<<

vol. 179, no. 2 the american naturalist february 2012

Haplodiploidy and the of : Split Ratios

Andy Gardner,1,2,* Joa˜o Alpedrinha,1,3 and Stuart A. West1

1. Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PS, United Kingdom; 2. Balliol College, University of Oxford, Broad Street, Oxford OX1 3BJ, United Kingdom; 3. Instituto Gulbenkian de Cieˆncia, Apartado 14, PT-2781-901 Oeiras, Portugal Submitted June 29, 2011; Accepted October 20, 2011; Electronically published December 21, 2011

the rearing of an extra sister to provisioning a cell for a daugh- abstract: It is generally accepted that from a theoretical perspec- ter of her own. (Hamilton 1964, p. 28–29) tive, should facilitate the . How- ever, the “haplodiploidy hypothesis” rests on theoretical arguments The eusocial societies are dominated by species with hap- that were made before recent advances in our empirical understand- lodiploid genetics, especially the social —the ing of sex allocation and the route by which eusociality evolved. Here , , and . Although eusociality is also found we show that several possible promoters of the haplodiploidy effect would have been unimportant on the route to eusociality, because in diploid species, such as , its distribution is sig- they involve traits that evolved only after eusociality had become nificantly biased toward haplodiploid families (Crozier established. We then focus on two biological mechanisms that could 2008). Hamilton (1964, 1972) suggested that this was be- have played a role: split sex ratios as a result of either queen virginity cause haplodiploidy facilitates the evolution of altruistic or queen replacement. We find that these mechanisms can lead hap- helping. Altruistic helping behaviors are favored if the ben- lodiploidy to facilitating the evolution of helping but that their im- efit of helping relatives outweighs the costs to the altruist portance varies from appreciable to negligible, depending on the and to other relatives, with all costs and benefits weighted assumptions. Furthermore, under certain conditions, haplodiploidy by the genetic relatedness of the recipients to the actor can even inhibit the evolution of helping. In contrast, we find that the level of promiscuity has a strong and consistently negative in- (Hamilton 1963, 1964, 1970). Haplodiploidy involves fe- fluence on selection for helping. Consequently, from a relatedness males developing from fertilized (i.e., diploid) and perspective, monogamy is likely to have been a more important driver having both a mother and a father, and males developing of eusociality than the haplodiploidy effect. from unfertilized (i.e., haploid) eggs, and having no father. Hamilton (1964, 1972) suggested that because this leads Keywords: , helping, inclusive fitness, , monog- to a worker being more related to her full sisters (-for- p amy, sex allocation. life relatedness,R 3/4 ) than to her own daughters (R p 1/2 ), haplodiploidy makes eusociality easier to evolve, even in the absence of efficiency benefits to Introduction cooperation. Trivers and Hare (1976) showed that while Hamilton’s If a is fertilized by only one male all the she “haplodiploidy hypothesis” can work, things are not so simple. Haplodiploidy also leads to a female being less receives is genetically identical. Thus, although the relationship related to her brothers (p ) than to her sons of a mother to her daughters has the normal value of 1/2, the R 1/4 (p ). In the simplest case, with an unbiased sex ratio relationship between daughters is 3/4. Consider a species where R 1/2 the female consecutively provisions and oviposits in cell after among reproductives, the relative benefit of rearing sisters is exactly canceled by the relative cost of rearing brothers, cell so that she is still at work when the first of her female and so haplodiploidy has no overall influence (Trivers and offspring ecloses, leaves the nest and mates. Our principle tells Hare 1976). A female-biased sex ratio does not solve this us that even if this new adult had a nest ready constructed problem, because the benefit of more sisters is exactly and vacant for her use she would prefer, other things being counterbalanced by the fact that it increases the average equal, returning to her mother’s and provisioning a cell for number of mates for each male, making worth * Corresponding author; e-mail: [email protected] relatively less (Trivers and Hare 1976; Craig 1979). Con- Am. Nat. 2012. Vol. 179, pp. 000–000. 2011 by The University of Chicago. sequently, in order to make the haplodiploidy hypothesis 0003-0147/2012/17902-53146$15.00. All rights reserved. work, it is required that workers preferentially help sisters DOI: 10.1086/663683 but that the population sex ratio is not biased to the same 000 The American Naturalist

Table 1: Split sex ratios and the evolution of eusociality Empirical evidence that this has Could such split sex ratios have occurred on Reason for split sex ratios led to split sex ratios the route to eusociality? Partially overlapping generations (Seger No (West 2009) Potentially 1983) Worker control of sex allocation in some No, but would be transient Potentially (to be analyzed in a companion broods and queen control in others (Triv- (West 2009) paper: J. Alpedrinha et al. unpublished ers and Hare 1976) manuscript) Variation across colonies in the relative cost No (West 2009) Potentially of producing males and females (Grafen 1986) Virginity (or any other factor which con- Yes, this occurs in solitary and Yes strains some queens to produce only social hymenopteran species, males; Taylor 1981; Godfray and Grafen although usually at low 1988) (!6%) frequencies (Godfray and Hardy 1992; West 2009 Competition for mates between related males No (West 2009) Potentially, although LMC is very rare in so- (local mate competition, LMC; Frank cial Hymenoptera (West et al. 2005) 1987) Synergistic benefits of rearing siblings (Frank No (West 2009) Potentially and Crespi 1989) Variation in response to whether sibmated or No (West 2009) No evidence for such sex ratio shifts in any not (Greeff 1996; Reece et al. 2004) organism, and sibmating is rare in social Hymenoptera Competition between related females for re- Yes, in ants (West 2009) No, this occurs in multiple-queen colonies, sources (local resource competition; which only evolved after obligate eusocial- Brown and Keller 2000) ity was established (Boomsma 2007, 2009; Hughes et al. 2008) Queen control (Passera et al. 2001) Yes, in ants and bees (West No, this occurs only in obligately eusocial 2009) colonies Relatedness asymmetry due to variation in Yes, in ants (Chapuisat and Kel- No, multiple only evolved after obli- queen mating frequency (Boomsma and ler 1999; Meunier et al. 2008; gate eusociality was established (Boomsma Grafen 1991) West 2009) 2007, 2009; Hughes et al. 2008) Relatedness asymmetry due to variation in Yes, in ants and wasps (Cha- No, this occurs in multiple-queen colonies, queen number (Boomsma and Grafen puisat and Keller 1999; Meu- whereas eusociality has only arisen in mo- 1991; Boomsma 1993) nier et al. 2008; West 2009) nogynous species (Boomsma 2007, 2009; Hughes et al. 2008). Relatedness asymmetry due to queen re- Yes, in bees (Chapuisat and Kel- Yes placement (Boomsma 1991) ler 1999; Meunier et al. 2008; West 2009) Note: Although there are many mechanisms that lead to split sex ratios in hymenoptera, only two of these (virginity and queen replacement) are supported by empirical evidence (column 2) and are also likely to have occurred during the transition from solitary to eusocial living (column 3). extent. Trivers and Hare (1976) suggested that this could facilitates the spread of genes that lead to more specialized happen if workers at some nests gained control of sex helping en route to eusociality. allocation and biased this toward sisters. Such sex ratio Since Trivers and Hare’s (1976) landmark article, a large variation between broods, termed “split sex ratios,” can body of theoretical work has arisen showing that split sex allow helpers at the relatively female-biased broods to gain ratios can be favored in response to a multitude of selective the relatedness benefit of rearing sisters, without this being forces, with many of these scenarios being supported by exactly canceled by a reduced reproductive value of fe- the empirical data (table 1). Indeed, work on split sex ratios males, thanks to the relatively male-biased broods leading has even been hailed as one of the most successful and to a more even sex ratio at the population level (Seger productive areas of , with a rich in- 1983; Grafen 1986). The idea here is that this favors the terplay between theoretical, observational, and experi- initial evolution of helping at some broods and hence mental studies (West 2009). Furthermore, subsequent Haplodiploidy and Eusociality 000 work has suggested other ways that might allow haplo- is able to assess the sex ratio of the brood that she would diploidy to facilitate the evolution of helping, via its impact help to rear. Another issue is whether we are examining on relatedness, for example, by selecting for the enforce- the initial evolution (origin) of helping, or the subsequent ment of cooperation with (Ratnieks 1988) elaboration (maintenance) of helping (Charnov 1978). In or by increasing the benefit of helping siblings in struc- the former, a potential worker must decide whether to stay tured populations (Lehmann et al. 2008; Johnstone et al. at her mother’s nest and rear siblings or disperse away to 2011). Overall, this body of work has led to the general found her own nest, whereas in the latter, a potential assumption that from a theoretical perspective, the hap- worker must decide how much to invest in rearing the lodiploidy effect does facilitate the evolution of eusociality queen’s offspring versus selfishly pursuing her own re- (Seger 1991; Krebs and Davies 1993; Bourke and Franks production within the same nest. 1995; Crozier and Pamilo 1996; Queller and Strassmann Our aim in this article is to determine, from a theo- 1998; Alcock 2005). retical perspective, the extent to which the haplodiploidy Here, we reassess the haplodiploidy hypothesis on two effect can facilitate the evolution of eusociality. In order grounds. First, empirical progress has clarified the relevant to provide an illustrative overview, we first consider a biological scenarios. Eusociality has evolved under specific population divided into colonies that may vary in their conditions, with strict lifetime monogamy via the subsocial sex allocation but not in any other respect. We then con- route, where offspring stay at home to help their parents, struct models for two specific scenarios that the empirical and with single queens (Boomsma 2007, 2009; Hughes et data suggest could have played a role on the route to al. 2008; Duffy and Macdonald 2010; Bourke 2011). This eusociality: queen virginity and queen replacement (table means that several factors cannot have facilitated the evo- 1). Our aim with these models is not just to show whether lution of eusociality, including: (a) the most empirically haplodiploidy and split sex ratios can facilitate the evo- common causes of split sex ratios, which rely on variation lution of eusociality but also to parameterize the models in the number of queens and queen mating frequency and with empirical data and hence quantify their possible therefore evolved after eusociality was established (table importance. Although it seems reasonable that both 1); (b) worker policing in response to multiple mating mechanisms could facilitate the evolution of helping, we (Ratnieks 1988); and (c) any mechanism that relies on do not know whether the effect is large or negligible. helping siblings to rear offspring (the semisocial route; Quantification requires specific models because factors Lehmann et al 2008; Johnstone et al. 2011). Furthermore, such as queen replacement that lead to split sex ratios several other suggested mechanisms for producing split can cause variation in the relatedness structure within sex ratios are unlikely to be important, such as in response colonies, which may also mediate selection for helping. to sib mating or partially overlapping generations, as a For each of the situations that we consider, we examine recent overview of the literature has shown that there is (when relevant) how the impact of haplodiploidy varies lack of empirical evidence that they occur (West 2009). along the route to eusociality (i.e., initial evolution versus Consequently, if the haplodipoidy effect has facilitated the later elaboration of helping), and for different types of evolution of eusociality, it has done so via split sex ratios, trait (facultative vs. obligate helping). We consider a third and the potentially important causes of split sex ratios are possible cause of split sex ratios, Trivers and Hare’s limited to queen virginity, queen replacement, and partial (1976) idea of partial worker control of sex allocation, worker control (table 1). alongside the role of male production by workers, in a Second, haplodiploidy can have different influences at separate article (J. Alpedrinha et al., unpublished man- different stages of the evolution of eusociality and for dif- uscript), because it leads to only transient split sex ratios ferent types of helping trait. One issue is whether we are and hence is not amenable to the equilibrium analysis considering facultative helping at a fraction of broods or approach that we take in this study. obligate helping in all broods. Previous split sex ratio ar- guments have been relatively heuristic, showing how se- Split Sex Ratios and the Evolution of Helping lection for facultative helping can be increased at relatively female-biased broods. However, at relatively male-biased We examine how haplodiploidy influences selection for broods, potential helpers will be less related to the young helping when the sex ratio (proportion of reproductives that they could help to raise, which disfavors helping. that are male) may vary between broods (Grafen 1986). While this will not matter for facultative helping traits, We perform an inclusive fitness analysis (Hamilton 1963, which are only expressed at female-biased broods, it will 1964, 1970, 1972), weighing the b extra siblings that the have to taken into account when considering obligate help- female could rear if she were more helpful against the c ing traits, such as those committing the individual to per- extra offspring that she could rear if she were less helpful, manent sterility relatively early in her life and before she with the valuation of each relative being given by the prod- 000 The American Naturalist

