<<

DOCUMENT RESUME

ED 145 499 CS 501 878

AUTHOR Adler, Richard P.; And Others TITLE Research on the Effects of on Children; A Review of the Literature and Recommendations for Future Research. .INSTITUTION National Science Foundation, Washington, L.C. RANN Program. PUB DATE 77 GRANT APR-75-10126' NOTE 230p.; Some pages of docuent may be marginally legible due to size of type AVAILABLE FROM Superintendent of Documents, U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, D.C. 20402 (Stock No. 038-000-00336-4, $3.75 paper)

EDRS PRICE MF-S0.83 HC-$12.71 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS Bibliographies; Childhood Attitudes; *Children; *Childrens Television; Guidelines; *Literature Reviews; *Television Commercials; *Television Research; Television Viewing IDENTIFIERS *Advertising

ABSTRACT This report summarizes the present state of knowledge about the effects of television advertising on children. After a discussion cf children's television viewing patterns, the report reviews the existing research relevant to such,issuas as children's ability to distinguish commercials from program material; the influence of format and audiovisual techniques on children's perceptions of commercials; the effects of characters in commercials, self-concept appeals, offers, food advertising, theivolume and repetition of commercials, and medicine advertising; violence and unsafe acts in commercials directed to children; the effects of television advertising on consumer socialization; and television advertising and parent/child relations. The report then makes recommendations for future research. Appendixes present evaluations of 21 individual studies, a statistical profile of the national research resources currently available for relevant studies, and the children's advertising guidelines issued,by the National Association of Broadcasters, Code Authority. A bibliography of relevant publica.dons is included.(GW)

***********e*********************************************************** Documents acquired by ERIC include many informal unpublished * materials not available from other sources. ERIC makes every effort * * to obtain the best copy available. Nevertheless, items of marginal * * reproducibility are often encountered and this affects the quality * * of the microfiche and hardcopy reproductions ERIC makes available * * via the ERIC Document Reproduction Service (EERS). EDRS is not * responsible for the quality of the original document. Reproductions * * supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made from the original. * **************************************************************,******** U S DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH EDUCATION& WELFARE NATIONAL INSTITUTE OF EDUCATION TH,S DOCUMENT HAS PEEN REPRO DUCED E`AAC it Y AS RECEiVED FROM THE PERSON OR ORGANIZATONORGiN ATING IT POINTS OF vIE v4 OR OPINIONS STA TEO DO NOT NECE SSAR+L RE PRE ',ENT OF FILIAL NA TONAL iNST,TOTE 0$ EDur AEON P0,0 TJON OP POL

Research or, the Effects of Television Advertising on Children

A Review of the Literature and Recommendations for Future Research

Report prepared for: NATIONAL SCIENCE FOUNDATION Research Applications Directorate RANNResearch Applied to National Needs Division of Advanced Productivity Research and Technology

This material is based upon research supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. Apr'75-10126. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recom- mendations expressed in this publication are those of the author {s} and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. CONTENTS

Executive Summary 1 I.

i. Introduction 3 II. Background: Children's Television Viewing Patterns II H. I.Children's Ability to Distinguish Television Commercials from Program Material 25 2. The Influence of Format and Audio-Visual Techniques on Children's Perceptions of Commercial Messages 33 3. Source Effects and Self-Concept Appeals in Children's Television Ad- vertising 43 4. The Effects of Premium Offers in Children's Television Advertising 63 5. Violence and Unsafe Acts in Television Commercials Directed to Children 75 6. The Impact of Proprietary Medicine Advertising on Children .... 93 7. The Effects of Children's Television Food Advertising 99 8. The Effects of the Volume and Repetition of Television Commercials 109 9. The Effects of Television Advertising on Consumer Socialization .... 127 10. Television Advertising and Parent-Child Relations 133 I I.Summary of Research Findings 145 III.

12. Recommendations for Future Research 151 Appendices

A. Key Studies 155 B. Research Roster (Summary) 189 C. NAB and NAD Children's Advertising Guidelines 193

3 VIP

hor sale to, the Superintendent of Documents. t S tr,osernment Printing (Kee Washington, D.0 20402 - Price $3 75 stock No. 038-000-00336-4

4 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We wish to express our appreciation to the RANN program of the National. Science Foundation for its sup= port of the preparation of this report, and to NSF Program Officers, Allen Shinn, Charles Brownstein,and Rol- land Johnson, for their encouragement and advice. The conclusions and recommendations contained in this report are those of its authors, however, and do not represent the views of the National Science Foundation.

This project could not have been completed without the assistance of many members of the staffs of our respective institutions Harvard University, the University of Pennsylvania. the University of Hartford, the Science Institute, and the Aspen Institute. Also too numerous to acknowledge individually are those who took the time to respond to the questionnaire which led to formulation of our list oc issues, and those who supplied information for our research roster.

We are also grateful to the Children's Advertising Review Unit of the Council of Better Business Bureaus, Inc., and to its director, Emilie Griffin, for aiding in preparation of the Bibliography.

Finally, we are particularly indebted to the members of the project's Advisory Committee whose names ap- pear below. In addition to meeting with us on several occasions, they contributedindividually by reading and commenting on earlier drafts of this report. Although they are not responsible for the report's contents, our ad- visors played a significant role in its development

Advisory Committee

Seymour Banks Ithiel de Sola Pool Vice President in charge of Department of Political Science Media Research Massachusetts Institute of Leo Burnett, US A. Technology Peggy Charren Eli A Rubinstein President Medical School Health Sciences Center Action for Children's Television State University of New York at Stony Brook Walter J. Clayton, Jr. Alberta Siegel Director of Corporate Commodity Planning nepartment of Psychiatry Hershey Food Corporation Stanford University Medical Center Jam A. Dimling, Jr. Eddie Smardan Vice President and Research Director E. Smardan and Associates, Inc National Association of Broadcasters Mary Gardiner Jones Lawrence Zacharias Vice President, Consumer Affairs Division of Special Projects Western Union Federal Trade Commission

Principal Investigator Thomas S.Robertson, The Wharton School. Ugly ersity of Penns y Rama Richard P Adler, Graduate School of Education. John R. Rossuer, 1 he Wharton School, t ni et suN Harvard I ' my crsity of Pennsylvania Scott Ward, Marketing ScienceInstitute and Senior Investigaton Graduate School of Business. Harvard U no, ersit Bernard Z Friedlander, Department of Psychology. linty ersuy of Hartford Gerald S. Lesser, Graduate School of Education. Harvard University Ronald Eaber, Marketing Science Institute Laurene Meringoff, Graduate School of Education, David Pillemer, Graduate School of Education, Harvard University Harvard Universitv J EXECUTIVE SUMMARY

The effects of television advertising on children The report has four components have been a matter of concern for over a decade ( I ) identification of major policy issues of cur- That children are a special audience deserving of rent interest, special protection was formally recognized by the television industryin1961, when the National (2) review of existing research organized around Association of Broadcasters adopted its Adver- these issues, tising Guidelines The NAB subsequently expanded (3) recommendations for future policy-relevant its self-regulatory guidelines to include all catego- research; and ries of television advertising intended primarily for children Consumer advocacy groups, including Ac- (4) compilation of a national roster of tion for Children's Television and the Council on researchers on televisionadvertising and Media, Merchandising and Children, have raised children. numerous issues over the past few years related to advertising and children Partly in response to com- Two major conclusions emerge from our review. plaints from these organizations, the Federal Com- The first is a general evaluation, based on the availa- munications Commission and the Federal Trade ble evidence, of the impact of television advertising Commission have considered such issues as the on the child viewer The second relates to the role of amount of , the use of hero research in policymaking. figures or program personalities as product present- ers, and the use of premium offers in commercials First, it is clear from the available evidence th it In 1975, a second self-regulatory code for adver- television advertising does influence children. tisers was established through the Children's Adver- Research has demonstrated that children attend to tising Guidelines of the Council of Better Business and learn from commercials, and that advertising is Bureaus' N at lo na Advertising Division (NAD) at least moderately successful in creating positive at- titudes toward and the desire for products adver- Academic research on the effects of television ad- tised. The variable that emerges most clearly across vertising on children is relatively recent. However, numerous studies as a strong determinant of policymakers both inside and outside government children's perception of television advertising is the seem to be giving increasing recognition to the value child's age. Existing research clearly establishes that of empirical studies in th:s fieldThe results of children become more skilled in evaluating televi- research have already had some impact on sion advertising as they grow older, and that to treat policymaking; e g., generally, in the formulation of all children from 2 to 12 as a homogeneous group the NAD's 1975 guidelines foradvertising to masks important, perhaps crucial differences. These children, and more specifically, in the recent FTC findings suggest that both researchers and consideration of the use of premiums in children's policymakers give greater attention to the problems advertising of younger viewers, since they appeto be the most vulnerable. The report summarized here was supported by the RANT Program of the National Science Foundation From a policy standpoint, the most immediately to assess the current state of knowledge about the relevant research is that which either documents the effects of television advertising on children and to effects of specific advertising practices alleged to be recommend further research tofill gaps in that misleading or unfair to children's perceptions, or knowledge. Because of the limited amount of exist- which tests the efficacy of regulatory provisions in ing research, we have not attempted in this report to preventing such abuses. To cite a single example of retch definitive verdicts on all of the effects of the latter, several recent studies have shown that advertising on childrenThe current state of children's ability to understand and remember dis- knowledge is still inadequate in some areas, but is claimers required by industry codes (e g., "some sufficient in others to provide meaningful guidance assembly required" for a toy) is relate,' to how the to policymakers disclaimer is worded and presented Second, in terms of the ro!e of research in Other problems arose when groups or individuals policymaking, we recognize that research, no matter stated their concerns too broadly or too vaguely to how extensive and well conducted, will Inevitably be be amenable to empirical testing. Thus, Issues were but one element in determining policies and prac- often incompletely stated, alleging an effect without tices. Ethical, legal, economic, and political con- a specific cause (e.g., "children cannot understand siderationswillcontinue to demand attention commercials"), or a cause withour a specific effect Nevertheless, policies formulated in the absence or (e.g., "premium offers are unfair"). As far as possi- ignorance of research findings run the risk of being ble, we have stated Issues as connections between inappropriate, ineffective, or unfair. Thus, research specific advertising practices and specific outcomes can guide policy by providing concrete information (Including where appropriate such mediating varia- on the actual impact of television advertising on bles as age, sex, race, or level of viewing). Other children. In the long run, such research can provide issues may be legitimate but are probably matters for essentialfactual guideposts for unecting policy value judgment, not empirical testing. toward adequate safeguards against economicex- ploitation of these young viewers. Given these considerations, we derived our final list of ten issues which (a) were of greatest interest to The following sections summarize our identifica- the parties Involved; (b) were amenable to empirical tion of policy issues, review of existing research, and testing; and (c) offered some prospect of concrete recommendations for future research A national policy action based on empirical findings. The first research roster is desc-.bed in Appendix B. seven issues deal with particular advertising prac- tices which might be altered if empirical evidence IDENTIFICATION OF THE ISSUES were to suggest that alteration is advisable. These are We structured this review of existing research around specific policy Issues. This allowed us not 1. Children's ability to distinguish television only to evaluate research technically but to discuss commercials from program material. its relevance to policy concerns as well. 2.The influence of format and audio-visual techniques on ch.ldren's perceptions of com- To develop our list of issues, we surveyed major mercial messages. policy statements from government, Industry, and 3.Source effects and self-concept appeals in consumer groups. A preliminary list drawn from children's advertising. these statements was then circulated for comment to 4.The effects of advertising containing pre- advertisers, politicians, regulators, industry groups, mium offers. consumer groups, and academics, and a number of 5 The effects of violence or unsafe acts in key individuals were also personally interviewed. television commercials 6The impact on children of proprietary These responses revealed several obstacles to r...... ;:cine advertising. drawing up our final list of issues. There was no 7.The effects on children of television food ad- universal consensus on what the "real" policy issues vertising are, nor on the definition of "children's advertising." Since existing industry codes apply only to commer- cials shown during programs intended primarily for Three other Issues deal with possible longer term children, this means that they apply only to Saturday consequences of exposure to television advertising, morning television and a few other programs (e.g., or with mediators of the effects of advertising on Captain Kangaroo, ABCs Afterschool Specials) children: However, these programs account for no more than 15 percent of the weekly television viewing of 8.The effects of volume and repetition of televi- children 2-12 years old. Therefore, we included sion commercials. issues encompassing all advertising to which sub- 9.The Impact of television advertising on con- stantial numbers of children are exposed, not just to sumer socialization. commercials specifically intended for children and 10. Television advertising and parent-child rela- shown within children's programing tions.

7 ii REVIEW OF RESEARCH tion measures, however, are not a sufficient indica- tor of children's distinguishing abilities; other, more Existing research can and does document effects direct measures must be used during actual viewing on children of a variety of specificivertising prac- of televised sequences. Audio and video separation tices. Future studies will undoubtedly provide addi- devices used by the major networks have not been tional information on these effectsHowever,, tested, although ; study of a verbal announcement as whether a particular effect shuild be considered a separAtion device (used by some Post-Newsweek "positive," "negative," or "neutral" is not a question stations) found that It did not alter rung children's that can be resolved by empirical research, but is a visual attention patterns. The effectiveness of mattertobe determinedbythe judgment of various separation devicesinaiding children's policymakers(For example, research may ability to distinguish commercials from programs is demonstrate that awareness of a premium offer pre- an open question at tnis point. sented in a commercial plays a significant role in some Lbildren's product choices But whether or not 2. Format and Audio-Visual Techniques the premium itself is considered a "legitimate" pro- duct attribute is a matter of opinion.) Analysis. Many audio-visual techniques in com- meruals are simply aimed at gaining and holding At present, no single, widely accepted conceptual children's attention, policy issues arise overeffectsit" or theoretical framework exists for determining the other than attention. Visual techniques which have policy implications of empirical research results. drawn regulatory attention are those which tend to There are, to be sure, several broad principles on misrepresent the appearance of children's products which current regulations are based. The FTC has a of exaggerate product performance. Descriptions of legislative mandate to ensure that advertising (in- product characteristics (e.g., energy claims) and cluding adver Bing to children) be neither "decep- children's understanding of required disclaimers or tive" nor "unfair"; the NAD guidelines are intended accessory disclosures are also at issue. to "ensure that advertising directed to children is truthful, accurate, and fair to children's percep- Evidence. Visual or verbal misrepresentations tions." These are admirable standards, but if future and exaggerations are regarded as deceptive per se research is to be designed with clear policy rele- and their use prohibited accordingly, without any vance, these principles will have to be defined in need to document actual experienced by specific, operational terms. Even then, interpreta- children. However, little is known about children's tion of empirical results will still, ultimately, in- comprehension of product claims presentedvia volve value judgments by policymakersIn the techniques that are easily understood by adults. following summaries, we have indicated, where ap- Even older children have been shown to have propriate, which matters have been or can be ad- difficulty understanding certain types of claims: dressed by research and which cannot. notably superlatives, comparatives, and parity claims. Research on audio and video disclaimers 1. Program-Commercial Separation report the predictable but important findings that Analysis. The FCC and the NAB have recognizei video disclaimers alone are insufficient for younger, that children may have difficulty in distinguishing nonreading children, that dual audio-video claims commercials from program material. The NAB code communicate most effectively, even for older now requires broadcasters to use "an appropriate children; and that simplified wording significantly device" to separate commercials from surrounding affects comprehension. This research should lead to programs in children's program hours. tighter regulation of disclaimers, an area which is already quite well regulated. Evidence. A ntmber of studies have documented that children under eight years of age have substan- 3. Source Effects and Seit-Concept Appeals tial difficulty in comprehending the difference in purpose between commercials and programs. One Analysis. Characters as well as products appear in apparent indicator of this is that younger children's most children's commercials. Existing policy visual attention declines less when commercials ap- prohibits the use of certain characters as product pear than does that of older children Visual atten- presenters, e.g., celebrities and authority figures,

iii8 and prohibits the use of program characters as pre- Evidence. The allegation that premiums are ad ir- senters in commercials within or adjacent to their relevant product characteristic is a value judgment own programs ("hostselling"). Other presenter and not amenable to empirical testing. On the other characteristics, particularly race, sex, and occupa- hand, we can test the allegations that premiums dis- tion, may contribute to the development of socially tract children from considering legitimate product stereotyped perceptions This topic also includes attributes and that they multiply difficulty of choice self-concept appeals, which promise or imply per- between . Only the fist of these allegations sonal benefits to children from use or ownership of has been tested, and only on a limited basis Results the product. suggest that inclusion of a premium in a commercial does not seem to distract children from c pro- Evidence. A number of studies have demonstrated duct attributes (as measured by recall of the content that the mere appearance of a character with a prod- of the commercial), nor does the premium appear to uct can significantly alter children's evaluation of Increase children's evaluation of the product. These the product with the evaluation shiftli.g pos.tively or negative results should be interpreted with caution, negatively, dependiro on children's evaluation of however, as they are based primarily on a single the "endorser." This raises a potential fairness issue studs in which only one commercial was tested. The as to whether endorsement should be regarded as a issue of whether premiums increase difficulty of legitimate basis for promoting products to children. choke between brands orfacilitatechoice by differentiating' the total product package has "not The single available study on the "host selling" been investigated issue suggested that in- program placement of a com- merciaLcontaining program characters was no more 5. Violence and Unsafe Acts effective in stimulating children's desirefor the product than nonadjacent placement. Only one com- Analysis Although violence and unsafe acts ap- mercial was tested, however. The broader issues of pear infrequently in commercials directed to whether character usage in commercials contributes children, they warrant special attention because of to social stereotypes remains unresolved The poten- the potential seriousness of the effects that may be tial forstereotypingis apparent from content involved. A major issue is whether cartoon or make- analyses of sex and race representation in children's believe violence, the types most likely to be used in commercials, and secondary research (on television commercials, are harmless compared with realistic programs) has demonstrated that stereotyped beliefs violence. A largely unrecognized issue is the poten- result from heavy exposure to television program- tially interactive role of commercials during violent ing among adults.Finally, self-concept appeals programs. The main issue concerning unsafe act also have not been well researched. Although cer- portrayals is whether they are justified in certain cir- tain appeals undoubtedly imply benefits to cumstances, such as safety messages or public service children's health or social status, none a the few announcements. studies on this topic has actually incorporated self- concept measures Evidence. Children as young as four years old seem to he able to distinguish realistic from make 4. Premiums believe or cartoon violence. Despite many neutral or equivocal outcomes, and despite a tendency for Analysis. Premiums are heavily employed in com- weaker effects when the violence is animated, post- mercials for certain children's products, notably viewing aggression has sometimes been demon- cereals. The FTC has alleged that premiums are an strated with all three types of portrayals. Although Irrelevant product characteristic, that they distract the extent of antisocial behavior attributable to children from considering legitimate product at- television violence is still unclear, there seems to be tributes, and that they multiply the difficulty of sufficient risk in this type of content to support cur- choice between brands. Defenders argue that pre- rent code prohibitions on appeals to violence in miums may actually facilitate the choice between commercials directed to children Commercials, in otherwise fairly similar brands, and that premiums general, may also interact with violent programing. constitute a legitimate product attribute since they Some recent research suggests that commercial in- are part of the "total product package terruptions may heighten viewer arousal through

9 iv frustration and increase an immediate propensity children's television advertising Critics allegethat limited toward violence. However, other researchargues to the food products advertised represented a the contrary, suggesting a pacifying effectThese range of foods and that, due to theeffectiveness of habits and phenomena are-n-thy of furCner study, especially lood commercials, children's eating given that violent and "adult theme" programs are nutritional-values are being adversely biased toward watched by many children outside of code-covered the products advertised. It is furtheralleged that viewing periods Finally, evidence on theportrayal promotional characteristics such as sweetness, en- children to use of unsafe acts(e g ,incommercials promoting joyment, and premiums encourage safety) demonstrates that imitation depends not on nutritionally irrelevantcriteriain making food whether the acts are sanctioned or admonished in the choices The FTC is currently considering a rule re- commercial, but on whether chAren expect to be quiring expanded disclosure ofnutritional informa- personally rewarded or punished for the behavior tion in commercials for foodproducts This suggests thlt unsafe act portrayals may be hazardous, even in context, and should probably be EvidenceEmpirical evidence attests to the avoided whenever possible general effectiveness of .Jod avertising to children. While various statistics have been cited by parties U.S. 6. Proprietary Drug Advertising concerned with the nutritional health of the populace, including children, no evidencedirectly Analysis. Although commercials for over-the- links televised food commercials to these statistics counter (OTC) drugs are aimed at adults,children since the appropriate studieshave not yet been con- is are exposed to such advertisingwhen viewing other ducted to examine the alleged linkage. The same than- children's programing It has been alleged that true of the allegation that foodadvertising messages cumulative exposure to nonprescription, OTC drug encourage children's use of nutritionallyirrelevant advertising may promote a distorted sense of health criteria in making food choices. Thereis evidence and illness, a tendency to rely too heavily on that children are capable of learning nutritional in- proprietary medicines, and may contribute to the use formation when includedincommercials, and of illicit drugs. Defenders of OTC drug advertising preliminary research has been undertaken to argue that drug advertising is never directed tochild develop graphic devices to communicate nutritional audiences, and that drug advertising is intended to content of foods, further research isneeded to ensure comprehensible encourage the proper use of medicines. that this information is adequately to children. At a broader level is thevalue judgment Evidence. Most research on the effects of OTC of whether food advertisers should be held account- drug advertising has focused on teenagers rather able for dissemination of nutritionalinformation than children under age 12. No positive relationship beyond that intrinsic to their own products. has been found among teenagers between televised drug advertising exposure and illicit drug use, but 8. Volume and Repetition moderate positive relationships have been reported among teenagers betweer exposure toproprietary Analysis. Critics of children's advertising are con- drug advertising and reported usage of proprietary cerned about the cumulative effects of commercials medicines. Results of the limited research with on children. These concernsreduce to four main children suggest that exposure to OTC dreg adver- issues long-term effects (with age);heavy viewing dis- tising does, to a certain extent, affect children's at- effects (within age groups), clustered versus titudes toward illness and medication, with heavy tributed placement of commercials within programs; commercials. TV viewers perceiving a greater frequency ofillness, and the effects of repetition of single believing more in the efficacy of medicines and being snore receptive to their use.Further research which Evidence. The clearest findings in research on should have significant implications t >r OTC drug children and advertising are that children's unders- advertising policy is currently in progress. tanding of commercials increases with age (and thus with cumulative exposure) and that children'sliking this in- 7. Food Advertising of commercials decreases with age. Despite creased understanding of commercials andin- advertising, Analysis. Food products (including beverages and creasingly negative attitude toward snacks) represent the most prevalent category of behavioral evidence suggests, especially when age relevance of products is taken into account, that commercials (with age) has been shown to be accom- there is only a slight decline in children's stated panied by increases in the number of brands know n desire for advertised products and in actual requests in advertised product categories and by an apparent tar these items over the childhood period Heavy decrease in perceived differences between brands. television viewing (within age groups) seems neither However, since neither of these effects was associ- 10 retard nor accelerate children's understanding of ated significantly with-television viewing levels with- commercials, although it does set,m to produce more in age groups, it is impossible to conclude thaf ad- favorable attitudes toward advertising and adver- vertising alone was responsible for themUnfor- tised products Clustered formats for commercials tunately, attempts to measure consumer skills of shown during children's programs haveyetto other than awareness and differentiation have receive adequate experimental testing, but may help involvedlittleattentiontovalid andreliable younger children to distinguish commercials from measurement and have revealed mixed relation- programs by sharpening separation Clear benefits in shipsusually neutral but as often negative as posi- program-commercial separation should he docu- tivebetween television advertising exposure and mented before clustering can be justified. Finally, acquisition of these skillsAttempts to measure there is little evidence that repetition of individual children's values and attitudes toward consumer commercials leads to greater ' beyond behavior, e gmaterialism, have suffered similar children's ability to remember brand names adver- mea:,ement problems. In summary, the role of ad- tised. Neither the rate at which children seea par- vertising in consumer socials, anon has not been ade- ticular commercial (i.efrequency per program or quately documented. Better measures are required per week) nor the total number of times they en- as well as sophisticated research designs capable of counter it (beyond the first one or two exposures) isolating the contribution of television commercials seems to have any incremental effect on either their among other socialization forces. liking of the brand or their intention to requestor buy it. The effect of repetitionwmainly seems to be to 10. Tolevision Advertising and Parent-Child prevent children from forgetting their originally Relations learnedreactions tothe product. Although in- dividual commercials may be persuasive, research to Analysis. Parent-child interactions can be viewed date does not indicate that aggregated exposure to either as a mediator of television advertising to commercials in volume makes children anymore or children or as an our«nne of advertising The media- less persuasible, nor that, epetition of individual tion issue is to what extent parents influence the commercials produces other than a reminder effect. effects of advertising on their children The outcome issue is w iether advertising places strains on the 9. Advertising and Consumer Socialization parent-child relationship through parental denial of advertising-induced purchase requests. In both Analysis. Defenders of children's advertising have cases, questions me raised about patterns of parental argued that it contributes to children's general un- yielding and dAal, and the extent of conflict cre- derstanding of the economic environment and, more ated by request denials concretely, that it contributes to the development of children's product knowledge and consumer skills EvidenceParents' expressed attitudes towards Against this view is the allegation that advertising children's televisionadvertising are moderately fosters undesirable social values ;n children, with negative, but most parents do not favor abolishment materialism most often cited as an example, of children's advertising if it would mean 'discon- tinuation of children's programing There is.evi- v Evidence. There is no doubt that television com- dence that r rents tt..,d to overstate the degree of mercials play a role in initiating children's consumer control they exercise over children's TV viewing behavior at all age levels Commercials also serve as and, simultaneously, to overestimate their children's an important information source for products that understanding of commercials These findings sug- are advertised on television, ranking lower than in- gest that relatively little mediation of children's ex- store observation buthigher than interpersonal posure and reactions to commercials occurs in most sources. Whether commercials are informative in households Mediation is much more likely to occur the broader sense of consumer socializationis indirectly, when children request products they have unresolved atthis point. Long-term exposure to seen advertised Disappointment, conflict, and anger are reported when parents deny ithildren's purchase commercials directed to children "shall be requests Further research is needed to examine the clearly separated from program material by an. persistence of these etieets, and also (ro gage the ex- appropriate device In the absence of a more tent to which parents utilize these ,iecuirences for specific definition, each network has created a consumer instruLoor, purposes different separation device .These could be tested to determine if,in fact, they do ac- RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURE complish their intended purposes. Compartsohs could also be made among the various devices RESEARCH currently in use. These data would be of im- mediate relevance in refining, if necessary, the Empirical research will not resolve every issue definition of an "appropriate device pertaining to televised advertising to children Nevertheless, research documenting children's per- What is now the most effective form for dis- ceptions of and responses to advertising can con- claimers7 Self-regulatory codes now call for tribute significantly to the formulation of informed the use of certain disclaimers le g , "batteries policy. If this is to occur, several 'steps will be re- not included," "some a.sernbly required") in quired: Policymakers must formulate their concerns children's advertising. Existing evidence sug- as specifically as possible, so that they may be sub- gests that some forms of disclaimer. -resign& - jected to empirical testing, and then must give atten- candy more effective than others in com- tion to research results, when aailable, in making municating their messages to children Again, their decisions. Researchers must b- willing to un- empirical testing could suggest code refine- dertake studies which address policy concerns are ments to ensure that disclaimers fulfill their in- directly. tended purpose. In terms of future research efforts, we are recom- How can commercial food messages best com- mending that future studies move in three directions municate nutritional information to' children? more closely linked to ongoing policy concerns. We Although not specifically aimed at children's conceive of these as three "levels" of generality: ( I ) advertising, the proposed FTC rule requiring "mid-level" research to test specific hypotheses or disclosure of nutritional information would, premises on which existing or proposed regOlations affect food commercials to children. Howeve are based; (2) "macro-level" research on the role of requirements suitable for adults may not be ap- televisionand television advertisingin children's propriate for children. Experiments employing lives; and (3) "micro-level" research to document a variety of audio and visual' techniques could how children perceive actual, individual commer- help determine how to communicate required cials, We set no priorities among these three levels; nutritional information in ways most likely to all are important and necessary. Each is described be understood by children. briefly below. Too often in previous research at this level (e.g , 1. Research to address *poetic questions on premiums), single commercialshave been relevant to policy issues employed to represent an entire issue, thus- Itmiting Several current policy questions could be usefully the general izabil ity of the findings. file usefulnessof addressed using existing research methods. Each of future policy issue studies would be considerably the literature review chapters contains specific sug- enhanced if greater attention is given to selecting net gestions for further research relevant to the issues only representative samples of children. but repre- discussed. Such studies need not be elaborate, time sentative samples of commercials, consuming, nor expensive to conduct. If properly designed, they could have real impact on 'resolving 2. ResearCh on the role of television and certain policy issues. The following simply illustrate television advertising in children's lives the kinds of studies which seem most likely to yield practical guidance: This "macre" level of research is intended to determine, for the first time, the importance of What constitutes an "appropriate device" to televisionand televisionadvertisingas in- separate programs and commercials? The fluent es on children, in comparison with other ma- NAB Television Code currently requires that jor influences including parents, siblings, peers,

vl2 school, church, the community, and other media It considerable usefulness to regulatory agenciesand, is intended to help settle the prolonged controversy of course, advertisers between those who believe television is simply an in- nocuous source of lzisure time entertainment and thOse who believe the medium has become a primary APPENDICES shaping.force in children's lives. The followin - :untamed in appendices to the We are not proposing a long-term longitudinal main study, although this may be desirable Weenvision a program of research wah a relatively large and A. EVALUATION OF INDIVIDUALSTUDIES heterogeneous population of children, employinga variety of techniques and measures to determine the A group of 21 primary empiricalstudies of televi- relative importance of (and interactions between) sion advertising and childrenare reviewed in terms the sources from which children acquire informa- of their objectives, childsamples, methods, statisti- tion, which influence their values and attitudes, and cal tests, and results These evaluationsare intended shape their behavior, as a technical supplement toihe literaturereviews in the main body of the report Rather than a single project at a single institution, this research could be carried out in parallel at several locations one site might focus on preschool., B. RESEARCH RESOURCE MATER age children, a second on a mid- to late-elementary (SUMMARY) school population, a third on preadolescents. Stan- dardization of methods and instruments among the In addition to review' grexisting, research, the multiple sites would be of obvious importance, but project included a survey t ) identity individuals in- experimentation with alternative techniques should terested in and competent to conduct researchon be encouraged. When completed, the results should television advertisin.g and children An invitation to tell us, in considerable detail. the role that television respond to a questionnaire was widely circulated, and television advertising play for preschoolers, for so ne 345 responses were received. This appendix elementary school children, and for preadolescents, co. tains a statistical summary of these responses. and how television and television advertisingcom- TIfull roster of indi idual respondents, along with pare with other major forces in children's lives their affiliations, past experience, and currentin- terests, has been issued separately 3. Research on children's perceptions of specific commercials C. INDUSTRY CODES We are proposing here "micro" research which would systematically examine children's pereptions This appendix reproduces the sections of the Na- and misperceptions of individual television com- tional Association of Broadcasters Television Code mercials. White it would concentrate on commer- applicable to children and terevisein advertising, cials intended for children, it should also include and the Childrea's Advertising Guidelines ofthe adult-oriented commercials actually seen by subs- Children'S Review Unit ot the National Advertising tantial numbers of children Division, Council of Better Business Bureaus

The purposes of this research would be twofold First, it would develop methods to identify specific commercials which are confusing or misleading to BIBLIOGRAPHY children Second, it would build up a pool of data about children's perceptions of advertising, based on A comprehensive listing ot referencespertaining their responses to a range of specific commercials to television advertising and children. In additionto Ultimately, the research should lead to creation ot empirical studies, the bibliography includesimpor- standardized measures for testing commercials to tant policy statements, theoretical discussions, ensure that they are "truthful, accurate, and fair to reports of key hearings, and relevant st.t!ondary children's perceptionsSuch measures could he of references 13 VIII Part I

INTRODUCTION ANDBACKGROUND

A

14 Chapter i

INTRODUCTION

This report recommends a plan of futureresearch` explained the reasons for the Commission's concern and ad- on the effects of televisionadvertising upon about the nature of television programing and con- children. It is not an effort to reach a definitive ver- vertising directed at children: "Their ideas dict about those effects, since academic research on cepts are largely not yetcrystallized and are this issue is still relatively limited and there is not therefore open to suggestion ... and they do not yet enough information currently available to permit have, the experience and judgment always todis- such a judgment. However, this reportprovides a t,nguish between the real and fanciful." comprehensive review of existing research, sum- marizing the present state of knowledgeabout televi- Perhaps the major result of these inquiries has that sion advertising and children been a general acceptance of the principle children are a special television audiencedeserving Concern about television's impact on children is special attention and protection How thisprinciple much largely based on thefactthat most American should be applied in terms of specific issues is children spendia great deal of time watchingtelevi- less clearIncreasingly, policymakers in both looking to sion. The statistics thatdocument the medium's per- government and industry have been vasiveness are striking' The average child under12 academic research to provide empirical findings spends approximately 25 hours' per week watching about children's responses to television programing television (Nielsen, 1976), and the vast majorityof and advertising as a means for making more in- all children watch some television every day(Lyle formed decisions about policies and practices and Hoffman, 1972). The average high schoolgrad- For these reasons, our project began with an effort uate will have spent some 22 000hours in front of the set and may have been exposed to as many as to identify the issues which have beenraised in the 350,000 commercial messages (Liehert, 1976). recent past regarding the effects of televisionadver- tising on children We tried to state -these issues as Because of its massive presence in children'slives. clearly as possible, and then to review all existing television is believed to be a major vehicle fortheir research relevant to them, so that needed future acculturation to society's values Public concern has research could he proposed understandably focused on the possible negative im- pact of the medium The most prominent issuehas BACKGROUND TO THE CONTROVERSY been the continuing controversy over theeffects of The question of the effects of television advertis- televisionviolence, whichledtotheSurgeon dates at least General's study (1972) 1 ing on children has a history which from the early 1960's, when broadcastersadopted Since The 1970's have seen a marked increase inpublic guidelines for to children and governmental interest in the effects oftelevision then, the question has taken many forms, but it seems on children in other areas aswell For example, in to be based upon four fundamental concerns 1971, in response to a from the consumer group, Action for Children'sTelevision (ACT), the I That children are exposed to advertising for Federal Communications Commissionundertook a products or categories of products (such as broad inquiry into children's television In its report drLgs and heavily sugared foods) which may be on that inquiry, issued 3 yearslater, the Commission hazardous if misused, observed that "there are high public interest con- 2 That any advertising directed at children isde itexploits their siderations involved in the use of television. in Neu) "bad" he-cause relation to a large and important segment of the au- vulnerability, ad- dience, the Nations's children The FCC statement 3 That specific technique's used in television vertising may he deceptive or misleading to children, who lack the skills to evaluate them he question of violence in relation to children stele [sum advertising is considered in chapter S of this report properly,

