Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour, Second Edition Peter J Simmons and David Young Index More Information

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour, Second Edition Peter J Simmons and David Young Index More Information Cambridge University Press 0521627265 - Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour, Second Edition Peter J Simmons and David Young Index More information INDEX Entries in bold type refer to pages where an important term, set in bold in the text, is defined or used for the first time. acceptance angle 89 carrier frequency 161 action potential 26 cartridge 102 active membrane 33 cell body 23, 62 adaptation, sensory 82, 105 central pattern generator 166, 177, 192 dark 92, 104 centre surround receptive field 108 light 92 cercus 71 agonist 193 chaffinch 239 amacrine cell 23 channel 29 retinal 25, 107 chemical sensitive 32 amphibians 9 voltage sensitive 30, 40, 192 amplify 105, 106 circuit 24, 42, 99, 117, 125, 140, 176, 192, analogue signals in neurons 39, 81 201, 204, 206, 211, 225 Anser 4 closed loop 17 Apis 228 cochlea 149, 158 Aplysia 202, 236 cochlea nucleus 139, 149 apposition 88 cockroach 69, 77 approach 123 coding, sensory 81, 102 arthropod 4, 22, 23, 85 coincidence detection 140, 157 association 228 collision 123 associative learning 228 command neuron 43 axon 23, 24, 26, 38, 45, 51, 62, 68, 76, 96, commissural interneurons 192 140 computer model 15, 126, 193, 209 conditional oscillator 196 back propagation 210 conditioning 230 barn owl 132 connective nerve 24 bat 129, 142 constant frequency signals 145 bee 88, 228 context 217 binocular 90 contrast 96, 102, 105, 108 bipolar neuron 51 frequency 119–20 blowfly 87, 102, 104 control systems theory 15 Brachydanio 62 corollary discharge interneuron 57 Bufo 9 cranial relay neuron 67 crayfish 44 cable properties 34 cricket 174 call, bat 142 cue Calliphora 87, 104, 111 acoustic 138 campaniform organ 77 visual 7, 125 canary 243 capacitor 36 dark adaptation 92 Carausius 62 decibel 130 262 © Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 0521627265 - Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour, Second Edition Peter J Simmons and David Young Index More information Index 263 decision 26, 42, 52, 75 filter, sensory 18, 74, 99, 106, 112, 114 dedicated fixed action pattern 4 circuit 204 flight 109, 166 system 3, 129 fictive 172 delay line 140, 157 initiation 178, 182 dendrite 23, 24, 33, 40, 51, 62, 65, 66, locust 167–82 125, 225 muscle 167 sensory 79 rhythm 171 depolarisation 27 steering 167, 182 descending contralateral movement flow diagram 7 detector (DCMD) 122 flow field 112 deviation detector (DN) 182 fly 109 diffraction 87 fovea digital signals 39, 82 auditory 158–9 dipteran 95 eye 89 directional selectivity 71, 112 frequency analysis 158 mechanism of 114–20 frequency modulated signals 143 disinhibition 176, 216 Fringilla 239 Doppler shift 145, 158 dorsal ramp interneuron 189 ganglion 24 dragonfly 86, 93 giant neurons 42, 70, 71 Drosophila 86, 114, 235 glial cell 24 glomerulus 230 ear glutamate 193 bat 148–9 goldfish 61 owl 132–5 goose 4 ecdysis 222 graded potential 36, 39 ecdysone 224 as analogue signal 39 echolocation 130, 142 summation of 36 echo ranging 152 grasshopper 122 eclosion hormone 224 gull 4 ecology 95 egg retrieval 4 H1 neuron 112, 115 electrical inhibition 68 habituation 52, 59, 236 electrical synapse 33, 49, 66 hair cell 149 electromyogram (emg) 169 hair receptor 51, 53–4, 58 elementary motion detector 115 harmonics 146 encode 94 Hemicordulia 93, 104 escape running 43, 71 Homo 91 escape swimming honey bee 88, 228 crayfish 43, 48, 55 horseshoe bat 145 Tritonia 186 horseshoe crab 106 excitation 29, 40, 65, 74, 125, 189 housefly86 excitatory 27 HS neuron 112, 118–19 excitatory postsynaptic potential HVc 240–4 (EPSP) 32, 33, 36, 51, 66, 193, 202, hymenoptera 228 216 hyperpolarisation 27, 53 eye compound 86, 183 inhibition 40, 74 simple 183 lateral 106 presynaptic 58 facilitation 115, 236 inhibitory 