The of Imitation Without Cultural Transmission

Lee Altenberg∗1, Susanne Still1, and Christopher J. Watkins2 1University of Hawai‘i at M¯anoa 2Royal Holloway University of London

July 22, 2021

Abstract The evolution and function of imitation in animal learning has always been associated with its crucial role in cultural transmission and cultural evolution. Can imitation evolve in the absence of cultural transmission? We investigate a model in which imitation is unbundled from cultural transmission: an organism’s adult phenotype is plastically altered by its juvenile experiences of genetically determined juvenile traits within its co- hort, and the only information transmitted between generations is genetic. Transmission of phenotypic information between generations is precluded by the population being semelparous with discrete, non-overlapping gen- erations. We find that during a period of directional selection towards a phenotypic optimum, natural selection favors modifiers which cause an organism to bias its plastic phenotype in the direction opposite to the mean phenotype of the population. As the population approaches the phenotypic optimum and shifts into stabilizing selection, selection on the modifier reverses and favors strong imitation of the population mean. Imitation can become so strong that it results in “genotype-phenotype disengagement” (Gonzalez et al., 2017) or even “hyper-imitation”—where the adult phenotype overshoots the mean phenotype. Hyper-imitation produces an evolutionary pathology, where the phenotype is driven away from the optimum, resulting in a drop in mean fitness, and the collapse of imitation. Repeated cycles of hyper-imitation and its collapse are observed. Genetic constraints on hyper-imitation will prevent these cycles. In a model of purifying selection for an extreme phenotype, selection for anti-imitation continues even at a arXiv:2107.09761v1 [q-bio.PE] 20 Jul 2021 mutation-selection balance. These theoretical outcomes are all novel evolutionary and biological phenomena, and we discuss their implications.

Keywords: anti-imitation, cliff-edged fitness function, frequency- dependent selection, modifier gene, phenotypic plasticity, social learning ∗altenber@.edu

1 1 Introduction

Imitation is regarded as the core capability that enables cultural transmission and evolution, and conversely, cultural transmission is regarded as the principal function of imitation. Imitation can be considered as a form of phenotypic plasticity in which the development of an organism’s phenotype is sensitive to the phenotypes of other organisms. Imitation, in order to evolve as an adaptation, needs to confer a selective advantage upon the imitator. The question arises as to why it would be adaptive to imitate the states of other organisms. The classical proposed mechanisms are (1) the imitator can discriminate in the choice of models or behaviors to imitate, and can select the more adaptive among them (selective transmission), or (2) imitation is of conspecifics who have undergone individual learning in their development, which makes their behaviors more adaptive than the imitator’s default state (adaptive learning), and thus imitation can avoid costs of trial-and- error learning (Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, 1973; Richerson and Boyd, 1978; Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, 1983). This nearly universal bundling of imitation with the other processes of se- lective transmission and individual learning puts it in a box which limits our understanding of exactly what imitation is capable of doing in evolutionary dy- namics. The way to develop a more full understanding of imitation is to go outside the box and break it out of this bundle. Gonzalez et al. (2017) have initiated this unbundling by asking whether selective transmission and adaptive learning are necessary for the evolution of imitation. They investigate whether imitation could evolve in the absence of selective transmission and adaptive learning. They find a counterexample in a model where (1) imitators choose without bias the model individual to imitate, and (2) their imitated phenotype remains unimproved through any kind of learning during their lifetime. Under specific population dynamic conditions (a Moran model with oblique cultural transmission and strong viability selection) imitation evolves with high likeli- hood in the absence of selective transmission or adaptive learning. Additionally, they find that when cultural transmission evolves, it irreversibly displaces ge- netic information as the dominant mode of inheritance. In the current work, we unbundle imitation even further by removing cultural transmission from the dynamics. As in Gonzalez et al. (2017) we suppose that there is neither selective transmission nor adaptive individual learning. Their model instantiates cultural transmission through oblique transmission (Cavalli- Sforza and Feldman, 1981) in which juveniles imitate the phenotypes of adults and then exhibit phenotypes which can themselves be imitated by later gen- erations. This makes the adult phenotype a channel of transmission separate from the genotype. Here, we strip away cultural transmission by preventing any imitation of phenotypes that were themselves imitations. Imitation is restricted to one-shot sampling of genetically determined phenotypes of juveniles by ju- veniles. The adult phenotypes that result from imitation cannot themselves be further imitated; it is “acquired variation”, but not culturally transmitted (Boyd and Richerson, 1983). This eliminates the phenotype as a pool of transmitted information separate from the genotype. The only communication between gen- erations is genetic transmission. A common term used almost synonymously with imitation is social learn- ing, where the focal phenotype is behavior. Because there is nothing in our

2 treatment that restricts the phenotype to behavior, we will not speak of so- cial learning specifically here, but it should be understood that it is included within the more general category of phenotypic plasticity. It should also be understood that under the umbrella of “imitation” we include copying and em- ulation. Charbonneau and Bourrat (2021) point out that these distinctions depend on the level of course-graining applied to behavior. Our simple model here considers the phenotype at the most coarse-grained level where no inner structure is distinguished. We make no assumptions at all about the perceptual or cognitive level at which imitation occurs. Imitation in our model could be considered (Matthew Zefferman, personal communication) to be horizontal transmission (Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, 1981) in that juveniles collect information on other organisms in their gen- erational cohort, and this information affects their phenotype. We preclude this horizontally transmitted information from being transmitted to future genera- tions — it is terminal — so it cannot produce cultural evolution. This removal of horizontal transmission from its usual context of cultural transmission is therefore another unbundling to be found in our model. Lastly, we do not provide imitation as a ready-made form of genetic variation. To do so would be to create a form of mutational bias (Yampolsky and Stoltzfus, 2001) that could artefactually boost the of the trait. Imitation in nature involves multiple chains of information processing from sensory systems to effector systems (with the possible exception of prions, in which folding could be considered to be direct imitation). For a single mutation to produce an offspring with fully formed imitation from a parent with no imitation seems biologically unlikely. Instead, we here assume that: 1. an organism receives a perceptual signal about the phenotypes of its con- specifics; 2. this perceived signal is used by the organism’s developmental system to plastically shape its adult phenotype, 3. this plastic shaping is drawn from a set of genetically determined pheno- typic plasticity functions; and 4. mutation in the space of these possible functions is incremental and not biased toward imitation. The phenotypic plasticity function is modeled as a quantitative trait parameter that may range over a continuum going from no effect, to imitation, to what we call “anti-imitation”, in which the perceptual signal received by the organism causes its phenotype to develop in the opposite direction the phenotype that produced the signal. It should be clarified here that anti-imitation is not analogous to anti- conformism (Boyd and Richerson, 1985; Liberman et al., 2020; Denton et al., 2020). Conformism and anti-conformism are defined as forms of frequency- dependent transmission bias. Anti-imitation is unrelated to transmission bias, being the phenotypic response to a model, not the choice of the model. Anti- conformism is a form of imitation where there is transmission bias toward im- itating less-frequent phenotypes. Anti-imitation, in contrast, is a response of the organism to a model wherein it plastically alters its metric phenotype in a direction opposite to that of the model.

3 We suppose that organisms have a quantitative genetic trait which is ex- pressed early in development. Juvenile organisms gather sensory information about the quantitative trait as expressed by the juveniles in their cohort. A phenotypic plasticity function then combines the organism’s own juvenile quan- titative trait with this population information to determine the organism’s adult phenotype. Natural selection acts only on this adult phenotype after all plas- tic development has happened. To prevent any cultural transmission between generations, we model a semelparous species with discrete, non-overlapping gen- erations, where the parental generation is gone and any information about their plastic phenotypes erased by the time offspring develop. Our focal question is the evolution of this phenotypic plasticity function. We model a modifier gene that creates genetic variation over a space of plasticity functions. For a particular plasticity function to be adaptive, it must somehow take the information it has available — solely the distribution of genetically de- termined juvenile phenotypes in the population — and convert this information into modification of an organism’s development so that its fitness is increased, thereby increasing the frequency of the modifier gene variant. In a sense, the task of the plasticity function is to act like a population geneticist — to take a snapshot of the genetic variation in the population and make inferences from this information about the shape of the adaptive land- scape, specifically, the direction of a phenotypic optimum. The paradigm of “organism as population geneticist” could also be seen to encompass confor- mity and anti-conformity, since those mechanisms are built from an organismal assessment of the frequency of cultural traits in the population. The idea that a snapshot of the genetic variation in the population can give an organism infor- mation which it can use adaptively in its development is an instance of the idea of “genes-as-cues” (Leimar, 2005; Leimar et al., 2006). From Dall et al. (2015): Adaptive cue-integration systems of the sort outlined above will take any available sources of information (epigenetic and genetic) into account in determining phenotypes.

1.1 Our Contributions Our chief discoveries are the following: 1. During directional selection toward a phenotypic optimum, anti-imitation is strongly selected for, and it amplifies adult phenotypic and fitness vari- ance. 2. As directional selection shifts into stabilizing selection at a mutation- selection balance around a phenotypic optimum, strong selection for im- itation takes over, and imitation suppresses adult phenotypic and fitness variance. 3. Selection for imitation during stabilizing selection can reach such an ex- treme that genotype-phenotype disengagement occurs (Gonzalez et al., 2017), in which the the organism’s own genotype no longer influences its adult phenotype. 4. Stabilizing selection can further push the population into hyper-imitation which drives the phenotype away from the optimum to such a point that

4 the population is again under directional selection, and imitation levels collapse. This collapse phenomenon becomes very sharp in models of multivariate phenotypes. Evolution goes through repeated cycles of hyper- imitation and imitation collapse. The precise timing of these collapses of imitation involves a great deal of randomness. 5. If genetic constraints prevent the production of anti-imitation variants, populations under stabilizing selection reach stable high values of imita- tion.

6. With purifying selection for an extreme trait, the population mean phe- notype always falls short of the optimum, and very strong anti-imitation is selected for even at a mutation-selection balance. We are not aware of any of these phenomena save 3. having been documented nor hypothesized previously. As novel phenomena, they provide new search images for their occurrence empirical studies.

