Syncopation Creates the Sensation of Groove in Synthesized Music Examples

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Syncopation Creates the Sensation of Groove in Synthesized Music Examples http://www.diva-portal.org This is the published version of a paper published in Frontiers in Psychology. Citation for the original published paper (version of record): Sioros, G., Miron, M., Davies, M., Gouyon, F., Madison, G. (2014) Syncopation creates the sensation of groove in synthesized music examples. Frontiers in Psychology, 5: 1036 http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01036 Access to the published version may require subscription. N.B. When citing this work, cite the original published paper. Copyright © 2014 Sioros, Miron, Davies, Gouyon and Madison. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. Permanent link to this version: http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:umu:diva-93059 ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 16 September 2014 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01036 Syncopation creates the sensation of groove in synthesized music examples George Sioros 1, Marius Miron 2, Matthew Davies 2, Fabien Gouyon 2 and Guy Madison 3* 1 Department of Informatics Engineering, Faculdade de Engenharia, Universidade do Porto, Porto, Portugal 2 Institute for Systems and Computer Engineering - Technology and Science (INESC-TEC), Porto, Portugal 3 Department of Psychology, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden Edited by: In order to better understand the musical properties which elicit an increased sensation Sofia Dahl, Aalborg University of wanting to move when listening to music—groove—we investigate the effect of Copenhagen, Denmark adding syncopation to simple piano melodies, under the hypothesis that syncopation is Reviewed by: correlated to groove. Across two experiments we examine listeners’ experience of groove Maria Witek, University of Aarhus, Denmark to synthesized musical stimuli covering a range of syncopation levels and densities of Alan M. Wing, University of musical events, according to formal rules implemented by a computer algorithm that shifts Birmingham, UK musical events from strong to weak metrical positions. Results indicate that moderate *Correspondence: levels of syncopation lead to significantly higher groove ratings than melodies without any Guy Madison, Department of syncopation or with maximum possible syncopation. A comparison between the various Psychology, Umeå University, Mediagränd 14 A-123, Umeå, transformations and the way they were rated shows that there is no simple relation SE-901 87, Sweden between syncopation magnitude and groove. e-mail: [email protected] Keywords: groove, syncopation, movement, rhythm, listening experiment, music INTRODUCTION increase groove. The latter was confirmed by a systematic analysis Certain types of music induce the desire in humans to tap their conducted by Davies et al. (2013) on simple rhythms. feet, move their body, and dance. This feeling is commonly known Madison and Sioros (2014) examined how musicians create as “groove” and has a strong affective component, as well as groove when they are confined to a monophonic melody and a strong correlation with music appreciation (Madison, 2006). an isochronous beat. Professional musicians were asked to per- According to Janata et al. (2012), we find pleasurable the music form six simple and six more complex melodies in two different that makes us want to dance and such music elicits spontaneous expressive ways: maximizing and minimizing groove. Their per- rhythmic movements. Furthermore, Madison et al. (2011) show formances were then rated by listeners to examine to what extent that the propensity to move differs substantially from one piece of the manipulations of the musicians created the intended effect. music to another, but can be linked to certain rhythmical proper- The study revealed that musicians tended to shift the onset and ties of the music signal such as beat salience and the density of offset positions of the notes in the simple melodies to faster met- events. rical levels in order to increase groove in their performances, and There is a strong agreement in the previous studies toward that this increased the groove to the same level as the complex the definition of groove. Madison (2006) defined groove as the melodies. By virtue of shifting note onsets and offsets to faster “sensation of wanting to move some part of the body in rela- metrical levels, the musicians created a greater number of off- tion to some aspect of the sound pattern.” Similarly, Janata et al. beat events, thus, increasing the syncopation. When they tried to (2012) conducted a survey involving a wide variety of music minimize groove they were unable to decrease it below the dead- related terms, to find that “groove is that aspect of the music pan level for the simple performances, but the dead-pan level that induces a pleasant sense of wanting to move along with the