27, 56, 68–9 feature detector 99, 127 inhibitory motor neuron 56, 206 feedback 15, 16, 108, 180, 212, 225, 244 inhibitory postsynaptic potential (IPSP) feed forward 224 32, 33, 38, 56, 125 figure-ground neuron 114 insect 100 filiform sensillum 71 integration 24, 27, 33–40, 74 © Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 0521627265 - Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour, Second Edition Peter J Simmons and David Young Index More information 264 Index intensity-response curve 94, 105 memory 235 interneuron metamorphosis 222, 225–8 definition 24 Microchiroptera 130 flight generator 172–8 mollusc 186, 202, 235 local 24, 212 monosynaptic connection 58, 66, 67, non-spiking 212, 215–18 176, 203 spiking 212–15 Mormoopidae 146 intracellular electrode 26 moth 222 intrinsic neuromodulation 190 motif 237, 242 invertebrate 4, 23, 168 motor command 244 juvenile 235 control 212 juvenile hormone 227 giant neuron 45 neuron 23, 45, 67, 167, 171–8, 192–4, Kenyon cell 232 202, 206, 209, 215–17, 225–7 pattern 4, 18 lamina 100 mouse-eared bat 143 large monopolar cell 102–8 moustached bat 146 Larus 5 multimodal neurons 182 larva 222, 225–8 Musca 111, 161 lateral giant interneuron 45–60 muscle lateral inhibition 106 flight 167 leech 206 receptor organ 53–4, 56, 77 lens 86–8 vertebrate 167 light adaptation 92 mushroom body 232 Limulus 106 Myotis 143, 146–7, 152, 158 lobster 187, 194 lobula 101 nerve cell 2, 20–2 giant movement detector (LGMD) neurite 23 122 neuroethology 2 plate 109 neurohormone 185 local circuit 40 neuromodulator 187, 190, 232 locust 122 neuron flight 166–86 birth 243 leg 212–18 culture 211 Locusta 87 definition of 20 logarithmic scale 93 kinds of 23–4 Lymnaea 211 loss 242, 243 parts of 22–3 Manduca 222 neuronal loop 184 mantis 17, 89 neuropile 24 map neurotransmitter 31, 190, 193 neuronal 137, 138–42, 158 noise 33, 106 somatotopic 201, 213 nucleus 22, 235 topographic 100 auditory 138–42 Mauthner bird song 235, 240–4 inhibitor 69 nudibranch mollusc 186 neuron 61–9 null direction 112 mechanoreceptor 51, 53, 77, 79–83 medial giant interneuron 45–7 ocelli 183 medulla 100, 117, 125 octopamine 185–7, 232–3 Megachiroptera 130 ommatidium 86, 87–91, 101 membrane open loop 17, 133 cable properties 34 optic lobe, insect 100 potential 27, 80 optic tectum space constant 34 amphibian 11–15 time constant 36 bird 138 © Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 0521627265 - Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour, Second Edition Peter J Simmons and David Young Index More information Index 265 optical monitoring 204–6 proboscis extension 228 optomotor proprioceptive 165 neurons 109–14 refractory period 29 response 109 releasing mechanism 6, 11–15 owl 131 resistor 35 resolution, of eye 86 pacemaker neuron 166 resting potential 27 parallel pathway 100, 118, 142, 204 retina, vertebrate 11, 24–5, 107–8 passerine 235 retinula cell 91 passive membrane 33 rhabdom 91 pathway, sensory 100 rhabdomere 91 pattern generator 55, 166 Rhinolophidae 145 perception 7 Rhinolophus 145–6, 150–2, 158–62 Periplaneta 70, 78 rhythm generation 166, 171–99 phase locking 139 robot 201, 211 phase resetting 173 Rohon beard cell 192 phasic response 54, 82, 94, 103 Rousettus 153 photon bump 92 photoreceptor 86, 97, 100–4 Sarcophaga 95 potential 91–6 saturated response 94 plastic song 239 segmental giant neuron 49 plasticity 181 selectivity 76, 114–15 polar plot 72 sensitive period 239–40 polypeptide 221, 224 sensitisation 236 positive feedback 59 sensitivity 86, 92 postsynaptic neuron 30 sensory postsynaptic potential (PSP) 31 analysis 212 post-tetanic potentiation 236 filtering 51 preferred direction 112 modality 76–7, 182 presynaptic neuron 23, 51, 65, 76, 79, 82–4, 202–3, facilitation 236 205, 206–9, 212, 215, 218, 236 inhibition 58 sign stimuli 5, 9–11 neuron 30 silver staining 20 prey recognition 9–15, 161 simulation 193 Procambarus 44 snail 211 proprioceptor 77, 180, 217 social releaser 5 Pteronotus 146, 155–8 song pupa 222, 226 