{PDF EPUB} Titanic a Man-Made Disaster By

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

{PDF EPUB} Titanic a Man-Made Disaster By Read Ebook {PDF EPUB} Titanic A Man-Made Disaster by Catherine Sarah-Jane Cave Has a 220-year-old Mystery Been Solved By These Two Brothers? Have you heard stories of mysterious islands and buried treasures? Do you think they’re for real? If you are an avid fan of the hit fantasy series, Pirates of the Caribbean, you must be dreaming of pretty tough adventures just to score a trove and get filthy rich. In the real world, myths, tales and legends are passed down through generations, making them believe that a hidden treasure chest still exists. In Nova Scotia, Canada lies the one of the most highly-sought after secrets of all time: Oak Island and its mythical treasure. Since the 19th century, many attempts have been made by explorers from different parts of the globe to locate the cryptic artifacts and treasure. Despite the number of lives sacrificed on the island while braving the tumultuous journey of unearthing its bounty, many skilled and talented explorers are still inspired to risk it. The Lagina brothers have never been bothered by the horrific rumors. In fact, their interest in the island’s hidden treasure just keeps getting deeper. Find out what keeps them obsessed with cracking this 220-year old mystery. Just how close are the Lagina brothers to cracking the case? Read on to discover the mystery for yourself. Obsession at a Young Age. Little did an 11-year-old Rick Lagina realize that by merely flicking through a copy of Reader’s Digest in 1965, his life would completely change. The article that struck him was about the legend of a treasure being buried on an island in the wilds of Canada. He and his brother Marty got even more fascinated when their father showed them an article about Oak Island in The Wall Street Journal. Between the two brothers, Rick was more captivated to further investigate the buried treasure, especially when he learned that many people had tried their luck and yet failed to find it. He may have been young at the time, but he knew he had a big mission calling his name. Natural-Born Explorer. Rick Lagina’s fascination with the Oak Island treasure is not a surprise, as the young boy was already a natural explorer. He had already engaged in a treasure hunt back in 1964, in his hometown of Kingsford. In this exploration, he was thrilled to have seen a huge granite rock. As a young boy, his curiosity led him to check what was underneath, so he sought the assistance of his brother, Marty, and a crew of other kids their age in their neighborhood to move the rock. To his dismay, not a trace of any valuable item was found underneath the big rock except for a pile of dirt, but Rick knew there was something more out there in the world, just waiting for him to discover. A Strange Revelation. In 1796, another young boy discovered a strange circular depression on the ground of Oak Island. Eight years later, the Onslow Company launched a team to search the area. After locating the strange markings, the explorers dug deep until they hit something solid. They found a buried stone tablet with an odd inscription. Experts set to work, trying to decipher and understand the transcription, but it wasn’t until 1886 that it was finally translated. Read on to discover the shocking message engraved on the stone, and who was able to interpret its nerve-wracking message. The Tablet’s Curse. After almost a century, the tablet’s transcription was finally understood thanks to a Halifax professor. It read, “forty feet below, two million pounds lie buried.” No one had a clue what “pounds” meant on the inscription. They couldn’t identify whether it was a treasure chest worth two million pounds or a decoy. Adding more mystery to the island’s hidden treasure is the famous legend that said seven people must die before the Oak Island treasure is revealed. Six people have died already. Is this an indication that the island waits for one more life to be sacrificed before the treasure is discovered? The Treasure's Origins. The Oak Island mystery is 220 years old and no one has cracked the case yet, so plenty of theories have surfaced. Some people, oddly, traced the origin of the treasure back to William Shakespeare. Many of those who doubt the bard was real, believe his literary works were actually written by Francis Bacon. So some treasure hunters hypothesized that Bacon may have built a pit to bury Shakespeare’s manuscripts and his earnings. Another theory suggests that the hidden treasure of Oak Island belongs to Marie Antoinette, who