of groove in the complex melodies could be reduced to that of music.” Furthermore, there are strong clues that link certain prop- the simple melodies. This seemed to be achieved by two differ- erties of the music with the induced groove. Janata et al. (2012) ent means. One was to simplify the rhythmic structure toward conducted an extensive study on sensorimotor coupling which higher metrical levels, i.e., from 16th and 8th-notes to 8th, 4th, provides a broad analysis under the different facets of groove. The and half-notes, and the other was to break the rhythm such that computational part of the analysis suggests that music genre and the sense of a regular beat disappeared. Both of those devices faster tempi have a strong effect on the desire to move along to result in decreased syncopation but each in a different way. The the music. In comparison, Madison et al. (2011) claim that tempo first simply eliminates the syncopating notes by placing them on alone cannot explain groove. Consequently, Madison et al. (2011) the beat. In the second, syncopation disappears together with the found that low level descriptors, such as beat salience, event den- metrical feel. sity and fast metrical levels as physical attributes of the sound, While the results from Madison and Sioros (2014) appear to highly correlate with groove. In contrast and unlike what musico- indicate that the main physical correlate of groove was synco- logical literature points toward (Keil, 1995), microtiming does not pation, the design of the experiments was not fully controlled, www.frontiersin.org September 2014 | Volume 5 | Article 1036 | 1 Sioros et al. Syncopation creates groove since the musicians could, and did, change several aspects of their changing metrical positions only, without altering other expres- performances simultaneously. It is therefore difficult to determine sive or structural characteristics. The algorithm is an adaptation if the changes in some performance parameters were redundant, of the general syncopation transformations developed by Sioros independent, or interdependent, and also the unique effect of et al. (2013). While in the recent Witek study (2014)syncopa- each one of them. Therefore, there is a need to experimentally tion was measured in pre-existing drum loops, in our study we test the hypothesis that syncopation per se induces the sensation can generate and vary it in piano melodies using an automatic of groove independently of other expressive or structural features algorithm over which we have complete control. of the performances. We designed two listener experiments using simple piano Syncopation as a cognitive mechanism has been described as a melodies that were transformed in an automatic and controlled form of violation of metrical expectations (Huron, 2006, p. 297), way in order to generate syncopation without affecting other that is, temporal expectations that are established in the listener’s rhythmic or structural characteristics. In the first experiment we mind as a function of the temporal regularities in the music sig- explore the more general hypothesis that increasing the synco- nal. Several operational definitions of syncopation are based on pation to a moderate amount and introducing faster metrical the more general expectation principle, and provide algorithms levels results in an increased groove sensation. Then, in the sec- for measuring the amount of syncopation present in a rhyth- ond experiment, we aim to gain more insight on how the strength mic pattern (Longuet-Higgins and Lee, 1984; Gómez et al., 2005; of each individual syncopation felt in a melody influences groove Huron and Ommen, 2006; Sioros and Guedes, 2011). A common as well as distinguishing between different ways of introducing characteristic among many syncopation measures is the use of a faster metrical levels. Parallel to the syncopation transformations, metrical template that describes the strong and weak beats found density transformations that increase the number of events in in the musical meter. Normally, a note articulated in a weak met- the melodies are used as controls in order to verify the effect of rical position is followed by a note in the following strong position syncopation. resulting in a weak-strong rhythmic figure. Huron (2006, p. 295) Basedonrecentresultsonthistopic(Madison and Sioros, suggests that when the expected note on the strong position does 2014; Witek et al., 2014), our predictions for the outcome of not occur, the tension increases and the feeling of syncopation the listening experiments are: (1) the deadpan versions of the is evoked. The syncopation is then attributed to the “improper melodies that contain no syncopation would be rated lowest in binding” of a musical event found on a weak metrical position groove, (2) introducing a small number of strongly syncopating to an event in the following strong position, which is delayed or notes would be rated highest in groove, (3) an increased num- not present at all (see also London, 2012, p.
