosen, which alleviates the need to correct for guessing (Vandenberg &

Kuse, 1978). This test was chosen as a test of mental rotation for this study.

Assumption 1: Scores on the Mental Rotations Test will be able to differentiate level of spatial ability for the Allied Medical students in this sample. Conventional scoring of this test rewards one point only if both correct options have been chosen.

Mean scores on this test, using the conventional scoring method, range from 5 to 12.6 items correct out of a total of 20 (Cherney, 2008; Guillot A. , Louis, Thiriet, & Collet,

2007; Moe, Meneghetti, & Cadinu, 2009; Moe & Pazzaglia, 2006; Masters M. , 1998).

The mean scores on the Mental Rotations Test in this study were 4.27 out of 10 for version A, 3.22 out of 10 for version B, and 7.49 out of 20 for the combined test versions.

These mean scores are within the range reported in the literature. A combined score of

12 or greater was considered high ability (one standard deviation above the mean), and a score of 3 or less was considered low ability (one standard deviation below the mean).

Scores were slightly positively skewed for both the individual versions and the combined test. Almost 31% of the RS students had scores that placed them in the high ability group, while only 10% of the AT students were in this group. The OT group had the greatest proportion in the low ability group with 17.9% of OT students scoring 3 or fewer

216 correct out of 20. Mean scores for RS were the highest (8.34) and the mean for HIMS students was the lowest (6.70). No significant differences in means were found.

Assumption 2: The Mental Rotations Test will demonstrate acceptable reliability.

Two equivalent forms of the Mental Rotations Test were administered. Each form was analyzed for internal consistency using Kuder-Richardson formula 20 because of the dichotomous nature of the variables. The K-R 20 coefficient for version A of the test was

.76, and for version B was .64. It is not surprising that the second version of the test demonstrated lower reliability since over 75% of the subjects had scores of less than half of the possible 10. The correlation between forms of the test was r = .62. Reliability for the entire Mental Rotations Test was calculated by parallel forms and was r = .82. This degree of reliability is well within the acceptable range and offers acceptable evidence for assumption two.

Assumption 3: The other tests of spatial ability will demonstrate criterion validity evidence when compared to the gold standard Mental Rotations Test. This assumption was addressed individually with each of the other tests of spatial ability in this study.

Assumption 4: The Mental Rotations Test will demonstrate concurrent validity evidence when compared to the Visualization of Rotations Test. The Mental Rotations

Test and the Visualization of Rotations Test were chosen as measures of mental rotation ability for this study. If, in fact these two tests measure the same trait, there should be a large positive correlation. The correlation between these two tests is .42, but is lower than would be expected. In fact, the Mental Rotations Test had equivalent or higher correlations with the Visualization of Views Test (r = .42), version B of the Cube

217

Comparison Test (r = .47), version B of the Visualization of Developments Test (r = .43), and the combined Cube Comparison Test (r = .48). The assumption that the Mental

Rotations Test and the Visualization of Rotations Test measure the same spatial trait is weakly supported by the data obtained with this sample.

Assumption 5: If the Mental Rotations Test measures the trait of spatial ability, scores will reflect the male-favored gender differences commonly found in the literature, and will be positively related to the activities examined with the Spatial Experience

Questionnaire. The Mental Rotations Test typically shows large and consistent gender differences in spatial ability. In this study, men had an average score of 9.67 and women had an average score of 6.74. This difference was highly significant (t = 3.86, p < .001), but must be examined in light of the possibility of increased Type 1 error.

Version A of the Mental Rotations Test was positively correlated with participation in building models, but negatively correlated with performing gymnastics.

Version B was positively correlated with participation in playing chess, making or repairing furniture, and building models. Overall, the Mental Rotations Test was only correlated with two participation scores: making or repairing furniture and building models. From the enjoyment spatial scale, both the individual and the combined Mental

Rotations Test scores correlated positively with playing chess. Version A and the combined test were positively correlated with building models and playing pool. Version

B of the test was negatively correlated with sewing/embroidery and weaving/macramé. It is interesting to note that activities with positive correlations tend to be considered masculine activities while those with negative correlations tend to be considered feminine

218 activities. The values of the correlation coefficients range from -.18 to .31, and would be considered small to moderate effects. The values and signs of the correlation coefficients do support this assumption regarding gender differences on this test.

For this sample of students, differentiation of spatial ability similar to that reported in the literature was achieved by the Mental Rotations Test. Reliability evidence was satisfactory, well within the acceptable range. Moderately high correlation coefficients were found between the Mental Rotations Test and the Cube Comparison

Test, the Visualization of Views Test, and the Visualization of Rotations Test.

Significant male-favored gender differences were found in scores on the Mental

Rotations Test, and there were significant correlations with multiple spatial activities from the Spatial Experiences Questionnaire. Therefore, there is fairly strong evidence that this test can be interpreted as a valid measure of spatial ability in Allied Medical students. However, the magnitude of the correlation coefficients between this test and the Visualization of Rotations Test does not support the assertion that both tests are valid measures of mental rotation ability.

Limitations

Analysis of job-related documents was conducted in order to answer the first research question regarding the nature of spatial skills used by each of the Allied Medical professions in this study. Specificity of the documents was not consistent across the

219 professions. Interviews with professionals in each occupation, and observational studies would further clarify the nature of spatial skills in each of the Allied Medical professions.

A major goal of this study was to determine if spatial tests would show differences in ability between various Allied Medical professional groups. The sample consisted of a total of 128 students in five Allied Medical programs: Athletic Training,

Health Information Management and Systems, Medical Dietetics, Occupational Therapy, and Radiologic Sciences. The sample was drawn from the School of Health and

Rehabilitation Sciences at The Ohio State University, which also has programs in Health

Sciences, Medical Laboratory Sciences, Physical Therapy, and Respiratory Therapy. The sample groups were chosen because the skills required for each academic program were considered to be sufficiently different from the others. Although demographically this sample was similar the school as a whole, a larger and more diverse sample would add credence to the validity evidence for the chosen tests.

The Occupational Therapy students were unique in that they were graduate students. These students could potentially have completed another Allied Medical program as an undergraduate, or could have completed a non-medical undergraduate program prior to commencing their education in Occupational Therapy. Their undergraduate work could have influenced, or could have been influenced by their spatial skills. Further examination of the undergraduate education for this group of students might cast more light on their performance on the various spatial tests.

Testing took place for each group during a normal 90 minute class period, and instructions were read from the same script by the author for each group. Timing of the

220 tests was conducted using the same watch for each group. The author observed that most students seemed to attempt to answer as many questions as possible; however students had no extrinsic motivation to complete the tests or to do their best.

All tests were administered in the same order for the five Allied Medical student groups: Cube Comparison version A, Visualization of Views, Visualization of

Developments version A, Hidden Figures, Visualization of Rotations, Mental Rotations

Test version A, Cube Comparison version B, Visualization of Developments version B, and Mental Rotations Test version B. There was a one to two minute break between each test, and students completed the Spatial Experience Questionnaire before the last three tests were administered. Higher scores on the second versions of the Cube Test and the

Developments Test could be attributed to practice effects. Lower scores on the second version of the Mental Rotations Test could be attributed to fatigue.

Allied Medical professions have historically been female-dominated occupations.

This is reflected in the low number of males in some of the educational programs included in this study. Within-group gender differences could not be analyzed due to the small number of males in some sample groups. A study utilizing larger samples of students might reveal gender differences in spatial ability within Allied Medical groups.

Gender differences for the Visualization of Rotations Test and Mental Rotations

Test must be examined cautiously in this study. Multivariate analysis did find significant or near significant results for these tests, but observed power was relatively low.

Univariate tests were then conducted, but results must be evaluated in light of Type 1 error.

221

Conclusions and Recommendations for Future Research

This study provides some validity evidence for six tests of spatial ability which were used to examine differences in this skill in five groups of Allied Medical students.

Because the professional skills required for each of the Allied Medical program groups in this study are different, spatial tests should have the potential to differentiate ability and skill level. Each of the tests has been used to examine spatial skills in other groups, and for various ages and cultures. No studies were found that examine spatial skills in Allied

Medical professionals.

The nature of spatial ability examined in this study was based on the categories of this trait proposed by Linn and Petersen (1985), and Voyer, Voyer, and Bryden (1995).

The three categories are spatial perception, spatial visualization, and mental rotation.

Two tests were chosen to measure each category. The Cube Comparison Test and the

Visualization of Views subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization Test were chosen to examine spatial perception. The Visualization of Developments subtest of the Purdue

Spatial Visualization Test and the Hidden Figures Test were used to evaluate spatial visualization. The Visualization of Rotations subtest of the Purdue Spatial Visualization

Test and the Mental Rotations Test were used to evaluate mental rotation ability.

The tests used in this study were each able to differentiate sample participants into high, average, and low spatial ability (Table 37). Fourteen to 22% of students could be classified as high spatial ability depending on the test. Thirteen to 24% of students could be classified as low spatial ability in this group. None of the tests were able to

222 differentiate students in the different Allied Medical programs. The Cube Comparison

Test, Hidden Figures Test, Visualization of Rotations Test, and the Mental Rotations Test had acceptable reliability, although the correlations between the two versions of the Cube

Comparison Test and the two versions of the Mental Rotations Test were lower than expected. Correlations between the Mental Rotations Test and the other spatial tests were small to moderate. This offers some evidence that the Cube Comparison Test, the

Visualization of Views Test, the Visualization of Developments Test, and the

Visualization of Rotations Test are measuring a trait similar to the Mental Rotations Test; however the evidence is not particularly strong. Little evidence was found that these tests could be categorized as measuring spatial perception, spatial visualization, and mental rotation ability. Correlations between the tests used to measure each of these categories of spatial ability were only moderately correlated. Gender differences were found between some, but not all of the tests. The Hidden Figures Test found women excelling rather than men. Male-favored gender differences were found on the Visualization of

Views Test, the Visualization of Rotations Test, and the Mental Rotations Test. These differences were small, with effect sizes ranging between .03 and .13.

Scores from these tests, to some extent, may be interpreted as measures of spatial ability. However, validation evidence is not strong enough to use these tests as measures of the categories of spatial ability. Results from this validation study also show that the use of these instruments for any type of high stakes testing should be avoided in Allied

Medical students. Further examination of these tests would be needed prior to any use of test scores for admission decisions or requirements for additional prerequisite classes.

223

Tests should be examined in comparison with specific occupational spatial tasks. Other spatial tests should also be examined with this sample of students to determine if better methods of assessing spatial ability are available. There are a great many spatial tests available, and tests other than those used here may be more discriminating in evaluating spatial abilities. A larger and more diverse sampling of these professional groups may be helpful in examining the spatial skills needed in their individual practices, and in helping educators develop a curriculum which evaluates and teaches these skills.

Test Assumption 1 Assumption 2 Assumption 3 Assumption 4 Assumption 5 Differentiate Reliability Correlate Correlate Gender level of with MRT with Other Differences spatial ability Spatial Category Test Cube M = 7.27 (SD .68 (K-R 20) .29 (MRTa) .27 No Comparison, = 3.89) .30 (MRTb) (Visualization significant Version A 24 (18.7%) .33 (MRT of Views) gender high ability combined) differences 22 (17.2%) low ability Cube M = 8.64 (SD .79 (K-R 20) .41(MRTa) .25 No Comparison, = 4.78) .39 (MRTb) (Visualization significant Version B 24 (18.7%) .47 (MRT of Views) gender high ability combined) differences 21 (16.4%) low ability continued

Table 37. Summary of Validity Evidence for Spatial Tests

*Non-significant correlation

224

Table 37 Continued

Cube M = 15.86 .84 (Parallel .41 (MRTa) .30 No Comparison, (SD = 7.75) forms) .40 (MRTb) (Visualization significant Combined 26 (20.4%) correlation .47 (MRT of Views) gender high ability between combined) differences 17 (13.3%) forms r = .64 low ability Visualization M = 5.72 (SD .38 (K-R 20) .43 (MRTa) .27 (CCa) Male-favored of Views = 2.78) .41 (MRTb) .25 (CCb) gender 21 (16.5%) .42 (MRT .30 (CC difference high ability combined) combined) 26 (20.3%) low ability Hidden M = 6.57 (SD .78 (K-R 20) .12 (MRTa)* .23 (VoDa) Female- Figures = 3.58) .07 (MRTb)* .15 (VoDb)* favored 25 (19.5%) .13 (MRT .22 (VoD gender high ability combined)* combined) difference 27 (21.1%) low ability Visualization M = 4.24 (SD .62 (K-R 20) .27 (MRTa) .23 (Hidden No of = 2.08) .22 (MRTb) Figures) significant Developments, 18 14.2%) .28(MRT gender Version A high ability combined) differences 27 (21.1%) low ability Visualization 4.91 (SD = .74 (K-R 20) .33 (MRTa) .15 (Hidden No of 2.67) .46 (MRTb) Figures)* significant Developments, 19 (14.8%) .43 (MRT gender Version B high ability combined) differences 25 (19.5%) low ability Visualization M = 9.67 (SD .62 (parallel .34 (MRTa) .22 (Hidden No of = 4.14) forms) .41 (MRTb) Figures) significant Developments, 18 (14.1%) Correlation .41 (MRT gender Combined high ability between combined) differences 26 (20.3%) forms r = .53 low ability continued

225

Table 37 Continued

Visualization M = 4.28 (SD .70 (K-R 20) .43 (MRTa) .43 (MRTa) Male-favored of Rotations = 2.32) .32 (MRTb) .32 (MRTb) gender 24 (18.8%) .42 (MRT .42 (MRT difference high ability combined) combined) 30 (23.4%) low ability Mental M = 4.27 (SD .76 (K-R 20) -- .43 Male-favored Rotations, = 2.37) (Visualization gender Version A 28 (21.9%) of Rotations) difference high ability 31 (24.2%) low ability Mental M = 3.22 (SD .64 (K-R 20) -- .32 Male-favored Rotations, = 2.02) (Visualization gender Version B 20 (15.7%) of Rotations) difference high ability 27 (21.1%) low ability Mental M = 7.49 (SD .82 (Parallel -- .42 Male-favored Rotations, = 3.94) forms (Visualization gender Combined 23 (18%) Correlation of Rotations) difference high ability between 20 (15.6%) forms r = .62 low ability

Life experiences and activities are frequently found to relate to one’s level of spatial ability. Subjects completed a survey examining participation in and enjoyment of

25 different spatial activities. These activities have been shown to correlate with levels of spatial ability in previous research. High spatial ability subjects reported high rates of participation in each of the following activities: sketching house plans, using hand tools, skiing, jigsaw puzzles, building models, playing pool, weaving, using a map or compass, and using machine tools (McDaniel, Guay, Ball, & Kolloff, 1978).

In this study, sketching house plans was significantly correlated with scores on the Cube Comparison Test, and all three subtests of the PSVT. Building models and

226 playing pool were positively correlated with at least two of the PSVT subtests and with the Mental Rotations Test. Other activities that correlated positively with at least one of the spatial tests included playing chess, using hand tools, making or repairing furniture, arranging furniture, solving math riddles, and playing golf. Taking pictures and playing tennis were negatively correlated with at least one of the spatial tests.