2 The Model

We now specify the details of the model investigated here: the organism, the phenotypic plasticity function, fitnesses, and the population dynamics.

t generation number N population size D number of phenotypic dimensions in the multivariate quantitative model L number of loci for the biallelic major loci genotype L y = (y1, . . . , yL) 0, 1 major loci genotype ∈ { } D x = (x1, . . . , xD) [0, 1] or R multivariate juvenile phenotype xˆ ∈ sampled juvenile phenotype value PN x = (1/N) i=1 xi population mean juvenile phenotype value α phenotypic plasticity function parameter PN α = (1/N) i=1 αi population mean α φ = (1 α)x + α xˆ adult phenotype − φˆ optimal phenotype β selection strength parameter w(φ) = exp( β(φ φˆ)>(φ φˆ)) fitness of phenotype φ −PN− − w = (1/N) i=1 w(φi) mean fitness of the population

Table 1: Definitions of symbols used

2.1 The Organism Organisms in this model species contain 2 pieces of inherited information: 1. major loci that specify the juvenile phenotype,

2. a modifier locus that specifies the phenotypic plasticity function.

5 The genotype of the major loci determine the phenotype of the juvenile or- ganism. The juvenile organism observes the other juvenile phenotypes in the population, and these observations combined with its own juvenile phenotype are inputs into its phenotypic plasticity function. Its particular plasticity func- tion is determined by its modifier locus genotype. The organism then develops into an adult whose phenotype is the output of its plasticity function. The fitness of the organism is determined by its adult phenotype. We assume the organisms are haploid and reproduce asexually. During re- production, both the major loci and the modifier locus are subject to mutation. We use two models for the structure of these genotypes: 1. A quantitative-genetic genotype represented by a multivariate genotypic D value, y = (y1, . . . , yD) R , where D is the number of phenotype di- mensions, which mutation∈ perturbs by a normal Gaussian random variable distributed as (0, σ2). N L 2. A multilocus biallelic model, y = (y1, . . . , yL) 0, 1 , in which each of L loci independently mutates to the alternate∈ allele { with} probability µ. The quantitative genetic representation 1 is appropriate for modeling stabilizing selection, while the multilocus model 2 is appropriate for modeling purifying selection for an extreme phenotype (Charlesworth, 2013), which is how it is used in Gonzalez et al. (2017).

2.2 The Genotype-Phenotype Map The genotype of the major loci specifies the juvenile phenotype. In the case of the quantitative genetic model, the juvenile phenotype x is simply the genotypic value, y. In the multilocus case, we are interested in purifying selection on an extreme phenotype, so the juvenile phenotype is set to the fraction of 1 alleles in the genotype:

L 1 X x(y) = x((y , . . . , yL)) = yk [0, 1]. 1 L ∈ k=1 The juvenile phenotype x develops plasticly into an adult phenotype φ which determines fitness. The plasticity function is determined by the genotypic value of the modifier locus which is described next.

2.3 Phenotypic Plasticity Functions The space of possible phenotypic plasticity functions is enormous. In this study we restrict the space of functions to a family with a single parameter, α, where the adult phenotype φ is chosen to fall somewhere on a line between the organ- ism’s juvenile phenotype x and a sample value xˆ from the juvenile phenotypes it observes in its cohort. The parameter, α, determines where on this line its adult phenotype falls. So

φ = (1 α)x + αx.ˆ (1) −

6 We describe the plasticity as imitation when α > 0 and anti-imitation when α < 0. The case α = 0 represents an absence of phenotypic plasticity. We note that for α > 1, the plastic phenotype φ will overshoot the input phenotypex ˆ and be further from x thatx ˆ. We therefore refer to α > 1 as hyper-imitation. With a population of size N, there are potentially N 1 different values of the juvenile phenotype in an individual’s cohort that can be− detected. In the current PN work, we use the population mean juvenile phenotype x = (1/N) i=1 xi for the sample input to the plasticity function. Other choices not explored in the current work could include wherex ˆ is the juvenile phenotype of a single ran- domly encountered cohort member, or a function of several encountered cohort members. We illustrate the phenotypic plasticity function in Figures 1 and 2. Figure 1 shows a population of two organisms, with phenotypes being the shapes of ovals. The resulting shapes for the adult phenotypes of both organisms are shown for different values of α. Figure 2 shows a population of three organisms, with phenotypes depicted as the coordinates on the X axis. Again, the resulting coordinates of adult phenotypes are depicted for different values of α ranging from anti-imitation to genotype-phenotype disengagement to hyper-imitation.

JUVENILE JUVENILE PHENOTYPE MEAN PHENOTYPE 1 PHENOTYPE 2 : ANTI-IMITATION IMITATION IMITATION ANTI-IMITATION ADULT 3 3 2 2 2 2 1.5 1 1

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 1.0 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 1

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 1 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 1.0 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 0.5 0.4 0.5 , , , , , , -3 -2 --10.4 1 2 3 -3 -2 -1 1 2 3 -2 -1 1 2 -2 -1 1 2 -1.5-1.0-0.5 0.5 1.0 1.5 -1.0-0.5 0.5 1.0 0.4 x-0.5 1 -0.5 x x2 1 -1.0 1 2 -1 2 -1.0 -1 -1 -1 -1.5 -2 -2 -2 -2 -3 -3

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 ↵ = – 0.5 0 0.5 1.0 0.5 0 – 0.5

Figure 1: The phenotypic plasticity function depicted for a population of two or- ganisms. The two juvenile phenotypes, x1 and x2, are shown as two ellipses with different parameters. The population mean x = (x1 + x2)/2 produces the circle as the mean phenotype, depicted. Ellipses with the parameters φ = (1 α)x+α x are shown for α = 0.5 (anti-imitation, pushing the adult phenotype− away from the mean), 0.5 (imitation,− pushing the adult phenotype toward the mean), 0 (no plasticity), and 1 (all adult phenotypes equal the mean, i.e. genotype-phenotype disengagement (Gonzalez et al., 2017)).

7 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 3 2 1 1.5 HYPER-IMITATION 1.0 GENOTYPE–PHENOTYPE DISENGAGEMENT

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 1 2 3 0.5 IMITATION

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 x 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 x x 0.0 JUVENILE 1 2 3 ↵ PHENOTYPE x

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 1 2 3 -0.5 ANTI-IMITATION

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

AAACTXicbVDLbhNBEOw1j4QNAQeOXEZYkXKI7N0IQY5GCIlDDkGKH5JtWbPjtj3xPFYzvQmW5X/gCj/FmQ/hhhCzax8wSUujLlVXq3oqy5X0lCQ/o9qDh48e7+0/iQ+eHj57Xj960fW2cAI7wirr+hn3qKTBDklS2M8dcp0p7GWLD+W8d4POS2uuaJnjSPOZkVMpOAWqO8zncpyO642kmVTF7oJ0CxrtOlR1OT6K0uHEikKjIaG494M0yWm04o6kULiOh4XHnIsFn+GqOnLNjgM1YVPrwjPEKnZHx7X3S52dhl4qfAk0p3lYLZvfERtOmczuGw0Kmp6PVtLkBaERG+NpoRhZVibAJtKhILUMgAsnw8VMzLnjgkJO8fG/LiQ1+t3flH5krfJrFg8rk1Wr8K6lrOCqdfH+6mO/dYE4NngrrNbcTHyT8Ms6DiGn/0d6F3TPmunb5tnnN432+SZt2IdX8BpOIIV30IZPcAkdEHANX+EbfI9+RL+i39GfjbQWbXdewk7V9v4COeCzeQ== -1.0 1 2 3

-2.0 -1.5 -1.0 -0.5 0.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 0.5 1.0 x Figure 2: The effect of plasticity parameter α on the adult phenotype φ. Shown are three juvenile phenotypes, x1, x2, x3 whose values at α = 0 equal the adult phenotypes (green line). The mean x = (x1 + x2 + x3)/3 is set 0. The adult phenotypes φ1, φ2, φ3 resulting from values of α = 1, 0.5, 0.5, 1.5 are plotted along red lines. Genotype-phenotype disengagement− − occurs when α = 1 at which point all adult phenotypes coincide: φ1 = φ2 = φ3 = x = 0. The ranges of α corresponding to anti-imitation (α < 0), imitation (0 < α < 1), and hyper-imitation (1 < α) are labeled.

The phenotypic plasticity parameter has a simple effect on the variance of the adult phenotype as compared to the genetically determined variance of the juvenile phenotype. When the samplex ˆ is set to the population mean x, the ratio of adult to juvenile phenotypic variance at any stage of evolution evaluates to: Var[φ] Var[(1 α)x + αx] (1 α)2 Var[x] = − = − = (1 α)2. (2) Var[x] Var[x] Var[x] −

Therefore, imitation (0 < α < 1) reduces the adult phenotypic variance, while anti-imitation (α < 0) and large hyper-imitation (2 < α) amplify the adult phenotypic variance. Where there is no phenotypic plasticity (α = 0) adult phenotypes remain unchanged from juvenile phenotypes. Genotype-phenotype disengagement (α = 1) results in there being no adult phenotypic variance.