Recommended publications
  • Polyrhythmic Tapping: Examining the Effectiveness of the Strategy of Organizing Rhythmic Structures Through Synthesis
    ISSN 1303-0485 • eISSN 2148-7561 DOI 10.12738/estp.2015.1.1917 Received | 27 January 2013 Copyright © 2015 EDAM • http://www.estp.com.tr Accepted | 30 September 2014 Educational Sciences: Theory & Practice • 2015 February • 15(1) • 239-252 OnlineFirst | 20 February 2015 Polyrhythmic Tapping: Examining the Effectiveness of the Strategy of Organizing Rhythmic Structures through Synthesis Hamit Yokusa Mugla Sitki Kocman University Tuba Yokusb Mugla Sitki Kocman University Abstract In this study the strategy of organizing rhythmic structures through synthesis is named, and defined, and its procedures are described. Its effectiveness for teaching the execution of 3:2, 4:3, 8:3, 5:4, and 3:5 polyrhythmic structures is examined and described. Pre-test and Post-test Control Group Design was employed to test the effectiveness of the strategy of organizing rhythmic structures through synthesis. The participants of the study were undergraduate students (N = 18) in their second year of study at the Gaziosmanpaşa University Education Faculty’s Music Education Department in the fall semester of the 2012-2013 academic year. The Polyrhythmic Tapping Test was used as a measuring instrument. Two tests were employed in this study. The Wilcoxon Signed- Rank Test was employed to analyze the pre-test and post-test data from both the experimental and control groups. The Mann Whitney U Test was used as a one-sided test to determine the variances (for the experimental and control groups) before the experiment and to determine the variances after the experiment. The results confirm that organizing rhythmic structures through synthesis is an effective way to learn how to perform 3:2, 4:3, 8:3, 5:4, and 3:5 polyrhythmic structures.
    [Show full text]
  • Alisa Weilerstein, Cello Johann Sebastian Bach (1685–1750) E Six Suites for Unaccompanied Cello
    Wednesday, May 1, 2019, 8pm First Congregational Church, Berkeley Alisa Weilerstein, cello Johann Sebastian Bach (1685–1750) e Six Suites for Unaccompanied Cello Suite No. 1 in G major, BWV 1007 Prélude Allemande Courante Sarabande Minuet Minuet II Gigue Suite No. 2 in D minor, BWV 1008 Prélude Allemande Courante Sarabande Minuet Minuet II Gigue PAUSE This performance is made possible, in part, by Patron Sponsor Patrick McCabe. Cal Performances’ 2018 –19 season is sponsored by Wells Fargo. 15 Suite No. 3 in C major, BWV 1009 Prélude Allemande Courante Sarabande Bourrée Bourrée Gigue Suite No. 4 in E-flat major, BWV 1010 Prélude Allemande Courante Sarabande Bourrée Bourrée Gigue INTERMISSION Suite No. 5 in C minor, BWV 1011 Prélude Allemande Courante Sarabande Gavotte Gavotte Gigue Suite No. 6 in D major, BWV 1012 Prélude Allemande Courante Sarabande Gavotte Gavotte Gigue Opposite: Photo by Paul Stuart. 16 PROGRAM NOTES A Note from the Artist Johann Sebastian Bach In 1888, a 12-year-old boy in the provinces of e Six Suites for Unaccompanied Cello, Catalonia discovered something on a dusty shelf BWV 1007–1012 of an old music store: a tattered score of the Six In 1713, the frugal Friedrich Wilhelm I of Suites for Vio lon cello Solo by Johann Sebastian Prussia dismissed his household musical estab - Bach. He couldn’t believe what he had found, lishment in Berlin. e young, cultured Prince and immediately recognized it as an invaluable Leopold of Anhalt-Cöthen, 40 miles north of treasure. He bought the score and took it home Leipzig, took the opportunity to engage some with him to practice.