15 4 That long-term, cumulative exposure to televi- ing a client's products or services to young con- sionadvertising may have adverse conse- sumers or totheir parents (the ust'al quences on the development of children's purchasers) The advertising agencies often values, attitudes, and behavior conduct for their clients 3 1 he broadcasters (networks, their affiliates, and The first concern, relating to the safety of adver- independent stations) which sell air time to tised products, will not be treated per sein this children's advertisers within programs that at- report, since it as a direct effort not of advertising, tract young viewers. Because, unlike other but of the product itself. However, we will consider , the airwaves are a limited public the role of advertising in relation to children's at- resource, access and control of broadcast chan- titudes toward and consumption of foods andnon- nels are restricted by Federal agency licensing. prescription drugs. 1 he second concern, that adver- tising to children is de facto "bad," is primarily an Over the years, these three groups have developed ethical issue and, as such, is not amenable to empiri- self-regulatory codesrelatingto advertisingto cal research The third concern, about specificprac- children. Broadcasters have adopted standards for tices, has been the subject of most of the existing children's advertising through the "Television research and represents the largest portiotf the Code" of the National Association of Broadcasters literature reviewed in this report The four con- (NAB).2 The guidelines pertaining to children's ad- cern, about long-term effects,is the broadest of vertising have periodically been updated and ex- issues amenable to research, since itrelates to the panded, with a newly revised set of guidelines put Air very existence of a medium which advertises directly into effect as recently as September 1976 The NAB to children. Research may eventually be able to code prohibits certain products (e.g, drugs) from documentthelong-range developmental conse- being advertised directly to children, lists certain quences of television advertising, but such research presentational techniques which may andmay not be willrequire ascale, duration, and complexity used in commercials, and sets time limits for adver- greater than that of most research undertaken to tising during children's programing Alleged code date. violations are reviewed by the NAB's Television Code Authority, which may order that an ad be The major participants in the debateotter modified or discontinued by its members. The Code televised advertising to children can be categorized Authority also acts as a mandatory preclearance unit into three groupsindustry, government, and con- for all toy commercials and those offeringa toy pre- sumer-interest organizationswhich are involved in mium. In addition, each network reviews commer- policymaking on children's advertising. The actions cials (and programs) submitted for airing, evaluat- and positions taken by these th:ee groups define toa ing them on the basis of the NAB guidelines and of large degree the context in which specific issuesare legal policies and regulations. raised and resolved, as well as the contribution to be made by empirical research These groups are Advertisers and advertising agencies have also established a system of self -regulation through the The industryThe private, corporate sideof Council of Better Business Bureaus, Inc (CBBB). broadcast advertising breaks down into three The CBBB's National Advertising Division (NAD) subgroups. has responsibility for monitoring ads and actingon complaints received from the public. In thespring of I. The advertisers who manufacture and market 1974, a Children's Adverlising Review Unitwas set products andio1 services intended, at least in up within the NAD, with a specific mandate to part, for chid consumers. These advertisers review problems concerned withadvertising to generally conduct a great deal of market child:en. In June 1975, the Children's --both in-house or by research sup- Review Unit issued a set of "Children's Advertising pliersin order to pretest their products and Guidelines" whose stated pu:poseis "to ensure that advertising strategies with potential con- advertising directed to children is truthful, accurate, sumers. Virtually all of this researchis and fair to children's perceptuins." The provisions proprietary and, therefore, unavailable for in- clusion in this project =Not all television stations a:e members of NAB As of 2 The advertising agencies which are responsible December 1975, 415 of the 711 commercialalums on the air were NAB code sunscrinersPies,: 415 did, however. include for developing creative strategies for pgomot- most of the larger stations

4 16 of the NAD guidelines are similar, though not iden- Senate) has intermittently performed the same func- tical to the NAB code (See Appendix C for relevant tion sections of both codes) The consumer-tnterest organizations.The public The Federal Government.Congressional legisla- has remained relatively uninformed and tion has delegated most of the responsibilityfor unorganized in expressing its concern regarding toe regulation of broadcast advertising to two agencies uses and abuses of television advertisingfor children. Some organizations, such as the Con- I.The Federal Communications Commission,in sumers Union, provide the public with objective the Communications Act of 1934. was given evaluations of advertised products, and several ad- the authority to regulate "consis- vocacy groups have concentrated specifically on the tent with the public interest, convenience,and issues of children's televisionadvertisingmost necessity." The FCC thereby acquired broad notably, Action for Children's Television (ACT), in discretionary Powers, circumscribed by Boston, and the Council on Children, Media and relatively few specifically articulated Merchandising (CCMM), inWashington. These guidelines and the responsiblity for developing groups pursue reform through a varitty of methods, specific standards for broadcast regulation and including testimonyatgovernment hearings, policymaking.Ithas authority not only to publications, conferences, and research sponsorship. oversee technical aspects of licensing,but also They have also atten-pled to force governmental ac- to monitor industry behavior and to perform tion by filing with the regulatory commis- quasi-judicial and quasi-legislative functions sions and the courts. e., hold hearings and make rules).Its power derives from its authority to grant, renew, or Relations between industry, government, and the deny licenses to television stations and to levy consumer-interest groups have often been conten- fines for certain violations tious. ACT and CCMM charge that industry efforts toward self-regulation are more concerned with 2The Federal Trade (Ommissionis the agency than with the actual prevention of with principal responsibility for the regulation harm. Industry spokesmen, in turn, assert that con- of interstate commerce Since 1938, the FTC sumer groups are self-appointed advocates who do has been empowered to protect the consumer not accurately reflect the concerns of most parents. interest, as well as private competition, by Industry also tends to regard much government in- means of prohibiting "false advertisements" tervention as unwarranted, while consumer advo- and preventing "unfair or deceptive acts or cates perceive the government as timid and overly practicesThe Commission has the power to concerned with industry well beings act on commercials on a case-by-case basis, as well as to :ss broad Trade Rules and Regula- THE ROLE OF RESEARCH tions which Lan restrict or require certain ad- vertising practices As indicated earlier, the amount of publicly available research on the effects of televised adver- The roles of these two Federal agencies may be tising on children is still limited. The largest body of differentiated by noting that the FCC has jurisdic- existing research, is undoubtedly that conducted by tion over theamountandschedulingof advertising. advertisers in testing the effectiveness of individual while the FTC is responsible for the content of com- commercials and advertising campaigns. This mercial messages Both commissions have demon- researchtypically consists of studies employing strated a reluctance to impose specific regulations on advertisers and broadcasters, preferring to allow in- 'hor a useful, if partisan. review of government action (and dustry to regulate itself However. the commissions inaction) in this area. see the statement by Roher Choate in ftmath act Ad vertturt. and Children.Subcommittee on Com- have used the threat of government action to prod munications, U S House of Representatives (Government Print- industry reforms, and they sometimes hold hearings ing Office. 197h), No 94-31 in order to Locus attention and stimulatedebate on 4For a comparative study of attitudes of representatives from specificissuesCongress (especially through the each of these groups and a sample of opinion) see Communications Subcommittees of the House and Charles Atkin and J Culley (1975) small samples of children to determine the com- sufficiently sophisticated to separate the effects of prehensibility and persuasiveness of commercials television advertising from other influences upon the These studies are almost always kept confidential child, including family, peers, and school. Nor is because (according to a recent article) of "an un- there adequate research tp identity the interactions willingness to share private data with competitors between these other influences and television adver- and a general view that such research is of little in- tising. Yet, it is precisely these long-term effects of terest to the research community" (Griffin, 1976). television advertising, in the full context of the Griffin also suggests that "often the scope of the child's life, that are of the greatest concern. research is so small and specialized that no great enlightenment would result from publication Finally, since we are concerned here with However, studies of children's perceptions and of research intended to be "policy relevant," we should responses to specific commercials can yield impor- also consider the limitations of research from the tant data, and one of our principal recommendations perspectives of the advertiser and the regulator. is for a program of research along these lines. When an advertiser looks to research to assess the Academic research on children's television ad- effectiveness of his advertising, well-established vertisingis much less extensive. Although the research procedures can be followed. But the public bibliography in this report cites several hundred concern about advertisingisnot. of course, to references that have some bearing on our topic, only establish that advertising can benefitsales, but a smallfraction of these are studies that deal rather that advertising can mislead, confuse, or specifically with children and with television adver- perhaps produce harmful effects Thus, when adver- tising Many of the citations pertain to more general tisers look to research to demonstrate the absence of iksues and a number of the entries refer to speeches these effects, our procedures can never be fully con- and testimony rather than empirical research In ad- vincing: Research can never prove the absence of an dition, many of the empirical studies are small-scale, effect. and there has been little replication of findings From the regulator's point of view, it is important When the research proposed here has been car- to recognize that policy decisions are seldom based ried out, a more definitive verdict about the effects solely on research evidence, no matter how well of television advertising can be reached. However, planned and conductedthatresearch may be even then, no final answers will be possible, for the Research findings are typically only one element following reasons: First, although the short-term among the several factors that determine advertising effects of individual television advertisements or of policies and practices with regard to children Ethi- mall numbers of advertisements can be readily cal, legal, economic, and political forces will in- studied, such research does not encompass what is evitably continue to operate and to influence policy most at issuethe question of whether there are any long-term, aggregate effects upon children of either Despite these caveats, we believe that the research particular types of commercials or the whole volume we have proposed can help establish an empirical of television advertising Understanding the ejects basisformorerat ionaland more effective of advertising as an institution is simply beyond cur- policymaking. Indeed, the iesults of research have rent research protedures. Although we have recom- already had some impact on policymaking mended that research be designed to study advertis- generally, in the formulation of the NAD's 1975 ing at this level, we expect that current ignorance guidelines for advertising to children, and more about the en masse effects of televised advertising specifically, in the FTC's recent consideration of the will continue for some years to come. use of premium offers in children's advertising.

In regard to any long-term effects of children's ex- One reason for guarded optimism about the posure to particular categories of television advertis- future is that nearly all the parties interested in ing or to specific features of commercials, the neces- television advertising and children agree about the sary research methods may be more within reach. need for further research. In a speech announcing his Several longitudinal studies have been designed to Interestin these issues, Lewis A. Engman, then measure the various aspects of child development chairman of the FTC, acknowledged that "there is However, longitudinal research has not yet been really no definitive pool of informationon the

6 18

mucom111110111W specific impact of television advertising on children. a charge that a practice is "bad" without specific in- This remains a field which should be subject to more dications of the alleged consequences of the practice. extensive research " More recently, Emilie Griffin (1976), director of the Children's Advertising These problems indicate the need for a more pre- Review Unit of the NAD stated that "research is cise specification Of issues, as well as for a more ade- needed because itwill serve to make guidelines quate theoretical and empirical base for the in- codes, and government actions fair to children and vestigation of these issues. Thus, we followed three advertisers alike." distinct steps in our effort to define the issues more precisely and objectively: ( I ) we conducted an initial DEFINING THE ISSUES literature review; (2) we then surveyed interested parties in the controversy for their views of the We began this report by attempting to identify a issues, and only then did we (3) develop a frame- set of Issues that would serve as a framework for the work of issues that seemed to us most meaningful review of existing research. Several problems made and investigable. this selection process difficult. One fundamental difficulty is that the various issues raised in the The initial literature review was undertaken in debate are often rooted in personal values. Some cri- order to help develop a list of issues of concern to all tics feel that any advertising directed at children is Interested parties, and to identify a group of "key" unfair and unacceptable, while some defenders of studiesi.e., those studies which seemed most likely the system feel that any regulation of advertising to yield an understanding of some aspects of televi- practices is an abridgement of rights in our basically sion advertising's effects on children. Four docu- free-enterprise economy. Given such extreme points ments were particularly useful in helping to identify of view, objective discussion of any specific Issues is the issues, since they were statements of the major difficult indeed. Beyond a very few oasis points of areas of concern from the perspectives of industry agreementfor example,allwould agreethat (the NAB Television Code and the NAD guidelines), of government (a speech by past FTC children should not be harmed or exploitedcritics anu defenders of advertising share very little com- chairman, Lewis Engman), and of consumer-interest mon ground in the controversy. One man's "ex- groups (a series of children's advertising guidelines proposed by a group of consumer advisers to the ploitation of children" isanother's "consumer FTC). socialization In our second step, we surveyed advertisers and Another problem is that the issues which have advertising agencies, politicans, regulators, industry been advanced by various individuals or groups in- associations, consumer groups, and academics. All volved in tbe debate have frequently been stated in- were asked to evaluate our preliminary list of issues. completely or too broadly, alleging an effect without Forty-five responses were received out of the ap- a specific cause, or a cause without a specificeffect. proximately 125 questionnaires mailed. Few en- For example, the charges that advertising leads to tirely new issues were suggested in these responses; "materialistic values," or that " in rather, most respondents urged us tc modify the children's advertisingismisleading," would be wording of the issues we posed, or to broaden or nar- difficult to test empirically or to remedy with a row them. Some advertisers, for exanyle, urged us realistic policy for corrective action. The charge that to consider more of the "positive" results of adver- animation is misleading neither specifie: what tising on young people; some urged us to review the aspects of animation are misleading, nor how the constitutionality of regulations of advertising prac- children are misled. Similarly, it would be difficult tices affecting children. On the other hand, some to recommend action on the charge of materialistic consumerists urged us to focus -ri the nutritional values without a realistic and objective understand- characteristics of advertised food products prier to ing of this concept, a knowledge that advertising in the question of the effects of advertising itself. Many fact fosters the development of such atttitudes, and a of the responses from academics suggested topics for widespread agreement that such attitudes are detri- research which also raised policy issues. mental to children and/or to society One cannot realistically propose a corrective device without un- Following these preliminary activites, we were derstanding causal factors, and one cannot evaluate prepared to synthesize the results and specify the

1O framework of issues which would become the focus advertising; the final three Issues are concerned with for the project. We agreed on five points which were the cumulative exposure of children to television ad- useful in this synthesis vertising and with some potential mediating factors on the effects of this exposure- I We would distinguish between issues arising from advertising in general and issues related I. Children's abilityto distinguish television to the specific products advertised. We decided commercials from program materials. not to make judgments about the quality of ad- vertised products, since these questions were 2. The Influence of forma and audio-visual beyond the scope of the project and outside the techniques on children's perceptions of com- specific expertise of the project staff. mercial messages 3. Source effects and self-concept appeals in 2. The issues to be considered would encompass children's television advertising. all advertising to which substantial numbers of children are exposed, not just commercials 4 The effects of premium offers in children's specifically Intended for children and broad- television advertising. cast within the context of children's program- 5. Violence and unsafe acts in television com- ing. mercials directed to children. 3. While many of the issues pertain to alleged 6. The impact on children of proprietary negative effects, we would attempt, where medicine advertising possible, to identify Issues pertaining to posi- 7. The effects on children of television food ad- tive or beneficial effects of television adveiti3- vertising. ing. 8. The effects of volume and repetition of televi- 4. Although many of the alleged negativeprac- sion commercials. ticesin advertising occur relativelyinfre- 9 The Impact of television advertising on con- quently (e.g., portrayal of unsafe activities, sumer socialization urging parents to buy), we decided that since they d6 occur or have occurred, they should he 10. Television advertising and parent-child rela- included in the analysis. tions.

5. Because we believed that the issues should he THE ORGANIZATION OF THIS REPORT practical, we decided that the final set of Issues would be most productively stated mainly in Reviews of existing research relevant to each of terms of advertising characteristics or practices these Issues follow in chapters I through 10. In each which on the basis of research results could be of these chapters, we briefly Introduce the issue and corrected or modified. Thus, our main cri- then review the current and proposed regulations terion in selecting these issues was utility-. is regarding that issue. The intent here is to determine some action suggested. by the empirical to what extent existing regulations and industry knowledge :elating to the Issue? Does empiri- codes address the Issue. A third section in most of cal information suggest some change or some the chapters assesses the incidence of the advertising continuation in a p-actice? For example, one practices or characteristics in question. Here we rely cannot readily alter the television viewing pat- on data from available content analyses. The body terns of children from different socioeconomic of each chapter reviews and evaluates the empirical family backgrounds, although such differences research evidence relating to the practice or charac- may exist and may affect their responses to ad- teristic. Finally, each chapter summarizes the evi- vertising. One can, howpver, modify, elimi- dence and offers recommendations for future research nate, or leave unchanged certain advertising pertinent to that issue. A preliminary section to these practices, such as the use of "host- selling," on 10 chaptersBackground. Children's Television the basis of empirical evrdence of this prac- Viewing Patternspresents the available data on tice's effects on children how much television children watch, when they watch, and what they watch. Given these considerations, we derived the following list of ten issues The first seven deal with Chapter 11 reviews the evidence of the previous particular practices or characteristics of television chapters in terms of what it tells us about the links

8 20 between television advertising, its possible effects on Appendix B provides a statistical summary of the children, and the mediating variables of long-term results of a survey we conducted of researchers exposure, consumer socialization, and parent-child and research facilities which could be called upon relations The final chapter presents a more general to conduct future research projects Through a view of our suggestions for future research. Here, we widely circulated questionnaire, we sought to have not attempted to spell out research projects in identify researchers in academic and other in- detail, but rather to indicate the major directions we stitutions with (1) a professed Interest in assessing believe should be pursued in order to resolve the advertising's Impact on children, (2) a demon- most Important unanswered questions about the strated research competence, and (3)ar.ex- effects of television advertising on children. Some of pressedInterestin conducting policy-relevant our suggestions closely follow existinglinesof research. Some 345 responses were received A research; others propose entirely new directions full listing of individual respondentsalong with Beiuse the need for additional research is great, we their affiliations, past expel tence and current in- hope readers will give careful attention to this terestshas been issued separately chapter Appendix C contains the NAD's "Children's Televi- In addition to a bibliography, the back matter of sion Guidelines" and the NAB's "Children's Adver- this report includes three appendices. tising Guidelines

Appendix A consists of detailed, comparative The bibliography includes the research studies evaluations of 21 key studies specifically con- cited in the text, as well as a listing of important cell.I with the effects of television advertising on policy statements, theoretical articles and hooks, children. These studies are reviewed in terms of reports of key hearings, and relevant secondary their objectives, child samples, methods, statisti- references cal tests, and results. This appendix is intended as a technical supplement to the literature reviews in the main body of the report

(1 / 9 4-1, Chapter ii

BACKGROUND: CHILDREN'S TELEVISION VIEWING PATTERNS

Knowledge of children's television viewing pat- yielded estimates which varied by as much as 20 per- terns is relevant in several ways to a consideration of cent An additional problem with most scientific the etfects of television advertising on children. For studies of children's television viewing is that they example, data on children's total viewing time can are usually based on small samples taken over be used to estimate their total exposure to television limited periods of time and are therefore difficult to advertising Information on the times that children generalize watch television and the programs they favor is usetulin determining the kinds of commercials Third, most of the studies of children's viewing childrenarelikelytosee.Inthis background patterns lack a precise definition of "television view- chapter, we will review the available evidence on the ing." The term can be used to encompass a broad following specitic questions variety of behaviors, ranging from rapt involvement to mere presence in a room in which a television set At what age do chiidren begin watching televi- is operating. The latter extreme probably should be sion? (but is not always) excluded from the definition of How much television do children watch'' television viewing; but even within the "normal" How much television advertising are children range of viewing, research studies have established exposed to') that quite different levels of involvement can be in- When do children watch television? cluded. For example, both surveys and observations What kinds of programs do they prefer? have demonstrated that children's viewing is often What isthe relationship between children's accompanied by other activities, such as talking, eat- viewing patterns and the industry codes ing, and playing (Lyle and Hoffman, 1972a; Mur- governing advertising to children') ray, 1972); and laboratory studies have shown con- siderable variations in children's visual attention to Three important points should be noted at the the screen, even when watching television is their outset First, "children" in the context of this report only activity (Ward, 1972; Wartella and Ettema, are conventionally defined as youngsters between 1974). the ages of 2 and 12. Within such 'a wide age range, there are great differences, of course. The viewing In light of these limitations, the statistics cited in habits and preferences of a 3- or 4-year old are this chapter must be regarded as gross measures of different from those of a 6-year old, and vastly children's viewing time and not as precise records of different from those of a 10- or 11-year old Unfor- moment-to-moment attention by the children tunately, these differences are frequently ignored in studied Much of the data in this chapter comes from discussions of "children's television." Whenever the A. C. Nielsen Company, which compiles the best pcssible, we will take account of these age known and most detailed statistics on national differences in the following discussion. television audiences.' There are both important ad- vantages and disadvantages to these Nielsen data. Second, these is no single, wholly satisfactory Some of the advantages are: (1) The data are based source of data on children's television viewing pat- on a relatively large and representative national terns. There are many different ways to collect such sample (approximately 3,600 families), (2) The data, and each is likely to produce different results. For example, Schramm, Lyle, and Parker (1961) compared four separate measures of the amount of weekend television viewing bya group of 24 !Both A C' Nielsen and a second company, Arbitron, also children: ( 1 ) a child's general estimate; (2) an un- provide market-by-market data on the viewing audiences for supervised diary kept by each child; (3) a surrep- local stations These statistics are important for determining the audiences for nonnetwork syndicated programing and fur ex- titious measurement by older siblings; and (4) an in- aminingpossibleregionalvariationsinviewingpatterns terview in which the investigators aided the child's However. in this chapter, we are concerned with children's view- recall of programs watched These four methods ing patterns on the national level

212 Nielsen Company has been measuring television au- Although more recent data are not available, it diences since 1950 and therefore provides a valuable seems likely that with the increased ownership of historicalpicture of viewing patterns,(3) The television sets since 1961,2 and with the advent ot Nielsen Company analyzes the television audience piograming Intended specificall'y for preschoolers on the basis ot age and therefore provides viewing le.g.. Sesame Street), more children are becoming data specifically on children, as well as on teenagers regular viewers at a very early age Even on the basis and adults of these dated figures, we can conclude that a ma- pray ot children are watching television regularly The disadvantage ot the Nielsen data is that the before age 4 company's purposes are not scientific, therefore (1) Most of the data are proprietary and not publicly available, (2) The collection and nresentation of the HOW MUCH TELEVISION DO CHILDREN data are primarily shaped by the needs of Nielsen's principal clientsbroadcasters and advertisers WATCH? not 'by the priorities of scientific researrh. (3) The Children's viewing patterns vary considerably company subdivides its child audience into two categories onlychildren 2-5 years ot age and from individual to individual, as well as trom day to day for the same individualrs1,:vertheless, some children 6-11 Thus, differences in responses within gene- patterns can he discernedAccording to these age brackets cannot he determined trom the Nielsen data from November 1975. children 2-5 Nielsen dataBecause of these disadvantages. %A e years watched television an average of 26 hours and have supplemented theNielsen data, whenever 31 minutes per week, or more than 3' 4 hours per possible, with figures from other sources day Older children. ages 6-11. watched an average ot 25 hours and 49 minutes each week, or more than AT WHAT AGE DO CHILDREN BEGIN 31 2 hours per dayBy comparison, the average WATCHING TELEVISION? weekly viewing time for all persons was 26 hours and 59 minutes per week Teenagers and working Little attention has been given to when and how women and men under 55 watched less time than very young children begin watching television The children, nonworking women over 18 and men over only study of the beginnings of television use was 55 watched more than children conducted in 1961 by Schramm. Lyle. and Parker They tound that 14 percent ot the children studied made "regular use of the medium" by age 2 Over The data Indicate that during the months hen one-third did so by age 3, over two-thirds by age 4, school is in session. children under 6 tend to he and over 90 percent by age 6 (See table 11-11 heavier viewers than children 6 years and older Fhey also show that children's viewing hours vary somew hat from year to year (see table ii-2) Overall. Lyle and Hottman (1972a) have estimated that Table ii-1 children's average daily viewing time has increased approximately an hour per day over the past 20 Percentage of Children Using Television at years Ot course, these averages do not retlect in- Different Ages div idual variations in viewing patterns within an age group For example, among the 6th graders inter- Percent viewed by Lyle and Holtman. 25 percent reported Age using television watching 51 : hours ot television on a given day. 2 14 while another 25 percent watched no television at 3 37 all 4 65 5 82

6 91 {de% is1(11 .et nAncfship 11I S hitett,,Ids in,reased tr,n1 7 94 90 percent al the end ,d 19n1 t, 9' per,clit t.,\ the end t 19"S 8 95 i ken more titanhttik has hen tht inoc,,,,, In th.nik.,,ntatittng 9 96 Iv,. ,.t morn sets in:111 13 ocrLcnt .it the end .t 14,1 I. 41 per. LOH h the end "t 19"S I Nick,. n19-9, I Table ii-2

Children's Average Daily Viewing Time (1966-75)

2-5 year olds6-11 year olds

November 1966 3.14 2:59 November December 1968* 3 28 4:01 November 1970 3.40 3.11 November 1972 3.54 3:39 November 1974 3.45 3:26 November 1975 3:47 3.41

1968 data came from a 6-week measurement period (Nov 1 Dec 12), 4-week periods were used in the other years

A number of studies have attempted to determine HOW MUCH TELEVISION ADVERTISING ARE the factors that influence individual differences in CHILDREN EXPOSED TO? the amount of television viewed by children. A rela- tionship has been found, for instance, between high- Some of the issues we deal with in this report con- er viewing times and lower parental socioeconomic cern the long-term cumulative effects of television status (Schramm. Lyle, and Parker, 1961, McIntyre advertising on children. Thus, we have tried to and Teevan, 1972, McLeod, Atkin, and Chaffee, derive a rough estimate of the average child's yearly 1972) Viewing differences have also been corre- exposure to television advertising Our computa- lated with intelligence, ethnic background, and level tions, explained below, suggest that children on the ofsocialadjustment However, most otthese average are exposed to some 20,000 commercial differences are not dramatic and have been Sound messages each year, or slightly more than 3 hours of primarily among older children and adolescents television advertising each weekOf course, light Moreover, as television has become more universal, viewers (or heavy public television viewers) would research has indicated substantial reductions of see tewer commercials, and heavy viewers might see these differences (Roberts, 1976) considerably more

Allowing for individual differences, Ae can safely These figures were reached by multiplying 365 conclude that the average child over the past decade days per year times the average number of viewing has watched 3 to 4 hours of television per day hours and the approximate number of commercials Schramm, Lyle, and Parker's description in 1%1 of per hour The figure for the number of viewing hours the pervasive role of the medium in children's lives per day was based on an average of Nielsen estimates is still appropriate today for 1974 and 1975 (see table ii-2). Since Nielsen pro- vides separate data for 2-5 year olds and 6-11year olds, we computed estimates for each of theseage Throughout the preschool years,tele% ision groups, and we accounted for noncommercial public time far exceeds other media time, in tact. it television viewir g time by reducing these estimates usually exceeds the total ot all other media by 10 percent (probably a high figure. except for time . Two-thirds ofallchildrenare preschool devotees of SO anie Street) already television viewers before they have much experience with movies Even at the end of 10 years, when they are making some use ot 'these estimate% arelose to thrse made by others For ex all media, television is the only one they are ample Rohert Choate, in testimons netore the House Subcom using day after day At age 10, three-fourths of mutee an ( ommunicattons (1, +75 t stated that 'the average child all children, as we discovered, will b: likely to sees user 22.000veommercials each sear'I ev.is Enginar ex- thairman ot theI- he watching television on any given day. This is e,airnated that the ,INerage high school graduate will he seen kst),00(1 ,.ommer(ial messages 971S more than twice the percentage for any other !bat figure dts !Lied hs lh sears gives an as erage ot 21 87s tom medium at that age inertia's per sear l4 Reaching a fairly accurate estimate of the number monitored in his studies were 30 seconds in length, of commercial messages per hour was more we accepted his figure as an adequate estimate 6 Sec- problematic, since regulations governing the max- ond, not all nonprogram materialis advertising. imum amount of "nonprogram material" per hour This category also includes public service announce- differ for different times of the day and also change ments, promotional commercials for other television periodically. At present, the NAB code states that programs, and program credits in excess of 30 sec- nonprogram material may occupy no more than 91%2 onds(Code News, June 1974) Due to these varia- 'minutes per hour during prime time and weekend ble*, we chose to use conservative estimates We children's programing time,' no more than 12 have used 91/2 minutes per hour of nonprogram minutes per hour during weekday children's material as a base. At 30 seconds per commercial. programing, and no more than 16 minutes per hour this would result in 19 nonprogram messages per at all other tines s hour We then assumed that 10 percent of these 19 There were two other problems in determining messages, or two per hour, were not commercials, the number of commercials per hour. First, the leaving an average of 17 commercials per hour length of individual commercials varies, ranging from15 to 60 secondsIihwever, since Barcus Applying these figures to our formula, we arrived (1915a) reports that 98 percent of commercials at the following

Avg hrs/day Minus Avg hrs/day Avg no. Avg No. of TV viewing 10% PTV comm'l TV comm'Is/ comm'Is Age: ('74-75) viewing viewing hour Days viewed/yr

2-5 3 7 0.4 = 3.3 17 365 20,476 6-11 3 5 0.4 = 3 1 17 365 = 19,236

WHEN DO CHILDREN WATCH TELEVISION? viewing (36 percent) during prmie time, followed by the later afternoon and early evening hours (30 per- The kinds of commercials children see depend cent). Thus, for all children under 12, the hours upon the hours when they watch television Weekend from 4 30 p m. to1 100 p m , Monday,-SundAy mornings and especially Saturday mornings are count for more than half their average weekly view- generally thought of as "children's television hours" ing and do represent heavy viewing periods for children However, according to 1975 Nielsen data (see figure A second way of examining this information is to 11-1), the weekend daytime hours from 7 00 a m to look at the average number of children in the televi- 4:30 p m account for only 16 percent of the total sion audrence over the course of a day These data, weekly television viewing time for children 2 to 11 based on the 1974 Nielsen measurements (the most years old. For children under 6, the greatest amount recentyearfor which detailedhour-by -hour of viewing, 30 percent, occurs on weekday mornings statistics were available) are presented in figure 11-2 and afternoons (7 00 a m -4 30 p m )Late after- Figures ii-2a and b represent estimates of the na- noon and early evening hours (4 30-7 30 p m.) ac- tional child audience on Saturdays and Sundays, count for 27 percent of the total, and early evening from 7'00 a m to 6 00 p m Figure ii-2c represents and prime-time hours (7 30-11 00 p m ) account for the child audience on weekdays until 6 00 p m as 24 percent Children 6 to 11 years do most of their well as the number of children viewing tele. ision Mondays through Sundays during the evening hours, from 6 00 p m to% 30 p m Since e ening viewing ,Children's programing time is defined as "those hours other slightly from weekday'. to than prime timeinwhich programs initiallydesignedtor patternsdiffer only children under 12 sears 01 age arc scheduled' (NAB (ode June 1974) 'According to Barcus (1476), monitoring of weekend and 'Harcus studies V.I. IC lffilt1110.1 hid di ,111111CI,IIIN weekday afternoon children's programing indicated that the ac during children's programingffinAei.eiIt/ si,iinds this also, tual time devoted to nonprogram Material exceeded these limits hunme the standard length for niist tent,. 1.10,1111110 on some stations Gals

ri FIGURE 11-1

WEEKLY IEWIN, ACTIVITY FOR 01:1003,0:: Nowmber, 1:375

1MON -SU'N 3:10N-FRI 4SAT-S1'N RS :1-1:3,1--.