was speculated to have sent her maid to Nova Scotia to hide her wealth on the island, with the help of the French Navy. Of Sailors and Pirates. The list of theories goes on and on and some entries even involve the participation of pirates and sailors. Captain Kidd was theorized to have buried some of his treasure on Oak Island. Meanwhile, others believe that Captain Blackbeard admitted to stashing something there when he claimed to have hidden it “where none but Satan and myself can find it.” There was also a theory that suggested Spanish sailors had hidden their treasure in a secret location of the island. While these ideas may be intriguing, there is no proof to support any of them. However, the theory we’re about to reveal to you is by far the most engaging. Are you ready? Check this out. A Sacred Treasure. One of the most intriguing theories about the Oak Island treasure relates to the alleged Masonic markings that can be discovered all throughout the island. Pertaining to the “Secret Vault” in York Rite Freemasonry, this story could contain the lead needed to uncover the island’s buried treasure. Correlated with the Francis Bacon theory, the story goes that, as Bacon was leading a Rosicrucian project on Oak Island, he hid a number of religious artifacts there. Researchers, Daniel Ronnstam and Petter Amundsen, believed that the said artifacts could be hidden among Shakespeare’s manuscripts. So, Bacon and Shakespeare’s are now correlated. Hmm. Blast From the Past. Going back to 1975, when the first intriguing discovery happened, a young boy named Daniel McGinnis was wandering around Oak Island, when he saw a strange circular depression in the earth. Curious as to what he saw, he moved closer and saw a tree with branches cut away. He thought that the branches were intentionally removed so that the tree could serve a purpose: to act as a pulley. When he remembered all the pirate and treasure chest stories he’d heard over the years, the boy talked to his friends and asked for their help to investigate further. Further Investigation. Determined to know more details, Daniel McGinnis sought the help of his friends, John Smith and Anthony Vaughn, to uncover whatever might be hidden under the tree. With all their might and interest, the young boys started digging, and after reaching two feet down, they hit flagstone. The youngsters decide to remove the flagstone and keep digging, making it about another 30 feet down, finding layers over layers of oak logs. Eventually, they realized they couldn’t go any further and thought to themselves that they’d have to wait years before they’d have the ability to pursue their mission. However, this was just the start of their great adventure. Digging Deeper. Almost a decade later, the Onslow Company arrived and dug in the same part as the three boys had in the past. With their equipment, they were able to delve down further, and after reaching 60 feet, they saw more oak logs, this time with coconut fiber and charcoal in thick layers. Interested to find out what lay beneath, the team kept digging until they came across the stone tablet. Thrilled with the discovery and imagining the rich fruits of their labor were about to be uncovered, they pulled out another layer of oak logs. All of a sudden, water started flooding into the pit. Could this be another decoy? The only way they could stop the flooding was to make a tunnel to let the water out. A 45-Year Delay. Nothing they tried worked and the non-stop pouring of water made their mission impossible to carry out. The Onslow company realized that they could not push their search any further. The pit was designed with a 500-foot waterway that connects to Smith’s Cove. This means water can get in freely from the sea, and any time there’s an attempt to empty it, it quickly fills up again from the endless oceanic supply. This conundrum had a massive impact on the discovery of the mysterious treasure. Figuring a way around it caused a 45-year delay in the exploration. However, even such seemingly insurmountable obstacles cannot deter explorers. In 1847, a new group tried their luck. The question is, will they succeed? A New Group of Explorers. Unfazed by the challenges of the Oak Island treasure, a new team of explorers called The Truro Company, set foot on the island. They found an efficient way to drill samples of the pit without the water being an issue. Then their lucky day came: as they were drilling, they hit the jackpot. Tada! They successfully drilled through two chests filled with coins. They thought this was the first sign of the massive hidden treasure that was promised by the stone.