bird 235, 237–44 pyramidal cell 21 cricket 173–4 sonograph 237 rebound spike 192 sound 130 receptive field 73, 83, 106, 137, 138, frequency 130 214–15 nature of 130 reconfiguration 194–8 pressure level 130 receptor pulse 142–6 cell 76, 149 space constant 34 NMDA 193, 199, 236 spatial summation 38 olfactory 230 spike 26, 51–2, 65–73, 82, 153–5, 192–3, potential 80, 91–6 205, 219 protein 32 as digital signal 39, 82 redundant conduction velocity 26, 67 element 185 frequency 39, 74 signal 108 initiation 52, 65, 84, 192 reflex 54, 77 initiation zone 38, 81 gill withdrawal 202–6, 236 spinal cord 190 local bending 206–11 starfish 186 locust leg 212–18 steroid 221, 224, 245 © Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org Cambridge University Press 0521627265 - Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour, Second Edition Peter J Simmons and David Young Index More information 266 Index stomatogastric ganglion 194–8 tight junction 66 stretch receptor 178, 225–7 time summation 38, 202 constant 36 supernormal stimulus 7 marker neuron 153 superposition eye 88 Tipula 95 swimming toad 9, 70 crayfish 48–9, 54–5 tobacco hornworm 222 tadpole 190–4 tonic response 54, 82, 94 Tritonia 186, 188–90 tonotopic arrangement 137 syllable 237, 240–1 topographic map 100 synapse 30, 36–40, 52, 103, 125, 167, 214, transduction 79, 85, 92 218, 227, 236, 243–4 transmitter 196 mixed 49 trichoid sensillum 212 strength 52, 221, 236, 244 tritocerebral commissure giant neuron types of 30–3 (TCG) 182 synaptic connection 46, 102, 211 tuning 112, 150, 154, 157, 159–60 syrinx 337–8 Tyto 132 tadpole 186, 190 velocity 145 Taeniopygia 237 vertebrate 4, 22–3, 85, 165, 190 tail flip 44 Vespertilionidae 143 tectal cells 12–15, 135–8 VS neuron 112 tectum 11, 135, 138 VUMX1 neuron 233–5 tegula 178 Teleogryllus 174 wind-sensitive hairs 169 teleost 61 wing temporal summation 38, 202 muscles 167 Tenodera 16, 89 proprioceptors 178–82 thalamus 11–14 threshold Xenopus 186, 190 curve 150–1 for spike 28, 38–9, 52, 57 zebra finch 237 sensory 147 zebra fish 61 © Cambridge University Press www.cambridge.org.
Recommended publications
  • Adaptive Rescaling Maximizes Information Transmission
    Neuron, Vol. 26, 695±702, June, 2000, Copyright 2000 by Cell Press Adaptive Rescaling Maximizes Information Transmission Naama Brenner, William Bialek,* Adaptation to mean light level ensures that our visual and Rob de Ruyter van Steveninck responses are matched to the average signal in real NEC Research Institute time, thus maintaining sensitivity to the fluctuations Princeton, New Jersey 08540 around this mean. But the fluctuations themselves are intermittent, such that periods of large fluctuations are interspersed with periods of relative ªquiet.º Recent ob- Summary servations indicate that the intermittency of natural sig- nals is of a special form: statistics such as the variance Adaptation is a widespread phenomenon in nervous and correlation function are stationary over some re- systems, providing flexibility to function under varying gions, with slowly modulated parameters over larger external conditions. Here, we relate an adaptive prop- regions (Ruderman and Bialek, 1994; Nelken et al., 1999). erty of a sensory system directly to its function as a The principle of efficient coding suggests that the ner- carrier of information about input signals. We show vous system should adapt its strategy to these local that the input/output relation of a sensory system in statistical properties of the stimulus. Evidence for such a dynamic environment changes with the statistical statistical adaptation in the early stages of vision has properties of the environment. Specifically, when the been found in the fly (van Hateren, 1997) and in the dynamic range of inputs changes, the input/output vertebrate retina (Smirnakis et al., 1997), where mecha- relation rescales so as to match the dynamic range of nisms of gain control are well known (Shapley and Victor, responses to that of the inputs.