7:30-11P 4:30 -7: 301"1 7A01- 4:3')1"117A:1-4 : 30P.1 1 I OP ',.;ELF

DISTRIBUTION 1

OF HOURS IN 15 13# 33I=t 168:110 WEEK f

TOTAL 3 4 5 23'I hi ,' 10 11 : PERSONS

CHILDREN 5 I 36t 30*, 2r):49 6-11 14h 16'- P 13'.----1( If CHILDREN ;4 = 27-- h, 3, _6:11 2-5 I i

Sc)i 'hCR : :41 e ( ) weekends, they are combined here to present the au- shape. the weekday morning audience of older dience over the course of the entire week (Note that children is mu:'.! mailer because of school attend- these graphs represent averages at given times, not ance. By contrast, the portion of older children in cumulative audiences ) the audience during prime-time hours is significantly In general, the number of children watching greater (peaking at slightly more than 50 percent of all 6-11 year olds at 8:00 p.m.). Similar breakdowns television increases rapidly during the course of the were not available for weekend daytime viewing, but early morning hours (especially on Saturday), then smaller age-related differences would be expected. decreases during the late morning and early after- since school attendance or bedtime patterns are not noon. The number begins to increase again in mid- factors at these times. afternoon untilbout 5:30 p.m., when itfalls off temporarily (due, probably, to children's low in- WHAT KINDS OF TELEVISION PROGRAMS DO terest in news). The child audience then continues to rise to a peak at about 8:00 p.m. (see figure ii-2c) CHILDREN FAVOR? The size of the child audience falls off rapidly The kinds of commercials children see are deter- thereafter, although nearly 25 percent of all children mined nisi only by the hots when they watch televi- are still watching television at 10:00 p.m Even at sion, but also by the specific programs they watch. 11:00 p.m., approximately 10 percent of all 6-11 As with other aspects of children's television view- year olds are still counted in the television audience ing, their extremely diverse program preferences are Some age differencesin viewing patterns are related to age. Schramm, Lyle, and Parker ,(1961) reported that the favorite programs of preschoolers noted in Figure11-3,which presents the viewing pat- terns for children 2-5 and 6-1 Iyears.' While the are predominantly those designed specifically for graphs in the two figures are roughly similar in children: "the programs have animals, animated characters, or puppets as their chief charactersare

'These figures are in terms of both absolute numbers and of all in story form, are full .of action (often slapstick), percentages of children in each age bracket Since there are ap- and often have a heavy component of laughter." By proximately 9 million more 6.11 year olds than 2-5 year olds. the time children are midway through elementary the percentages provide more direct comparisons school, their preferences have broadened to include

1526 1'1, ,01.21, Li -2

A`'nliNCr

hildrn '1e1.-v 30n, By Hoit

'am 11) 11 12n 1 pm 2 3 4 5 h

am

11/= -am 11 12n 1pm 4

t t 1' 1 , .101-1 t1

i I- ltr 11 !21-1 1pm -1 4 r h 7 H 9 10 11 12mid *01--.,11n

1111, 0,u)

1 t v.it zn.an(1 i7v,,

trty-,

16 FIJURE 11-3

CHILD VIEWING AUDIENCE BY At,r,

7-00 am to 6:00 pm, Mon -'r r1

,J JO pm to 12:00 mid, lion -Sun

Children h-11

)

10,000

5,100

7am 8 9 10 11 12n 1pm 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12mid

.4"r -1 r 2 -,-.0.110011. Tot,I numb. r of children h-II 2:,7H",0'0

Children 2-5

10,000

5,000

7am 8 9 10 11 12n 1pm 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 31 12mid

r -h11,-ir,r 2-5 11,690,w)

S,-)URrE. I373 Nielr;(n data, from Katzman (1-J7)

17 28 r a

child-oriented adventure and variety programs, percentage of each group that named a particular situation comedies, and westerns. By the time show: children leave elementary school, their preferences encompass most categories of programing watched by adults. Table 11-4 Favorite Programs of School-Age Children These findings were confirmed in a study by Lyle and Hoffman (1972b), who reported on the favorite % of 6-7 % of 8-10 Typo of program television programs of preschool children (3-,.4-, year oldsyear olds and 5 -year olds) (see table 11-3) Situation comedies 51* 58 Table 11-3 Cartoons 45 40 Other children's shows 33 31 Television Program Preferences of Preschool Adventure shows 17 33 Children Game shows 16 25 Children's educational Program or type of program % of 3-, 4-, shows 19 7 5-year olds Science fiction shows 10 9 Westerns 10 10 Flintstones 26 Movies 5 15 Sesame Street (noncommercial) 16 General cartoons 12 *Percentages-add up to more than 100 percent because of fre- Violent cartoons -11 quent multiple responses Mickey-Mouse-type cartoons 5 Situation comedies 5 We can see that by age 6 Dr 7, children are watch- Family situation comedies 4 ing a broad variety of programs. This is confirmed Even within this 3-year age span, some striking tl Nielsen estimates (1976) of tin number of child changes were revealed by the study. For example, 30 viewers for various kinds of prime-time programing. percent of 3 year olds reported Sesame Street as their Nielsen divided prime-time programing into five favorite program, whereas only 12 percent of the 5- categories, with the estimated average child_ au- year olds expressed this preference. Conversely, dience for each type of program. The figures for the situation comedies were not considered favorites by period of October-December 1975: situation com- any 3-year olds, but 12 percent of the 5-year olds edy, 5.22 million; general drama, 3.68 million; chose the situation comedies. variety, 3.38 million; feature film, 2.83 million;.and suspense and mystery drama, 2.46 million. Although Evidence on the viewing preferences for older these are large numbers, the most popular programs children comes from a study in which a national among these genres draw substantially greater num- probability sample of 6-7 and 8-10 year olds were bers of children. For example, table ii-5 lists the asked to name the programs they had watched most child audience estimates by Nielsen for the 15 shows recently (Gene Reilly, 1973c). Table ii-4 shows the most watched by children in 1973:

2 9 Table 11-5

Most Popular Programs Among Children (1973)

Average number % of iledlefICO of child viewers % of all that are (millions) children children

1.Brady Bunch 12.08 33.1 40.2 2. Partridge Family 11.73 32.2 34.3 3. Wonderful World of Disney 11.02 30.2 27.5 4."in the Family 8.89 24.4 14.7 5.Emergency 8.99 24.4 23.7 6.NewScooby Doo Movie 8.89 24.4 70.2 7. The Waltons 8.89 24.4 23.7 8.Adam 12 7.47 20.5 20.7 9.Flintstones Comedy Hour 7.1'1 19.5 60.5 10.Josie and the Pussycats 7.11 19.5 68.4 11. Sonny and Cher 7.11 19.5 19.6 12.Sanford and Son 6.75 18.5 14.0 13.Room222 6.4 17.5 23.3 14.Bridget Loves Bernie 6.04 16.8 17.4 15. Mary Tyler Moore 6.04 16.6 15.5

It is Interesting to note that only three of these shows Advertising of products designed primarily for (The New Scooby Doo Movie, Flintstones Comedy children, or to auvertising which is telecast Hour,andJosie and the Pussycats)were seenon during programs designed primarily for weekend mornings. The remainder were all prime- children or within station breaks between such time tvening programs.8 consecutive programs, designed primarily for children. INDUSTRY CODES AND CHILDREN'S VIEWING PATTERNS In another sect:on of the NAB code, dealing with time standards for children's advertising, Other chapters of this report will consider specific "Children's Programing Time" is defined as "those advertising practices by the NAB and NAD codes. hours, other than prime time, in which programs ini- Here, we will be concernced only with the relation- tially designed primarily for children under 12 years ship between the preceding evidence about of age arc scheduled." children's viewing patterns and how these codes define "children's television." In an earlier version of the code (October 1973), "Children's Programing Time" was defined more According to the NAB "Children's Television simply to apply during "that continuous period of Advertising Guidelines" (effective September1, time between the hours of 7.00 a.m. and 2:00 p.m. on 1976), the guidelines are meant to apply to: Saturday and Sunday." Thus, in practice, the revised

R A more recent but less detailed listing of most popular 8 Swiss Family Robinson. programs shows that, although specific programs have changed. 9 Good Times. the general pattern has remained constant As of the fall 1975, 10 Scooby Doo, Where Are You', the IS most popular shows among children under 12 were II The Waltons, 12 Land of the Lott, 1 Six M Ilion Dollar Man, I 3 Sanford and Son, 2 Happy Days, 14 Rhoda. 3 Emergency. I5 When Things Were Rotten 4 Wonderful World Disney 5 Welcome Back, Kotler, As in 1973, just three of the top 15 programs appeared on Satur- 6*brae House on the Prairie, day morning (#7, #10, #12), the remainder were prime -tune 7 Shazam,Isis Hour, programs

'9 3 0 language of the 1976 code extends the applicability n- 'Ilion viewers (58 5 percent children). whereas all of the guidelines very little. They now apply only to network programs shown from 7 30-9 00 p m at- programs -shown on weekend mornings and to a tracted an average of 5 43 million children as part of limited number of other programs oriented a total audience of 26.28 million viewers (20.7 per- specifically to children (such as Captain Kangaroo cent children) Thus, audiences for early evening and occasional afternoon children's "specials") The prime-time programs contained, on average, nearly guidelines do not apply to most afternoon program- I5 million more children than the audiences for ing nor to prime timewhich are periods of substan- programs intended specifically for children. In light tial child viewing, as we have seen. In fact, children of these figures, it appears that "children's program- on average do only about 15 percent of their view ing ing"is being defined partly by a program's at times when advertising is regulated by guidelines popularity among children, but also partly by its un-

intended to protect children Only 3 out of the 15 popularity among adults .. most popular programs among children are broad- cast during this so-called "Children's Programing Critics have charged that as a result of these Time policies, children iire exposer' to a large amount of advertising not intended for them and not covered At first glance, the NAD guidelines seem to be by regulations ostensibly intended to protect them. broader in application Thcy are based not only on a This includes advertising for potentially hazardous program's intended audience, but on the actual com- productssuch as nonprescription drugs, alcoholic position of that audience They are meant to app141 beverages, power tools, household cleaners, and both to "children's programs", (i.e., programs in- other chemical productsproducts for which adver- tended primarily forchildren) and to those tisingis not permitted during children's viewing "programs in which audience patterns typically con- hours. The critics assert that if such advertising is tain more than 50 percent children designed to mot:., ate adults to use them, it may also motivate children to use them In practice, however, these guidelines rarely ap- Two counterarguments are offeredFirst, that ply outside of weekend mornings. Population children are interested only in products appropriate statistics indicate that children between the ages of 2 to their ages (toys, snack foods, etc.) and disregard and II comprise approximately one-fifth of the U S. commercial messages thatare obviously adult- population. (As of September 1975, Nielsen esti- oriented Second, that when children are a minority mated that there were 34.81 million children, out of of the total audience (e g., during prime time), they a total viewing audience of 200 17 million persons, are most frequently watching television in the com- so that children ages 2 through 11 represented 17.4 pany of adults, who can correct any potential misun- percent of the total viewing audience ) Thus, there derstandings of adult-oriented advertising are far fewer potential child viewers than nonchild viewers. For a program to attain an audience of 50 percent children. more than four times as many po- SUMMARY: NEEDED RESEARCH ON tential child viewers must watch the program for ev- CHILDREN'S VIEWING PATTERNS ery potential adult viewer , Itis clear that watching television is a nearly This means that the application of the NAD universal experience for children growing up in this guidelines is determined at least as much by adult country. Most children begin watching television at viewing patterns as by children's. As table ii-5 indi- an early age. After age 2, the majority of children cates, only throe of the 15 most popular shows watch some television every day and average be- among children in 1973 (New Scooby Doo Movie, tween 3 and 4 viewing hours per day through age 11. Josie and the Pussycats, Flintstones) had audiences This volume of television viewing means that a child that were more than 50 percent children. For the will see some 20,000 commercial messages each three most popular showsBrady Bunch. Partridge year, or approximately 3 hours of television adver- Family, World of Disneychildren comprised 40.2, tising each week. 34.3, and 27.5 percent of the audience respectively. Moreover, according to 1975 Nielsen data, the We have seen that some children are likely to be average network children's program attracted an au- watching television at any time of the day or night, dience of 3.94 million children, out of a total 6.73 but that their viewing is heaviest during prime-time

310t evening hours and late afternoons (more than half of Most of the research (and virtually all of the , viewing by childrenages 2-11). The weekend laboratory studies) conducted to date on the effects daytime "children's hours" represent less than one - of television advertising on children have been con- tifth of children's total weekly viewing. We have also cerned with child-oriented commercials. Research is seepthatvery young childrentendtoprefer needed that will examine such questions as Do specifically child-oriented programs, but that their children discriminate in any significant ways be- tastes rapidly broaden to include virtually the entire tween programing or advertising intended for spectrum of program types. Thus, by the time a child children and that intended for adults') How much at- leaves elementary school,hisor her program tention do children give to adult-oriented commer- preferences are likely to be closer to those of adults cials? How do they respond to them? Is there a po- than those. of preschool siblings. tential for children to misunderstand commercial messages addressed to adults? Is A child's reception On the whole, knowledge of children's viewing of adult advertising affected by the presence or ab- patterns is fairly extensive, especially in comparison sence of adults9 Some of these questions are touched with other areas reviewed in this report. One topic, upon in other sections of this report deserving further research attention is the influence of factors in the background and environment of children which might determine individual viewing patterns. Another importantissue: Should "children's television" be defined for regulatory 9The consumer advisors to the FTC's Children's Television purposes as programs intendedspecifically for Advertising Project proposed two categories of advertising affecting children, each with Its own set of regulations (1) A children, or should the definitioninclude any 'children's commercial" would he defined as "a commercial in program watched by a substantial number of or near a program r or which children comprise over 50 per- children? If the latter, how should the term "sub- cent of the audience, and (2) a "family commercial" would be stantial" be defined99 We have seen that mostrf defined as "a commercial in or near A program for which children comprise over 20 percent but not more than SO percent children's television viewing, and therefore the com- of the audience- (Consumers Advisors, 1974)Acccirding to mercials they are exposed to, occurs at times other these standards, 7 out of the 12 prime-time programs most popu- than those covered by children's advertising lar with children (see table ii-31 would he classified as "family guidelines programing

1.4

I'

0 !.14 Part II

LITERATURE REVIEWS

As indicatedin the Introduction, chaptersi policy question may be treated in relation to more through10 inthis section review theexisting Jhan one issue. Thus, the question of the clustering of research relevant to the 10 key issues we have iden- ads is discussed in terms of the program-commercial tified. As i result of organizing the review in this separation issue in chapter I, and as a "volume" way, the same study may be cited in several chapters. issue in chapter 8. For example, results from Atkih's study (1975f) of "parent-child communication in supermarket break- Finally, we remind the reader that 21"key fast selection" are discussed in the chapters on pre- studies" are reviewed technically in appendix A. A mium offers (4), television food advertising (7), and list of the studies included in this review appears at parent-child relations (10). In addition, a specific the beginning of the appendix, page 289.

233,3 a Chapter 1

CHILDREN'S ABILITY TO DISTINGUISH TELEVISION COMMERCIALS FROM PROGRAM MATERIAL

In its 1974 inquiry into chndren's programing toassigndifferentintentions tothetwo? and advertising practices, the FCC called attention Specifically. do children identify the selling of to the Federal Communications Act requirement a product as the intention of a commercial, and that all advertisements on and television indi- do they distinguish this intention from the in- cate clearly that they are paid for and by whom tention of program content? Finally, to the ex- tent that children perceive commercials to be The rationale behind this provision is, in part, distinct from programs and are able to unders- that an advertiser would have an unfau kdvan- tand the intention of the commercials, tage over listeners if they could not differenti- does this awareness act as a mediator between ate between the program and the commercial commercial messages and their 'resulting per- message and were, therefore, unable to take its suasive effects? paid status into consideration in assessing the message. (FCC, 1974) CURRENT AND PROPOSED REGULATIONS

In considering the question of fairness to young In the course ofits1974 inquiry,the FCC viewers of television advertising, the Commission reported that children, especially young children, was concerned with two different but related kinds apparently have considerable difficulty distinguish- of viewer comprehension: The firstinvolves the ing commercial from program matter. The FCC ability of a viewer simply to perceive commercials as referredto research evidence which found that distinct and separate material from the adjacent children do not begin to understand the selling in- programing,, the second deals with the viewer's un- tent of commercials until they have started grade derstanding of the selling purpose of television com- school.1 On the basis of the information the Com- mercials. For adultviewers, we can generally mission had gathered, it concluded that: assume that perception of a is accompanied by an understanding of its - If advertisements are to be directedto tional purpose. The same assumption cannot be children, then basic fairness requires that at made when the viewers are children That is, same least a clear separation be maintained between children may be able to correctly identify a televi- the program content and the commercial sion message as a commercial and still not com- message so as to aid the child in developing an prehend its purpose. ability to distinguish between the two.

We must consider two aspects of comprehension The Commission suggested that either an an- when questioning whether a child's inability to make nouncement or some form ofisual segment might accurate distinctions between commercial and be used before and after each commercial interrup- program material may provide the advertiser with tion 2 Following discussion that same year with the an unfair advantage Commission's chairman and staff, the NAB amended its advertising code in 1975 to take this 1. Do children perceive commercial messages to basic fairness requirement into account: "Commer be distinct and different from program con- cials, whether live, film or tape, within programs in- tent" What conditions act to blur cnildren's itially designed primarily for children under 12 perception of the separation between commer- cial advertisements and program content? To the extent that children do not spontaneously 1The studies referred to by the FCC (BIM. Spencer, and Ward, 1972, Ward, Reale, and Levmson, 1972) are discussed separate commercial and program content, later in this chapter what can be done to assist them in making the 2 Che FCC did not consider "clustering" of commercials at distinction" the beginning and end of a program to be necessary in pr.oviding 2. To the extent that children th.tdistinguish coin- a clear separation between advertising aad program content (see mercials from program content, are they able later in this chapter and also Lhapter 8)

354 years of age shall be clearly separated from program Neither organization has made any formal at- material by an appropriate device "3 tempt to address the question of children's com- prehension of the different intentions of advertising The FCC also identified a specific advertising and programing. Clearly, this issue and its resolu- practicethe use of progran, characters to promote tion are far more complex and difficult than the products ("host-selling")which,inits opinion, physical separation of commercials and programs. takes unfairadvantage of childrenby m4king The FCC acknowledged this problem in its public differentiation of commercials more difficult Two remarks during the 1974 inquiry "We recognize problematic effecti of host-sellinewere pointed out that this (the maintenance of at least a clear separa- by the FCC. (I) ,the program and the commercial tion') may be an incomplete solution to the problem become interwoven, thereby hindering the distinc- ..the broadcast of an announcement and/or a tion between them, and (2) the sales technique takes visual device can only aid children in identifying advantage of thetrust which children placein commercials." program characters The CommiFsion expressed the belief that "... the use of a program host, or other The responsibilityfor providing a separation program personality, to promote products in the device between commercials and programing, in, program in which he appears is (not) a practice tended specifically for children has been assumed by which is consistent with licensees' obligation to oper- the individual broadcaster Each of the three net- ate in the public interest works has designed its own nonprogram "bumpers" to serve this function during children's programing: In 1975, both the NAB and the NAD incorpor- ABC inserts A 5-second animated musical logo ated a restriction on host-selling into their codes for ("Funshine Saturday")before each commercial children's advertising The NAB Code states break. CBS presents a brief animated segment with an audio announcement (e.g., "We'll return right No children's program personality or cartoon afterthese messages," "and now back o our character shall be utilized to deliver commer- program") before and after each set of commercials. cial messages within or adjacentto the NBC displays a 3-second program title cara on the programs in which such a personality or car- screen, with no audio announcement. Television stink, toon character regularly appears This provi- tions owned by Post-Newsweek have been experill, sion shall also apply to lead-ins to commercials menting with a format which clusters commercials at when such lead-ins contain sell copy or imply the beginning and end of each children's show, pre- endorsement of the product by program per- ceded in each case by the following voice-over sonalities or cartoon characters notification.'ion.'

Similarly, the NAD rules specify that "program per- Post-Newsweek stations do not place commer- sonalities or program characters (live or animated) cials within children's programsItis our on children's programs should not be useu to pro- policy to cluster commercials at the opening mote products, premiums, or services in or adjacent and closing of each program, so there will not to any program where the personality or characters be any confusion between the sales message appear " and the entertainment portions of the program We hope you like this approach to children's The specific form that this separation device should assume programing and would enjoy hearing your was not specified by the NAB beypnd the indication that the sole comments use of afade to black" would not he adequate 4While the Commission noted that the use of program characters to commercials on programs other than the ones cn which they appear might still take unfair advantage of the trust `The effect of this clustering approach on children s ability to relationship between the child and the performer, it recognized discriminate between commercial and program material is con- thatit might not he practically feasible for small stations to sidered in a study conducted by Duffy and Rossiter (I 975), avoid using children s show personnel in commercial messages iescribed in the Research Evidence section of this chapter (and on other programs als, chapter 8)

3 5 As is apparent from these examples, no standar- sample of 67 children, ranging in age from 5 to 12 dized procedure exists for providing the required years. Children's responses to the direct question, "clear separation" between commercial and "What is the difference between a TV program and a program material!, Later in this chapter, we will ex- TV commercial?" revealed clear differences bet- amine the question of whether the various devices ween younger (5-8) and older (9-12) children in the currentlyinuse actually achieve their intended degree of verbalizeddiscrimination between effect of providing children with a clearly dis- programs and commercials. Younger children tinguishable separation between advertisement and generally exhibited a low level of differentiation, program content. often based on recognition of different perceptual cues, (e.g., "commercials are short and programs are With regard to the voluntary ban on host-selling, long"). In contrast, most of the older children's the broadcasters have again assumed primary responses indicated a high level of differentiation, responsibility for interpreting and carrying out the based upon some understanding of the meaning of code The network or station sells advertisers the op- the message (e g., "programs are supposed to enter,- portunity to have a commercial run during a specific tam," "commercials try to sell things") program on a given date. Then the broadcasters themselves put together t1actual sequence of com- These findings consistently demonstrate a posi- mercials in each of the commercial groups ("pods"), tive relationship between children's age and their and must separate commercials featuring children's ability to describe the difference between commer- program hosts and characters from the programs in cial and program material More specifically, the which these hosts or characters appear younger children (ages 5-8 years) either expressed confusion about the difference or used superficial RESEARCH EVIDENCE perceptual or affective cues as the basis for the dis- tinction. This evidence appears tc, have been influen- Perceptionof (ommercials asdistinctfrom tial, in the FCC's 1974 recommendation to licensees programsIn a pilot study conducted byBlatt, that special measures be taken to ensure an adequate Spencer, and Ward (1972), 20 children ranging in separation between television advertisements and age from 5 to 12 years, were exposed to a videotape programs directed to all children of typical Saturday morning programing and com- mercials and then interviewed the following day All of these studies were based solely on about what they had viewed. The authors found that children's verbal responses to abstract questions and although the children in all age groups could iden- not on other measures of their ability to discriminate tifythe term "commercials," the younger (kin- between program and commercial material in an ac- dergarten) children exhibited some confusion about tual viewing situation A number of other st _dies the concept and Judged the relationship between monitored children's attention patterns in an ongo- commercials and programs on the basis either of ing viewing situationIn one early attempt to ex- affect ("commercials are more funny") or of coinci- amine attention patterns prior to and during com- dential reasoning ("commercials are shorter than mercial messages, mothers of 5- to12-year old programs") children were trained to observe and record infor- mation about their children's normal viewing -Subsequent research extended and confirmed the behavior (Ward, Levinson, and Wackman, 1972) findings of this exploratory effort (Ward, Reale, and Analysis of these data indicated a tendency for the Levinson, 1972, Ward and Wackman, 1973) Per- children to exhibit a drop in attention when a corn,- sonal in-home interviews were administered to a mercial was shown,-compared with their attention to the prior programing In addition, the children's at- tention generally continued to decline during later 'However the NAB code does specify that the number of program interruptions in Lhildren's programing time shall not commercials, both within a series of commercials exceed two within a AO-unnute program or four within a 60- and over the course of the program However, the mmute program Another code provision specifies that a max- smallest decreases in attention occurred among the imum of 9 minutes 30 seconds of nonprogram material is per- youngest (5-7) viewers, that is, they displayed higher mitted in any M) minute period during Lhildren's progr.oning levels and more stable patterns of attention to both time Hence. commervials may he (and are) shown at the begin- ning and at the end of programs. and between programs luring commercials and programs other than the older station breaks children.

23 The researchers speculatedthatthisgreater jacent advertising are attributable to the children's stabilityin younger children's attention patterits recognition of the distinctive nature of the two The across both program and commercial materials may Wartella and Ettema findings suggest only that the be evidence of the difficulty they have indis- degree of attention paid to television material may criminating between the two .The researchers be a function of the material's specific audio-visual therefore inferred that the lower levels and greater features, quite apart from whether children perceive differentiation in older children's attention to com- the material as a commercial or a program. mercials indicate their greater awareness of and im- munity to television advertising 7 Another study of the relationship between chang- ing perceptual features of television stimuli and In a subsequent experimental study of children's children's attentional behavior measured preschool attention to television commercials, the investiga- children's visual attention to varied television tors adopted another point of view, hypothesizing material coded for the presence or absence of par- that younger children are likely to displaymore ticular stimuli (Levin and Anderson. 1976) The differential attention to television materialif the researchersfotindthatspecificfeatures of the material is varied in the complexity of its stimuli material, such as lively music and active motion, as ( Wartella and Ettema, 1974) They based their hy- wellas more generalizedvisualandauditory pothesis on the premise that the younger and more changes, enhanced the children's attention to the "perceptually bound" the children, the greater the screen Although these findings were based upon influence of perceptual, rather than cognitive stimuli children's responses to program material, rather on their attention patterns. Twelve commercials-of than to advertising, it is plausible to expect that simi- varying visual and auditory complexity were in- lar changes in attention might occur as a conse- terspersed, in three blocks or "pods," throughout'a quence of the abrupt perceptual changes in the shifts television program shown to 120 children 3 to S between programing and commercialmessages. years old 8 The criteria and methods used to record However, the relative influence of perceptual and the degree of children's attention were the same as cognitive factors on children's attention patterns re- those employed in the earlier study by Ward et al mains a moot point until children's ability to dis- As the investigators expected, the youngest (3-4) tinguish program and commercial contentias directly viewers showed the greatest differences in their at- measured Specifically, the effectiveness of current lention-to high versus low complexity commercials network separation devices needs to he subjected to and were generally less stable in their level of atten- testing tion from one observation to the next Commercials rated high inauditorycomplexity were given more Effect5 of cluttered commercial formate.Some attention by the children than those rated low, in this research has been conducted on children's responses quality, regardless of the visual complexity rating to clustered versus dispersed commercial formats (Atkin, 1975h, Duffy and Rossiter, 1975)Atkin As in the Ward. Levinson. and Wackman study. -randomly assigned 500 preschool through 5th grade the researchers interpreted the tendency of the children to view seven commercials which were children to change their level of attention during the either dispersed throughout a cartoon program or shifts between program and commercial material as clustered before or following the show. The results an indication of the children's awareness of The indicated that overall attention to the commercials difference between them However, we cannot con- was significantly higher in the clustered presenta- clude, on the basis of this evidence alone, that tion However, no significant differences were found differences in attention level to programing and ad- in the children's recall of the commercials or ex- pressed preferences for advertised products

'It should he noted that all ,bildren were not obsersed slew When Duffy and Rossiter exposed 1st and 4th mg the sane telesision material, the particular shows and av grade classes to the two commercial formats (the cornpanving advertising monitored in the stud% reflected the clustered commercials were preceded by a voice- preferences of each viewer over notification), the researchers reported that the he children were separated into three age groups and the first graders watching the clustered version paid sig- members ot each age group were randomiN assigned to sleA one of four versions ot this programIn each setsion, the order ut nificantly more attention to the commercials than commercials within each block was rotated However the Hocks the fourth grade childrtm Among the fourth gra- themselves were not rotated within the program ders, the dispersed format produced significantly

28 (3 higher attention patterns 9 As in the Atkin study, along with) the program in a cluster of commercials however, the two formats made no significant ("nonadjacent" condition) Presentation of the ad- difference in recall of advertised brand named by vertisment in the clustered, nonadjacent condition either age group 10 (Children at both grade levels produced both greater attention to the commercial tended to verbalize a preference for the clustered and somewhat more expressed desire for the cereal. commercial treatment, although approximately 40 Children in, the nonadjacent condition were also percent of the 1st graders either could not verbalize slightly less accurate about where the cereal eating a clear differentiation or were unable to offer a took place reasoned preference between the two formats ) As Atkin points out, the findings suggest that the In neither study was children's ability to discrimi- nearby presence, rather than direct adjacency, of the nate between commercials and program material program to the commercial may be sufficient both to directly assessed. However, Duffy and Rossiter in- increase the effectiveness of a commercial featuring ferred, on the basis of data showing 1st graders to be the program characters and to create confusion bet- less attentive to dispersed commercials, that the ween the two, especially for younger children.' 1 As clustered format did not aid the younger children in noted above, current NAB code restrictions prohibit discriminating between the commercials and the only directadjacency. To the extentthatthe program. Since the clustered structure contained a children's desire for the product was enhanced by commercial warning announcement and the dis- their identification with the familiar cartoon charac- persed version did not, the effectiveness of the warn- ters, rather than by program-commercial confusion, ing per se cannot be determined without testing a the current industry restrictions on the placement of dispersed-plus-warning version host-selling commercials may be addressing only a part of the full issue Effects of host-selling Program hosts and charac- ters are now prohibited from appearing in commer- Perception of the intent of commercials. In the cials within or directly adjacent to their programs Ward and Wackman (1973) study cited earlier, the In sight of this restriction, itis of interest to note a 5-12 year olds were also questioned about the pur- study which examined the effectivenss of a "Pebbles" pose of commercials ("Why are commercials shown cereal commercial featuring the Flintstones cartoon on TV`' ")Nearly one-half (47 percent) of the characters (Atkin, 1975b) The children were ob- children verbalized low levels of understanding of served to pay slightly more attentionto the the selling motives of commercials. The least aware Flintstones cereal advertisement when it was shown children were more likely to be younger (5-8) and to in the context of a Flintstones cartoon program, he evaluated as responding at a "lower cognitive rather than with a Bugs Bunny cartoon The younger level "12 In discussing these findings, the researchers children (3-7 year olds) in the study expressed sig- offered this interpretation nificantly more desire for the cereal when the com- mercial appeared on the same tape as the Flintstones While young children may simply lack infor- program. The younger children were also more mation about the nature of television advertis- likely to mistakenly recall the Flintstones characters ing, or fail to comprehend this information, it as eating cereal in the program rather than inthe may be that low cognitive-level children can- commercial not abandon their own perspective and take the perspective of the advertiser when viewing Atkin also compared children's responses to the commercials commercial when shown within the Flintstones car- Robertson and Rossiter (1974) hypothesized that toon ("adjacent" condition) and shown outside (but the ability torecognize the persuasive intent of com- mercials wild depend in part upon the child's mak- yAtkin's overall finding of higher attention to clustered com- ing a numbc- of prior cognitive distinctions (a) dis- mercials may he partially explained by the fact that over half (57 crimination between programing and commercials, percent) of the children included in his sample were less than )l years old lotntetestingly, an inverse relation was found between visual I lAtkin's conclusions are discussed further in chapter 3 attention and brand name recall On the whole. the leastwell- '--The researchers constructed a three-level scale of cognitive watched commercials were the best recalledAs the authors functioning based upon Paiget's theory of cognition develop- pointed out, this discrepancy suggests the importance of auditory ment Lower cognitive level responses were identified with stimuli and attention Paiget's 'preoperational- of thought (b) recognition of an external source (i e., a spon- Robertson and Rossiter found, for example, that sor), (c) perception of an intended audience as the more than half (53 percent) of the first graders inter- target of the advertiser's message; (d) awareness of viewed "wanted every toy or game they saw adver- the symbolic, as opposed to realistic, nature of com- tised on television," as compared with only 6 percent mercials; and (e) recall of personal experiences in of the fifth grade childrenObserving that older which discrepancies had been discovered between children have a better understanding of the per- productsas advertised- and products in actuality In- suasive intent of commercials, these researchers con- terview responses from a sample of 289 I st, 3d, and cluded thatthe development of persuasive-intent 5th grade boys offered support for this hypothesis, attributions acts as a cognitive defense to p'ersua- suggesting that those children who were capable of sion."11 This is not to say, of course, that commer- recognizing commercials as persuasive messages met cials do not,influence purchases or purchase re- these antecedent or perhaps concurrent criteria. quests The economic realities of commercial broad- casting offer clear evidence that commercials can As in the Ward and Wackman study, Robertson sell products to viewers even when their sales intent and Rossiter found the development of persuasive- is clearly understood intent awareness to he positively related to age Whereas just over half (53 percent) of 1st graders SUMMARY OF RESEARCH EVIDENCE exhibited understanding of the selling intent of television advertisements, almost all 5th graders (99 Children's ability to distinguish between program percent) understood itAs the authors explained, and cor'mercial content has only been measured in- "Age, as a variable, reflects not only maturational directly, using either verbal or attentional measures factors but also cumulative experience with com- Studies using verbal responses to general question- mercial messages ing found a positive relationship between age and children's verbal ability to differentiate between programs and commercials. Younger children, par- These two studies present complementary ap- ticularly below ages 8 or 9, either express confusion proaches to the efforts to examine the development or base their discrimination of commercials on affect by children of a mature concept of television adver- or on superficial perceptual cues, such as a commer- tising Ward and Wackman's study delineates cer- cial's shorter length Older children are able to dis- 'tain cognitiveabilities which seem to underlie tinguish program and commercial material on the children's grasp of thenature of commercials, basis of an overall understand ira, of each message's Robertson and Rossiter's analysis identifies a num- meaning. ber of specific cognitive distinctions which children make when they are able to comprehend the intent of Studies of visual attention patterns have tended to commercial messages Infer discrimination of commercial and program material from observed changes in children's atten- Persuasive effects of commercials.There is consis- tion levels between program segments and adjacent tent evidence, booed on verbal reports, that younger commercial announcements. However, there is also children who do not understand the persuasive in- evidence suggesting,alternatively,thatthese tent of commercials are more likely to perceive them changes in attention are attributable to the specific as truthful messages, whereas older children who audio-visual changes taking place between advertis- can discern persuasive intent tend to express skepti- ing and programing sequences Further research is cal,less accepting attitudes toward commercials required beflire we can determine the relative in- (Robertson and Rossiter, 1974; Ward, Reale, and fluence of specific perceptual features of commer- Levinsbn, 1972; Ward and Wackman, 1973), In ad- cials and commercial breaks versus children's recog- dition, various investigations of the influence of nition of the distinct nature and purpose of commer- television advertising on children's purchase re- cials The effectiveness of current network separa- quests have revealed that younger children express tion devices also remains untested higher levels of purchase requests for certain adver- tised products than older viewers (Atkin,I 975g, l 'The relative appeal of advertised to and games for 1st Gene Reilly Grup, I974a, Ward and Wackman, and 5th grade children may also he a factor See chapter_ 8 for i972, Robertson and Rossiter, 1974) further discussion of this question

33 Comparisons made between dispersed and Theretore, there is still a question as to whether clustered commercial formats indicate that children. younger children especially can tell the difference and particularly the younger children 3-8. are more between programs and commercials, and whether attentive to clustered commercials and express a current forms of program-commercial separation preterence for this torm of presentation No signifi- are achieving their intended purpose Both of these cant differences were tound between the two formats questions are deserving of and amenable to further in children's acquisition of intormationfrom the testing based on the useful groundwork provided by commercials or in their expressed desires for the ad- existing studies. vertiseproducts For example, the separation devices could be The single study investigating the influence of tested by exposing randomly assigned groups of host-selling revealed greater desire among younger children to a television program with standard com- children (below 8) for the advertised product when mercial breaks using one of the separation devices. the commercial presented the animated character A "no device" version of the program could provide featured in the adjacent program On the basis of this a baseline measureChildren's attentioninthis evidence, however, it cannot be determined whether ongoing viewing situation would be monitoredIn the younger children's Increased product interest addition, each child might be asked to ident.fv (by was due to confusion between the programand the means of a simple response, such as raising onehand, commercial or to heightened identification with the pressing a button, or telling the researcher) whenhe character or she was seeing either a commercial or a program. Alternatively,if the videotape were stopped at A substantial proportion of children, particularly specific pointsfor example, jest after the separa- those below 8 years, express little or no comprehen- tion device--children could be asked to anticipate sain of the persuasive intent ofcommercials. ("guess") what they thought was going to happen Development of this understanding may depend next upon children's general level of cognitivefunction- ing as well as their ability to make a numberof In light of previous stuuies in which auditory specific prior distinctions about the nature of Com- stimuli were found to influence children's attention mercials, beginning with the discrimination of com- to program and commercial material, it would also mercials as distinct trom programs be instructiv., to have the children perform t'.ese tasks on the basis of either visual or sound-track Younger children who are unaware of the selling content alone, as well as tinder conditions in which motives of television advertising tend to express botn auditory and visual information were a'aila- greater belief in commercials and a !nigher frequency ble. Comparisons of the various testing situations of purchase requests for certain advertised products should reveal the effe,of eac`i separation device than do older children who display an understand- auditorily and/or visually, and should also reveal ing of the intent of commercials Thesedifferences the age-related ability of the children to distinguish suggest that a more mature concept of the nature and commercials from programs, and their ability to purpose of advertising acts as a mediatinginfluence identify or anticipate the onset of the commercials. between commercials and their effectiveness as per- suasive messages The specific program and commercial material to be used in such a study should he selected with care. NEEDED RESEARCH For example, commercials for products familiar to the children may '...more easily recognized as ad- Research evidence indicatintthat young children vertising. Similarly, children may associate commer- have difficulty discriminating between programing cials with certain frequently used audio-visual tech- and advertising has already led to the banning of one niques, such as quick-cut editing style("montage") type of host-selling and to the requirement of a clear or musical jingles. The reasons that thechildren separation device at commercial breaks on differentiate a given commercial froma given children's programs. However, neither the ability of program should be probed, in order to gain insight children to distinguish commercials from programs into the specific content or characteristics that may nor the separation devices currently used tofacili- serve as identifying cues for children. Bearing in tatethis distinctionhas been directlyassessed mind the existing research findings of consistent