    [Show full text]
  • Ludwig Van Beethoven a Brilliant Pianist, but When Born: December 16, 1770 He Was Around 30 Years Old Died: March 26, 1827 Beethoven Began Going Deaf
    SymphonySecond No. Movement 8 in F Major Ludwig van Beethoven a brilliant pianist, but when Born: December 16, 1770 he was around 30 years old Died: March 26, 1827 Beethoven began going deaf. Even though he could no Ludwig van Beethoven was longer hear well enough to born in Bonn, Germany. His play the piano, Beethoven father, who was a singer, composed some of his best was his first teacher. After a music after he lost his while, even though he was hearing! still only a boy, Ludwig became a traveling Beethoven is considered performer, and soon he was one of the greatest musical supporting his family. geniuses who ever lived. He may be most famous for his In his early twenties nine symphonies, but he also Beethoven moved to Vienna, wrote many other kinds of where he spent the rest of music: chamber and choral his life. Beethoven was one pieces, piano works, string of the first composers to quartets, and an opera. make a living without being employed by the church or a member of the nobility. At first, he was known as Beethoven’s Music Listen to the second movement of Beethoven’s 8th Symphony, then answer the questions below. 1. How many “ticks” do you hear before the melody begins? a. 2 b. 5 c. 7 d. 8 2. What instrument plays the melody first? a. violin b. viola c. cello d. bass 3. Does the orchestra get loud suddenly? a. yes b. no 4. Does the music sound like it’s jumping around the orchestra? a.
    [Show full text]
  • TIME SIGNATURES, TEMPO, BEAT and GORDONIAN SYLLABLES EXPLAINED
    TIME SIGNATURES, TEMPO, BEAT and GORDONIAN SYLLABLES EXPLAINED TIME SIGNATURES Time Signatures are represented by a fraction. The top number tells the performer how many beats in each measure. This number can be any number from 1 to infinity. However, time signatures, for us, will rarely have a top number larger than 7. The bottom number can only be the numbers 1, 2, 4, 8, 16, 32, 64, 128, 256, 512, et c. These numbers represent the note values of a whole note, half note, quarter note, eighth note, sixteenth note, thirty- second note, sixty-fourth note, one hundred twenty-eighth note, two hundred fifty-sixth note, five hundred twelfth note, et c. However, time signatures, for us, will only have a bottom numbers 2, 4, 8, 16, and possibly 32. Examples of Time Signatures: TEMPO Tempo is the speed at which the beats happen. The tempo can remain steady from the first beat to the last beat of a piece of music or it can speed up or slow down within a section, a phrase, or a measure of music. Performers need to watch the conductor for any changes in the tempo. Tempo is the Italian word for “time.” Below are terms that refer to the tempo and metronome settings for each term. BPM is short for Beats Per Minute. This number is what one would set the metronome. Please note that these numbers are generalities and should never be considered as strict ranges. Time Signatures, music genres, instrumentations, and a host of other considerations may make a tempo of Grave a little faster or slower than as listed below.