    [Show full text]
  • Lobula Plate Tangential Cells in Drosophila Melanogaster; Response Properties, Synaptic Organisation & Input Channels”
    Lobula Plate Tangential Cells in Drosophila melanogaster ; Response Properties, Synaptic Organization & Input Channels Maximilian Albert Jösch Krotki München 2009 2 Lobula Plate Tangential Cells in Drosophila melanogaster ; Response Properties, Synaptic Organization & Input Channels Dissertation Zur Erlangung des Doktorgrades der Naturwissenschaften (Dr.rer.nat.) der Fakultät für Biologie der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München Angefertigt am Max-Planck-Institüt für Neurobiologie, Abteilung „Neuronale Informationsverarbeitung‟ Vorgelegt von Maximilian Albert Jösch Krotki München 2009 3 Hiermit erkläre ich, daß ich die vorliegende Dissertation selbständig und ohne unerlaubte Hilfe angefertigt habe. Sämtliche Experimente wurden von mir selbst durchgeführt, außer wenn explizit auf Dritte verwiesen wird. Ich habe weder anderweitig versucht, eine Dissertation oder Teile einer Dissertation einzureichen bzw. einer Prüfungskommission vorzulegen, noch eine Doktorprüfung durchzuführen. München, den 01.10.2009 1. Gutachter: Prof. Dr. Alexander Borst 2. Gutachter: Prof. Dr. Rainer Uhl Tag der mündlichen Prüfung: 26. November 2009 4 Table of Contents Table of Contents .............................................................................................................................. 5 List of Figures ..................................................................................................................................... 8 1 Abstract ........................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Coding of Time-Varying Signals in Spike Trains of Integrate-And-Fire Neurons with Random Threshold
    Communicated by Anthony Zador ~ Coding of Time-Varying Signals in Spike Trains of Integrate-and-Fire Neurons with Random Threshold Recently, methods of statistical estimation theory have been applied by Bialek and collaborators (1991) to reconstruct time-varying velocity signals and to investigate the processing of visual information by a di- rectionally selective motion detector in the fly's visual system, the H1 cell. We summarize here our theoretical results obtained by studying these reconstructions starting from a simple model of H1 based on ex- perimental data. Under additional technical assumptions, we derive a closed expression for the Fourier transform of the optimal reconstruc- tion filter in terms of the statistics of the stimulus and the characteris- tics of the model neuron, such as its firing rate. It is shown that linear reconstruction filters will change in a nontrivial way if the statistics of the signal or the mean firing rate of the cell changes. Analytical expressions are then derived for the mean square error in the recon- structions and the lower bound on the rate of information transmission that was estimated experimentally by Bialek et al. (1991). For plausible values of the parameters, the model is in qualitative agreement with experimental data. We show that the rate of information transmission and mean square error represent different measures of the reconstruc- tions: in particular, satisfactory reconstructions in terms of the mean square error can be achieved only using stimuli that are matched to the properties of the recorded cell. Finally, it is shown that at least for the class of models presented here, reconstruction methods can be understood as a generalization of the more familiar reverse-correlation technique.