Further studies are needed to examine spatial ability in Allied Medical

Professionals. Examining the job-related tasks that professionals perform, using an observational qualitative approach would further help to clarify the nature of spatial tasks required in each occupation. A study comparing skills in specific spatial tasks to the spatial tests used in this study could supplement the validity evidence found here. Also, in order to further investigate the spatial skills in Allied Medicine, this study could be repeated using practicing professionals rather than Allied Medical students.

227

References

Afifi, A., Clark, V., & May, S. (2004). Computer-Aided Multivariate Analysis (4th ed.). Boca Raton, Florida: Chapman & Hall/CRC.

Allen, M., & Hogeland, R. (1978). Spatial problem-solving strategies as functions of sex. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 348-350.

American Educational Research Association, A. P. (1999). Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, D.C.: American Educational Research Association.

American Physical Therapy Association: Information for Consumers. (2008). Retrieved July 12, 2008, from APTA: The American Physical Therapy Association: http://www.apta.org/AM/Template.cfm?Section=Physical_Therapy&Template=/ TaggedPage/TaggedPageDisplay.cfm&TPLID=217&ContentID=34361

Anastakis, D., Hamstra, S., & Matsumoto, E. (2000). Visual-spatial abilities in surgical training. The American Journal of Surgery, 179: 469-471.

Anastasi, A., & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological Testing (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

Athletic Trainers. (2010). Retrieved April 1, 2013, from O*NET Online: http://www.onetonline.org/link/summary/29-9091.00

Athletic Trainers. (2012, March 29). Retrieved April 1, 2013, from Bureau of Labor Statistics: http://www.bls.gov/ooh/healthcare/athletic-trainers.htm

Athletic Training. (2012). Retrieved April 1, 2012, from School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences: http://www.hrs.osu.edu/at/1343.cfm

Babbie, E. (1995). The Practice of Social Research (7th ed.). Belmont, California: Wadsworth Publishing Company.

Baenninger, M., & Newcombe, N. (1989). The role of experience in spatial test performance: A meta-analysis. Sex Roles, 20 (5/6): 327-344.

Baker, S., & Talley, L. (1972). The relationship of visualization skills to achievement in freshman chemistry. Journal of Chemical Education, 49 (11): 775-776.

Barratt, E. (1953). An analysis of verbal reports of solving spatial problems as an aid in defining spatial factors. Journal of Psychology, 36: 17-25. 228

Battista, M. (1990). Spatial visualization and gender differences in high school geometry. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21 (1): 47-60.

Ben-Chaim, D., Lappan, G., & Houang, R. (1988). The effect of instruction on spatial visualization skills of middle school boys and girls. American Educational Research Journal, 25 (1), 51-71.

Berbaum, K., Smoker, W., & Smith, W. (1985). Measurement and prediction of diagnostic performance during radiology training. American Journal of Roentgenology, 145: 1305-1311.

Berg, C., Hertzog, C., & Hunt, E. (1982). Age differences in the speed of mental rotation. Developmental Psychology, 18 (1), 95-107.

Bishop, A. (1980). Spatial abilities and mathematics education--a review. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 11, 257-269.

Blajenkova, O., Kozhevnikov, M., & Motes, M. (2007). Object and spatial imagery: Distinctions between members of different professions. Cognitive Process, 7: 20- 21.

Blajenkova, O., Kozhevnikov, M., & Motes, M. (2006). Object-spatial imagery: A new self-report imagery questionnaire. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20: 239-263.

Bock, R., & Kolakowski, D. (1973). Further evidence of sex-linked major-gene influence on human spatial visualizing ability. American Journal of Human Genetics, 25, 1- 14.

Bodner, G., & Guay, R. (1997). The Purdue visualization of rotations test. Retrieved July 20, 2008, from The Chemical Educator: http://chemeducator.org/bibs/0002004/00020138.htm

Bryant, F. (2000). Assessing the Validity of Measurement. In L. Grimm, and P. Yarnold, Reading and Understanding More Multivariate Statistics. Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association.

Caplan, P., MacPherson, G., & Tobin, P. (1985). Do sex-related differences in spatial abilites exist? American Psychologist, 40 (7), 786-799. Carmines, E., & Zeller, R. (1979). Reliability and Validity Assessment. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

229

Carter, C., LaRussa, M., & Bodner, G. (1987). A study of two measures of spatial ability as predictors of success in different levels of general chemistry. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 24: 645-657.

Castelli, L., Corazzini, L., & Geminiani, G. (2008). Spatial navigation in large-scale virtual environments: Gender differences in survey tasks. Computers in Human Behavior, 1643-1667.

Cherney, I. (2008). Mom, let me play more computer games: They improve my mental rotation skills. Sex Roles, 59, 776-786.

Clem, D., Anderson, S., Donaldson, J., & Hdeib, M. (2010). An exploratory study of spatial ability and student achievement in sonography. Journal of Diagnostic Medical Sonography, 26 (4), 163-170.

Cochran, K., & Wheatley, G. (1989). Ability and sex-related differences in cognitive strategies on spatial tasks. Journal of General Psychology, 116 (1), 43-55.

Cohen, C., & Hegary, M. (2007). Indivudual differences in use of external visualisations to perform an internal visualisation task. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21: 701- 711.

Colom, R., Contreras, M., Arend, I., Leal, O., & Santacreu, J. (2004). Sex differences in verbal reasoning are mediated by sex differences in spatial ability. The Psychological Record, 54, 365-372.

Conner, J., Schackman, M., & Serbin, L. (1978). Sex-related differences in response to practice on a visual-spatial test and generalization to a related test. Child Development, 49, 24-29.

Connor, J., Serbin, L., & Schackman, M. (1977). Sex differences in children's response to training on a visual-spatial test. Developmental Psychology, 13 (3), 293-294.

Davis, D., & Eliot, J. (1994). Comparison of related perceptual tests. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79: 399-402.

D'Costa, A. (2005). Exploring the nature of spatial ability: A theory-based test- development approach.

De Lisi, R., & Cammarano, D. (1996). Computer experience and gender differences in undergraduate mental rotation performance. Computers in Human Behavior, 12 (3), 351-361.

230

Delgado, A., & Prieto, G. (1996). Sex differences in visuospatial ability: Do performance factors play such an important role? Memory & Cognition, 24(4), 504-510.

Description of Occupational Therapy. (2012). Retrieved August 2, 2012, from The School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences: http://hrs.osu.edu/ot/1308.cfm

Dev, P., Friedman, C., Dafoe, B., & Felciano, R. (1992). Testing spatial understanding of anatomy. Symposium on Computer Applications in Medical Care, 16: 804-805.

Dick, W., & Hagerty, N. (1971). Topics in Measurement: Reliability and Validity. New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc.

Dietitians and Nutritionists. (2010). Retrieved April 1, 2013, from O*NET Online: http://www.onetonline.org/link/summary/29-1031.00

Dietitians and Nutritionists. (2012, July 18). Retrieved April 1, 2013, from Bureau of Labor Statistics: http://www.bls.gov/ooh/healthcare/dietitians-and- nutritionists.htm

Dunn, A., & Eliot, J. (1993). Extraversion-introversion and spatial intelligence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77: 19-24.

Ecuyer-Dab, I., Tremblay, T., Joanette, Y., & Passini, R. (2004). Real-life spatial skills, handedness, and family history of handedness. Brain and Cognition, 57: 219-221.

Edens, K., & Potter, E. (2007). The relationship of drawing and mathematical problem solving: Draw for Math tasks. Studies in Art Education, 48(3): 282-98.

Eisenberg, T., & McGinty, R. (1977). On spatial visualization in college students. Journal of Psychology, 95 (1): 99-104.

Ekstrom, R., French, J. W., & Harman, H. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.

El Koussy, A. (1935). An investigation into the factors in tests involving the visual perception of space. The British Journal of Psychology Monograph Supplements 20, 1-89.

Eliot, J. (2002). About spatial intelligence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94: 479-486. Eliot, J. (1980). Classification of figural spatial tests. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 51: 847-851.

Eliot, J. (2000). The Nature and Measure of Spatial Intelligence. Unpublished manuscript. 231

Eliot, J., & Czarnolewski, M. (2007). Development of an everyday spatial behavioral questionnaire. The Journal of General Psychology, 134(3):361-381.

Eliot, J., & Smith, I. (1983). An International Directory of Spatial Tests. Windsor, England: The NFER-Nelson Publishing Company Ltd.

Fan, X., & Konold, T. (2010). Canonical Correlation Analysis. In G. Hancock, & R. Mueller (Eds.), The Reviewer's Guide to Quantitative Methods in the Social Sciences (pp. 29-40). New York: Routledge.

Feingold, A. (1988). Cognitive gender differences are disappearing. American Psychologist, 95-103.

Feldt, L., & Brennan, R. (1989). Reliability. In R. L. Linn, Educational Measurement (3rd ed.). New York: Macmillan Publishing Company.

Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. (1978). Sex-related differences in mathematics achievement and related factors: a further study. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 189-203.

Fennema, E., & Sherman, J. (1977). Sex-related differences in mathematics achievement, spatial visualization and affective factors. American Educational Research Journal, 14 (1), 51-71.

Friedman, C., Dev, P., Dafoe, B., Murphy, G., & Felciano, R. (1993). Initial validation of a test of spatial knowledge in anatomy. Proceedings - the Annual Symposium on Computer Applications in Medical Care. :791-5, 1993. (pp. 791-795). AMIA, Inc.

Fruchter, B. (1954). Measurement of spatial abilities. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 14, 387-395.

Garg, A., Norman, G., Eva, K., Spero, L., & Sharan, S. (2002). Is there any real virtue in virtual reality? The minor role of multiple orientations in learning anatomy from computers. Academic Medicine, 77 (7): 97-99.

Garg, A., Norman, G., & Sperotable, L. (2001). How medical students learn spatial anatomy. The Lancet, 357: 363-364. Geiser, C., Lehmann, W., & Eid, M. (2008). A note on sex differences in mental rotation in different age groups. Intelligence, 36: 556-563.

Gilligan, J., Welsh, F., Watts, C., & Treasure, T. (1999). Square pegs in round holes: has psychometric testing a place in choosing a surgical career? A preliminary report

232

of work in progress. Annals of the Royal College of Surgeons of England, 81, 73- 79.

Goldstein, D., Haldane, D., & Mitchell, C. (1990). Sex differences in visual-spatial ability: the role of performance factors. Memory & Cognition, 18 (5), 546-550.

Green, C., & Bavelier, D. (2006). Effect of action video games on the spatial distribution of visuospatial attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32 (6), 1465-1478.

Grimm, L., & Yarnold, P. (1995). Principal-Components Analysis and Exploratory and Confirmatory Factor Analysis. In L. Grimm, & P. Yarnold (Eds.), Reading and Understanding Multivariate Statistics (pp. 99-136). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Guay, R. (1978). Factors affecting spatial test performance: sex, handedness, birth order, and experience. 1978 Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association (pp. 1-19). Toronto: American Educational Research Association.

Guay, R. (1976). Purdue Spatial Visualization Test. Purdue Research Foundation.

Guay, R. (1980). Spatial ability measurement: A critique and an alternative. 1980 Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association. Boston.

Guay, R., & McDaniel, E. (1977). The relationship between mathematics achievement and spatial abilities among elementary school children. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 211-215.

Guilford, J. (1967). The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc.

Guilford, J., & Lacey, J. (1947). Printed Classification Tests. Washington, DC: AAF Aviation Psychology Research Report 5.

Guilford, J., Fruchter, B., & Zimmerman, W. (1952). Factor analysis of the Army Air Forces Sheppard Field Battery of experimental aptitude tests. Psychometrika, 17 (1), 45-68.

Guillot, A., Champely, S., Batier, C., Thiriet, P., & Collet, C. (2007). Relationship between spatial abilities, mental rotation and functional anatomy learning. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 12:491-507. Guillot, A., Louis, M., Thiriet, P., & Collet, C. (2007). The Effects of expertise level and motor skill characteristics on mental rotation. In L. Boyar, New Psychological Tests and Testing Research (pp. 231-241). New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

233

Guttman, R. (1974). Genetic analysis of analytical spatial ability. Behavioral Genetics, 4 (3), 273-284.

Halpern, D. (1989). The disappearance of cognitive gender differences: What you see depends on where you look. American Psychologist, 1156-1158.

Hassler, M., Birbaumer, N., & Feil, A. (1985). Musical talent and visual-spatial abilities: a longitudinal study. Psychology of Music, 13 (2), 99-113.

Health Information Management and Systems. (2012). Retrieved March 30, 2012, from The School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences: http://www.hrs.osu.edu/hims/

Hedges, L., & Nowell, A. (1995). Sex differences in mental test scores, variability, and numbers of high-scoring individuals. Science, 269, 41-45.

Hegarty, M., & Kozhevnikov, M. (1999). Types of visual-spatial representations and mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(4): 684- 689.

Hegarty, M., Keehner, M., Cohen, C., Montello, D., & Lippa, Y. (2007). The role of spatial cognition in medicine: Applications for selecting and training professionals. In G. Allen (Ed.), Applied Spatial Cognition: From Research to Cognitive Technology (pp. 286-311). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Hegarty, M., Keehner, M., Khooshabeh, P., & Montello, D. (2009). How spatial abilities enhance, and are enhanced by, dental education. Learning and Individual Differences, 19: 61-71.

Hegarty, M., Montello, D., Richardson, A., Ishikawa, T., & Lovelace, K. (2006). Spatial abilities at different scales: Individual differences in aptitude-test performance and spatial-layout learning. Intelligence, 34: 151-176.

Heil, M., & Jansen-Osmann, P. (2008). Sex differences in mental rotation with polygons of different complexity: Do men utilize holistic processes whereas women prefer piecemeal ones? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61: 683- 689. Huck, S. (2004). Reading statistics and research (4th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education Inc.

Hyde, J. (1981). How large are cognitive gender differences? A meta-analysis using ω2 and d. American Psychologist, 36 (8), 892-901.

234

Hyde, J., Geiringer, E., & Yen, W. (1975). On the empirical relation between spatial ability and sex differences in other aspects of cognitive performance. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 289-309.

Jansen-Osmann, P., & Heil, M. (2007). Suitable stimuli to obtain (no) gender differences in the speed of cognitive processes involved in mental rotation. Brain and Cognition, 64: 217-227.

Jardine, R., & Martin, N. (1984). No evidence for sex-linked or sex-limited gene expression influencing spatial orientation. Bahavior Genetics, 14 (4), 345-354.

Jensen, A. (1998). The g Factor: the science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.

Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. (2007). Sex differences in mental abilities: g masks the dimensions on which they lie. Intelligence, 35: 23-39.

Just, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1985). Cognitive coordinate systems: Accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability. Psychological Review, 92 (2), 137-172.

Kane, M. (2011). Validating score interpretations and uses: Messick lecture, language testing research colloquium, Cambridge, April 2010. Language Testing, 29 (1), 2- 17.