314 0 differences in the discriminatory abilities of younrer pared against their more general level of concep- and older children, investigations should take tualization special care to raise these questions among preschool (3-5) as well as among older (b-1 2) children.' Now childrer a( quire an under: nding of the persu ASIVC intent cf commercials is another impor- particular program-commercial combina- tant line of needed research There is considerable tion3 aiso warrant careful considerationFor in- research evidence available indicating that children stance, do children find it more difficult to discrimi- below ages 7 or 8 tend not to have this understand- nate adjacent programs and commercials when both ing, and that young children without this awareness are in animated form; or when the same, or similar, respond ditferentl} to television advertisements,in characters appear in both the program and the ad- terms of both greater expressed belief inthe; vertisement.' A giv,program and its adjacent ad- truthfulness and more frequent requests for the ad- vertising should tconsidered as a hypothetical con- vertised products Researchers Mould examine the tinuum from similarity 'to contrast, with various relationship between a general understanding of the points of minimal difference below which children purpose of commercials and specific attributes of of a particular age might not he able to makean ac- -nmercials, such as particular product claimsor curate distinction between them. If Information were qualifiers If such investigations were systematicail# also obtained verbally from children about their conducted, we might develop measures which would abwact understanding of the nature and purpose of help us to predict when a particular advertising television advertising, then the specific perceptual practice would be deceptive (i.e,mi,..inderstood) attributes used by children of different ages to make for children at a given age and level of cognitive program-commercial distinctions could be com- functioning

3241 Chapter 2

THE INFLUENCE OF FORMAT ANDAUDIO-VISUAL TECHNIQUES ON CHILDREN'S PERCEPTIONS OFCOMMERCIAL MESSAGES

Many audio- v isual techniques in commercials are speed, method of operation, size, color, simp'y aimed at gaining and holding children's at- durability, nutrition, noise, etc te tion Policy issues arise with regard to production techniques such as those which may tend to In addition to these general statements, both in- misreprcit the appearance of children's products dustry codes include gwdelines for the provision of or exaggerate product performanceAnother issue specific kinds of information about product charac- Code concerns theability of children to understand teristics and functions For example, the NAB descriptions of product characteristics int ,mtner- spec that advertisements intended for children era's or the meaning of disclaimers and , shall

CURRENT AND PROPOSED REaULATIOt Provide audio disclosure when a product re- quires assembly, The television and advertising industries hay e Provide audio or video disclosure as to a prod- formally acknowledged the influence of commercial uct's methodof operation andsource of formats and production techniques on children's power,' perceptions of television advertising The NAB, for example, otters the following statements of princ'rle Provide simultaneous audio and video dis- in its 197(, cock, closures when items, such as hat eries needed to operate a product as demonstrated in thead- In order to redtiee the possibility of misimpres vertising, are not included, and .ions being created, all information (onthe Avoid competitive'compar any e superiority characteristics and functional aspects of a pro- claims about (toys and other durable) pro- shallhepresentedin a duct orservice) ducts 2 straightforward manner devoid of language or productio,, techniques which may exaggerate The NAB guidelines tstablished for toy products functions of a or distort the characteristics or contain the most explicit instructions for theproduc- product tion of television advertisements Thus, in orderthat In order to help assure that advertising is non- the "audio and video production techniques (shall) explonaiive in manner, style, and tone, such not misrepresent the appearance andperformance of advertising shall avoid usiro exhortative toys," lay advertising shall seek language It shall also avoid employing irritat- irg, obtrusive or strident audio-techniques or To present the toyin its actual merits as a video devices, such as cuts of less than one sec- playthingIt shall neither exaggerate nor dis, ond in length, a series of fast cuts, kand) special tor, play valve, effects of a psychedelic nature (e g ,flashing To limit any view of a to:, or demonstration of colors flashing lights, flashing s. pered copy, itspertorrnancetothat whichachildis or other effects which could overglamorize or reasonably capable of reproducing, mislead)

With regard to the presentation of advertising, the the Li)tfc imitsatrs that the side use of a sunerim NAD's children's advertising guidelines (1975) call nosed snit o title is not Considered to he adequate =Such Liam}, disallowedhei.atri,e true itis tot particular care by advertisers to assure that iii ieeu suLti Tete ...ices mas makt a child dissatisfied with a toy he Airl',10} possesses or may is cis Halls% added)I his reason Copy, sound, and visual presentations, as well partiLul iris notessorths in that it attempts to take into con- as the a ivertisement in itstotality. do not sideration other unAsirahle r thus hcsond theCreation of mislead performance characteristics such as misimpression, about produ,IN

3.42 To employ the complete and authentic ing to which children are usual' xposed. Still, sound(s) of the toy, these studies arc useful in their dtcriptions of the To confine their use of generic stink film specific stimulus properties of advertising messages footage, real-life counterparts of toys, tantasy directed to children and animation (in none of which either a child or toy appears) to the first one-third of the A study by Barcus (1975a), the most recent of commercial, and these analyses of children's commercials, coincides most closely with current practices and regulations To clearly disicose the ortginal purchase in the and is therefore the primary source of incidence in- bony of the commercial and (by video, with formation reported below. Barcus analyzed a sam- audio disclosure where necessary for clarifica- ple of 403 (137 different) commercials broadcast on tion) in the closing 5 seconds a weekend morning during April 1975, appearing on five Boston stations, including three network affili- The NAB guidelines also urge that music, sound ates and two Independent UHF stationsOther effects, volume level, tempo, and other audio tech- studies, in particular an earlier analysis by the same niques be used with restraint and discretion In terms author (Barcus, 1971), will serve as useful sources of of the video portions of children's commercials, cau- compa,tive or supplementary descriptions in the tion is advised in the use of certain video techniques follownig summary (e g , camera angles, special lenses, special lighting, and dazzling visual effects). The guidelines point out According to these content analyses, television that the use of such techniques becomes questionable advertising directed primarily at children may be when the appearance or performance of a toy is dis- characterized as follows torted or exaggerated

I Almost all commercials are 30 seconds in INCIDENCE OF SPECIFIC AUDIO-VISUAL length The percentage of broadcasttime FEATURES devoted to commercial messages has decreased since 1971 (15 9 percentin1975 from an Several recent studies have examined the inci- earlier 18 8 percent), reflecting changes in the dence of specific techniques and features in commer- NAB code as to the maximum amount of ad- cials directed at young audiences (Atkin, 1975d, ertising permissible during children's Barcus, 1971, 1975 a &h, Doolittle and Pepper, programs (9 5 minutes hour as of January 1975, Wimck, Williamson, Chuzmir, and Wmick, 1976) Nevertheless, there are about as many 1973) Typically, these .*Jdies video-tape a sample commercials per hour now as there were in of commercials broadcast during Saturday morning 1971 Apparently, this has been accomplished children's programing and later analyze the material by reducing the average length of the commer- for the presence or absence of various features (e g cials animation, form of product di play, particular prod- 2 The types of products and services promoted to uct claims or disclosures, etc )The analysisis children represent afairly limited range of generally based upon a previously detined coding items cereals (25 percent), candy'sweetse g,, system cakes, cookies, fruit drinks (25 percent); toys (18 percent), eating places t 10 percent), and Although this method of content analysis pro- miscellaneous products, such as movies (10 vides a systematic procedure for describing the fre- percent) (Barcus, 1975a) Only slight shifts in quency of particular features of advertisements, such the incidence of these product categories were descriptions are not comprehensiveRather, they reportedfromtheearlierBarcus figures, renresent the features and techniques which the par- cereals, candy/sweets, and toys were the staples ticular researcher considers interesting or impor- for both periods Atkin's comparison of Satur- tantFurther, by sampling commercials aired on day morning commercials appears in 1972 and weekend mornings,' these researchers have limited 1973 found a substantially higher frequency of their analyses to only a small portion of the advert's- toy advertisements (58 percent), but this is at- tributable to the November sampling, when pre-Christmas toy is increased 'the maps cx,,epli,ri is a qud rt Aeckda 3 More than half (58 percent) of the commer- mer,Rils tHar..tp, 19-sh cials use a live-action format, the remainder 213 employ animated techniques, either in com- 1973). The use of contemporary, local settings bination with live-action (26 percent) or alone may in part reflect advertisers' attemptsto (16 percent) Again, Barcus found little change comply with the NAB guidelines, which in the use of these general format styles be- recommend placing toy products in settings tween the two time periods (1971,1975) which achildis reasonably capable of Product classes diffe4---tubstantially intheir reproducing mode of presentationtoy advertisments are 6 Children's commercials are heavily populated almost exclusively live-action (94 percent)4 as by white males Adult males tend to be the an- are eating places, candy and sweets commer- nouncers or authoritative voices forproducts, cials tend to be about evenly divided between and both men and boys outnumber females as live-action and animation; and cereal commer- characters (Barcus, 1971,1975b, Doolittle cials rely heavily on animation (80 percent). In and Pepper, 1975, Liebert, Schuetz, and part, this difference probably reflects toy ad- Sprafkin, 1975, Verna, 1975)' According to vertisers' compliance with the restrictions im- the most recent figures ( Barcus, I 975a), almost posed on the use of fantasy and animation in half (48 percent) of the characters are children, the NAB guidelines 27 percent are animals and others, 23 percent 4 Toy commercials usually display the product areadults, anda minor 2percent are in usei.e , show children playing with the toy teenagers With the exception of those whose (90 percent)5 whereas cereal advertisements sex was not identifiable, about seven outof ten are about equally likely to use a pict ire of the of these characters were male in the Barcus product as they are to depict a person eating it. study. In terms of race, 92 percent of the While differencesin specific code require characters (when race was reported) are white merits may help to explain the choice of display and 8 percent are black, members of other style, it is assumed that the way these products ethnic groups appear to be virtually absent as are presented also suits the objectives ofthe characters advertisers 6 Toy advertisers undoubtedly find 7 In terms of verbal content, product brand it desirable to Aow the toy itself in active use names are repeated an average of 3 5 timesper Analysis of specific product claims indict' commercial (Atkin, I975d) and musi- that action, speed, and power are among the at- cal jingles are often used to present brand tributes most often refuted to verbally in toy names or other product-related content commercials (Atkin, 1975d).7 In contrast, (Atkin, 1975d, Winick et al., 1973). Children's cereal marketers prefer to useanimationa and commercials also tend to provide little infor- fantasy (animated "presenter" characters are mation about the "hard qualities" of products, often engaged inst-rt dramatic skits during suchasprice,size,materials, quantity, the body of the commercial) to convey their durability, etc (Atkin, 1975d; Barcus, 1971, product messages 1975a; Winick et al , 1973). While this finding 5 With regard to physical and temporal settings, is rather general and less clearlyquantified most children'!: commercials occur in the pres- than those cited above, the infrequency of such ent (86 percent) and take place in generalized descriptions seems inconsistent with the NAB's locations outdoors (34 percent) orinor general guideline stating that "the disclosure of around the home (31 per 2111). This description information on the characteristics and func- of commercial settings is consistent ,with those tional aspects of a product/service is strongly of earlier studies (Barcus, 1971, Winick et al , recommended The incidence of disclaimers (e g., "batteries not included,"items sold separately,") is as follows overall, 22 percent of commercials in- 'Atkinhighcr (-rail !Ise rum if,' perLeni) clude some kind of audio disclaimer, 11 per- IN probably due to ihe greater pn,p niu,nit tio, i.11111e1 cent use video disclaimers, and 8 percent pre- his NairpleI 91Sdi 'In (Air of the iitherttNini.111N fromior,1 htI. sent such qualifiers simultaneously in both tv.ii or more tiui,ther but iittl Nepaiatel audio and video ( Barcus, I975a). When com- 1197shi paredagainstBarcus' earlierstudy,these 'In add.tion althouv,h aniniatioh i, moo expen.ie io pro duLe thin Itte aLtionanimated ,iiinnieri.lak tend tu I1,11r.,I'Ic i t,Ili,- further di,i.tiscion longer life e this di, nut tu.ed updating as See ,floptt r figures represent an increase in the practice of quately. As Winick et al explain, "the unique providing audio and/or video disclaimers. ambience of a given commercial-is not likely to 8 The use of special effects in children's commer- be captured by content analysis." cials has been examined to some extent. An Features which are 'rare or totally absent but earlier study, for example, reported 1:4 percent whose inclusion in children's commercials is' of the advertisements to be using such "striking appropriate or even required may he over- visual techniques" as fast-cutting or psy- looked, particularly when the primary purpose ch effects and almost half (46 percent) to of such analysis is simply to describe whatcur- b,sing "attention-grabbing music" as part of rent messages are like. In the studies reported their soundtracks (W 'nick, et ai., 1973). A sub- here the researchers did attempt to include sequent study found close-ups in about 40 per- analyses of certain areas where there wasa cent of the commercials, particularly in toy ad- paucity of information about "hard product vertising, but .acre was minimal use of other qualities," or only minimal occurrence ofa techniques, such as accelerated or slow-motion specific technique (use of off-pace motion) or multiple-camera angles (Atkin, 1973d) In some cases, these and other researchers RESEARCH EVIDENCE have gone on to make evaluative judgments about the extent to which the use of such tech- There has been little research on children'sper- niques exaggerated a product's attributes. ceptions of particular formats or audio-visual tech- Winick et al.k1973) indicated that exaggerated nique% in commercials. Much of the research- interest effects were created by special visual tech- in the effects of television advertising on children niques (i.e., "sparkling") in 7 percent of the has concentrated instead on whole commercials, commercials and by sound effects ("snap or without considering their component features crackle") in 2 percent of the cases. Atkin (Robertson and Rossitet,1Q74, Ward, Reale, and (1975d) described product performanceas Levinson, 1972, Ward and Walkman, 1973). being "moderately exaggerated" in about halfMoreover,' although individual children's adver- of the Saturday morning commercials studied. tisers and their ad agencies otten conduct prebroad- Barcus (1975b) notedindividualcasesin cast research with children to test the relative eiTec- which the use of particular visual techniques t iveness of al teriative strategies or presentations, the appeared to be potentially misleading or con- results of such in-house testing are not published and fusing to a young childfor example, the use are generally unavailable to the public of tight close-upto display a , without providing a perspective by which a child could Studies of television programs Most of the studies judge the dell's actual size. that assciate television techniques with particular effects upon young viewers have examinedprograms Unfortunately, the usefulness of such evaluative rather than commercials Considerable researchis information is limited The absence of explicitcri- available, for example, on instructional television teria upon which these judgments were based makes material and the effects of various production tech- their replication difficult, and, more Importantly, niques and other format elements on children's the question of whether a particular technique exag- learning patterns. Schramm (1972) has summarized gerates the attributes of a product needs to be tested the findings from many of these studies andreports against the perceptions of the child viewers them- that the following audio-visual techniques seemedto selvesIn addition, the descriptive power of these facilitate learning providing viewers witha subjec- content analyses has two major limitations tive view (e.g., a camera angle recording what the viewer would see if performing the task himself). in- Some attributes of commercials. specifically creasing the size of printed labels on the screen, and those more quantitative in nature (e.g. num- naming objects on the sound track as they are pre- bers and types of characters. brand namesented on the picture tracks On the basis of such .:ferences), are amenable to objective findings, Schramm offers the general observation measurements, while otherle easily measured qualities may evade description For 1 his finding is id partiLuliir interest in L,,m,idering the cite. example the emotional tone conveyed by a dueness ut the sa-ious torms e dissiosures curreutls particular commercial is difficult to code ade- used in Lhiluren s tommerLials

36 45 hat "what makes the difference is usually . . how increased by the presence of lively music, singing, the pictoi ial treatment fits the learning goal rhyming, sound effects integral to the central ac- tivity, and auditory changes in general. The useof More recently, in one of a series of such evalua- other special effects (e g., pixilation, slow orfast mo- tive studies. Friedlander and his associatesmeasured tion) neither enhanced nor lowered attentionsignifi- preschool children's comprehensionof specific cantly visual and verbal elements presented in a3-minute segment of an informationaltelevision program Salomon (1976) used Sesame Street marcrial in an (Friedlander, Wetstone, and Scott, 1974).Although attempt to investigate the effects of particularfilm the segment was generally accepted as age-appropri- techniques and fc mats on the development of cor- ate material, the resultsindicated that more than responding cognitive skills among children. For ex- half of the 31 children demonstrated comprehension ample, he identified the close-up as atechnique of less than half of the tested information.Based which calls upon the skill of relating parts to the upon their analysis, the researchers suggestthat the whole. A sample of 317 5, 7, and 8 year old Israeli failures in comprehension were not necessarily at- children were tested before and after the first broad- tributable only ,o the children's cognitive limita -.. casting season of Sesame Street Salomon found that tions The lower levels of learning were alsorelated the children's knowledge of the program's content to the use of particular programtechniques, such as and their mastery of the selected cognitive skills thepresentation of informationin only one were unrelated at the beginning of thebroadcast modality, either visual or auditory, rather than in season, but these two factors came to beclosely in- both terrelated among the heavy viewers of the show The author surmises that "improvements in skill mastery As final examples of research on theeffects of for- came to serve the extraction ofknowledge." More fnat and audio-visual techniques intelevision generally, this evidence is consistent with the notion programing, an impressivenumber of studies have that the ability of children to acquireand under- investigated children's responses to the Children's stand information from television isbased upon Television Workshop (CTW) production,Sesame mastery of the particular perceptualand cognitive Street 9 The CTW research staff developed and sus- skills called upon to extract this information from tained a systematic program of child-watching and the format in whichitispresented ("television interviewing during the planning ofthe program literacy"). Moreover, such mas ery improves with This formative research provided the basis forlater increased exposure to this kyfid of material testing of the appeal and teaching valueof various production techniques used in the show (e gmusic Although there are obvious differences between animation, puppet's) (Lesser, 1974, Palmer,1972). instructional television programs and commercials, tnis program research can be usefullyapplied in Other researches interested inthe particular several ways to the stuuy of the effects oftelevision effects of television on young children havealso advertising. studied Sesame Street programs Foi example, Levin and Anderson (1976) measured the attentionof 1 to - I. In spite ofthen-differences in content and pur- 4 year old children to particular featuresof the pose, programs and commercials share manyaudio- show By rating the presence and absenceof these visual characteristics. Therefore, the specific features in parallel to the ratings of the children's at- program techniques and'forrhats which have been tention patterns, the investigatorsdetermined found to significantly influence attention and facili- whether attention was increased ordecreased during tate learning are probably worthy of seriousin- the occurrence of a specific characteristicof the vestigation within the context of advertising show's contentFor example, active movement, research. As- a member of the research staff at animation, letters andscript, reverse motion, and Children's Television Workshop has pointed out visual changes generally tended to elevate attention (Fowles, 1975) Among the auditory characteristics, attention was We can say with great assurance that children 3-10 years old are learning a wide variety of tudreti im.luiltng etaluatronti \ number the ntoid rci_ent skills,concepts,andattitudes from of sci,erai adaptation,. ot 5, wm 'trtr in other Lountrie Appe,tr facts, Sesame Street and The Electric' Company. Since In the J,,u hil Mt,111,11, 011,07197,,jf) 2 46 these programs are not very different from variations in the auditory complexity of commer- television commercials intheir production cials have a greater effect on children's attentional techniques, communications strategies, and at- behavior than variations in visual complexity. In tractiveness to children it would be strange in- another study cited in chapter I, Duffy and Rossiter deed if the learning stopped with a switch of a (1975) found an unexpected inverse relationship channel. between children's observed visual attention to the commercials and their verbal recall of the brand 2. The research designs and measures developed names advertised. Although both studies relied upon and used to examine various effects of program visual measures of attention, their findings suggest material on children may also be appropriate the need to examine children's auditory attention as methods for examining the impact of commercial wel1.10 messages. For example, the flexibility and economy of formative rese,h methods used on earlySesame Rust and Watkins (1975) also ..leasured Street material maywell have come to suit the needs children's visual attention to a series of commer- of those broadcasters and regulators who are respon- cials. A sample of 80 children, 6 to 9 years old, were sible for screening advertising intended for children given the choice of watching either the television and for evaluating its acceptability for broadcast. screen or a simultaneous slide show (a "distractor"). By videotaping the children as they watched the two 3. In the course of studying the extent to which presentations, the researchers were able to deter- children attend to and learn from particular mine what percentage of the sample chose to look at programs, researchers have raised a number of far- each commercial on a moment-to-moment basis. reaching questions about "unintended" effects ot this Their analysis indicated that the average attention material on children. In the summative evaluation of level peaked during the moments chaiacterized by Sesame Street,attempts were made to investigate the most physical action, while low attention occur- viewers' attitudes toward school and relations with red at the more static shots.11 Rust and Watkins peers (Ball and Bogatz, 1970, Bogatz and Ball, followed up the viewing with a group discussion in 1971) Further, the Workshop staff has continued to which information was obtained from the children give attention to the potential teaching value of about their recall of and attitudes toward the com- program features that the producer might think of as mercials. The methodology employed in this study mere incidentals of plot and setting (Fowles, 1975). offers an example of how response measures can he Thus, just as Salomon's work raises questions about fully combined and applied in studying the kinds of "media literacy" skills which may unin- children's reactions to specific commercial mes- tentionally be called upon and developed in child sages. viewers, a parallel set of questions can be raised about the "unintented" effects which exposure to Krugman's work (1968) is noteworthy as a final television advertisements has on children. In fact, example of research on viewers'attentional the issues of deception, fairness, and even safety in responses to particular techniques and properties of children's commercials may all be considered ques- television advertising. According to Krugman, a tions of effects which were "unintended" by the viewer's "direct response" to advertising shot,ld be sponsors and advertising agencies responsible for measured ,not only by visual attention or looking their creation behavior (using eye-movement recording), but by the thinking of th' subject (i e., thoughts which came Studies of advertisingA number ot studies have spontaneously to mind while an ad is being viewed) examined children's visualattentionto commercial and by the subject's feelings (i e., changes in pupil- messages with specific audio-visual features (Atkin, size as a measure of response intensity). Krugman's I975b, Krugman, 1968; Rust and Watkins, 1975, Wartella and Ettema, 1974)

' It(N powhle to make exast Twiner (sal measurements For example, the'India and Ettema study, shildm listeningresponsestotele.isionprogram described in chapter 1,indicated that children's at- soundtrasks(Friedlander and etstone 19'4) tention increased with the auditory complexity of a commercial, regardless of its visual complexity On ' I his is sonsi tent (Aith l_cin and Anderson s (197t (finding that (I, live-1-1()%crilentin the 5, Nam( Sneer material enhanced the basis of their findings, the authors suggested that press hool:m% vt sual attention

47 specific research on these three processes deals with the audio-visual disclaimer reported having heard television and print advertisements intended for (as opposed to having seen) the statement This sug- adult audiences, but the study raises some provoca- gests that hearing the information was more Influen- tive questions for those studying children's responses tial for these children than seeing it. 14 to advertising Krugman examined the manner in which tlis subjects looked at the ach, that are easy to Another study has examined the relationship be- learn, as compared with ads that are difficult to tween how a disclaimer is worded and comprehen- learn. He analyzed the eye,moveuaents of the fespon- Sion (Liebert, Liebert, Sprafkin, and Rubinstein, dents as they were exposed to different ads for 10 1976). Two toy commercials were shown to a group seconds, and then interviewed them for their recall of 240 6 and 8 year oldtin one of three versions: no of the ads. He found that the ads which were scanned disclaimer, a standard disclaimer ("some assembly less were better recalled. On the basis of this evi- required"), or a modified disclaimer ("you have to dence, It may be possible to conclude that there is a put It together"). The children who were exposed to finite amount of information to be learned for each the standard disclaimer demonstrated no better un- advertisment. If so, it should be possible to deter- derstanding that the toy had to be put together than mine how many exposures are required to learn the those who saw the same commercial with no dis- information in a specific ad. This would be useful in claimer. This held true for the older as well as the that almost all of the research on children's learning younger children. However, the children who heard and understanding of television adver ising has been the modified disclaimer showed significantly greater based upon their responses to a single exposure of comprehension of its meaning. The authors conclude the material being studied.12 that this "seems to indicate that wording appropriate to young children plays a crucial part in their ability The more policy relevant issues of how particular to understand the disclaimer content." While the formats contribute to or detract from accurat e per- results of these two studies are hardly surprising, ception and recall of commercial content have been they suggest that refinements of current code provi- addressed by a small but growing number of studies sions on disclaimers are needed to ensure that they achieve their intended purpose. I. The question of whether a particular audio and/or visual featureis peree;ved and remembered by 2. There has been very little in-depth probing of children is examined in a study wh 41 attempted to the question of children'sinterpretationof proc"ict determine how two different techniques for present- information of other commercial content as it is L-a- ing a product disclaimer affected children's aware- veyed by means of specific audio-visual techniques ness and recall of the disclosed information (Atkin, or formats. A noteworthy effort in this direction, 1975b). A sample of 500 preschool and grade- however, is a pilot study in which the researchers ex- school children were exposed to a Mattel "Ver- amined children's perceptions of the meaning of tibird" commercial in which either a video superim- specific verbal slogans and product claims taken posed title -,,r a video title supplemented by an audio from a Christmas toy catalog (Burrs!! and Rossiter, voice-over was used to present a disclaimer of "bat- 1975). The slogans and claims employed various teries not included." When directly asked about rhetorical devices, such as questions ("Who could what is not included when the toy is purchased, resist adorable Ginny?") or qualified superlatives Children exposed to the video-plus-audio version ("the best of its kind"). A small sample of 2d and 4th were more than twice as likely to mention "bat- grade girls with high verbal ability were shown pic- teries" as those viewing the video-only version (43 tures of the products, with a or claim printed percent versus 18 pet cent).1, Further, in followup below each picture. Only one-third of the younger questioning,75 percent of the children who viewed children and two-thirds of the older ones could ac- curately paraphrase the claim. In addition, very few of the children's interpretations of these statements few studies have compared c hildren's responses to single included recognition of the linguistic device versus mdltiple exposures to a particular commercial i Goldberg and Dorn. 1974, Atkin, 1975h) employed. Many children, for example, incorrectly The greater awareness and recall reposed for the audio- visual form of disclaimer otters considerable support for the "As noted previsously, the importantance of auditory factors guideline in the NAB code which specifies that children s adver- in children's attention to and learning from television advertis- tisements shall "pros ide simultaneous audio and visual dis ing deserved further study See Scirramm, 1972, Wartella and Et- closures when items such as batteries 4 rc nut included tema, 1974. Duffy and Rossiter, 1975

4b39 inferred that "the best of its kind" meant "the best," author points out, the research nFeds to be extended instead of Just "among the bestWhile this study and replicated, "the results do suggest that the cereal deals with print-ad information (perceived visually), commercials have the capacity to lead children to as opposed to television advertising (which is per- engage in behavior that increases the risk to their ceived auditorially as well as visually), the applica- physical being "16 tion of psycholinguistic analysis to the study of children's comprehension of verbal product claims 4. Finally, changesinchildren's behavior, suggests a productive area for future research.15 whether undesirable or beneficial, may well be the most compelling evidence of leatning that research 3. The persuasiveness of specific product claims can provide in studying the effects of Commercials. It in a particular audio and/or visual form has been in- is also probably the most difficult to document, and vestigated in a number of studies. In one study, 136 very few studies have assessed the behavioral effects children, 2d, 7th and 8th graders, were exposed to of particular audio-visual techniques or modes of four commercials which had come under the review presentation in children's commercials of the FTC for their use of possibly deceptive prod=' uct displays-or "belief statements" (e.g., "Wonder __Bread is the best thing your mother can give you to In an attempt to use behavior as an outcome,17 grow fast") (liaefner, Leckenby, and Goldman, Atkin (1975b) showed a large sample of preschool 1975). Although comparisons between children's and grade-school children one or two versions of a responses before and after viewing the commercials commercial for a building block game. In one pres- revealed no significant difference& ir. their overall entation, two children were shown constructing a attitudes toward the advertised brands, the children tall elaborate structure with the blocks, in the other, showedincreased acceptance of specific claims a much more modest structure was depicted. The about the product (belief statements) contained in voice-over soundtracks accompanying these two the commercials. Further, the younger children visual presentations were also different. In the more showed greatershiftsinbelief thanthe older elaborate format, for example, the viewer was en- children. couraged to build "a sky-high tower so you can be the champion", the modest version told viewers "it's Another similar study was sponsored by the FTC fun . anyone can play Blockhead." to test whether children's exposure to a series of breakfast cereal commercials could have an adverse Based on observerratings of thechildren's effect (Poulos, 1975). In one of the commercials, the behavior in block play following the viewing, Atkin advertised cereal was associated with wild-growing reported that the children exposed to the "ex- vegetation, in another, an adult is shoiwn picking travagant claim" presentation were more likely than wild berries and other plants while mentioning that those seeing the "modest claim" version to display each is edible In some of the ads, the berries were hostile behavior, in the form of anger, verbal aggres- actually added to the bowl of cereal The Commis- sion, and/or physical aggression (28 percent versus sion was concerned with whether this advertising 18 percent) The children were also asked about the tended to lead children to pick and consume poten- expectations they had for their own performange tially harmful plants. with the game Overall, those children exposed to the "extravagant claim" version were somewhat Four of these commercials were shown to a small sample of 5 to 11 year olds after they had been tested for their beliefs about the edibility of a variety of contrast to the Haetner ct al(1975) investigation of commercial messages, the Messages studied by Poulos (that wild edibleand nonedible plants shown to the children in plants are edible) was not primary to the comineroals Rather, it colored photographs The children were then tested was embedded, both visually and verbally, into the context of the again, and the ratings of edibility increased the most primary message For ihis reason, the Poulos study othYrs a good for the nonedible plants that most closely resembled example 41t what was earlier noted as the potential tin this case, negatlye) teaching value of mtidental messages from television those pictured in the commercials Although, as the material rhis research is noteworthy in as use of multiple response measuresThese include responses observed during viewing Further linguistic analysis of prohlematit verbal- product Ie gvisual attention expressions of irritation and or e loyment, claims can he found in a paper by Rossiter oemrtie .tcria serbalitationsi and the Obtained subsequent to viewo g g in ( finump,i-ary 4(herrtonv (19751 brand name recall, desire for the advertised product I

4149 more likely to estimate that they could build a high- features of commercial messages, we reviewed a er tower than the actors on the commercial (64 per- number of studies that examined the etfects ot these cent versus 55 percent) Further analysis .ndicated features in instructional program materialThese that this greater expectation ot personal perfor- studies of programing etfects have potential applica- mance occurred almost exclusively among the tions for,future advertising research, in terms of both younger viewers ,(77 percent in the "extravagant" their approach to problems (i.e. the questionsraic,-d presentation versus 62 percent in the "modest").18 and the methodologi used) and their substantive findifig. With regard to findings, for example, t-h SUMMARY AND NEEDED RESEARCH evidence of poorerlearning by children when program content was presented in only one modality A limited amount of research has investigated the (either auditory or visual) supports similar findings specific audio-visual properties of television adver- from a study evaluating learning of product infor- tising and their various effects on children. Sev eral mation from different tormat conditions in a com- studies analyzed advertising content, documenting mercial the incidence of particular techniques or features of commercials designed primarily for children. Pro- Several studies have used a variety ot response duction techniques and format features have been measures to investigate the effects of particular found to vary substantially according to product stling:us variables on children's attention to com- classe.g., almost all toy commercials use a live-ac- mercials or, in some studies, children's programs In tion format, whereas advertising for cereals relies one study, for example, an alternative visual -dB- heavily on animation. In a few cases, these analyses tractor" was provided to approximate a natural have also attempted to identify the prevalence of viewing situation. Using such methods, researchers commercial practices which afttcifically required have been able to measure changes in the level of (e.g , positive disclosures) or restricted (e.g., the use children's visual attention as they are exposed to of special effects) by industry codes regulating ad- partlealar audio-visualfeaturesinthe televised vertising to children material. These attentional studies of programs and advertising material have documented the enhanced It would be useful if future studies of this kind visual attention that results trom such features as ac- based thou descriptive categories even more closely tive movement, animation and lively musicall 4 with current industry guidelines. In the case of posi- commonly used in advertising messages tive disclosures, for example, both the incidence of such disclosures ("batteries not included," "items In future studies, these measures of tention sold seperately") and their presentational forms could be refined and expanded For example, eye- (audio and/or video, length of time, placement) need movement records would provide a more precise to be documented. Such an analysis would help to* and informative measure ot visual attention In ad- inform both regulators and advertisers about thp dition, findings from several different studies have ways in which currently imposed gelines are indicated the need to consider auditory as well as being followed. The usefulness of these content visual factors in children's attention to and learning analyses is limited, however, especially in their ab- of television content. Other responses which deserve sence of information about child'ren's own percep- more careful study include the intensity of response tions of these television formats and techniques. Ad- and arousal, verbal comments, expressions ot emo- ditional research is therefore necessary to investigate tion, and nonverbal behavior such as imitation children's perceptions of particular audio-visual features of television advertising and the effects of In research investigating children's learning from these perceptions. television advertising messages, their recall. com- beliefs orattitudes,and Because of the limited amount of research assess- prehension, expressed ing children's responses tospecific audio-visual behavior subsequent to viewing specific commercials have all served as response measures Such studies 1,1 he useot"extravagant-toyproduct displays or have demonstrated that children's recall ot product demonstrationsisspecificallydisallowed by the NAB s disclaimers is influenced by the form in which they guidelines, which state that toadvertisingshall neither exag- are presented For example, a disclosure about bat- gerate nor distort play valueand shall seek "to limit In) demonstration ofa toy's performance to that which a child is teries for a toy was recalled better when presented in reasonably capable id reproducing, both audio and video form, as oppo,ed to video 0 only, recall has also shown to he enhanced when the measured byresearchFutureresearchshould wording of a disclosure was simplified A study of turther explore children's ability to accurately per- print-adproduct claimsrevealedthatchildren ceive and understand commercial information pre- (especially younger ones) finditdifficult to ac- sented by means of specific copy, sound, and visual curately Interpret such common linguistic der ices as techniquesInvestigators should attempt to deter- qualified superlatives mine, for example, whether there are specific presen- tational forms which are more likely to produce ac- Other studies have found that the content of a curate recall and comprehension of such important commercial, whether it is central or incidental to the information as the size and components of a product, commercial's message, may bring about shifts in the way a product works, and a realistic perception children's beliefs.Similarly, the behavior of of its performance. This research should give special children using an advertised product has been found attention to the perceptions of younger children. ---to-vary depending upon the way the product's per- formance is demonstrated in a commercial The cumulative impact Of specific presentational forms in commercials also requires examination. While the evidence from these studies is often For example, what are the effects on children of the preliminary, somewhat scattered, and still unrepli- repeated emphasis on action/performance/speed in cated, their findings verify (1) that specific audio- toy products, or the repeated use of animation and visual features of commercials can make significant fantasy in cereal commercials' What are the more differences in children's learning, and (2) that such long-term effects of commercial techniques and for- ettects on children's learning and behavior can he mats on children's perceptual and cognitive skills9

v

51 42 Chapter 3

SOURCE EFFECTS AND SELF-CONCEPT APPEALS IN CHILDREN'S TELEVISION ADVERTISING

Characters of sarious kindshuman and nonhu- 4 Certain types of advertising appeals, usual) man, I.% e- ietion or animatedappear as product but not always involving product presenters or presentees or product users in most children's com- users, may deleteriously affect a child's self- mercials I heirassociation %kiththe advertised concept We call this the seit-concept effect pioduct ranges from Met e) a cinncidental presence Personal enhancement appeals, social status inthe :ononercialtoopen endorsements and appeals, exaggerated or unrealistic product testimonialsfThe term "source Meet- refers to the usage portrayals, and competitive product ap- impact of these LharaLters on the television au- pealN tall into this broad category dience CURRENT CODES Criticsottelevisionadvertisingforchildren allege that the use ot such characters in commeroals This section reviews what current NAB and NAD creates source ettects tha; take unfair advantage ot self- regulatory codes allow and prohibit regarding the young viewer-I he following listing detines a source factors and self-concept appeals in television number of these alleged negato, source effects advertising directed at children

