<<

Perception & Psychophysics 1993, 54 (6), 794-807 Harmonic accents in inference of metrical structure and perception of patterns

LLOYD A. DAWE, JOHN R. PLAT!', and RONALD J. RACINE McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada

Research on rhythmic structure is somewhat fragmented, due in part to differential use ofter­ minology and a lack of research on the timing of harmonic accents. In this study, a harmonic and a temporal accent were pitted against each other in such a way as to form different rhythm patterns. In addition, two harmonic conditions that varied in the frequency of chord presenta­ tions (i.e., the composite rhythm) but not in the frequency of chord changes (i.e., the harmonic rhythm) were presented. Musicians and nonmusicians were requested to report perceived rhythm patterns in an attempt to determine the relative salience of the harmonic and temporal accents. In addition, a behavioral measure of the perceived meter was taken. Results indicated that the location of chord changes was the main determinant of subjects' rhythmic perceptions and the perceived onset of a measure. Furthermore, although subjects primarily inferred different meters based on the composite rhythm, an interaction of metrical and rhythmic choices was found, in­ dicating that perception of rhythm patterns and inference of metrical structure may not always be independent.

Although there is a large body of literature on the per­ focus on more specific aspects of music. Generally, rhythm ception of rhythmic structure in music, certain relevant refers to any form of reiteration. More specific use of the variables remain uninvestigated. In particular, although term has been in reference to the meter-the regular recur­ the timing of salient harmonic events plays an important rence of grouped strong and weak beats, or heavily and part in most musical theories of rhythm, empirical inves­ lightly accented tones. Cooper and Meyer (1960) defined tigations of its role are lacking. This article reports an rhythm as the way in which one or two unaccented events experiment that was designed as an initial attempt to in­ are grouped in relation to an accented event; and to many, vestigate the timing of chord presentations and changes the term is used to refer to the temporal aspects of the as cues for the perception of rhythm and the inference duration of individual notes and rests (e.g., Jones, 1987). of meter. Some theorists (Agmon, 1990; Boretz, 1971; Lerdahl We have identified two possible reasons for the cur­ & Jackendoff, 1983) have thought of rhythm as being rent inadequate state of research on rhythm. First, the multi-leveled, with the structure at each rhythmic level study of rhythmic structure lacks acceptance of a com­ being dependent upon the pattern of time spans at lower mon terminology; and second, there is a lack of studies rhythmic levels. Events that differ in perceptual salience concerned with the temporal pattern ofharmonic accents. create the hierarchy. Highly salient events serve as de­ The resulting difficulties are discussed in the following fining boundaries for long durations at the upper end of sections. the hierarchy, while less salient events serve as the de­ fining boundaries ofshorter time spans lower in the hier­ A Definition of Rhythm archy. To start with the lowest level, the term rhythm has Several researchers employ the term "rhythm" in ref­ been used to refer to the surface duration pattern that re­ erence to different phenomena. While it is true that all sults from accents associated with the onset and termina­ studies of rhythmic structure can be thought ofas analy­ tion of individual notes and rests. We refer to this com­ ses ofthe pattern of time durations that results from vari­ ponent ofrhythmic structure as the temporal aspect of the ous events that occur in a musical flow, researchers often musical presentation. In keeping with traditional music­ theoretical notions, two intermediate-level duration pat­ terns are of importance for the perception of rhythmic This research was supported by a predoctoral fellowship from the Nat­ structure (Lee, 1985; Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983; Palmer ural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to L.A.D. & Krumhansl, 1990). First, the regular reiteration of and by Grant A8269 from the same body to J.R.P. We are indebted strong and weak beats or pulses and the hierarchical pat­ to several individuals for suggestions and comments on an earlier draft tern of time spans that results from this regular reitera­ ofthis paper; in particular, we would like to thank Kate Kewish, Mari tion are referred to as the "metrical structure." Second, Riess Jones, and two anonymous reviewers for their insightful critiques. Requests for reprints may be addressed to L. A. Dawe, Department of the pattern of durations based on salient timbre-based, Psychology, Social Science Centre, University of Westem Ontario, Lon­ temporal, harmonic, and melodic phenomenal events (e.g., don, ON, Canada N6A 5C2. chord changes, relatively large interval jumps, and rests)