    [Show full text]
  • A History of Rhythm, Metronomes, and the Mechanization of Musicality
    THE METRONOMIC PERFORMANCE PRACTICE: A HISTORY OF RHYTHM, METRONOMES, AND THE MECHANIZATION OF MUSICALITY by ALEXANDER EVAN BONUS A DISSERTATION Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Music CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY May, 2010 CASE WESTERN RESERVE UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF GRADUATE STUDIES We hereby approve the thesis/dissertation of _____________________________________________________Alexander Evan Bonus candidate for the ______________________Doctor of Philosophy degree *. Dr. Mary Davis (signed)_______________________________________________ (chair of the committee) Dr. Daniel Goldmark ________________________________________________ Dr. Peter Bennett ________________________________________________ Dr. Martha Woodmansee ________________________________________________ ________________________________________________ ________________________________________________ (date) _______________________2/25/2010 *We also certify that written approval has been obtained for any proprietary material contained therein. Copyright © 2010 by Alexander Evan Bonus All rights reserved CONTENTS LIST OF FIGURES . ii LIST OF TABLES . v Preface . vi ABSTRACT . xviii Chapter I. THE HUMANITY OF MUSICAL TIME, THE INSUFFICIENCIES OF RHYTHMICAL NOTATION, AND THE FAILURE OF CLOCKWORK METRONOMES, CIRCA 1600-1900 . 1 II. MAELZEL’S MACHINES: A RECEPTION HISTORY OF MAELZEL, HIS MECHANICAL CULTURE, AND THE METRONOME . .112 III. THE SCIENTIFIC METRONOME . 180 IV. METRONOMIC RHYTHM, THE CHRONOGRAPHIC
    [Show full text]
  • Program Notes
    Program Notes for kids Ludwig & The Kings Saturday, October 21, 2017 8:00 p.m. Michigan Theater Handel Excerpts from Solomon Bloch Schelomo: Hebraic Rhapsody Intermission Ellington King Solomon from Three Black Kings Beethoven Symphony No. 8 The Kings Three of the pieces on this concert are about King Solomon. The biblical King Solomon was known for being wise, wealthy and a great writer. As you listen to these three pieces, what do you no- tice about each composer’s portrayl of King Solomon? Are they similiar or different? Would you have guessed from listening that all three pieces are about the same person? Solomon by George Frideric Handel About the Music The Baby Story What kind of piece is it? Solomon’s most famous Solomon is an Oratorio, like Handel’s famous Messiah. An oratorio is like act: Two people came to an opera in that it tells a story using singers and an orchestra. But unlike the King, each claiming a an opera, there are no sets or costumes. And the subject of an oratorio is baby as their own. King always religious. Solomon suggested that they cut the baby in half What is it about? and each “parent” gets It’s about King Solomon from the Bible. It celebrates the king’s wisdom half a baby! The wise king (see the Baby story), his accomplishments (building the Temple in Jerusa- knew that the real parent lem), and his love for his wife. But, really it’s about King George II who, would never agree to that during Handel’s lifetime, wisely led a prosperous and happy England.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Practice Strategies, Metronome Use, Meter, Hand, And
    Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 2004 Effects of practice strategies, metronome use, meter, hand, and musical function on dual-staved piano performance accuracy and practice time usage of undergraduate Melody A. Hanberry Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_dissertations Part of the Music Commons Recommended Citation Hanberry, Melody A., "Effects of practice strategies, metronome use, meter, hand, and musical function on dual-staved piano performance accuracy and practice time usage of undergraduate" (2004). LSU Doctoral Dissertations. 1187. https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_dissertations/1187 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in LSU Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized graduate school editor of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please [email protected]. EFFECTS OF PRACTICE STRATEGIES, METRONOME USE, METER, HAND, AND MUSICAL FUNCTION ON DUAL-STAVED PIANO PERFORMANCE ACCURACY AND PRACTICE TIME USAGE OF UNDERGRADUATE NON-KEYBOARD MUSIC MAJORS A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in The School of Music by Melody A. Hanberry B.M., William Carey College, 1998 M.M., Louisiana State University, 2000 May 2004 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS To the many wonderful people who have contributed to the completion of this dissertation, thank you. For the encouragement, support, help, professional opinions, and availability of those who served as members of my committee, both past and present, you are regarded with utmost appreciation and respect.