2.4 Fitness Functions We assume there is an optimal phenotype φˆ with maximal fitness. For the fitness function, we use a Gaussian function of the departure of the adult phenotype φ from the optimal phenotype φˆ:

β(φ φˆ)>(φ φˆ) w(φ) = e− − − , (3) where β is the selection strength parameter and > is the transpose. The fitness function can be expressed in terms of the plasticity parameter α and the juvenile

8 phenotype x of the organism and its sample valuex ˆ from the population:

w(φ(x)) = exp( β(φ(x) φˆ)>(φ(x) φˆ)) − − − = exp( β(x + α(ˆx x) φˆ)>(x + α(ˆx x) φˆ)), − − − − − For given values of x,x ˆ, and φ, the optimal valueα ˆ that maximizes fitness is obtained by differentiation:

∂w(φ) ∂ = exp( β(x + α(ˆx x) φˆ)>(x + α(ˆx x) φˆ)) ∂α ∂α − − − − − = 2β(ˆx x)>(x + α(ˆx x) φˆ)w(φ). − − − − ∂w(φ) hence = 0 when x =x ˆ or when ∂α

(x xˆ)>(x φˆ) α =α ˆ := − − . (4) (x xˆ)>(x xˆ) − −

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

FITNESS

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 ˆ

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 xˆ x

Figure 3: Fitness as a function of juvenile phenotype x [ 2, 2] and plasticity parameter α [ 4, 6], in relation to the population sample∈ − x ˆ set tox ˆ = 0, ∈ − and the optimal phenotype set to φˆ = 1, and β = 1. This is the univariate phenotype case only. Forx ˆ < x < φˆ, the optimal values of α to maximize fitness are negative, while for x < xˆ or x > φˆ, the optimal values of α are positive. The curve for the optimal α isα ˆ = (x φˆ)/(x xˆ). − −

Figure 3 plots the fitness in the univariate case as a function of juvenile phenotype x, and plasticity parameter α in relation to the population samplex ˆ, and the phenotypic optimum φˆ. We see that for x intermediate betweenx ˆ and φˆ, the optimal values of α to maximize fitness are negative (anti-imitation), while for x outside the range [ˆx, φˆ], the optimal values of α are positive (imitation). These optimalα ˆ produce an adult phenotype φ(x) = (1 αˆ)x +α ˆxˆ which is − exactly the optimal phenotype φˆ.

9 2.5 Wright-Fisher Population Dynamics The natural choice for a population model of a semelparous organism with discrete, non-overlapping generations is the Wright-Fisher model. In choosing a finite population model we discover that sampling variability plays a crucial role in the population dynamics. We represent the population state as a vector of N random variables that represent each organism, which is a pair G = (Y,A) of its major loci genotype Y and modifier genotype A. For the stabilizing selection model Y RD, and in the purifying selection model Y 0, 1 L. To cover both we will∈ say simply that ∈ { } Y Ω. The population vector is G (Ω R)N . The evolutionary trajectory consists∈ of a sequence G(0), G(1), G(2)∈ ,...,×G(t),....

(1) PLASTIC ADULTS DEVELOPMENT

(2) SELECTION

JUVENILES

(4) MUTATION GENE POOL (3) WRIGHT-FISHER SAMPLING

Figure 4: Depiction of the life cycle: (1) Juvenile to adult plastic development. (2) Selection on adult phenotype to produce the gene pool. (3) Wright-Fisher sampling. (4) Mutation.

The evolutionary dynamics are captured by the following recurrence relation. The life cycle is depicted in Figure 4. After plastic development from juvenile to adult, there is Wright-Fisher sampling of the parent population G(t) weighted by fitness, to produce a “gene pool” GWF (t): WF Gi (t) = Gj(t), i, j = 1,...,N, with probability pj(t), (5) WF where each Gi (t) is sampled independently from G(t) with the probability distribution weighted by fitness:

w(φ(Xj(t)) pj(t) = , probability proportional to fitness w( ), (6) Z(t) · where Xj(t) = x(Yj(t)) is the juvenile phenotype as a function of genotype Yj(t), and

N N X X Z(t) = w(φ(Xj(t))) = w(φ(x(Yj(t)))), is the probability normalizer. j=1 j=1

10 The mean fitness of the population is

N N 1 1 X 1 X w(t) = Z(t) = w(φ(Xj(t))) = w(φ(x(Yj(t)))). (7) N N N j=1 j=1

Finally, mutation acts to produce the next generation

G(t+1) = (G1(t+1),...,GN (t+1))

= ((Y1(t+1),A1(t+1)),..., (YN (t+1),AN (t+1))) where

WF 2 Ai(t+1) = A (t) + χi, χi (0, σ ) (Gaussian mutation), (8) i ∼ N (9)  WF 2 Y (t) + ξi, ξi (0, σ I) (stabilizing selection model),  i ∼ N Yi(t+1) = (10)  WF (Yi (t) + βi) mod 2, (purifying selection model)

L with βi 0, 1 , and each βij, j = 1,...,L, is sampled independently with probabilities:∈ { }

Prob[βij = 1] = µ

Prob[βij = 0] = 1 µ. − Modular arithmetic is employed as a simple way to implement mutations using addition. The mutation rates used here are large enough to keep the populations poly- morphic in the face of strong natural selection. These models would thus be classified as in the “strong mutation, strong selection” regime. The alternative “weak mutation, strong selection” regime not studied here would leave the pop- ulation monomorphic for most generations, experiencing rapid selective sweeps of new advantageous mutations.

3 Results

We organize the results into two main sections: 3.1, evolution of the juvenile phenotypes under different fixed values of the imitation parameter α; and 3.2 joint evolution of α and the juvenile phenotypes. Under each section we examine the stabilizing selection model 3.1.1, 3.2.1, and the purifying selection model 3.1.2, 3.2.2.

3.1 Imitation and the Speed of Adaptation and Genetic Load Here we examine the evolutionary trajectories of the juvenile phenotype under different fixed values of the imitation parameter α.

11 3.1.1 Stabilizing Selection Model First we investigate the evolutionary trajectories for fixed imitation parameters, where there is no evolution in α. This is implemented by setting the values of αi at the modifier locus to a single value α which does not undergo mutation. We initialize the juvenile phenotypes far from the phenotypic optimum, so that they begin in a state of directional selection.

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 ↵ =0, 0.25, 0.50, 0.75 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 ↵ = 8, 4, 2, 1, 0 ˆ 10 10 ˆ 8 8

6 6

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

4 4 ↵ =0.95

2 2

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 ↵ =1.0 Juvenile Phenotype Juvenile 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 Phenotype Juvenile 500 1000 1500 2000 Generation Generation

Figure 5: Evolutionary trajectories of the population mean juvenile phenotype for four negative values of α (left), and five positive values of α (right), with α = 0 in both. Other parameters: φˆ = 10, N = 1024, β = 1, σ2 = 0.01, D = 1.

Figure 5 shows evolutionary trajectories for five values of α in the nega- tive range (left graph) and in the positive range (right graph). We see that the more negative α is—i.e. the greater the anti-imitation—the faster is the evolution of the juvenile phenotypic toward the optimal phenotype. In the left graph we see the transition in each trajectory from directional selection to sta- bilizing selection, arrived at sooner for more negative α. In the right graph, we see the continual slowing down of phenotypic evolution as α increases in the positive range α 0, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 0.95 . Finally, at α = 1.0, there is genotype-phenotype disengagement,∈ { and the juvenile} phenotype evolves strictly under genetic drift. Why does anti-imitation produce faster evolution under directional selec- tion? Anti-imitation repels plastic phenotypes away from the population mean. Genotypes that produce phenotypes less optimal than the mean produce adult phenotypes even less optimal, while genotypes closer to the optimum than the mean produce adult phenotypes even closer to the optimum. Anti-imitation therefore disadvantages disadvantageous genotypes, and advantages advanta- geous genotypes—the “Matthew effect” (Merton, 1968). It therefore amplifies the strength of directional selection. An examination of plasticity from the vantage point of evolvability (Al- tenberg, 1994, 1995a,b) gives additional insight. Organisms with the highest probability of generating mutant offspring fitter than the mean w are also those closer to φˆ than is x. These organisms are the likely ancestors of the eventual future population, and it is these organisms whose fitnesses are increased by anti-imitation. Meanwhile, organisms with below-average fitness—further from φˆ than is x—are much less likely to give rise by mutation to the descendent pop- ulation, and it is these organisms whose fitnesses are decreased by anti-imitation. Therefore, anti-imitation increases evolvability by increasing selection for indi- viduals most likely to give rise to the fittest in the next generation.

12 Figure 6 shows the evolution of the population mean fitness for the whole range α 8, 4, 2, 1, 0, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1.0 . Here we see that more anti- imitation∈ produces {− − the− − most rapid increases in} mean fitness. For α = 1.0, which produces genotype-phenotype disengagement, there is no increase in mean fitness. For α = 0.95, mean fitness increases, but because it is so slow to evolve toward the phenotypic optimum, it is not visibly different from 0 in the plot.

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 ↵ = 8, 4, 2, 1, 0, 0.25, 0.50, 0.75 1.0

0.8

0.6 Mean

Fitness 0.4

0.2

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 ↵ =0.95, 1.0 500 1000 1500 2000 Generation

Figure 6: Evolutionary trajectories in the population mean fitness w(t) for α 8, 4, 2, 1, 0, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 0.95, 1.0 . Other parameters: φˆ = 10, N ∈= {1024, − −β =− 1, σ−2 = 0.01, D = 1. }

An important new phenomenon appears in the stabilizing selection phase: a tradeoff between the speed of adaptation and the equilibrium genetic load (suppression of mean fitness below the maximum possible fitness). The value α = 8 produces the fastest increase in mean fitness during directional selection, but it− also produces the greatest genetic load during stabilizing selection. The fluctuations in mean fitness seen during the stabilizing selection phase in Figure 6 are due to finite population sampling effects. The amount of genetic load for different values of α is shown in Figure 7. The averages of 24 repeated evolutionary trajectories are shown for each value of α. The figure corresponds to a closeup of Figure 6 for the last 300 generations of the evolutionary trajectories, and extends it by 200 generations. We see that in this stabilizing selection phase, the mean fitnesses of the populations increase with α. However, for α = 0.75 the speed of evolution is so slow that it reaches stabilizing selection after all the other trajectories; it crosses the trajectories of the smaller values of α to emerge as the highest mean fitness on the right. Due to stochasticity from finite sampling, the standard deviations of the trajectories for α = 0, 0.25, 0.5, and 0.75 considerably overlap.

13 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

1.00 ↵ = 0.75 0.5 0.25 0 0.99

Mean –1 Fitness 0.98 –2

0.97

–4

0 100 200 300 400 500 Generation

Figure 7: Zoom in on Figure 6, showing the population mean fit- ness w(t) averaged over 24 evolutionary trajectories for each α 4, 2, 1, 0, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75 . Shaded bands are one standard deviation.∈ Shown{ − − are− generations 1700–2200} continuing Figure± 6. Other parameters: φˆ = 10, N = 1024, β = 1, σ2 = 0.01, D = 1.

