Age Differences in the Verbal Span of Apprehension

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Age Differences in the Verbal Span of Apprehension Child Development, September/October 1999, Volume 70, Number 5, Pages 1082–1097 The Role of Attention in the Development of Short-Term Memory: Age Differences in the Verbal Span of Apprehension Nelson Cowan, Lara D. Nugent, Emily M. Elliott, Igor Ponomarev, and J. Scott Saults In previous studies of memory span, participants have attended to the stimuli while they were presented, and therefore have had the opportunity to use a variety of mnemonic strategies. In the main portion of the present study, participants (first- and fourth-grade children, and adults; 24 per age group) carried out a visual task while hearing lists of spoken digits and received a post-list digit recall cue only occasionally, for some lists. Under these conditions, list information presumably must be extracted from a passively held store such as au- ditory sensory memory. The results suggest that each individual has a core memory capacity limit that can be observed clearly in circumstances in which it cannot be supplemented by mnemonic strategies, and that the ca- pacity limit appears to increase with age during childhood. Other, attention-demanding processes also contrib- ute to memory for attended lists. INTRODUCTION included in tests of intelligence, increases with age in childhood (Dempster, 1985). The explanations of short- Years of effort have been devoted to an understand- term memory development, however, have varied ing of short-term memory (see Case, 1995; Cowan, widely (Cowan, 1997). They have focused primarily 1995; Engle and Oransky, 1999), by which we mean on increases in knowledge, metaknowledge, and strat- the temporarily increased availability of information egies (e.g., Case, Kurland, & Goldberg, 1982; Naus, in memory that may be used to carry out various Ornstein, & Aivano, 1977), with considerable empha- types of mental tasks. By all accounts, the capacity of sis also on the speed of various processes (e.g., Cowan short-term memory is limited, which constrains the et al., 1998; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993; Hitch & quality of human performance. Researchers do not Towse, 1995; Kail & Park, 1994). What has been rela- agree, however, on any particular theoretical explana- tively rare, though, is research emphasizing the pos- tion of the observed limitations in short-term mem- sibility of developmental change in the capacity of ory. Proposals have included storage capacity limita- short-term memory; that is, the number of items that tions (Case, 1995; Halford, Maybery, & Bain, 1988; can be held in mind at a time (Cowan, 1999; Halford Miller, 1956), time limitations because of memory de- et al., 1988; Halford, Mayberry, O’Hare, & Grant, cay (Baddeley, 1986), limitations in the speed and effi- 1994; Pascual-Leone, 1970). ciency of mnemonic strategies such as rehearsal (Bad- It is difficult to identify one basic short-term mem- deley, 1986) or short-term memory search (Cowan et ory faculty separated from all others. Nevertheless, al., 1998), limitations in knowledge about the stimuli considerable research suggests that there is an identi- (Hulme et al., 1997), and limitations in how well at- fiable faculty: a short-term memory store with a ca- tention can be controlled and irrelevant information pacity considerably smaller than the typical digit inhibited (Engle, Conway, Tuholski, & Shisler, 1995). span, as discussed by Broadbent (1975). As Miller There also are principles of item distinctiveness, ret- (1956) noted, adults can remember lists composed of roactive and proactive interference between items, about seven meaningful items or “chunks.” That and mental structure, which are certain to influence memory limit is undoubtedly reached, however, with performance on memory tasks in general, including the use of a variety of mnemonic strategies. Partici- those taken to index short-term memory processes pants may enhance memory by repeating items to (Baddeley & Hitch, 1993; Cowan, Wood, Nugent, & themselves and forming new, meaningful associa- Treisman, 1997; Nairne, Neath, Serra, & Byun, 1997). tions between items shortly after they are presented It is difficult to determine which limitations are criti- (i.e., through rote and elaborative rehearsal; see Naveh- cal in particular tasks. Benjamin & Jonides, 1984). When one examines as- The debate about the nature of short-term memory pects of short-term memory that are unlikely to be in- limitations has important implications also for the de- fluenced by such strategies, one finds a limit in adults velopment of short-term memory in childhood. It has long been known that performance on simple short- © 1999 by the Society for Research in Child Development, Inc. term memory tasks, such as the digit span task that is All rights reserved. 