Table 2: A summary of model notation used in the main text Symbol Definition a Potential for helping

aOBL Potential for obligate helping

aFAC Potential for facultative helping

vf Reproductive value of a juvenile female

vm Reproductive value of a juvenile male

pd Consanguinity of a female to her daughter

ps Consanguinity of a female to her son

pf Consanguinity of a female to a queen-derived juvenile female

pm Consanguinity of a female to a queen-derived juvenile male f Degree of monogamy (probability that maternal sisters are paternal sisters) a Relative productivity of workerless colonies (virginity model) u Frequency of unmated queens (virginity model) q Frequency of queenright colonies (queen replacement model) z Colony sex ratio ¯z Population sex ratio

¯zM Sex ratio of mated-queen colonies (virginity model)

¯zU Sex ratio of unmated-queen colonies (virginity model)

¯zR Sex ratio of queenright colonies (queen replacement model)

¯zL Sex ratio of queenless colonies (queen replacement model)

uct of its reproductive valuev and its consanguinity p to to her own sons and daughters, respectively; and vf and the focal female (app. A; Hamilton 1964, 1970; Taylor and vm are the reproductive values of a female and a male, Frank 1996; Frank 1998; Rousset 2004; Grafen 2006a, respectively. If the worker is trading b of the queen’s off- 2006b). We compute the threshold ratio c/b, below which spring against c of her own offspring, then she increases helping is favored and above which helping is disfavored, her inclusive fitness by helping to rear the queen’s offspring ϩ Ϫ 1 ϩ Ϫ and denote this a. This is the “efficiency ratio” of Charnov ifb[zvmm p (1 z)vp ff] c[¯¯zv ms p (1 z) vp fd] . Rear- (1978) and Grafen (1986), and the “potential for altruism” ranging this condition into the formc/b ! a obtains the of Gardner (2010). A higher value of a corresponds to a potential for helping a, and this is given by dividing the scenario where helping is more readily favored. In partic- valuation of the queen’s offspring by the valuation of the ular,a 1 1 indicates that helping is more readily favored worker’s own offspring. under the given scenario than it is under the assumption If the focal female knows the sex ratio of her mother’s of diploidy with full monogamy (Grafen 1986). Model brood and adjusts her helping accordingly, we obtain a p ϩ notation is summarized in table 2. potential for facultative helping of aFAC[zv m p m Ϫ ϩ Ϫ (1 z)vpff]/[¯¯zv ms p (1 z)vp fd]. Substituting in the ap- propriate reproductive values and consanguinity coeffi- Origin of Helping cients (app. B), this is We assume an otherwise solitary species, and examine the inclusive fitness consequences for a female (the “worker”) 11Ϫ zz a p (1 ϩ 2f) ϩ ,(1) who chooses to stay with her mother (the “queen”) and FAC 41[]Ϫ ¯¯zz rear siblings rather than dispersing and rearing her own brood in her own nest. We allow the sex ratio z of the where f is probability that two given maternal sisters are queen’s offspring to depart from the population average also paternal sisters. In figures 1–3, we assume f p 1 ¯z. The expected sex ratio of the worker’s own offspring, because eusociality has evolved only in monogamous spe- should she disperse and raise her own brood, is simply cies. The corresponding result for diploidy is a p (1 ϩ the population average¯z . Thus, the inclusive fitness val- f)/2 (app. B), so haplodiploidy promotes facultative help- uation that the worker places on queen-derived offspring ing when the right-hand side (RHS) of equation (1) ex- ϩ Ϫ ϩ iszvmm p (1 z)vp ff , and the inclusive fitness valuation ceeds(1 f)/2 , and haplodiploidy inhibits facultative ϩ Ϫ ϩ that she places on her own offspring is ¯zvms p (1 helping when the RHS of equation (1) is less than (1 1 ϩ 1 ¯z)vpfd, where pm and pf are the consanguinities of the f)/2. If¯¯z 1/[2(1 f)] (i.e.,z 1/4 , under full monog- worker to the queen’s sons and queen’s daughters, re- amy), then haplodiploidy promotes the origin of facul- ! spectively; ps and pd are the consanguinities of the worker tative helping in relatively female-biased broods (z ¯z ) Haplodiploidy and Eusociality 000

Figure 1: Potential for helping in a generic model of split sex ratios, assuming full monogamy (f p 1 ). A, In the context of the origin of p helping, haplodiploidy does not promote obligate helping (aOBL 1 , dashed black line), but it promotes facultative helping in colonies that have relatively female-biased sex ratios (z ! ¯¯zz when1 1/4 ) and inhibits facultative helping in colonies that have relatively male-biased sex ratios (z 1 ¯¯zz when1 1/4 ). The solid black line indicates facultative helping when the population sex ratio is¯z p 0.5 , and the solid gray lines indicate facultative helping for other population sex ratios, with all lines intersectinga p 1 atz p ¯z . B, In the context of the elaboration p of helping, haplodiploidy does not promote obligate helping (aOBL 1 , dashed black line), but it promotes facultative helping in colonies that have relatively female-biased sex ratios (z ! ¯z ) and inhibits facultative helping in colonies that have relatively male-biased sex ratios (z 1 ¯z ). The solid black line indicates facultative helping when the population sex ratio is¯z p 0.5 , and the solid gray lines indicate facultative helping for other population sex ratios, with all lines intersectinga p 1 atz p ¯z . and inhibits the origin of facultative helping in relatively brood), haplodiploidy neither promotes nor inhibits the male-biased broods (z 1 ¯¯zz ). If! 1/[2(1 ϩ f)] (i.e., ¯z ! origin of facultative helping, irrespective of the population 1/4, under full monogamy), then the opposite is true (fig. sex ratio, because the increased relatedness to siblings is 1A) because in such strongly female-biased populations, exactly balanced by the decreased reproductive value of brothers are more valuable than sisters (Trivers and Hare daughters (Craig 1979). 1976). In the absence of split sex ratios (z p ¯z for every If the focal female does not know the sex ratio of her 000 The American Naturalist