Copyright 1993 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 794 HARMONIC ACCENTS, RHYTHM, AND METER INFERENCE 795 that serves as the basis of themes, motives, sections, and chord, and that associated with a change of chord. Both phrases is called "phrase structure." Finally, the overall types ofevents are based on a chord's onset, with the dis­ musical structure that results from the interaction of phrase tinction resting on whether the chord being presented is and metrical structure is referred to as "rhythm." I the same or different from that previously presented. This As such, rhythm can be defined as the complex pattern distinction, although subtle, is important not only theo­ of successive and simultaneous time spans that result from retically but also functionally. While it is true that all phe­ the interaction of a phrase and the metrical structure, both nomenal accenting associated with a change of chord is of which are, to varying degrees, themselves dependent also associated with the accenting resulting from chord on different timbre-based, melodic, harmonic, and tem­ onset, the reverse is not necessarily the case. One can have poral events. An adequate delineation of rhythmic struc­ multiple presentations of the same chord. This leads to ture will depend, therefore, on identification of the per­ a consideration of two important constructs in most music­ ceptually salient events upon which the complex pattern theoretical accounts of rhythm-harmonic rhythm and of time spans is built, as well as of the relationship be­ composite rhythm. tween phrase and meter. Harmonic Rhythm and Composite Rhythm Harmonic Accents: Phenomenal and Harmonic rhythm is the sequential pattern of durations Structural Accents provided either by chord changes or by implied triadic All rhythmic cues can be categorized on the basis of the patterns within a melody (Piston, 1948; Smith & Cuddy, locus of their perceptual salience. One useful categoriza­ 1989). Composite rhythm is the sequential pattern ofdu­ tion is based on the difference between structural and phe­ rations associated with the onset of individual note and nomenal accenting, which was made explicit by Lerdahl chord presentations in a polyphony. To determine the com­ and Jackendoff (1983). The distinction is founded upon posite rhythm one need only initiate a new timed duration the view that perception is based on both data-driven (i.e., whenever a note in the polyphony begins or ends. In short, concerned with phenomenal accents) and conceptually the composite rhythm will consist ofa series of the notes driven (i.e., concerned with structural accents) process­ of shortest duration across the multiple lines of music. ing. Phenomenal accents have been defined as any events As with the distinction between the two harmonic phe­ at the musical surface that give emphasis or stress to a nomenal accents discussed above, harmonic rhythm con­ moment in the musical flow (Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983). tributes to and is part of the composite rhythm, but the Examples ofphenomenal accents include relatively large composite rhythm can display serial time patterns not ap­ interval jumps, contour changes, changes in dynamics, parent in the harmonic rhythm. Thus, while it is theoret­ notes of relatively long duration, chord changes, and so ically possible for the harmonic and composite forth. Structural accents result from more abstract prop­ in a musical presentation to be identical (e.g., when the erties and cognitive principles associated with tonal and onset ofevery note creates a new chord), it is usually the diatonic organization. Although not explicitly linked to case that the harmonic rhythm represents higher-order the accent identifications, the work of Krurnhansl and her temporal relationships. colleagues offers a general basis from which one might Several theorists and researchers have focused on the identify structural relationships between various notes, role of harmonic rhythm in the perception of a metrical chords, and tonal centers or keys (Bharucha & Krumhansl, structure (Cooper & Meyer, 1960; Piston, 1948; Smith 1983; Cuddy, Cohen, & Miller, 1979; Krumhansl, 1979, & Cuddy, 1989). The consensus is that in Western tonal 1990; Krurnhansl, Bharucha, & Kessler, 1982; Krurnhansl music, harmonic changes coincide with important metri­ & Castellano, 1983; Krurnhansl & Shepard, 1979). cal locations, especially the first beat ofthe measure. Little Traditionally, researchers have emphasized the struc­ empirical research has been conducted on the role of com­ tural facet of a harmonic presentation (e.g., the hierar­ posite rhythm in the perception of meter and phrase. Ac­ chical ordering ofharmonic stability) rather than the phe­ cording to several theorists, however, the serial durations nomenal aspects of chord presentations. With structural created by individual note onsets, in addition to the , accenting of a chord presentation (e.g., chord progres­ will determine the perception of iterative pulses from sions), the emphasis is placed on how the harmonic event which one can infer a metrical structure (Lerdahl & Jack­ relates to other chord presentations either in the future endoff, 1983; Longuet-Higgins & Lee, 1982, 1984; or in the past within a tonal context. In contrast, with the Longuet-Higgins & Steedman, 1971; Steedman, 1977). In phenomenal accenting of a chord change, concern is with addition, some theorists (e.g., Lerdahl & Jackendoff, the event itself, separate from any tonal relationships. This 1983) believe that the choice of tactus (i.e., the metrical phenomenal/structural distinction in chord presentations level at which a listener taps his or her foot) and meter may have some use for investigative purposes. When the is related to the timing of harmonic accents. However, phenomenal facet ofharmonic presentations is the focus, the specific relationship between harmonic and compos­ the timing of chord presentations and chord changes is ite rhythm in rhythm perception has yet to be investigated. much more the principal interest than when the focus is In addition to the relationship between harmonic and strictly on structural relationships. composite rhythms, two other lines of investigation found This study deals with two types of harmonic phenome­ in the psychological literature are relevant to this trea­ nal accents: that associated with the presentation of a tise. The first is the notion of accent weights and the role 796 DAWE, PLATT, AND RACINE of accent coincidence in phrase structure investigations; responding to strong metrical locations and to shorten the the second, the focus of metrical-structure investigations, duration of notes that fall on weak beats (Clarke, 1985; has to do with the nature of meter and its relationship to Gabrielsson, 1974, 1987) and to use melodic features such phrase structure. as a subtle change in dynamics to communicate the meter (Schachter, 1980; Sloboda, 1983; Yeston, 1976). There Phrase-Structure Investigations are problems associated with this evidence, however. One The literature on rhythmic structure contains several could reasonably argue that subtle variations of notes cor­ areas of dispute, notably that regarding the role of phe­ responding to metrically significant locations detract from nomenal and structural accents in the perception of phrase accurate perception ofa metrical structure. That is, inso­ structure (Jones, 1987; Palmer & Krumhansl, 1987a, far as a performance fails to produce the precise time ra­ 1987b). Some theorists(e.g., Palmer & Krumhansl, 1987a, tios upon which a metrical structure is built, perceived 1987b) acknowledge subtle differences in the perceptual meter may differ from the scored meter (Jones, 1990). salience of various melodic or temporal events, but insist Arguments that subtle performance cues mark important that the two theoretical component categories combine in metrical locations would be strengthened if there was evi­ an additive fashion to determine the perceived structure. dence that performers consistently employed them, but Others (e.g., Jones, 1987) have argued that events either the evidence suggests the contrary (Jones, 1990; Sloboda, are or are not accents, leaving the notion of accent strength 1983; Vos & Handel, 1987). Many of the cues found to (i.e., perceptual salience) dependent on coincidence of occasionallymark metrically important locations are either melodic and temporal features, which can then be used absent or used to mark phrase boundaries in performance to direct attention to anticipated events at regular time in­ (Palmer & Krumhansl, 1990), and are thus ambiguous tervals (Jones, 1987). Recently, Jones (1992) has modi­ communicators of the meter. Yet more often than not, fied her views to include the notion of accent weights­ listeners unambiguously perceive the "correct" metrical an acknowledgment that phenomenal accents can differ structure. in their perceptual salience. She differs principally from Some theorists believe that meter is independent of Palmer and Krumhansl (1987a, 1987b) in that, accord­ phrase structure and is an