Recommended publications
  • The Origin and Spread of Modern Humans 1. Modern
    THE ORIGIN AND SPREAD OF MODERN HUMANS • Modern Humans • The Advent of Behavioral Modernity • Advances in Technology • Glacial Retreat • Cave Art • The Settling of Australia • Settling the Americas • The Peopling of the Pacific 1. MODERN HUMANS Anatomically modern humans (AMHs) evolved from an archaic Homo sapiens African ancestor • Eventually AMHs spread to other areas, including western Europe, where they replaced, or Interbred with, Neandertals Out of Africa II • Accumulating to support African origin for AMHs • White and Asfaw: finds near village of Herto are generally anatomically modern • Leakey: Omo Kibish remains from 195,000 B.P. appear to be earliest AMH fossils yet found • Sites in South Africa of early African AMHs 1 • Anatomically modern specimens, including skull found at Skhūl, date to 100,000 B.P. • Early AMHs in Western Europe often referred to as Cro Magnons, after earliest fossil find of an anatomically modern human in France • AMHs may have inhabited Middle East before the Neandertals GENETIC EVIDENCE FOR OUT OF AFRICA II • Researchers from Berkeley generated a computerized model of Homo evolution • Based upon the average rate of mutation in known samples of mtDNA • Only the mother contributes mtDNA • Everyone alive today has mtDNA that descends from a woman (dubbed Eve) who lived in sub-Saharan Africa around 200,000 years ago GENETIC EVIDENCE FOR OUT OF AFRICA II • In 1997, mtDNA extracted showed that the Neandertals differed significantly from modern humans • 27 differences between modern humans and Neandertal •
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioural Complexity and Modern Traits in the Philippine Upper Palaeolithic
    Behavioural Complexity and Modern Traits in the Philippine Upper Palaeolithic AlFred F. PAwlIk introduction The discussion of cultural, cognitive, and behavioral modernity has a long tradition in europe’s prehistoric archaeology ( Dibble 1989; Hahn 1986; Jelinek 1982; klein 1995, 1999; Mellars 1989a, 1989b). The appearance of specialized blade indus- tries, bone and antler tools, and especially figurative art, musical instruments, and personal ornaments are seen as significant indicators of the highly developed cultural and cognitive abilities of their makers (Clottes 2001; Conard 2003; Conard et al. 2004). The seemingly sudden appearance of expressive art and symbolism together with complex tool technologies in europe at around 40,000 years ago has been attributed to explosive cultural and cognitive advancement with the arrival of ana- tomically modern Homo sapiens ( Klein and Blake 2002; Mellars 1991; Mithen 1996). Whether this Upper Palaeolithic revolution in europe was due to social factors or genetic mutation, was related to changes in the ecosystem, or has a cultural expla- nation (such as competition with another human species, the Neanderthals) is still under debate ( Bar-Yossef 2002; Conard et al. 2004; d’errico 2003; Haidle 2006; McBrearty and Brooks 2000; Mellars 2005; Zilhão 2001). Yet, the “human revolu- tion” model is used to explain the success of the Homo sapiens immigrants over the Neanderthals ( Bräuer and Smith 1992; Conard 2006, 2008; Mellars 2005). On the other hand, potential indicators of an earlier and gradually developing cul- tural and cognitive modernity have been seen in African assemblages. The appearance of some modern cognitive traits (e.g., production of projectile points, shell-fishing, personal ornaments, notational or incised pieces, and pigment processing) in Africa has been dated back to the Middle Pleistocene, earlier than the first evidence of ana- tomically modern hominids 200,000 years ago (Henshilwood et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Ahead of the Game: Middle and Upper Palaeolithic Hunting Behaviors in the Southern Caucasus
    Ahead of the Game: Middle and Upper Palaeolithic Hunting Behaviors in the Southern Caucasus The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Adler, Daniel S., Guy Bar#Oz, Anna Belfer#Cohen, and Ofer Bar# Yosef. 2006. Ahead of the Game: Middle and Upper Palaeolithic Hunting Behaviors in the Southern Caucasus. Current Anthropology 47, no. 1: 89–118. Published Version doi:10.1086/432455 Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:12242824 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#LAA Current Anthropology Volume 47, Number 1, February 2006 89 Ahead of the Game Middle and Upper Palaeolithic Hunting Behaviors in the Southern Caucasus by Daniel S. Adler, Guy Bar-Oz, Anna Belfer-Cohen, and Ofer Bar-Yosef Over the past several decades a variety of models have been proposed to explain perceived behavioral and cognitive differences between Neanderthals and modern humans. A key element in many of these models and one often used as a proxy for behavioral “modernity” is the frequency and nature of hunting among Palaeolithic populations. Here new archaeological data from Ortvale Klde, a late Middle–early Upper Palaeolithic rockshelter in the Georgian Republic, are considered, and zooar- chaeological methods are applied to the study of faunal acquisition patterns to test whether they changed significantly from the Middle to the Upper Palaeolithic.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Evolution to Revolution
    Research Ideas and Outcomes 5: e38546 doi: 10.3897/rio.5.e38546 Research Article Language evolution to revolution: the leap from rich-vocabulary non-recursive communication system to recursive language 70,000 years ago was associated with acquisition of a novel component of imagination, called Prefrontal Synthesis, enabled by a mutation that slowed down the prefrontal cortex maturation simultaneously in two or more children – the Romulus and Remus hypothesis Andrey Vyshedskiy ‡ ‡ Boston University, Boston, United States of America Corresponding author: Andrey Vyshedskiy ([email protected]) Reviewable v1 Received: 25 Jul 2019 | Published: 29 Jul 2019 Citation: Vyshedskiy A (2019) Language evolution to revolution: the leap from rich-vocabulary non-recursive communication system to recursive language 70,000 years ago was associated with acquisition of a novel component of imagination, called Prefrontal Synthesis, enabled by a mutation that slowed down the prefrontal cortex maturation simultaneously in two or more children – the Romulus and Remus hypothesis. Research Ideas and Outcomes 5: e38546. https://doi.org/10.3897/rio.5.e38546 Abstract There is an overwhelming archeological and genetic evidence that modern speech apparatus was acquired by hominins by 600,000 years ago. On the other hand, artifacts signifying modern imagination, such as (1) composite figurative arts, (2) bone needles with an eye, (3) construction of dwellings, and (4) elaborate burials arose not earlier than © Vyshedskiy A. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
    [Show full text]
  • Cultural Modernity: Consensus Or Conundrum?