Kane, M. (2006). Validation. In R. Brennan (Ed.), Educational Measurement (4th ed., pp. 17-64). Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers.

Kaufman, S. (2007). Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: Can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity? Intellignece, 35: 211-223.

Keehner, M., Cohen, C., Hegarty, M., & Montello, D. (2004). Cognitive factors and interactivity: Implications for the design and implementation of 3-D computer visualizations for medical education. 12th Annual Medicine Meets Virtual Reality (MMVR12) Conference, (pp. 14-17). Newport Beach, CA. Keehner, M., Guerin, S., Miller, M., Turk, D., & Hegarty, M. (2006). Modulation of neural activity by angle of rotation during imagined spatial transformations. NeuroImage, 33: 391-398.

Keehner, M., Tendick, F., Meng, M., Anwar, H., Stoller, M., & Duh, Q. (2004). Spatial ability, experience, and skill in laperoscopic surgery. American Journal of Surgery, 188: 71-75.

235

Keith, T., Reynolds, M., Patel, P., & Ridley, K. (2008). Sex differences in latent cognitive abilities ages 6 to 59: evidence from the Woodcock-Johnson III tests of cognitive abilites. Intelligence, 36, 502-525.

Kelley, T. (1928). Crossroads in the Mind of Man: A Study of Differentiable Mental Abilities. Palo Alto, California: Stanford University Press.

Keppel, G., & Wickens, T. (2004). Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Pearson Prentice Hall.

Kesner, M., & Linzey, A. (2005). Can computer-based visual-spatial aids lead to increased student performance in anatomy and physiology? The American Biology Teacher, 67(4): 206-212.

Knapp, T., & Mueller, R. (2010). Relaibiltiy and Validity of Instruments. In G. Hancock, & R. Mueller, The Reviewer's Guide to Quantitative Methods in the Social Sciences (pp. 337-355). New York: Routledge.

Kovac, R. (1989). The validation of selected spatial ability tests via correlational assessment and analysis of user-processing strategy. Educational Research Quarterly, 13(2): 26-34.

Kurpius, S., & Stafford, M. (2006). Testing and Measurement A User-Friendly Guide. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Kyllonen, P., Lohman, D., & Snow, R. (1984). Effects of aptitudes, strategy training, and task facets on spatial task performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76 (1): 130-145.

Kyllonen, P., Lohman, D., & Woltz, D. (1984). Componential modeling of alternative strategies for performing spatial tasks. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76 (6), 1325-1345.

Levinson, A., Weaver, B., Garside, S., McGinn, H., & Norman, G. (2007). Virtual reality and brain anatomy: a randomized trial of e-learning instructional designs. Medical Education, 41: 495-501. Levy, J. (1976). Cerebral lateralization and spatial ability. Behavior Genetics, 6 (2), 171- 188.

Li, C., Zhu, W., & Nuttall, R. (2003). Familial handedness and spatial ability: a study with Chinese students aged 14-24. Brain and Cognition, 51, 375-384.

Linn, M., & Petersen, A. (1985). Emergence and characterization of sex differences in spatial ability: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 56: 1479-1498. 236

Lohman, D. (1989). Human intelligence: an introduction to advances in theory and research. Review of Educational Research, 59 (4), 333-373.

Lohman, D. (1988). Spatial abilities as traits, processes, and knowledge. In R. Sternberg, Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence (Vol. 4, pp. 181-248). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Lohman, D. (1996). Spatial ability and g. In I. Dennis, & P. Tapsfield (Eds.), Human Abilities: Their Nature and Measurement (pp. 97-116). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Lord, T. (1990). Enhancing learning in the Life Sciences through spatial perception. Innovative Higher Education, 15 (1), 5-16.

Lunneborg, C., & Lunneborg, P. (1984). Contribution of sex-differentiated experiences to spatial and mechanical reasoning abilities. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 59, 107- 113.

Luursema, J., Verwey, W., Kommers, P., & Annema, J. (2008). The role of stereopsis in virtual anatomical learning. Interacting with Computers, 20: 455-460.

Maccoby, E., & Jacklin, C. (1974). The psychology of sex differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Manger, T., & Eikeland, O. (1998). The effects of spatial visualization and students' sex on mathematical achievement. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 17-25.

Martin-Dorta, N., Saorin, J., & Contero, M. (2008). Development of a fast remedial course to improve the spatial abilites of engineering students. Journal of Engineering Education, 97 (4), 505-513.

Massa, L., Mayer, R., & Bohon, L. (2005). Individual differences in gender role beliefs influence spatial ability test performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 99-111. Masters, M. (1998). The gender difference on the Mental Rotations test is not due to performance factors. Memory & Cognition, 26 (3), 444-448.

Mathewson, J. (1999). Visual-spatial thinking: an aspect of science overlooked by educators. Science Education, 33-54.

McDaniel, E., & Guay, R. (1976). Spatial abilities, mathematics achievement, and the sexes. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association. San Francisco. 237

McDaniel, E., Guay, R., Ball, L., & Kolloff, M. (1978). A spatial experience questionnaire and some preliminary findings. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association. Toronto.

McFarlane, M. (1925). A study of practical ability. The British Journal of Psychology, Monograph Supplements 8, 1-75.

McGee, M. (1979). Human spatial abilities: Psychometric studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal, and neurological influences. Psychological Bulletin, 86 (5): 889-918.

McGee, M. (1976). Laterality, hand preference, and human spatial ability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 42, 781-782.

Medical Dietetics. (2012). Retrieved April 1, 2012, from School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences: http://hrs.osu.edu/md/index.cfm

Medical Records and Health Information Technicians. (2010). Retrieved April 1, 2013, from O*NET Online: http://www.onetonline.org/link/summary/29-2071.00

Messick, S. (1989). Validity. In R. Linn, Educational Measurement (3rd ed.). New York: Macmillan Publishing Company.

Michaelides, M. (2003, April). Age and gender differences in performance on a spatial rotation test. Poster session presented at the AERA Annual Meeting . Chicago, IL.

Miller, R. (2000). Approaches to learning spatial relationships in gross anatomy: Perspective from wider principles of learning. Clinical Anatomy, 13: 439-443.

Moe, A., & Pazzaglia, F. (2006). Following the instructions! Effects of gender beliefs in mental rotation. Learning and Individual Differences, 16, 369-377.

Moe, A., Meneghetti, C., & Cadinu, M. (2009). Women and mental rotation: Incremental theory and spatial strategy use enhance performance. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 187-191.

Monahan, J., Harke, M., & Shelley, J. (2008). Computerizing the mental rotations test: Are gender differences maintained? Behavior Research Methods, 40 (2): 422-427.

Moore, D., & Johnson, S. (2008). Mental rotation in human infants: A sex difference. Psychological Science, 19(11): 1063-1066.

238

Murphy, K. (2010). Power Analysis. In G. Hancock, & R. Mueller (Eds.), The Reviewer's Guide to Quantitative Methods in the Social Sciences (pp. 329-336). New York: Routledge.

Newcombe, N., & Bandura, M. (1983). Effect of age at puberty on spatial ability in girls: A question of mechanism. Developmental Psychology, 19 (2), 215-224.

Newcombe, N., Bandura, M., & Taylor, D. (1983). Sex differences in spatial ability and spatial activities. Sex roles, 9 (3), 377-386.

Newcombe, N., Dubas, J., & Baenninger, M. (1989). Associations of timing and puberty, spatial ability, and lateralization in adult women. Child Development, 60, 246- 254.

Nilsson, T., Hedman, L., & Ahlqvist, A. (2007). Visual-spatial ability and interpretation of three-dimensional information in radiographs. Dentomaxillofacial Radiology, 36:86-91.

Nunnally, J., & Bernstein, I. (1994). Psychometric Theory (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill, Inc.

Occupational Therapists. (2010). Retrieved April 1, 2013, from O*NET Online: http://www.onetonline.org/link/summary/29-1122.00

Occupational Therapists. (2012, June 26). Retrieved April 1, 2013, from Bureau of Labor Statistics: http://www.bls.gov/ooh/healthcare/occupational-therapists.htm

Occupational Therapy. (2012). Retrieved April 1, 2012, from The School of Health and Rehabilitative Sciences: http://hrs.osu.edu/ot/1308.cfm

Olson, D. M., & Eliot, J. (1986). Relationships between experiences, processing style, and sex-related differences in performance on spatial tests. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62: 447-460. Olson, D., Eliot, J., & Hardy, R. (1988). Relationships between activities and sex-related differences in performance on spatial tests. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 67, 223- 232.

Parsons, T., Larson, P., Kratz, K., Thiebaux, M., Bluestein, B., Buckwalter, J., et al. (2004). Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial rotation in a virtual environment. Neuropsychologia, 42, 555-562.

Peters, M. (2005). Sex differences and the factor of time in solving Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotation problems. Brain and Cognition, 57: 176-184.

239

Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse Mental Rotations Test: different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 39-58.

Peters, M., Lehmann, W., Takahira, S., Takeuchi, Y., & Jordan, K. (2006). Mental rotation test performance in four cross-cultural samples (n=3367): Overall sex differences and the role of academic program in performance. Cortex , 42, 1005- 1015.

Polgar, S., & Thomas, S. (2000). Introduction to Research in the Health Sciences (4th ed.). London: Churchill Livingstone.

QRS International. (2012, August 31). NVivo . Version 10 . Retrieved from http://www.qsrinternational.com/support.aspx

Quaiser-Pohl, C., & Lehmann, W. (2002). Girls' spatial abilities: Charting the contributions of experiences and attitudes in different academic groups. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 245-260.

Quinn, P., & Liben, S. (2008). A sex difference in mental rotation in young infants. Psychological Science, 19(11): 1067-1070.

Radiologic Sciences and Therapy. (2012). Retrieved April 1, 2012, from The School of Health and Rehabilitative Sciences: http://hrs.osu.edu/rd/

Radiologic Technologists. (2010). Retrieved April 1, 2013, from O*NET Online: http://www.onetonline.org/link/summary/29-2034.00

Radiologic Technologists. (2012, March 29). Retrieved April 1, 2013, from Bureau of Labor Statistics: http://www.bls.gov/ooh/healthcare/radiologic-technologists.htm

Reio, T., Czarnolewski, M., & Eliot, J. (2004). Handedness and spatial ability: Differential patterns of relationships. Laterality, 9 (3), 339-358. Rilea, S. (2008). A lateralization of function approach to sex differences in spatial ability: A reexamination. Brain and Cognition, 67, 168-182.

Rilea, S., Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., & Boles, D. (2004). Sex differences in spatial ability: A lateralization of function approach. Brain and Cognition, 56, 332-343.

Risucci, D. (2002). Visual spatial perception and surgical competence. The American Journal of Surgery, 184: 291-295.

Rochford, K. (1985). Spatial learning disabilities and underachievement among university anatomy students. Medical Education, 19: 13-26. 240

Rosse, C. (1995). The potential of computerized representations of anatomy in the training of health care providers. Academic Medicine, 70: 499-506.

Salkind, N. (2004). Statistics for People Who (Think They) Hate Statistics (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications, Inc.

Sanders, B., & Soares, M. (1986). Sexual maturation and spatial ability in college students. Developmental Psychology, 22 (2), 199-203.

Shepard, R., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three dimensional objects. Science, 171: 701-703.

Sidhu, R., Tompa, D., Jang, R., Grober, E., Johnston, K., Reznick, R., et al. (2004). Interpretation of three-dimensional structure from two-dimensional endovascular images: Implications for educators in vascular surgery. Journal of Vascular Surgery, 39(6): 1305-1311.

Signorella, M., Krupa, M., & Jamison, W. (1986). A short version of a spatial activity questionnaire. Sex Roles, 14 (9/10), 475-479.

Smalley, S., Thompson, A., Spence, M., Judd, W., & Sparkes, R. (1989). Genetic influences on spatial ability: Transmission in an extended kindred. Behavior Genetics, 19 (2), 229-240.

Smith, I. (1964). Spatial Ability: Its Educational and Social Significance. San Diego: Robert R. Knapp.

Smoker, W., Berbaum, K., Luebke, N., & Jacoby, C. (1984). Spatial perception testing in diagnostic radiology. American Journal of Roentgenology, 143: 1105-1109.

Sorby, S., & Baartmans, B. (2000). The development and assessment of a course for enhancing the 3-D spatial visualization skills of first year engineering students. Journal of Engineering Education, 301-307.

Spearman, C. (1904). "General Intelligence", objectively determined and measured. The American Journal of Psychology , 15: 201-292.

Spearman, C. (1938). Proposed explanation of individual differences of ability by "sampling". British Journal of Psychology. General Section. , 29(2): 182-191.

Spearman, C. (1946). Theory of general factor. British Psychological Society, 36(3): 117- 131.

241

Stumpf, H. (1993). Performance factors and gender-related differences in spatial ability: Another assessment. Memory & Cognition, 21 (6), 828-836.

Stumpf, H. (1989). Sex-related differences in spatial ability: More evidence for convergence. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 69, 915-921.

Tavakol, M., & Dennick, R. (2011). Making sense of Cronbach's alpha. International Journal of Medical Education, 2, 253-255.

The Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics. (2012). Becoming an RD or DTR. Retrieved August 2, 2012, from EatRight.org: http://www.eatright.org/BecomeanRDorDTR/content.aspx?id=8142

Thurstone, L. (1947). Multiple Factor Analysis. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

Thurstone, L. (1924). The nature of general intelligence and ability (III). British Journal of Psychology. General Section, 14(3): 243-247.

Thurstone, L. (1938). The perceptual factor. Psychometrika , 3(1):1-17.

Vandenberg, S., & Kuse, A. (1978). Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualiztion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47: 599-604.

Vandenberg, S., Kuse, A., & Vogler, G. (1985). Searching for correlates of spatial ability. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 60, 343-350.

Voyer, D., Butler, T., Cordero, J., Brake, B., Silbersweig, D., Stern, E., et al. (2006). The relation between computerized and paper-and-pencil mental rotation tasks: A validation study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28, 928- 939. Voyer, D., Nolon, C., & Voyer, S. (2000). The relation between experience and spatial performance in men and women. Sex Roles, 43 (11/12), 891-915.

Voyer, D., Rodgers, M., & McCormick, P. (2004). Timing conditions and the magnitude of gender differences on the Mental Rotations Test. Memory & Cognition, 32 (1), 72-82.

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., & Bryden, M. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin, 117 (2), 250-270.

242

Waggett, J., & Lane, D. (1990). Sex differences in the personality and cognitive correlates of spatial ability. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(6), 1037-1039.

Wang, L. (1993). The Differential Aptitude Test: A review and critique. Southwest Educational Research Association. Austin, Tx: ERIC.

Wantzel, K., Hamsta, S., Anastakis, D., Matsumoto, E., & Cusimano, M. (2002). Effect of visuo-spatial ability on learning of spatially complex surgical skills. The Lancet, 359: 230-231.

Wattanawaha, N., & Clements, M. (1982). Qualitative aspects of sex-related differences in performances on pencil-and-paper spatial questions, grades 7-9. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74 (6), 878-887.