inferred mental structure (Ben­ ing to her updated theory (Jones, 1992), perception of jamin, 1984; Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983). Evidence sup­ melodic structure is based upon an interactive rather than porting this viewpoint is found in the fact that exact time an additive model of temporal and pitch events. ratios are rarely, if ever, produced by performers (Gabriels­ One of the main difficulties with these theories has been son, 1986, 1987; Jones, 1990; Palmer, 1989; Palmer & the exclusion of harmonic phenomenal events from both Krumhansl, 1990). The basis for inferenceof metrical struc­ theoretical discussions and investigations of phrase struc­ ture is rarely agreed upon. Lerdahl and Jackendoff (1983) ture. It is possible that harmonic events are perceptually believe that metrical accents arise from the existence of more salient than events within the other theoretical com­ a periodic mental framework or schema. To establish a ponent categories (i.e., timbre-based, temporal, and me­ particular metrical structure, a listener must infer the lodic). Theorists have long emphasized the fundamental metrical accents from the location of phenomenal and role of harmonic functions in traditional Western music. structural accents in the music. However, according to Indeed, it was undoubtedly this emphasis that led to abun­ Lerdahl and Jackendoff, metrical accents themselves are dant research directed at the structural relationships of defined as qualitatively distinct types of accents. Simi­ harmonic events (e.g., Bharucha & Krumhansl, 1983; larly, Benjamin (1984) claimed that the role of meter is Krumhansl, 1979; Krumhansl et al., 1982; Krumhansl & to give time points an identity independent of the tonal, Castellano, 1983; Krumhansl & Shepard, 1979). motivic, or harmonic accents present. Several researchers and theorists (Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983; Longuet­ Metrical-Structure Investigations Higgins & Lee, 1982, 1984; Longuet-Higgins & Steed­ There is also considerable debate regarding both the ba­ man, 1971; Steedman, 1977) have suggested various rules sis for metrical structure and its relationship to phrase that a listener might use to infer meter. Despite differ­ structure. While most will agree that phrase structure and ences among these accounts, it is possible to discern a metrical structure are two theoretically distinct structural common underlying assumption-namely, that the temporal components of music, it is questionable whether the two phenomenal accent of a note of relatively long duration structures are perceptually independent. While some the­ serves as a cue for the perception of metrical structure. orists argue that meter is predominantly dependent upon In addition, music-theoretical views point to the role of the occurrence of direct phenomenal accents in the micro­ harmonic and composite rhythms, although to date the structure of the music (e.g., Berry, 1976;Cooper & Meyer, amount ofempirical research that has been conducted to 1960), others claim it is an inferred mental structure (e.g., verify the psychological importance of these factors is very Benjamin, 1984; Dawe, Platt, & Racine, in press; Ler­ small (e.g., Dawe et al., in press; Smith & Cuddy, 1989). dahl & Jackendoff, 1983; Palmer & Krumhansl, 1990). Proponents of the position that metrical structure is per­ The Current Investigation ceived directly from the music point to research on the Many of the issues discussed above were addressed in performance of musical compositions which has found the present study. With respect to the phrase-structure musicians to slightly elongate the duration of notes cor- literature, we were interested in determining the relative HARMONIC ACCENTS, RHYTHM, AND METER INFERENCE 797 accent weight of temporal and harmonic accents and the Short duration condition possible role of accent coincidence in defining perceptu­ J jl J~ jl jl J jl jl jl jl ally salient events in the music. In order to investigate these issues, a harmonic accent (i.e., a chord change) was ~:;:?~~~~ systematically pitted against a temporal accent. The tem­ poral accent was created by playing a note of relatively Composite: J! ))) )) l jl ) l )) long duration (L) followed by four notes of short dura­ tion (S). Chord-change locations were systematically Harmonic: ][ ] varied relative to the location of the temporal accent, re­ sulting in 5 possible harmonically defined rhythm patterns (LSSSS, SLSSS, SSLSS, SSSLS, and SSSSL). Subjects were requested to report the perceived rhythms. Based on subjects' responses, the relative phenomenal accent weight for the temporal and harmonic accents in the phras­ b). Long duration condition ing of music could be determined. It was assumed that if the two phenomenal events differed in their perceptual J 1> 1> 1> 1> ~~:~)l~)~) ~ salience, the more prominent event would define the ~: phrase boundary and thus the perceived rhythm pattern. A temporal accent rather than a melodic or timbre-based accent was selected to be pitted against the harmonic ac­ Composite: J )))) J cent because of its known role as a boundary cue. With ][ meter, long-duration notes tend to be heard as initiating Harmonic: major metrical units of time (Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983; Figure 1. The composite rhythm and harmonic rhythm patterns Longuet-Higgins & Lee, 1982, 1984; Longuet-Higgins of stimuli. & Steedman, 1971; Steedman, 1977). In contrast, long durations tend to mark the end of phrases in music (Boltz, 1989; Dowling, 1973; Garner, 1974; Royer & Garner, that meter. Figure 2 illustrates that the onset of some 1966, 1970; Vos, 1977; Woodrow, 1909) as well as in rhythm patterns, as defined by the location of a chord speech (Cooper, Paccia, & LaPointe, 1978; Lehiste, change, would coincide with accented locations in a 3/4 1975; Oller, 1973; Scott, 1982). and 6/8 metrical structure if one assumes that major metri­ We were also interested in determining the relationship cal time spans were inferred to begin with the onset of between meter and phrase structure. We wanted to de­ the temporal accent. This figure also shows an important termine (a) whether subjects would consistently and un­ constraint of metrical structure in traditional Western ambiguously infer a metrical structure in the absence of music, as well as the relationship between 3/4 and 6/8 performance cues in the microstructure of the music (as time. In traditional Western music, meter involves a hi­ suggested by the work of Essens & Povel, 1985; Povel, erarchy of simultaneous time spans that either double or 1981; Povel & Essens, 1985; and Povel & Okkerman, triple in duration from one lower level to the next higher 1981), and (b) upon what basis such an inference would one. This is evident in the time-span reduction (after Ler­ be made. Specifically, we wanted to determine the rela­ dahl & Jackendoff, 1983) in which dots represent beat tive contribution of composite and harmonic rhythms to events in time. the inference of a metrical structure. In order to address Movement from the lowest metrical level to the inter­ this issue, two harmonic conditions that varied in the fre­ mediate level can involve the doubling of the duration be­ quency of chord presentations (i.e., the composite rhythm) tween beats, which is followed by a tripling as one moves but not in the frequency of chord changes (i.e., the har­ from the intermediate to the next highest metrical level. monic rhythm) were presented. In Figure 1, the compos­ In this case, one would hear a meter of 3/4 time in which ite rhythm patterns are located immediately below the there is a strong accent (indicated by the number of dots staves. The harmonic rhythm is represented by the group­ beneath each specific location) associated with the onset ing below the composite rhythm. While only one exam­ of the first note (see Example A, Figure 2). Coincidence ple is given in each of the long- and short-duration har­ of the chord change with this metrical accent could bias monic conditions, the experiment itself employed five subjects to report the pattern LSSSS. Two slightly weaker stimuli that differed in the relationship between the har­ metrical accents would fall on the first and third eighth monic rhythm and the melodic line (see Table 1). The in­ notes. Chord changes coinciding with these salient metri­ teresting question that arose concerned the meter in which cal locations could bias subjects to report SSSSL (Exam­ subjects would hear these presentations. With a quarter ple B) and SSLSS (Example C), respectively. It should note followed by 4 eighth notes, a multitude of meters be noted that these accents correspond to each notated beat is theoretically possible, but two meters are probable­ in 3/4 time (i.e., 3 beats per measure, with a quarter note 3/4 and 6/8. receiving a beat). Ifa particular meter was inferred it could also bias sub­ Alternatively, movement from the lowest level to the jects to perceive and report rhythm patterns consistent with intermediate level can involve the tripling of the time span 798 DAWE, PLATT, AND RACINE