So we see a tradeoff between rate of evolution under directional selection and genetic load under stabilizing selection. An analogous tradeoff is classically observed for a different phenomenon—mutation rates—under directional and stabilizing selection. But it must be remembered that here the mutation process is the same for all of these runs, with a variance on the mutational perturbation of the juvenile phenotypes of σ2 = 0.01. The only difference between these populations is the imitation parameter α, which affects the plastic phenotype, but has no affect on the inherited genetic information of the major loci. Figure 8 shows the evolution of juvenile phenotypes in the hyper-imitation range α 1, 1.25, 1.5, 1.75, 2.0 . Here evolution takes the juvenile phenotypes ∈ { } in the opposite direction from the optimal phenotype φˆ = 10, a pathological situation in which stabilizing selection never arrives.

14 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 ˆ = 10

Generation 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 ↵ =1.0 500 1000 1500 2000

-5

-10 Juvenile Phenotype -15 1.25 -20 1.5 -25

AAACT3icbVBNa1NBFL0vVds+v1JduhkMBReSvFek7UaIiOCiiwpNG2hCuG9y0wydj8fMfWoI+RPd6p9y6S9xJ857zcJYDwz3cO+5nDunKLUKnGU/k9bWvfsPtnd204ePHj952t57dh5c5SUNpNPODwsMpJWlASvWNCw9oSk0XRTX7+v5xWfyQTl7xouSxgavrJopiRxbwxHqco7i7aTdybpZA3GX5GvS6behwelkL8lHUycrQ5alxhAu86zk8RI9K6lplY6qQCXKa7yiZXPmSuzH1lTMnI/Psmi6Gzo0ISxM8TrWWhFqYpDncbUuYUNskQtV/G90WfHseLxUtqyYrLw1nlVasBN1BmKqPEnWi0hQehUvFnKOHiXHpNL9v11YGQqbv6n92DkdViIdNSbLXhV8TzuJunfy7uzDsHdCNLH0RTpj0E5Dl+nrKo0h5/9GepecH3Tzw+7Bpzedfv82bdiBF/ASXkEOR9CHj3AKA5Cg4Qa+wffkR/Ir+d1aS1vJmjyHDbR2/wB+6rMo ↵ = 2.0 1.75 0.35

0.30

0.25

Mean 0.20 Fitness 0.15

0.10 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

0.05 ↵ =2.0, 1.75, 1.5, 1.25

500 1000 1500 2000 Generation

Figure 8: Evolutionary trajectories of the population mean juvenile phenotype for values of α in the hyper-imitation range beyond 1.0. The plasticity function with α > 1 causes the adult phenotype φ to overshoot the population sample phenotype x. Thus juvenile phenotypes x farthest below x have adult pheno- types closest to φˆ = 10, making them the fittest. Evolution pathologically sends juvenile phenotypes racing away from the optimum (top graph) which causes the population mean fitness to plummet in time (bottom graph), faster with greater α. Other parameters: φˆ = 10, N = 1024, β = 1, σ2 = 0.01, D = 1.

For the pathological cases of α 1.25, 1.5, 1.75, 2.0 shown in Figure 8, the mean fitness of the population∈ actually { evolves to decrease} . This can be interpreted as an effect of frequency-dependent selection, because the adult phenotypes are frequency-dependent. It is well-known that under frequency- dependent selection, the mean fitness need not increase under evolution (Turner, 1967; Altenberg, 1991; Asmussen et al., 2004). The steady decrease in mean fit- ness as a response to natural selection can be considered to be an “evolutionary pathology” (Nunney, 1999; Altenberg, 2005a). So we have a situation where α maximizes the long-term mean fitness as it approaches α = 1, but when it exceeds 1, evolution pushes the population away from the optimum in this pathological manner.

15 3.1.2 Purifying Selection Model In this section we investigate the model for purifying selection, where the juvenile phenotype is determined by a multilocus bi-alleleic (0,1) genotype in which fitness increases as a function of the number of 1 alleles in the genotype. The juvenile phenotype x is the fraction of alleles in state 1. The number of loci is set to L = 128. Selection is for the extreme phenotype φˆ = 1.0, which only the all-1s geno- type can achieve, all other genotypes producing juvenile phenotype of lesser value. Thanks to the phenotypic plasticity function, however, adult phenotypes have the potential to reach 1.0. Fitness is again a Gaussian function of the departure of the adult phenotype from φˆ = 1.0. Since the juvenile phenotype falls in the small range x [0, 1], we rescale the selection intensity to β = 10 to make it roughly equivalent∈ to the stabilizing selection model. Populations are initialized with genotypes that are half 0 alleles and half 1 alleles, which is the equilibrium genotype under mutation alone. Figure 9 shows the evolution of the juvenile phenotypes for the range of α 4, 2, 1, 0, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 1.0, 1.25, 1.5, 1.75, 2.0 . Plotted are the averages of∈ 24{− independent− − runs for each parameter value. Shaded} bands are one standard deviation. We see that the evolutionary trajectories± are ordered by their α value, and maintain their order throughout the generations. At each time point the tra- jectories are monotonically decreasing in α. Strong anti-imitation, α < 0, yields the fastest evolution and phenotypes closest to φˆ = 1 at mutation-selection balance. For α = 1, genotype-phenotype disengagement occurs for the adult phenotypes and the genotype remains near the pure-mutation balance of half 0s, half 1s, to give juvenile phenotypes near 0.5. Hyper-imitation, α > 1, again produces an evolutionary pathology: the juvenile phenotypes evolve away from the optimum φˆ = 1.0 toward 0, faster with greater hyper-imitation.

16 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

1.0 ˆ 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 -4 ↵ = -2 ANTI-IMITATION (NEGATIVE) -1 } 0.8 0 NO PLASTICITY (0) 0.25 0.5 } IMITATION (POSITIVE < 1) 0.6 0.75 1.0 GENOTYPE-PHENOTYPE DISENGAGEMENT (1) 0.4

0.2 1.25 1.5

Juvenile Phenotype Juvenile 1.75 2.0 } HYPER-IMITATION (POSITIVE > 1) 0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 Generation

Figure 9: Evolutionary trajectories in the mean population juvenile phenotype x(t) for the multilocus purifying selection model. Plotted are the average of 24 runs. The shaded areas are one standard deviation. The trajectories are labeled by α and the range they± belong to: anti-imitation, imitation, and hyper- imitation. Other parameters: φˆ = 1.0, N = 1024, β = 10, µ = 0.1/L, L = 128.

When we examine the population mean fitnesses for the purifying selection model, we see a very different outcome from the stabilizing selection model. Figure 10 shows the mean fitness w(t) for the range of α from the average of 24 evolutionary trajectories. We no longer see the tradeoff between speed of evolution and equilibrium mean fitness that we observed with stabilizing selection (Figures 6, 7). Figure 10 shows that anti-imitation gives not only the fastest adaptation, but also the highest mean fitness at mutation-selection balance. As expected from the graphs of the phenotypes, the highest mean fitnesses at mutation-selection balance are attained with anti-imitation, and the genetic load increases with increasing α. The genetic load increases dramatically as α approaches 1 which gives genotype-phenotype disengagement. For all the values of α > 1 in the hyper-imitation range, the mean fitness at mutation- selection balance is near zero. This can be understood because with purifying selection for an extreme phe- notype, the juvenile phenotypes x are always less than the optimum φˆ = 1. The population is therefore always in a state that is like directional selection where the juvenile phenotypes are all on one side of the optimum. The juvenile pheno- types that are above average (x > x) have their adult phenotypes brought closer to the optimum φˆ = 1 by anti-imitation, whereas imitation shifts their adult phenotypes away from the optimum. The reverse holds for juvenile phenotypes below average (x < x). In Section 3.2.2 we will see that evolution favors the modifier gene which improves the fitness of the best genotypes even if it lessens it for the worst.

17 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 ANTI-IMITATION (NEGATIVE) 1.0 ↵ = -4, -2, -1 0 NO PLASTICITY (0) 0.25 0.8 0.5 IMITATION (POSITIVE < 1) 0.75 0.6 Mean Fitness 0.4

0.2 GENOTYPE-PHENOTYPE 1.0 DISENGAGEMENT (1) 1.25, 1.5, 1.75, 2.0 0 200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 HYPER-IMITATION (POSITIVE > 1) Generation

Figure 10: Evolutionary trajectories in the population mean fitness w(t) for the multilocus purifying selection model. for α ranges over (top to bottom) 8, 4, 2, 1, 0, 0.25, 0.5, 0.75, 0.95, 1.0, 1.25, 1.5, 1.75, 2.0 . Other parame- {− − − − } ters: φˆ = 10, N = 1024, β = 10, µ = 0.1/L, L = 128. No tradeoff is observed between speed of evolution and equilibrium genetic load. Rather, both decrease monotonically with α.

3.2 The Evolution of Imitation and Anti-Imitation In the previous section, we examined how populations evolve under different fixed values of the imitation parameter α. One could argue that evolution will produce the value of α that optimizes the population mean fitness, but such optimality arguments rely on group selection (Feldman et al., 1996). Modifier gene theory was developed to give a Darwinian mechanistic underpinning to the evolution of properties that do not fit the mold of individual fitness-determining phenotypes (Fisher, 1932; Nei, 1967; Feldman, 1972). Okasha (2018) calls this the “strategy of endogenization”, wherein mechanistic models are investigated which posit genetic variation for the property in question, and the fate of that variation is analyzed under the action of population dynamics. In this section we follow the “strategy of endogenization” and include genetic variation in the modifier locus controlling α. We ask how the α values evolve under the same population dynamics as with fixed α except that the modifier locus controlling α is subject to mutation and is no longer fixed. We will see that, for the most part, those values of α that gave rise to greater mean fitnesses are the same values that give a selective advantage to genetic variation of the modifier gene. However, as very large value of imita- tion evolve we encounter paradoxical and complex evolutionary dynamics that are not derivable from group-selection arguments, showing the importance of specific mechanistic modeling for the evolution of non-fitness parameters.