Figure 2: Potential for helping in a model of split sex ratios owing to virginity, assuming full monogamy (f p 1 ). A, In the context of the origin of helping, haplodiploidy always promotes helping. As the rate of unmatedness is generally !6%, this suggests the potential for helping isa ! 1.03 . B, In the context of the elaboration of helping, haplodiploidy always promotes helping. Again, as the rate of unmatedness is generally !6%, the potential for helping isa ! 1.07 . Furthermore, it is predicted to be lower if workerless colonies suffer reduced productivity (a ! 1 ; witha r 1 asa r 0 ), as will be the case when helping is selected for. mother’s brood, the decision as to whether or not she Elaboration of Helping should help must be made by taking an average over this uncertainty, and this is equivalent to evaluating the right- We next consider the evolutionary elaboration of helping hand side of equation (1) atz p ¯z . This obtains in a social haplodiploid species, by examining the inclusive p ϩ aOBL (1 f)/2, which is identical to the condition for fitness consequences for a newly eclosed female (the obligate helping under diploidy, so haplodiploidy neither “worker”) who chooses to help her mother (the “queen”) promotes nor inhibits the origin of obligate helping (fig. to rear siblings rather than selfishly rearing her own off- 1A). spring within the same colony. We imagine that the sex Haplodiploidy and Eusociality 000

Figure 3: Potential for helping in a model of split sex ratios owing to queen replacement, assuming full monogamy (f p 1 ). In the context of the elaboration of helping, haplodiploidy can promote or inhibit obligate helping (dashed black line). Over the empirically estimated ! ! ≈ range of queen survival (0.6 q 0.8 ), haplodiploidy has a promoting effect, with maximum potential for obligate helping at aOBL 1.07 whenq ≈ 0.69 . Moreover, haplodiploidy always promotes facultative helping in relatively female-biased (i.e., queenright) colonies (solid ≈ black line). The maximum potential for facultative helping is ataFAC 1.50 , for a range of empirically valid rates of queen survival. ratio of the colony is controlled by its cohort of workers, (2) is less than(1 ϩ f)/2 . That is, haplodiploidy promotes and we allow the sex ratio z among the queen’s offspring the elaboration of facultative helping in relatively female- to depart from the population average¯z . We assume that biased broods (z ! ¯z ) and inhibits the elaboration of fac- the expected sex ratio of the worker’s own offspring, ultative helping in relatively male-biased broods (z 1 ¯z ), should she choose to reproduce, is also z (this assumption irrespective of the actual population sex ratio (fig. 1B). In is relaxed by J. Alpedrinha et al., unpublished manuscript). the absence of split sex ratios (z p ¯z for every brood), Thus, the inclusive fitness valuation that the worker places haplodiploidy neither promotes nor inhibits the elabora- ϩ Ϫ on queen-derived offspring iszvmm p (1 z)vp ff , and the tion of facultative helping, irrespective of the population inclusive fitness valuation that she places on her own off- sex ratio (Craig 1979). ϩ Ϫ spring iszvms p (1 z)vp fd . We assume that worker re- If the focal female does not know the sex ratio of her production is sufficiently rare to be considered negligible mother’s brood, the decision as to whether she should for the purpose of calculating reproductive values (this help must be made by taking an average over this un- assumption is relaxed by J. Alpedrinha et al., unpublished certainty, and this is equivalent to evaluating the RHS of p p ϩ manuscript). equation (2) atz ¯z . This obtainsa OBL (1 f)/2 , If the focal female knows the sex ratio of her mother’s which is identical to the condition for obligate helping brood, and adjusts her helping accordingly, we obtain a under diploidy, so haplodiploidy neither promotes nor p ϩ potential for facultative helping of aFAC[zv m p m inhibits the maintenance of obligate helping (fig. 1B). Ϫ ϩ Ϫ (1 z)vpff]/[zv ms p (1 z)vp fd]. Substituting in the ap- propriate reproductive values and consanguinity coeffi- cients (app. B), this is Split Sex Ratios Owing to Virginity 1 ¯z(1 Ϫ z) a p ϩ f .(2) FAC 2 z(1 Ϫ ¯¯z) ϩ z(1 Ϫ z) Virginity can lead to split sex ratios because unmated fe- males cannot produce daughters but can produce sons Again, haplodiploidy promotes facultative helping when (Taylor 1981; Godfray and Grafen 1988). Hence, colonies the RHS of equation (2) exceeds(1 ϩ f)/2 and haplodip- founded by unmated queens cannot produce workers or loidy inhibits facultative helping when the RHS of equation female reproductives and so specialize in male reproduc- 000 The American Naturalist tion, whereas colonies founded by mated queens can pro- helping under split sex ratios caused by queen virginity. duce workers and reproductives of either sex. Empirical estimates of the frequency of unmatedness sug- gest that is it usually low in outbreeding species, in the range0.00 ! u ! 0.06 (with a mode of 0˜.00; Godfray and Origin of Helping Hardy 1992). Assuming full monogamy (f p 1 ), this We consider the evolutionary origin of helping in an oth- would lead to the potential for helping in the range erwise solitary haplodiploid species, by examining the in- 1.00 ! a ! 1.03 (with a mode of ∼1.00), which even in clusive fitness consequences for a newly eclosed female the best case scenario is only marginally greater than the (the “worker”) who chooses to stay with her mother (the corresponding value for diploidy (a p 1 ; fig. 2A). That “queen”) and rear siblings rather than dispersing and rear- is, it is only for species in which the cost/benefit ratio lies ing her own brood. We assume that a fraction1 Ϫ u of within the narrow range1.00 ! c/b ! 1.03 that the hap- queens are mated and are able to exhibit any sex allocation lodiploidy effect can matter. Below this range, helping is ≤ ≤ disfavored in both diploids and haplodiploids, and above strategy0 z M 1 , whereas a fraction u of queens are unmated (or by some other constraint are able to produce this range, helping is favored in both diploids and hap- only sons; Godfray 1990) and are constrained to exhibit lodiploids. Higher levels of virginity are observed in species p with local mate competition, where males do not disperse, a sex allocation strategy ofz U 1 . We assume that a mated queen controls her own sex allocation, and we find but this does not occur on the route to eusociality (West that her convergence stable strategy (Taylor 1996) is et al. 1997). 1 Ϫ 2u 1 if u ≤ 2(1 Ϫ u)2 Elaboration of Helping ¯z p (3) M 1 0ifu ≥ We next consider the evolutionary elaboration of helping { 2 in a situation where helping is already common, by ex- (see app. C for derivation). The population sex ratio is amining the inclusive fitness consequences for a newly p ϩ Ϫ eclosed female (the “worker”) who chooses to help her given by¯¯z u (1 u)zM , or mother (the “queen”) to rear siblings rather than selfishly 11 if u ≤ rearing her own offspring within the same colony. We 22 ¯z p .(4)consider that colonies founded by unmated queens have 1 ! ! u if u ≥ a fraction0 a 1 of the productivity (i.e., number of { 2 reproductive offspring) enjoyed by colonies founded by mated queens, owing to the absence of workers in the Only mated queens can produce daughters, so the focal former. Also, we now assume that the workers control sex worker must be at the nest of a mated queen, and hence, allocation in the mated-queen colonies, and we find that there is no sense in discriminating facultative versus ob- their convergence stable sex allocation strategy is ligate helping. The expected sex ratio of the worker’s own offspring, should she disperse and raise her own brood, 1 Ϫ [1 ϩ (1 ϩ 2f)a]u 1 if u ≤ is simply the population average¯z , as she could be either 2(1 ϩ f)(1 Ϫ u)1ϩ (1 ϩ 2f)a ¯z p a mated or an unmated queen. Thus, the inclusive fitness M 1 0ifu ≥ valuation that the worker places on queen-derived off- { 1 ϩ (1 ϩ 2f)a ϩ Ϫ spring is¯¯zvpMm m(1 z M)vp f f , and the inclusive fitness valuation that she places on her own offspring is (6) ¯¯zv p ϩ (1 Ϫ z)vp. The potential for helping is a p ms fd (see app. C for derivation). The population sex ratio is [¯¯¯¯zvp ϩ (1 Ϫ z )vp]/(zv p ϩ (1 Ϫ z)vp] and, substi- Mm m M f f m s f d given by¯z p [ua ϩ (1 Ϫ u)z ]/[1 Ϫ (1 Ϫ a)u] , or tuting in the appropriate reproductive values, consan- M guinity coefficients, and sex ratios, this obtains 11 if u ≤ 2(1 ϩ f)1ϩ (1 ϩ 2f)a 1 Ϫ u ϩ f 1 ¯z p . if u ≤ ua 1 2(1 Ϫ u)2 if u ≥ a p .(5) {1 Ϫ (1 Ϫ a)u 1 ϩ (1 ϩ 2f)a 1 ϩ 2f 1 if u ≥ {4(1 Ϫ u)2 (7) Again, haplodiploidy promotes helping, relative to dip- The inclusive fitness valuation that the worker places 1 ϩ 1 ϩ Ϫ loidy, whena (1 f)/2 , which is true for allu 0 (fig. on queen-derived offspring is¯¯zvpMm m(1 z M)vp f f , and 2A). Hence, haplodiploidy always promotes the origin of the inclusive fitness valuation that she places on her own Haplodiploidy and Eusociality 000