Example A: Chord-change based rhythm of: LSSSS

Chord Changes: -} -} -¥ Temporal Line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level • J level d. level •

Example B: Chord-change based rhythm of: SSSSL

Chord Changes: ~ ~ Temporal Line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level • • J level • d. level

Example C: Chord-change based rhythm of: SSLSS

Chord Changes: ~ ~ Temporal Line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level • J level d. level •

Example D: Chord-change based rhythm of: LSSSS

Chord Changes: ~ ~ {t Temporal line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level • ••••• •• •• oJ. level d. level •

Example E: Chord-change based rhythm of: SSSLS

Chord Changes: ~ {t Temporal line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level • •• • • J. level d. level

Figure 2. Coincidence of chord-change-based rhythm patterns and metrical accents when major metrical time spans are inferred to begin with the onset of the note of rela­ tively long duration. In Examples A, B, and C, the inferred metrical structure is 3/4 time. In Examples D and E, the meter is 6/8 time. HARMONIC ACCENTS, RHYTHM, AND METER INFERENCE 799

Example A: Chord-change based rhythm of: LSSSS

Chord Changes: ~ ~ ~ Temporal Line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level .I level d. level

Example 8: Chord-change based rhythm of: SSSSL

Chord Changes: ~ ~ Temporal Line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level .I level d. level

Example C: Chord-change based rhythm of: SSLSS

Chord Changes: ~ ~ Temporal Line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level .I level d. level

Example 0: Chord-change based rhythm of: LSSSS Chord Changes: ~ ~ t Temporal Line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level • .I. level d. level

Example E: Chord-change based rhythm of: SSSLS

Chord Changes: ~ ~ Temporal Line: L SSSSL SSSSL Metrical Grid: J level •• .I. level d. level

Figure 3. Coincidence of temporal and metrical accents when major metrical time spans are inferred to begin with the location of a chord change. In Examples A, B, and C, the inferred metrical structure is 3/4 time. In Examples D and E, the meter is 6/8 time. 800 DAWE, PLATT, AND RACINE

followed by a doubling (see Example D, Figure 2). In this within a tonal context. By systematically varying the lo­ case, one would hear a meter of 6/8 time in which there cation of a chord change relative to a long-duration note is a strong accent associated with the onset of the quarter and asking subjects to report the perceived structure and note and a slightly weaker accent falling on the second meter, several current and relevant theoretical issues were eighth note. Chord-change-based rhythm patterns of addressed, notably those concerning the relative accent LSSSS (Example D) and SSSLS (Example E) would coin­ weighting of temporal and harmonic accents and the role cide with these metrical accents. It is important to note of accent coincidence in phrase-structure investigations. that these two metrical accents do not correspond to each The effects of formal musical training on perceived ac­ notated beat in 6/8 time (i.e., in 6/8 time there are 6 beats cent weights was also of interest. Finally, the issue of per measure, with the eighth note receiving one beat). This whether or not subjects can unambiguously infer a metri­ is because the tactus of a musical sequence invariably cor­ cal structure from the composite and harmonic rhythm responds to an intermediate level of the metrical hierar­ in the absence ofperformance cues directly addresses the chy in which the beats pass by at a moderate rate (i.e., relationship of meter to phrase structure. only 2 beats per measure; Lerdahl & lackendoff, 1983). As stated earlier, these predictions are based on the as­ METHOD sumption that the measure would be perceived to begin with the onset of the temporal accent. However, one of Subjects the reasons this investigation was undertaken was to es­ Twenty-four experimentally naive volunteers participated in this study. They were tentatively divided into two groups on the basis tablish whether harmonic and temporal phenomenal ac­ of musical training. There were 12 musicians (5 males and 7 fe­ cents differ in perceptual salience. Subjects may perceive males), with a range of4-11 years and a mean of6.7 years of for­ instead the onset of the measure as coinciding with the mal musical training; their ages ranged from 18-22, with a mean chord change. Nevertheless, the inferred meter could still of 20.1 years. Twelve subjects were nonmusicians, with an age­ bias subjects to perceive and report rhythm patterns con­ range of 19-21 and a mean of 19 years. Four of these subjects were sistent with that meter. male and 8 were female. Subjects were recruited from introduc­ tory psychology classes, andwere given course credit for participat­ Figure 3 shows that if this were to occur, the same ing in the experiment. rhythm patterns found to coincide with the locations of metrical accents in Figure 2 would again be found to coin­ Apparatus cide with metrical accents, but for a different reason. That The stimuli were generated by a Yamaha CX5M music computer is, a bias may be found for the rhythm patterns of LSSSS and presented, via a NAD 3020e integrated amplifier, from two (Example A, Figure 3), SSSSL (Example B), and SSLSS speakers placed approximately 2-3 ft on either side ofa video mon­ itor. Each subject sat at a table and responded to the stimuli by mov­ (Example C) with a 3/4 meter because of the coincidence ing a joystick that controlled the movements of a cursor visible on of the temporal and metrical accents. Similarly, a bias a Hitachi 1481 color monitor, situated directly in front of them, for the rhythm patterns LSSSS (Example D) and SSSLS at eye-level. The cursor had to be moved into one of five appropri­ (Example E) may be found with a 6/8 meter. ately labeled boxes to indicate the response choice for each trial. Finally, we were interested in determining if musical Parameters and data for each trial were automatically recorded and training would result in a difference in the inferred meter stored by the computer on disk. A table and chair were also set and/or a difference in the weighting assigned to the phe­ up for the experimenter, who sat directly behind the subject. nomenal accents under investigation. Musical experience StimuU and, in particular, formal musical instruction may change The stimuli for this experiment consisted of a temporal presen­ one's explicit knowledge and vocabulary ofmusic. Such tation and a corresponding harmonic component. On any given trial, a knowledge base could alter the way a subject perceives one basic rhythm pattern of a long-duration tone (L) followed by the musical structure through conceptually driven process­ four short tones (S) was presented 10 times (LSSSSLSSSS ...). In ing ofincoming information. Thus, the participants in this musical notation, the long-held note was a quarter note (i.e., it received one beat in common time) and the short-duration notes experiment were divided into two groups on the basis of were equal to eighth notes (i.e., they received one halfbeat in com­ whether or not they had ever received formal musical mon time). In real time, the long-duration notes were 444 msec training. and the short-duration notes were 222 msec. There were no pauses between notes. All of the 50 tones presented for each trial were Summary Middle C. The reason for presenting all the tones at the same fre­ quency was in order to eliminate melodic phenomenal accents (e.g. , This experiment was designed to investigate the roles contour changes and large interval jumps) and structural accents. of two functionally distinct types of harmonic phenome­ Different harmonic presentations accompanied the temporal line. nal accents-those resulting from chord onset and those Chords were presented in such a way as to break up the rhythm resulting from chord change-in the perception of rhyth­ pattern in relation to the long-duration tone, resulting in one of five mic structure. These accents are associated with the music­ possible rhythm patterns (i.e., LSSSS, SLSSS, SSLSS, SSSLS, or theoretical notions of composite and harmonic rhythm, SSSSL). The harmonic in all conditions consisted of a diatonic (I-V-I) in the key of C Major. respectively. With its emphasis on the phenomenal aspect Harmonic were presented in a long- and a short­ ofchord presentations, this study departs from traditional duration condition (see Table 1). In the long-duration harmonic con­ approaches to harmonic investigations that have focused dition, each chord was presented for a duration of 3 beats (i.e., on the structural relationship among chord presentations 1,332 msec) before changing. In the short-duration harmonic con- HARMONIC ACCENTS, RHYTHM, AND METER INFERENCE 801