Witkin, H., Moore, C., Goodenough, D., & Cox, P. (1977). Field-dependent and field- independent cognitive styles and their educational implications. Review of Educational Research, 47 (1), 1-64.

Yen, W. (1975). Independence of hand preference and sex-linked genetic effects on spatial performance. Perceptuan and Motor Skills, 41 (1), 311-318.

Zacks, J. (2008). Neuroimaging studies of mental rotation: A meta-analysis and review. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20 (1), 1-19.

Zimowski, M., & Wothke, W. (1988). The measurement of structural visualization: An evaluation of spatial and nonspatial sources of variation in the Wiggly Block and Paper Folding test scores (Technical Report No. 1988-5). Chicago: Johnson O'Connor Research Foundation (ERIC Document Reproduction Services No. ED305384).

243

Appendix A: Spatial Tasks for Athletic Training

Task Spatial Component Athletic Training (from Board of Certification. Role Delineation Study for the Entry-Level Certified Athletic Trainer. 5th ed. Omaha, NE: Board of Certification; 2004 and O*Net Online I. Prevention Educate the appropriate patient(s) about risks associated with Pattern recognition-visually compare patient features participation and specific activities using effective with information provided on consultation communication techniques to minimize the risk of injury and Visualization-identify visually common indicators of illness risk (anatomic and physiologic) Identify risks Visualization-picture actions related to sport or other Communicate effectively activity; assessment of patient motion, actions, etc. Educate effectively Pattern recognition-compare movement, actions with Identify appropriate resources known normals Visualization-preparation of visuals to educate target population Pattern recognition-preparation of posters and pamphlets to educate target population Spatial orientation-discuss/describe ergonomic principles Embedded information-identify physical information about patient that might be disguised or hidden (e.g. deformity that blends with or is hidden by clothing) interpret pre-participation and other relevant screening Visualization-identify visually common indicators of information in accordance with accepted guidelines to minimize risk (anatomic and physiologic) the risk of injury and illness Pattern recognition-interpret visual screening Identify conditions that may limit or compromise information participation Spatial orientation-of self and patient during screening Collect and appropriately apply preparticipation and evaluation process screening information Visualization-picture anticipated actions/movements Identify appropriate resources associated with sport or physical activity Identify and apply established guidelines and regulations Instruct the appropriate patient(s) about standard protective Visualization-picture anticipated actions/movements equipment using effective communication techniques to associated with sport or physical activity minimize the risk of injury and illness Pattern recognition-prepare and instruct patient to don Educate patients on the selection of standard protective equipment appropriately protective equipment Spatial orientation-translate method of applying Communicate effectively protective equipment from self to patient Fit standard protective equipment Visualization-determine if patient has correctly Interpret rules regarding protective equipment applied protective equipment Visualization-make appropriate changes to protective equipment to fit the patient Mental rotation-may be needed to translate manufactures’ guidelines in applying and/or fitting protective equipment continued

Table 38. Spatial Tasks in Athletic Training

244

Table 38 Continued

Apply appropriate prophylactic/protective measures using Visualization-picture internal anatomic structures commercial products or custom-made devices to minimize the Visualization-picture know mechanisms of injury or risk of injury and illness illness Identify injuries, illnesses, and conditions that Mental rotation-identification of anatomic structures in warrant the application of custom-made or sectional images commercially available devices Spatial orientation-picture relationship between patient Fabricate and fit custom-made devices and protective equipment in order to select and Select and apply commercial devices properly apply Pattern recognition-to interpret instructions/guidelines for safe application of commercial products Embedded information-choose appropriate parts of commercial devices to use or modify Visualization-of relationship between patient and custom-made devices Mental rotation-to devise and fit custom-made devices Identify safety hazards associated with activities, activity areas, Visualization-inspect areas visually for hazards (e.g. and equipment by following accepted procedures and guidelines moisture in activity area, surface irregularities or in order to make appropriate recommendations and to minimize obstructions) the risk of injury and illness Embedded information-inspect area for hazards that Conduct inspections for hazards may be partially hidden or blend into background Recognize hazards Visualization-inspect equipment to identify potential Recommend and implement appropriate methods for hazards addressing hazards Visualization-determine dimensions of activity area visually Visualization-how and if corrections can be accomplished Visualization-of activity and its relationship to the activity area or potential hazards in area or equipment Pattern recognition-visualize patterns of action associated with activity or sport Spatial orientation-picture appropriate ergonomics associated with activity or activity area Maintain clinical and treatment areas by complying with safety Visualization-of patient illness or injury to determine and sanitation standards to minimize the risk of injury and illness appropriate placement of therapeutic or rehabilitation Operate or apply therapeutic modalities and equipment rehabilitation equipment Spatial orientation-of self and patient to safely apply Recognize noncompliance with safety and sanitation therapeutic or rehabilitation equipment (includes standards knowledge of ergonomics) Recognize malfunction or disrepair of therapeutic Mental rotation-to correctly apply some therapeutic or modalities, rehabilitation equipment, or furnishings in rehabilitation equipment in relation to pictured illness clinical and treatment areas or injury Comply with manufacturer’s recommendations for Pattern recognition-be able to recognize malfunctions maintenance of equipment in equipment or potential safety hazards Maintain a safe and sanitary environment in Pattern recognition-to interpret manufacturer compliance with established standards (e.g. OSHA, guidelines or instructions for equipment universal precautions, local health department, and Embedded information-locate hazards that might be institutional policy) partially hidden or blend into background continued

245

Table 38 Continued

Monitor participants and environmental conditions by following Visualization-assess patient motions and activities accepted guidelines to promote safe participation Visualization-assess body characteristics Recognize characteristics in participants that would Visualization-picture normal and abnormal anatomic predispose them to environmental and ergonomic risk structures; picture pathology, deformity, or disease Use available resources to gather/interpret associated with known illness(es) information regarding environmental data Embedded information-recognize physical Recognize environmental and ergonomic risks characteristics that might be partially hidden or Facilitate appropriate action in response to disguised environmental and ergonomic risk Pattern recognition-recognize signs/symptoms and compare with known diseases/disabilities/deformities Spatial orientation-picture ergonomic principles related to self and patient during participation in activity Visualization-assess environmental conditions as they relate to participant; determine if environmental hazards intersect with activity area Pattern recognition-to interpret graphs and charts (e.g. weight charts, body composition charts) and visually compare with patient Visualization-picture responses to hazards (e.g. changes in size/shape of activity area, placement of obstacles, patient clothing or equipment) Facilitate physical conditioning by designing and implementing Visualization-picture location and space needed to appropriate programs to minimize the risk of injury and illness develop conditioning program Address the components of a comprehensive Visualization-visual assessment of patient physical conditioning program characteristics Educate appropriate patients in the effective Pattern recognition-picture appropriate steps in an application of conditioning programs (e.g. guardian exercise or conditioning program and recognize if and administration) patient is performing correctly Assess appropriateness of participation in Pattern recognition-to determine is appropriate conditioning programs progress is being made in exercise or conditioning Instruct in the use of appropriate conditioning program at various stages/intervals equipment (e.g. bikes, weight machines, and Spatial orientation-to demonstrate and assist patient in treadmills) appropriate exercise or conditioning activity Correct or modify inappropriate, unsafe, or dangerous Spatial orientation-to demonstrate and assist patient in activities undertaken in conjunction with physical use of conditioning equipment conditioning programs Facilitate healthy lifestyle behaviors using effective education, Visualization-assess patient to identify physical communication, and interventions to reduce the risk of injury characteristics of nutritional and stress-related and illness and promote wellness disorders Recognize signs and symptoms of nutritional and Embedded information-identify characteristics of stress-related disorders nutritional disorders that might be partially hidden or Educate appropriate patients on nutritional disorders, disguised (e.g. oral disorders associated with maladaption, substance abuse, and overtraining nutritional disease) Access information concerning accepted guidelines Visualization-picture normal and abnormal anatomic for nutritional practices structures; picture pathology, deformity, or disease Communicate with appropriate professionals associated with nutritional/stress disorders regarding referral and treatment for patients with Pattern recognition-recognize signs/symptoms nutritional and stress-related disorders associated with nutritional/stress disorders Address the issue of special nutritional needs in Visualization-to prepare and communication regard to competition or activity (e.g. pre- and post- information to patient and others (e.g. for referrals) game meals and nutritional supplements) continued

246

Table 38 Continued

II. Clinical Evaluation and Diagnosis Obtain a history through observation, interview, and/or review of Visualization-assess physical characteristics of patient; relevant records to assess current or potential injury, illness, or movements; actions condition Visualization- picture normal and abnormal anatomic Identify the extent and severity of injuries, illnesses, structures; picture pathology, deformity, or disease and conditions Embedded information-identify characteristics of Relate signs and symptoms to specific injuries, disorders that might be partially hidden or disguised illnesses, and conditions Pattern recognition-recognize signs/symptoms Obtain and record information related to injuries, associated with pathology, disease, deformity illnesses, and conditions Mental manipulation-visualize the rotation/motion of Recognize predisposing factors to specific injuries, anatomic structures within the body illnesses, and conditions Visualization-estimate and/or measure various Identify anatomical structures involved in injuries, physical characteristics illnesses, and conditions Pattern recognition-recognize predisposing conditions, Interpret medical records and related reports signs, symptoms which may lead to disease or injury Identify psychosocial factors associated with Visualization-picture delayed reactions to activities, or injuries, illnesses, and conditions to disease or injury Identify nutritional factors related to injuries, Visualization-relate medical information and reports to illnesses, and conditions patient characteristics (e.g. radiographic and sectional Identify the impact of supplements and prescription images) and nonprescription medications associated with Visualization-in order to communicate descriptions or injuries, illnesses, and conditions instructions Interview and communicate for the purpose of gathering information related to the condition inspect the involved area(s) visually to assess the injury, illness, Visualization-determine size/extent of injured area or health-related condition Visualization-assess physical characteristics of patient; Properly expose the area in order to evaluate the movements; actions involved area Spatial orientation-placement of self and patient to Assess immediate and delayed physiological expose injured area with regard to proper body responses to injuries, illnesses, and health-related mechanics and to avoid further harm or injury conditions Visualization- picture normal and abnormal anatomic Identify bony surface landmarks and soft tissue structures; picture pathology, deformity, or disease abnormalities of specific/special injuries, illnesses, Embedded information-identify characteristics of and health-related conditions disorders that might be partially hidden or disguised Identify the relationship and severity of pathological (e.g. edema, symmetry, rashes, discoloration, etc.) signs of injuries, illnesses, and health-related Pattern recognition-recognize signs/symptoms conditions associated with pathology, disease, deformity Assess pre-existing structural abnormalities and Mental manipulation-visualize the rotation/motion of relate them to pathomechanics of injuries, illnesses, anatomic structures within the body and health-related conditions Visualization-relate surface landmarks to internal anatomic structures Pattern recognition-differentiate between pre-existing conditions and injury or illness Palpate the involved area(s) using standard techniques to assess Visualization-relate bony landmarks to internal the injury, illness, or health-related condition anatomic structures Locate and palpate bony landmarks, articulations, Visualization-picture pathologic disorders and ligamentous structures, musculotendinous units, and mechanisms of injury other soft tissues Pattern recognition-picture signs/symptoms of injury Recognize severity of pathological signs and during assessment and palpation symptoms of injuries, illnesses, and health-related Embedded information-identify characteristics of conditions disorders that might be partially hidden or disguised Assess immediate and delayed physiological before and during palpation(e.g. edema, symmetry, response to injuries, illnesses, and health-related rashes, discoloration, etc.) conditions Palpate appropriate structures in order to assess the integrity of human anatomical/physiological systems continued

247

Table 38 Continued

Perform specific tests in accordance with accepted procedures to Visualization-assess normal and abnormal physical assess the injury, illness, or health-related condition characteristics Assess muscular strength through the use of manual Visualization-to apply and measure results of tests or non-manual muscle tests Pattern recognition-recognize signs/symptoms of Assess joint range of motion using test and injury or illness measurement techniques Pattern recognition-compare patterns of test responses Identify structural and functional integrity of to known illnesses or injuries anatomical structures Embedded information-identify characteristics of Identify appropriate specific/special tests for disorders that might be partially hidden or disguised particular injuries before and during testing (e.g. edema, symmetry, Assess neurological function rashes, discoloration, etc.) Identify the signs and symptoms related to Spatial orientation-to position self and patient to specific/special tests properly perform physical assessments Identify location, type, function, and action of each Visualization-to choose appropriate assessment tool joint for potential injury or illness Use equipment associated with specific/special tests Mental manipulation-if patient cannot assume normal Perform specific/special tests position to perform test or use assessment tool Interpret the information gained from specific/special tests Formulate a clinical impression by interpreting the signs, Pattern recognition-compare signs/symptoms with symptoms, and predisposing factors of the injury, illness, or known illnesses or injuries and determine initial condition to determine the appropriate course of action impression Interpret the pertinent information from the Visualization-picture appropriate course of action (e.g. evaluation activity, exercise, rehabilitation) Synthesize applicable information from an evaluation Identify appropriate courses of action Educate the appropriate patient(s) regarding the assessment by Visualization-of anatomy, physiologic actions, and communicating information about the current or potential injury, illness/injury illness, or health-related condition to encourage compliance with Visualization-may need to prepare visual instructions recommended care or information for patient Use both verbal and written forms of communication Pattern recognition-to interpret guidelines and Interpret medical terminology and describe the nature instructions for activities/exercises, and to of injuries, illnesses, and health-related conditions in communicate steps to patient basic terms Utilize appropriate counseling techniques Share assessment findings with other healthcare professionals Visualization-to translate patient assessment using effective means of communication to coordinate information into appropriate visual material for appropriate care referrals (e.g. charts, graphs, diagrams, measurements) Communicate with healthcare professionals Collaborate with healthcare professionals Use medical terminology and nomenclature Direct a referral to other medical personnel continued