ϩ Ϫ offspring is¯¯zvpMm s(1 z M)vp f d . Assuming that worker in already social species where workers have seized control reproduction is sufficiently rare to be considered negligible of the colony sex ratio, as queen-controlled sex allocation for the purpose of calculating reproductive value, the po- does not give rise to split sex ratios. p ϩ Ϫ tential for helping is a [¯¯zvpMm m(1 z M)vp f f]/ We consider a model that is identical to the generic ϩ Ϫ [¯¯zvpMm s(1 z M)vp f d] and, substituting in the appropri- model of split sex ratios presented above, except that we ate reproductive values, consanguinity coefficients, and sex now assume that only a proportion q of colonies are ratios, this obtains headed by their original queen, and that a proportion 1 Ϫ q of colonies are headed by one the original queen’s (1 Ϫ u)(1 ϩ f)1 ≤ daughters. One consequence of queen replacement is that Ϫ ϩ if u ϩ ϩ a p 2[1 u(1 fa)] 1 (1 2f)a males gain extra reproductive value, owing to their ability 1 ϩ 2f 1 . if u ≥ to father and mate with replacement queens (Trivers and { 21ϩ (1 ϩ 2f)a Hare 1976; app. D). We assume that colony sex allocation (8) is controlled by workers, and we find that the convergence stable state of the population, in terms of the sex allocation Haplodiploidy promotes helping, relative to diploidy, of queenright (¯¯zzRL ) and queenless ( ) colonies, is given by whena 1 (1 ϩ f)/2 , which is true for allu 1 0 (fig. 2B). ! ! p However, the empirical estimate of0.00 u 0.06 indi- (z¯¯RL, z ) cates a maximum potential for helping of only a ≈ 1.07 3 Ϫ q 1 under full monogamy (f p 1 ), which is only marginally 0, if q ≤ p []4(1 Ϫ q)3 greater than the corresponding value for diploids (a Ϫ ϩ ͱ ϩ ϩ 1 3 9 8f(1 2f) 1; fig. 2B). Again, this means that the haplodiploidy hy- (0, 1) if ≤ q ≤ 34f pothesis has explanatory power only insofar as ancestral Ϫ ϩ ͱ ϩ ϩ 3q Ϫ 1–2(1 Ϫ q2)f 3 9 8f(1 2f) taxa fell into the cost/benefit range defined by 1.00 ! ,1 if q ≥ ! {{}2q[2 ϩ (1 ϩ q)f]4f c/b 1.07. Moreover, the potential for helping can be sub- stantially lower if workerless colonies suffer a productivity (9) penalty relative to colonies that contain workers (a ! 1 ), with the haplodiploidy effect vanishing in the limit of low (see app. D for derivation). This solution can be used to r ϩ p ϩ Ϫ productivity of workerless colonies (a [1 f]/2 as calculate the population sex ratio¯¯z qzRL(1 q) ¯z . a r 0; fig. 2B). Overall, this suggests that as helping If the focal female knows the status of her natal colony spreads through the population, and becomes more effi- when deciding whether or not to help, and facultatively cient, the benefit of haplodiploidy will be removed. adjusts her helping according to this information, then she may be expected to help more in colonies where the sex ratio is more female biased, that is, queenright colonies. Split Sex Ratios Owing to Queen Replacement Here, the sex ratio among the queen’s offspring is¯zR . We We next consider the scenario where split sex ratios evolve assume that the expected sex ratio of the worker’s off- owing to queen replacement, when the queen is lost from spring, should she choose to reproduce, is also¯zR . Thus, some colonies and replaced by a mated daughter. Assum- the inclusive fitness valuation that the worker places on ϩ Ϫ ing haplodiploidy then, in colonies where the original queen-derived offspring is¯¯zvRm p mR(1 z R) vp f fR , and queen is still present (“queenright” colonies), the workers the inclusive fitness valuation that she places on her own ϩ Ϫ are more related to the queen’s daughters (sisters, r p offspring is her own offspring is¯¯zvRm p s(1 z R) vp f d . p ϩ 3/4) than to her sons (brothers,r p 1/2 ). In contrast, in Thus, the potential for helping is aFAC[¯zvp R m mR Ϫ ϩ Ϫ colonies where the original queen has been replaced by (1 ¯¯zRffRRms)vp ]/[zv p (1 ¯z R) vp fd]. Substituting in the one of her daughters (“queenless” colonies), the workers appropriate reproductive values, consanguinity coefficients are more related to the new queen’s sons (nephews, and sex ratios, we obtain: r p 3/4) than they are to her daughters (nieces,r p 3/8 ). a p This favors workers to bias the colony sex ratio toward FAC females in queenright colonies, and toward males in Ϫ ϩ ͱ ϩ ϩ 1 ϩ 2f 3 9 8f(1 2f) queenless colonies, as has been observed and experimen- if q ≤ tally demonstrated in cooperative bees (Boomsma 1991; 24f Ϫ ϩ ͱ ϩ ϩ . Mueller 1991; Packer and Owen 1994). In contrast, under q[2 ϩ (1 ϩ q)f] 3 9 8f(1 2f) if q ≥ diploidy, queen replacement does not drive split sex ratios, {4q Ϫ 2(1 Ϫ q2)f 4f owing to the symmetry of male and female inheritance. This mechanism applies only to the elaboration of helping, (10) 000 The American Naturalist

Sincea 1 (1 ϩ f)/2 is true for allq ! 1 , haplodiploidy portant for the evolution of eusociality, because they rely promotes the maintenance of facultative helping in queen- on biological assumptions that the comparative data sug- right colonies, under split sex ratios caused by queen re- gest did not occur en route to eusociality, such as multiple placement (fig. 3). This effect of haplodiploidy is sub- mating or associations between same-generation breeders stantial: assuming full monogamy (f p 1 ), the potential (the “semisocial route”); (2) the most plausible route by p for facultative helping can be as great asaFAC 1.50 . In which the haplodiploidy hypothesis could work is with natural populations, the empirically observed range of split sex ratios, building on Trivers and Hare (1976); (3) queen survival rates is0.6 ! q ! 0.8 , which would give although split sex ratios can be favored for many reasons, ≤ ≤ 1.23 aFAC 1.50 (fig. 3) there are only two mechanisms that have both been ob- If the decision to help rear the queen’s offspring versus served empirically and are consistent with the biology of own offspring is taken without reference to whether the primitively social hymenopterans—virginity and queen re- queen is original or a replacement, then the inclusive placement; and (4) while these two mechanisms can lead fitness value of queen offspring and own offspring must to haplodiploidy favoring eusociality, the overall effect is be taken as an expectation over this uncertainty. Hence, likely to be small and can even be negative. p the potential for obligate helping is given by aOBL ϩ Ϫ ϩ Ϫ ϩ Ϫ {q[¯¯zvR m p mR(1 z R)vp f fR] (1 q)[ ¯zv L m p mL (1 ϩ Ϫ ϩ Ϫ ϩ Split Sex Ratios ¯¯zLffL)vp ]}/{q[zv Rms p (1 ¯z Rfd)vp] (1 q)[ ¯zv Lms p Ϫ (1 ¯zLfd)vp]}. Substituting in the appropriate reproductive We have examined two specific mechanisms that could values, consanguinity coefficients and sex ratios, this ob- have led to evolutionarily stable split sex ratios. First, un- tains mated queens are constrained to produce only sons, whereas mated queens may produce both sons and daugh- a p OBL ters (Godfray and Grafen 1988). Hence, workers—who (1 ϩ q)(1 ϩ 2f)1 are female and therefore necessarily born into mated- if q ≤ 43queen colonies—have the option of rearing a cohort of Ϫ ϩ ͱ ϩ ϩ (5 ϩ q)(1 ϩ 2f)13 9 8f(1 2f) siblings with a sex ratio that is female biased relative to if ≤ q ≤ . 16 3 4f the average for the population, which favors helping in Ϫ ϩ ͱ ϩ ϩ [1 ϩ q ϩ 2(1 Ϫ q)f][2 ϩ (1 ϩ q)f] 3 9 8f(1 2f) haplodiploids. However, under the empirically plausible if q ≥ { 84f range of unmatedness rates (0%–6%, with a mode of 0%), the potential for helping is boosted by only 0%–3% when (11) considering the origin of helping and only 0%–7% when considering the subsequent elaboration of helping. More- over, this effect is predicted to be substantially lower if Here,a 1 (1 ϩ f)/2 is not always satisfied. Thus, OBL colonies with mated queens and workers have increased haplodiploidy sometimes promotes and sometimes inhib- productivity, because this reduces the extent to which their its the maintenance of obligate helping if there is queen offspring are female biased relative to the population av- replacement, relative to the basic model of diploidy (fig. erage. That is, the situation that is most conducive to the 3). Assuming full monogamy (f p 1 ), then 0.75 ! evolution of helping (i.e., when helping leads to a large a ! 1.07. Considering the empirically observed range OBL increase in the colony’s productivity) is precisely the sit- of queen survival rates (0.6 ! q ! 0.8 ), then 1.05 ! uation that erodes the impact of haplodiploidy on the a ! 1.07, with the greatest potential for obligate helping OBL potential for helping. beinga ≈ 1.07 atq ≈ 0.69 . Note that the potential for OBL Second, split sex ratios may evolve in response to when obligate helping under diploidy is actually lower than queens die and are replaced by their daughters (Boomsma (1 ϩ f)/2 with queen replacement, owing to the reduced 1991). In colonies that retain the original queen (the relatedness of a worker to the offspring of her queen. mother of the workers), the workers are more related to However, it is not meaningful to compare the potential the female (sisters) than the male (brothers) reproductives for obligate helping under haplodiploidy with this lower and so are favored to produce a relatively female-biased value: all that we learn is that queen replacement inhibits sex ratio. In contrast, in colonies where the original queen helping more under diploidy than under haplodiploidy. has died and has been replaced by one of her daughters (sister of the workers), the workers are less related to the Discussion female (nieces) than the male (nephews) reproductives and so are favored to produce a relatively male-biased sex ratio. We have shown that: (1) many of the proposed conse- We have found that this can lead to selection for helping quences of haplodipoidy are unlikely to have been im- being either promoted or inhibited by haplodiploidy, de- Haplodiploidy and Eusociality 000