Table I __ ~Schematic ReJJresentation of Stimuli ------Temporal Line Condition SSSSL Long I G 2 G 3 G 4 G 5 Short I 2 3 4 5 Note-This table is read by following the location of a chord change upward to see where it intersects the temporal line. The five create five rhythm patternson the basis of a chord change: LSSSS, SLSSS, SSLSS, SSSLS, and SSSSL.

dition, each chord was presented six times (222 msec each) before It was assumed a priori that if subjects tapped their foot changing, with each presentation having the same duration as a short­ two times per iteration, then they were operating in a 6/8 duration note in the temporal line. The only exception to this gen­ time meter, and that if they tapped their foot three times, eral description of the harmonic conditions was at the beginning they were operating in a 3/4 time meter. This behavioral and end of trials, where the duration between trial onset and chord change and between chord change and trial termination was sys­ consistency was believed to be due to the grouping ofiter­ tematically varied to create the five rhythm patterns (see Table I). ative pulses established by the harmonic rhythm. While It is important to note that the harmonic rhythm patterns are the tapping one's foot four or five times per iteration was the­ same in both harmonic conditions, while the composite rhythm pat­ oretically possible, such behavior would not be consis­ terns differ slightly (see Figure I). The stimuli were presented tent with the pattern set by the composite and harmonic through speakers at approximately 72 dB (SPL), at a tempo of 135 rhythms. beats per min. This tempo was selected to ensure that any tapping rates (see Procedure, below) would fall within the preferred range Our first analysis was in order to establish objective of200-900 msec (Fraisse, 1963; Handel, 1989), regardless of which support for our arbitrary musical-experience criterion metrical level was perceived to be the tactus in either a 3/4 or a which resulted in subjects being placed into one of two 6/8 meter. The timbre was that of an FM-synthesized piano. The subject groups. Response profiles consisting of percent­ temporal line was presented at a slightly greater intensity than the age counts for the two dependent variables of rhythmic harmonic accompaniment (approximately 4 dB difference), to facili­ choice (LSSSS, SLSSS,SSLSS, SSSLS, SSSSL)and metri­ tate perception of the temporal accent. cal choice (3/4 and 6/8 time) were computed for each sub­ Procedure ject. These profiles were specified for the five rhythms There were 60 trials in total, 30 in each of the two harmonic con­ defined by chord-ehange locations(LSSSS, SLSSS, SSLSS, ditions. Each harmonic condition was presented in a blocked for­ SSSLS, SSSSL) in each of the two harmonic conditions mat. Half of the subjects received the short-duration harmonic con­ (long- and short-duration), resulting in a 10 X 10 matrix. dition first, while the other half received the long-duration condition The 24 matrices (one for each subject) were then analyzed first. The five different stimuli based on location of the chord change by means of the hierarchicalclustering module in Statistica. were presented in random order six times. The subjects were in­ structed to listen to the stimuli and then choose which rhythm pat­ The solution, which is shown in Figure 4, supported our tern they had heard by placing the cursor in one of five appropri­ original subjectiveclassification. In the figure, our original ately labeled boxes (i.e., LSSSS, SLSSS, SSLSS, SSSLS, or group of musically experienced subjects, indicated by a SSSSL). They were under no time constraints, and started each trial solid line, forms a relatively tight cluster on the right side by placing the cursor into a startbox. The experimenter ensured of the solution. Similarly, the nonmusicians, represented that all of the subjects understood what each schemata represented by the dotted line, form a cluster on the left side of the by having them tap each of the five rhythms prior to the presenta­ tion of the stimuli. They were further instructed to tap their foot solution. The higher level of the connections among the during the presentation of the stimuli. They were also told that there nonmusicians relative to that seen in the musicians' cluster were no correct answers and that it was possible that all trials would is indicative of greater variability in the pattern of re­ sound as if they had identical rhythm patterns. An experimenter sponses for nonmusicians compared with that of musi­ sat immediately behind each subject and noted how many times per cians. Furthermore, the pattern of responses for two of iteration ofthe rhythmic pattern the subject tapped his or her foot. the nonmusicians was found to be more similar to that Subjects had no difficulty in carrying out this task. Finally, they found for musicians than that found for their fellow non­ were required to fill out a standard questionnaire regarding their musical experience and formal training. musicians. This is shown in Figure 4, where the two non­ musicians' pattern of responses that were closest to those RESULTS AND DISCUSSION of the musicians join with the latter's cluster rather than with the nonmusicians' cluster. An investigation of the The first result to be noted was that subjects invariably musical questionnaires completed by these two subjects tapped their foot only two or three times per iteration. indicated that they were the only "rionmusicians" with 802 DAWE, PLATT, AND RACINE