    [Show full text]
  • Digital Piano
    Address KORG ITALY Spa Via Cagiata, 85 I-60027 Osimo (An) Italy Web servers www.korgpa.com www.korg.co.jp www.korg.com www.korg.co.uk www.korgcanada.com www.korgfr.net www.korg.de www.korg.it www.letusa.es DIGITAL PIANO ENGLISH MAN0010006 © KORG Italy 2006. All rights reserved PART NUMBER: MAN0010006 E 2 User’s Manual User’s C720_English.fm Page 1 Tuesday, October 10, 2006 4:14 PM IMPORTANT SAFETY INSTRUCTIONS The lightning flash with arrowhead symbol within an equilateral triangle, is intended to alert the user to the presence of uninsulated • Read these instructions. “dangerous voltage” within the product’s enclosure that may be of sufficient magni- • Keep these instructions. tude to constitute a risk of electric shock to • Heed all warnings. persons. • Follow all instructions. • Do not use this apparatus near water. The exclamation point within an equilateral • Mains powered apparatus shall not be exposed to dripping or triangle is intended to alert the user to the splashing and that no objects filled with liquids, such as vases, presence of important operating and mainte- shall be placed on the apparatus. nance (servicing) instructions in the literature accompanying the product. • Clean only with dry cloth. • Do not block any ventilation openings, install in accordance with the manufacturer’s instructions. • Do not install near any heat sources such as radiators, heat reg- THE FCC REGULATION WARNING (FOR U.S.A.) isters, stoves, or other apparatus (including amplifiers) that pro- duce heat. This equipment has been tested and found to comply with the limits for a Class B digital device, pursuant to Part 15 of the FCC Rules.
    [Show full text]
  • The Coordinative Structure of Polyrhythmic Performance and Korte’S Third Law
    The Coordinative Structure of Polyrhythmic Performance and Korte’s Third Law THESIS Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Emanuele Rizzi Graduate Program in Psychology The Ohio State University 2015 Master's Examination Committee: Dr. Richard Jagacinski, Advisor Dr. Alex Petrov Dr. Steven Lavender Copyrighted by Emanuele Rizzi 2015 Abstract Previous research examining the performance of bimanual polyrhythmic tapping has utilized temporal covariance analysis to determine the interdependence of limbs being coordinated. The prevailing finding is that the perceptual-motor system couples the actions of the left and right hand into a dependent or integrated unit, with one notable exception in a study by Krampe et al. (2000). This study found that concert level pianists performing a 4:3 bimanual polyrhythm could exhibit relatively independent (i.e. parallel) performance between the two hands. In other coordination experiments parallel performance between the upper and lower body was achieved fairly regularly in the timing structure of moderately skilled golfers during their swing (Jagacinski et al., 2011). Combining aspects of these previous works, we tested the effects of speed on the coordinative structure of skilled drummers’ hands and right foot while performing a 4:3:2 polyrhythm. All participants showed parallel performance between one or more limb pairs in both 3-limb polyrhythmic conditions and bimanual polyrhythmic conditions. Given that participants were coordinating both of their hands and their foot, they exhibited coordinative structures that mixed both integration and parallelism between limb pairs. Faster performance speed resulted in more parallel performance as well as more variable performance.
    [Show full text]
  • Numbers and Tempo: 1630-1800 Beverly Jerold
    Performance Practice Review Volume 17 | Number 1 Article 4 Numbers and Tempo: 1630-1800 Beverly Jerold Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarship.claremont.edu/ppr Part of the Music Practice Commons Jerold, Beverly (2012) "Numbers and Tempo: 1630-1800," Performance Practice Review: Vol. 17: No. 1, Article 4. DOI: 10.5642/ perfpr.201217.01.04 Available at: http://scholarship.claremont.edu/ppr/vol17/iss1/4 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Journals at Claremont at Scholarship @ Claremont. It has been accepted for inclusion in Performance Practice Review by an authorized administrator of Scholarship @ Claremont. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Numbers and Tempo: 1630-1800 Beverly Jerold Copyright © 2012 Claremont Graduate University Ever since antiquity, the human species has been drawn to numbers. In music, for example, numbers seem to be tangible when compared to the language in early musical texts, which may have a different meaning for us than it did for them. But numbers, too, may be misleading. For measuring time, we have electronic metronomes and scientific instruments of great precision, but in the time frame 1630-1800 a few scientists had pendulums, while the wealthy owned watches and clocks of varying accuracy. Their standards were not our standards, for they lacked the advantages of our technology. How could they have achieved the extremely rapid tempos that many today have attributed to them? Before discussing the numbers in sources thought to support these tempos, let us consider three factors related to technology: 1) The incalculable value of the unconscious training in every aspect of music that we gain from recordings.