Table 3: Empirical objections to the haplodiploidy hypothesis and their current status Potential problem Current status If queens mate multiply, this removes the relatedness advantage of Irrelevant, as multiple mating only evolved after obligate helping to raise siblings (Bourke and Franks 1995; Queller and eusociality was established (Boomsma 2007, 2009; Strassmann 1998) Hughes et al. 2008) Assumes subsocial route, where offspring help mothers, and does Irrelevant, as phylogentic evidence provides no evidence not work with semisocial route where sisters cooperate (Bourke for eusociality ever evolving by the semisocial route and Franks 1995) (Boomsma 2007, 2009; Hughes et al. 2008) Requires sex ratio manipulation with worker control (Charnov Not a problem, as considerable evidence for worker con- 1978; Stubblefield and Charnov 1986) trol of sex allocation, including prior to the evolution of permanent eusociality (Mueller 1991; West 2009) There are alternative explanations for why eusociality is common in True, but does not exclude a role of haplodiploidy the Hymenoptera, such as extended parental care (Stubblefield and Charnov 1986; Queller and Strassmann 1998) and monog- amy (Boomsma 2007, 2009) Given more recent discoveries of eusociality (albeit facultative, not Valid question, but it does not exclude a role of haplo- obligate) in other taxa (Aoki 1977; Jarvis 1981; Crespi 1992; diploidy. Crozier (2008) has shown that eusociality is Kent and Simpson 1992; Duffy 1996), has haplodiploidy really significantly more common in haplodiploid families, evolved more often in haplodiploids? but this does not allow for fact that families are not phylogenetically independent. There has been no phy- logenetic study testing whether the rate of transition to eusociality is significantly higher in haplodiploids. pending on: (a) the incidence of queenright colonies; and previous work on the haplodiploidy hypothesis. Most pre- (b) whether the workers can facultatively adjust their help- vious articles have examined whether the haplodiploidy ing behavior according to the queenright/queenless status hypothesis can be made to work (e.g., Trivers and Hare of the colony, or are obliged to help equally in both types 1976; Seger 1983; Grafen 1986; Stubblefield and Charnov of colonies. However, under the empirically supported 1986; Godfray and Grafen 1988). In contrast, our aim has range of probabilities of queen survival (60%–80%), hap- been to quantify the extent to which the haplodiploidy lodiploidy always promotes helping, with the potential for effect favors the evolution of eusociality. We have focused facultative helping boosted by up to 50% and the potential on those scenarios that are biologically most plausible (ta- for obligate helping boosted to up to 7%. The overall ble 3), and found that the extent to which haplodiploidy importance of this mechanism will depend on how fre- favors eusociality will be either: (a) small (unmated fe- quently queen replacement leads to split sex ratios: to date, males); or (b) small to medium but not widespread (queen it has been found only in some cooperative bees, sug- replacement). In addition, we have clarified the distinction gesting it is not a general factor on the route to eusociality between selection on facultative versus obligate helping. (Boomsma 1991; Mueller 1991; Packer and Owen 1994). The latter is less aided by haplodiploidy, because the in- Furthermore, our analyses are likely to have overesti- creased relatedness to siblings when helping at female- mated the extent to which haplodiploidy favors eusociality biased colonies is negated by the decreased relatedness via queen replacement. We followed previous analyses in when helping at male-biased colonies. This means that assuming that when workers selfishly produce their own haplodiploidy will be less likely to favor helping if females offspring within the queen’s nest, these offspring will ex- must choose whether to help before they know which type hibit the same sex ratio as the queen-derived juveniles of colony they will be helping in, especially with regard (Craig 1979). However, the female bias in the population to commitments to expressing helping that sex ratio means that males have higher reproductive value are made relatively early in development; for example, pre- than females, so that when workers are reproductive they eclosion. Overall, our results suggest that, for the scenarios will be favored to produce sons rather than daughters. we consider here, haplodiploidy would have had only a This would tend to decrease the potential for facultative minor influence on the evolution of eusociality. helping in queenright colonies and obligate helping in both colony types. We consider this effect, and the consequences Manipulation, Maternal Care, and Monogamy of reproduction by workers more generally, elsewhere (J. Alpedrinha et al., unpublished manuscript). In this final section, we briefly consider other factors that Our emphasis in this article has been different to most may have influenced the evolution of eusociality. First, 000 The American Naturalist parental manipulation or parasitism may have helped the 2009; Hughes et al. 2008), and the evolution of facultative evolution of eusociality, by enforcing cooperation on cooperative breeding is more common in species with workers (Alexander 1974; Charnov 1978). However, the lower rates of promiscuity (Cornwallis et al. 2010). Con- extent to which workers will be favored to resist versus sequently, the hunt to find a way for haplodiploidy to push acquiesce to their queen depends on their relatedness to the potential for helping higher than unity (a 1 1 ) may (and the reproductive value of) the different types of off- have been misguided. Instead, a more important factor spring, just as when considering cases with split sex ratios may have been the need for monogamy to keep the po- (Crozier 2008). Consequently, it is wrong to think of pa- tential for helping at unity (a p 1 ), and some small ef- rental manipulation and kin selection as competing hy- ficiency benefit for rearing siblings over one’s own off- potheses for the evolution of eusociality (Crozier 2008). spring (b/c 1 1 ). Efficiency benefits appear to arise from Furthermore, explicit theory has shown that queen ma- the life insurance of allowing helpers to complete parental nipulation is equally likely to occur in diploids and hap- care after the of the mother, or the fortress-defense lodiploids, and so it will not lead to haplodiploids being benefits of protecting a common nest (Hamilton 1964, predisposed to eusociality (Charnov 1978). 1972; Queller 1989, 1994; Foster 1990; Gadagkar 1991; Second, it is possible that haplodiploidy has predisposed Queller and Strassmann 1998; Field et al. 2000; Strassmann certain taxa to eusociality, for reasons that are separate and Queller 2007). from Hamilton’s (1964, 1972) suggestion concerning the asymmetry in relatedness to sisters versus daughters. Wade (2001; Linksvayer and Wade 2005) has suggested that ma- ternal care—a prerequisite for eusociality—evolves more Acknowledgments readily in haplodiploids than in diploids. Reeve (1993; We thank S. Alonzo, K. Boomsma, C. Cornwallis, A. Gra- Reeve and Shellman-Reeve 1997) has suggested that, even fen, A. Griffin, R. Trivers, and two reviewers for discussion when helping genes experience the same systematic selec- and comments; and Balliol College, the European Research tion pressure under diploidy and haplodiploidy, they may Council, the Leverhulme Trust, the Royal Society, and the be better protected from stochastic loss under haplodip- Programa Doutoral em Biologia Computacional–Instituto loidy. Fromhage and Kokko (2011) have suggested that Gulbenkian de Cieˆncia/Fundac¸a˜o para a Cieˆncia e Tec- haplodiploidy can enhance synergistic interaction between nologia (SFRH/BD/33206/2007) for funding. genes for helping. However, these three ideas require re- strictive assumptions, which make them unlikely to be of general importance. Specifically, they require that maternal care genes have particular, deleterious pleiotropic effects APPENDIX A (Wade 2001); helping genes are overdominant (Reeve 1993); or that the worker phenotype is controlled by a Reproductive Value and Relatedness single of large effect (Fromhage and Kokko 2011). More generally, we emphasize the importance of con- Inclusive fitness is gained by sending copies of one’s genes structing realistic models of specific scenarios that are led into future generations. Hence, an actor is predicted to by and parameterized with empirical data. behave as if she values the reproductive success of her Third, both theory and data suggest that monogamy has relatives, as they may carry copies of her genes and pass played a key role in the evolution of eusociality. Strict them on to their descendants (Hamilton 1964). Specifi- lifetime monogamy leads to a worker being equally related cally, the value that she places on a relative is given by the to her own offspring and to the offspring of her mother product of the relative’s reproductive value (v ; i.e., how and hence to a potential for helping ofa p 1 (Boomsma well they transmit copies of their own genes into future 2007, 2009). In this case, any small efficiency benefit from generations; Fisher 1930) and the consanguinity of the rearing siblings (b/c 1 1 ) would lead to helping being fa- relative to the actor (p; i.e., the probability the relative’s vored by (i.e.,b/c 1 1/a ). Multiple mating and actor’s genes are identical by descent; Bulmer 1994). reduces the relatedness of siblings and decreases selection Reproductive value describes the expected contribution for any form of cooperation (Charnov 1981). For example, of genes made by an individual or class of individuals to if females mate with two or three males, this reduces the a generation in the distant future (Fisher 1930; Taylor potential for helping toa p 3/4 or 2/3, respectively, and 1990; Grafen 2006b). Typically, reproductive value is first so substantial efficiency benefits to cooperation would be calculated for a class, and then the class’s reproductive required (b/c 1 4/3 or 3/2, respectively). Consistent with value is shared equally over all individuals in that class. this, obligate eusociality has evolved only in lineages where For example, in diploids, the probability that a gene picked strict monogamy is the ancestral state (Boomsma 2007, at random from a distant future generation descends from Haplodiploidy and Eusociality 000 a male ancestor in the present generation is 1/2. Hence, coefficients have been defined as the probability that a p the class reproductive value of males iscm 1/2 , and the gene picked at random from the actor is identical by de- p reproductive value of an individual male isvmmmc /N , scent with any gene in the recipient (Trivers and Hare where Nm is the number of males in the population. Al- 1976; Charlesworth 1980). If the recipient is haploid this ternatively, reproductive values may be scaled by any ar- is simply the consanguinity of the actor and recipient, bitrary constant, to make the quantities more manageable. whereas if the recipient is diploid this is twice the con- For example, multiplying all individual reproductive values sanguinity of the actor and recipient. This corrects for by the total number of individuals in the population, we reproductive value but only owing to a mathematical co- p p Ϫ p Ϫ p havevmmc /¯¯z 1/[2(1 z)] and v ffc /(1 ¯z) incidence: in both diploids and haplodiploids, the ratio of 1/[2(1 Ϫ ¯¯z)], wherez is the proportion of reproductive in- female to male is equal to the ratio of female to dividuals who are male. male individual reproductive values (under the assump- Consanguinity is defined as the probability that two tion of vanishingly rare worker reproduction and an even genes picked at random from two given individuals are sex ratio). This correction does not work more generally, identical by descent (Bulmer 1994). Thus, the probability for example, under certain hypothetical haplotriploid of drawing a given allele from the recipient given that it modes of inheritance (Grafen 1986) or, more importantly, has already been drawn from the actor is x p p ϩ (1 Ϫ when worker reproduction is common and/or sex ratios p)x¯, where p is the consanguinity of the actor and recipient are biased (see apps. B–D). andx¯ is the frequency of the allele in the whole population. This allows a regression interpretation for consanguinity: rearranging, we havep p (x Ϫ x¯¯)/(1 Ϫ x) , that is, con- sanguinity measures the concentration of the actor’s genes APPENDIX B in the recipient. Split Sex Ratios and the Evolution of Helping It is often natural to seek a measure of relative genetic similarity such that the similarity to oneself is 1. This is For the purpose of calculating reproductive value, we cen- obtained by dividing consanguinity between actor and re- sus the population at the moment of production of off- p cipient by the consanguinity of actor to self, that is, r spring. The proportion of genes in female larvae at the p/pself. This is the regression coefficient of relatedness time of census that derive from the females of the last (Hamilton 1970; Michod and Hamilton 1980; Pamilo and ℘ p census isfRf 1/2 and hence the proportion of genes Crozier 1982; Grafen 1985) and, for outbred diploids, this in female larvae that derive from the males of the last takes the familiar values ofr p 1 for self,r p 1/2 for full census is℘ R p 1 Ϫ ℘ R p 1/2 . The proportion of p p f mff sibs,r 1/4 for half sibs, andr 1/8 for cousins. More genes in male larvae at the time of census that derive from generally, since throughout our analysis we will assume ℘ p the females of the last census ismRf 1 , and hence the outbreeding and diploid actors (i.e., females), the consan- proportion of genes in male larvae that derive from the guinity to self for the actor is alwaysp p 1/2 , and hence self males of the last census is℘ R p 1 Ϫ ℘ R p 0 . These p m mmf the regression coefficient of relatedness isr 2p . quantities can be summarized in a gene flow matrix: The regression relatedness values for a female to her p son and to her brother under haplodiploidy arer 1 and ℘℘ p p fRffRm r 1/2, respectively. Many readers will be more familiar M ℘℘.(B1) []mRfmRm with values of 1/2 and 1/4, respectively. The latter values refer to “life-for-life” relatedness coefficients, which were The class reproductive values are given by the dominant commonly used in the kin selection literature on haplo- left eigenvector of the gene flow matrix, that is, the solution p • p diploids to account for both genetic similarity and repro- to(c fm, c ) (c fm, c ) M (Taylor 1996). This yields c f p ductive value effects in a single coefficient (Hamilton 1972; 2/3 for females andcm 1/3 (Price 1970; Taylor 1996). Trivers and Hare 1976; Grafen 1986). Assuming an even Individual reproductive values can be expressed as the class sex ratio and vanishingly rare worker reproduction, these reproductive value divided by the proportion of the pop- p life-for-life relatedness coefficients can be recovered by ulation that belongs to that class, that is,vmmc /¯z for p Ϫ simply multiplying the regression coefficient of relatedness males andvffc /(1 ¯z) for females (Taylor 1996). by individual reproductive value, that is, R p r # v The consanguinity of a female to her brothers and sisters p p ϩ (Hamilton 1972; Bulmer 1994), and scaling all reproduc- ispmf1/4 andp (1 2f)/8 , respectively, where f is tive values such that the reproductive value of a female is the consanguinity of two random sperm that fertilize the p vf 1. Thus, the life-for-life relatedness of a female to eggs of the same female. The consanguinity of a female p # p p p her son isR 1 1/2 1/2 and to her brother is to her sons and daughters ispsd1/2 andp 1/4 , R p 1/2 # 1/2 p 1/4. Sometimes, life-for-life relatedness respectively. 000 The American Naturalist