•••••••••••••••••• The loglinearanalysis was implemented with the loglinear module in Statistica. An appropriate model was deter­ mined by first conducting tests of partial and marginal association. These tests examine all possible effects and interactions. The test of partial association involved the comparison of a model that incorporated all the equiva­ lent and lower-order interactions, including the interaction under consideration, with a model of all the equivalent ••••••••• and lower-order interactions except the one under con­ .• sideration. Ifthe difference between the two models was statisticallysignificant, the interaction was retained; if not, it was excluded. The test of marginal association involved the comparison of a model without any interactions with a model that included the interaction under consideration. •••••••••••••••• ! Again, if the difference between the two models was sta­ tistically significant, the interaction was retained. The re­ ·• sults of these tests for association indicated that the sig­ . · nificant effects were: choice [partial x2(4) = 13.19, P < · 2 ~" .. "" · .01; marginal x (4) = 13.19,p < .01], choice x train­ ·. ·• ing [partial X2(4) = 23.82, p < .001; marginal X2(4) = · .:. 2 ·• •­ 22.16, p < .001], choice x location [partial x (l 6) = • 2 ! .. 462.16, P < .001; marginal x (l 6) = 472.71, P < · ~I "1 . II •I 2(4) ·• .· • .001], choice x duration [partial X = 32.56, p < · • 2(4) • · · .001; marginal X = 13.68, P < .01], meter x dura­ ·I · 2 · · · tion [partial ~(1) = 856.81, P < .001; marginal x (l ) = ·• · 829.89, p < .001], choice x meter [partial X2(4) = · II·• - 2(4) • 87.80, p < .001; marginal X = 77.90, P < .001], 1 2 choice x location x training [partial x (l 6) = 106.23, P < .001; marginal ~(l6) = 107.18,p < .001], choice x duration x training [partial X2(4) = 15.10, P < .005; marginal X2(4) = 11.38, P < .05], meter x duration x training [partial x2(l) = 9.71, P < .005; marginal X2(1) = 15.43, p < .001], and, finally, choice x loca­ Figure 4. Hierarchical clustering (Ward's method) of the 24 sub­ tion x duration [partial x2(l 6) = 40.30,p < .001; mar­ jects based on the Euclidean distances of individual response pro­ ginal X2(16) = 51.26, p < .001]. flles. Solid lines represent individuals with formal musical training; broken lines represent nonmusicians. The ordinate represents a The second stage ofthe analysis first fitted a model that proportional measure of dissimilarity relative to the maximum Eu­ contained only the significant effects from the first stage. clidean distance. The best fitting model was determined by systematically removing each effect and seeing if a significant reduc­ tion in the fit occurred. Ifso, the effect was retained. All possible combinations of effects were systematically fit­ some notable musical experience. They both had some ted by this backward elimination procedure until the best guitar experience, although they did not play often, nor model was obtained. This model contains 4 three-way had they had any formal musical training. interactions that account for all of the observed variance: Because the measures used in this study were discrete choice x location x duration; choice x location x train­ and categorical, a loglinear analysis was performed on ing; meter x duration x training; and choice x meter the data, with four design and two response variables. The x location [X2(296, N = 1,440) = 202.11, P = 1.00]. design variables were the musical training of the subject The significant interaction of rhythmic choice with lo­ (inexperienced or experienced), the location of the chord­ cation of a chord change and training, presented in Ta­ change relative to the long-duration note (resulting in the ble 2, shows that subjects predominantly chose rhythm rhythm patterns of LSSSS, SLSSS, SSLSS, SSSLS, and patterns on the basis ofthe location ofthe chord change. SSSSL), the duration ofthe harmonic condition (short or With most rhythmic choices along the negative diagonal, long), and the order of presentation for the two durations. the pattern of results for both musicians and nonmusicians The response variables were the subject's choice of indicates several things. First, when the location of the rhythm (LSSSS, SLSSS, SSLSS, SSSLS, SSSSL) and the chord change coincided with the onset of the temporal ac­ meter in which he or she was operating (3/4 or 6/8), based cent (i.e., LSSSS), subjects predominantly chose that on the foot tapping. rhythm pattern (51.4% of the time for nonmusicians and HARMONIC ACCENTS, RHYTHM, AND METER INFERENCE 803

Table 2 harmonic accompaniment and in a 6/8 meter when they Effect of Rhythmic Choice by Location of were presented with a short-duration harmonic accom­ ____~~(:hord Cbange and~~s~~ Tra!niJl~ paniment. Furthermore, this pattern of metrical responses Rhythm on the Basis of the Chord Change Location was more strongly evident in the data of the musicians Rhythmic ~ ~------_.------compared with those of the nonmusicians. When the chord Choice LSSSS SLSSS SSLSS SSSLS SSSSL presentation was long, subjects more often than not tapped Nonmusicians their feet 3 times per iteration in support of a 3/4 meter LSSSS 51.4% 26.4% 18.1% 16.7% 27.8% (87.2 % and 92.2 % for nonmusicians and musicians, respec­ SLSSS 13.9% 29.2% 13.9% 19.4% 18.1% tively). In contrast, when the chord presentation was short, SSLSS 13.9% 9.7% 34.7% 22.2% 16.7% SSSLS 11.1% 19.4% 15.3% 29.2% 15.3% subjects were observed to tap their feet more often in sup­ SSSSL 9.7% 15.3% 18.1% 12.5% 22.2% port of a 6/8 meter (81.7 % and 93.3% for nonmusicians Musicians and musicians, respectively). Because the psychologically meaningful patterns of re­ LSSSS 63.9% 8.3% 11.1% 9.7% 6.9% SLSSS 11.1 % 50.0% 2.8% 12.5% 15.3% sponses for the two three-way interactions of choice x SSLSS 8.3% 6.9% 59.7% 9.7% 4.2% location x duration and choice x meter X location are SSSLS 1.4% 13.9% 15.3% 55.6% 11.1% nicely evident in the four-way interaction of choice x SSSSL 15.3% 20.8% 11.1 % 12.5% 62.5% meter X duration X location, the results can be com­ pletely specified by examining the four-way interaction presented in Table 3. When the duration ofthe chord pre­ 63.9% of the time for musicians), supporting theories of sentation was long, subjects predominantly perceived a musical structure based on accent coincidence (Benjamin, 3/4 metrical structure (89.7% ofthe time). When the per­ 1984; Berry, 1976; Jones, 1987). Second, when the rhythm ceived metrical structure was 3/4 time under the long­ pattern on the basis of the chord change was SLSSS, duration harmonic condition, the pattern of rhythmic SSLSS, SSSLS, or SSSSL, subjects chose these rhythms choices on the basis ofthe location ofa chord change was most often (29.2%, 34.7%, 29.2%, and 22.2% of the as expected. Subjects chose rhythm patterns predominantly time, respectively, for nonmusicians, and 50.0%, 59.7 %, on the basis ofa chord change with a bias for the rhythm 55.6%, and 62.5% of the time, respectively, for musi­ patterns LSSSS, SSLSS, and SSSSL (61.0%, 55.6%, and cians). In addition, when subjects chose a particular 45.7%, respectively). Occasionally (only 10.3% of the rhythm pattern (e.g., LSSSS), they did so most often when the chord change supported that rhythm pattern. This means that subjects principally used the location of the chord change as a determinant for the perceived rhythm pattern. Finally, musicians and nonmusicians differed in two distinct ways, the first being that, from the greater percentage of responses along the negative diagonal for musicians compared with nonmusicians, it is apparent that the harmonic accent associated with a chord change was perceptually more salient for musicians than for nonmu­ sicians. The other difference is that, compared with mu­ sicians, nonmusicians showed a greater bias to select a rhythm pattern on the basis ofthe temporal accent alone. This becomes apparent when one compares the percent­ age of responses for the rhythm pattern LSSSS, when the chord-ehange location supports the rhythm patterns SLSSS, SSLSS, SSSLS, and SSSSL, for musicians and nonmusi­ cians. In all instances, the percentage of responses is higher for nonmusicians than for musicians: 26.4% > 8.3% (SLSSS), 18.1 % > 11.1 % (SSLSS), 16.7% > 9.7% (SSSLS), and 27.8% > 6.9% (SSSSL), respectively. In fact, for nonmusicians, grouping on the basis of the tem­ poral accent (LSSSS) was slightly more salient than was that done on the basis of the rhythm defined by a chord­ change location when the chord-supported rhythm was SSSSL (27.8% compared with 22.2%). The significant interaction of metrical choice by the du­ ration condition of chord presentation and training indi­ cated that subjects were for the most part operating in a 3/4 meter when they were presented with the long-duration 804 DAWE, PLATT, AND RACINE