0009-3920/99/7005-0004 Cowan et al. 1083 of about three or four items, not seven. Evidence of There is a logical explanation for why the span of this limit includes the maximum number of items that apprehension that Sperling (1960) and Darwin et al. participants group together in recall, the maximum (1972) observed was appreciably smaller than the list length resulting in errorless performance, and the short-term memory limit that Miller (1956) observed. list length for which proactive interference from pre- Miller concentrated on procedures in which items vious, similar items is not obtained (Broadbent, 1975; were presented one at a time. Under those circum- Halford et al., 1988). This memory limit might reflect stances, mnemonic strategies, like rehearsal, can be the amount that can be subsumed in the focus of at- used during the list presentation. In contrast, when an tention at one time (Cowan, 1995). It also appears to array is presented, there are too many concurrent items be the same as what Sperling (1960) called the “span for mnemonic strategies to be applied efficiently. The of apprehension.” In the present article, develop- number recalled then may represent the number that mental changes in this span of apprehension will be can be processed in parallel in a very short time, with examined in children and adults. little chance of enhancing performance with strate- Sperling’s (1960) well-known study was designed gies requiring serial processing. to examine visual sensory memory, and it demon- The presentation of spatial or spatiotemporal ar- strated that that form of memory has a very large ca- rays may cause particular difficulties when the partic- pacity. The study, however, also established a span of ipants are young children, who may become con- apprehension. The procedure involved very-briefly- fused about how to report the items. To examine the presented character arrays. In a “partial report” pro- development of the span of apprehension in a simpler cedure, a postarray cue indicated which row of the manner, we have developed a modification of the whole array to report. With a short postarray delay before report procedure that permits the serial presentation the cue, participants could typically report at least of items. Lists of spoken digits were presented. To three of the items in a single cued row, suggesting that limit the contribution of mnemonic strategies, in one most items in the array were temporarily available for condition the participants were asked to ignore most report. In the “whole report” procedure, there was no of the spoken lists and play a silent computer game, post-array cue, and the entire array was to be reported. both to hold attention and to tie up articulatory pro- However, participants could only report about four cesses that otherwise could be used for covert verbal items in the entire array. In the partial report condition, rehearsal. Only occasionally within this procedure, increases in the delay before the cue impaired perfor- the game was interrupted by a postlist signal to recall mance and, when the cue was delayed 1 s, the propor- the most recent list of digits. We presumed that partic- tion of the cued row that was recalled was no greater ipants could do this by searching through a sensory than recall of the same row in the whole report condi- memory record of the series of recent spoken events tion. This suggested that the whole report limit repre- to identify some of the digits, which could then be sented a basic capacity limit of about four items. saved in a capacity-limited form of storage. The re- Sperling inferred that the mechanism is as follows. sults suggest that such a process was indeed possible, A large number of items is at first represented in an un- and that a procedure logically analogous to Sperling’s processed form in sensory memory storage. This mem- whole report procedure works for serial lists of spo- ory, however, quickly fades. A limited number of items ken stimuli. (about four in adults) can be processed from this sen- Past research gives few clues about the ages at which sory store, and they become represented in a capacity- capacity limits in storage should increase. Memory limited short-term store. Items must be entered into span increases steadily during the ages of 3 to 10 capacity-limited storage before they fade from sensory years (e.g., Dempster, 1985), but the basis of this in- memory, and a postarray cue in the partial report pro- crease is unclear. We investigate whether a develop- cedure simply limits the items that the participant is mental change in the short-term memory capacity asked to process before sensory memory fades. The limit is a viable account of at least some of the devel- same processes appear to take place in audition. Dar- opmental change in span. win, Turvey, and Crowder’s (1972) results using spa- Performance also was examined in a condition in tiotemporal arrays of spoken items were analogous to which the spoken items were to be attended when those of Sperling (1960), though the value of the partial they were presented. The magnitude of capacity- report cue persisted in this modality until the cue was limited storage should be the same for both attended delayed by 4 s (as compared to 1 s in vision).