I Certain types of characters in commercials. I Contusion notably program personalities and cartoon characters, mal, contribute to chndren's contu- The NAB does not spei:ify any potential source sion between programs and advertisements. effects in its code The NAL) does specifically refer Vve have termed this the onlusion elle( tThis to potential contusion between program content and Lontusion effect may he heightened when the advertising contentThe NAB code, identical in program personalities and cartoon characters effect to that of the NAD, states appear in commercials shown v.ithrn or adja- cent to their osn programs We call this the ad- No children's program personality or cartoon

tat emVtile( character shall be utilized to deliver commer- cial messages within or adjacent to the figures. such as a 2 Celebrities or authority programs in which such a personality or car- policeman, an astronaut, or a mother, can lead toon character regularly appears. This provi- Children to attribute 'o the endorsed product sion shallalso apply to lead-ins to com- qualities it does not have We call this the en- mercials dorsement 01(11 Ihe endorsement effect may he heightened when the celebrity or authority Note that the cid/them v elle( t actually provides the figure openly endorses or uses the product basis for the NAB and NAD codes on program per- the threu endorsement etteuand may he sonalities and cartoon characters reduced when the celebrity or authority figure merely appears in the commercial without en- 2f-_ndorsement Ole( I dorsing or using the product in the indirect en- dorsement cite( I The endorsement effect may The NAD code allows all types of endorsers buft- also he reduced or eliminated if the celebrity seems to prohibit the endorsement ttfect itself or authority figure does not appear in real life, but is either acted or animated We call this the Advertisement should not falsely imply that /tunas v et/L.( purchase and use of a product or service will 3 Certain characteristiLs of product presenters or confer upon the user, the prestige, skills, or usersnotably their sex, race, , or !The relationship ot source cttects to selt-concept effects social behavior, can contribute to children might he expressed in terms ot who' presents the product ver- learning social stereotypes We call this the sus "how" it is presented Other presentation issues are discussed wt nit steronvn ette( t in chapter 2 of this report

43 52 other special qualities tit characters appearing Advertisments shall not portray attitudes and in the commercial or ad Material henettts at- practices inconsistent with generally recog- tributed to the product or sere ice should he in- nized social values and customs.' herent in the use thereof The NAD's guidelines, section C, proVide a much more detailed lis,ing of the criteria which social I he NAB code, on the other hand, apparently portrayals in commercials should meet In addition tries to minimize the endorsement ettect hv placing to social stereotyping, the NAD prohibits unaccept- restrictions on the type ot endorser and the type ot able reflections ot social, legal, moral, institutional, endorsementapparently based on the assumption or tamiiy values, disdain for parents and other that some types of endorsers are more effective than sources ot child guidance, undesirable liking habits others The NAB's prohibitions: look simple but and manners. and poor (other than 'Mound') use of are, in tact, quite complicated They ai e summarized language in table 3 -I 4Self-concept Effect

3 Sot la! Stereotype Lt!e t Various advertising practices may reflect negatively on a child's self- concept. Four of the most Inits preamble, the NAB code recognizes that prevalent self- concept concerns are covered in the "advertising . can serve toinform children of codes many aspects of the society and world in which they live Social stereotypes are not explicitly men- Personal enhancement appeals Both the NAB and tioned. The code simply states that the NAD allow personal enhancement appeals which promise such benefits as strength, growth,

Table 3-1

NAB Restrictions on Endorsements

Human, Human, actor/actress real-life portrayals Animated

Celebrities (a famous person or character) Indirect only Allowed** Allowed Authority figures (e.g , athlete, mother, astronaut, Indirect only* Allowed** Allowed policeman) Product characters (identified with or "created for" the Not Allowed Not Allowed Allowed product) All others Allowed Allowed Allowed

*indirect means that this type of endorser can appear in commercials but must not verbally endorse the product nor be shown using or consuming it This relates to the direct endorsement effect issue already referred to The tact that animated characters are allowed in all cases and actor/actress portrayals in some, relates to the famasy effect issue

Iwo otherspe,ihssrnrelfaLtors ate mentioned NAB prohibits use ofre.rli.iissar atmospherein duidren s Lom- meroals pUrtraals whisk ue,gesi rewrnmend hildren sI A Adsrusing, si,t1,,ns IJ and IIsaka, ()ilk,. di,,riminate and or ifinn,,derate useof produLts such ,ts snaLks, tions and AnswersIa 11,11,, I 19's 5ends guru and soh drinks

44 physical proficiency, and intelligenceprovided should be nonderreaning to competitors' prod- they are "constructively handled- and "ac- ucts, should not involve comparisons with pre- curately reflect documented evidence" (to use the vious versions of the same product, must be ade- NAB's words) and "do not mislead" (to quote the quately documented, and must not be based on a NAD guidelines) biased selection of comparative attributes Addi- tionally, the NAD appears to prohibit subjective Social status appeals Sozial status appeals, usually superiority claims or "puffery peer status appeals, are prohibited by both the NAB and the NAD The NAB code states that INCIDENCE Appeals shall not be used which directly or by implication contend that if children have Incidence refers to the prevalence of commercial a product they are better than their peers or practices relating to potential source effects and self- lackingitwillnot he accepted by their concept appeals, not to the incidence of effects per peers se However, to maintain continuity, the headings in this section retain the word "effect- when, more cor- The NAD's provision is similarly worded and rectly, they should be "potential for effect identical in effect 1, Confusion hilt -t Product usaKe portrayals Product usage portrayals might presumably affect a child's self-concept if Content analyses by Barcus are very helpful in the child cannot duplicate the portrayalThe that they provije evidence on prograth personality NAB and the NAD have similar codes ..overing and cartoon endorsements as well as'incidence esti- usage portrayals except that the NAB refers to mates on the adjacency phenomenon Adjacent en- toys and premium offers whereas the NAD refers dorsemtnts (either within or contiguous to the en- to all products advertised to children Both codes dorser's regular program )were negligible at the prohibit exa4gerated portrayals of play value or most recent count in early 1975 none on weekend performance characteristics, usage demonstra- tions which a "a child" (NAB) or an "average mornings and 1 percent during weekday afternoons ,Vonadiacent endorsem nts were recorded at an inci- child- (NAD) isnotreasonably capable of dence of about 6 percent (7 percent on weekends, reproducing, settings and contexts which a child and 5 percent on weekdays) However, this figu:e cannot reasonably reproduce, and portrayals of probably overestimated the use of program per- children possessing unfair or inequitable rumbers sonalities and cartoon characters since "endorsers" of pr Jucts or premiums in the Barcus analyses included celebrities Competitive product appeal,The NAB code is (specifically "sports figures" and "figures in the clearly against competitive product appeals. news"). Even so, the incidence of nonadjacent en- allegedly because of apotentialself-concept dorsements by program personalities and cartoon effect The code prohibits both overt competitive characters seems quite low considering that this is a app Is(comparison advertising) andcovert permissible practice. competitive appeals (superiority claims)

Positive exposition of a product's on at- 2 Lndorsement Effect tributes are acceptable (sic)However, because of their potential to encourage dis- The endorsement effect reEites to the type of en- satisfaction on achild'spart,competi- dorsers and the type of endorsement that each is per- tive comparison superiority claims or tech mitted to make Again, Barcus provides the most re- niques are disallowed cent incidence data, except that his content catego- ries do not correspond to those specified by the NAB The NAD also discourages Lompetitixe ap- code An analysis of 1971 commercial content by peals but allows them in cases where a "true and Winick et al. (1973) is of some help in that practices significant advantage may existthat can he which are not affected by (Ai& changes can he readily understood bychildren Such claims roughly estimated from thisk idler study From

r- 45.) s. these unmated breakdowns, we can draw a number 3, Social Stereotype Lllect of tentative conclusions about endorsement The potential for social stereotype ell, cts lies in Probably g0 percent of children's commercials the presence of various social cues in commercials employ- an on-camera presenter 4Inother seen by children The following figures are based on words, only about 20 percent portray no visi- commercials shown for the most -)ait during ble source, although, of course, auditory cues programs designed for children on weekday after- et -114 present noons and weekend mornings No data,are available that reflect the incidence of social cues in other com- lent visible product presenters in The mop mercials that childi en may see commerciats directed to childreH are unmixed characters (4., percent), followed by anonymous Ak,e of product presenters The tollowing table is human adult or child actors and .i.tresses (32 based on a simple average of Barcus' weekday after- percent) Cartoon program characters selling noon and weekday morning content anal} si,in products in commercials that are not adjacent early 1975 to their on programs account for the next largest frequency (4 per,:.t) Table 3-2

t hereisa 2 percent incidence of real-life celebrity endorsements, even though this prac- Spokespersons charactrda tice is now prohibited by the NAB ,:ode Ap- parently, not all advertisers are observing tl.is Adults on-camera 62°/0 33°,'0 prohibition c off-camera 15°0 Virtuallyallchildren's commercials would Teenagers* 16% 6% qualify as direct endorsements under the NAB's Children 7 550 definition. Besides direct (an esti- For some reason Barcus coded animals in this cate- mated 20 percent), there are on-camera gory as well Thus, this figure representsall others usage/consumpt,on portrayals (an estimated 60 besides adults and children percent 1plus off-camera verbal statements where only the product is shown (an estimated If we focus only on y Bible age cues, and ignore the 20 percent) 6 Although these are all different teenage incidence,itmay he Loncludcd that degrees of endorsement, they are all direct en- children, overall, see about one and a hail limo as dorsemtnts under the NAB code many adults as children in commercials directed at them However, they see about mitt ante's as marry adults as children acting "s sp,kcspersons or endors- ers

,Readers who shesis Barsus1197Sa, h) will find a %el.\ high Set of prodia t preservers Again this table I, based ins dense (about 41 pertem of iitt-stage anruntn. H.V.C,er on an average of figures in Barcus (1975a hi this does rs,,t preclude the a Iditinnal presence of an on Lames 1 presenter The KO per,eqt figure is estimated from the older Table 3-3 Vs, %nick et11 1.1971) data but is likels to he LurrenIty applicable since there nase been no Lode changes attesting the practice

`An astisle 1,11t 4v, September 11, 1976, ipostdat Other mg the September 1 1976 NAB ban) rt, orts that SL' aper Spokespersons characters ,anu turing (n and the Muller Jordan Herrick adsertising Male 90% 67% agensy will he using Ale, k,rrasa t.elebrits and, as an athlete Female 10% 33% prohahly also an authority figure by s AB definitionsto pro a-hireits Super bias'lineofchildren'saLtiotitosstin children s showssports programsin prime time network shows Overall, children see at least three times a man ^Estimated trout 116ures pro, 'Jed 1hinisk et al i1971 males as females in commercials directed at ,acm iillectLA1 in 191, these 111:1\ he Y/111CA11,11 nat.Lurate but are at< .r spokespersons or endorsers, the law) is rune ledieS1 asaiihie estimate, '16 addiiiiin to %colons Iand 1 K a the NAP s ( hildren tiies as nia-v males as tem Some intersting

TV Adsertising Li de of September 1 1976 see !si \Bs Ques results on sex cues comecan a content analysis of thins and Answeis ( lanuars I 197s 1974 children's Saturday morung commercials by

46 r L) McArthur and Eisen (n d ). Including only commer- qu atly rewarded for their actionseven thoiigh the cials in which human males or females appeared as rewards are usually within the context of ,tereotyped central characters, these investigators found, predic- "housewife'' or "mother" roles tably, that :nales were portrayed mainly in authority Race of prodit(t petsenters Although there are no roles whereas females were portrayed primarily as data available ter the period, it is likely that. pr.ur -passive" product users However, although many to thelate1960's children's commercials Strt' more males than females were shown (80 percent vs almost exclusively populated by white characters 20 percent), both sexes appeared equallyas human or animated Although the figrres may vary "reward-givPrs- and "reward-getters As in reliability, itis interesting to note the trend ot McArthur and Eisen commented, advertising may ra..nal characteristics in children's commercials since he the one media area in which women are fre- then Table 3-4

Data Percent of commercials* Study collected in which nonwhites appeared Barous (1971) 1971 27% `Winick et al. (1973, 1971 24% Atkin & Heald (in press) 1972 18°0 Atkin & Heald (in press) 1973 24%, Doolittle & Pepper (1975) 1974 17°0

Most of the studies focused on Saturday morning commercials

Intherr istrecent content ana:yses (Barons (At upatton ot prothi(t presenter, Recent specific 1975a. b). with 1975 data, a much lower -Amy, hite data on ot.c.)oations d,.-sicted children's commer incidence was obtained vials are n vailal leIn a study using 1971 data, Table 3-5 Win:ck ei (1973; reported that occupational cues were detectable in 38 percent ot children \ .aturday Weekend Weekday morningsafternoons morning commercials and that a diversity ot blue collar and white collar occupations was portrayed 95% Whites 92% Occupations shown in children's onmerc:als are Blacks 3% 7% most often discussed in the tontext ot set rues- - Other /0.5 /0 5 notably the roles played by women Available con- tent analyses indicate that lust a; women are under- The explanation for BarLus lower figures is that his represented in children's commercials, th 1975 figures are based on the percentage ot total . occupa- tional roles are also underreptesented in the media characters rather than on a percentage ot total com- ingeneral, women are most utten portrayedin mercials While nonwhite characters appear in di.!t domestic roles ;Busby 1975), althoughthisis 20 percent of children's commercials, they appear in16,hit. alone in only about 2 percent (Atkin & Heald, in 6 according to an Inv estigation by the Na- tional Advertising Rey iew Board (Adlernstng Age, press)Typically, the practice is to add nonwhite April 21, 1975) Children s commercials almost ex- characters to depict an "integrated" racial setting clusively portray women in a single role, "house- Atkin,. and Heald 's statistics of 11;72-73 document wife," versus 17 occupational roles shown for men thi,. phenomenon (Busby. 1975. Verna. 1974) This finding may be Table 3-6 misleading. however The label might lust as often be 'mother,a role we might expect to he dominant Average PO. of Average no. of in commercials dir .cted at children Data characters in characters in col- all-white commercials which .S0( Jai beltai !or of prod lit I pros. Wet According 10 lected commercials contain nonwhites theBarousstudies corniner,:i.ilsshown during 1972 315 491 weekend mornings and weekday atternoons d is- 1973 270 333 n'ayeda variety of settings for exhihiting social behat !orin or around the home (30 percent), out- Social status appealsBarcus comes closest to door settings (29 percent), public places (15 per- measuring the incidence of s^cial status appeals with cent), and plates of work (4 percent) 8 However, no his Peer Status/Popularity code. The averaged inci- analysts hasyetact .mpted to evaluate whether dence for this type of claim was 2 percent. A 1975 behat ior depicted in children's commercials is in ac- example would be the commercial in which a young Lord with "recognized" or "accepted" social values girl, upon procuring a bland of fried chicken, is and trustoms, as required by the NAB and NAD allowed into a boys' play club This practice is codes prohibited by the NAB and NAD codes

4 .St /Mei t Product usage portrayals No data are available on the extent to which children's commercials exagger- Four types of claims were idtnoto Hier as ate product ownership, product performance, or has mg a potential hearing on children s sell-con- play settings beyond a degree reproducible by the cepts Barcus (1975a, h) ;'gain provides the most re- '',.average" child cent and specific incidence figures Competitive product appeals Overt comparisons Personal einem appealsTable 3-7 uses between competing brands or models of a product Barcus' coding ,ategoriesthree of which relate "comparative advertising" in adult L.Immercial ter- quite closels to personal enhancemer' claims by ad- minology--are appaiently negligible in children's s ertiscrs commercialsBarcus' nearest category is entitled Comparative/Associative but this appears to refer to Table 3-7 similes and metaphors (e g., "casts as good as gold") In any case, the incidence for tnis category was only Incidence 1 percent. Covert compa:isonssuperiority or uniqueness Fun/Happiness 7% claimswere more prevalentUniqueness claims Fun taste, fun to eat, lots of laughs, g , "the one and only ..") were recorded at 4 per- cup of happiness centSuperiority claims, which almost exclusively involved subjective superlatives or "puffery" (e.g , Health /Nutrition /Well -being 13 the greatest, best, fantastic) rather than objective Low fatgood foryou, balanced compt nive claims le g , best selling) were recorded breakfast, look thinner at 7 per,.. Thus, the estimated total for this prac- tice, which is prohibited by the NAB, is 11 percent Action/Strength/Speed/Power RESEARCH EVIDENCE (no defining examples)

I (Ontusum Effect Adventure I his effect alleges that the use of program per- An adventure in every bite sonalities and cartoon characters in commercials Total 27% contributes to children's confusion between program and advertising content 9 Even by 5 or 6 years of age, approximately 25 percent of all child viewers In ,ill, ipprosimatcly one in tom children's commer- still experience difficultyin separating programs .ials cart ies lea.... an implicit promise of personal from commercials (see chapter 1 of this report) enhancement or benefit from product consumption or useIt should he !bud that dot I/mewed claims of thir example recent ,011%t nt o der In. the I-I(against this type are allowed by industry Lodes Hudson Pharmaceutical Corporation allege, that comme, tats which use'wick r Matta character who appears, asSpide, on I he Llectr,( ompam,chiudrer.s program'has the ten- 1111,,th;ut, if, te,,1'tomliar, us tI `-i'c h)ail dency and capacits to hlur for children the distraction hetwcen 114, Bor,u,iii ,, i, p i,unded to the piogram content and athertising f i deralI rade C'ornmission r (,n sent Ordtr File do 762 los-it r 48') Does the use of program characters contribute to program groups paid slightly more attention to the this discrimination difficulty Only one published Flintstones commercial than those in the control study has examined this question (Atkin, 197 5b) group, they also tended to want the advertised cereal The study also attempted to test the adjacency effro, significantly more oftenThese results, incidently, which hypothesizes that. the confusion effect can be were stronger in the nonadjacency group. This casts reduced or eliminated if the program characters a: e further doubt on the implicit NABNAD hypothesis used "outside" rather than within or adjacent to that nonadjacent commercials of this type are less their own programs The NAB and NAD codes are efficacious than adjacent or in-program commer- based on the adjacency hypothesis. cials

Atkm's study used three test conditions One A 1975 study by Donohue should also be men- group of children saw a commercial for "Pebbles" tioned In a survey sample of 162 6- to 9-year-old cereal, featuring the Flintstones cartoon program, black children of low socioeconomic status in New we call this the "adjacency group," A second group Orleans, Donohue found that 27 percent of boys and saw the same commercial and the same cartoon 42 percent of girls claimed that "their favorite TV program but in a noncontiguous sequence---this is character" appears in commercials The two favorite the "nonadjacency groupA third group saw the characters in commercials were Bugs Bunny and Flintstones commercial during a Bugs Bunny car- Fred Fltntstone. However, these figures are likely to toon program, we call this the "control group he overestimates in thatthey do not measure the proportion of commercials in which the characters "Confusion" was measured by asking the children appeared (the children may have been reporting whether they remembered seeing the Flintstones about relatively few commercials), they are self- characters eating cereal and, if so, where(The reports and thus depend onthereliabilityof characters ate cereal only in the commercial, not the children's memories, and the sample's and the cartoon program.) Evidence for the confusion effect region's viewing content may he somewhat atypical was obtained in that one-fourth of the children who in terms of NAB code-covered stations. The Barcus recalled the Flintstones characterS eating cereal figures should he taken as mole accurate thought they did so in the program. This error was most prevalent among the younger children in the 2 Endorsement Eltect sample 13- to 7-year-olds)However. the overall magnitude of the findings was diminished somewhat The endorsement issue involves not one but three by the fact 'that significantly.fewer children in the alleged effects the direct endorsement ettcct, the in- Flintstones program groups (compared to the con- direct endorsement effect. and the fantasy effect. trol group) recalled the cereal-eating event in the first placeSince we have no measure of "spon- Direct endorsement effect The allegation here is taneous" confusion, we have to rely on "recalled" that certain types of product presenters can lead eonfusion e ,the percentage recalling the event childrentoattributetothe endorsed product multiplied by the percentage confused)By this qualities it does not have The recent FTC consent weighted measure, the confusion effect results are order against "Spider-Man" Vitamins, for instance,

equivocal since the confusion trends are offset by states that "The use of such a hero figure . has the lower recall of the confusion tendency and capacity to lead significant numbers of children to believe that the endorsed product has Atkin's study also did not support the culta«licr qualities and characteristicsitdoes not have effect hypothesis In fact, the "rionadjacency group" (Federal Trade Commission Consent Order File No. experienced slightly more confusion than the "adja- 762 3054) cency group In sum, Atkin's comparison group produced equivocal results for the confusion effect Endorsements by real-lite celebrities and and at the same time no sum- rt for the adjacency authority figures are prohibited by the NAB for effect. commercials appearing inchildren's programs 10 Ironically, the single published study dealing with There were other findings in the Atkin study this phenomenon (Atkin, 1975h) seems to suggest which suggested consequences other than the confu- sion effect For example, children in the Flintstones 'Other than thenit re appe,.trart,e' possibil its see table 3 -I

49 30

..1. that real-life celebrities and authority figures doitot passive (indirect) forms of endorsement any less produce an endorsement effect. In this study, an effective than direct endorsements and outright astronaut character was used as a "hero figure" en- testimonials? Since there are no studies available dorser iI and compared with a "man in the street" that compare different degi ees of "directness" of en- endorser Atkin's summary of the study implies that dcrsement, we can only compare across studies. the astronaut figure was slightly less effective in in- In a pilot study using print ads, Hyams, Tanner, ducing children's desire for the cookies advertised. and Rossiter (1975) found that a black athlete en- However, careful reading of the study shows that the endorsement effect depended on whether or not the dorseran authority figure by NAB definition produced a markedly greater liking for the endorsed children liked astronauts When subgroup analyses were conducted the following results were revealed producta fictitious brand of sodathan either a black businessman endorser of a white businessman Table 3-8 endorser. The black athlete advertisement used no direct endorsement whatsoeveronly a picture of Desire for the athlete juxtaposed with a picture of the product. the The endorsement effect, incidentally, was stronger advertised among the older children in the sample (5th graders) product than the younger children (2d graders) Dramatic Experimental group: source effects have been obtained in other studies Children who liked astronauts with an equally indirect product-endorser relation- " much" or "very much" (60 ship. percent of group) 32% Perhaps the most straightforward illustration of Children who liked astronauts "not the indirect endorsement effect comes from a plis.,t so much" (40% of group) 22% study by Iskoe (1976) In this study, Iskoe picked five pairs of similar products and randomly assigned Control group: five endorsers to them Four of the endorsers were "Man in street" version 29% celebrities and some were also authority figures as defined by the NAB. The fifth was an "ordinary The results indicate a negative endorsement effect male Independent ratings of children's product among a substantial subgroup of children and no sig- preference were made first An endorser was then nificant positive endorsement effect among the re- "attached" to each product and the products re- mainder rated The data in the following table are paired comparison prefe'rences in which the children were Of course, this is only one study Che commercial required to select one item from each pair. The sam- may have been atypical of endorsement commer- ple comprised 225 children across 1st. 3d. and 5th cials There is little doubt that endorsement effects grades, with both sexes and also race represen- existHowever, littleis known about the source tatively included characteristics and processes that produce them Nor Several conclusions are apparent from this study have effects or outcomes other than "product liking" First, silent endorsement clearly works: Increases in been studied. Proposals for obtaining This informa-,, product preference of up to 67 percent were ob- tion are considered in the Needed Research section of this chapter. tained purely on the basis of a product-endorser jux- taposition. Second, although interaction effects are Indirect endorsement effect The NAB code does likely between various endorsers and products, not all endorsers are equally effective. Mohammed Ali not prevent real-life celebrit:es or authority figures from appearing in commercials directed to children, seems muck more effectivethan, for example, Lucille Ball, who as "Lucy" actually produced a elthough the code does prevent these figures from verbally endorsing the product and from being negative effect in one instance On the other hand, shown using or consuming it. The question is. are the "Ordinary male" enchrser was just as effective as the average of the celebrities (22 5 percent vs. 19.6 percent) This reinforces the point made in connec- 11The endorser, who was identified as "Astronaut Alan Col- tion with Atkin's "astronaut study" earlier. that en- lins," was presumably a celebrity and an authority figure since "astronaut" is one of the roles c..ed by the NAB as exemplifying dorsement effects depend on how children regard an authority figure in children's advertising, see table the endorser.

50 Table 3-9 Endorsor and product pair Unendorsed Endorsed Difference* preference preference Mohammed All

Frisbee 36% 60% +67% , Beachball 64% 75% +17% President Ford Cookies 56% 68% +21% Donuts 44% 57% +30% Captain Kangaroo Gloves 49% 55% +11% Ski cap 51% 59% +16% Lucille Ball Doll 45% 45% 0% Gun 55% 52% -5% Ordinary male Potato chips 55% 67% +22% Pretzels 45% 56% +23%

'Difference = (Endorsed Pref Unendorsed Pref 100)%

In sum, although no research has been done which authority figures. But none of these studies I' as com- actually compares direct with indirect endorse- pared the effectiveness of farasy portrayals vs real- ments, separate studies show that both can produce I ite endorsers. Therefore, we shall devote considera- significant effects.Itisnot known whether the ble attention to suggested future research on the fan- NAB's prohibition of direct endorsement tasy effect in the Neec:ed Research section of this (testimonials or overt use/consumption) reduces en- chapter. dorsement effects by celebrities or authority figures, but it certainly does not remove these effects. 3.Social Stereotype Effect.

Fantasy effectImplied in the NAB code is a sec- The development of socially stere&.yped percep- ond means of reducing or eliminating the endorse- tions in children is undoubtedly a long, cumulative ment effectthe fantasy effect Animated characters process. The incidence figures for social cues associ- are universally allowed as presenters; they !lay ated with characters in children's commercials dealt portray celebrities or suthority figures, and mayprimarily withratios.young-old, male-female, or serve as specially created "product characters." Also black-white. In the general context of television in keepingwiththe fantasy idea, actors and actressesviewing, it has been shown that ratios of violence are allowed to portray celebrities and authority and the social characteristics of aggressors and vic- figures so long as their portrayals are recognizable tims can affect adult viewers' perceptions of ,society by children as "slice of life." (Presumably this means (Gerbner and Gross, 1975). We might expect similar that children see acted portrayals as being of a fan- effects from commercials, although itwould be tasized nature.) The NAD code does not contain any difficult to separate them from the effects of televi- fantasy effect assumption; the NAB's current code, sion programing as a whole. however, virtuallydependson it. We might nevertheless ask whether the social No published studies exist which specifically test characteristics of commercials produced any short- the fantasy s,ffectThe Flintstones study (Atkin,term effects regardless of any long-term conse- 1975a) employed animated cartoon characters and quences. In particular, by using certain characters as other studies (e.g., Hyams et al,1975; Robertson et presenters or product users, commercials may take al., 1975) have used actors and actresses to portray advantage ofexistingsocial stereotypes to produce

516 0 endorsement effects,leto imbue the product with significant at the 01 levelIn sum, the studies indi- qualities or characteristics it would not otherwise cate that adults are more effective than children as have Thus, children may find models in commer- presentersespeciallyif they are shown with cials credible, attractive, or persuasive solelyon\he children as a Joint endorsement basis of their social characteristics, quite apart from any celebrity, authority, or other special status Setties The endorsement effect question in rela- characteristics they may possess There is considera- tion to sex cues is whether males or females are more ble evidence that this type of "social endorsement" effective presenters of products in children's adver- effect exists We will examine this evidence in terms tising Empirical research on sex cues has typically of the most salient social cues among characters in employed child rather than adult models, although children's commercials age, sex. race. occupation, children are atypical as presenters (see table 3-2 ancl and social behavior 3-3)

Age cues Most of the concern about age cues has The single study that has employed adult models centered on the issue of adult vs child presenters in research on sex roles in children's televisionad- thisissuemay looklikeanauthorityfigure vertising was conducted in the Atkin series ( I975c). phenomenon but, as seen in table I, only particular In this study Atkin was interested in the auditory sex adult roles are seen as authoritative by the NAB cues provided by oft-camera announcers.';In- Thus there is a separate adult vs child issue 12 terestingly, only 47 percent of the children in the Atkin study correctly recalled that a woman's voice The overwhelming usage of adult presenters sug- had been used in the version of the commercial using gests that adults are more effective presenters than a female announcer. recall did not differ by sex of children But there is a qualification A study by viewer Older children were much more often cor- Robertson, Rossiter, and Brenner (1975) utilized a rect than younger children. perhaps reflecting in- child endorser, a parent endorser, and a parentlus- creasing sex-role awareness Overall, use of a female child endorsement Based on ratings of affect (lik- announcer made no difference _Is no recall of the ing) toward the endorse,' product, the endorsement commercial, belief in the advertised claim, or desire effects were exactly in that order- most effective was to use the product. By subgroups, buys were some- the parent-plus-child endorsement. The difference what more influenced by the female announcer, as between the child endorser and parent-plus-child were older childrenHowever, there was no endorsements was significant at the 05 level, with difference in response between the children who younger children ( 7- to 9-year-olds) somewhat more recalled a female voice and those who did not recall influenced by the parental inclusions than older (10- the announcer's sex correctly to 12-year-old) children. The Robertson et alstudy was conducted with a mixed-race, mi%ed-sex sam- The apparent implications of this study are either ple The finding of parent-child superiority was that the sex of an off-camera announcer makes no replicated in a followup study by Gardner (1975) differenceor that a female announcerissuffi- with an all-white. mixed-sex samplewith results ciently novel to make her Just as effective as a male announcer But there is a third possibilityAtkin

'= interestingly enough the NAB is concerned about therole used a Bufferin commercial which was probably of children in adult commercials NAB apparently feelsthat fairly familiar to the children Recall of the com- dominant use of children in commercials ostensihlv intendedfor mercial, belief in the claim "But-term works faster adults may redefine the attual target ot the commercials as heing than plain aspirin," and desire to take the product intended for childrenIn ads ertising of any product designed may well have neen established prior to the test. for and directed to adults a commercial is exempt from the pro Ausertisinj Guidelines even if it is Thus the experiment may imply nothing more about v [shins of the Cnildren 1 shown curing or adtacent to children s programing and it sex cues than that boys and girls are equallylikely to hear them Any use ot a child is limited to a real-lite situation and, it the child is used as other than incidental, hackground character such useisconfined to a situation which the parent adult-childrelationshipisestahlis'J and the parent adult remains the dominant character ' 'fiarcus 1197Sa hi found that an d'a rage ot 41 percent of children s commercials use an ott stage %OA! Although our esti- ( hildren s rk Athertistng mates are that only 20 percent use soil is an oft stage totice with 'section III2 no on stage presenter G1 52 A similar drawback occurred in a second study by Race cues The evidence on endorsement effects Atkin ( I975c), which used boy and girl models en- due to racial characteristics of product endorsers is dorsing an established producta Tyco race car set. complicated Four published studies have tested the Only 41 percent of the children in the girl endorser effects of black and white endorsers with black and version recalled the model's'4 sex accurately; again white child samples 16 All show a tendency for this did not differ by sex of viewer Female endorser awareness of racial cues to increase with age; young effects were found only among those children who children seem little affected by racial differences. had perceived the female endorsementThis subgroup was much more likely than the "nonper- The complication in the race cue studies lies with ceiver" subgroup to approve of girls playing with older (1(- to 12-year-old) children Robertson et al, race car sets However, their own desire for the toy (1975) found a tendency for children of this age to was only minimally affected, with boys wanting it be influenced by an "own race" effect although 'his much more than girls regardless of the commercial trend was notstatisticallysignificant. Gardner manipulation (1975), using a white sample only, also found no sig- nificant "own race" endorsement effect. However, Less equivocal sex cue results were obtained in Hyams, Tanner, and Rossiter (1975) found very the Robertson et at study (1975) and the Gardner strong "own race'effects for older children, par- (1975) replication The products, fictitious brands ticularly black children,'' and Atkin ( I 975b) found of cereal, were unknown to the children, so pre- an apparently significant "cross-race" effectfor viously established attitudes were not a confounding older black children, but no effect for older white tactor Across the total sample there were no signifi- children cant sex cue effectsHowever, subgroup analyses provided a different picture in both studies, older What should be concluded from these studies? boys were more influenced by male endorsers (n.s at First, it is clear that race cue endorsement effects 1st grade. .05 at 3d and 01 at 5th). Girls showed a again independent of the product's own charac- similar trend in r-;ponse to female endorsers, but terisiticscan occur Second, whether these effects this trend did not reach statistical significance, i e , occur depends critically on the prior socialization of male models vere equally effective with girls Two the child. Older children seem more liable to be qualifications should be added to these results. A affected by racial cues in commercials, but how they pilot study by Sobel and Rossiter (1975) did show a are affected almost undoubtedly depends on their marked "own sex" trend for girls." The stuck also own socialization experiences with regard to race. It included a third sex cue condition. boy-plus-gi., en- is noteworthy, for example. that the studies cited dorsement This turned out to be equally as effective here drew their samples from schools with quite as male-only endorsement with older boys but not ..t different interracial histories, which may account for all appealing to girls their discrepant findings

Taken as a whole, these studies suggest that sex Occupational cues As noted, occupational cues cue endorsement canbeeffectiveinchanging .4..2 probably present in about 40 percent of commer- children's liking for productsquite apart from any cials intended for children Evidence on long-term intrinsic characteristics the product may possess stereotyping effects attributable to commercials is although the magnitude of the effect depends on almost impossible to obtain, but in the short term, prior sex-typing of the product. Sex cue endorse- occupational cues undoubtedly have authoritative ment seems to be more effective with older children, or status oriented dimensions which make them a probably because of preexisting sex-role awareness special subcategory of endorsement effects Note the Finally, findings lean toward an "own sex" effeCt in earlier reference to the study by H yams et al. (1975) general, but the evidence is not conclusive on this which demonstrated that the occupation of "athlete" point

',Actually models sex Atkin used two boss and two girls in 'One of these studies was not sufticientls well controlled to each commerc.al to minimize idiossncratic cues he assessed " i he termmarked trend- is used hecausthese data were This ,vas the only studs to include an integrated setting not tested for statistical sinificance The magnitude of the Older children of both races in this sample preferred this setting effect, however was greater than the significant differences in least although their response to 3cn,ss-sez" setting was only the Robert on et at and Oardner studies slightly less negatise in an endorser role is more effective with children repair is seen as a low status job for persons of either than the occupation of businessnian sex.