APPENDIX C Elaboration of Helping

Here we derive the workers’ convergence stable sex allo- Split Sex Ratios Owing to Virginity cation strategy. This is modeled by assuming that the queen produces fertilized (female) and unfertilized (male) eggs Origin of Helping in equal numbers, and the workers choose which half of these are to be raised to reproductive maturity. The ex- Here we derive the queen’s convergence stable sex allo- pected fitness of a female in a mated-queen colony, cation strategy. We model this by assuming that she pro- before the enactment of the worker sex allocation decision, duces fertilized (female) and unfertilized (male) eggs in Ϫ is therefore1 z M , that is, the probability that she will equal numbers but has resources to raise only half of these be reared to reproductive maturity. The average fitness of eggs to maturity, so by choosing which individuals to raise, Ϫ her class is1 ¯zM , and hence her relative fitness is she determines the sex ratio of her offspring. The expected p Ϫ Ϫ WfMM(1 z )/(1 ¯z ). Similarly, the relative fitness of a fitness of a female egg laid by a mated queen, before the male egg, expressed relative to the average for all males, enactment of the sex allocation decision, is therefore isW p [ua ϩ (1 Ϫ u)z ]/[ua ϩ (1 Ϫ u)¯z ] . As before, Ϫ mMM 1 z M, that is, the probability that she will be reared to the direction of selection acting on the sex ratio of mated- reproductive maturity. The average fitness of her class is queen colonies is given by equation (C1), where Ϫ ¯ p Ϫ 1 zWMf, and hence her relative fitness is (1 ѨW /Ѩz p Ϫ1/(1 Ϫ ¯z ) and ѨW /Ѩz p (1 Ϫ u)/[ua ϩ Ϫ ¯ fM M mM z MM)/(1 z ). Similarly, the relative fitness of a male egg, (1 Ϫ u)¯z ] are the fitness effects of the worker sex allo- p M expressed relative to the average for all males, is Wm cation decision on female and male eggs, respectively;g˜ is ϩ Ϫ ϩ Ϫ ¯ [u (1 u)z MM]/[u (1 u)z ]. Natural selection favors the average of the workers’ genetic values for their sex an increase in the population average value of any trait if p allocation trait;dzM /dg˜ 1 is the genotype-phenotype individuals carrying genes for this trait are fitter on av- p p p p map; anddg˜˜/dgfMp f3/8 and dg/dg mMp m 1/4 erage. In a class structured population, fitness is averaged are the coefficients of consanguinity between worker and p using class reproductive values as weights, that is, W queen’s female offspring and between worker and queen’s ϩ cWffcW mm(Taylor 1996; Taylor and Frank 1996; Frank male offspring, respectively (Taylor 1990, 1996; Taylor and 1997, 1998; Rousset 2004; Taylor et al. 2007). If a gene Frank 1996; Frank 1997, 1998; Rousset 2004; Taylor et al. affecting the trait of interest has genic value g, then the 2007). Substituting in the class reproductive values obtains condition for natural selection to favor an increase in the a condition for increase in sex ratio of mated-queen col- 1 population average value of this trait isdW/dg 0 . Hence, oniesdW/dg 1 0 in terms of model parameters, and we the direction of selection acting on the sex ratio of mated use this to obtain equation (6) in the main text. queens is given by