time), subjects showed evidence of operating in 6/8 time music perception, a listener can pick up temporal invari­ when the chord presentation was of a long duration. Under ances in the musical flow (i.e., regularly occurring ac­ such conditions, subjects only chose three different rhythm cents) that can then be used to guide attention to future patterns. It is noteworthy that the rhythm patterns SSLSS anticipated events. The abstraction of a time interval from and SSSSL were never chosen, and that the pattern LSSSS unfolding events is a dynamic process based in part on was chosen only when it was supported by a chord change. both fulfillment and violation of temporal expectations Subjects predominantly chose the rhythm patterns SLSSS (Jones, 1990). From this dynamic process emerges a met­ and SSSLS under such conditions. Under these conditions, ric of directed attentional resources to which events oc­ subjects chose the rhythm pattern SLSSS every time the curring in the presentation are compared. location of the chord change supported the rhythm pat­ In this study such a metric could have been established tern SSSSL. This pattern makes sense, given that sub­ by the regular occurrence of the temporal accent and/or jects were operating in a 6/8-time meter. In a 6/8 meter, the harmonic accent of a chord change. The onset of the 3 eighth notes are grouped at the intermediate level of note of relativelylong duration occurred every 1,332 msec. the metrical grid (refer to Figures 2 & 3). If subjects used If this accent was used to establish an internal rhythmic­ the location of the chord change as a metrical cue, then ity of attention, then the harmonic accents could have been the two rhythm patterns supported by the metrical struc­ judged relative to the regular occurrence of the temporal ture would beSSS-SLand SL-SSS when the chord-ehange­ accent. In other words, it may be the way in which the based rhythm pattern was SSSSL. Subjects chose the lat­ duration of time established by the regular recurrence of ter of these two alternatives. We interpret these findings the temporal accent is parsed into smaller time spans by as evidence that the perceived rhythm pattern biased sub­ the occurrence of other events (such as chord onsets or jects to infer a meter consistent with that rhythm. chord changes) that is of psychological significance in es­ When the duration ofthe chord presentation was short, tablishing a meter or rhythm (refer to Figure 2). If the subjects predominantly perceived a 6/8 metrical structure occurrence of a chord change was perceived to coincide (87.5% ofthe time). When the perceived metrical struc­ with a regular metrical dividing point (Smith & Cuddy, ture was 6/8 time and the duration of the harmonic con­ 1989), the range of probable inferred meters would be dition was short, the pattern of rhythmic choices on the reduced. basis ofthe location ofa chord change was as expected­ However, the regular occurrence of a chord change also subjects chose rhythm patterns predominantly on the ba­ created a temporal invariance equal to a duration of sis ofa chord change with rhythm patterns that coincided 1,332 msec. In contrast to the previous consideration, with the location of accents in the metrical structure (i.e., therefore, it may be the way in which the time spans LSSSS and SSSLS) receiving the highest proportion of created by the chord change are parsed by the occurrence rhythmic response (41.7% and 47.2%, respectively). Oc­ of other events (such as chord onsets and the temporal casionally (only 12.5% ofthe time), subjects showed evi­ accent) that is important (refer to Figure 3). Ifthe occur­ dence of operating in 3/4 time when the chord presenta­ rence ofa temporal accent was perceived to coincide with tion was of a short duration. Under such conditions, a metrically strong position, the range of probable meters subjects only chose four different rhythm patterns. It is would also be reduced. noteworthy that the rhythm pattern SSSLS was never Which of these two interpretations is supported by the chosen under such conditions. Subjects predominantly data? The subjects primarily used the location of the chord chose the rhythm patterns LSSSS, SSLSS, and SSSSL change to determine the perceived rhythm pattern. This under such conditions. Again, we interpret this in terms implies that the harmonic phenomenal accent was more of a perceived rhythm pattern biasing subjects to select salient than the temporal accent in determining the phrase a compatible metrical structure. structure. In terms of the metrical structure, the primary These fmdings are ofsome importance, particularly for basis for determining an inferred meter was provided by theories of metrical structure. The data clearly support the two harmonic-accompaniment conditions that differed the conclusion that meter is not necessarily communicated in their composite-rhythm patterns. It was based on the through performance cues (Essens & Povel, 1985; Povel, frequency of chord presentations within the temporal or 1981; Povel & Essens, 1985; Povel & Okkerman, 1981). harmonic time span that subjects primarily inferred a 6/8 That is, metrically important locations were not marked or a 3/4 metrical structure. In those few instances where by phenomenal accents such as a slight elongation of a selection of the metrical structure was not based on the note's duration or a subtle change in dynamics (Clarke, composite rhythm, it appeared to be based on the rhythm 1985; Gabrielsson, 1974, 1987). Nevertheless, subjects pattern as defined by the location of the chord changes, inferred a metrical structure and then interpreted the mu­ implying that the chord change was also more salient than sical sequence in terms of that structure. the temporal accent for meter determination. Similar con­ It is clear that subjects could have used different means clusions have been made elsewhere (e.g., Dawe et al., to arrive at the inferred meter and perceived rhythm. Spe­ in press). cifically, the stimuli contained a number of temporal in­ Finally, evidence in favor of the chord change serving variances that subjects could have used. According to as the more prominent accent comes from an observa­ Jones's influential work on attentional rhythmicities in tion made during the experimental procedure and from HARMONIC ACCENTS, RHYTHM, AND METER INFERENCE 805 reports made by subjects upon completion of the ex­ from this experiment. First, the assumption of equal sa­ perimental session. In both harmonic conditions, the du­ lience of phenomenal accents in phrase-structure deter­ ration between chord changes was typically 1,332 msec. mination was not supported. There was evidence that the The only exception was at the beginning and end oftrials, location of a harmonic accent was fundamental in estab­ where the duration between trial onset and chord change lishing a phrase boundary and, thus, the reported rhythm was systematically varied to create the five rhythm pat­ pattern. In addition, musicians showed far less variance terns (refer to Table 1). The result of this manipulation than nonmusicians in their tendency to employ the chord­ was that some rhythm patterns as defined by the location change location as a cue for the rhythm pattern and in of a chord change would not be completed at the end of their inference of a metrical structure. a trial. Interestingly, the experimenter noted that with most Second, support was found for models of phrase struc­ such trials, the subjects continued to tap their feet to the ture that rely on the coincidence of phenomenal accents projected completion of the rhythm pattern. In addition, to serve as psychologically important events (e.g., Ben­ subjects reported that some trials appeared to end prema­ jamin, 1984; Berry, 1976; and Jones, 1987). Overall, turely (i.e., before they were expected to end). Jones and whenever both the relatively long-held note and the chord her colleagues have reported that the way in which a du­ change supported the same rhythm pattern, subjects ration is temporally patterned can evoke expectations of showed a higher percentage of choice for the rhythm pat­ when a stimulus should end (Jones & Boltz, 1989). Thus, tern based on the accent coincidence (LSSSS) than was we conclude that a chord change was perceptually more evident for rhythm patterns based on either the chord salientthan a temporal accent in both phrase- and metrical­ change or the temporal accent alone. structure determination. Third, there was evidence for the unambiguous infer­ There are several possible reasons for this finding. In ence of metrical structure supporting conclusions made this experiment, only one was em­ by others regarding meter determination (Essens & Povel, ployed (i.e., the I-V-I progression). This was because of 1985; Povel, 1981; Povel & Essens, 1985; Povel & Ok­ our interest in the timing of phenomenal accents, such as kerman, 1981). In this study, the meter was determined chord changes, in the determination of rhythmic struc­ primarily by the frequency of chord presentation within ture, excluding structural factors. However, the structural the time spans defined by the chord changes, supporting features of a chord progression may affect the perceptual conclusions made in another study (Dawe et al., in press) significance of the chord-change location. Structural fac­ that chord changes serve as important cues for meter de­ tors might have been of particular concern in this experi­ termination. Based on the results of this experiment, it ment because of the dissonance created by the suspension seems that music with frequent chord presentations is of Middle C in the temporal line while the dominant chord heard with a faster beat than a piece with the same pat­ was being played as a harmonic accompaniment. How­ tern of harmonic changes (i.e., with the same harmonic ever, early indications from several experiments explor­ rhythm) but fewer chord presentations. This perception ing the contribution of structural accenting to the percep­ of a faster beat can lead to the adoption of a slower tactus tualsalienceof chord changeshave suggestedthat struetural and thus the perception of a different metrical structure factors (e.g., significance of a chord progression within a (e.g., moving from 3/4 to 6/8 time). Once the meter was particular key, dissonance, etc.) would not have played a determined, inferred metrical accents resulted in their lo­ role in the type of rhythm-determination task employed in cation in the music becoming perceptually more salient. this study (Dawe, Platt, & Racine, 1993). That is, we interpret the data in terms of inferred metri­ Alternatively, it may simply be that some forms of pitch cal accents serving to heighten attention to events at those information (e.g., chord change) are perceptually more locations. In this way, we agree with models such as salient than temporal information in the determination of Jones' (1987, 1992), which argue that the occurrence and! rhythmic structure (Dawe et al., in press). Although this or coincidence of accents may serve to heighten expecta­ may sound somewhat counterintuitive, it has been shown tions and attention for specific locations. in studies of speech rhythm that when intensity, duration, Finally, perception of rhythm patterns and a metrical and pitch contributions to the perceived stress of a sylla­ structure were not always independent. Subjects had a ten­ ble are pitted against one another (e.g., Fry, 1958), the dency to select rhythm patterns on the basis of harmonic fundamental frequency (i.e., pitch) is the dominant stress information (i.e., the location of a chord change) and cue, followed by duration and, finally, intensity features showed a bias for those patterns consistent with the in­ (Handel, 1989). Similar weightings of rhythmic cues may ferred meter. However, in those cases where the inferred exist in music. meter was not consistent with the composite rhythm, it was still consistent with rhythm patterns defined by the CONCLUSIONS location of a chord change. Put another way, subjects sometimes inferred a meter on the basis of the rhythm Despite the limitations resulting from the use of one pattern that occurred within the time span defined by a chord progression, a number of conclusions can be drawn chord change rather than on the basis of the composite 806 DAWE, PLATT, AND RACINE