Recommended publications
  • Development of Strategies for Recall and Recognition
    Developmental Psvchology 1976, Vol. 12, No'. 5. 406-410 Development of Strategies for Recall and Recognition BARBARA TVERSKY AND EVELYN TEIFFER Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel Kindergartners, third graders, and fifth graders viewed 30 pictures of familiar objects, and then their free recall of the object names and their recognition of the original pictures were tested. The recognition test included pairing each picture with another similar picture of the same object. Half the subjects in each age-group were prepared for recall with a strategy known to improve it in adults, and half were prepared for recognition with a strategy known to improve recognition in adults. Children encoded the stimuli differentially in accordance with the expected mem- ory task and retrieved different stored information for each task. Both free recall and picture recognition memory improved with age. The recall strategy improved free recall performance at all ages, but the recognition strategy improved recogni- tion performance only at the oldest age tested. Investigators of both children's and ing pictorial comparison (Tversky, 1973a). adults' memory have found different Finally, there is evidence that the cognitive strategies beneficial for different memory skills underlying various strategies develop tasks (e.g., Flavell, 1970; Paivio, 1971). at different ages. Rohwer (1970) has argued Strategies are ways of encoding or repre- that children are unable to utilize imaginal senting material to facilitate later retrieval. codes effectively until they are efficient at For instance, Paivio and Csapo (1969) found verbal encoding; presumably the verbal that whereas verbal codes are particularly code allows effective access or retrieval of effective in sequential memory tasks, imagi- the image.
    [Show full text]
  • The Neuroscience of Memory Development: Implications for Adults Recalling Childhood Experiences in the Courtroom
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by City Research Online Howe, M. L. (2013). Memory development: implications for adults recalling childhood experiences in the courtroom. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(12), pp. 869-876. doi: 10.1038/nrn3627 City Research Online Original citation: Howe, M. L. (2013). Memory development: implications for adults recalling childhood experiences in the courtroom. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(12), pp. 869-876. doi: 10.1038/nrn3627 Permanent City Research Online URL: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/4186/ Copyright & reuse City University London has developed City Research Online so that its users may access the research outputs of City University London's staff. Copyright © and Moral Rights for this paper are retained by the individual author(s) and/ or other copyright holders. All material in City Research Online is checked for eligibility for copyright before being made available in the live archive. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to from other web pages. Versions of research The version in City Research Online may differ from the final published version. Users are advised to check the Permanent City Research Online URL above for the status of the paper. Enquiries If you have any enquiries about any aspect of City Research Online, or if you wish to make contact with the author(s) of this paper, please email the team at [email protected]. 1 The Neuroscience of Memory Development: Implications for Adults Recalling Childhood Experiences in the Courtroom Mark L. Howe1 1Department of Psychology City University London Northampton Square London EC1V 0HB UK [email protected] IN PRESS: Nature Reviews Neuroscience Preface Adults frequently provide compelling, detailed accounts of early childhood experiences in the courtroom.