APPENDIX D dW dW dW p fmϩ c fmc , dg dgfm dg Split Sex Ratios Owing to Queen Replacement ѨWdz dg˜˜ѨWdz dg p c fMϩ c mM ,(C1)One consequence of queen replacement is that males gain fmѨ ˜˜Ѩ zdgdgMfMMmMzdgdg extra reproductive value, owing to their ability to father and mate with replacement queens. Again, for the purpose Ѩ Ѩ p Ϫ Ϫ Ѩ Ѩ p Ϫ whereWfM/ z 1/(1 ¯z M) and W mM/ z (1 of calculating reproductive value, we census the population ϩ Ϫ u)/[u (1 u)¯zM] are the fitness effects of the queen’s sex at the moment of production of reproductive offspring. allocation decision on female and male eggs, respectively; The proportion of genes in female larvae at the time of g˜ is the queen’s genetic value for her sex allocation trait; census that derive from the females of the last census is p ℘ p ϩ Ϫ p ϩ dz M /dg˜ 1 is the genotype-phenotype map; and fRf q/2 (1 q)/4 (1 q)/4, and hence the pro- p p p p dg˜˜/dgfMp d1/4 anddg/dg mMp s 1/2 are the co- portion of genes in female larvae that derive from the ℘ p Ϫ ℘ p Ϫ efficients of consanguinity between mother and daughter males of the last census isf Rmf1 Rf (3 q)/4 . and between mother and son, respectively (Taylor 1990, The proportion of genes in male larvae at the time of 1996; Taylor and Frank 1996; Frank 1997, 1998; Rousset census that derive from the females of the last census is ℘ p ϩ Ϫ p ϩ 2004; Taylor et al. 2007). Substituting in the class repro- mRf q (1 q)/2 (1 q)/2, and hence the pro- ductive values obtains a condition for increase in sex ratio portion of genes in male larvae that derive from the males 1 ℘ p Ϫ ℘ p Ϫ strategy employed by mated queensdW/dg 0 in terms of the last census ismRmm1 Rf (3 q)/2 . Using of model parameters, and we use this to obtain equation the procedure outlined in appendix B, the class repro- p ϩ ϩ p Ϫ (3) in the main text. ductive values arec fm(2 2q)/(5 q) and c (3 Haplodiploidy and Eusociality 000 q)/(5 ϩ q). It is useful to define four classes of young re- identify the convergence stable state of the population productives, according to their sex and the colony type (Taylor 1996). This obtains equation (9) in the main text. from which they were reared. So, the class reproductive p Ϫ Ϫ value of queenright females isc fR[q(1 ¯¯z R)/(1 z)]c f , p Literature Cited that of queenright males iscmR[qz¯¯ R/z]c m , that of queen- p Ϫ Ϫ Ϫ less females isc fL[(1 q)(1 ¯¯z L)/(1 z)]c f , and that of p Ϫ Alcock, J. 2005. behavior. 8th ed. Sinauer Associates, Sun- queenless males iscmL[(1 q)¯¯z L/z]cz m , where ¯ R and ¯ derland, MA. zL are the average sex ratios of queenright colonies and Alexander, R. D. 1974. The evolution of social behaviour. Annual queenless colonies, respectively. Review of Ecology, Evolution, and 5:325–383. The sex allocation decision of the workers is modelled Aoki, S. 1977. Colophina clematis (Homoptera: Pemphigidae), an in the usual way. An equal number of male and female species with soldiers. Kontyu 45:276–282. larvae are produced, and the workers choose which of these Boomsma, J. J. 1991. Adaptive colony sex ratios in primitively eu- to rear. The expected fitness of a female egg in a queenright social bees. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 6:92–95. colony, prior to the enactment of the worker sex allocation ———. 1993. Sex ratio variation in polygynous ants. Pages 86–109 Ϫ in L. Keller, ed. Queen number and in . Oxford decision, is therefore1 z R , that is, the probability that University Press, Oxford. she will be reared to reproductive maturity. The average ———. 2007. Kin selection versus : why the ends do Ϫ fitness of her class is1 ¯zR , and hence her relative fitness not meet. Current Biology 17:R673–R683. p Ϫ Ϫ isWfR(1 z R)/(1 ¯z R) . Similarly, the relative fitness of ———. 2009. Lifetime monogamy and the evolution of eusociality. p Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sci- a queenright male isWmRz R/¯z R , that of a queenless p Ϫ Ϫ ences 364:3191–3208. female isWfL(1 z L)/(1 ¯z L) , and that of a queenless p Boomsma, J. J., and A. Grafen. 1991. Colony-level sex ratio selection male isWmLz L/¯z L . Thus, natural selection maximizes p ϩ ϩ ϩ in the eusocial Hymenoptera. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 4: the quantity W cWfR fRcW mR mRcW fL fLcW mL mL 383–407. (Taylor 1996; Taylor and Frank 1996; Frank 1997, 1998; Bourke, A. F. G. 2011. Principles of social evolution. Oxford Uni- Rousset 2004; Taylor et al. 2007). Hence, the direction of versity Press, Oxford. selection acting on the sex ratio of queenright colonies is Bourke, A. F. G., and N. R. Franks. 1995. Social evolution in ants. given by Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Brown, W. D., and L. Keller. 2000. Colony sex ratios vary with queen dW dW dW number but not relatedness asymmetry in the . p fRϩ mR c fRc mR , Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 267:1751– dg dgfR dg mR 1757. Bulmer, M. 1994. Theoretical evolutionary ecology. Sinauer Associ- ѨWdzdg˜˜ѨWdzdg p c fR Rϩ c mR R ,(D1) ates, Sunderland, MA. fRѨ mR Ѩ zdgdgRfRR˜˜zdgdg mR Chapuisat, M., and L. Keller. 1999. Testing kin selection with sex allocation data in eusocial Hymenoptera. Heredity 82:473–478. Ѩ Ѩ p Ϫ Ϫ Ѩ Ѩ p Charlesworth, B. 1980. Models of kin selection. Pages 11–26 in H. whereWfR/ z R1/(1 ¯¯z R) andW mR/ z R1/z R are the fitness effects of the worker sex allocation decision on Markl, ed. Evolution of social behavior: hypotheses and empirical tests. Chemie, Weinheim. female and male eggs, respectively, in a queenright colony; Charnov, E. L. 1978. Evolution of eusocial behavior: offspring choice g˜ is the average of the workers’ genetic values for their sex or parental parasitism? Journal of Theoretical Biology 75:451–465. p allocation trait;dz R/dg˜ 1 is the genotype-phenotype ———. 1981. Kin selection and helpers at the nest: effects of pa- p p map; anddg˜˜/dgfRpdg fR and/dg mRp mR are the coef- ternity and biparental care. Animal Behaviour 29:631–632. ficients of consanguinity between worker and queen’s fe- Cornwallis, C., S. A. West, K. E. Davies, and A. S. Griffin. 2010. male offspring and between worker and queen’s male off- Promiscuity and the evolutionary transition to complex societies. spring, respectively, in a queenright colony (Taylor 1996; 466:969–972. Craig, R. 1979. Parental manipulation, kin selection, and the evo- Taylor and Frank 1996; Frank 1997, 1998; Rousset 2004; lution of altruism. Evolution 33:319–334. Taylor et al. 2007). Since the queen’s offspring are the Crespi, B. J. 1992. Eusociality in Australian gall . Nature 359: p p worker’s siblings, these arepfR3/8 andp mR 1/4 . Sub- 724–726. stituting in the class reproductive values obtains a con- Crozier, R. H. 2008. Advanced eusociality, kin selection and male dition for increase in sex ratio of queenright colonies haploidy. Australian Journal of Entomology 47:2–8. dW/dg 1 0 in terms of model parameters. We consider the Crozier, R. H., and P. Pamilo. 1996. Evolution of social col- action of selection on the sex ratio of queenless colonies onies: sex allocation and kin selection. Oxford University Press, Oxford. in exactly the same way, deriving a condition for increase. Duffy, J. E. 1996. Eusociality in a coral-reef shrimp. Nature 381:512– In this case, the coefficients of consanguinity to female 514. and male eggs are those for nieces and nephews, that is, Duffy, J. E., and K. S. Macdonald. 2010. Kin structure, ecology and p p pfL3/16 andp mL 3/8 . We use these conditions to the evolution of social organization in shrimp: a comparative anal- 000 The American Naturalist