    COMMENTARY Cultural modernity: Consensus or conundrum? Nicholas J. Conard1 Department of Early Prehistory and Quaternary Ecology, and Senckenberg Center for Human Evolution and Paleoecology, Universität Tübingen, 72070 Tübingen, Germany he development of the Out of cognitive capacities of past humans, neu- Africa model for the origins and rological modeling, or linguistic spec- T dispersal of modern humans in ulation cannot answer this question the 1980s and 1990s led the field without explicit links to the archaeological of paleoanthropology to drop other ap- record. Hard data about the material cul- proaches to the evolution of modernity. ture of the past, like that from Diepkloof, Earlier models suggesting a gradual evo- provide the key source of information lution from archaic to modern human needed to make progress on the debate morphology and cultural behavior simul- over the origins of modern behavior. taneously across the Old World have been The dominant views in the field suggest replaced by models pointing to Africa as that modern humans, after evolving ana- the only continent where modern humans tomically roughly 200,000 years ago in evolved and modern patterns of cultural Africa (3), gradually (4), or suddenly (5), behavior emerged. The important paper became culturally like all living people by Texier et al. (1) in a recent issue of today. Most researchers point to im- PNAS is the latest of a number of recent portant finds of new technology, evidence papers pointing to South Africa as a key Fig. 1. Fourclassesofsymbolicartifactsthatarefirst for changed patterns of subsistence and area for studying the origins of cultural documented outside Africa.
    [Show full text]
  • Dietary and Behavioral Strategies Of
    University of Arkansas, Fayetteville ScholarWorks@UARK Theses and Dissertations 5-2011 Dietary and Behavioral Strategies of Neandertals and Anatomically Modern Humans: Evidence from Anterior Dental Microwear Texture Analysis Kristin Lynn Krueger University of Arkansas, Fayetteville Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarworks.uark.edu/etd Part of the Biological and Physical Anthropology Commons Recommended Citation Krueger, Kristin Lynn, "Dietary and Behavioral Strategies of Neandertals and Anatomically Modern Humans: Evidence from Anterior Dental Microwear Texture Analysis" (2011). Theses and Dissertations. 92. http://scholarworks.uark.edu/etd/92 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UARK. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UARK. For more information, please contact [email protected], [email protected]. 1 DIETARY AND BEHAVIORAL STRATEGIES OF NEANDERTALS AND ANATOMICALLY MODERN HUMANS: EVIDENCE FROM ANTERIOR DENTAL MICROWEAR TEXTURE ANALYSIS DIETARY AND BEHAVIORAL STRATEGIES OF NEANDERTALS AND ANATOMICALLY MODERN HUMANS: EVIDENCE FROM ANTERIOR DENTAL MICROWEAR TEXTURE ANALYSIS A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Anthropology By Kristin L. Krueger University of Wisconsin-Madison Bachelor of Science in Anthropology, 2003 University of Wisconsin-Madison Bachelor of Science in Spanish, 2003 Western Michigan University Master of Arts in Anthropology, 2006 May 2011 University of Arkansas ABSTRACT The extreme gross wear of Neandertal anterior teeth has been a topic of debate for decades. Several ideas have been proposed, including the excessive mastication of grit- laden foods and non-dietary anterior tooth use, or using the anterior dentition as a clamp or tool.