    [Show full text]
  • Memory: Issues of Import to School Psychologists
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 365 927 CG 025 192 AUTHOR John, Kirk R. TITLE Memory: Issues of Import to School Psychologists PUB DATE Apr 93 NOTE 24p.; Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Association of School Psychologists (25th, Washington, DC, April 13-17, 1993). PUB TYPE Speeches/Conference Papers (150) Guides General (050) EDRS PRICE MF01/PC01 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Adolescent Development; Adolescents; Child Development; Children; *Cognitive Development; Elementary Secondary Education; *Memory; *School Psychologists ABSTRACT This document defines memory as a complex, interactive process that is a prerequisite for all higher learning. Without intact memory skills, a host of disorders may ensue ranging frow mild learning problems to disorientation and helplessness (Lezak, 1983). Because of the pervasive and central role memory plays in people's lives, school psychologists should have at the very least a basic understanding of the memory process. In this regard, this paper addresses selected topics from the knowledge base on memory that have relevance for school psychologists. First, an information-processing model of memory is presented and the three separate memory systems through which information is processed are described (sensory memory, short-term memory, and long-term memory). A section on the developmental aspects of memory considers memory development in infants, children and adolescents. Metamemory, or the individual's conscious awareness of his/her own memory capabilities and functions, also is explained in this section. The final major section of the paper focuses on memory and reading. This information is discussed in the context of how it can be applied by school psychologists in their decision-making.
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Trends in the Metamemory-Memory Behavior Relationship: an Lntegrative Review*
    CHAPTER 3 Developmental Trends in the Metamemory-Memory Behavior Relationship: An lntegrative Review* Wolfgang Schneider lntroduction Since the beginning of the 1970s, increasing attention has been di­ rected toward the development of children's awareness of their own mem­ ory. This phenomenon has been termed metamemory by John Flavell (1971), who broadly defined it as the individual's potentially verbalizable knowledge concerning any aspect of inforrnation storage and retrieval. Whereas the interest in metamemory development has been growing rap­ idly since then, documenting the construct's general usefulness in a wide area of different memory situations, there are at least two critical points that deserve special consideration: (1) the conceptualization of the con­ struct and (2) its status as a predictor of actual memory behavior. While the latter is the topic of the present chapter and is discussed in more detail, we first concentrate on some problems related to the definition of meta­ memory. The first taxonomy of classes of memory knowledge was provided in the metamemoryinterview study by Kreutzer, Leonard, and Flavell (1975). •This manuscript was prepared during a stay at the Department of Psychology, Stan­ ford University, which was made possible by a grant from the Stiftung Volkswagenwerk, Hannover, Federal Republic of Germany. METACOGN!TIO , COGNIT IO , 57 Copyright © 1985 by Academic PTess AND HUMA PERFORMA CE All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. VOL. l ISBN 0-12-262301-0 58 Wolfgang Schneider Subsequently, Flavell and WeHman (1977) offered a more systematic and elaborated attempt to classify different types or contents of metamemory. Although alternative conceptualizations have been presented (see Chi, 1983), and more extended and general models of metacognition have been developed since (see FlaveH, 1978, 1979, 1981; Kluwe, 1982), the classifi­ cation scheme of FlaveH and WeHman has been used in the majority of empirical studies concerned with the development of metamemory.
    [Show full text]
  • Models of Working Memory