3.2.1 Stabilizing Selection Model Figure 11 shows the evolution of α during an evolutionary run of the stabilizing selection model, in which the population begins far from the optimal phenotype, but finally reaches a mutation-selection balance surrounding the phenotypic optimum. The population is initialized at α = 0 and x = 0 with a phenotypic

18 optimum of φˆ = 10. Both α and x mutate by addition of a Gaussian random variable with mean 0 and variance σ2 = 0.01. Shown are the average population juvenile phenotype x(t), the mean fitness w(t), and the mean α (in orange). For comparison, the trajectories for fixed α = 0 are plotted in blue. Compared with fixed α = 0, in the lineage with α evolving genotypic evo- lution is somewhat faster, and the mean fitness increases much faster. We see that α itself evolves along a rather meandering course, but goes clearly nega- tive (anti-imitation) until directional selection ceases and stabilizing selection begins, at which point α begins to increase. This is consistent with previous results for evolution under different fixed values of α.

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

10 ˆ Mean 8 Juvenile 6 4

Phenotype 2

200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 1.0

0.8

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 Mean 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 0.6 ↵ evolving ↵ =0 Fitness 0.4

0.2

200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 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

200 400 600 800 1000 1200 1400 ↵ =0 -0.1

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 -0.2 ↵ -0.3 -0.4 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 -0.5 ↵ evolving -0.6 Generation

Figure 11: Evolutionary trajectories for 1500 generations showing the popula- tion mean phenotype x(t), fitness w(t), and α, in orange. For comparison, an evolutionary run with fixed α = 0 is plotted in blue. Other parameters: φˆ = 10, N = 1024, σ2 = 0.01, D = 1.

To get a better idea of the trends in α evolution, we show the average α values from 24 runs of the evolutionary trajectories in Figure 12. Now it can be clearly seen that α evolves to decrease for the duration of directional selection, and as soon as the juvenile phenotypes reach the phenotypic optimum φˆ = 10 and stabilizing selection takes hold, α evolves to increase until the end of the simulation.

19 1.0

Imitation 0.5 Parameter

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 0 ↵ 1000 2000 3000 4000

-0.5

-1.0 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 10 ˆ Mean 8 Juvenile 6 4

Phenotype 2

1000 2000 3000 4000 1.0

Mean 0.8 Fitness 0.6 0.4

0.2

1000 2000 3000 4000 Generation

Figure 12: The average of 24 evolutionary trajectories for 1500 generations showing the evolution of the population mean α. Corresponding evolution of the mean juvenile phenotype x(t), and mean fitness w(t) are shown. Orange bands are one standard deviation. Anti-imitation α < 0 evolves during the period of directional± selection (generations 0 to 1000). When the juvenile phenotype ≈ has reached the phenotypic optimum φˆ = 10, stabilizing selection ensues, and α evolves to steadily increase from anti-imitation to imitation (α > 0). Other parameters: N = 1024, σ2 = 0.01, D = 1.

To see what happens after long periods of stabilizing selection, the evolu- tionary trajectories are extended to 12,000 generations in Figure 13. There we see the initial evolution of α into anti-imitation during directional selection, fol- lowed by upward evolution in a meandering fashion throughout the period of stabilizing selection, into the imitation range α > 0, until it reaches genotype- phenotype disengagement (α = 1), but then goes further into hyper-imitation (α > 1). At this point, around generation 7,700, something unusual happens. We see in the top graph that there is a sudden drop in mean fitness. This is shown in detail in the middle graph where the Y axis is enlarged. There is a steep plunge in the mean fitness when α enters the hyper-imitation zone.

20 1.0

0.8

Mean 0.6 Fitness 0.4 0.2

2000 4000 6000 8000 10 000 12 000 1.00 Mean 0.98 Fitness 0.96 (closeup) 0.94 0.92

0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10 000 12 000 Hyper- { 1.5 imitation 1.0

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 0.5

↵ 0 2000 4000 6000 8000 10 000 12 000 -0.5 Generation

Figure 13: Extended evolutionary trajectories to 12,000 generations showing the population mean fitness w(t) (top), zoomed in (middle), and mean evolved α (bottom), in orange. For comparison, an evolutionary run with fixed α = 0 is plotted in blue behind the orange. During directional selection α evolves negative. During stabilizing selection, α evolves to increase, with a great deal of fluctuation. When α evolves into the hyper-imitation range, the mean fitness drops, and a rapid collapse of α ensues. Other parameters: φˆ = 10, N = 1024, σ2 = 0.01, D = 1.

This is followed at around generation 8,100 by a dramatic collapse in α from it peak of around 1.3, down to 0.4, back in the middle of the imitation range. This collapse in α results in a sudden restoration of the mean fitness to its maximum range, greater than the equilibrium mean fitness seen with fixed α = 0. After this point, α is again seen to increase until it reaches hyper- imitation, upon which the mean fitness shows a sudden decline, and α begins a more gradual decline into the imitation range. This evolution into the hyper-imitation range followed by a collapse of α becomes even more apparent in a model with a multivariate phenotype. As > an illustration, we extend the phenotype to 16 dimensions, x = (x1, . . . , x16) and run evolution for 12,000 generations. A run is shown in Figure 14a. The top graph shows the trajectory of α: we see 11 repetitions of the pattern that α evolves into the hyper-imitation range followed by a dramatic evolutionary collapse of α to the middle of the imitation range. The middle graph in Figure 14a shows the evolution of population means of the 16 juvenile phenotypic variates. When α enters the hyper-imitation zone, the juvenile phenotypes start diverging from their optimal value (here we set φˆ = 1.0 to shorten the time to reach stabilizing selection). This is a consequence of the “evolutionary pathology” that juvenile phenotypes evolve away from the optimum when α > 1. As a consequence, the mean fitness of the population evolves lower during this phase (bottom graph). At some point, the random

21 changes in frequency due to Wright-Fisher finite population sampling cause the mean phenotypes to diverge sufficiently from φˆ that population effectively re- turns to the regime of directional selection. At this point, α is strongly selected to decrease, producing the collapse of α into the mid-imitation range. Subse- quently, the phenotypes evolve back toward φˆ and stabilizing selection returns, causing α to evolve larger. Finally, α again enters the hyper-imitation zone and the cycle repeats.

(a) α free (b) α ≤ 0.95

Figure 14: An extended evolutionary run with 16 phenotypic variables. (a) α is allowed to mutate into the hyper-imitation range. (b) α is kept below 0.95. In both (a) and (b), the top graph shows the evolution of α; middle shows the evolution of the population means of each of the 16 variables x1(t),..., x16(t); and the bottom shows the mean fitness w(t). Other parameters: φˆ = 1, N = 1024, σ2 = 0.01, D = 16.

The collapse phenomenon can be shown to require that α enter the hyper- imitation range by re-running the evolutionary dynamics where an upper limit is placed on the possible values of α below 1.0. An evolutionary run is shown in Figure 14b with the limit α 0.95. After the population enters the stabilizing selection phase, it remains there,≤ with continual limited fluctuations in α, x, and w due to finite Wright-Fisher sampling.

3.2.2 Purifying Selection Model For different fixed values of the imitation parameter α in the purifying selection model, Figure 9 revealed that decreasing values of α both speed up evolution and produce higher mean fitnesses at mutation-selection balance. This would lead us to expect that when α is controlled by the modifier locus, α would evolve to decrease. This is indeed what is observed.

22 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 1.0 ˆ 0.8 Juvenile 0.6 0.4 Phenotype 0.2

5000 10 000 15 000 1.0 0.8 Mean 0.6 Fitness 0.4 0.2

5000 10 000 15 000 Generation 5000 10 000 15 000 -0.5 -1.0

AAACTXicbVDLbhNBEOw1j4Tl5cCRywgrEgdk70aI5GhASBxyCFLsWIotq3fcjofMYzXTC1iW/4Er/BRnPoQbQsyuc8CElkZdqq5W9VRRahU4y34krRs3b93e2b2T3r13/8HD9t6jYXCVlzSQTjs/KjCQVpYGrFjTqPSEptB0Vly+qednH8kH5ewpL0uaGLywaq4kcqSGY9TlAqftTtbNmhLXQX4FOv02NHUy3Uvy8czJypBlqTGE8zwrebJCz0pqWqfjKlCJ8hIvaNUcuRb7kZqJufPxWRYNu6VDE8LSFM9jrxWhBgZ5EVfrFrbEFrlQxf9G5xXPjyYrZcuKycqN8bzSgp2oExAz5UmyXkaA0qt4sZAL9Cg55pTu/+3CylDY/k3tx87psBbpuDFZ9arge9pJ1L3jV6dvR71joqmlT9IZg3YWukyf12kMOf830utgeNDNX3YP3r/o9F9v0oZdeAJP4RnkcAh9eAcnMAAJH+ALfIVvyffkZ/Ir+b2RtpKrncewVa2dP6D9s7g= ↵ -1.5 -2.0 -2.5 -3.0 -3.5

Figure 15: Average of 24 evolutionary trajectories for the purifying selection model. Shown are the population average juvenile phenotypes (top), mean fitness (middle), and imitation parameter α (bottom). Long after mutation- selection balance is achieved in the juvenile phenotype, α continues to evolve deep into the anti-imitation range. Shaded bands are one standard deviation. Other parameters: φˆ = 1, N = 1024, β = 10, σ2 = 0.01,± L = 128.