rhythm defined by that chord change. This suggests that KRUMHANsL, C. L. (1990). Tonal hierarchies and rare intervals in music subjects principally used information within the time span cognition. Music Perception, 7, 309-324. defined by the chord change rather than information within KRUMHANSL, C. L., BHARUCHA, J. J., 8< KESSLER, E. J. (1982). Per­ ceived harmonic structure of chords in three related musical keys. the time span defined by the temporal accent to infer the Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Perfor­ metrical structure. mance, 8, 24-36. KRUMHANSL, C. L., & CASTELLANO, M. A. (1983). Dynamic processes in music perception. Memory & Cognition, 11, 325-334. REFERENCES KRUMHANSL, C. L., & SHEPARD, R. N. (1979). Quantification of the hierarchy of tonal functions within a diatonic context. Journal ofEx­ AGMON, E. (1990). as cognitive science: Some concep­ perimental Psychology:Human Perception & Performance,S, 579-594. tual and methodological issues. Music Perception, 7, 285-308. LEE, C. S. (1985). The rhythmic interpretation of simple musical se­ BENJAMIN, W. (1984). A theory of musical meter. Music Perception, quences: Towards a perceptual model. In P. Howell, I. Cross. & 1, 355-413. R. West (Eds.), Musical structure and cognition (pp. 53-69). Lon­ BERRY, W. (1976). Structural functions in music. Englewood Cliffs, don: Academic Press. NJ: Prentice-Hall. LEHISTE, 1. (1975). Theperception ofduration within sequences offour BHARUCHA, J., & KRUMHANSL, C. L. (1983). The representation ofhar­ intervals. Paper presented at the 8th International Congress of Pho­ monic structure in music: Hierarchies of stability as a function of con­ netic Sciences, Leeds, UK. text. Cognition, 13, 63-102. LERDAHL, F., & JACKENDOFF, R. (1983). A generative theory oftonal BOLTZ, M. (1989). Rhythm and "good endings": Effects of temporal music. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. structure on judgments. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 9-17. LONGUET-HIGGINS, H. c., & LEE, C. S. (1982). The perception of mu­ BORETZ, B. (1971). In quest of the rhythmic genius. Perspectives of sical rhythms. Perception, 11, 115-128. New Music, 9-10, 149-155. LoNGUET-HIGGINS, H. C., 8

STEEDMAN, M. 1. (1977). The perception of musical rhythm and meter. NOTE Perception, 6, 555-569. Vos, P. (1977). Temporal duration factors in the perception of audi­ I. Lerdahl and lackendoff acknowledge that phrase structure (i.e., tory rhythmic patterns. Scientific Aesthetics, I, 183-199. grouping) is not strictly hierarchical, although they treat both meter and Vos, P., & HANDEL, S. (1987). Playing triplets: Facts and preferences. grouping as such, with grouping overlaps and elisions given special treat­ In A. Gabrielsson (Ed.), Action and perception in rhythm and music ment (see Section 3.4, pp. 55-62 of Lerdahl & Jackendoff, 1983). (pp. 3547). Stockholm: Royal Swedish Academy of Music Publications. WOODROW, H. (1909). A quantitative study of rhythm. Archives ofPsy­ chology, 14, 1-66. YESTON, M. (1976). The stratification ofmusical rhythm. New Haven, (Manuscript received May 6, 1992; CT: Yale University Press. revision accepted for publication April 19, 1993.)