    MODELS OF WORKING MEMORY Mechanisms of Active Maintenance and Executive Control Edited by AKIRA MIYAKE AND PRITI SHAH PUBLISHED BY THE PRESS SYNDICATE OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CAMBRIDGE The Pitt Building, Trumpington Street, Cambridge, United Kingdom CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS The Edinburgh Building, Cambridge CB2 2RU, UK http: //www.cup.cam.ac.uk 40 West 20th Street, New York, NY 10011-4211, USA http: //www.cup.org 10 Stamford Road, Oakleigh, Melbourne 3166, Australia © Cambridge University Press 1999 This book is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provisions of relevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take place without the written permission of Cambridge University Press. First published 1999 Printed in the United States of America Typeface Stone Serif 9/12 pt. System QuarkXpress™ [HT] A catalog record for this book is available from the British Library. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Models of working memory : mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control / edited by Akira Miyake, Priti Shah. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and indexes. ISBN 0-521-58325-X. – ISBN 0-521-58721-2 (pbk.) 1. Short-term memory. I. Miyake, Akira, 1966– . II. Shah, Priti, 1968– . BF378.S54M63 1999 153.1´3 – dc21 98–35134 CIP ISBN 0 521 58325 X hardback ISBN 0 521 58721 2 paperback 1 Models of Working Memory An Introduction PRITI SHAH AND AKIRA MIYAKE Working memory plays an essential role in complex cognition. Everyday cog- nitive tasks – such as reading a newspaper article, calculating the appropriate amount to tip in a restaurant, mentally rearranging furniture in one’s living room to create space for a new sofa, and comparing and contrasting various attributes of different apartments to decide which to rent – often involve mul- tiple steps with intermediate results that need to be kept in mind temporarily to accomplish the task at hand successfully.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion and Memory: Children’S Long-Term Remembering, Forgetting, and Suggestibility
    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 72, 235–270 (1999) Article ID jecp.1999.2491, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Emotion and Memory: Children’s Long-Term Remembering, Forgetting, and Suggestibility Jodi A. Quas University of California, Berkeley Gail S. Goodman University of California, Davis Sue Bidrose, Margaret-Ellen Pipe, and Susan Craw University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand and Deborah S. Ablin University of California, Davis Children’s memories for an experienced and a never-experienced medical procedure were examined. Three- to 13-year-olds were questioned about a voiding cystourethrogram fluoros- copy (VCUG) they endured between 2 and 6 years of age. Children 4 years or older at VCUG were more accurate than children younger than 4 at VCUG. Longer delays were associated with providing fewer units of correct information but not with more inaccuracies. Parental avoidant attachment style was related to increased errors in children’s VCUG memory. Children were more likely to assent to the false medical procedure when it was alluded to briefly than when described in detail, and false assents were related to fewer “do-not-know” responses about the VCUG. Results have implications for childhood amnesia, stress and memory, individual differences, and eyewitness testimony. © 1999 Academic Press Key Words: children’s memory; infantile amnesia; emotion; attachment; suggestibility; eyewitness testimony. That emotions play a role in influencing the memorability of personal expe- riences has been recognized since, if not before, the early writings of Freud This study was funded by grants to Gail S. Goodman from the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues (Division 9 of the American Psychological Association) and the University of California, Davis Faculty Research Grant Program, and a grant to Margaret-Ellen Pipe from the New Zealand Health Research Council.
    [Show full text]
  • Short-Term Memory Capacity and Recall of Students with and Without Intellectual Giftedness: an Empirical Inquiry
    The University of Southern Mississippi The Aquila Digital Community Dissertations Fall 2017 Short-Term Memory Capacity and Recall of Students with and without Intellectual Giftedness: An Empirical Inquiry Angela Foil Ellison University of Southern Mississippi Follow this and additional works at: https://aquila.usm.edu/dissertations Part of the Gifted Education Commons, and the Special Education and Teaching Commons Recommended Citation Ellison, Angela Foil, "Short-Term Memory Capacity and Recall of Students with and without Intellectual Giftedness: An Empirical Inquiry" (2017). Dissertations. 1464. https://aquila.usm.edu/dissertations/1464 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by The Aquila Digital Community. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of The Aquila Digital Community. For more information, please contact [email protected]. SHORT-TERM MEMORY CAPACITY AND RECALL OF STUDENTS WITH AND WITHOUT INTELLECTUAL GIFTEDNESS: AN EMPIRICAL INQUIRY by Angela Mae Foil Ellison A Dissertation Submitted to the Graduate School, the College of Education and Psychology, and the Department of Curriculum, Instruction, and Special Education at The University of Southern Mississippi in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy December 2017 SHORT-TERM MEMORY CAPACITY AND RECALL OF STUDENTS WITH AND WITHOUT INTELLECTUAL GIFTEDNESS: AN EMPIRICAL INQUIRY by Angela Mae Foil Ellison December 2017 Approved by: ________________________________________________ Dr. Gregory W. Smith, Committee Chair Assistant Professor, Curriculum, Instruction, and Special Education ________________________________________________ Dr. Sharon E. Rouse, Committee Member Associate Professor, Curriculum, Instruction, and Special Education ________________________________________________ Dr. Steven R. Chesnut, Committee Member Assistant Professor, Educational Research and Administration ________________________________________________ Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Metamemory for Prospective and Retrospective Memory Tasks
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by University of Hertfordshire Research Archive CHILDREN’S METAMEMORY PREDICTIONS 1 Metamemory prediction accuracy for simple prospective and retrospective memory tasks in 5-year-old children Lia Kvavilashvili1 & Ruth M. Ford2 1 University of Hertfordshire, UK 2 Anglia Ruskin University, UK * Both authors have contributed to the paper equally Address for correspondence: Lia Kvavilashvili Department of Psychology University of Hertfordshire College Lane Hatfield, Hertfordshire AL10 9AB United Kingdom Ph. +44 (0)1707 285121 Fax. +44 (0)1707 285073 E-mail. [email protected] Word count: 9,229 CHILDREN’S METAMEMORY PREDICTIONS 2 Abstract It is well documented that young children greatly overestimate their performance on tests of retrospective memory (RM) but the present investigation was the first to examine their prediction accuracy for prospective memory (PM). Three studies were conducted, each testing a different group of 5-year-olds. In Study 1 (n=46), participants were asked to predict their success in a simple event-based PM task (remembering to convey a message to a toy mole if they encountered a particular picture during a picture-naming activity). Before naming the pictures the children listened to either a reminder story or a neutral story. Results showed that children were highly accurate in their PM predictions (78% accuracy) and that the reminder story appeared to benefit PM only in children who predicted they would remember the PM response. In Study 2 (n=80), children showed high PM prediction accuracy (69%) regardless of whether the cue was specific or general, and despite typical over-optimism regarding their performance on a 10-item RM task using item-by-item prediction.
    [Show full text]
  • Longitudinal Working Memory Development Is Related to Structural Maturation of Frontal and Parietal Cortices
    Longitudinal Working Memory Development Is Related to Structural Maturation of Frontal and Parietal Cortices Christian K. Tamnes1, Kristine B. Walhovd1, Håkon Grydeland1, Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/25/10/1611/1779760/jocn_a_00434.pdf by MIT Libraries user on 17 May 2021 Dominic Holland2, Ylva Østby1, Anders M. Dale2, and Anders M. Fjell1 Abstract ■ Parallels between patterns of brain maturation and cognitive points in 79 healthy children and adolescents in the age range development have been observed repeatedly, but studies directly of 8–22 years. Longitudinal change in cortical and subcortical testing the relationships between improvements in specific cog- volumes was quantified by the use of Quantitative Anatomical nitive functions and structural changes in the brain are lacking. Regional Change. Improvement in working memory was related Working memory development extends throughout childhood to cortical volume reduction in bilateral prefrontal and posterior and adolescence and likely plays a central role for cognitive devel- parietal regions and in regions around the central sulci. Impor- opment in multiple domains and in several neurodevelopmental tantly, these relationships were not explained by differences in disorders. Neuroimaging, lesion, and electrophysiological studies gender, age, or intelligence level or change in intellectual abil- indicate that working memory emerges from coordinated inter- ities. Furthermore, the relationships did not interact with age actions of a distributed neural network in which fronto-parietal and were not significantly different in children, young adoles- cortical regions are critical. In the current study, verbal working cents, and old adolescents. The results provide the first direct memory function, as indexed by performance on the Keep Track evidence that structural maturation of a fronto-parietal cortical task, and volumes of brain regions were assessed at two time network supports working memory development.