    [Show full text]
  • Development of Behavioral Modernity by Hominins Around 70,000 Years Ago Was Associated with Simultaneous Acquisition of a Novel
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/166520; this version posted March 15, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY 4.0 International license. Development of behavioral modernity by hominins around 70,000 years ago was associated with simultaneous acquisition of a novel component of imagination, called prefrontal synthesis, and conversion of a preexisting rich-vocabulary non-recursive communication system to a fully recursive syntactic language Andrey Vyshedskiy1,2* 1Boston University, Boston, USA; 2ImagiRation LLC, Boston, MA, USA *Corresponding author: Andrey Vyshedskiy, Ph.D., Boston University, Boston, USA, Tel: +1 (617) 433-7724; E-mail: [email protected] Keywords: Language evolution, hominin evolution, human evolution, recursive language, flexible syntax, human language, syntactic language, modern language, Cognitive revolution, Great Leap Forward, Upper Paleolithic Revolution, Neanderthal language, neurolinguistics Abstract There is an overwhelming archeological and genetic evidence that modern speech apparatus was acquired by hominins by 600,000 years ago 1. On the other hand, artifacts signifying modern imagination, such as (1) composite figurative arts, (2) bone needles with an eye, (3) construction of dwellings, and (4) elaborate burials arose only after 70,000 years ago. It remains unclear (1) why there was a long gap between acquisition of modern speech apparatus and modern imagination, (2) what triggered the acquisition of modern imagination 70,000 years ago, and (3) what role language might have played in this process. Our research into evolutionary origin of modern imagination has been driven by the observation of strict dependency between childhood use of full recursive syntactic language and imagination.
    [Show full text]
  • Michelle C. Langley Editor
    Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology Series Michelle C. Langley Editor Osseous Projectile Weaponry Towards an Understanding of Pleistocene Cultural Variability Osseous Projectile Weaponry Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology Series Edited by Eric Delson Vertebrate Paleontology, American Museum of Natural History New York, NY 10024,USA [email protected] Eric J. Sargis Anthropology, Yale University New Haven, CT 06520,USA [email protected] Focal topics for volumes in the series will include systematic paleontology of all vertebrates (from agnathans to humans), phylogeny reconstruction, functional morphology, Paleolithic archaeology, taphonomy, geochronology, historical biogeography, and biostratigraphy. Other fields (e.g., paleoclimatology, paleoecology, ancient DNA, total organismal community structure) may be considered if the volume theme emphasizes paleobiology (or archaeology). Fields such as modeling of physical processes, genetic methodology, nonvertebrates or neontology are out of our scope. Volumes in the series may either be monographic treatments (including unpublished but fully revised dissertations) or edited col- lections, especially those focusing on problem-oriented issues, with multidisciplinary coverage where possible. Editorial Advisory Board Ross D. E. MacPhee (American Museum of Natural History), Peter Makovicky (The Field Museum), Sally McBrearty (University of Connecticut), Jin Meng (American Museum of Natural History), Tom Plummer (Queens College/CUNY). More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/6978
    [Show full text]
  • Neanderthal Cognitive Equivalence: Epistemological Problems and a Critical Analysis from Radical Embodiment
    Neanderthal cognitive equivalence: epistemological problems and a critical analysis from radical embodiment Dissertation der Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen zur Erlangung des Grades eines Doktors der Philosophie (Dr. phil.) vorgelegt von Duilio Garofoli aus Rom/Italien Tübingen 2015 Gedruckt mit Genehmigung der Mathematisch-Naturwissenschaftlichen Fakultät der Eberhard Karls Universität Tübingen. Tag der mündlichen Qualifikation: 07.07.2015 Dekan: Prof. Dr. Wolfgang Rosenstiel 1. Berichterstatter: Prof. Dr. Katerina Harvati 2. Berichterstatter: PD Dr. Miriam Noël Haidle ii Table of Contents Summary ......................................................................................................................................................... 1 Zusammenfassung ....................................................................................................................................... 2 List of publications in the thesis .............................................................................................................. 3 A. Accepted works ...................................................................................................................................... 3 Personal contribution .................................................................................................................................. 4 1. Introduction ..............................................................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Neanderthal Behavioral Modernity and Symbolic Capabilities