However, the principal. controversy surrounding The most intriguing result of this experiment was occupational cues in commercials involves their in- the apparent strength of a single "learning trial" in teraction with sex cues Most often the controversy the face of considerable previous learning. The centers on the occupational stereotyping of women change effects, incidently, were stronger among the Content analysts imply that women in commercials older and presumably more role-socialized are often stereotyped as "housewives although as, children A deMyed followup measure of occupa- we already commented, the women might also he tional suitability would have been an interesting ad- design4ed as "mothers.- In either case, the occupa- dition to the experiment tions portrayed oy women are clearly limited

Although the development of social stereotypes is Itis tempting to conclude from the counter- presumably a long process. a very interesting experi- stereotyping effects in the Atkin experiment that ment by Atkin (1975c) suggests that certain commercia., must also produce stereotyping effects, stereotypes of women's occupations may be but such a Conclusion is not logic ally warranted reversed, at least temporarily, in a single trial. In However, single commercials are clearly capable of this exile. iment, Atkin prepared three versions of a affecting children's short-term social beliefs. This is commercial in which a 35-year-old woman was an unusual finding given that such effects were shown discussing the occupation-related advantages popularly supposed to result 4inly from multiple ex- of eyeglasses. In one version, the woman portrayed posures. the role of a judge, in another, the role of a computer programer, and in the third, the role of a television, Soc lul Mll:tor The final subcategory of social technician repairing a TV setSeparate groups of stereotype effects concerns social behavior which ac- children saw each version, and a fourth group. which cording to the NAB code must not be portrayed as did not see the commercial, served as a control The being "inconsistent with recognized social values dependent effects measure consisted of a checklist of and customs.- Some types of behavior, such as occupations suitable for women in which thetoles of violence, would he almost universally regarded as judge, computer programer, and TV technician were antisocial, but the classification of living habits, included Results were as follows manners, or language is distinctly more subjective. It is not surprising, then, that no one has attempted to .ble 3-10 define the stimulus characteristics needed for an Percent of children who effects study on the more general forms of social checked this occupation behavior seen in commercials. as suitable for women We might ask, however, whether the phenomenon Control Experimen- Occupation of learning social behavior from commercialsis roup talrou demonstrable Atkin (1975b) has shown that it isin Computer programer 54% 62% this experiment, a group of children who saw an an- Judge 27% 51%* tilittering commercial exhibited significantly less lit- TV technician 26% 360/ tering behavior than a group who had not seen it. Iii ever, in Atkin's commercial, a public service *Significant difference (pi_ 05) ann,uncement, the focus was on littering, whereas the Sort of behavioral effects that the NAB and NAD The most dramatic result was the percentage of are concerned about would presumably be of a more children accepting the role of judge as suitable for incidental nature Behavioral modeling of incidental women after seeing a woman portray the role in the content in children's commercials has been commercial. The remaining results, although not demonstrated in another context '" However. no statistically significant. indicated that the other two studies are available on children's incidental learn- commercials had a similar effect of reversing the ing of routine social behaviors from commercials. stereotype. Their smaller impact may have been due, respectively, to less stereotyping of the computer 'n 'latch 0.osnicr,ttn)ns res. nv,ekiin ,harner s ,1 (his programer occupation and to the possibility that TV Fern t

5463 4. Self-concept Effects In Sum, children undoubtedly encounter many persoaal enhancement claims in commercials, and The four types of endorsements most likely to litany of these claims are undoubtedly effective (as affect a child's self-concept has e already beer, iden- well as valid). No direct nonproprietary data are tifiedpersonal enhancement appeals, social status available on the incidence or magnitude of these appeals, product usage portrayals, and competitive effects or on the failure of the claimed effects product appeals. This section will review the availa- ble evidence on the effects of each Socialstatusappeals Children's commercials which imply that ownership of a product will lead to Personal enhancement appeals Some 27 percent of increased social status, or that lack of the product children's commercials employ appeals oriented will have the opposite efft :t, are prohibited by the toward personal enhancementranging from tem- NAB and the NAD While the incidence of these porary effects such as fun and adventure, to more types of appeals are currently at a very low 2 per- permanent personal states such as strength, health, cent, they nevertheless exist Consequently, we may or well-being (see table 3-7). Many products ac- raise the issue of whether social status appeals are tually deliver such benefits, of course The main effective with children concern of critics and regulators is not so muchwith these claims per se as with their documentation or An experiment by Shaak, Annes, and Rossiter proof.' 9 Usually such documentation requires (1975) tested the effectiveness of a commercial with survey evidence demonstrating that theclaimed a social status theme. A storyboard commercial for a benefit is experienced or attested to by a large ma- fictitious brand of cookies was shown to a group of jority of children. This type of survey has not ap- 2d and 5th graders The final frame of the three- peared in the academic literature on the subject, but frame storyboard depicted two different endings In there is little doubt that there are proprietary com- one version, a child is shown winning new friends by mercial surveys which document various personal dispensing the cookies, in the control group version, enhancement eftects the child retains only his previous friends. This sup- ple manipulation produced an increase in affect or There is one academic study which indirectly rated liking toward the cookies of 42 percent (sig- bears on the question of personal enhancement out- nificant at the 001 level). the "peer reinforcement" comes. The study measured children's reported- effect was much stronger for the 5th graders (pt. 05) satisfaction with Christmas prpents, many of which than for 2d graders (n.s , though directionally con- were products advertised on television (Robertson sistent)indicating once again that code regulated and Rossiter, 1975). In 90 percent of the cases, this advertising practices do not always affect only the sample of 253 .children expressed satisfaction with youngest children in the audience. In fact, social the products (58 percent of the presents were rated status appeals would be expected to be more rele- better than expected). Younger children (1st grad- vant to olderchildren for whom peer acceptance is ers) experienced slightly less satisfaction (84 per- more important for their self- concept cent). Dissatisfaction among heavy TV viewers was 24 percent versus only g percent among light TV Real commercials may utilize sortewhat more viewers. However,itisnot clear that personal subtle allusions to socialstatus.21For example, enhancement appeals were responsible for all of this commercials which simply show children of the effect. Even if we make the rough assumption that 16 target group's age using the product may border on perce t2° of this dissatisfaction was due totelevision social status or peer status appeals.22 However, these advertised products, an alternative explanation is subtler executions probably depend more on source that young children are not experienced fudges of effects than on self-concept effects specifically product portrayals, whether or not any personal benefits are implied 1.y the commercials Product usage portrayalsPortrayals of products being used which might potentially harm a child's l4For example, the FTC's celebrated charge against Wonder self-concept include exaggerated portiayals of the Bread advertising included thenegation that ittends to ex- ploit children's aspirations for rapid and health,. growth The FTC eventually ruled in tasor of Wonder Bread in regard to this 21Although the above execution seemed no less obvious than portion of the complaint the 'boy s club- commercial mentioned in the Incidence section 2"24% :2See the pre tous discussion of social cues product's play value or performance characteristics, greater frustration after seeing the extravagant ver- usage demonstrations whichare difficultfor sion of the commercial without inferring that they children to emulate, settings or contexts which are were frustrated because their expectations were riot difficult for children to recreate, and portrayals of a attained child with unrealistically large numbers of product! or premiums (All of these are prohibited by indus- A broader evaluation of this experiment might try codes ) criticize it as atypical the product usage portrayal was greatly exaggerated But once again, it is possi- Atkin (1975h) tested the first, and perhaps the ble to offer the alternative hypothesis that slight ex- second of these prohibited portrayals He showed aggerations, if not attainable, would be even more two versions of a "Blockhead" building blocks com- frustrating since more children may he tempted to mercialone in which a child constructs a modest emulate them This is not an easy issue to resolve tower of blocks, and anotnei in which an extremely without a more sophisticated experiment complicated and extravagant tower was constructed Afterward, the:hildreninthe experiment were No experiments have been reported on the other given the opportunity to play with an identical set of code-regulated aspects of product usage portrayals blocks Children exposed to the extravagant usage The research paradigm for each would he essentially portrayal were generally unsuccessful in building similar, requiring some demonstration that similarly complicateJ constructions and during their children's self-images are involved in their appraisal attempts they displayed more 'anger, verbal aggres- of commercials which include product usage sion, and physical aggression" (28 percent) than portrayals children exposed to the modest version (I 8 percent) This was a statistically significant, although not Competitive' product appeals The NAB prohibits large, difference "competitive comparative isuperiority claims or techniques" expressly because of their alleged "po- Interestingly enough, had Atkin stopped this ex- tentialto encourage dissatisfaction on a child's periment before the behavioral stage, the implica- partThe implication is that the child's self-concept tions might have been quite different. The ex- is affected We found it useful, as the NAD does, to travagant version produced only one-half the level distinguishovert competitive appeals (so called of brand, name recall that the modest version gener- "comparative advertising" in which a direct brand atedperhaps because of a distraction effectAlso, comparison is made by name) and covert competitive as a measure of commercial-induced desire, children appeals (indirect, anonymous comparisons in which seeing the extravagant version were slightlyless the superiority or uniqueness of the advertised brand likely to state that the blocks would he fun to play is implied) with Overt comparisons are extremely rareabout I Nevertheless, there was slight evidence that the percentin commercials directed to children 21The extravagant version generated greater ethettations usual tear expressed by advertisers is that a com- of successful emulation than the modest version, and parative commercial may give the compentivebrand istkin's analysis would have been less equivocal it he a "tree plugConsequently, the technique tends to could have shown that the subsequent behavioral he used only after careful pretesting or when the effects of frustration were higher for children with brand is relatively unknown or has a lot to gain by greatercommercial-induced expectationsThis the comparison would have constituted clearer evidence of a sell- concept effect How ever, as Atkin noted, it could he Untoitunately, the one experiment designed to argued alternatively th,at self-concept "protection" test children's reactions to comparative advertise- might also lead children to lie about their expecta- ments (Atkin,1975c) examined these concerns tions. about advertising strategy rather than the alleged "dissatisfaction effect The experiment involved Despite the difficultyof obtaining avalid measure of mediational expectations, the exaggera- ko,rdIng ret.ent tion effects can he regarded as having considerable Lumnicrt 1,11s ,..,,mprisc ah,ut 10',01 primc time teleithm face validityIt is difficult to account for children s ,erasing

so u two versions of a commercial, one using a com- Wtnick et al. (1973) speculate that parison strategy, the other (control treatment) men- "by five, a child can probably understand 'best,' and tioning only the advertised product The results soon after, the meaning of 'better.' However showed no differences in the children's recall of the Wrighter (1972) points out that although the for- advertised product between the two versions mally taught quality hierarchy is "good-better-best," However, the children's recall of the "compared" advertising suggests a different hierarchynamely brand indicated a sizable "free plug" effect for that "good-best-better:" It is more difficult for an adver- product. Atkin also found that the comparative tiser to support a claim of "better" than one of commercial did not produce an increased desire for "bestTherefore, in advertising, "better" tends to the advertised productThe null effects on the become the superlative "desire" variable are equivocal, as Atkin noted, because he used two highly familiar brands a A pilot 'study by Burrall and Rossiter (1975) Hershey bar and a Nestle s bar. But again, the ex- tends to support Wrighter's position that children periment did not examine relative dissatisfaction cannot discriminate parity claims fo2- what they effects nor anything related to children's self-con- really are The study used claims which appeared in cepts children's Christmas toy catalogs rather thanin commercials, but they are representative of the types It is noteworthy in the Atkin experiment that the of audio claims children may encounter in commer- children did not ,eem ti) have any difficulty under- cials. The most interesting claims involved the standtut: the comparative commercial The NAD (in phrases "the best of its kind" and "two of the most contrast with the NAB) allows comparative claims delightful . The sample was deliberately slanted in cases in which true, understandable differences to reflect high verbal ability; subjects were 2d and exist between the brands and in which comparisons 4th grade girls from an upper middle class suburban are not based on selectRe inclusion of comparative school. Based on the first parity claim, 30 percent of attributes. Commercials meeting these criteria may the 2d graders and 40 percent of the 4th graders well be quite informative to children in a aconsumer rated the advertised item as better than similar prod- socialization sense However, no research has Been ucts.2s Figures for t'ie second claim were 20 percent conducted on this contention, apart from the tangen- and 50 percent It seems reasonable to hypothesize tial finding in Atkin's experiment that the compara- that the incidence of children who were apparently tive commercial produced much greater learning Of fooled by the claim would be higher in a sample of one of two product information dimensions children of lower verbal ability. Also, the increased effect with age hints that children may be learning Despite NAB prohibition, covert 'competitive advertising's new quality hierachy. The experiment claims--or implied "superiority" claimsare esti- obviously needs replication, but it is indicative of the mated to appear in about 11 percent of children's type of effect that subtle competitive claims can have commercials. Some of these claims may be based on on children's evaluations of advertise( oducts documentable superiority (e g"best sel'ing" ) although others are clearly subjective opinions, (e g., But whether or not such heightened evaluations "the greatest." "fantistic") 24 affect a child's self - -corn ept would. require further demonstration: for instance, that advertising might Perhaps the most prevalent type of covert com- make the childfeel disappointed with presently petitive claim in adult commercials is the so-called owned products or with an advertised product, once "parity claim." Obvious examples are slogans which it had been purchased simply state about are brand that "it's best." This may mean among the best or on a par with other top SUMMARY AND NEEDED RESEARCH brands Less obvious examples are employed by two well known brands of toothpaste "You can't beat I Contusion Elle( for fighting cavities"' and "Only your Although asubstantialproportion of young dentist can give you a better fluoride treatment than children experiences confusion between programs z'Prior familiarity with the advertised items was not a con- 24As Present.' (19'Si has iihser% edsuperinritv claims 111,1y louncling factorthe catalog w1, newts issued with limited cir- even he implied hs a products name g ,11,ini(,r Bread or Lulatinn and children said, with only nne or two exceptinns, that Super blocks) thev had seen neither it rim- the items :,etore

566 and commercials, there is no evidence in support of 2 Endorsement Effect the hypothesis that the use of_program characters in commercials contributes to this contusion. The one Only one study has tested the effects of a direct, study on thisissue produced equivoca! results, -type endorsement by a celebrity/author- because the confusion measure was too delayed to be ity figure, although others have tested less direct en- conclusive dorsements in studies of source effects The study ac- tually found negative endorsement effects: -subjects A true test of the confusion effect would require a who did not like the endorser gave the endorsed more instantaneous measure of children's ability to product lower ratings Neverthelecs, other research distinguish program content from commercial con- leaves little doubt that positive endorsements effects tent. Otherwise, it is not clear whether we are testing can also occur children's memories or their actual perceptual in- ability to discriminate. Inability to recall a "host" At least four studies have shown that a significant commercial could indicateanextreme lack of dis- change in rated affect toward the endorsed product crimination, or lack of attention, or lack of reten- can occur with even a passive or "mere appearance" tion. A better measure of confusion might be a sig- endorsement. It is therefore highly improbable that nal-stopping technique in which different subjects prohibition of direct endorsements will remove en- are stopped at random, intervals and questioned dorsement effects, although it may reduce them about their immediate perceptions The entire area of source effects research in Future research should also examine whether live children's advertising is badly in need of theoretical program personalities are any different from ani- direction and improved methodology The follow- mated cartoon characters in their potential for con- ing are the most important points fusion. Both types of endorsers are prohibited from selling within or adjacent to their own programs, yet On the source or "stimulus" side, tit_ c has different restrictions are applied to human and ani- been no specification of the characteristics or proc- mated endorsers elsewhere; generally, animated esses by which endorsers are supposed to produce characters are given much freer reign It is not clear their effects For example, the FTC consent order why this should he so. against Spider-Man Vitamins alleges; that this en- dorser has the tendency and capacity to take advan- The single study of the culla(en( v effe(t hypothesis tage Of the trust relationship developed between did not support the contention that program per- children and the pi ogram character 26 However, sonalities and cartoon characters are less likely to source theory in social psychology2- suggests that produce confusion if they do not appear in commer- any one or a combination of the following factors cials placed within or adiacent to their own Lan produce an endorsement Meet program;. However, the nonadjacency condition used in the experiment was confounded by an at- tempt to.test another manipulation at the same time Source characteristic Process 1 Credibility Internalization The same comments on the need for an instan- a Expertness taneous measure of confusion apply here, as does the b Objectivity need to include program personalities and cartoon 2. Attractivereris Identification characters in the research. A new study is needed Likeability which pro, .des parameters for the adjacency b Similarity phenomenon Relevant experimentaN-reatment° for 3 Pow9r Compliance commercial placement would he within program, contiguous to program, noncontiguous but within a short time of the program, and noncontiguous and "Note that the f I( smo s a marked departure from the well separated from the program. The first two pre ious I-1(' policy of allowing NAB self-regulation Spider- treatments would correspond to the adjacenLy ban man ,sa pgraro characternota human celebrityor an of the NAB while-the 'remaining two would test the authority. and as such tth he ..ited under N Ali rules for : --,iing and the ,onlio effect, not for the endorsement effectiveness of the separation implicitly required in ettek.t the code's provisions g KCIMJI1I )S8 41L(frutrt1)7%

58() I.

Thus, celebrity endorsers may represent a cOmbina- cartoons andiction as a "game" and thus fantasy tion of likeability and perhaps expertise. Authority presenters a erceived as giving a weaker form of figures, on the other hand, may combine credibility. endorsement than real-life presenters. (b) Or, to the and powerthe latter being defined as the potential contrary, that fantasy endorsements are more effec- to reward or punish the messabe receiver and thus to tive because children are less skeptical of claims control compliance with the message With some made by fantasy presenters than they are of the same theoretical direction, we might then be able to ascer- claims made by real-life presenters. (c) That humor tain why certain categories of sources are or should or magic associated with fantasy presenters adds ex- he regulated in children's advertivrtg tra appeal to the presentation and the product. (d) That animated acions are easier for young children 2. Researcn on source effects should include an to comprehend than live-figure action. (e) That, in assessment of thesource's characteristicscred- cartoon program slots, animated product presenters ibility, attractiveness, powerindependent of how (not necessarily progran: characters) male the tran- the source is used in a commercial The product sition from program r commercial "minimally jar- should also be assessed independently Only then ring" to young viewers (Winrck et al.'s expression) can we judge what, and how much, a source adds to a and may also prevent .Jedline in attention. (f),That product's intrinsic characteristics via endorsement certain fantasy characters such as Fred Flintstone or Bugs Bunny are so familiar that they have lost much 3 On the effect or "response" side, most studies of their fantasy content. of source effects in children's advertising have been content to record changes in overall affect or rated We suggest two other areas for future research. liking toward the endorsed product. But these (g) That children's perceptions of "slice of life" changes can only be taken as evidence of an endorse- portrayals beexamined to see whether these indeed ment effect (a) if overall likeability is viewed as a differ from "reallife" portrayals; for example, product characteristic, and (b) if the endorsement whether role- played authority figures are any effect is interpreted to include quantitative changes different from actual authority figures. (h) Also, the in existing product characteristics and not just the source characteristics and processes model outlined addition of a qualitatively "new" characteristic previously would seem to be equally applicable to More informative evidenbson endorsement effects fantasy endorser research. A theory-based approach would be obtained it we mcNrporated specific prod- would allow us to determine whether it is really fan- uct attributes in the research and measuredspecific tasy-reality that is the relevant d mension or whether changes in children's beliefs about these attributes as the true differences between sources lie in their cred- well as the more global index of overall product ibility, attractiveness, or relative power Iikeability One final, general remark should be made con- 4If the intent of the NAB provisions on indirect cerning the endorsement ..:feet No one has yet in- endorsement is to reduce rather than to remove a vestigated the question of whether children regard given source's impact, a relevant study would he to an endorsement as being an attribute of the product, provide parameters to the direct endorsement varia- i e., that one of a product's characteristics is that it is ble by including three experimental treatments ap- "recommended by " This research pearance only, visual endorsement in whichthe en- would provide insight on children's reasoning in dorser is seen using or consuming the product but product and source evaluation, It would also raise not verbally endorsingit, and a straightforward the issue of whether endorsements are a "legitimate" testimonial or "fair" basis on which to promote products to children The fantasy effect phenomenon is also in dire need of theoretical direction The fantasy effect is proba- 3. Social Stereotype' Effect bly the most important assumption in the NAB's children's presenter code and it therefore demands It is almost impOssible to determine whether or more than just an intuitive appraisal. Some theoreti- not commercials contribute to the formulation of calbeginnings were mentioned inthe study by social stereotypes. Ratio analyses of social cue con- Winick et al (1973) from which the following list of tent in commercials (young-old, malt-female, black- hypotheses was derived (a) That children perceive white) suggest that they at least have the potential to

5C S do soChildren's commercials are biased toward presenters and still more effective if shown with adults on the age ratio and toward males )n the sex children in a joint endorsement (b) Sex cue findings ratioThey arealso somewhatbias,,.toward were not as clear cut. Experiments employing male minority groups (mainly black; on the race k .1t10 and female endorsers re( e 1 significant "own sex" effect for boys and for especially among If these ratios can aft...ct children's beliefs about older children. Most studio, ye been dons with the real worldand there is secondary evidence chiV models Given the prevalence of adults :a trom television prop am research that they canan childrca's commercials, a logical study would be a extremely conplex and value-laden issue is raised sex-by-age "crossed" experiment on endorsement The issue is whether commercials should be ex- effects.(c) Race cue endorsement findings were pected to match real world ratios or whether some strong but contradictorya critical variable ap- kind of over-compensation is justified(e g ,for peared to be the prior racial socialization of the par- women or for racial minority groups) The prosocial ticipating children. (d) Occupational cue research potentialof commercials inthisrespectisin- has been confined mainly to studies of the occupa- terestingly illustrated by the tendency of children's tional stereotyping of women As only one effects commercials notonlytooverride the non- study has been conducted with children's commer- white,white ratio slightly, but ,o accomplish this by cia's, this is a prime area for further research (e) almost exclusively depicting integrated racial set- Research on social behavior cues has not been pur- tings in children's commercials Value judr-aents sued apart from one study of a public service an- aside, it would be of empirical interest to kru ,v what nouncement on littering This neglect is partly due effects such practices have to thelifficulty of defining what is "acceptable" social behavior beyond certain exireme examples Evidence from o e study dealing with occupa- tional stereotypes fo, women,28 raised another issue A final category of needed research on the social worthy of attention Although the development of stereotype effect should be mentioned. In the sex cue social stereotypes is undoubtedly a cumulative pro,..- studies, it was frequently, and not too surprisingly, ess, this studs indicated that at least a short-term found that many products are sex-role stereotyped reverse stereotyping effect could take place in a by children This raises the possibilitytor other single exposureFuture research on this social cues as wellthat source eff-cts may operate phenomenon should include mess 'rement of the in reverse That is, products shown in commercials permanence of the effect and investigation of the may affect children's perceptions of the characters possibilities for counterstereotype portrayals tor associated with those products Girls who are shown other social cue categories playing with "male" toys tor example, may be seen as unfeminine Again, the purpose is not to pass Social cues in commercials also nave relevance to judgment on such portrayals but simply to alert a previous topic, the endorsement eftect Here we future researchers to the bidirectional possibilities of are concerned not with whether social cues in com- the source effect mercials contribute to the learning of s -reotypes. but whether previously learned stereotypes con- 4.Self-concept Ellett tribute to children's evaluatiorof products For ex- ample. children may react to models in commercials Isolated studies have been conducted with each of because of the credibilit,attractiveness, or power four types of presentations related to self-concept associated with their social characteristicsquite appeals personal e rnancement appeals, social status apart from any celebrity, authority, or othei special appeals, product usage portrayals, and competitive stus characteristics they may possess. The evidence product appeals There are two faults common to the showed that children do respond to social cues as a existing studies on these self-concept issues The source effect(a) Age cue research indicated that faults, re sufficiently criticalto deserve special adults were generally more etfective than children as emphasis

a None of the studies has included measurement ha n Ics -NIt seems pertinent to reiterate that a separate-, of the principal variablethe child's self-con- ofhouse .siteandmotherstrwid he madeI",) own in cstig.th,rs tail to do thisI he two roles oh.iousl, ha,,e quite cept While s, of the studio have demon- ditterent tor Lhildren s perspeLtR.es strated that personal enhancement appeals,

60 social status appeals, and competitive product st itus appeals are very rare in children's commer- appeals are effective with children, and that cials but it could be argued that certain endorsers product usage portrayals may be emulated, n( carry social status connotations Furtherresearch, attention has been given to any tran,ient o- therefore, should probably be directed to social permanent effects on the chili's self-concept status as a subcategory of the endorsementeffect, rather than as a practice in its own right (c) Product Researchers have tailed to realize that, in this usage portrayals represent a prime exampleof a area and others, the chow.: of commercial's is research area in whrh the two general faults listed animportalt sampling problemIn many above need to be overcome Highly exaggerated cases, extreme examples have been selected portrayals may actually discourage imitation and no argument has been advanced as to why whereas more moderate portrayals might invite it the particular commercial was chosen or why it The central research issueis again, however, was assumed to be a representative case As we whether achild'sinability'to emulate usage pointed out in the discussion, many alleged portrayals actually affects the child's self-concept. effects depend on a "parameterized" stimulus (d) Competitive product claims con route a topic for is These may not be linear parameters, so it which researchis strongly recommendedOvert also possible that selecti«.4 extreme exam- competitive appeals.if presented fairly and ac-, ples may actually overlook the critical range curately, may contribute positively to children's consumer socialization Covert competitiveappeals, In terms of needed research 'a) It seems incum- on the other hand, are often undetected inincidence bent upon advertisers t., document their claims of counts since they are little understoodMoreover, temporary benefits, such as fun and adventure, or when they do occur, they tend to be subtle and to more permanent benefits such as strength,health. hold negative implications for children's consumer and well-being No further academic research is socialization However, itis likely that competitive recommended on this topic (b) Social status appeals prothict appeals have more effy on children's un- are more relevarto older children who are more derstanding of reasoning and logic than on their self- aware of the need ,Jr. social acceptanceOvert social concepts per se

; 61 Chapter 4

EFFECTS OF PREMIUM OFFERS IN CHILDREN'STELEVISION ADVERTISING

Premiums are heavily employed as a purchase in- that is almost always complete!! irrelevant to centiveincertain children's product categories, merits of the principal product. thereby greatly notably cereals. The FTC has alleged that premiums increasing the -likelihood that the child's represent an irrelevant product characteristic, that response to the ad will reflect confusion they distract children from considering legitimate productattributes, and that they multiply the The premium offer characteristically bears no difficulty of choosing between brands. Defenders of relation tothe criteria which would guide premiums argue that they may actuallyfacilitate the choice if th-; product stood alone Instead, the choice between otherwise fairly similar brands, and premium's main purposeieto distractthe that premiums constitute a legitimate product at- buyer's attention from those attributed and to tribute since they are part of the "total product motivate purchase not on the m_rits ofthe package product but in order to obtain the premium

CURRENT AND PROPOSED REGULATION The injection of a premium into a buying deci- sion cannot help but multiply the difficultiesof

The FTC published a proposed guide in 1974 choice . Merely by adding another group of which would eliminate all forms of premium offers factors that compete with those already de- in television advertising addressed to children under manding the child's attention, the premium 12 The prohibition would include offers of "pack- must inevitably increase the likelihood of con- ins" (small toys or other objects included in the tusion and of the purchase ot an inferior prod- package), "pack-ons" (e.g., cut-outs on the side of a uct cere' Ibox), reusable containers. self-liquidatig premiums, and any kind of remuneration tor An industry spokesman, Dr Seymour Banks.1 purchase, including tree gifts questions whether premiums are in tact "completely irrelevant to the merits of the principal product," as The FTC proposal has attracted much crit sm stated in the FTC's proposed guide He cites the case from advertisers, who have been using premium ot presweetened breakfast cereals, the products that offers in ace reance with the result...0ns of existing include the greatest proportion of premium offers industry codes and guidelinesFor example, the The various brands of presweetened cereals are very NAD code permits premium otters but stresses the similar, differing mm6 in shapes and colors Vir- need to place major emphasis in advertising on the tually all of them contain the same ingredients and product itself, with the premium as clearly second- nutritional contentDrBanks argues that a ary Some industry spokem an have proposedfurther purchaser, faced with su:h an array of similar restrictions, such as limiting the time devoted to the brands, will choose one brand on the basis of a dis- premium offer within a commercial For example, criminating attribute, such as the premium. He ques- the Cracker Jack Division of Borden Foods has pro- tion-s, then, whether premiums are, in fact, an "ir- posed that only one-third or 10 seconds of a com- relevant" product attribute mercial, whichever is less, should be used to present the premium offer "Relevance" and "irrelevancc" are not directly amenable to re!..earLh. Issues about premiums that The issues are suminarized le rationale for the are open to empirical analysis seem to he FTC's proposed guide

T'te very purpose of the prem., advertise iFrum comment,, MI an earlier draftd this p per by Banks, vice president Media Reseal,hI Co Burnett Inc men.: io locus the child's attention on a factor To what extent do premium otters "contuse" RESEARCH EVIDENCE children tram regarding whatisactually being sold Some useful background information forthe issues raised by premiums is provided in a purely to what extent do premium messages distract descriptive survey of attitudes toward and uses of children from using other product teatures rn premium otters among mothers and children Con- makig product choicesDoes a premium ducted by The Gene Reilly Group (197,1 the na- offer "multiply the difficulties- of product or tional survey sample of 1,200 was city (dee evenly be- brand choice') tween mothers and two age-groups of children (7-9 Do premium otters increase intratamily con and 10-12 years)Although the study focused on flict' premiums designed for adults as well as for children, certain findings are relevant to this discvssion A final research issue arises from the compromise proposal by industry groups to place time limit,: on Premiums werenos( often associated with the premium otters within a commercial cereal products by both mothers (82 percent) and children (91 percent) 4 !nstead of a total ban on premium otters, would placing time limitations on the pre- 82 percent of chi:dren said that they would most expect to fine premium advertising on miu ,ter (e g10 seconds of a total of 30) television, rattier than in other media achic the objective of making the premium otter a secondary attribute') "'fluid such a Children report receiving an average of 20 pre- restriction lessen the potential of premium miums in the past year offers to contuse children') Would it make their choices between products relatively less Premium iiseind mostpositiveattitudes ddficul02 toward premiums, occur among mothers and children in the lower socioeconomic strata INCIDENCE OF PREMIUM OFFERS 84 percent of children who said that they re- cently acquired a premium said that it was ob- Within the past 5 years, two studies have found taineck with the _purchase of a product. rather that about 10 percent of the commercials on Satur- than b) sending away for it day morning te:evision contained premiums (Winick et al 1973, Atkin, 1974) However, the percentage Children were generally satisfied with the most of premium offers is substantially higher for specific recent premium received, dissatisfaction was products Atkin found, for example, that 34 pc:Lent linked to "lack tit quality of all cereals advertised on Saturday morning con- Mothers preferred educational premiums and tained a premium otter other premiums th,u could be used by several children or by the whole tamely They cited Precise data are not available on the prey lichee plastic toy,, toys needing assembly, and easily, of prerni,.m advertising for other kinds of products broken toys as premiums they did not like or services (such as tast food restaurants) nor are there precise data on the nature of the premium Both mothers and children reported that pre- miums didnot otters themselves Some anecdotal information is influence their purchase behav lor However, the social desirability has available for example, Shimp et al (197(,) cite an FTC source indicating that most premium otters oc- tit such questioning makes the s alldit!, of the response go oonable cur within the "last half' tit commereials addressed to children

:Nye reignite thatdifficultch-ices arc not TIC t noted hothhilt, oft( lit, d and adult-oriented1 'cents had One could argue, in tact, that thenere hem Loaf tot Itcoupons glassware cteI premium. Hite reterteet to Sine e learning tioueer to the nu, it that choice daft,. alts ma% ke re the surf not at lcar which tpe it premium the mothers lated to a child s choosing a brand other than his or her ideal refer Mos,' data reported abOse are tro it children or tram choice (as determined, for example h% desoed features) choice mothers n Ihttete tem(is cleat Ixto child oriented pre- difficult, max he !MUMS 86 percent of mothers rated premn.ns offered essing and finds a developmental trend in children's to children as being only lair" or -poor.in ability to remember relevant and irrelevant infor- overall quality hey generally opposed the mation (Maccoby and Hagen, 1965, Hagen, 1976, idea of television advertising that includes pre- Hagen and Hall, 1973) In this research, the central. mium otters Again, social clesirability facto!, 'relevant" information was defined as that which is make the %Akin\ of the lattcdata question. necessary to the performance of a task, such asiden- tifying the position of pictures in a sequential array Incidental, or "irrelevant," information was not In general, one could interpret these data as necessary to the completion of the task The older demonstrating that mothers serhaily express some- children (5th and 7th graders) inthese studies what negative attitudes toward premiums and are remembered central information much better than not influenced by premiums intheir purchasing younger children (1st and 3d graders), but memory However, the sheer numbers of premiums obtained of incidental information did not change with age by the children in the sursey suggest that actual These findings were interpreted by the researchers as behavior is not consistent with these negat se at- a developmental change in attention selectivity'As titudes grow older they are better able to allocate their attention and to s:lect the information neces- Its' issues of (mill\ it n1(did illstr,A (ion from other sary to completion of a task produ, t features Much research iichild psychology has examined children's attention to and selection of Pick et al(1975) summarired this research on information from audio and or s isual snmul, (see children's processing of central and in !dental infor- Accentuating what is irrele- chapter I ) Vs, hilefessstudies has efocused mation by concluding specificallyon teles Ishii)ads ertnang, much less vant often makes it more difficult to attend to and tele% ised premium advertising, the existing research use what is relevant This is especially true for helps us to understand children's information pros.- younger childrenI he es dence also indicates a essing,*hich may have relevance for their responses clear developmental improsement in the effective- to premium advertising on television ness of children's search strategies Olderchildren engage in more search Jetts itythan younger Of particular interest are the studies presiously hildrtm, but are also better able to ignore or dis- cited in this report, of children's attention patterns regard irrelevant information while stewing commercials embedded in programs in both laboratory and natural, in-home environ- Several other research efforts in t'us area clearly ments, (Wartella and Enema, 1973, Ward and demonstrate that(I) older children can use more Wackman, 1971, Atkin, 1974'Ihese findings eon- dimensionsinproblem solving than younger sistcntly show age-related decreases (from nursery children, and (2) children s reinforcement ex- school to 6th graders) in children's attention to com- neriences c I play a siondieant iole in their deter- mercials, ref afis: to program content Some of the mination of the salience (St different kinds of infor- data suggest that younger childien silos,greater mation (W itryol, i owden, and I agan, 1976, Spe ce sanance ,nattention pattern,perhaps redwing Ind Segner, 1967, Odom, 1972, Odom and Corbin, their shorter attention spans Only the Atkin study 1973, Odom and (iuinan, 1972) To the extent that examined children's attention to commercials with these findings can he applied to questions of pre- and without premium offers and no dine! ences %Acre IUM offers inteles hum advertising, they might and in the children's attention patterns to the tsso suggest age-related differences in children s ahil1ties kinds of comniercidls Because these studies used to es al llate premiums and other product feat oresin rather gross measures of attention, they shed In;le making produc., dunces, Moreoser, children's ex- light on the characteristics of commercials Inch periences of posinse and negatis c reinforcement in alfect attentionIn childrenI hey do suggest using premiums orgnolls seed in adseitisg may hosveser, that commercials flask- notabledrasyme foster, or mhibit, the responsiseness of the children posse!' for youngei children to future premium offers