    [Show full text]
  • Working Memory Beats Age: Evidence of the Influence of Working Memory on the Production of Children’S Emotional False Memories
    fpsyg-12-714498 August 12, 2021 Time: 13:34 # 1 BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT published: 18 August 2021 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.714498 Working Memory Beats Age: Evidence of the Influence of Working Memory on the Production of Children’s Emotional False Memories Chiara Mirandola1,2* and Francesca Pazzaglia2 1 Department of Philosophy, Sociology, Education and Applied Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy, 2 Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy Emotional valence and working memory ability (WM) affect false memories’ production in adults. Whereas a number of studies have investigated the role of emotional valence in children’s tendency to produce spontaneous false memories, individual differences in WM have not been previously included. In the current article, we were interested in investigating whether emotion and WM would interact in influencing the propensity to incur inferential false memories for scripted events. Ninety-eight typically developing Edited by: children ( first-, third-, and eighth- graders) were administered the Emotional false Beatriz Martín-Luengo, National Research University Higher memory paradigm – allowing to study false memories for negative, positive, and neutral School of Economics, Russia events – and a WM task. Results showed that regardless of age, valence influenced false Reviewed by: memories’ production, such that positive events protected against incurring distortions. Kate Cockcroft, University of the Witwatersrand, Furthermore, WM interacted with valence, such that children with higher WM abilities South Africa produced fewer false memories for negative events. Concerning confidence judgments, Carlos M. Gómez, only the youngest group of children claimed to be overconfident when committing false University of Seville, Spain memories for negative and neutral events.
    [Show full text]
  • Sleep, Memory and Emotion
    G. A. Kerkhof and H. P. A. Van Dongen (Eds.) Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 185 ISSN: 0079-6123 Copyright Ó 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. CHAPTER 4 Sleep, memory and emotion Matthew P. Walker* Sleep and Neuroimaging Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA Abstract: As critical as waking brain function is to cognition, an extensive literature now indicates that sleep supports equally important, different, yet complementary operations. This review will consider recent and emerging findings implicating sleep, and specific sleep-stage physiologies, in the modulation, regulation and even preparation of cognitive and emotional brain processes. First, evidence for the role of sleep in memory processing will be discussed, principally focusing on declarative memory. Second, at a neural level, several mechanistic models of sleep-dependent plasticity underlying these effects will be reviewed, with a synthesis of these features offered that may explain the ordered structure of sleep, and the orderly evolution of memory stages. Third, accumulating evidence for the role of sleep in associative memory processing will be discussed, suggesting that the long-term goal of sleep may not be the strengthening of individually memory items, but, instead, their abstracted assimilation into a schema of generalized knowledge. Forth, the newly emerging benefit of sleep in regulating emotional brain reactivity will be considered. Finally, and building on this latter topic, a
    [Show full text]
  • Infantile Amnesia: a Critical Period of Learning to Learn and Remember
    The Journal of Neuroscience, June 14, 2017 • 37(24):5783–5795 • 5783 Viewpoints Infantile Amnesia: A Critical Period of Learning to Learn and Remember X Cristina M. Alberini and Alessio Travaglia Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003 Infantile amnesia, the inability of adults to recollect early episodic memories, is associated with the rapid forgetting that occurs in childhood.Ithasbeensuggestedthatinfantileamnesiaisduetotheunderdevelopmentoftheinfantbrain,whichwouldprecludememory consolidation, or to deficits in memory retrieval. Although early memories are inaccessible to adults, early-life events, such as neglect or aversive experiences, can greatly impact adult behavior and may predispose individuals to various psychopathologies. It remains unclear how a brain that rapidly forgets, or is not yet able to form long-term memories, can exert such a long-lasting and important influence. Here, with a particular focus on the hippocampal memory system, we review the literature and discuss new evidence obtained in rats that illuminates the paradox of infantile amnesia. We propose that infantile amnesia reflects a developmental critical period during which the learning system is learning how to learn and remember. The long-lasting influence of episodic infantile experiences ries (also referred to as hippocampal memories) are severely and the paradox of infantile amnesia compromised by challenges occurring early in life. These chal- How do we develop our abilities to learn and remember facts, lenges may be either psychological (e.g., trauma, neglect, or de- people, things, relationships, and places? These memories define privation of social experience) or biological (e.g., the impact of our identities; they store autobiographical episodes that can be genetic mutations on development).
    [Show full text]