ysis. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277: 2008. Ancestral monogamy shows kin selection is the key to the 575–584. evolution of eusociality. Science 320:1213–1216. Field, J., G. Shreeves, S. Sumner, and M. Casiraghi. 2000. Insurance- Jarvis, J. U. M. 1981. Eusociality in a : cooperative breeding based advantage to helpers in a tropical hover . Nature 404: in naked mole-rat colonies. Science 212:571–573. 869–871. Johnstone, R. A., M. A. Cant, and J. Field. 2011. Sex-biased dispersal, Fisher, R. A. 1930. The genetical theory of natural selection. Clar- haplodiploidy and the evolution of helping in social insects. Pro- endon, Oxford. ceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, doi:10.1098/ Foster, W. A. 1990. Experimental evidence for effective and altruistic rspb.2011.1257. colony defence against natural predators by soldiers of the gall- Kent, D. S., and J. A. Simpson. 1992. Eusociality in the Aus- forming aphid Pemphigus spyrothecae (: Pemphigidae). troplatypus incompertus (Coleoptera: Curculionidae). Naturwis- and 27:421–430. senschaften 79:86–87. Frank, S. A. 1987. Variable sex ratios among colonies of ants. Be- Krebs, J. R., and N. B. Davies. 1993. An introduction to behavioural havioral Ecology and Sociobiology 20:195–201. ecology. 3rd ed. Blackwell Scientific, Oxford. ———.1997. Multivariate analysis of correlated selection and kin Lehmann, L., V. Ravigne´, and L. Keller. 2008. Population viscosity selection, with an ESS maximization method. Journal of Theo- can promote the evolution of altruistic sterile helpers and euso- retical Biology 189:307–316. ciality. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 275: ———. 1998. Foundations of social evolution. Princeton University 1887–1895. Press, Princeton, NJ. Linksvayer, T. A., and M. J. Wade. 2005. The evolutionary origin and Frank, S. A., and B. J. Crespi. 1989. Synergism between sib-rearing elaboration of sociality in the aculeate hymenoptera: maternal ef- and sex ratio in Hymenoptera. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobi- fects, sib-social effects, and heterochrony. Quarterly Review of Bi- ology 24:155–162. ology 80:317–336. Fromhage, L., and H. Kokko. 2011. Monogamy and haplodiploidy Meunier, J., S. A. West, and M. Chapuisat. 2008. Split sex ratios in act in synergy to promote the evolution of eusociality. Nature the social Hymenoptera: a meta-analysis. Behavioral Ecology 19: Communications 2:397. 382–390. Gadagkar, R. 1991. Demographic predisposition to the evolution of Michod, R. E., and W. D. Hamilton. 1980. Coefficients of relatedness eusociality: a hierarchy of models. Proceedings of the National in sociobiology. Nature 288:694–697. Academy of Sciences of the USA 88:10993–10997. Mueller, U. G. 1991. Haplodiploidy and the evolution of facultative Gardner, A. 2010. Sex-biased dispersal of adults mediates the evo- sex ratios in a primitively eusocial . Science 254:442–444. lution of altruism among juveniles. Journal of Theoretical Biology Packer, P., and R. E. Owen. 1994. Relatedness and sex ratio in a 262:339–345. primitively eusocial halictine bee. Behavioral Ecology and Socio- Godfray, H. C. J. 1990. The causes and consequences of constrained biology 34:1–10. sex allocation in haplodiploid . Journal of Evolutionary Biology 3:3–17. Pamilo, P., and R. H. Crozier. 1982. Measuring genetic relatedness Godfray, H. C. J., and A. Grafen. 1988. Unmatedness and the evo- in natural populations: methodology. Theoretical Population Bi- lution of eusociality. American Naturalist 131:303–305. ology 21:171–193. Godfray, H. C. J., and I. C. W. Hardy. 1992. Sex ratio and virginity Passera, L., S. Aron, E. L. Vargo, and L. Keller. 2001. Queen control in haplodiploid animals. Pages 402–441 in D. L. Wrensch and M. of sex ratio in fire ants. Science 293:1308–1310. A. Ebbert, eds. Evolution and diversity of sex ratio in insect and Price, G. R. 1970. Selection and covariance. Nature 227:520–521. mites. Chapman & Hall, New York. Queller, D. C. 1989. The evolution of eusociality: reproductive head Grafen, A. 1985. A geometric view of relatedness. Oxford Surveys in start of workers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Evolutionary Biology 2:28–89. of the USA 86:3224–3226. ———. 1986. Split sex ratios and the evolutionary origins of eu- ———. 1994. Extended parental care and the origin of eusociality. sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology 122:95–121. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 256:105– ———. 2006a. Optimization of inclusive fitness. Journal of Theo- 111. retical Biology 238:541–563. Queller, D. C., and J. E. Strassmann. 1998. Kin selection and social ———. 2006b. A theory of Fisher’s reproductive value. Journal of insects. BioScience 48:165–175. Mathematical Biology 53:15–60. Ratnieks, F. L. W. 1988. Reproductive harmony via mutual policing Greeff, J. M. 1996. Alternative mating strategies, partial sibmating by workers in eusocial Hymenoptera. American Naturalist 132: and split sex ratios in haplodiploid species. Journal of Evolutionary 217–236. Biology 9:855–869. Reece, S. E., D. M. Shuker, I. Pen, A. B. Duncan, A. Choudhary, C. Hamilton, W. D. 1963. The evolution of altruistic behavior. American M. Batchelor, and S. A. West. 2004. Kin discrimination and sex Naturalist 97:354–356. ratios in a parasitoid wasp. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 17: ———. 1964. The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I, II. Jour- 208–216. nal of Theoretical Biology 7:1–52. Reeve, H. K. 1993. Haplodiploidy, eusociality and absence of male ———. 1970. Selfish and spiteful behaviour in an evolutionary parental and alloparental care in Hymenoptera: a unifying genetic model. Nature 228:1218–1220. hypothesis distinct from kin selection theory. Philosophical Trans- ———. 1972. Altruism and related phenomena, mainly in social actions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 342:335–352. insects. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 3: Reeve, H. K., and J. S. Shellman-Reeve. 1997. The general protected 193–232. invasion theory: sex biased in parental and alloparental care. Evo- Hughes, W. O. H., B. P. Oldroyd, M. Beekman, and F. L. W. Ratnieks. lutionary Ecology 11:357–370. Haplodiploidy and Eusociality 000

Rousset, F. 2004. Genetic structure and selection in subdivided pop- Taylor, P. D., and S. A. Frank. 1996. How to make a kin selection ulations. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. model. Journal of Theoretical Biology 180:27–37. Seger, J. 1983. Partial bivoltinism may cause alternating sex-ratio Taylor, P. D., G. Wild, and A. Gardner. 2007. Direct fitness or inclusive biases that favor eusociality. Nature 301:59–62. fitness: how shall we model kin selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20:301–309. ———. 1991. Cooperation and conflict in social insects. Pages 338– Trivers, R. L., and H. Hare. 1976. Haplodiploidy and the evolution 373 in J. R. Krebs and N. B. Davies, eds. Behavioral ecology: an of the social insects. Science 191:249–263. evolutionary approach. Oxford, Blackwell. Wade, M. J. 2001. Maternal effect genes and the evolution of sociality Strassmann, J. E., and D. C. Queller. 2007. Insect societies as divided in haplo-diploid organisms. Evolution 55:453–458. organisms: the complexities of purpose and cross-purpose. Pro- West, S. A. 2009. Sex allocation. Princeton University Press, Prince- ceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104: ton, NJ. 8619–8626. West, S. A., E. A. Herre, S. G. Compton, H. C. J. Godfray, and J. M. Stubblefield, J. W., and E. L. Charnov. 1986. Some conceptual issues Cook. 1997. A comparative study of virginity in fig wasps. Animal in the origin of eusociality. Heredity 57:181–187. Behavior 54:437–450. Taylor, P. D. 1981. Sex ratio compensation in ant populations. Evo- West, S. A., D. M. Shuker, and B. C. Sheldon. 2005. Sex ratio ad- justment when relatives interact: a test of constraints on adapta- lution 35:1250–1251. tion. Evolution 59:1211–1228. ———. 1990. Allele-frequency change in a class structured popu- lation. American Naturalist 135:95–106. ———. 1996. Inclusive fitness arguments in genetic models of be- Associate Editor: Sean H. Rice haviour. J. Math. Biol. 34:654–674. Editor: Ruth G. Shaw

A () preparing for her nuptial flight. Photograph by L. Sundstro¨m.