    [Show full text]
  • Toward an Ecology of Visual Information Processing
    15_Invert_381-404.qxd 5/9/07 4:01 PM Page 381 15 Going Wild: Toward an Ecology of Visual Information Processing Jochen Zeil, Norbert Boeddeker, Jan M. Hemmi, and Wolfgang Stürzl ARC Centre of Excellence in Vision Science and Centre for Visual Sciences Research School of Biological Sciences The Australian National University Canberra ACT 2601, Australia WITH ALL OUR EXTENSIVE KNOWLEDGE OF SENSORY CODING, sensory- motor feedback loops, and motor control, we are still largely ignorant about what enables animals to be such successful autonomous agents. Even in arthropods with their tiny brains, we do not fully understand how behaviorally relevant information is acquired, filtered, and processed to guide behavior. This poses a fundamental challenge to neurobiology (e.g., see Pflüger and Menzel 1999): Most of what we know about the neural basis of visual processing, for instance, comes from experiments using well-defined simple patterns, such as black and white stripes or ran- dom dots, which were chosen to facilitate our interpretation of the neu- ronal responses. In addition, in most cases, we have no alternative but to study neurons out of context, in stripped-down organisms and in iso- lated or sliced-up brain preparations, in order to be able to measure their response properties. Neurophysiological studies have revealed a wealth of information on neurons and many of their interesting properties, but these properties are not necessarily the only ones that are relevant under natural operating conditions. Nervous systems are not general information transmission and processing devices, but they have evolved to solve specific computational tasks that are relevant to the survival and the fitness of an animal in a given environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Encoding of Motion in Real Time by the Fly Visual System Martin Egelhaaf* and Anne-Kathrin Warzecha†
    454 Encoding of motion in real time by the fly visual system Martin Egelhaaf* and Anne-Kathrin Warzecha† Direction-selective cells in the fly visual system that have large By what mechanisms, and how reliably, time-dependent receptive fields play a decisive role in encoding the time- stimuli are encoded by the nervous system is currently dependent optic flow the animal encounters during locomotion. being investigated intensively in the context of optic Recent experiments on the computations performed by these flow processing in the visual systems of various animals, cells have highlighted the significance of dendritic integration such as monkeys, pigeons and flies. The fly is highly and have addressed the role of spikes versus graded specialised to evaluate time-dependent optic flow to membrane potential changes in encoding optic flow information. control its often virtuosic visually guided orientation It is becoming increasingly clear that the way optic flow is behaviour. In the fly, it is possible to track motion infor- encoded in real time is constrained both by the computational mation processing physiologically from the retina to needs of the animal in visually guided behaviour as well as by visual orientation behaviour [3]. the specific properties of the underlying neuronal hardware. Optic flow is initially processed in the first and second Addresses visual areas of the fly’s brain by successive layers of retino- Lehrstuhl für Neurobiologie, Fakultät für Biologie, Universität Bielefeld, topically arranged columnar neurons. There is detailed Postfach 10 01 31, D-33501 Bielefeld, Germany knowledge on the first visual area, the lamina, both at the *e-mail: [email protected] ultrastructural and functional level.
    [Show full text]
  • Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour Second Edition
    Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour Second Edition This new edition of Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour has been updated and expanded by Peter Simmons and David Young in order to offer a comprehensive introduction to the field of neuroethology while still maintaining the accessibility of the book to university students. Two new chapters have been added, broadening the scope of the book by describing changes in behaviour and how networks of nerve cells control behaviour. The book explains the way in which the nervous systems of animals control behaviour without assuming that the reader has any prior knowledge of neurophysiology. Using a carefully selected series of behaviour patterns, students are taken from an elementary-level introduction to a point at which sufficient detail has been assimilated to allow a satisfying insight into current research on how nervous systems control and generate behaviour. Only examples for which it has been possible to establish a clear link between the activity of particular nerve cells and a pattern of behaviour have been used. Important and possibly unfamiliar terminology is defined directly or by context when it first appears and is printed in bold type. At the end of each chapter, the authors have added a list of suggestions for further reading, and specific topics are highlighted in boxes within the text. Nerve Cells and Animal Behaviour is essential reading for undergraduate and graduate students of zoology, psychology and physiology and serves as a clear introduction to the field of neuroethology. is a Lecturer in the Department of Neurobiology, University of Newcastle upon Tyne, UK, and is a Reader in the Department of Zoology, University of Melbourne, Australia.