248

Table 38 Continued

III. Immediate Care Employ life-saving techniques through the use of standard Visualization-picture internal structures while emergency procedures in order to reduce morbidity and the preparing for and during CPR; picture internal incidence of mortality anatomic structures when preparing for and treating Perform cardio-pulmonary resuscitation techniques other emergency situations and procedures Pattern recognition-recognize patterns of movement or Implement federal and state occupational, safety, and actions that might indicate a life-threatening or health guidelines emergency situation Remove protective equipment and use removal Mental rotation—to appropriately don personal devices protective equipment Use emergency equipment Embedded information—locate and dispose of Implement immobilization and transfer techniques potentially hazardous material (e.g. locate all sharps Implement emergency action plan(s) prior to disposing of procedure trays and drapes) Manage common life-threatening emergency Pattern recognition—recognizing appropriate set-up of situations/conditions (e.g. evaluation, monitoring, and sterile trays provision of care) Visualization—to find appropriate artery and take Transfer care to appropriate medical and/or allied pulse health professionals and/or facilities Mental manipulation—to apply equipment and obtain Measure and monitor vital signs blood pressure Spatial orientation—to position self and patient Embedded information—to recognize abnormalities that might be disguised (e.g. pathological features that might affect blood pressure readings) Pattern recognition—to determine if equipment is properly applied; to recognize patterns of abnormalities in relation to potential pathology Visualization—of patient movement and placement; depth perception; estimation of distances Spatial orientation—of self, patient, equipment Mental manipulation—of movement/transfer of the entire system Pattern recognition—to picture steps of safe transfer Embedded information—recognize lines, tubes, and other equipment against background Pattern recognition-recognize normal and abnormal readings from emergency equipment (e.g. EKG reading) Prevent exacerbation of non-life-threatening condition(s) Visualization-of normal and abnormal anatomic and through the use of standard procedures in order to reduce physiologic systems morbidity Visualization-of instructions/guidelines for proper use Implement federal and state occupational, safety, and of medical equipment health guidelines standards and guidelines Spatial orientation-awareness of self and patient Use standard medical equipment position in order to apply and use medical equipment Remove protective equipment and use removal Pattern recognition-to discern if equipment is devices functioning appropriately Implement immobilization and transfer techniques Pattern recognition-evaluation of patient movements Obtain vital signs or actions when treated; evaluate if condition is stable Manage non-life-threatening conditions (e.g. or is worsening; evaluate patterns of motion/action evaluation, monitoring, and provision of care) which indicate patient can return to activity Use standard medical equipment Spatial orientation-of self and patient to remove Implement emergency action plan(s) protective equipment and devices safely Transfer care to appropriate medical and/or allied Visualization-of mechanism of injury health professionals and/or facilities Visualization-may be needed to explain mechanism Determine appropriateness for return to activity and/or severity of injury to other medical professionals Apply pharmacological and therapeutic modalities Embedded information-recognize visual indicators of injury that might be disguised or hidden by clothing or equipment continued

249

Table 38 Continued

Facilitate the timely transfer of care for conditions beyond the Visualization-of normal and abnormal anatomic and scope of practice of the athletic trainer by implementing pathologic conditions appropriate referral strategies to stabilize and/or prevent Pattern recognition-of normal and abnormal exacerbation of the condition(s) actions/motions Implement the emergency action plan(s) Spatial orientation-of self and patient to immobilize or Recognize acute conditions beyond the scope of the move safely athletic trainer Visualization-to communicate patient condition Communicate with other medical and allied effectively to other healthcare providers healthcare providers Manage life- and non-life-threatening conditions until transfer to appropriate medical providers and facilities Direct the appropriate patient(s) in standard immediate care Visualization-to communicate patient condition procedures using formal and informal methods to facilitate effectively to patient, family, or other healthcare immediate care providers Communicate effectively with appropriate patients Implement the emergency action plan(s) Educate patients regarding standard emergency care procedures IV. Treatment, Rehabilitation, and Reconditioning Administer therapeutic and conditioning exercise(s) using Visualization-of normal and abnormal anatomic and standard techniques and procedures in order to facilitate physiologic systems recovery, function, and/or performance Visualization-of nature of injury Apply exercise prescription in the development and Spatial orientation-of self and patient to explain, implementation of treatment, rehabilitation, and demonstrate, and evaluate prescribed exercise(s) reconditioning (e.g. aquatics, isokinetics, and closed- Pattern recognition-to determine if patient is chain) performing motion/action in correct sequence Evaluate criteria for return to activity Embedded information-evaluate for hidden or disguised physical signs of injury, re-injury, or damage Pattern recognition-evaluate motion/actions to determine if treatment is successful Visualization-for measurements and evaluations of rehabilitation (e.g. range of motion, proprioception measurements) Administer therapeutic modalities using standard techniques and Visualization-of normal and abnormal anatomic and procedures in order to facilitate recovery, function, and/or physiologic systems performance Visualization-of nature of injury Apply thermal, electrical, mechanical, and acoustical Spatial orientation-of self and patient to explain, modalities demonstrate, apply and evaluate prescribed therapeutic Apply manual therapy techniques modality(s) Mental manipulation-may be required to correctly apply therapy/modality to injured area Apply braces, splints, or assistive devices in accordance with Visualization-of normal and abnormal anatomic and appropriate standards and practices in order to facilitate physiologic systems recovery, function, and/or performance Visualization-of nature of injury Apply braces, splints, or assistive devices Spatial orientation-of self and patient to explain, Fabricate braces, splints, or assistive devices demonstrate, apply and evaluate prescribed device(s) Mental manipulation-may be required to correctly apply device to injured area Visualization-to fabricate devices for particular patient need(s) continued

250

Table 38 Continued

Administer treatment for general illness and/or conditions using Visualization-to locate, measure, and apply topical standard techniques and procedures to facilitate recovery, products function, and/or performance Mental rotation—to appropriately don personal Apply topical wound or skin-care products protective equipment Apply universal precautions Embedded information—locate and dispose of Refer to appropriate healthcare providers potentially hazardous material (e.g. locate all sharps Recognize the status of systemic illnesses prior to disposing of procedure trays and drapes) Recognize atypical psychosocial conditions Visualization- of normal and abnormal anatomic and physiologic systems Visualization-of nature of injury Pattern recognition-recognize visual patterns of illness (infections) or psychosocial reactions of illness Reassess the status of injuries, illnesses, and/or conditions using Visualization-determine size/extent of injured area standard techniques and documentation strategies in order to Visualization-assess physical characteristics of patient; determine appropriate treatment, rehabilitation, and/or movements; actions reconditioning and to evaluate readiness to return to a desired Spatial orientation-placement of self and patient to level of activity expose injured area with regard to proper body Interpret assessment information necessary to mechanics and to avoid further harm or injury modify, continue, or discontinue treatment plans Visualization- picture normal and abnormal anatomic Apply functional criteria for return to activity structures; picture pathology, deformity, or disease Embedded information-identify characteristics of disorders that might be partially hidden or disguised (e.g. edema, symmetry, rashes, discoloration, etc.) Pattern recognition-recognize signs/symptoms associated with pathology, disease, deformity Mental manipulation-visualize the rotation/motion of anatomic structures within the body Visualization-relate surface landmarks to internal anatomic structures Pattern recognition-differentiate between pre-existing conditions and injury or illness Visualization-relate bony landmarks to internal anatomic structures Visualization-picture pathologic disorders and mechanisms of injury Pattern recognition-picture signs/symptoms of injury during assessment and palpation Embedded information-identify characteristics of disorders that might be partially hidden or disguised before and during palpation(e.g. edema, symmetry, rashes, discoloration, etc.) Visualization-assess normal and abnormal physical characteristics Visualization-to apply and measure results of tests Pattern recognition-recognize signs/symptoms of injury or illness Pattern recognition-compare patterns of test responses to known illnesses or injuries Embedded information-identify characteristics of disorders that might be partially hidden or disguised before and during testing (e.g. edema, symmetry, rashes, discoloration, etc.) Spatial orientation-to position self and patient to properly perform physical assessments Visualization-to choose appropriate assessment tool for potential injury or illness Mental manipulation-if patient cannot assume normal position to perform test or use assessment tool continued

251

Table 38 Continued

Educate the appropriate patient(s) in the treatment, Visualization-of anatomy, physiologic actions, and rehabilitation, and reconditioning of injuries, illnesses, and/or illness/injury conditions using applicable methods and materials to facilitate Visualization-may need to prepare visual instructions recovery, function, and/or performance or information for patient Identify appropriate patients to educate Pattern recognition-to interpret guidelines and Communicate appropriate information instructions for activities/exercises, and to Disseminate information to patients at an appropriate communicate steps to patient level Provide guidance and/or counseling for the appropriate patient(s) Visualization-of anatomy, physiologic actions, and in the treatment, rehabilitation, and reconditioning of injuries, illness/injury illnesses, and/or conditions through communication to facilitate Visualization-may need to prepare visual instructions recovery, function, and/or performance or information for patient Identify appropriate patients for guidance and Pattern recognition-to interpret guidelines and counseling instructions for activities/exercises, and to Use appropriate psychosocial techniques (e.g. goal communicate steps to patient; to interpret graphs and setting and stress management) in rehabilitation charts (e.g. goal setting charts) Refer to appropriate healthcare professionals Use effective communication skills Provide guidance/counseling for the patient during the treatment, rehabilitation, and reconditioning process V. Organization and Administration Establish action plans for response to injury or illness using Visualization-in interpretation of policies, guidelines, available resources to provide the required range of healthcare regulations (e.g. judge size, shape, distance, services for patients, athletic activities, and events dimensions, etc) Organize resources and personnel Pattern recognition-form forms, interpretation of Interact with appropriate administration leadership research studies, graphics Obtain appropriate policies, guidelines, and regulations Interpret regulatory policies Establish policies and procedures for the delivery of healthcare Pattern recognition-to interpret charts and graphs and services following accepted guidelines to promote safe other visual information participation, timely care, and legal compliance Visualization-for preparation of visual information for Apply existing guidelines disseminating policies/procedures or for Interact with appropriate patients documentation Complete the documentation process Organize policies and procedures in a logical fashion Ascertain appropriate policies, guidelines, and regulations Apply evidence-based and epidemiology studies Apply statutory, regulatory, and other legal provision Establish policies and procedures for the management of Visualization-picture equipment and its use to healthcare facilities and activity areas by referring to accepted determine if guidelines are being followed guidelines, standards, and regulations to promote safety and Pattern recognition-are appropriate steps being legal compliance followed when using equipment Comply with equipment manufacturer’s operational Embedded information-when setting up or conducting regulations/guidelines facility inspections need to recognize hidden or Comply with institutional and governmental policies disguised hazards and procedures for maintenance of facilities and Pattern recognition-are forms completed appropriately equipment Apply OSHA standards Recognize potential safety and environmental hazards Assure compliance of involved staff continued

252

Table 38 Continued

Manage human and fiscal resources by utilizing appropriate Visualization-when delegating tasks to staff leadership, organization, and management techniques to provide Pattern recognition-for inventory control efficient and effective healthcare services Visualization-facility planning or organization Manage human resources (e.g. delegating, planning, staffing, hiring, firing, and conducting performance evaluations) Manage financial resources (e.g. planning, budgeting, resource allocation, revenue generation) Use computer software applications (e.g. word processing, database spreadsheet, and Internet applications) Maintain records using an appropriate system to document Pattern recognition-when creating and maintaining services rendered, provide for continuity of care, facilitate visual records and documentation communication, and meet legal standards Visualization-picture anatomy or pathology when Create and complete the documentation process interpreting medical records Dictate medical records Mental rotation-for sectional images Use computer applications for record keeping Apply knowledge of medical terminology and abbreviations Interpret medical records Adhere to legal requirements/procedures pertaining to medical records Develop professional relationships with appropriate patients and Visualization-to appropriately communicate with entities by applying effective communication techniques to patients and other professionals (e.g. to explain enhance the delivery of healthcare pathology, disability, deformity) Mitigate conflict Visualization-to plan meetings and prepare appropriate Plan meetings visual material Respect diversity of opinions and positions Interpret medical terminology for appropriate patients Nurture professional relationships Use effective communication styles and techniques Network and recruit qualified medical team members VI. Professional Responsibility Demonstrate appropriate professional conduct by complying Visualization-to interpret and apply relevant research with applicable standards and maintaining continuing competence to provide quality athletic training services Obtain, interpret, evaluate, and apply relevant research data, literature, and/or other forms of information Obtain, interpret, evaluate, and apply relevant policy and position statements Obtain, interpret, and apply the BOC Standards of Practice Obtain, interpret, and apply NATA Code of Ethics Apply evidence-based medicine (EBM) Adhere to statutory regulatory provisions and other legal responsibilities relating to the practice of athletic training by maintaining an understanding of these provisions and responsibilities in order to contribute to the safety and welfare of the public Research and apply state and federal statutes, regulations, and adjudications Research professional standards and guidelines (e.g. BOC, NATA, state organizations) Research practice methods and procedures continued

253

Table 38 Continued

Educate appropriate patients and entities about the role and Visualization-for preparation of formal and informal standards of practice of the athletic trainer through informal and visual displays for patients and the public formal means to improve the ability of those patients and entities to make informed decisions Communicate information through various methods Identify the appropriate patients and/or entities Apply relevant information to specific employment and/or practice settings

254

Appendix B: Spatial Tasks for Health Information Management and Systems

Tasks—Health Information Management and Systems Spatial Components (HIMS) From: American Health Information Management Association AHIMA) Commission on Certification for Health Informatics and Information Management (CCHIIM) Health Data Management Manage health data elements and/or data sets Develop and maintain organizational policies, procedures, and guidelines for management of health information Ensure accuracy and integrity of health data and health record Pattern recognition-determine if data is appropriately documentation entered Visualization-of anatomy, pathology, procedures Manage and/or validate coding accuracy and compliance Code diagnosis and procedures according to established Spatial orientation-to perform coding procedures using guidelines computers and other equipment Present data for organizational use (e.g. summarize, synthesize, Visualization-to prepare presentations and condense information) Pattern recognition-to ensure information is correctly presented Embedded information-to ensure information is correctly presented Health Statistics and Research Support Identify and/or respond to the information needs of internal and external healthcare customers Filter and/or interpret information for the end customer Pattern recognition Embedded information Visualization-to determine needed information Analyze and present information for organizational management Visualization-to prepare presentations (e.g. quality, utilization, risk) Pattern recognition-to ensure information is correctly presented Embedded information-to ensure information is correctly presented Use data mining techniques to query and report from databases Pattern recognition Embedded information Information Technology and Systems Implement and manage use of technology application Spatial orientation-for correct use of computers and other technology Develop data dictionary and data models for database design Pattern recognition Embedded information Visualization-anatomy, pathology, procedures Manage and maintain databases (e.g. data migration, updates) Apply data and functional standards to achieve interoperability of healthcare information systems Apply data/record storage principles and techniques associated with the medium (e.g. paper-based, hybrid, electronic) continued Table 39. Spatial Tasks in Health Information Management and Systems

255

Table 39 Continued

Evaluate and recommend clinical, administrative, and specialty service applications (e.g. financial systems, electronic record, clinical coding) Manage master person index (e.g. patient record integration, customer/client relationship management) Organization and Management Develop and support strategic and operational plans for facility- Visualization wide health information management Pattern recognition Monitor industry trends and organizational needs to anticipate Embedded information changes Pattern recognition Perform human resource management activities (e.g. recruiting Visualization-to prepare recruiting and other visual staff, creating job descriptions, resolve personnel issues) media Conduct training and educational activities (e.g. HIM systems, Visualization-to prepare training and educational coding, medical and institutional terminology; documentation media and regulatory requirements) Visualization-to explain/describe anatomy, pathology, equipment/procedures Establish and monitor productivity standards for the HIM function Optimize reimbursement through management of the revenue cycle (e.g. chargemaster maintenance) Develop, motivate, and support work teams and/or individuals (e.g. coaching, mentoring) Prepare and manage budgets visualization Analyze and report on budget variances Determine resource needs by performing analyses (e.g. cost- benefit, business planning) Evaluate and manage contracts (e.g. vendor, contract personnel, Pattern recognition maintenance) Organize and facilitate meetings Visualization-to prepare visual media Advocate for department, organization and/or profession Manage projects Prepare for accreditation and licensing processes (e.g. Joint Commission, Medicare, state regulators) Privacy, Security, and Confidentiality Design and implement security measures to safeguard Protected Health Information (PHI) Manage access, disclosure, and use of Protected Health Information (PHI) to ensure confidentiality Investigate and resolve healthcare privacy and security issues/problems Develop and maintain healthcare privacy and security training programs Legal and Regulatory Standards Administer organizational compliance with healthcare information laws, regulations and standards (e.g. audit, report and/or inform; legal health record) Prepare for accreditation and licensing processes (e.g. Joint Commission, Medicare, state regulators) CHPS: Management and Administration Provide guidance regarding applicable standards of accreditation agencies (Joint Commission, AAAHC, AOA, NCQA) Administer an appropriate organization infrastructure for privacy and information security Create, document, and communicate company privacy and Visualization-for preparation of presentations security policies, procedures, and guidelines continued