Figure 15 shows the evolution of the population mean α values averaged over 24 independent evolutionary runs (bottom graph). Populations are initialized with major genotypes having 64 0-alleles and 64 1-alleles to give x(0) = 0.5, and with modifier alleles set to α(0) = 0. The simultaneous values of mean juvenile phenotype x(t) and mean fitness w(t) and plotted in the top and middle graphs. While each evolutionary trajectory in α is highly meandering, the trajectories show a secular decline in α, continuing far into the anti-imitation range long after the phenotype and mean fitness have reached a mutation-selection balance. How can this evolution of anti-imitation under purifying selection be un- derstood? Because the optimal phenotype is extreme, there is no way for the population to surround the optimal phenotype as it does in the stabilizing selec- tion model, where the population mean at mutation-selection balance is closer to the optimum than is the typical individual. Mutation pressure in the biallelic multilocus model pushes the population very strongly away from the optimal genotype (1, 1,..., 1). Thus the population will enter a perpetual state simi-

23 lar to directional selection, where anti-imitation moves fitter individuals’ adult phenotypes toward the optimum, while moving less fit individuals’ adult phe- notypes away from the optimum. The net effect is that the advantage from boosting the fitness of above average individuals outweighs the cost of reducing the fitness of below average individuals—the “Matthew effect” (Merton, 1968) again.

4 Discussion

The main general finding of this study is to confirm the value of “genes as cues” (Dall et al., 2015)—in this case, the cues being genes of other organisms. We show that information on the distribution of genetically determined phenotypes in a population is actionable information: by organisms becoming “population geneticists” and using this information in the right way to plastically change their phenotypes, this information can give it a selective advantage, and genetic variation for organismal capabilities that use this information can come under strong selection. On the flip side, use of information by the wrong phenotypic plasticity func- tion can slow adaptive evolution, increase genetic load, or even produce an evo- lutionary pathology where phenotypes evolve in the wrong direction and fitness decreases in evolution. When modifier genes are introduced that produce variation in a space of phenotypic plasticity functions, these functions will evolve to utilize this pop- ulation information to produce adaptive phenotypic plasticity. The adaptive phenotypic plasticity can evolve to such an extreme, however, that evolution ventures into evolutionary pathology with the evolution of hyper-imitation fol- lowed by imitation collapse. Some points should be emphasized. None of the phenomena observed here involve alteration of the mutation process. There is never any “directed mu- tation” in this model. Mutation remains the same Gaussian normal random variable throughout all the simulations. Moreover, the “direction” that directed variation would require is also entirely missing from this model: the population genetic information available to the organism never tells it where the pheno- typic optimum is; all the organism knows about is its own juvenile phenotype and the mean juvenile phenotype of the population. Even with a lack of this direct information about the phenotypic optimum, however, the knowledge of the population mean phenotype combined with the right phenotypic plasticity function enables the lineage to accelerate its evolution or maximize its mean fitness. We now discuss several aspects of the study.

4.1 Biological Reality This model of phenotypic plasticity and the behavior it exhibits are novel on a number of fronts. We are aware of no previous work that investigates anti- imitation: the adoption of traits in the direction opposite to that of a model. Every other component of our model, however, when considered separately, has appeared in many models in the literature, these being: natural selection on a quantitative trait, phenotypic optima, Gaussian selection, phenotypic plastic- ity, imitation, horizontal transmission, Wright-Fisher sampling, modifier genes,

24 and semelparous organisms with discrete non-overlapping generations that never interact with their ancestors. Each of these common components is in turn based on widely observed empirical biological phenomena. But when combined all together, we observe phenomena that are qualita- tively novel, and for which we know of no empirical documentation, namely: evolution of imitation, anti-imitation, and hyper-imitation in the absence of cultural transmission, determined by the extent of directional, stabilizing, or purifying selection, and evolutionary pathologies with the repeated buildup and collapse of imitation. It should also be noted that in the two models examined here, the absence of phenotypic plasticity (α = 0) was never an evolutionarily stable strategy under any condition of the population. There was always strong selection for either imitation or anti-imitation. What are we to make of the novelty of these phenomena? They really do go “outside the box”. We propose two principal interpretations: 1. Since these phenomena have never been documented in nature, the model is invalidated. 2. Since imitation has been previously thought of only as a phenomenon at- tending species with social learning, and the usefulness of anti-imitation never imagined, hypothesis-driven research has never considered these phenomena as hypotheses, and so they could have been entirely over- looked. But with these phenomena as new search-images, perhaps they can become documented. We leave it as an open question which of these may be correct. It may simply be that in nature, the complete set of mechanisms we model never appear together in a single species. But there are other instances where empirical phenomena had never been investigated until they were postulated theoretically, some phenom- ena quite illustrious. The possibility that some structures have been overlooked even in the most thoroughly studied organisms is illustrated by the recent dis- covery of new organs in humans — the central nervous system lymphatic vessels (Louveau et al., 2015), plantaris ligamentous tendon (Olewnik et al., 2021), and tubarial salivary glands (Valstar et al., 2021) — and in plants: the discovery of an entirely unknown, environmentally-induced plastic organ, the cantil, in heavily studied Arabidopsis thaliana (Gookin and Assmann, 2021). Given the common nature of each of the elements of the models we investi- gate here, we would expect that the evolutionary dynamics exhibited by their combination would be at least partly manifest in some species. We hope that researchers of phenotypic plasticity and animal behavior might reexamine their knowledge base in light of these phenomena.

4.2 Plasticity-First Evolution The hypothesis of “plasticity-first” or “plasticity-led” evolution proposed by West-Eberhard (2003) continues to attract interest (Levis and Pfennig, 2016), and one may ask whether the model and phenomena found here provide exam- ples of it (Justin Yeh, personal communication). The narrative of plasticity-first evolution is that a novel environment elicits novel phenotypes throughout a pop- ulation due to phenotypic plasticity, and this is followed by natural selection on

25 any genetic variation that optimizes the form and expression of the novel phe- notypes. A change in the environment is represented in our models by the population starting out far from the phenotypic optimum. Because the phenotype in our model is an unstructured quantitative trait, the only option for novelty is its magnitude. We could therefore consider an adult trait as “novel” when it is it differs from the genetically determined juvenile phenotype, which happens with any nonzero value of imitation parameter α. But these “novel” adult phenotypes are produced not by environmental change but by imitation (or anti-imitation as the case may be). That is, the adult phenotype φ = (1 α)x + α xˆ is a new value different from x due not to − a change in the phenotypic optimum φˆ representing environmental change, but due to phenotypic plasticity being sensitive when α = 0 to the standing genetic variation in the population. So changes in the environment,6 φˆ, do not affect phenotypic expression but only alter fitnesses. The production of novel values of φ under the action of α is solely the result of the genetic variation for the juvenile phenotype present in the population, with no relation to φˆ. So we must conclude that our model is not an example of plasticity-first evolution.

4.3 Interaction with Cultural Transmission While, for the purpose of investigating imitation without cultural transmission, we construct the life cycle and demography to hermetically seal off one gen- eration from the next, the phenomena we document could also influence the dynamics of populations that do have cultural transmission. The phenotypic plasticity in our models, based on organisms observing their conspecifics, can have the same underlying perceptual and behavioral mechanisms as those that would be involved in cultural transmission. Once an species has evolved the capability of social learning, or more gener- ally the capability to obtain sensory signals from the phenotypes of conspecifics and to incorporate these signals into plastic behavior or development, the dy- namics we document can come into play. In other words, we know of many species that engage in imitation and social learning. There is no a priori reason these sensory and cognitive capabilities could not tune in to genetically deter- mined juvenile phenotypic variation. In this case, the selective advantage of anti-imitation during directional selection and imitation during stabilizing se- lection could operate even when individuals from the previous generation are available to provide information. More complex models in which organisms can incorporate multigenerational information into their plasticity function merit investigation.

4.4 Heritability Measures of heritability partition phenotypic variance into genetic and environ- mental terms. The environmental variance is presumed to be caused by different environments experienced by each organism. In our models, the environment as represented by the phenotypic optimum, φˆ, is always kept constant in time, so all organisms experience the same external environment. If, however, we consider an organism’s cohort as part of its environment, then this environ- ment changes constantly throughout evolution. In this case we can interpret

26 the plastic change from juvenile to adult phenotype, x to φ, as an effect of the environment (the organism’s cohort), and use this to calculate the broad sense heritability of the population, which is just the reciprocal of the variance ratio (2):

Var[x] 1 H2 = = . Var[φ] (1 α)2 − This expression for the broad sense heritability H2 is less than one (the normal state of affairs) only for α < 0 (anti-imitation) or α > 2 (hyper-imitation), while for the imitation range, 0 < α < 1, heritability is greater than 1, as well as for the low hyper-imitation range 1 < α < 2. Heritability becomes infinite at genotype-phenotype disengagement (α = 1) as there is no variation in the adult phenotype. Heritability values greater than one occur with imitation because the environmental effect (regression toward the genotypic mean x) is negatively correlated with the genotypic values. If a quantitative breeder were trying to measure heritability of a trait, and was unaware that the adult phenotypes were the plastic products of imitation, then the shift in the mean of the offspring compared to the mean of the selected parents due to selective breeding would be incongruously large. Conversely, if anti-imitation were shaping the adult phenotypes, selective breeding would produce an anomalously small response in the juvenile phenotypes from what was expected based on the adult phenotypes.

4.5 Numerical Exploration Our emphasis here has been to present the broad qualitative behaviors of the imitation model. For this reason we have not pursued exhaustive exploration of the parameter space. We use population size N = 1024 throughout. This value is small enough to manifest the stochastic phenomena due to Wright-Fisher sampling, while large enough that natural selection will not be swamped out by stochastic sampling. We have chosen phenotypic optima φˆ = 10 or φˆ = 1 and mutation intensity σ2 = 0.01 or µ = 0.1/L that allow both directional and stabilizing or purifying selection phases within the number of generations that we ran. With a great deal more simulation it would be possible to quantify relationships between the parameters and such population properties as speed of evolution, equilibrium genetic load, phenotypic variance, etc., but such refined relationships are beyond the goals of the present work.