    [Show full text]
  • Universality and Individuality in a Neural Code
    Universality and individuality in a neural code Elad Schneidman,1;2 Naama Brenner,3 Naftali Tishby,1;3 Rob R. de Ruyter van Steveninck,3 William Bialek3 1School of Computer Science and Engineering, Center for Neural Computation and 2Department of Neurobiology, Hebrew University, Jerusalem 91904, Israel 3NEC Research Institute, 4 Independence Way, Princeton, New Jersey 08540, USA felads,[email protected], fbialek,ruyter,[email protected] Abstract The problem of neural coding is to understand how sequences of action potentials (spikes) are related to sensory stimuli, motor out- puts, or (ultimately) thoughts and intentions. One clear question is whether the same coding rules are used by different neurons, or by corresponding neurons in different individuals. We present a quantitative formulation of this problem using ideas from informa- tion theory, and apply this approach to the analysis of experiments in the fly visual system. We find significant individual differences in the structure of the code, particularly in the way that tempo- ral patterns of spikes are used to convey information beyond that available from variations in spike rate. On the other hand, all the flies in our ensemble exhibit a high coding efficiency, so that every spike carries the same amount of information in all the individuals. Thus the neural code has a quantifiable mixture of individuality and universality. 1 Introduction When two people look at the same scene, do they see the same things? This basic question in the theory of knowledge seems to be beyond the scope of experimental investigation. An accessible version of this question is whether different observers of the same sense data have the same neural representation of these data: how much of the neural code is universal, and how much is individual? Differences in the neural codes of different individuals may arise from various sources: First, different individuals may use different `vocabularies' of coding sym- bols.
    [Show full text]
  • On the Biological Plausibility of Grandmother Cells: Implications for Neural Network Theories in Psychology and Neuroscience
    Psychological Review © 2009 American Psychological Association 2009, Vol. 116, No. 1, 220–251 0033-295X/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0014462 On the Biological Plausibility of Grandmother Cells: Implications for Neural Network Theories in Psychology and Neuroscience Jeffrey S. Bowers University of Bristol A fundamental claim associated with parallel distributed processing (PDP) theories of cognition is that knowledge is coded in a distributed manner in mind and brain. This approach rejects the claim that knowledge is coded in a localist fashion, with words, objects, and simple concepts (e.g. “dog”), that is, coded with their own dedicated representations. One of the putative advantages of this approach is that the theories are biologically plausible. Indeed, advocates of the PDP approach often highlight the close parallels between distributed representations learned in connectionist models and neural coding in brain and often dismiss localist (grandmother cell) theories as biologically implausible. The author reviews a range a data that strongly challenge this claim and shows that localist models provide a better account of single-cell recording studies. The author also contrast local and alternative distributed coding schemes (sparse and coarse coding) and argues that common rejection of grandmother cell theories in neuro- science is due to a misunderstanding about how localist models behave. The author concludes that the localist representations embedded in theories of perception and cognition are consistent with neuro- science; biology only calls into question the distributed representations often learned in PDP models. Keywords: grandmother cells, distributed representations, parallel distributed processing, localist repre- sentation, connectionism Of the many different schools of neural network modeling, that it is not a principle of neural network theories in general (cf.
    [Show full text]
  • FROM INSECTS to ROBOTS Nicolas Franceschini
    ACT Workshop on Innovative Concepts. ESA-ESTEC 28-29 January 2008. TOWARDS AUTOMATIC VISUAL GUIDANCE OF AEROSPACE VEHICLES: FROM INSECTS TO ROBOTS Nicolas Franceschini Biorobotics Movement Science Institute Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS) and University of the Mediterranean 163, Avenue de Luminy (CP938) MARSEILLE F-13288, cedex 9, France [email protected] ABSTRACT Equipped with a less-than-one-milligram brain, insects fly autonomously in complex environments without resorting to any Radars, Ladars, Sonars or GPS. The knowledge gained during the last decades on insects’ sensory-motor abilities and the neuronal substrates involved has provided us with a rich source of inspiration for designing tomorrow’s self- guided vehicles and micro-vehicles, which are to cope with unforeseen events on the ground, in the air, under water or in space. Insects have been in the business of sensory-motor integration for more than 100 millions years. They can teach us useful tricks for designing agile autonomous vehicles at various scales. Constructing a “biorobot” first requires exactly formulating the signal processing principles at work in the animal. It gives us, in return, a unique opportunity of checking the soundness and robustness of those principles by bringing them face to face with the real physical world. Here we describe some of the visually-guided terrestrial and aerial robots we have developed on the basis of our biological findings. All these robots react to the optic flow (i.e., the angular speed of the retinal image). Optic flow is sensed onboard the robots by miniature vision sensors called Elementary Motion Detectors (EMDs).