256

Table 39 Continued

Review relationships to indentify business associates Ensure appropriate contract development and management procedures comply with business associate requirements Ensure the maintenance of the inventory of software, hardware, Pattern recognition-to determine if appropriate and all information assets equipment is available and in proper working order Pattern recognition-to determine if software is functioning as needed Participate in business continuity planning for planned downtime and contingency planning for emergencies and disaster recovery Perform data criticality analysis Establish and maintain facility security plan to safeguard unauthorized physical access to information and prevent the theft or tampering Participate in analysis, implementation and decisions regarding privacy and security solutions Develop, deliver, evaluate, and document training and awareness Visualization-to prepare presentation media of privacy and security Pattern recognition-evaluation of presentation media Work with appropriate organization officials to ensure information used or disclosed for research complies with applicable privacy regulations Facilitate ongoing assessments of organizational policies, procedures, and practices related to privacy and security Regulatory Requirements, Investigation, and Compliance Assess and communicate risks and ramifications of breaches of privacy and security, including those by business associates to leadership Establish incident response plan and identify team members (for Pattern recognition-to prepare flow charts and other example, Human Resources, Legal, Risk Management, Physical media for response plans Security, Legal Law Enforcement, Public Relations) Coordinate privacy and security compliance documentation required by law Ensure and monitor compliance with state and federal laws and regulations related to privacy and security Coordinate the organization’s response to inquiries and investigations from external entities relating to privacy and security Develop system to maintain and retain applicable documentation Establish compliance indicators and develop methods to measure compliance to improve organizational performance Coordinate incident investigations and response Develop, implement, and ensure follow-through on a system to evaluate risk Enforce privacy and security policies, procedures, and guidelines to enable compliance with federal, state, and other regulatory or accrediting bodies Monitor appropriateness of access to identifiable health information Establish a compliant investigation and resolution process Information Technology Monitor data backup plan Develop and manage strategic information security plan Pattern recognition-to determine if there are security risks to information from computer/software Assess security risks and identify threats and vulnerabilities continued

257

Table 39 Continued

Establish audit controls (for example, logging guidelines, administrative access) Ensure technical safeguards such as configuration management, intrusion detection, and preventive countermeasures are adequate for the organization Ensure the documentation of the maintenance of software, hardware, and all information assets Ensure that preventive measures are in place to prevent attacks (for example, malicious code, hacking) Establish internal standards to determine compliance to security requirements by system, network, application, and user Ensure that the transmission of secure and private information is protected appropriately Implement disaster recovery plan as needed after disaster has occurred Establish guidelines, procedures, and controls to ensure the integrity, availability and confidentiality of communication across networks (for example, wireless Internet, secure sockets, VPN’s, and PKI) Ensure the use of event triggering to notify abnormal conditions within a system (for example, intrusion detection, denial of service, and invalid log-on attempts) Establish and manage process for verifying and controlling access authorizations and privileges including emergency access (for example, context-based access, role-based access, and user- based access) Establish and manage authentication mechanisms (for example, guidelines, unique user ID, password, biometrics, PIN, token, telephone call back) Develop process for the use of cryptography, digital signatures, and public and private key infrastructure technologies Provide forensic services (for example, data recovery, evidence preservation, and event tracing) Physical Safeguards Establish media control practices that govern the receipt, removal, or disposal (internal and external destruction) of any media containing data Establish physical security mechanisms to limit the access to authorized personnel for approved activities (for example, workstation placement, fax machine control, printer control) Establish reasonable safeguards to reduce incidental disclosure Ensure use of generally accepted physical and system security principles Health Information Management Recommend appropriate de-identification methodologies Ensure that recipients of secure and private information are permitted to receive the information (subpoena, court orders, search warrants) Ensure the rights of the individual who is a subject of individually identifiable health information (amendments, access, restrictions, confidential communications) Define HIPAA-designated record sets for the organization Identify information and record sets requiring special privacy protections Identify permitted disclosures (for example, research, marketing, fund development, valid authorizations) continued

258

Table 39 Continued

Identify permitted uses of health information (for example, treatment, payment, healthcare operations, minimum necessary, need-to-know) Ensure protocols are in place to verify identity of recipients of health information Certified Coding Specialist (CCS) Health Information Documentation Interpret health record documentation using knowledge of anatomy physiology, clinical disease processes, pharmacology, and medical terminology to identify codeable diagnoses and/or procedures Determine when additional clinical documentation is needed to assign the diagnosis and/or procedure code(s) Consult with physicians and other healthcare providers to obtain further clinical documentation to assist with code assignment Consult with reference materials to facilitate code assignment Identify patient encounter types Identify and post charges for healthcare services based on documentation Diagnosis Coding Select the diagnoses that require coding according to current coding and reporting requirements for acute care (inpatient) services Select the diagnoses that require coding according to current coding and reporting requirements for outpatient services Interpret conventions, formats, instructional notations, tables, and definitions of the classification system to select diagnoses, conditions, problems, or other reasons for the encounter that require coding Sequence diagnoses and other reasons for encounter according to notations and conventions of the classification system and standard data set definitions (such as Uniform Hospital Discharge Data Set [UHDDS]) Apply the official ICD-9-CM coding guidelines Procedural Coding Select the procedures that require coding according to current coding and reporting requirements for acute care (inpatient) services Select the procedures that require coding according to current coding and reporting requirements for outpatient services Interpret conventions, formats, instructional notations, and definitions of the classification system and/or nomenclature to select procedures/services that require codign Sequence procedures according to notations and conventions of the classification system/nomenclature and standard data set definitions (such as UHDDS) Apply the official ICD-9-CM coding guidelines Apply the official CPT/HCPCS Level II coding guidelines Regulatory Guidelines and Reporting Requirements for cute Care (Inpatient) Service Select the principal diagnosis, principal procedure, complications, comorbid conditions, other diagnoses and procedures that require coding according to UHDDS definitions and Coding Clinic for ICD-9-CM Evaluate the impact of code selections on Diagnosis Related Group (DRG) assignment continued

259

Table 39 Continued

Verify DRG assignment based on Inpatient Prospective Payment System (IPPS) definitions Assign the appropriate discharge disposition Regulatory Guidelines and Reporting Requirements for Outpatient Services Select the reason for encounter, pertinent secondary conditions, primary procedure, and other procedures that require coding according to UHDDS definitions, CPT Assistant, Coding Clinic for ICD-9-CM, and HCPCS Apply Outpatient Prospective Payment System (OPPS) reporting requirements: Modifiers CPT/HCPCS Level II Medical necessity Evaluation and Management code assignment (facility reporting) Data Quality and Management Assess the quality of coded data Educate healthcare providers regarding reimbursement methodologies, documentation rules, and regulations related to coding Analyze health record documentation for quality and completeness of coding Review the accuracy of abstracted data elements for data base integrity and claims processing Review and resolve coding edits such as Correct Coding Initiative (CCI), Medicare Code Editor (MCE) and Outpatient Code Editor (OCE) Information and Communication Technologies Use computer to ensure data collection, storage, analysis, and reporting of information Use common software applications (for example, work processing spreadsheets, and e-mail) in the execution of work processes Use specialized software in the completion of HIM processes Privacy, Confidentiality, Legal, and Ethical Issues Apply policies and procedures for access and disclosure of personal health information Apply AHIMA Code of Ethics/Standards of Ethical Coding Recognize/report privacy issues/problems Protect data integrity and validity using software or hardware technology Compliance Participate in the development of institutional coding policies to ensure compliance with official coding rules and guidelines Evaluate the accuracy and completeness of the patient record as defined by organizational policy and external regulations and standards Monitor compliance with organization-wide health record documentation and coding guidelines Recognize/report compliance concerns/finding

260

Appendix C: Spatial Tasks for Medical Dietetics

Dietitian From: Commission on Accreditation for Dietetics Education 2008 Foundation Knowledge and Competencies – Dietitian Education (http://www.eatright.org/uploadedFiles/CADE/CADE- General-Content/3-08_RD-FKC_Only.pdf)

Select appropriate indicators and measure achievement of clinical, programmatic, quality, productivity, economic or other outcomes Apply evidence-based guidelines, systematic reviews and scientific literature (such as the ADA Evidence Analysis Library, Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews and the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, National Guideline Clearinghouse Web sites) in the nutrition care process and model and other areas of dietetics practice Justify programs, products, services and care using appropriate Pattern recognition-to locate, interpret, and evaluate evidence or data professional literature Visualization-anatomic and pathologic concepts Evaluate emerging research for application in dietetics practice Pattern recognition-for evaluation of data Embedded information-to recognize important information Visualization-of anatomic and pathologic concepts Conduct research projects using appropriate research methods, ethical procedures and statistical analysis Professional Practice Expectations: beliefs, values, attitudes and behaviors for the professional dietitian level of practice Practice in compliance with current federal regulations and state statures ad rules, as applicable and in accordance with accreditation standards and the ADA Scope of Dietetics Practice Framework, Standards of Professional Performance and Code of Ethics for the Profession of Dietetics Demonstrate professional writing skills in preparing professional Visualization-of anatomy, pathology, procedures, communications (e.g. research manuscripts, project proposals, etc education materials, policies and procedures) Pattern recognition-in preparation of materials and of appropriate data Design, implement and evaluate presentations considering life Visualization-of materials experiences, cultural diversity and educational background of the target audience Use effective education and counseling skills to facilitate behavior Visualization-of anatomy, pathology, procedures, change and other information which would help in counseling patients/clients Spatial orientation-assists with non-verbal communication (body language) continued Table 40. Spatial Tasks for Medical Dietitians

261

Table 40 Continued

Demonstrate active participation, teamwork and contributions in Spatial orientation-assists with non-verbal group settings communication (body language) Assign appropriate patient care activities to DTRs and/or support Visualization-of anatomy, pathology, procedures, personnel considering the needs of the patient/client or situation, etc the ability of support personnel, jurisdictional law, practice Embedded information-recognize information guidelines and policies within the facility regarding patient/client condition that might be disguised or hidden (e.g. body characteristics, rashes, etc.) Refer clients and patient to other professionals and services when Visualization-of roles/services/procedures provided needs are beyond individual scope of practice by other health care professionals Demonstrate initiative by proactively developing solutions to Visualization-of anatomy, pathology, procedures, problems etc. Apply leadership principles effectively to achieve desired outcomes Serve in professional and community organizations Establish collaborative relationships with internal and external stakeholders, including patients, clients, care givers, physicians, nurses and other health professionals, administrative and support personnel to facilitate individual and organizational goals Demonstrate professional attributes such as advocacy, customer focus, risk taking, critical thinking, flexibility, time management, work prioritization and work ethic within various organizational cultures Perform self assessment, develop goals and objectives and prepare Visualization-of necessary organization and content a draft portfolio for professional development as defined by the of portfolio Commission on Dietetics Registration Pattern recognition-to organize and assemble portfolio Clinical and Customer Services: development and delivery of information, products and services to individuals, groups and populations Assess the nutritional status of individuals, groups and populations Visualization-mentally measure size, shape, in a variety of settings where nutrition care is or can be delivered abnormalities of body Visualization-anatomy, pathology, disease processes, physiology Embedded information-seek out visual information regarding nutritional status that might be hidden or disguised (e.g. rashes and other skin conditions, other visual clues of nutritional/metabolic disease) Diagnose nutrition problems and create problem etiology, signs and Visualization-mentally compare client physical symptoms (PES) statements attributes to known characteristics of nutritional diseases and disorders Pattern recognition-evaluate client for visual patterns of diseases of disorders Embedded information-look for information that may be disguised or hidden relating to nutritional diseases/disorders Plan and implement nutrition interventions to include prioritizing Visualization-compare client physical attributes to the nutrition diagnosis, formulating a nutrition prescription, know nutritional disorders establishing goals and selecting and managing intervention Visualization-plan menus and other nutritional interventions that are appealing to client Visualization-of pathophysiologic processes Monitor and evaluate problems, etiologies, signs, symptoms and the Visualization-compare client physical attributes to impact of interventions on the nutrition diagnosis know nutritional disorders Visualization-plan menus and other nutritional interventions that are appealing to client Visualization-of pathophysiologic processes continued

262

Table 40 Continued

Develop and demonstrate effective communications skills using Visualization-to plan and implement visual oral, print, visual, electronic and mass media methods for communication tools; maximizing client education, employee training and marketing Demonstrate and promote responsible use of resources including Visualization-plan and implement eye-appealing employees, money, time, water, energy, food and disposable goods menus and instructional media Develop and deliver products, programs or services that promote consumer health, wellness and lifestyle management merging consumer desire for taste, convenience and economy with nutrition, food safety and health messages and interventions Deliver respectful, science-based answers to consumer questions Visualization-evaluate non-verbal signals from concerning emerging trends clients Coordinate procurement, production, distribution and service of goods and services Develop and evaluate recipes, formulas and menus for acceptability Visualization-evaluate visual appeal of food and affordability that accommodate the cultural diversity and health Visualization-measurements and estimations needs of various populations, groups and individuals Spatial orientation-preparation of food, measurements, etc. Embedded information-visual inspection of recipes/menus when prepared for distracting appearances Visualization-inspect facility for food and other hazards Practice Management and Use of Resources: strategic application of principles of management and systems in the provision of services to individuals and organizations Use organizational processes and tools to manage human resources Perform management functions related to safety, security and Visualization-picture chain of infection, potential sanitation that affect employees, customers, patient, facilities and food-preparation hazards food Visualization-for education of patients and clients Spatial orientation-interacting with equipment during food preparation and facility sanitation procedures Apply systems theory and a process approach to make decisions and maximize outcomes Participate in public policy activities, including both legislative and regulatory initiatives Conduct clinical and customer service quality management activities Use current informatics technology to develop, store, retrieve and disseminate information and data Prepare and analyze quality, financial or productivity data and develops a plan for intervention Conduct feasibility studies for products, programs or services with consideration of costs and benefits Obtain and analyze financial data to assess budget controls and maximize fiscal outcomes Develop a business plan for a product, program or service including Visualization-of facility equipment needs; mental development of a budget, staffing needs, facility requirements, estimations of supply needs equipment and supplies continued