Other research to child psychology has somierile 1sso studies hase directly examined children's sance to premium ads criising For example Hagen responses to premium offers m television coin nler has studied aspects of addict) s information pro.- lals1 hey f tossed sin111.11 procedures, exposing

ar Table 4-1 Recall of Specific Commercial Elements

Premium No premium Grade: 1st 3d 6th 1st 3d 6th Brand name

Accurate 2 4 7 2 5 9 Cereal only 8 4 4 8 7 3 Symbol

Accurate 8 8 7 7 9 12 Other animals 3 2 0 5 2 0

Premium

Accurate 9 11 12 Other objects 2 0 0 . II= (12' (12) (12) (12) (12) (12)

Source Rubin, 1972 Based or responses to the c^,In-ended question "Tell me what happensin the movie you Just saw

children tq cereal commercials embedded in although almost all of them remembered that the programing and then immediately measuring the commercial concerned cereal The 3d graders it ;re verbal responses Jf the children Both studies also split evenly in both condittons There was high rer.all involved exposure to a single test commercial as op- of the specific premium offer, a toy car, in all age posed to the repetitive exposures which would occur groups. in the course of normal television viewing In both conditions, the product symbol was ac- curately recalled by a relatively high percentage Gt In Robin's (1972) study, 1st, 3d, and 6th grade all of the-chileren. It is interesting to note that all of children were exposed to a 30-second commercial, the 6th graders accurately recalled the symbol in the either with or without a premium offer Immediately no-premium condition, whereas only 7 ut the 12 after exposure, they were asked a series of questions mentioned a symbol in the premium condition This about the commercial. what they recalled, what they suggests the possibility that the premiums distracted felt they were supposed to want, what they were sup- attention from the brand name posed to do with the product, etc Since some of the questions were fairly abstract, they may have been The salience of the premium offer was assessed by difficult for the younger children to answer e g , examining children's responses to the question "What do you think (mOvies. ads, commercials) aft. "W hat du you think you arc supposed to want atter supposed to be for?" "Why do you think people seeing the movie"'" Results appear in table 4-2 In make them?" Further, some caution must he used in both the premium .ind no-premium condition, most interpreting resuits because of the limited verbal ut the 3d and 6th graders exposed to the premium ad abilities of younger children. Nevertheless, most said that the commercr41 A as trying to make them children did provide some answers to each question want both the cereal and the premium. Among 1st graders, there was a substantial difference between Data on the recall of the children were obtained the two conditions In the no-premium condition, 9 from the question. "Ted me what happens in the ot the 12 children said the advertiser wanted them to movte you just saw?" "Anything else?" As table 4-1 buy cereal But in the premium conditon, only one indicated, only the oth graders had reasonably high I st grader thought he was supposed to want cereal recall of the brand name in the commercial, both Four ot the 1st graders thought they were supposed with and without the premium Only a few I st grad- to want the premium'. and six ul them didn't know ers recalled the brand name in either condition what they were sepp,,,ed to want

66 Table 4-2

What Children Think They Are Supposed ToWant OW- Exposure to a Commercial

Premium No premium 6th Supposed to want Grade: 1st 3d 6th 1st 3d 9 a 10 Cereal 1 3 5 Cereal (emphasized) plus premium 5 4 Premium only ,4 3 3 2 1 Other 1 0 0 1 Don't know 6 1 0 1 a (12) (12) (12) (12) (12) (12) n=

Source Rubin (1972)

Table 4-3

Recall of Product Information by Timing of Premiumreature

Timing of premium Age beloW 7 years Age 7 and older Correct Number of Correct Number of responses responses responses responses

10/20a 10 09b 11 11 59 39 15/15 817 12 10 62 37 20/10 8 70 10 10 00 38 0/30 9 50 12 10 97 38 n= (45) (152)

a To be read 10 seconds of commercial devoted to premiuminformation/20 seconds devoted to product information the commer- b Indicates the mean number of correct responses to the 15 questionsconcerning product information presented in cials

Source Shimp, Dyer and Divita 1975

I hew results suggest that the insertion pit tilt pre- form 4)tdiage Clashes of es emits and no basic mium information LontAsed the oungestchildren shin,line 114)10) itlassL: that e, en the but not the Om sI his appears to support the younger childrenreLoyedinformation shout the the general research which indicated that the acycntua premium commerytal, but chose to talk about them It may Hon tit incidental information makes itdifficult for prem tun, hecause it was more salient to young children to attend to and use tdo, ant infor- he that the premium iiCr d Id not interfere v, iththe mation Rubm's data must he Interpretedwithto transmissionit information shout the product, hut, non, howeyer, for reasons beloind thesmall ,ample rather, superseded it sties A Illal(K prt 411CM is thatthe no-pi em turn r mercial featured a Lon entional slot s Thne formal In Shimp, Dyer, and this ltd.), (1975)studs, 197 while the commercial with the premium otter used a children, 1st to hth graders, were presentedwith one of four versions of a 30-second commercial fora hy- very few children chose an experimental product potheticalcerealproduct named SnappyFruit brand against two known brands--nly about 10 Smacks One version had no premium offer, the percent of childrenineach group ranked the other three contained 10, 15, or-20 seconds of pre- unknown experimental product as their first choice mium information Immediately atter exposure, the children were given questionnaires consisting of Since the four groups did not differ in preference true/false questions about the product and premium for the advertised product, the authors of the study offer Rankings of the children's preferences for the argue that premium advertising appear to have little advertised product and two alternative cereals were impifct on the children's actual preference for cereal also obtained. Shimp et al 's subjects were primarily brands However, the authors do not report (1) older children, only 45 of the 197 childrenwere whether the children had a history of requesting or below the age of seven Thus. there was an under- using either of the two known cereals in the brand representation of young children, the age group for choice or (2) whether the two known cereals also whom there is the most concern advertised a premium and, if so, whether the pre- mium was visible on the box displayed to the The Shimp findings for recall are similar to those children in the brand-choice test Consequently, it is of the Rubin study. In all of the test situations, difficult to determine whether the low frequency of children below 7 years old recalled less information choice of the experimental cereal can be accounted about the product than older children (see table for by the commercial, the premium advertisement, 4-3). Out of 15 truelalse items used to test the or other factors children, the younger children correctly answered only slightly more items than would be expected by In short, the Shimp et alresearch suffers from chance alone The young children demonstrated the several severe limitationsin choice of subjects, best call in the commercial which comprised a 10- measurement procedures, and data analysis second premium offer and 20 seconds of productin- Replication of this study is needed formation Shimp et al assigned significance to this finding, concluding that premium advertisingmay Three other studies are relevant to issues concern- enhance the attention of younger children Thiscon- ing children's abilities to use multiple attributes in clusion appears unwarranted, however, in that the product selectionsWartelta and -Ettema (1973, authors do not report whether these differencesin 1974) and Atkin (1974), previously described in recall among the experimental conditionswere chapterI, embedded commercials in half-hour statistically significantAlso, Shimp et aldo tilt programs and asked children immediately after report the nature of the kinds of information the viewing the program. to recall Je commercials children recalled from the commercial (1 e, brand Although we noted earlier that such open-ended name, symbol, etc ), therefore, we cannot directly questions may tax the verbal abilities of the younger compare this research to Rubin's findings children and consequently may fail to elicit their recall, the central findings from these two studies Shimp et at also measured the children's attitudes were nevertheless consistent Few of the children toward the advertised product and premium Utiliz- recalled having seen specific commercials during the ing control and experimental groups. the Shimp program For example. in Atkin's study, only one- study found no differences in attitudes toward the fourth of the subjects could recall the commercial cereal product, suggesting that the premiums made with a premium and only 21 percent could recall the the advertised product neither more nor less attrac- no-premium commercialIn the Wartella-Ettema tive to the children 'However, thine was a moder- study, even smaller percentages of children remem- ate. significant c( -relation ( 20) between the attitude bered seeing a specific commercial of the children toward the premium and their choice of a product brandThis suggests that the more The 1973 Wartella and Lttema study also children liked a premium obiect, themore attractive assessed specific information that children recalled the particular brand w as to them On the other hand, The findings indicated that the youngest children 3 -4 years old) recalled only visual apd auditor) im- ages from the commercial For the older two age 'Stump et ai do not rcpiirt their data h!, age gr. ups thus we groups (5-b and 7-8 yetrs), there v4 as increasing Lanni,t disLuss age related ditteren,es in the children s attitudes recall of claims about the pro uct g "It tastes

68 t) Table 4-4 on personal interviews with mothers ofchildren ranging in age from 3 to 11. Of the 301 mothers tn. Mother's Reports of Child's terviewed, 211 stated that their children "some- Requests for Cereals times" request specific cereals seen on TV. These mothers were asked to report the reason their Question: "When your child asks for a specific children usually give for his/her cereal requests (ta- ble 4-4); 47 percent said that their children cited the cereal, what does he/she usually say . .what reasons does he/she give for wanting it?" (If premium as the basis for the request. Those mothers premium not a major reason offered by mother: who did not spontaneously mention premiums were "Does he/she ever say that he/she wants a asked if premiums were ever given as the reason, and cereal so he/she can get a premiJm or in about one-third then indicated that premiums were the box?") sometimes the reason for a particular brand's re- quest Grade:Pre-K 1st-3d4th-5th One may question the validity of mothers' reports Premluio cited of their children's attempts to influence purchases. originally 47% 51 % 42% Assuming these reports are valid, however, the find- Premium cited ings do not necessarily mean that premium offers in followup 35% 36% 40% "distract" children from consideration of other Premium not cite° 18% 13% 18% features of the productNor do they indicate whether children examined other product features (75) (81) (55) n= and, finding most brands indistinguishable, chose to base their preferences and requests on the premium Source Atkin (1975g) offer. We can say, however, that Atkin's findings reflect the salience of premium offers in breakfast cereal advertising and suggest that children like pre- good," "It's good for kids,") although recall was still miums predominantly of images

- Atkin ( I975f) also made in-supermarket observa- Studies cited earlier (Ward and Wackman, 1972, tions. Potentially, this should be the more valid 1974) also assessed the information children remem- method of gathering data on this subject Observer ber from commercials Children were asked to name were placed unobtrusively around the cereal aisle of their favorite commercial, then to describe what a supermarket. Listening to mother-childdialogues happens in the commercial The data indicated a regarding cereal purchases, they found that 9 per- developmental progression of recall from one or two cent of all children's cereal requests contained an visual images for young children (1st and 2d grad- explicit mention of a premium offer ers) to increasingly complex, multidimensional, and complete recall among older children (5th and 6th One final study provides additional information graders) In Rubin's (1972) study a similar develop- about the considerations of childrenin product mental trend was indicatedYoung children, selection, and the relative importance of premiums preschoolers, and early grade school children (Reilly, 1973a). Personal interviews were conducted recalled fewer images from the commercials As age with a national sample of 6- to 15-year-old children, increased. recall included more and more different covering a wide range of media-related topics. One kinds of information, and both the story line and series of questions concerned children's product purpose of the commer.ial became better under- choices "When you see a TV commercial for a stood. product, would you like the product more if (two alternative choices) Table 4-5 shows the results Pretntum offers and purchase requests Two other from all the questions which offered "prizes inside studies by Atkin (1975g and f) examined the impor- the package" or "offers on or inside the package" as tance of premiums in children's decisionmaking, one of the alternatives Particular attention should using (a) mother's reports of the nature of children's he paid to the age differences in these data Most of purchase requests, and (h) in-store observations of the youngest children chose the premium alterna- mother-child interactions The first study was based tive: the older children made this choice less often

69 ", Table 4-5

Children's Preferences in Television Commercial Appeals

Question: "When you see a TV cori....iercial for a product, would you like the product more if it...

Age: 6-7 8.10 11-12 13-14

Base: Only those responding 145 285 240 229

Had an offer 52% 40% 34% 31% Were nutritious 43% 54% 58% 59% No difference 5% 6% 8% 10%

Base: Only those responding 152 288 234 228

Had a prize inside 57% 51% 38% 29% Were nutritious 35% 42% 51% 55% No difference 8% 7% 11% 16%

Base: Only those responding 147 280 234 231

Were natural 29% 39% 52% 58% Had a prize inside 67% 56% 42% 29% No difference 4% 5% 60/0 13%

Base: Only these responding 145 278 236 230

Were enriched or fortified 28% 45% 58% 62% Had a prize inside 67% 52% 35% 26% No difference 5% 3% 7% 12%

Source Redly, 1973a.

Several reasons for this finding are possible. Pre- limited, the specific kinds of informationthat miums are usually designed for younger children children of different ages select from advertising is and therefore lost their appeal for older children not very well known. Rubin's research indicates that Older children may also have a greater tendency to a product's brand name, symbol, and type of pre- respond with what they perceive to be a more mium are three key features recalled to varying socially accepted answer based on the quality of the degrees by different age children. The Rubin, War- product itself. Finally, older children may realize tella-Ettema, and Ward-Wackman studies also sug- that the true worth of a purchase is in the product gest a general movement with increasing age from and not the premium. the recall of a few perceptual images to more com- plex forms of recall. A better understanding of To sum up, the evidence from research both on children's information selection from commercials child development and on television commercials awaits more research. seems clearly to indicate that the selective nature of children's attention to and information drawn from Existing research is limited in another significant television commercials is not random and that there way. None has addressed the question of the cumula- are specific, developmental changes which occur as tive effects of commercials on children's s-lection children grow olderBecause theresearch on and use of information. Yet repetitiveness would children's attention to television commercials is so seem to be the single most pervasive feature of

4S television commercials The research reported in the Atkin also observe', chat denial of cereal requests nest section, although not directly assessing the im- ended in conflict rr HT frequently when requests pact of repeated commercials, may provide some in- were based on premiums (29 percent vsonly 8 per- cereals)The sightsinto., theeffects of thischaracteristic on cent for requests for nonprenuum children's use of information from commercials. mothers reported that their children reacted some- what more negatively, with anger or disappointment, Intrafamny conflicts. Does premium advertising when their requests were based on a premium (table on television stimulate children torequesi purchases 4-6). Also, mothers who received premium-based by their parents, and do these requests, in turn, purchase requests reported a higher frequency of generate in-family conflict`' An early study by Ward arguments following the denial. and Wackman (1974) found a small, statistically sig- nificant correlation between the frequency of purchase requests and a general measure of parent- Another area a intrafamily conflict which has child conflict (r = .18, p.05) More specific data on not been explored to data is sibling conflict.Atkin this topic are provided in Atkin's (1975) super- hints at this when he cites among the reasons the market observations, which report that most mothers used in rejecting their children's purchase mothers yield to cereal requests in the supermarket requests in the supermarket that "other children in (75 percent). However, premium-based requests the family would fight over possession of the pre- were accepted somewhat less frequently (68 percent) mium." Unfortunately, Atkin does not give specific than requests based on other reasons (80 percent). figures on the frequency of this reason.

Table 4-6

Outcomes of Cereal Requests By Reason Cited for Request

Request because Request for of promiuma other reasons (n = 99) (n = 112)

When your child asks for a certain cereal, do you ever tell him/her that he/she can't have it? 78% Yes 72% No 22% 28% (If yes:) How does he/she react when you say no? Angry 6% 4% Disappointed/pouting 25% 18% 33% Doesn't bother child 34% Understands denial 8% 9% Persistence in request 2% 3% Substitute request 4% 4O/0 (If yes.) When you say that he/she can't have a cereal, how often do you argue with him/her? Woula you say a lot, sometimes, or never? Argue a lot/sometimes 42% 25% Argue never 36% 47%

Mothers were categorized into the "Premium" classificatio 1 if they cited premiums inresponse to the open-ended question con- cerning the reasons given by the child for wanting the cere II The "Otherreasons" category included those who originally gave nonpremium reasons, even thouch they subsequently respunded positivelyto the followup direct question about premium-based requests

Source Atkin, 1975g

7c fi In sum, the data concerning intrafamily conflict commercials containing premium uffers, and that appear to indicate that premium-based requests du they also have the least organized recall (Ward and increase conflictHowever, the frequency and Wackman's studies show the same general effects) seriousness of these conflicts have not yet been ex- and are very likely to confuse the purpose of pre- amined mium messages Some younger children may think the premium is the primary product Older children, lime' -ba,ed restrictionsA finalissue concerns however, are mu_ ibetter able to distinguish the whether the proposal for time limitations on pre- product from the premium offer In trying to answer mium offers within television commercials would be the questions posed at the beginning of this chapter, effective to ensure that the premium would be we must constantly distinguish the differences in viewed by children as a "secondary" feature of the children's levels of cognitive development. product Stump et al 's data, discussed earlier in this chapter, are directly relevant to this point, Children Premium offers do not appeal to significantly whb viewed the 30-second commercial in which half -affect children's recall of product attributes. Stages or more of the time was devoted to a premium of cognitive development are of much greater im- message has a lower recall of the product informa- portance in this area However, premiums some- tion than the children who saw a message for the times do far outweight all other product features in same product without the premium offer However, children's brand choices Again, we see this more those children who saw the commercial with a 10- frequently in younger children second premium message actually had a higher recall of product information than the group that It appears that there may he a relationship be- saw the no-premium version. This was true for both tween intrafamily conflict and purchase requests younger and older children. when premiums are involved Data are not available to indicate whether this conflict is greater than that As we noted, the authors argue that their findings involved in other product requests support limitation on time (10 seconds with:. second commercials) which could be devoted to a On the question of time-basedf estrictions on pr premium offer for maximum recall of a commercial mium advertising, data are inconclusive. Time pro- However, such a conclusion seems premature Time- portions may he a rough guide to influencing limitation proposals assume that children's attention children's attention patterns and selection of infor- to information is proportional to the amount of time mation, but more research is needed on (a) the rela- devoted to its presentationThis assume) that the tive efficacy of different cues in advertisements, and features of all commercial messages are weighted (h) the variations in attention and learning which equally, with the only difference being the time may result from differences in both the timing and devoted to them However, these assumptions run saliency of the content of commercials counter to much current literature on children's at- tention, which defines attention as a selective proc- ess which may bear no relation to the time devoted NEEDED RESEARCH to presentation To date, there is no sufficient infor- mation to- make a judgment regarding the possible The limited samples, and the analytic problems of effectiveness of time limitations Research is needed many of the studies reviewed here, might suggest which varies the time devoted to premium offers that top priority _,could be given to expansion and with other presentational characteristics of televi- partial replication of the research to date. Howev;er, sion advertising the findings from the studies of premium advertis- ing, plus basic developmental studies with children, would appear to he sufficient evidence that younger SUMMARY children weigh premium offers heavily in their atten- tion to and recall of advertising Whether such a It Tears that the key determinant of a child's reaction can properly be called "confusion" would likelihoodof being "confused" by premium seem to depend on how one defines the term At the messages is hisiher stage of cognitive development. very least, however, the reaction may he charac- Rubin') data shows that yourgc1 children have the terized as a tendency of younger children to evaluate ereatest difficulty in comprehending the purpose of other features of commercials less Judiciously than if

(I 72 O

these features had appeared in messages without pre- seem advisable to promote research on the effects of mium otters time allocations to premium and nonpremium con- tent That is. since not al, elements within a 30-sec- ond i.-pot are equally salient to children, allocating findingsare confirmedacross Because these specifictimesto premium and nonpronium several studies and are consistent with what one messages cannot m.and of itself ensure that children would expect from cognitive development theOry, will use all of the elements in making product and and because findings of age-related differences do brand evaluations Even if it were confnmed that not readily lend themselves to policy alternatives, children derhonstrate the greatest recall ot all com any replication and expansion of previous research mercial elements when 10 seconds is devoted to the along these Imes should not be the top priority for premium and 20 seconds devoted to other product future work Rather, priority should be given to features, ail adootion of such time restrictions would research which examines variNts techniques and not be warranted, since the presentation of commer- features in television commercials which might help cials could he designed to accentuate premium por- ch.ldren evaluate and use all of the elements of tions and "downplay'' the other portions of the ad- product and -brand information in forming judg- vertisements. ments about advertised products.In addition, a series of instructional television messages could be In summary, because industry guidelines endorse designed_ to help children evaluate the various the desirability ot ensuring that premium otters are features of commercials in making consumer deci- "secondary,- the chief research need atlas area is to sions identify efficacious methods of presentation to en- sure that children evaluate premium offer., in adver- Without some better understanding of presenta- tising messages, and consider them in the context of tional modes of children's commercials, it does not the product and the message

Si

73 Chapter 5

VIOLENCE AND UNSAFE ACTS IN TELEVISION COMMERCIALS DIRECTED TO CHILDREN

Although violence and unsafe acts are relatively 3. Unsafe act- in safety messages There has bet., infrequent in commercials directed at children, this little controversy regarding the proposition issue warrants specialattention because of the thatunsafeacts shouldnot be shown in seriousness ot the effects that may be involved. children's commercials. However, public serv-, Three potential problems are examinedinthis ice announcements (PSA's) which attempt to chapter, two relating to depictions of violence and teach children not to engage in unsafe behavior the third to depictions ot unsafe acts. The three are have generally been considered exempt from treated together here because their effects largely in- this proposition. The issue is whether the very volve a common mechanism among children 'nitra- portrayal of unrecommended or dangerous tion acts might wad to children's imitation of the acts. I Funtasv violence in (amino-rails When violence is depicted in commercials' is most likely to CURRENT AND PROPOSED CODES be in the form ot tantasyi e., in a cartoon or other make-belteve presentationTelevision Portrayals of violence in children's commercials industry spokesmen have long argued that are prohibited by both the NAB and the NAD. The "fan,.y violence" is not the same zs either NAB code explicitly recognizes the possibility of real-lite or realistic (acted) violence and that psychological as well as physical or behavioral children can distinguish the two. The usual effects. corollary to this arg.itoent is the belief that fan- Material shall not be used which can reasona- tasy violence is harthless and may even serve as bly be expected to frighten children or provoke valuable "cathartic" release for the audience. anxiety, nor shall material be used which con- We shall examine the evidence on children's tains a portrayal of or appeal to violent, abilityto distinguish fantasy violence from dangerous, or otherwise antisocial Ix ,avior realistic and real-lite violence, as well as tht types of effects that various lcIs of violent The NAB also prohibits dramatizations of any prod- portrayals may have on children. uct in a "realistic war atmosphere" and advertising of feature films ether than t' 'se appropriate for a Commercials adtacent to violent programs An general family audience. The NAD code is fairly increasing number of individual sponsors are similar. It prohibits portrayals of violence, appeals withdrawing commercial support from certain tofear, and other portrayals which corn;,ene television programs that tney consider to be generally accepted social, legal, or moral values. excessively violent This moralistically moti- v ated action hides a more serious considera- Portrayals of unsafe acts are also pro10,ited by tion for advertisers that commercials may ac- both codes. NAB's general policy is contai, to the tually interact with violent program content to following aftectv,-wers; andspecificallychildren's, behavior and attitudes toward violence i We Advertisements and products aovertised shall shall review some recent etidence which sug- be consistent with generally recognized stand- gests that commercials may heighten the im- ards Of safety. Advertisements shall not in- pact ot violent program content in certain cir- clude demonstrations of any product in a man- cumstances, while reducing its impact iri ner that encourages harmful use or dramatiza- ' others tions of actions inconsistent with generally recognized standards of safety. The NA D code s similar. In general, it prohibit.. ,.n- I Interaction effects arc also of interest to advertisers in that violent or exuting programs may influence a commercial's efleL- safe detnonsti "lions and portrayal of people tiveness engaged in unsafe acts However, the NAD code 7.62 aft kLs porira!,!! -I an unsafe !Lt it the poi traLaloc- popular sports programs (kr h!,..:h are to he seen cursin a "specticsittetymessage The NAB hy many older children esumahh makes this c%ception also. although their odes do not eLplicitly cosr the satch, message During the v.eekday afternoon time slot, a tradi- Lont,JgelLy tionally heaV51e5Ing period f)1\T, (mug audiences. liareus (1975b) found a number of commehJals pro- moting teleisi in programs 5), hiLh typically eontiin INCIDENCE Liolent action hus, children are likel\ tr see some ,tots of physical viinence in current commercials, 110h iii t m t omMen ridst_ntortunately. the most although the number probably does not teach the recent content arid'sis of children's commercials Ih-percent figure estimated h\the1971'A [nick (BarLus, 1975a) did not employ a wiolence category data In an earlier content analysis of children's commer- cials m l' 71. V et al (1973) found aI h per- Another category of LommerLials v)hich are of Lent incidence of Lnildren's Lmmercials that %Lout(' concern here arc those(k, hut emphis potentially ,rtrohith' \meet Get biter's (19-'2i definition of frightening or tear-arousing themes The presenta- ense inhis content ana ,sisfor the Surgeon tionofsuch themesisprohibited hy current (tenet-al', report On LiolenLe an(h. Lrt expres children's codes, but lust under onc-Lthird (29 per- ',pm ut phLsiLal force against others or sell, or (a) Lent ) of the 1971 commercials studied Winid( et Lomp_Iling of .Ltion ag4inst one's skill on pain of al has h.se had this characteristic in their use of hang hurt or killed I he (tcrttner definition in- hilarre story settingsRii,irre spokesmen appeared dudes -orthiyals t.:a!-LLorld ntlence gfunks inI() percent 14 these commercials, noinsters and hint of lights or (t,artare, t ecitam.sportsas \Nell as %,itches in 7 percenttit comparable Lategowation the fartiasy s mlence children _.re most likely to see in LLas used in the :nitre recent Rareus st idles, so \e do progiams o. outline' Lials not knoss the current incidence t.ffrightening or tear arousing themes In the past task Learxfi:iP. tien- I he Lommehnals citedht, miLketid all mg or tear-arousing themes scent to has e become einploLA dlamsin their product presentations lesscunt ro( ersiiiithan Moreits -rtN,s iolent I \ piLdIsituationsint:oh:mgstll,ims are portrayals drago9 des ourira s illage. gangster!, (Lalking Although not stated in an\ of the asailahle Lon- taLingIL timard a Lounterteln aSias e master ih....!rseeing gal leL slat. c estout had _ ti-ntanalyses.itseems safeto[flierthatmost man threatening a poker cheater and ( aunt L iidenLe or tear-arousing themes Pt childu's Loni- Disieula holding a %whin (p3(t) inercials are make-helieLe portrayals and usually unmatedThis contrasts ith tendent. y tImard Som.'Loritemporai sLommerciols,Mould,ilk no:laminated ,onituLreials for clirtdten in general meet (ierhner's physical iolence definition, the BarLu, i1975,11 seckendit tuning count of although these are not necessarily sho(Ln during or children's commercials listedit) pet cent animated, awaL lit to childrenprograms FxamplLs are an percenmi,ed. and 'X r,t Lent winatun aced anunatLd col:nut:ILIA for a sNel1 knoL),n- brand of ( ompatable iigures for \Lei...iii\ afternoon Loininci rut drink and a comincr..ial for a lesset knimi were 12 percent, ant: -8 percent hi and of beer, both of tylua shim, a hosing match Bin Lus, 1-)751» \hother commercial tot aloss calorie beer Lon-

Llu, Ls %kith the line,And if Lou don t belie% e me I!Hit tit t k 1 he modenLe of 111 hreok Soul n f he k,cr caturlterciats are not L mime in L.Jren's prograni, is r cies ant tool!! ex- thteLted at children, of course hilt tilt:\ are airy .1 oat amination of ehitettets Li mmeroals tor the

,tc (1,-1,f11.1 rItil 111%, dlIiII,r (,,, yeti r ',it i,t hc.tit Lniiiun, do , t1, I h, and the -tulthee tltrf,1;4 erett,tt.0,1e .2 "1" ,,t the, lit mdkr,LrIdent al, tr,!1`'mph. 5/ ,otith; r

It,its '1, heir kJ, 11,2. a hIt.11,t. ,0111.11 s.Iue1',ni ,( I II e / ( 1 ht.ttip Oho and ,r//t ill d rate tete.lt ; e I'I, tie., nnc ,cr,,nI 14-2, 14-, iett t,inrf ci, , telI hi' ,11, ect k ,LPir,oru,tr ilk ,r le, t, d inr '1, ocn,11,_ &rink. th, i, rl

L stated earlier name's, the possihilitv of interaction dence fittures for potentialls trightening or tear- between the :mufflers:Os and siolent nrograin con- arousing themes in the program, (see table 5 -2) tent A related possibilio,is that toung,er children Data arc again from ftireus I1975a) are unabletodiscri, Amite commeicials and program content, especiallyithoth proar tit and I Ao general conclusions ale appa, nt nom the commercial are animated tii -uresIntahle 5-2First, children ,ateLing ,Lckend morning programs may encounter unitafa- A recent studs ot19's 7t,Satit das imriung e character-produced rather that) children's programs hsMedia\).tHm Research naturaloraccidental actsin atleasthaltthe ( enter' tound that an aggressis e act occUrtCd es ery programs Second, situations sshich could r Iten- tvo) minutes, rising to almost once a minute in car- Halls produce tear. amsietvor at least generalised toon programs' Barcus (I.l"sa) pros ides a BIM C aiousel or ecdation in children 111,ts occur in at detailed breakdossn along %kith a 0)111pAtison of the least halt the programs figures from tour sears , triter tHarcus, 19'1) d,ihle s-1 shows the plot-relcanLe of these ,01cnt .11.11 malc af m onmzet, ;al%Xs ,sith the lk t (1, Accordingtothesefigures -Os percentofthe I, rolence, the _item bpi dctiamg unsafe acts are children's weekend morning programs anal\ zedin not alw clear -cut Some critics allege that certain 1975 contained violence hs the (lerhner definition products, such as tiressorks ur trugs. are m them- In nail of these programs s hilene s.ibis the plot hut sels es ups tic lot children and that merely to ads el these ssere all corned, programs and, therefore. the Use them constitutes an unsate act11, this chapter. 'saturated usage 'ot sioleace ssas pEohahls all in the sse skill canine our discussion to acts sshich etas fantasy idiomRealistic acts ofWit:MC also oc result in phssical harm to children regardless ot the curretCin about halt ofall the sseckend morning product associated \Anti the actions programs mhe noncomeds program t, but pies), na- Hs at a considerahls.ossei rate in that these acts ssere 'subordinateit. the plot P d.Ittrt.rat t t its tt I , HII Isisth it ,,,1111(dr its phs.l, tltitrt.t1

t, tit,rrIs I III, t.,thht. ,tth,t't.t.sitn tilethottn Since imitation rotas he It a tunction ot type th 'uh tht.tr tur, I tc 1)rt the 'niter h cid up portrayal than of rate,'itis useful to e \amine a 1,,L phI1,,t1 Hire it hart! Irtittr breakdossn (it the t\pes of s ',dello that occur led tit u1,1 t,pc,ltllt Ht.in 1,11111111 r I,It It, tits. wpm, Pr the c(mted, and nima)mech pr()grams and the trh., HI11,1111,,,11111,,Ii1,t N1,1,11114,1 Is th, ,,il. , .1, tlf I {Ink I, '' )th pr,0111,111- r tt -1; t.r,ti. t1,, ItLd HI OILIt ' (

I 'Is,,1 I,I t- t\^ I itstti(,,, Rine. t, mirtults ttirce r tdtIP , tit and II'' 1,111118 '11 CI, HI dr tin t I iftt, 'I ot I p the., r; ( tyro ir, d his r. petit o ol `" IL", 101e rr t,1' " 111,1 'PI ItI HI mist', Hr. 1,1,1 1,1 (

LittI HI I

Table 5-1 Plot Relevance of Violence for Weekend Morning Cnildren's Programs

!Iercent of Percent of Percent of Percent of comedy noncomedy all programs all programs programs programs (1975) (1971)

Saturated usage; plot would not 35 0 29 30 exist without it Subordinate usage, could be 25 46 29 23 eliminated without afkrt,ng plot Total 60 46 58 53

Sou", Barcus) 1-175a) Table 5-2 Types of Violence. in Weekend Morning Chi!dren's Programs

Percent of Percent of Percent of Percent of comedy noncomedy all programs all programs (1975) (1971)

Human violence weapons 58* 15 50 57 . Human violence without weapons 35* 15 31 37 Death or injury as a result of 19 39 23 (NC)** violence Natural or accidental violence 1 2 31 16 (25) Fear-arousal s, Pons Chase scenes 53 39 5O (53) Frightening situations 11 77 24 (NC)

Since the comedy programs incLided cartoons, these figures include animated portrayals Noncomparable coding criteria in tne 1971 study Soui,e Barcus (1971 1975a)

Recent incidence figures for portrayals of unsafe RESEARCH EVIDENCE acts are difficult to obtainWinick etal(1973) tot.nd that 4 7 percent of children's commercials in The rev' w of ev rdenccn v iolence and unsafe 1971 portrayed "an activity or situation inconsistent acts in children's television commercials is with generally recognized safety standards These organized according to the three issues identified in Included a person driving a truck with one hand and the introductory section of this chapter The first a situation in which two children are shown eating in issue concerns fantasy violence, the type most likely a rowboat without ault supervision The more re- to he utilized in, children's commercials\\ e will cent studies by Barcus (1975a, h) did not screen Locus initially on the question of whether ch,.aren commercials for depiction of unsafe acts, although arc capable of detecting the cue; which distinguish such acts undoubtedly occut red in some of the com- fantasy violence from more realistft -porti ayals of mercials One example would he the celebrated violence Then we will see whether this makes any "wild plants" cereal commercial (Poulo,,, 1975), difference in terms of children's behav Hu, including another would he a current commercial which shows both the short-run Meets in the in-homeiewing a child licking a kmfe used for scooping peanut but- context and the longer-run eft -cIs, such as interpei- ter sonal aggression, tolerance of aggression, and catharsis Since the industry and commercial groups are agreed on theundesirabilityofunsafeactsin The second issue concerns the interaction be- children's commercials, the main issue here is the tween commercials and violent programs A e shall depiction of unsafe acts in public service.announce- see that Itis necessary to go beyond the quest 'in of ments The incidenceit PSA s addressed to immedi- whether commercials are violent and to consider the ate physical safety (rather than long-term physical or potentially favorable or unfavorable role that com- mental health) is relatively low during ch:l iren's mercials may play in the overdll viewing experience viewing hours During the morning hours the three Finally, we will examine the ev 'demi: on portrayals major networks over one weekend, Barcus (1975a) of unsafe actsInparticular, we will examine counted I() safety commercials (bike safety, seat- whether unsafe acts can he sately portrayed in safety belts, etc ) This figure gives us some idea of the inci- ,sages to children dence of safety commercials, but no count is availa- ble as to how inai,f these PSA's depicted unsafe Children's pert epoon of lamas' ( UV\ So tar in this acts as a means of demon' tracing tf e safety message report we have followed the definition violence