4.6 Related Models in The idea that organisms can get information on the location of an optimal phenotype by sampling the phenotypes of the population is the foundation for the widely used heuristic optimization method of Differential Evolution (DE) (Storn, 1996). While in our model mutation is a random variable sampled from a constant Gaussian distribution, in Differential Evolution the mutation distribution varies as a function of samples drawn from the population. A mutation distribution typically used in DE is as follows: an individual D y1 R is mutated by adding a random variable ξ where each coordinate ξi, i =∈ 1,...,D, is randomly chosen to be either 0, or the ith coordinate of a vector

27 z := yj y1 + α(yk yh), where the other three distinct individuals j, k, h = 1 are randomly− sampled− from the population (Li and Li, 2019). 6 In Differential Evolution, therefore, the distribution of genotypes in the pop- ulation is utilized by the mutation operator itself, not as in our model to alter solely a non-inherited adult phenotype. This use of population information to alter the mutation mechanism makes Differential Evolution “Lamarckian” in the terminology of memetic algorithms (Whitley et al., 1994). Whereas the model we consider would be termed “Baldwinian”, in that the plastic adult phenotype is not inherited, and the mutation process is not influenced at all by the distribution of organisms in the population.

4.7 The Phenotypic Plasticity Function Our choice of phenotypic plasticity function, φ = (1 α)x + αx, was crafted to be maximally simple and interpretable as an implementation− of imitation. It assumes that juvenile and adult phenotypes are for the same trait, and even keeps the population mean phenotype unchanged from juvenile to adult, φ = x. But these assumptions are not fundamental to the phenomena identified here. Since natural selection acts only on the adult phenotype φ, not x, there is no reason that φ and x even need to be within the same metric trait. The phenotype x could be for a trait entirely unrelated to the adult phenotype φ except that x is utilized as a cue by the developmental system, much as phenotypic plasticity is usually thought to utilize environmental cues. The phenotypic cue x also need not have been an ancestral part of the developmental pathway for the adult phenotype φ, but could have recently become incorporated into the signaling network for the development of φ. As long as x is genetically transmitted, and perception of x in conspecifics leads to plastic alteration of some adult phenotype under selection, then x will come under selection, and information about the distribution of x in the pop- ulation can provide actionable information that results in natural selection for the plasticity functions that properly utilize this information. A more general form that treats x and φ as different metric traits could be formally given as φ :(y, x, x,ˆ α) φ. Here, φ can be a completely dif- ferent trait from x, but the development7→ of φ utilizes information from the organism’s own genotype y, its juvenile phenotype x, and its population sam- plex ˆ as cues, parameterized by α. For imitation its form could be narrowed to φ(x, x,ˆ α) = γ((1 α)x + αxˆ) where the function γ maps between different phenotypic characters.− In the present work we let γ be the identity map, but this could be relaxed to study a wider space of phenotypic plasticity functions. If γ were a linear map, the model would be equivalent to the current model with a rescaling of the phenotypic optimum φˆ and mutational variance σ2. In our models, it took hundreds of generations for the phenotypic plasticity function to switch from the anti-imitation evolved during the directional selec- tion to the strong imitation selected for during the stabilizing selection phase. The mutational variance of α throughout this study was set to σ2 = 0.01. A shorter time to adaptive response of α could be achieved by a higher mutational variance for α. Such higher variance could be achieved through epigenetic inheri- tance. Further exploration of the mutational process for the imitation parameter is merited.

28 However, another potential way to shorten the time to adaptive response is if the phenotypic plasticity function were sophisticated enough to detect, solely through population sampling, when the population changed from the directional selection phase to the stabilizing selection phase. The phenotypic plasticity func- tion here took only a single datum on the population distribution of juvenile phenotypes. A question for further exploration is whether more detailed sample information can be utilized for to detect the change from directional to stabi- lizing selection and instantly change plasticity from anti-imitation to imitation. The necessary information could potentially be contained in the higher moments of the sample distribution of x (skewness, kurtosis, hyperskewness, hypertailed- ness, etc.), and also in the history of samples taken by successive generations of the organism, which could be stored epigenetically. A rich space of variation in the plasticity functions could be implemented using genetic programming (Zhang and Ciesielski, 1999) or neural network models (Stanley, 2007).

4.8 Evolutionary Pathologies Hyper-imitation produces the bizarre situation in which organisms have a selec- tive advantage when their juvenile phenotypes are further from the phenotypic optimum than the population mean, which produces the outcome (with fixed α) that evolution takes the population away from the phenotypic optimum, and the mean fitness of the population plummets. When α is free to evolve, evolution for increasing imitation during stabilizing selection gives rise to these pathological hyper-imitators. This creates an instability during stabilizing selection so that fluctuations of the population mean away from the optimum get amplified, with the resultant drop in mean fitness and collapse of imitation. These phenomena count as one more addition to the gallery of phenomena caused by frequency- dependent natural selection, which include dynamical chaos (Altenberg, 1991) and mean fitness decrease (Asmussen et al., 2004). The situation where α is produces ever greater mean fitness as it approaches 1 from below, but becomes pathological when it slightly exceeds 1, could be considered a kind of “cliff-edged fitness function” (Nesse, 2004). The concept was introduced by Nesse as a potential explanation for the evolutionary per- sistence of diseases like schizophrenia: selection pushes some phenotypic traits up to the edge of a cliff in phenotype space, like Half-Dome in Yosemite which has a smooth increasing approach from one side of the optimum, and a plum- meting drop just beyond the optimum. Mutational variation beyond the edge then produces genotypes with severely deleterious phenotypes. In the models here α does not directly map to fitness, but is rather a parameter in the evolu- tionary dynamics. So it could more accurately be described as an example of “cliff-edged dynamics”. We found that this instability is prevented by genetic constraints that pre- vent the production of hyper-imitation. This is a situation therefore that is a prime candidate for the evolution of evolvability suppression. This idea, first published by Nunney (1999) and elaborated in Altenberg (2005a), is that when genetic variation has an immediate selective advantage, but produces evolution in a direction that produces long-term harm to the population (an evolutionary pathology), then under certain circumstances, modifiers that suppress the gener- ation of this variation will selected for, and can come to prevent the evolutionary pathology.

29 We would expect that introduction of an additional modifier to the current model that shapes the distribution of mutation effects on the α modifier would evolve so as to keep α less than one.

4.9 Purifying versus Stabilizing Selection Very different outcomes were observed between the two models of stabilizing and purifying selection. In stabilizing selection, the juvenile phenotypes of the population surround the phenotypic optimum when mutation-selection balance has been reached, thereby siting the population mean closer to the optimum than the juvenile phenotype of the typical individual. This creates a great advantage to imitating the population mean. In purifying selection, the optimal phenotype is at the extreme of what genetic variation is capable of producing, and mutational pressure pushes the population mean away from this optimum. The juvenile phenotypes are thus incapable of surrounding the phenotypic optimum. For the fitter part of the population, the population mean is in the opposite direction from the optimum. This makes anti-imitation an advantage for the fitter organisms, and a disadvan- tage for the less fit. But it is this fitter part of the population that contribute most to the future population, so it is advantageous for a plasticity function to adopt anti-imitation and push the adult phenotypes closer to the extreme optimal phenotype. Thus imitation never evolves in the purifying selection model, not to mention hyper-imitation, neither during directional selection phase nor as the popula- tion reaches mutation-selection balance. There are no cliff-edged dynamics, nor emerging evolutionary pathology, and no instability. The model of cultural transmission studied by Gonzalez et al. (2017) is also of purifying selection. But in their model, juveniles are produced one by one rather than in a cohort, and they imitate only adult phenotypes, which may themselves have been imitated from earlier generations. Under those circum- stances, imitation is found to evolve under a wide range of population param- eters. Their model does not include the potential for anti-imitation, but its inclusion would be unlikely to change any outcomes because the models used for imitation — typical adult phenotypes — are likely to be better than default phenotype of the typical offspring. So this shows how the demographic structure of the population can be determinative in how imitative phenotypic plasticity evolves.

4.10 Developmental Structuring of Phenotypic Variation Our simple low-dimensional models with isotropic phenotypic variation share in common with many quantitative genetic models the artifact of being unable to capture the variational properties (Altenberg, 1995a) of very complex, high- dimensional genotype-phenotype maps. It is proposed in Altenberg (2005b) that phenotypic variation lies in low-dimensional structures embedded within the high dimensional space of complete phenotype descriptors, and that these structures are the result of developmental dynamics. Because phenotypic vari- ation is not omnidirectional, it cannot be assured that evolution can fill any selective bottle (Pattee, 1971; Gould and Lewontin, 1979). The probability that

30 a mutation will be adaptive — i.e. its evolvability — i.e. whether the dis- tribution of fitness effects of mutation has a fat upper tail (Altenberg, 1995a) — depends on how well this developmentally structured variation aligns with selective gradients on the space of phenotypes (Altenberg, 2005b). The capabilities needed for an organism to perceive phenotypes in con- specifics and translate those perceptions into phenotypic plasticity will of ne- cessity be generated by complex processes. This process complexity will give a particular structure to the map from juvenile genotype, to juvenile pheno- type, to perception of that phenotype, to plastic adult phenotype. Therefore the variation in the phenotypic plasticity function may be much more complex than simple imitation or anti-imitation. Exactly how complex organismal structures shape the variational proper- ties of phenotypes and their phenotypic plasticity are central open questions in developmental and systems biology. Cellular automata models of develop- ment have yielded emergent properties not observed in Gaussian quantitative genetic models (Hordijk and Altenberg, 2020). An open direction for inves- tigation would be to install developmental models into the situation studied here which give an interesting structure to the phenotypic variation involved in horizontal transmission of phenotypic information, and observe to what extent imitation and anti-imitation emerge as evolutionary outcomes.