    [Show full text]
  • Variability in Spike Trains During Constant and Dynamic Stimulation
    R EPORTS 19. Typically, pairs of stacks of images of secondary and tertiary dendritic branches (test and control sites) Variability in Spike Trains were collected at ;15-min intervals. Images were stored digitally and analyzed offline essentially un- processed. The numbers and lengths of protrusions During Constant and Dynamic (lower limit, 0.54 mm) in a field of view (45 mmby45 mm) containing 69 6 22 mm of dendritic length were measured, keeping track of the fates of individual Stimulation structures in optical sections. The limited z-resolution of our microscope (;1.5 mm) did not allow us to reliably detect and analyze protrusions that did not Anne-Kathrin Warzecha* and Martin Egelhaaf project laterally from the dendritic shaft, resulting in an underestimate of the densities of protrusions. In a recent study, it was concluded that natural time-varying stimuli are Measurements were done in two-dimensional projec- represented more reliably in the brain than constant stimuli are. The results tions, resulting in an underestimate of the lengths of protrusions. To calculate the error resulting from presented here disagree with this conclusion, although they were obtained from projections, let r be the dendritic radius and l the the same identified neuron (H1) in the fly’s visual system. For large parts of the length of spiny protrusions. Then the length of the neuron’s activity range, the variability of the responses was very similar for projection of the protrusion—the measured length l9—is given by l95[(l 1 r) cosu –r], where u is the constant and time-varying stimuli and was considerably smaller than that in angle with respect to the horizontal.
    [Show full text]
  • Insect Inspired Visual Motion Sensing and Flying Robots
    Insect inspired visual motion sensing and flying robots Thibaut Raharijaona, Lubin Kerhuel, Julien Serres, Frédéric Roubieu, Fabien Expert, Stéphane Viollet, Franck Ruffier, Nicolas Franceschini To cite this version: Thibaut Raharijaona, Lubin Kerhuel, Julien Serres, Frédéric Roubieu, Fabien Expert, et al.. Insect inspired visual motion sensing and flying robots. Handbook of Biomimetics and Bioinspiration: 2 Electromechanical Systems, 2014, 978-981-435-4950. 10.1142/9789814354936_0022. hal-02294399 HAL Id: hal-02294399 https://hal-amu.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-02294399 Submitted on 23 Sep 2019 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Chapter 1 Insect Inspired Visual Motion Sensing and Bio-Inspired Flying Robots Thibaut Raharijaona, Lubin Kerhuel, Julien Serres, Frédéric Roubieu, Fabien Expert, Stéphane Viollet, Franck Ruffier and Nicolas Franceschini ∗ Aix-Marseille University, CNRS, Institute of Movement Science, Biorobotics Department, UMR7287, 13288, Marseille, France Flying insects excellently master visual motion sensing techniques. They use dedi- cated motion processing circuits at a low energy and computational costs. Thanks to observations obtained on insect visual guidance, we developed visual motion sensors and bio-inspired autopilots dedicated to flying robots. Optic flow-based visuomotor control systems have been implemented on an increasingly large num- ber of sighted autonomous robots.
    [Show full text]
  • The Architecture of the Desert Ant's Navigational Toolkit (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)
    Myrmecological News 12 85-96 Vienna, September 2009 The architecture of the desert ant's navigational toolkit (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) Rüdiger WEHNER Abstract In the last decades North African desert ants of the genus Cataglyphis FOERSTER, 1850 – and more recently their eco- logical equivalents in the Namib desert (Ocymyrmex EMERY, 1886) and Australia (Melophorus LUBBOCK, 1883) – have become model organisms for the study of insect navigation. While foraging individually over distances of many thou- sand times their body lengths in featureless as well as cluttered terrain, they navigate predominantly by visual means using vector navigation (path integration) and landmark-guidance mechanisms as well as systematic-search and target- expansion strategies as their main navigational tools. In vector navigation they employ several ways of acquiring infor- mation about directions steered (compass information) and distances covered (odometer information). In landmark guid- ance they rely on view-based information about visual scenes obtained at certain vantage points and combined with certain steering (motor) commands of what to do next. By exploring how these various navigational routines interact, the current position paper provides a hypothesis of what the architecture of the ant's navigational toolkit might look like. The hypothesis is built on the assumption that the toolkit consists of a number of domain-specific routines. Even though these routines are quite rigidly preordained (and get modified during the ant's lifetime by strictly task-dependent, rapid learning processes), they interact quite flexibly in various, largely context-dependent ways. However, they are not suited to provide the ant with cartographic information about the locations of places within the animal's foraging environment.
    [Show full text]