263

Table 40 Continued

Complete documentation that follows professional guidelines, guidelines required by health care systems and guidelines required by the practice setting Participate in coding and billing of dietetics/nutrition services to Visualization-of anatomy, pathology, procedures, obtain reimbursement for services from public or private insurers services

264

Appendix D: Spatial Tasks in Occupational Therapy

Tasks in Occupational Therapy Spatial Component Develop/teach computer skills Help with dressing, cooking, eating, grooming, driving Mental rotation, visualization to understand then teach these tasks; perception of gravity/orientation Develop adaptive equipment visualize potential equipment; visualize how potential equipment will interact with client; may need to mentally rotate parts of potential equipment or client Demonstrate/teach use of adaptive equipment see above Evaluate/modify work/home environment Visualization of environment in relation to pt; may need to pictures patterns of behavior in relation to environment; mental estimation of sizes/shapes/ other measurements Record activities/progress Pattern recognition Body mechanics Visualization; perception of space/gravity Patient assessment, medical history Pattern recognition, visualization Perform job/task analysis Charting Recognizing treatment/equipment hazards Mental manipulation, visualization Determine, prescribe, perform, teach exercises 1.1 interview individual/group/relevant others to obtain Social perception skills information regarding their health/social/occupational history, Visualization of activities described by clients and and current needs and concerns with respect to engaging in others occupations Recognition of patterns of activities Visualize anatomic structures and/or their relationships with other structures or the environment Mentally compare environments/activities as described by client/others with known environments/activities Determine useful information/observations from variety of data obtained from interviews 1.2 observe individual’s/group’s performance in environments Visualize anatomic structures to collect information about factors that influence occupational Recognize normal and non-normal patterns of performance movement Evaluate physical/mental abilities of client Spatial orientation/body orientation required to assist Evaluate patient skills and/or capacities patients in transfer or standing/walking Observe patient and their environment Mental imaging to determine best way to communicate Analyze patient activities instructions/movement/evaluative techniques to patient Knowledge of group dynamics and behavior, Visualization of patient environment (flexibility of ethnicity, cultures (and their history and origin) closure) Recognize/pick out hazards or obstacles in patient environment (flexibility of closure) Information ordering—must be able to recognize, visualize, and communicate patterns or visual information to client in particular order (when giving instructions or obtaining information) Quickly and accurately compare similarities/differences of objects/pictures/patterns/activities (perceptual speed) Continued

Table 41. Spatial Tasks in Occupational Therapy 265

Table 41 Continued

1.3 integrate the information gathered regarding the impact of Visualize nature and extent of injury and mentally impairment, disability, or condition on the individual’s/group’s compare to normal anatomy and physiology occupational roles in order to form a hypothesis to guide Recognize patterns of movement and activities intervention Mentally compare observed anatomy and activities Knowledge of information and techniques to with known diseases, deformities, injuries diagnose and treat human injuries, diseases, Visualize ramifications of diseases, deformities, deformities (includes symptoms, treatment injuries alternatives, drug properties and interactions) Visualize nature of injuries, diseases, deformities with Knowledge of biology; plant and animal organisms, environment of client tissues, cells (functions, interdependencies, Recognize patterns of movement/activities interactions Visualize spatial orientation of client, environment, Knowledge of math, physics potential equipment Recognize/visualize effects of treatments and medications on spatial activities of client Information ordering—must be able to recognize, visualize, and communicate patterns or visual information to client in particular order (when giving instructions or obtaining information) Quickly and accurately compare similarities/differences of objects/pictures/patterns/activities (perceptual speed) Visualize changes in environment or part of environment after all or parts of it have been rearranged 1.4 screen individual/group to determine need for OT services, Visualize anatomy and anatomic relationships using tools or methods used by occupational therapy Visualize nature and extent of injury and mentally practitioners compare to normal anatomy and physiology Analyze medical data Visualize ramifications of diseases, deformities, Knowledge of information and techniques to injuries diagnose and treat human injuries, diseases, Recognize patterns of movement/activities deformities (includes symptoms, treatment Visualize spatial orientation of client, environment, alternatives, drug properties and interactions) potential equipment Knowledge of math, physics Recognize/visualize effects of treatments and medications on spatial activities of client Information ordering—must be able to recognize, visualize, and communicate patterns or visual information to client in particular order (when giving instructions or obtaining information) Quickly and accurately compare similarities/differences of objects/pictures/patterns/activities (perceptual speed) Visualize changes in environment or part of environment after all or parts of it have been rearranged Body mechanics—visualize and arrange own body to safely assist client in activities or in working with assistive equipment Visualize movements of client Visualize relationship between client and assistive equipment Evaluate medical data such as radiographic images; mental manipulation required to identify and interpret anatomic information on cross-sectional images Depth perception continued

266

Table 41 Continued

1.5 select assessment instruments appropriate to chosen frame(s) Speed of closure—make sense of, combine, organize of reference or model(s) of practice to be administered as part of information into meaningful patterns in order to the evaluation process choose appropriate screening tools for evaluation Visualize operation of screening tools Mentally manipulate orientation of tools to most appropriately assist with evaluation Recognize patterns of movement or activities Visualize trajectories or paths of objects in movement 1.6 administer standardized screening and/or assessment Same as 1.3, 1.4, 1.5 instruments, based on normative or criterion-referenced data using instrument protocols, to gather information about factors that influence occupational performance Identify body response variations—categorize, estimate, recognize differences/similarities Detect changes in circumstances/events 1.7 administer non-standardized screening and/or assessment Same as 1.3, 1.4, 1.5 instruments using generally accepted practices to gather information regarding factors that influence occupational performance Identify body response variations—categorize, estimate, recognize differences/similarities Detect changes in circumstances/events

1.8 recommend referral(s) to other professionals for additional Visualize activities of other professionals evaluation to obtain a better understanding of Recognize patterns of activities which could be better individual’s/group’s impairment, disability, or current condition treated by other methods or professionals Consult with rehabilitation team to select activity Recognize patterns of activities/movement which program could benefit from combination treatments Coordinate occupational therapy with other Recognize patterns of interaction between practitioners therapeutic programs Choose relevant visual information from observations to determine whether referrals are necessary and with which branch of medicine

1.9 interpret evaluation findings based on relevant frame(s) of Requires all of the above spatial skills reference or model(s) of practice to determine the facilitators and barriers that impact occupational performance

1.10 formulate conclusions regarding problems in occupational Requires all of the above spatial skills performance to select possible intervention strategies to improve individual’s/group’s occupational performance

1.11 document results of the evaluation process using Visualize movements/activities appropriate formats to ensure accountability of service and to Draw, graph or diagram information from evaluation meet standards for reimbursement Pattern recognition to interpret graphs and charts, other medical information Speed of closure—make sense of, combine, organize information into meaningful patterns Identify relevant/important information from wealth of observations and other visual cues continued

267

Table 41 Continued

Develop intervention plan… Visualize optimal ways to educate client/others 2.1 prioritize intervention needs in collaboration with regarding intervention needs individual/group/relevant others to facilitate active participation Recognize relevant patterns of information and/or during intervention activities Visualize spatial orientation of client/other’s/practitioner to determine how and when to recommend treatments/therapy

2.2 select frame(s) of reference or model(s) of practice and specific approaches based on best practices to guide the intervention planning process

2.3 set measurable goals based on individual’s/groups’s needs Pattern recognition—to develop/use charts graphs and best practices to achieve client-centered outcomes

2.4 select intervention approaches that are designed to establish Visualize activities, movements, occupational skills or restore the individual’s/group’s skills or abilities consistent Visualize environment(s) in which skills are needed with frames of reference or models of practice Visualize how skills can be accomplished in the above 2.5 select intervention approaches designed to modify activities environments or performance environments relevant to the Visualize outcomes/goals for patient individual’s/group’s occupations Visualize/mentally manipulate or rotate assistive 2.6 select intervention methods/modalities and activities and/or adaptive equipment consistent with goals based on the individual’s/group’s priorities Visualize anatomic structures and relationships and results of the evaluation to reach client-centered outcomes Picture relationships between assistive/adaptive 2.7 select environment(s) that best facilitates performance in equipment and client order to achieve identified goals Recognize patterns of activities or movements, normal and abnormal Determine if activities can be performed by client and/or caregiver; must be able to picture activities/movements and spatial orientation of client/caregiver Estimate size, distance, quantity Visualize necessary resources to perform movement/activity 2.8 estimate frequency and duration of intervention needed to Pattern recognition when working with charts/graphs meet identified goals, according to service delivery systems or other indicators of outcomes

2.9 document intervention plan according to applicable Pattern recognition when working with charts/graphs regulations, to create a written record of the recommended or other indicators of outcomes actions of the OT practitioner

continued

268

Table 41 Continued

Implement interventions… See 2.4-2.7 3.1select intervention options in collaboration with individual/group/relevant others in order to maximize individual/group participation during the session Select activities for work/life-management skills Help client improve decision-making, abstract reasoning, memory, sequencing, coordination and perceptual skills, computer use

3.2 provide interventions in environments, settings, and times Provide documentation for assistive/adaptive that are relevant to individual/group and to pre-established goals equipment; requires visualization, pattern recognition, to maximize outcomes mental manipulation Prepare detailed instructions and/or drawings to tell others how devices/parts/equipment/structures are to be made, assembled, modified, maintained, used; requires visualization, pattern recognition, mental manipulation, perspective, ability to recognize and incorporate details into a whole Visualize environment as is and as could be for optimum interaction with client Recognize patterns of activities/movement Body mechanics—coordination of self and client in relation to space; body orientation 3.3 adapt and/or grade techniques and activities based on Assist client with dressing, cooking, eating, grooming, individual’s/group’s response to enhance task-specific driving; requires ability to visualize activities, patterns performance of activities or movement, mentally manipulate images Knowledge of engineering/technology to of equipment or assistive devices design/produce goods and services Visualization of client/equipment in order to prepare Knowledge of design: plans, blue prints, drawings, drawings or diagrams or instructions models Visualization of self, and relation of self to client to perform and instruct Awareness of body orientation Visualize alternate uses of common/everyday equipment to adapt to client’s needs Identification of hidden visual information in observations of client/equipment Recognition of patterns of activity

3.4 adapt the environment based on individual’s/group’s needs Visualize occupational tasks to promote active participation in meaningful occupations Visualize skills required for client to perform Knowledge of materials, methods, tools for occupational tasks construction/repair of houses or other structures Recognize/evaluate patterns of movements involved in performing occupational tasks Visualize tools required/involved in performing tasks and how those tools must interact with client Visualize needed changes in environment Visualize hazards/barriers 3.5 select tools, materials, and assistive technology based on Visualize interaction of client with tools, equipment, individual’s/group’s needs to facilitate the engagement in assistive devices meaningful occupations Recognize patterns of assembly, use, and misuse of Order supplies/equipment (splints, braces, computer- tools, equipment, devices aided adaptive equipment) Body/spatial orientation for OT and client Lay out materials such as puzzles, scissors, eating utensils Clean and repair the above equipment/tools

continued 269

Table 41 Continued

3.6 adapt and/or grade tools, materials, and assistive technology Recognize anatomy and anatomical relationships based on individual/group needs to facilitate the engagement in Mentally rotate or manipulate visual information meaningful occupations related to anatomy Visualize relationship between equipment, tools, devices and client Picture changes in equipment in relation to client’s deformity, disease, disability 3.7 design and/or construct tools, materials, assistive technology Visualize perspective (size, shape, length) when based on individual/group needs to facilitate the engagement in preparing diagrams or designs of new tools and meaningful occupations equipment Recognize/visualize spatial orientation cues when instructing/demonstrating the use of new or adapted equipment Mentally manipulate visual information related to tools and equipment when trouble-shooting potential problems Identify hidden visual information when designing, developing, demonstrating, using adapted or newly designed tools and equipment 3.8 educate individual/group/relevant others using instructional Visualize figural/symbolic information needed to methods, to promote safe use and proper care of tools, materials, educate clients and their caregivers or relevant others and assistive technology Recognize, communicate, diagram patterns (activities, 3.9 educate individual/group/relevant others, using instructional equipment assembly, tool usage, etc.) methods, to promote health and wellness in order to maintain optimal occupational performance 3.10 educate individual/group/relevant others, using instructional methods, about prevention methods that minimize the impact of condition or disability on occupational performance 3.11 develop home/community programs that support Visualize figural/symbolic information needed to occupational performance, based on client needs and priorities, educate clients and their caregivers or relevant others to promote engagement in meaningful occupations Recognize, communicate, diagram patterns (activities, 3.12 educate individual/group/relevant others, using equipment assembly, tool usage, etc.) instructional methods, to implement and continue a home/community program to support engagement in meaningful occupations 3.13 recommend equipment, strategies, and services to the individual/group/relevant others based on client needs in order to meet priorities Observe client and other caregivers regarding success of tools, equipment, devices 3.14 monitor the individual/group response to intervention in relation to desired functional outcomes to make decisions about the future direction of intervention 3.15 modify the intervention plan based on individual/group response to achieve targeted outcomes 3.16 document response to intervention and changes in functional outcomes using appropriate documentation standards to reflect progress toward targeted outcomes 3.17 recommend follow-up services and programs as needed by collaborating with individual/group/relevant others to transition smoothly and safely to future environment(s) 3.18 document the individual’s/group’s occupational skill level at time of discharge, reflecting outcomes of the intervention process Monitor customer satisfaction continued

270

Table 41 Continued

Provide services that address performance needs Visualization of environment, population needs 4.1 conduct a needs assessment to identify occupational Recognize patterns of activities/movements performance barriers and needs of populations being served to Visualize relationships of environments and plan appropriate intervention populations Advise on health risks in the workplace Visualize potential environmental changes Advise on transition to retirement 4.2 make intervention recommendations based on needs See 4.1 assessment of populations to achieve desired outcomes Prepare visual presentations; visualization, perspective, pattern recognition, flexibility of closure 4.3 develop wellness, prevention, and/or educational programs prepare visual presentations; visualization, perspective, based on intervention recommendations to meet the needs of pattern recognition, flexibility of closure specific populations Develop and participate in health promotion programs 4.4 implement population-based programs using methods prepare drawings/diagrams appropriate to the populations served to achieve desired rearrange equipment and other materials; visualize outcomes optimal patterns for equipment and materials 4.5 monitor effectiveness of population-based programs to determine need for modification 4.6 serve as a resource person or consultant by providing information and expertise that will assist other service providers to meet the needs of populations being served