4.11 Further Exploration The models studied here are kept at their most simple so that the relationships between the elements of the model and their behavior are as clear as possible. But there are many obvious elaborations that can be made which bring in additional biological phenomena. 1. Organisms here are all asexual and haploid. Sexual reproduction, diploid, and genetic recombination all merit additional treatments. Models with sexual reproduction need to be very carefully crafted when incorporating imitation, because a model combining blending inheritance with added variation mimics at the genetic level imitation at the phenotypic level. 2. The populations here are unstructured, and only a single population statis- tic, the mean juvenile phenotype, is extracted by organisms to use to plastically modify their adult phenotypes. Population subdivision and kin recognition would alter the information used by the organism. Genetic variation in the choice of individuals to collect phenotypic information from could come under selection. One can ask whether related individuals would come to be preferred or avoided as sources of population information to determine the plastic adult phenotype. 3. Selection here does not vary in time. If the phenotypic optimum changes in time, then the population will be moved away from stabilizing selection to different stages of directional selection. The most notable phenomenon demonstrated here is that stabilizing selection favors imitation while di- rectional selection favors anti-imitation. We would expect therefore that different temporal patterns of change in phenotypic optima would produce shifts toward anti-imitation to varying degrees.

31 4. Mutation rates explored here put the populations in the “strong mutation” regime, which keeps the populations polymorphic. In a “weak mutation” regime where the population is monomorphic most of the time, there would be no genetic variation from which the plasticity function could draw, so the adult phenotype would be identical to the juvenile phenotype during each monomorphic epoch, and there would be no selection on α. However, during a selective sweep the population would be polymorphic, and the phenotypic plasticity function would manifest itself. Since the invading mutant would benefit from the magnification under anti-imitation of its phenotypic difference from the inferior wild-type, we expect that anti- imitation would again be favored during positive selection in the weak- mutation regime, but this needs to be specifically modeled.

5 Conclusions

Here we have shown in a simple model that information on the distribution of genetically determined phenotypes in a population is usable by a species. When properly incorporated into an organism’s phenotypic plasticity, this population genetic information can speed adaptive evolution or reduce genetic load. There is thus selective opportunity for the organismal capabilities of imitation and anti-imitation. Cultural transmission of information between generations is not necessary for the evolution of imitation or anti-imitation. No special assumptions are required to realize this selective potential beyond an organismal ability to perceive a mean genetically determined phenotype for the population. The novelty of these phenomena raises the question of whether the conditions in the model never occur in nature, or whether these phenomena have simply never been investigated as empirical hypotheses. This report is only an initial exploration of this space of models. The results reported here are a novel demonstration of the existence of selection potential for the “organism as population geneticist”.

Acknowledgements

All three co-authors were supported by a grant from the Foundational Ques- tions Institute and Fetzer Franklin Fund, a donor advised fund of Silicon Valley Community Foundation, FQXi Grant number FQXi-RFP-IPW-1913. L.A. thanks Marcus W. Feldman for support from the Stanford Center for Computational, Evolutionary and Human Genomics and the Morrison Institute for Population and Resources Studies, ; the Mathematical Biosciences Institute at The Ohio State University, for its support through U.S. National Science Foundation awards DMS-0931642 and DMS-1839810, “A Sum- mit on New Interdisciplinary Research Directions on the Rules of Life.”

References

Altenberg, L. 1991. Chaos from linear frequency-dependent selection. American Naturalist, 138(1):51–68.

32 Altenberg, L. 1994. The evolution of evolvability in genetic programming. In Kinnear, K. E., editor, Advances in Genetic Programming, chapter 3, pages 47–74. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.

Altenberg, L. 1995a. Genome growth and the evolution of the genotype- phenotype map. In Banzhaf, W. and Eeckman, F. H., editors, Evolution and Biocomputation: Computational Models of Evolution, volume 899 of Lecture Notes in Computer Science, pages 205–259. Springer-Verlag, Berlin.

Altenberg, L. 1995b. The Schema Theorem and Price’s Theorem. In Whitley, D. and Vose, M. D., editors, Foundations of Genetic Algorithms 3, pages 23–49. Morgan Kaufmann, San Mateo, CA. Altenberg, L. 2005a. Evolvability suppression to stabilize far-sighted adapta- tions. Artificial Life, 11(4):427–444.

Altenberg, L. 2005b. Modularity in evolution: Some low-level questions. In Callebaut, W. and Rasskin-Gutman, D., editors, Modularity: Understanding the Development and Evolution of Natural Complex Systems, pages 99–128. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. Asmussen, M. A., Cartwright, R. A., and Spencer, H. G. 2004. Frequency- dependent selection with dominance: A window onto the behavior of the mean fitness. Genetics, 167:499–512. Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. 1983. The cultural transmission of acquired vari- ation: Effects on genetic fitness. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 100(4):567– 596.

Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and Feldman, M. W. 1973. Cultural versus biological inheritance: Phenotypic transmission from parents to children (a theory of the effect of parental phenotypes on children’s phenotypes). American Journal of Human Genetics, 25(6):618–637. Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and Feldman, M. W. 1981. Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.

Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. and Feldman, M. W. 1983. Cultural versus genetic adapta- tion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A., 80(16):4993– 4996. Charbonneau, M. and Bourrat, P. 2021. Fidelity and the grain problem in cultural evolution. Synthese, pages 1–22.

Charlesworth, B. 2013. Stabilizing selection, purifying selection, and mutational bias in finite populations. Genetics, 194:955–971. Dall, S. R., McNamara, J. M., and Leimar, O. 2015. Genes as cues: phenotypic integration of genetic and epigenetic information from a Darwinian perspec- tive. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 30(6):327–333.

33 Denton, K. K., Ram, Y., Liberman, U., and Feldman, M. W. 2020. Cultural evo- lution of conformity and anticonformity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117(24):13603–13614.

Feldman, M. W. 1972. Selection for linkage modification: I. Random mating populations. Theoretical Population Biology, 3:324–346. Feldman, M. W., Otto, S. P., and Christiansen, F. B. 1996. Population genetic perspectives on the evolution of recombination. Annual Review of Genetics, 30(1):261–295. Fisher, R. A. 1932. The evolutionary modification of genetical phenomena. In Proceedings of the 6th International Congress of Genetics I, pages 165–172. Gonzalez, M., Watson, R., and Bullock, S. 2017. Minimally sufficient condi- tions for the evolution of social learning and the emergence of non-genetic evolutionary systems. Artificial Life, 23(4):493–517. Gookin, T. E. and Assmann, S. M. 2021. Cantil: a previously unreported organ in wild-type Arabidopsis regulated by FT, ERECTA and heterotrimeric G proteins. Development, 148(11):dev195545.

Gould, S. J. and Lewontin, R. C. 1979. The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 205(1161):581–598. Hordijk, W. and Altenberg, L. 2020. Developmental structuring of phenotypic variation: A case study with a cellular automata model of ontogeny. Evolution & Development, 22(1):20–34. Leimar, O. 2005. The evolution of phenotypic polymorphism: random- ized strategies versus evolutionary branching. The American Naturalist, 165(6):669–681. Leimar, O., Hammerstein, P., and Van Dooren, T. 2006. A new perspective on developmental plasticity and the principles of adaptive morph determination. The American Naturalist, 167(3):367–376. Levis, N. A. and Pfennig, D. W. 2016. Evaluating ‘plasticity-first’ evolution in nature: key criteria and empirical approaches. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 31(7):563–574.

Li, Y. and Li, G. 2019. Differential with an evolutionary state estimation method and a two-level selection mechanism. Soft Comput- ing, pages https://doi.org/10.1007/s00500--019--04621--z. Liberman, U., Ram, Y., Altenberg, L., and Feldman, M. W. 2020. The evolution of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Theoretical Population Biology, 132:69–81. Louveau, A., Smirnov, I., Keyes, T. J., Eccles, J. D., Rouhani, S. J., Peske, J. D., Derecki, N. C., Castle, D., Mandell, J. W., Lee, K. S., et al. 2015. Structural and functional features of central nervous system lymphatic vessels. Nature, 523(7560):337–341.

34 Merton, R. K. 1968. The Matthew effect in science. Science, 159(3810):56–63. Nei, M. 1967. Modification of linkage intensity by natural selection. Genetics, 57:625–641. Nesse, R. M. 2004. Cliff-edged fitness functions and the persistence of schizophrenia. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(6):862–863. Nunney, L. 1999. Lineage selection: Natural selection for long–term benefit. In Keller, L., editor, Levels of Selection in Evolution, pages 238–252. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ. Okasha, S. 2018. The strategy of endogenization in . Syn- these, pages 1–23. Olewnik,L., Karauda, P., Gonera, B., Kurtys, K., Tubbs, R. S., Paulsen, F., Szyma´nski,R., and Polguj, M. 2021. Impact of plantaris ligamentous tendon. Scientific Reports, 11(1):1–8. Pattee, H. H. 1971. Physical theories of biological co-ordination. Quarterly Reviews of biophysics, 4(2-3):255–276. Richerson, P. J. and Boyd, R. 1978. A dual inheritance model of the human evolutionary process I: basic postulates and a simple model. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 1(2):127–154. Stanley, K. O. 2007. Compositional pattern producing networks: A novel abstraction of development. Genetic programming and evolvable machines, 8(2):131–162. Storn, R. 1996. On the usage of differential evolution for function optimization. In Proceedings of North American Fuzzy Information Processing, pages 519– 523. IEEE. Turner, J. R. 1967. On supergenes. I. The evolution of supergenes. The Amer- ican Naturalist, 101(919):195–221. Valstar, M. H., de Bakker, B. S., Steenbakkers, R. J., de Jong, K. H., Smit, L. A., Klein Nulent, T. J., van Es, R. J., Hofland, I., de Keizer, B., Jasperse, B., Balm, A. J., van der Schaaf, A., Langendijk, J. A., Smeele, L. E., and Vogel, W. V. 2021. The tubarial salivary glands: A potential new organ at risk for radiotherapy. Radiotherapy and Oncology, 154:292–298. West-Eberhard, M. J. 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford University Press. Whitley, D., Gordon, V. S., and Mathias, K. 1994. Lamarckian evolution, the Baldwin effect and function optimization. In International Conference on Parallel Problem Solving from Nature, pages 5–15. Springer. Yampolsky, L. Y. and Stoltzfus, A. 2001. Bias in the introduction of variation as an orienting factor in evolution. Evolution & Development, 3(2):73–83. Zhang, M. and Ciesielski, V. 1999. Genetic programming for multiple class object detection. In Australasian Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, pages 180–192. Springer.

35