    [Show full text]
  • Mid-Semester Report
    BME 200/300: Biomedical Engineering Design (Fall 2009) Project #6: Physiologic Metronome Mid-Semester Report October 21, 2009 Prepared by: Ben Fleming (Communicator) Cole Drifka (BWIG) Jeff Theisen (BSAC) Adam Pala (Team Leader) Client(s): Dr. Bill Fahl, Department of Oncology (University of Wisconsin-Madison) Vanessa Young, Undergraduate Music Student (Carnegie Mellon University) Advisor: Dr. Paul Thompson, Department of Biomedical Engineering (University of Wisconsin-Madison) 2 Table of Contents Problem Statement 3 Background Information 3 Design Motivation 4 Client’s Requirements and Design Constraints 4 Human Factors and Ergonomics 4 Design Alternatives 5 Permanent Features of the Design 5 Type of Tactile Stimulus 6 Location of the Tactile Stimulus 9 Mode of Attachment of the Tactile Stimulus 13 User Input and Output 15 Proposed Design 17 Future Work 18 Ethical Considerations 18 References 18 Appendix 19 Product Design Specifications (PDS) 19 3 Problem Statement The purpose of this design project is to develop a metronome device which will maintain a constant, adjustable tempo for the practicing musician. A key feature that the client requires is that the device’s tempo-maintaining mechanism be inaudible. Such a feature allows the musician to practice and improve musical performance using a more intuitive approach—one that does not distract the musician while playing music (i.e. audible ticks, as used in conventional metronomes). Background Information Metronomes are defined as any device that provides a regular beat [1]. In most cases, metronomes are used to provide timing or rhythm to musicians. Metronomes use visual, auditory, and tactile stimuli to provide the musician a set tempo.
    [Show full text]
  • Optimizing Gait Outcomes in Parkinson's Disease with Auditory Cues: the Effects of Synchronization, Groove, and Beat Perception Ability
    Western University Scholarship@Western Electronic Thesis and Dissertation Repository 7-30-2019 2:30 PM Optimizing Gait Outcomes in Parkinson's Disease with Auditory Cues: The Effects of Synchronization, Groove, and Beat Perception Ability Emily A. Ready The University of Western Ontario Supervisor Grahn, Jessica A. The University of Western Ontario Joint Supervisor Holmes, Jeffrey D. The University of Western Ontario Graduate Program in Health and Rehabilitation Sciences A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the equirr ements for the degree in Doctor of Philosophy © Emily A. Ready 2019 Follow this and additional works at: https://ir.lib.uwo.ca/etd Part of the Occupational Therapy Commons, Physical Therapy Commons, Physiotherapy Commons, and the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Ready, Emily A., "Optimizing Gait Outcomes in Parkinson's Disease with Auditory Cues: The Effects of Synchronization, Groove, and Beat Perception Ability" (2019). Electronic Thesis and Dissertation Repository. 6443. https://ir.lib.uwo.ca/etd/6443 This Dissertation/Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Scholarship@Western. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Thesis and Dissertation Repository by an authorized administrator of Scholarship@Western. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Abstract This dissertation explores a common, rehabilitative strategy for mitigating gait impairments in Parkinson’s disease (PD) called Rhythmic Auditory Stimulation (RAS). The effects of this intervention on gait in PD are well documented but highly variable, which poses difficulty for appropriate therapeutic application. Part of this variability may be related to individual musical abilities, such as beat perception accuracy, as most RAS interventions involve synchronizing with a beat.
    [Show full text]