4.7 develop a system for tracking population outcomes to charts and graphs--patterns measure success and evaluate program effectiveness 4.8 document program effectiveness using information gathered through program evaluations to make decisions about the future direction of specific population-based programs manage, organize, promote OT services Recognize patterns in quantitative or qualitative data 5.1 coordinate multiple services in collaboration with others to Observation of job performance; mentally compare ensure that individuals/groups are receiving quality care with optimal performance knowledge of administrative and clerical procedures, Pattern recognition in preparation of management of files/records charts/graphs/visual instructive or educational material design forms Visualization, pattern recognition, mental manipulation knowledge of sales and marketing; demonstrations, required to oversee use of materials, equipment, promotional products facilities 5.2 participate in the documentation of ongoing processes for Preparation of educational or continuing education quality improvement to ensure quality of services materials—visualization, perspective, pattern 5.3 implement program changes as needed based on quantitative recognition, flexibility of closure and qualitative information to improve service efficiency and effectiveness 5.4 supervise occupation therapists in their job performance in accordance with facility policies and within regulatory guidelines to monitor the quality of service provision by the therapists Knowledge of recruitment, training, personnel information systems, etc. 5.5 supervise occupational therapy assistants in their job performance in accordance with facility policies and within regulatory guidelines to monitor the quality of service provision by the assistants 5.6 supervise students on fieldwork placement in accordance with facility policies and fieldwork criteria to develop the skills of the students 5.7 supervise non-OT personnel in their job performance in accordance with facility policies and within regulatory guidelines to monitor the quality of services provided continued

271

Table 41 Continued

5.8 document services rendered using established guidelines to Visualization, pattern recognition, perspective for meet the requirements of regulatory agencies and/or funding preparation of drawings, diagrams, charts, graphs, etc. sources Complete and maintain records

5.9 participate in multidisciplinary team meetings using a client- Visualization of interventions used; needed in centered approach to coordinate services, and report evaluation preparation of drawings, illustrations, charts, graphs results and intervention outcomes Visualization of activities of other professionals in Knowledge of media production; ability to inform or meetings entertain via visual media 5.10 comply with appropriate safety regulations, laws, ethical Visualization of potential hazards (with clients and/or codes, facility policies and procedures, and guidelines governing equipment) OT practice to eliminate or decrease liabilities Recognition of patterns in graphs and charts, and other visual information 5.11 participate in professional development activities to remain current in occupational therapy practice 5.12 apply information from continuing professional activities to must be able to visualize information in order to apply remain current in occupational therapy practice in practice 5.13 participate in research, make presentations at professional Visualization—preparation of educational or research meetings, and/or write publications to contribute to the materials knowledge base of the OT profession Ability to recognize patterns in qualitative or quantitative data Find/recognize hidden information/figures/objects in data or other information sources Perspective, depth perception, awareness or size, shape, length, etc. in developing visual materials Visualize material to be presented 5.14 promote occupational therapy by educating other service Visualization—preparation of educational or providers and the general public about the benefits and promotional materials contribution of OT in maintaining the quality of life Perspective, depth perception, awareness or size, shape, length, etc. in developing visual materials

272

Appendix E: Spatial Tasks in Radiologic Science

Task Spatial Component Radiologic Technology (from ARRT Task Inventory for Radiography and O*NET Online) Confirm patient’s identity Pattern recognition-visually compare patient features with information provided on request Evaluate patient’s ability to understand and comply with Visualization—assessment of patient motion, actions, requirements for the requested examination etc. Pattern recognition—compare patient movements, actions, etc. with known care factors Explain and confirm patient’s preparation (e.g. diet restrictions, preparatory medications) prior to radiographic/fluoroscopic examinations Examine radiographic requisition to verify accuracy and Pattern recognition—recognize signs/symptoms and completeness of information (e.g. patient history, clinical compare with known diseases/deformities/disabilities diagnosis) Visualization—picture normal and abnormal anatomic structures; picture pathology, deformity, or disease associated with diagnosis Sequence imaging procedures to avoid residual contrast material Visualization—of anatomy and physiologic path of affecting future exams contrast media Responsible for medical equipment attached to patients (e.g. Visualization—of path of medical lines/tubes IVs, oxygen) during the radiographic procedures Spatial orientation—of self and patient and relationship between these two Embedded information—recognize lines, tubes against background Mental manipulation—to determine safest manner to manipulate equipment when transferring patient or in relationship to radiographic equipment continued

Table 42. Spatial Tasks in Radiologic Science

273

Table 42 Continued

Provide for patient safety, comfort, and modesty Visualization—interaction of patient/clothing/equipment Mental rotation—of patient/clothing/equipment in relation to each other to determine safest relationships Visualization—of anatomic structures in relationship to radiographic shielding or other protective equipment Communicate scheduling delays to waiting patients Verify or obtain patient consent as necessary (e.g. contrast studies) Explain procedure instructions to patient or patient’s family Visualization—of anatomy/pathology/disability, etc.

Practice standard precautions Mental rotation—to appropriately don personal protective equipment Embedded information—locate and dispose of potentially hazardous material (e.g. locate all sharps prior to disposing of procedure trays and drapes) Pattern recognition—recognizing appropriate set-up of sterile trays Follow appropriate procedures when in contact with patient in Visualization—picture appropriate anatomical isolation structures that may be hidden by isolation covers Spatial orientation—to position self and patient while avoiding transmission of pathogens Mental manipulation—to determine correct placement of radiographic equipment while avoiding transmission of pathogens Select immobilization devices, when indicated, to prevent Visualization—of potential movement which could be patient’s movement and/or ensure patient’s safety detrimental to image Visualization—of positioning of immobilization devices Mental manipulation—to plan and apply immobilization devices Visualization—of pertinent anatomy Visualization—picture anatomic pressure points Use proper body mechanics and/or mechanical transfer devices Visualization—of patient movement and placement; when assisting patient depth perception; estimation of distances Spatial orientation—of self, patient, equipment Mental manipulation—of movement/transfer of the entire system Pattern recognition—to picture steps of safe transfer Embedded information—recognize lines, tubes, and other equipment against background Prior to administration of contrast agent, gather information to Visualization—of patient size to determine accuracy of determine appropriate dosage, and to determine if patient is at information and to determine dosage of contrast increased risk of adverse reaction Visualization—types of reactions

continued

274

Table 42 Continued

Confirm type of contrast media and prepare for administration Embedded information—locate pertinent information about contrast prior to and during preparation (e.g. important label information, color/clarity of contrast agent) Visualization—depth perception in preparing equipment and drawing up contrast agent Pattern recognition—to determine if all appropriate equipment is prepared and available as needed Use sterile or aseptic technique when indicated Visualization—picture appropriate anatomical structures that may be hidden by isolation covers Spatial orientation—to position self and patient while avoiding transmission of pathogens Mental manipulation—to determine correct placement of radiographic equipment while avoiding transmission of pathogens Perform venipuncture Visualization—of path, depth, size, shape of vessels Visualization—of relationship of needle and vessel Embedded information—recognize contraindicating information (e.g. rash, swelling, or sclerosis of vessel) Mental manipulation—properly apply tourniquet Pattern recognition—to recognize visual indicators of success/failure Administer IV contrast Pattern recognition—to recognize visual indicators of success/failure (e.g. extravisation of contrast) Observe patient after administration of contrast media to detect Pattern recognition—to recognize visual indicators of adverse reactions impending reaction Embedded information—detect reaction indicators that may be partially or totally disguised Spatial orientation—to assist positioning of patient if treatment is needed

Obtain vital signs Visualization—to find appropriate artery and take pulse Mental manipulation—to apply equipment and obtain blood pressure Spatial orientation—to position self and patient Embedded information—to recognize abnormalities that might be disguised (e.g. pathological features that might affect blood pressure readings) Pattern recognition—to determine if equipment is properly applied; to recognize patterns of abnormalities in relation to potential pathology Recognize need for prompt medical attention and administer Pattern recognition—to recognize visual indicators of emergency care impending reaction Embedded information—detect reaction indicators that may be partially or totally disguised Spatial orientation—to assist positioning of patient if treatment is needed continued

275

Table 42 Continued

Explain post-procedural instructions to patient or patient’s Visualization—to determine most appropriate methods family to describe care to patient or family Maintain confidentiality of patient’s records Document required information on patient’s medical record (e.g. Pattern recognition—to recognize correct charting radiographic requisitions, radiographs) protocols; to determine if all appropriate information is included Clean, disinfect or sterilize facilities and equipment, and dispose Visualization—to determine what surfaces were of contaminated items in preparation for next examination touched by patient Embedded information—to ensure that all necessary surfaces have been cleaned Evaluate the need for and use of protective shielding Spatial orientation—of patient Visualization—of relationship between pertinent anatomy and shielding material Mental manipulation—may need to deviate from normal relationships due to pathology or deformity Take appropriate precautions to minimize radiation exposure to Visualization—mentally compare field size and patient pertinent anatomy Mental manipulation—may be needed to alter procedure for patient disability, disease, or deformity Question female patient of child-bearing age about possible Pattern recognition—to evaluate responses to pregnancy and take appropriate action (i.e. document response, questions contact physician) Visualization—to evaluate physical appearance

Restrict beam to limit exposure area, improve image quality, and Visualization—mentally compare field size and reduce radiation dose pertinent anatomy Visualization—to mentally calculate/estimate divergence of primary beam Mental manipulation—may be needed to alter procedure for patient disability, disease, or deformity Set kVp, mA and time or automatic exposure system to achieve Pattern recognition—to evaluate settings of primary optimum image quality, safe operating conditions, and minimum exposure factors radiation dose Prevent all unnecessary persons from remaining in area during Visualization—judge distance from and path of x-ray exposure primary beam to others Take appropriate precautions to minimize occupational radiation Spatial orientation—to judge relationship of self to exposure primary beam Visualization—judge distance from and path of primary beam to others Wear a personnel monitoring device while on duty Evaluate individual occupational exposure reports to determine Pattern recognition—to recognize location and if values for the reporting period are within established limits reporting style of pertinent information continued

276

Table 42 Continued

Prepare and operate radiographic unit and accessories Visualization—of movement and relationships of all a. three-phase generator components of system b. high frequency generator Visualization—depth perception and distance Prepare and operate fluoroscopic unit and accessories estimation; estimation of height; estimation of a. fixed fluoroscopic unit and accessories clearance between equipment and patient b. pulse fluoroscopy Spatial orientation—of self and patient in relationship c. digital fluoroscopy to equipment d. mobile fluoroscopic unit Visualization—of pertinent anatomy in relationship to Prepare and operate specialized units equipment a. chest unit Mental manipulation—to determine if equipment can b. tomography unit or should be placed in a position to image pertinent c. mammography unit anatomy d. bone densitometry unit Pattern recognition—are equipment components e. panorex unit properly placed, aligned, locked in relation to each Prepare and operate digital imaging devices other and patient a. computerized radiography Pattern recognition—is equipment moving b. direct digital radiography appropriately c. picture archival and communication systems (PACS) Pattern recognition—is appropriate information found in visual displays; can image be windowed or leveled to enhance diagnostic accuracy Embedded information—is appropriate information recognized in visual displays; does windowing/leveling hide or disguise pertinent information Visualization—picture distortion caused by angulation of tube/part/image receptor Remove all radiopaque materials from patient or table that could Visualization—picture pertinent anatomy and visible interfere with the radiographic imge or potentially hidden artefacts Select appropriate film/screen combinations Visualization—judge appropriate size/shape of image receptor for pertinent anatomy Select equipment and accessories (e.g. grid, compensating Visualization—of pertinent anatomy filters, shielding) for the examination requested Mental manipulation—to position equipment in proper relationship to patient and central ray Visualization—grid alignment; appropriate distance from patient or tube Use radiopaque markers to indicate anatomical side, position or Visualization—relationship between marker and other relevant information (e.g. time, upright, decubitus, post- pertinent anatomy void) Visualization—will marker be visible on image receptor (e.g. is it within borders of cassette, is it collimated off image) Explain breathing instructions prior to making the exposure Visualization—will part be totally included on image receptor (e.g. expansion of chest on inspiration) Visualization—did patient breathe appropriately prior to exposure continued

277

Table 42 Continued

Position patient to demonstrate the desired anatomy using body Spatial orientation—of self and patient in relation to landmarks equipment Visualization—of pertinent anatomy and bony landmarks Visualization—of disease, deformity Mental manipulation—in order to explain appropriate positioning to patient Mental manipulation—to adjust positioning to unusual circumstances Visualization—of relationship between tube, image receptor and body part Embedded information—recognition of disguised or hidden information (e.g. location of lines, tubes, disabled extremities that might not be readily visible) Pattern recognition—does patient follow instructions Pattern recognition—does automated equipment move into position appropriately Determine appropriate exposure factors using calipers and Mental manipulation—to place calipers in appropriate technique charts position in relationship to pertinent anatomy

Modify exposure factors for circumstances such as involuntary Visualization—to determine if pathology/deformity motion, casts and splints, pathological conditions, or patient’s may have an effect on the image requiring change in inability to cooperate technical factors Visualization—picture extent and/or path of movement Visualization—judge thickness of casts or splints Visualization—of immobilization devices in relation to patient movements Visualization—if and how a holder may be needed Spatial orientation—if holder is needed must be able to position them safely in relation to patient, equipment, and beam (may also require mental manipulation to accomplish this) Process exposed image Visualization—relationship between cassette and Reload cassettes and magazines by selecting film of proper size processing equipment (depth perception, estimation of and type size, distance) Prepare digital/computed image receptor for exposure Pattern recognition—do films match cassettes Embedded information—recognize foreign materials that might superimpose screen Verify accuracy of patient identification on radiograph Pattern recognition—is all appropriate information on image; is all pertinent information free of pertinent anatomy Evaluate radiographs for diagnostic quality Pattern recognition—recognize normal and abnormal anatomic patterns Embedded information—locate and identify pathology and or artifacts Mental manipulation—of anatomic patterns/appearance on cross-sectional images and compare with norms Pattern recognition—is all appropriate identifying information included and not superimposing pertinent anatomy continued

278

Table 42 Continued

Determine corrective measures if radiograph is not of diagnostic Visualization—compare images with standards quality and take appropriate action Visualization—of pathology or deformity Mental manipulation—to determine type and possibility of corrective action Store and handle film/cassette in a manner which will reduce the possibility of artifact production

Recognize and report malfunctions in the radiographic or Visualization—picture normal orientation and fluoroscopic unit and accessories relationships of equipment and its parts Embedded information—identify information which may be disguised (e.g. frayed wires) Pattern recognition—compare technical manual diagrams to equipment Pattern recognition—identify abnormal movement

Perform basic evaluations of radiographic equipment and Pattern recognition—identify common malfunctions accessories Embedded information—identify disguised a. beam restriction system malfunction information b. beam alignment Mental manipulation—evaluate normal and abnormal c. source-to-image receptor distance indicator equipment motion d. radiation protection devices (lead aprons and gloves) Pattern recognition—evaluate technical manuals, graphs, charts Recognize and report malfunctions in processing equipment Pattern recognition—identify common malfunctions a. perform start-up or shutdown procedures on automatic Embedded information—identify disguised processor malfunction information b. darkroom cleanliness Mental manipulation—evaluate normal and abnormal c. daily processor cleaning (e.g. clean rollers, check transport equipment motion system, solutions) Pattern recognition—evaluate technical manuals, d. daily sensitometry graphs, charts Spatial orientation—to remove or replace processor components Perform basic evaluations of processing equipment and Embedded information—identify hidden or disguised accessories information (e.g. locate areas of poor film-screen a. darkroom fog (e.g. safelight, light leak) contact on wire mesh test image; locate dust/debris on b. screen cleanliness screens) c. screen-film contact Visualization—determine size/location of light leaks in darkroom

279