    Field Notes: A Journal of Collegiate Anthropology Volume 7 Article 5 2015 Neanderthal Behavioral Modernity and Symbolic Capabilities Liam McGill Harvard University Follow this and additional works at: https://dc.uwm.edu/fieldnotes Recommended Citation McGill, Liam (2015) "Neanderthal Behavioral Modernity and Symbolic Capabilities," Field Notes: A Journal of Collegiate Anthropology: Vol. 7 , Article 5. Available at: https://dc.uwm.edu/fieldnotes/vol7/iss1/5 This Current Debate is brought to you for free and open access by UWM Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Field Notes: A Journal of Collegiate Anthropology by an authorized administrator of UWM Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Neanderthal Behavioral Modernity and Symbolic Capabilities Liam McGill Harvard University, Harvard College Abstract: In recent years, the distinction between anatomically modern hu- mans (AMHs) and Neanderthals has come into question in light of genetic evidence that suggests they interbred. Some claim that this distinction can be maintained by delineating anatomical, developmental, and behavioral differ- ences between the two species. This paper examines the body of evidence for and against behavioral modernity in Neanderthals by using their capacity for symbolic thought as a proxy for modern behavioral capabilities. Evidence for colorant usage, personal ornamentation, symbolic etchings, and interactions between AMHs and Neanderthals supports the hypothesis that Neanderthals were capable of symbolic thought and thus possessed a behavioral modernity similar to that of early AMHs. The emergence of these behaviors seems to be closely tied to cultural/demographic explanations rather than genetic/ cognitive explanations and suggests promising opportunities for future re- search.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Approaches to Creativity
    P1: OJL Trim: 7in × 10in Top: 0.498in Gutter: 0.871in CUUS1027-15 cuus1027/Kaufman ISBN: 978 0 521 51366 1 May 10, 2010 20:17 CHAPTER 15 Evolutionary Approaches to Creativity Liane Gabora and Scott Barry Kaufman 1. Introduction Studies at the intersection of creativity and evolution are not limited to investi- Many species engage in acts that could be gations into the biological evolution of a called creative (J.C. Kaufman & A.B. Kauf- highly creative species. Creative ideas them- man, 2004). However, human creativity is selves might be said to evolve through cul- unique in that it has completely transformed ture. Human creativity is distinctive because the planet we live on. We build skyscrap- of the adaptive and open-ended manner in ers, play breathtaking cello sonatas, send which change accumulates. Inventions build ourselves into space, and even decode our on previous ones in ways that enhance their own DNA. Given that the anatomy of the utility or aesthetic appeal, or make them human brain is not so different from that applicable in different situations. There is no of the great apes, what enables us to be so a priori limit to how a creative idea might creative? Recent collaborations at the fron- unfold over time. A cartoon character may tier of anthropology, archaeology, psychol- inspire the name and logo for a hockey team ogy, and cognitive science are culminating (the Mighty Ducks), which might in turn in speculative but increasingly sophisticated inspire toys, cereal shapes, cigarette lighter efforts to piece together an answer to this designs, or for that matter work its way into question.
    [Show full text]
  • The Spread of Modern Humans in Europe SPECIAL FEATURE
    The spread of modern humans in Europe SPECIAL FEATURE John F. Hoffecker1 Institute of Arctic and Alpine Research, University of Colorado, 1560 30th Street, Campus Box 450, Boulder, CO 80309-0450 Edited by Richard G. Klein, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, and approved May 15, 2009 (received for review March 28, 2009) The earliest credible evidence of Homo sapiens in Europe is an made a significant genetic and/or cultural contribution to the archaeological proxy in the form of several artifact assemblages modern human population of Europe. (Bohunician) found in South-Central and possibly Eastern Europe, The interpretation of the genetics of living human popula- dating to <48,000 calibrated radiocarbon years before present (cal tions, supplemented with the analysis of fossil DNA extracted BP). They are similar to assemblages probably made by modern from Neanderthal and early modern human specimens, favors humans in the Levant (Emiran) at an earlier date and apparently the replacement model. The limited number of mtDNA lineages represent a population movement into the Balkans during a warm among living non-African populations is consistent with a model climate interval [Greenland Interstadial 12 (GI 12)]. A second of rapid dispersal, initially eastward out of Africa and subse- population movement may be represented by a diverse set of quently northward into the Eurasian interior (5). Results to date artifact assemblages (sometimes termed Proto-Aurignacian) found from ongoing reconstruction of the Neanderthal genome indi- in the Balkans, parts of Southwest Europe, and probably in Eastern cate a distinctive pattern that cannot be found among living Europe, and dating to several brief interstadials (GI 11–GI 9) that humans and suggests minimal genic exchange between the two preceded the beginning of cold Heinrich Event 4 (HE4) (Ϸ40,000 cal taxa (6).
    [Show full text]