78 used m Gerhner (1972)Critics of this definition to labelItas fantasy, the felllmt -t.tting of make have argued that no account is takenit(ties which belieseit may also hay e humui assi cue1 v ',dent may render the violence less real or nteaningtulI or act in a fictional hut ttay al may he Ws- example, Gene Mater, a( BS y ice-presidentre- 'anguished front real p atravals cently offered the following criticism hen Bugs one cue the viessets knowledge that it Is a fictional Bunny pours a pitcher of milk «y5.a Llopmunk s setting and ih it the port) ayal is acted (the beer coin head, that's violence to Dean (Jerbner 1),(50o look intrcials mentioned eat het would ht th's category ) uvon that as violence' I sure as hell don t (14)111 y uncut act 'n a re,r1 poi trawl has hy definition 110 Street Jourmt!, October 19, 197(1)I he real question, fantasy cuesitis, huwey cr 14/rtfci\li clam Iive,tr obviously, is not whether 'Yir\later sees this as on tape, rather than the actual tot tto; event violence, but whether children do- In other words, are children capahie of detecting thefantasy cues' idence that children canintact distinguish that might allow them to -discounta portrayal as these tour types of y unent portrayals %,o, provided not really violent' in a study by Snow (1974) Open-ended am v revs s using a structured format. wore conducted with Si) The 1972 Surgeon Genet al's report on tetes tsioit middle-class boys and girlsIu of them aged 4 to violence concluded that young children arc not and 31 aged 9 to 12Sllov, first asked the Lhii,l; en capable of detecting fantasy cues about lour different types of programs to see which theywould labelas make belieyc and which The very young laY e difficulty comprehending (cal 1 he results ai e stifIlljtal tied inI able 4 the contextual setting in which vdent acts arc depicted and do not grasp the meaning of cues or labels concerning the make believe It would appear that the h..))))a and rerrot5 settmg titlie«iti /zed %ACP, NUM,. ILIttto signify to even the characterofy iolenceappealsits fictional yohngest children in the sample that the program programs was make-belly e On the ''then 'laud, another Lon Howes er, only one study (I 'titer an51.1Lol)ert19'2) (LiaporarY but mote realistic setting illiaas 1111,01) was cited in support of this conciusum Th's and two ploduo'd, as we might expect, mixed ratings tri)111 more reset,studies are revie'yed below,Before ex- the childrenwith the younger ),:isildrenr, _ amining these studies, however, we hive found it ticularly , regarding the program as real or at least worthwhile to attempt a classification on %totem mire real or (ealtsoc than it is make-behey rI Ho. 1- portrayals based on the fantasy -cue hypothesisIn soon new, - {as it_garded as real by all tine children T-4)1e 5-3, types of violent portrayals arc itlentified these results suggest that childrenby the age of in an approximate continuum of fantasy -cue associa tour, can distinguish real from make stffitent tions un a general hosts

A violent act in a cartoon to gthe fruit di ink commercial mentioned 5:5-her) typical'y has titres \IT. i. t,F, cues to label it as fantasy animation hums'[old it .1' .1.'; .1 ,;. 1 many eases a is remote in type 1115 ..1 place A violent act by hamans in a irk ikt 111,1 R, o + in 1,h! _ii , r1, ,.(+11.1, beheye porrayal or settinge g science action r, , 1 % a pr.,:rt 1.is 1 period settings 5.owns) has at I,ist ors L tic

Table 5-3

Types of Violent Portrayals and Possib!e Fantasy Cues

Possible fantasy cues Cartoon violenre animatior,, humor remote setting Human, make-believe violence humor, remote setting Human, realistic (acted) violence fictional setting Human, real-I fe violence no 4antasy cues Table 5-4 fantasy cues present, however, a significant portion of the children, especially the younger children, Children's Perc.dptIon, of "Real" vs. "Make- identified the portrayals as violence Believe" Programs A sl'ghtly different approach was used in the Percent describing study by Liefer and Roberts (1972) These inv?.stiga- programs as "make- ,ors were interested not so much in what children believe" choose to label as violence, but in whether children 4-8 yrs. 9-12 yrs. can understand the "motives" and "consequences" Saturday morning 100 100 associated with various types of violence v Based on cartoons ir, _rviews with the children following their viewing Bewitched 100 100 of six programs, Liefer and Roberts found that kin- Brady Bunch 26 45 dergarten children could answer only about one- News 0 third of the questions on motives and consequences, 3d graders about hal; of the questions. and 6th grad- Source Snow (1974) ers about 80 percent A later study by Collins (1975) found that even by the 5th grade, children men- Next, Snow asked the children which of a scree of tioned less than half of the motives and conse- portrayals (described yerbally, not shown) they quences cited by adults in subsequent descriptions of would regard as ylidence the word "Y iolence- was violent portrayals Children apparently focus on the explained to the children as "physically or verbally acts themselves hurting someone the results are summarized in I able 5-5, and then ht the fantasy Luc hypothesis The most interesting facet of the Liefer and yery well Roberts study was that the children's understa 'ding of motives and consequences was shown to he inde- 1he data ill the table indicate that the more fan- pendent of the type of program they had viewed tasy cues preconted in a portrayal the itire likely Fantasy violence, as depicted in cartoon progi ams I the portrayal as that th'e children would to: or make-believe wester ns, was no better understood yirdenceI he order of erre, eness of the fantasy by the children than realistic violencL in adult crime cues appeared to he as tolh)Vt'l animafion. humor, tit MILIS In addition, adults proved to he very poor and a renuiite setting,I ntortunately an example a iudges of which sequct the children would he realistic portrayal re g /Joh«,Stns) was notrn- cIuded in the..now stud'.1() we cahoot gauge the etteLticenessur a fictional pr Tram cue alone It is \IttIR et,are used here in the sense tit pt itched mint reasonably site bet, howcyer, that su h examples it tst tttr ih tel (surfeit se t:uest while Lonsequent..esrefer would tecetse somewhere t,s,veen us and IOU pet H the tut,,,n tt the aLt t,thiet_ttt LuestI ht, tsuhtt:LtRe-Met_ LcIlt `1"1""1"10-TIltql latme.,I sett with yarious e fur ,Itstirkti,tn Nis suggested 1-", Sul, end (tutkin (14"h)

Table 5-5

Portrayals Regarded as Violence by Children

Percent describing portrayals as violence Age 4-8 yrs. 9-12 yrs.

Roadrunner cartoons (a,h,r) 26 16 Nonanimated clowns fighting (1-,r) 47 36 GunsmoKe (r) 68 65 News film of Vietnam wa' 100 100 a animated h humor r remote setting Source Snow able to understand accord.ng to the mom es-and- Effeas of different types of violent portrayals We consequences ccueria have identified v la the fantasy-cue hypothesis, four different types of violent portrayals on television What are the implications of theLiefitr and cartoon violence (with three fantasy cues), make- Roberts study and the more recent studies by Snow believe violence (two cues), realistic violence (one aid colhns) cue), and real -life violenceI he first two types are, or have been, employed in children's commercials I 1 he fantasy -v iolence position adonted by many net win k spokesmen scenisini,iallyto Ther! are at least eight types of elf et is which ex- be supported by the research, tha. children, posure to various types ot violent portrayals may even as young as 4 years, can fairly reliably tell produce These range from direct Imitation to direct the difference between tantasv content and counter-imitation, with six possibilities in between, realistic content To organize these typologies, and to provide a 2 This does not meanthatfantasycontent framework for classifying research in the area, a removes the label ot v lolence Even the most summary table is provided in Table 5-6 Note that strongly cued fantasy (animated, humorous, tour 01 the possible effects are negative by usual and remote from real life, such as Roadrunner societal standards, while lour other effects variables or BugsBurns' cartoon programs) may he (number 5 and 6) are either neutral or positive labeled as "violent- by as man, as one in tour 4 to 8 year olds Table 5-6 ako identifies two important mediating 3 This labeling is based largely on purely "physi- variables in this effects pi ocess the covert eimmtive cal" fantasy cues (as noted) and not on under- and emotional (arousal) responses to televise n con- standingof more sophisticated"ci tent which may serve as mental or physiological cues cues, such as the motives or consequences for subsequent overt responses Arousal is a par- ass )ciated with the violent action ticularly important mediating variable because, as a physiologicalenergizer,itlargely determines 4 11th,ighthestudies dealtwith isipn whether any mentally acquired responses to violence progra,a sequences, there seems to be no com- will actually appear as overt behavior Although pelling reason why these conclusions would cognitive labeling and arousal are the twt, most im- differ for violent themes employed in telev portant mediating variables, a number of other, sion commerc. ils mainly personal churazteristic may also be mediat- 5 I he most important conclusion is that these ing factors in the behav !oral effects of the various research esults do not indicate whether tan types Of television v iolence O these, we have listed tasv violence has any Less effect then r:alistic ag, because-of its obvious relevance to children's iolence The results indicate only that various research, and . child's past history ot reinforcement types ofviolent portrayals may he labeled ofaggressive or violent behaviorPsychologists differently by youngsters according to their ini- generally consider pastreinforcement as aroot tial perceptual responses to Cie content c,ouse of what is commonly known as a predisposi- tion for aggressive or violent behavior in children The next Icgical question, obviously, is whether these differentialperceptions of violent content With this framework as a guide, we can now ex- make any difference in children's behavior following amine the two questions most relevant to an assess- exposure to various types ot violent portrayals ment of the type, of fantasy violence that might be employed in children's commercials F it.whether p,N,psychol,,gist adults oils differ ,ith pschIligists fantasy violence produces negative effects, and sec- regarthng their concepts ,ititdence In an interesting msestiga ond, whether fanta, violence and more realistically tum of the ,alidity Luc-labeling ettects, Kane, Joseph, and violent portrayals produce different effects. 1?.deschi (1976) asked college students to eatu Ate pr",..edural desLriptions from the classic.. Herk(iwitt experiments n aggres- sion One mat or finding was thathats! -doing behave was Answer., to the first question derive from a twin- labeled as 'aggressive- only when its perceived nwti.athin was ar.ti no-mative rtartq d nog behras torhat was either instigated her of sti.es in which television fantasy violence is by ano,ther person or due t'an attaLG b!. an"ther persun was mot compared with a control condition of bland or non- aheled aggreSS)%c violenttelevisionNequenLes. The following,in

81 chronological order, are summary re',ultl, the ma- probably not doe to imitation, but to signifi- jor studies which have employed cartoons asthe fan- cantly higher arousal engendered by the car- tasy-violence stimulus toon programs, since the children recalled fewer aggressive incidents from the cartoons, Siegel (1956) found no statisti«illy sivnificant yet rated the cartoons as significantly more ex- ofinterpersonal differenceinfrequency citingThis study has other methodological assaults in children's play following exposure nroblems, too, which renderits findings to a Woody Woodpecker cartoon vs a bland equivocal cartoon. Subjects were Stanford University nursery-school children and thus were proba- Nonconservative, even biased evaluations of the bly upper-middle class first tour of these studies are widespread For exam- Bandura, Ross, and Ross (1963), using the ple, Liehert et al (1973), cite Siegel's study as show- "Bobo doll" measure and arms:tie (frustrating) ing "somewhat more" (original italics) aggression the children slightly, found a significant in- following theviolent cartoon when intactthe (-ease in physical and verbal assaults against differences were not statistically significant Com- the doll following exposure to a televised car- stock (19;6) citesthe 13andura etalresults as toon-like portrayal of t'le ...arne behavior by a though cartoon violence and realistic violence led to lady dressed in a cat cc,.tume vs no-exposure similar" levels of aggression, when in fact they did control Subjects were again from the Stanford not Co ostock also fails to mention the presenceof nursery school frustration or arousal the measurement situation, mentioned Steuer, Applefield, and Smith (1971) founti an even though these two facets are clearly in an earlier reviev by the same author(Comstock, apparent tendencfor increases in interpersonal physical assaults in play following the 1975) Both Liehert et al and Comstock ( 1975) cite children's exposure to Satarday morning the Steueretal experiment as supporting the program excerpts A majority of the excerpts violence-aggression hypothesis when the results are Finally, there is the Stein and were violent cartoons, the control excerpts tentative at best Friedrich experiment Liehert et alcite this study were nonviolent Subjects werenursery-school children of mixed race and SFS scores Results uncritically as supporting the violence-aggression hypothesis, as does Comstock (1975) The Surgeon are tentative since the experiment used only General's report (1972) correctly points out the five pairs of children (10 total), three of which possibility of a regression artifact confounding the showIan apparent increase inaggression while two didnot The measure is also ques- results, yet concludes that they plausibly support the tionable because it depends partly on retalia- y lolence-aggression hypothesis tory action by the "control' child in each pair As an illustration of how later interpretations of Stein and Friedrich (1972) found equityual iolence research can va7y, it is worthwhile to ex- results following children's exposure to six Bat- amine the Stein and Friedrich experiment in greater man and six Superman cartoon programs vs detailThe rest- archers divided their sample into neutral programming over a tour -week period high and Low aggressives_based on pre-exposure ob- Subjects were nursery-school children with servations of physical and verbal aggression and a mixed SES scores The results cannot he taken sum measure of the two called "interpersonal ag- as either clearly favoring or not favoring the gresrion" (how one division was derived from three fantasy-violence hypothesis median scores was not stated) The children were Heiler and PolAcy (19761 found (-go:vocal then reobserveddlowing exposure to either violent resultsininterpersonalphysicalaggression cartoons or neutral programs. The regression ar- following childrtn's exposure to five violent tifact phenomenon would predict that the aggression Saturday morning cartoon programs vs five scores of the two groUps v.ould, from pre to post, bland children s programs Subjects were 20 merge toward the overall sample mean - -i e , scores psychiatrically normal but"broken home" of high aggressives would decline and scores of low child ren in a Pnliadelphia boarding institution aggressives would increaseIn the presence of this Althoughthecartoonprograms produced artifact, the only convincing evidence that the ex- more aggression than the bland programs, in- perimental treatments had any effect would. be it the spection of the data indicates that this was pre-post changes were directionally opposite to the artifact's"pull,- and significantlyso. No such physical aggression measure did any significant changes were observed The low agife4sives showed change occur high aggressives' scores in the violent slight increases on all three aggress4 measures cartoon ondition simply declined significantly less which were entirely in line with regression toward than their scores in the neutral condition, this is not the mean This occurred regardless of violentor the same as saying that they increased significantly neutral exposure, thus clearly indicating no violence more, yet this is the interpretation that is implied effect The conservative interpretation, given that regres- sion toward the mean would predict a decline and The high aggressives turned out to be the critical that the conclusions are based on declines rather group on which cons lusions were based On the ver- than on counter-regression increases, would be no bal aggression measure, the high aggressives' scores violence effect showed slight declines in line with the regressionar- tifact, again regardless of type of exposure On the Actually, Stein and Friedrich could have gotten physical aggression measure, which many would around the regression problem by simply dropping argue is the mcre serious, the high aggress-mes' scores the high aggressive low aggressive split and either stayed the same (violent cartoons) or declined analyzing the trends for the total sample. Based on slightly (neutral programs) but these were not sig- their data in table 15, p. 246, the total sample results nificantly different patterns, i e, no violence effect can be estimated as follows can he concluded Only on the combined verbal and

Table 5-7

Pre-post change scores Violent cartoons Neutral programs

Interpersonal aggression (PA + VA belT.v) 015 -.022 Physical aggression .010 -013 Verbal aggression 005 -.010

The differences between the violent cartoon and bly, even from commercials (although the latter neutral program groups are, respectively, .037, 023, seems unlikely, given the infrequency of commer- and .005 scale points Given that a difference of 104 cials of this type) We shall return later to the scale points was required for the 05 level of signifi- arousal aspect of violent portrayals in commercials. cance, almost three times the magnitud: of the largest difference here, Stein and Friedrich's overall A second question-concerns the matter of whether results would almost certainly be nonsignificant fantasy violence and more re alistic violent portrayals produce different effects (This question In summary, it is clear that car:oon violence can deals with the differences between the stimulus-con- instigate aggression in children, although this effect tent variables in the left-hand column of Table 5-6 )

is by ho means a universal outcome nor an unequi - Various studies have employed various comparisons cal one In fact, the findings of most studies are con- in regard to this question; the three studies cited sistent with the interpretation that aggression occurs below all involve cartoon violence and at least one only if the children's arousal levels are raised (either other more realistic form of violence by the cartoons or by other subsequent means, such bandura, Ross, and Ross (1963) found that as frustration during play). Moreover, the effect of depictions of violence.by a cartoon model and the cartoon violence is mainly to energize preexist- by d filmed human model produced the san:: ing aggressive responses, rather than to stimulate im- level of subsequent "Bobo doll" aggression by itation of new responses (this phenomenon is seen the children (A live human model produced especiallyinthose studies whichmploy pre- significantly more aggression than the cartoon measures of physical aggression and then use post- model, but the difference between the live and measures of the -cone behavior to gauge the effects) filMed models-was not significant I The children may have learned these preexisting responses from previous cartoons, from other televi- Liefer and Roper ts (1972) found the same level sion programs, from personal experience, or, possi- of aggression regardless of whether the violence

1 ( ) 83 portrayals were in cartoon, make-believe, or Interpreted as inditteren«'. as is shown by a third ex- realistic programs However, the researchers' periment measure of aggression, which involved -pro- Drabman anu Thomas (1976) reported the tective" picture choices rather than actual in- results of an experiment in which one group of terpersonal aggression, was somewhat indirect children individually watched realistic McCarthy et al (1975), in a longitudinal field violence (a TV detective show excerpt) while study, found that children who lacked favora- another (control) group were shown an excit- ble real-life experiences turned to television. ing but nonviolent baseball sequence After ex- including violent programs, as an apparent posure, the children were led to believe that a fulfillment mechanismTheir viewing of tight was breaking out between two children in violent programing was also associated with another room The children exposed to the fighting at school and juvenile delinquency violent film took a significantly longer time to records. Among these children, there were no summon help, thus suggesting that exposure to differencesinaggressive behavior between televised fantasy violence eitherincreased those who characteristically watened violent their tolerance oproduced indifference cartoon or make-believe programs and those toward real violence who watched more realistic programs contAtn- ing violence. Thus, the answer to the fantasy violence versus realistic violence question is complex On the one hand, there is no clear evidence that highly fantastic Three other experiments are worth mentioning. violence (cartoons) produces any more aggression Two of these compared realistic acted violence with than moderately fantastic violence (make-believe) teat -fur c iolence or even less fantastic violence (realistic but acted) Feshhack (1;72) showed a film of a real-life On the other hand, none of these levels of portrayed civil not to two groups of 9 to 11 year olds violence seem to produce as much aggression as One group was told it was a Hollywood movie, real-life violence the other was sold the truth Compared with a third control group. which did not see the film. It is possible that arousal. rather than any blind the -real- group engaged in significantly more learning or imitatton of violent content. may he the interpersonalaggression. and the "movie' critical factor. Such a theory was advanced as early group in significantly less interpersonal aggres- as 1968 by Zillman (who, in turn, based his theory sion. subsequent to the film These results have on a theory of drive and ptrformance proposed by beeninterpretedas support forafantasy psychologist Clark Hull). The theory, which has violence-catharsis hypothesisThe results cceive'l .:xtensive -empirical support in the Ai)o k of would not he consistent with a differential Zillman a id his colleagues, postulates that people arousal hypothesis, unless it could be shown will perfoi m whatever response is salient or ap- that the -movie- idea produced -de-arousing.- propriate to then: in a given situation. provided they perhaps skeptical reactions among the are sufficiently aroused or energized A notable children feature of the theory is that the arousal may derive from any excitational eventThat is. an exciting No',Ie (1973) found similar results after show- sports program, a loudr frustrating commercial in- inga war film documentary and an "ar- terruption, conceivably an exciting commercial, or tistically produced" fictional war film to 6 to 7 even a video failure could have the same effect of year olds Although there was no control con arousal as a violent television sequence. ifthese damn, the real -life portrayal produced more events eLcited equivalent degrees of emotional e,- destructive and less constructive play than the citation fictional portrayal. Since the content differed between the films. this result would he consis None of the maior studies on television violence ent with a differential arousal interpretation and its effects on children is immune from the criti- cism that arousal of preexisting responses, rather Both studies imply that realistic acted violence than the learning of new responses, (Amid have pro- (bLi not depictions of real vm!er.-e) may have a duced the observed effects For the types of violence pacifying effec, 1-1,,vo2ver, this effect might better he portrayed occasionally in «moneruals, and also the 91 84 types portrayed most frequently in children's exampl,-, older girls in the experiment demonstrated pro,trams, thereissimply no clear evidence ot a very large increase to he/pini,, responses following "blind- imitationIn the few children's studies in the televised violence which aggression has occuried following exposure to s, '

( 2d t es As negative and signith al(p and does not support of what emit child peIeeleell AS the normal ornormatis: their claim that the two response, ire inviependent 1, svrist Oltt NpolinielltAl situation In adopting this asign, the ins estigators argued Worchel et aldid not take direct physiological that must prey mu) experiments were at,pi,al in that measuresofarottsalduring the experiment they employed short film segments. which usualls However, atter the viewing, the subjects rated the portrayed violence Out of context. rather than full- films as fairly equal's instils mg and interesting (the length films which Lontamed contexts other than ratings of their siolence and humor contents violenceIn addition. they noted that films shown taturall,tat differed)Subjects who viewed the films without commercials were atypical of usual teles '- with commercials also rated themselses as feeling mon exposureI he researchers reasoned that under slightly (though _:ruticantiv) more tense and angry the conditions of their experiment. two competing atterthe slew mg. but alsoparadoxicallymore hypotheses could he testedI he first, following happy and rested I helatter two ratings pose Singer (19711. was that commercials 'nightbreak problems for the hush at ion hypothesis, especially in the spell'' of siolence sequences. in the films and thus that the ins estigators speculate that the commercials decrease aggressise tendencies of the suhiects could have led simultaneously to hoth frustration following stewingI he alterns)tise prediction. and repel following Berkowitz (1962) was that the cmmercial There are other pioblems with this interesting nterruptions might Lonstrtute a source off rustra- study (ion. since they osteisihly block the ''ongoing drys 1 I hecommer.lswere actuallypobit. ,crvice of the viewer to see the mos toI he conihmat ion of ilimmunLements antiaredesert:dbythe frustration plus the aggressike cues in the s iolent authors as "tairls entertamm.1..Nloreok cr. th:- musics should increase the aggressise tendencies of .(fi-ding to the methodological description In the subjects tolhoss, mg s lest rig the studs. the commercials (10 commercials arranged in tour 2- minute blocks) were placed V orchel et al preferred the Berkowitz predittion so asto mo,,t breaking stens. continuity' of Increased aggression following the s iolent mos les (italics added)I his is ratl.er atypical of tele\ I- shown With commercials Thes made no prediction sion, in which commercials are oiten placed at regarding the retain]: effects of realI -Iftua) sersus the peak of an exciting scene, soth the resolu- acted ( lid (hne) siolence hcLause of their Loncern tion left to follow the Comm rani break that the depiction ot the unpleasant real -late conse- quences of the s iolenee m ittp a Wright introduce an 2 Itis possible that the pl,tement of PSA's inhibiting fat tor in the subletts suh,equent aggres- s iolent films may !lase had a pm limit,' ettett or, more presrsely. a nrunt/4.7ins; effect Look sion again at the re.,ults mFiguh 8-1 One would expect 'aggression- to he indexed hs a tend- The main measure of txperiment enty not t()1/4 ant to hire the assistant Hoseser cOnstsftd of the subjects %. lo tate the none 1.1the (mean) ratings tend in this ag- luployahilits of the LsLarL:11 asi I withad run pests direction the dominant response in the films I eassistant Jenne, a'..: thice tour of the six experimental conditions was in blunders. two root to and one Jolioss mg (dn., tau ti hp( the assistantIn thi two remaining Much would pr, >vide the s Jisit_ts with t,. ,sons ; conditions-- the two s ittlent tilms with Lom- ituatang his tor ,etc-c;as a measure of aggres- met Lialst heresponses wereessentially Is itt sum) the result, from .ire sh tsii neutral t r theratingsindicatedan - figure 5-8 di? (hitt:rents'. as to \lhether the stssh,ant should or v.) should not he hired (the midpoint of the stale I he findings, re consistent with the Ms t_.12.itq-s was 16, and the two means in question were predictions Based on theassist,mi ratingnicasuic 16,6 1()I/he /id (hie and Is S totittp of aggression, theiolent ftlnts shown with tommei Itwouldhediftisulitointerpretsuch oats apparengy produced stt,,ntlicanth ni,( aggrcs responses asaggressise Anilternatis in- mon than the same films shown %1/410(lt Lut111111.1 terpretation,based on thearodsalIneors, LIdIS 1 he siolent hlms with tomtits-L.1,11s also pro night be thot the dominant or',ippropriate duffed significantly more aggression than the non- response in the subjects' situation was to tom- siolent film with or +. allow commerLials signiri ply with the experimenters' apparent judgment cant different es in the aggression ratings were found in ahed\ has mg hired the assistant or, mote het .seen :he acted and real s iolemc Illudsto gist' aY:111patileil(ffulp011'IP in a

9 :3 Figure 5-8

Subjects' Ratings of the Employabilit) of the Research Assistant Js

Definitely31[- should not30 hire*

28

26

24

22

20

18 16.6 (n.s.) 15.5 (midpoint)16 with (sig.) commercials 14 (sig . ) t 12 (sig.)

10 $ 4 8.8 ..-.4 .-- 4without ..". 8 (n.s.) .... 9.5 commercials 11'... 9.2 6 7.4

4

Definitely 2 should hire1

I Nonviolent Acted Violence Real Violence (The Mouse (The Wild One) (Attica) That Roared)

"The subject!) were asked to rate on a 31 -point scale their extent of agreement with the statement. The assistant would make a very good experimenter and should definitely be hired for the jobA rating of 1 equalled "strongly agree, 31 equalled "strongly disagreeA rating of 16, the midpoint, presumabl% indicated a neutral opinion on the question Source Worchel, et al (1976)

8794 fellow student whom, they were told, was one Inthe other experiment (Liebert and Baron. of three candidates for a single joh for the next 1972), commercials preceded and followed a par- academic year (the assistant made three blun- ticularly violent 312-minute segment tropidle I N- ders, but he was not directly offensise to the tinidzahh'sAs noted carlier, it was not clear that the viewers in any was) It is possible that the com- s iol2rce (rather than arousal) produced the oh- mercials (which were, after all, "entertaining" servedaggressiveeffects(the"hurting-helping- public service announcements which ap- measui el, but it remains possible that the commer- parently did not interrupt the continuity of an cials frustrated the children's desire to see more of scenes within the movies), may have "toned an exciong sequence. thus increasing aroual in the down'' the otherwise arousing properties of the childrenAlternatively, itthe V ()rebel etalhy- violent films, thus leading toless enertwatton pothesis is correct, the aggression 111.1 not have oc- of the appropriate response by the subject Ot curred itthe siolent scenes had (lot been accom- course, Zillman's arousal theory cannot he panied by commercials, A noncommercial control correctlyappliedunlessthereisactual condition would he needed to settle these interesting physiological measurement ofarousal, and questions arousal decline, during the films Nevertheless, this is a possible interpretation, and one which 1 he possdility ot commercial-program interac- raises questions regarding the Vvorchel et al tion is further complicated by n work conclusion that commercials exacerbate on arousal theory One studs (Zillman;:ost, and violence in Wins Day. 1974) suggested that brie) exposure to a bland 3 A minor pointis that the mosses were pro- film clip following a siolent him clip can rAuce ag- jected on a hlm screen rather thaii on a telk gression Bland film ellpF might simulate the effects sion.monitor and were shown in an experimen- ot commercials, although the clip in this study as tal rather than an in-home setting These fac- longer( 71 2 minutes) than normal commercial tors might fuse inflated the os eral I degree ot breaks In- the same experiment, the bland tiln clip arousal and, therefore, theaggression- ex- did not dissipat'e the (higher ) arousal and aggression pressed by the subjects produced by anerotu himclip, suggesting that ex- tremely nos el or at (using content might he hard to 4 Another minor point is that the subjects were ercome, esp:cialls by a break of relatis els short college students, making the results ()Ms in- &ration 1' The picture therefore rmiams compli- directlyapplicabletochildrentallother catd and requires further research studies cited inthis chapter had children as subjects) I hefindings shoulo therefore he A number offurther considerathnis could he regarded as tentanse floweser, the V orchel raised such as the relatise effects of different types etalfindings are e only important enough of commere ie k , s nilent ones) in violent to warrant rephcanse research with hettet con- programs, or the eft:els o: commercials in highly trols and with a more direct measure ot aggres- arousing but nom totem programs (e gchase scenes sion or suspense scenes. which (teem in mans children's The potential of commercials to interact with programs) siolent programs may depend on where the commel cials are placed Two of the children's studies cited \nother consideration is that sndent programs earlieractually used commercials as separation Inasdecreaset he Medi% enessofeommei cials devices or filler materialIn one experiment i( (d- ims, 1975), the commercials were deliberatels in- serted to separate siolent intent tramthe v Iulenceit II sh,ttid he n tt, sl that to ail .0 IllItnan -spt.t.ftlints t1e self, and then again to separate the violence from its ,uhe,tsltIR p ,) the "n"" 11 '')11 tt..di latev.h,.lamer um_the r-tcm,1 tmv,1 t1 consequences 'These separations which might has e I C..r)11..... I OILrito., elfC I Alth been frustrating quite (wait from their intended pur- c1,101,e 11,1,, he C11 fht,1111. ti ith x111 con pose, produced more aggression than no separa- tent pl c salons tog })endues tru,tratttt the tionhut only among the youngest children in the hetttre them 1,) pits with the th11-0I Sc i clue ttt st(fdy (3d graders as opposed to hth and 10th grai- ittltnan xrtrintent. Is that the', tiont.t,Itit.: rtdustntri sir pis \entI,i i tggresswnt;tcfrit,prm.,..M,,nI lin. Mtainv: repro ers) The study employed a rather indirect measure .u' t..1 ompeihng iffinl nit ,'IS 111.11 of aggressionthe projectise picture-choice the innnad.itcarililcinf aggre measure .stn

hh I tat placed In adjacent to the s iolent material (,-ldn and Parke, 1964, and Bandura, 1965)In other trsitie Aix, October IS1976)Ads ertisers are in- words, children will learn and imitate -ven those creasingly raising this question [min!. hecause ;attic hehas iors w Inchareportrayedasunsafe or television vrers are claiming to products dangerous, it they themsels es evaluatele behaviors advertised on excessivels s totem programs (accord- as has mg some direct, functional s alue for them ing to a recent JV1 altert hompson surs ey , about (Bandura, 1972) 1 4 percent of all viewers and 2percent of college educated viewers) ,scheinseis are also concerned Two experiments by Atkin (1975c) sunort this that commercials may he frustrating to s 'ewers when contention In o: experiment, children were shown they interrupt an exciting plot, thereby leading to a PSA which pointed out the danger ot sugar in caus- lesser resentment of the ounnierciais on the other ing cavitiesI hese children were then compared hand, there is the possihilits that exciting programs with a co 'rot group ot children who did not see the may heighten %lesser attention and perhaps carry commercial I-or the exposed group, the annsugar user the high siesser ins ols ement to the _ommercial PSA produced significantly more negative answers In this regard, ads cruisers also isondei whether such to the item ''sugar is good for you" as well as signifi- involsement might "SSami-the commercials, or at cantly greater expres don of concern about cavities least the initial one or two id a seriesThe VL orchel Howes er, there was no duteteme in the two groups' et al study found no different e in subjects' recall of stated intentions to eat fewer sugary foodsEvi- the number of commercials shown during violent dently, the direct pleasurable consequences of eating versus nonviolent films Howes eiads ertisers .11-e sweets were perceis ed by the children as more im- likely to want evidence ot ettectiveness holm,' a mediately roles ant than the portrayed consequences recall of the number of commercialsI he quo ,on of cavities ot a program's effects on L ommercials remains an in tere.mng corollary to the s wie".ce issue, trann the In the second experiment, children were exposed perspecti e of ads ernsers ,l1 well as regulators to alternative set ions of an aspirin commercial The 'satety- version contained the message. "But Lnsate au\ to satets toesa I he unsafe at.c, remember, only, use when you issue1'1 not urn slated to the s mtence issue ith need itarnd don ttake too many tablet, The both,thefea isthatportrayalsotdangti-uus safety message had no molt t on the children', stated hal at, tor May he imitated childrenI he tear intention's to use the product, nor on the number of might even he more salient for unsafe acts. since the tablets they would take Atkin points out, correctly, objectis e likelihoodand probably the sublectise that the one-line warning may nut has e been salient likelihood perccised by parents) of a child being to the children (recall, of the warner was not hurt accidentially is tar greater than that oft child inea,irediIt is conceivable that repeated exposures 1-.ing hurt in or es, ins ols ed In acts of s udenee to such a warning or to negatively portrayed conse- With portrasals of violence, the centralissueis quenc.'s of a behavior might influence children's 'Doe, this do any harm'''' VL ith ponrayals of unsafe own perceptions ot the actual behavior and its eonse- acts in safety messages, the central question is 'Does quenve.s"this possibility remains untested because this do any good'.. ,oththeBandura andtheAtkinexperiments employed only single exposures Thus, the portrayed In the typical safety message, sate helms iorIs consequences would he weakly registered by the portrayed as rewarding and unsafe hehas ioris childrenin compass, awith theirr eviously portrayed as punishing or detrimental in its conse established perceptions quences11:,,se;cr, a long histors of research on children's 'inflame hehas un has demonstrated that A third experiment by Atkin (1975h) priNides punishment generIlly has only a temporary clfectIn some support for this idea this experiment, one tact, punishes' mr is leaf ned lust as readily as group of children was shown an antilitterang PSA, rewarded hehaviorAlso, whether a child actualls another control group did not see the commercial imitates an ohsers ed behavior depends not tin the Nlany children were in hs the commercial, rew ard, or punishment of the hchas ior of the model ,Atkin report, hes ause thsy had seen it frequently on but MKT' on the expected thrett ressaid or pupsh- prior to the expel anent Thus, for many of ment of tne child for the Inman\ e hehas ior (See, for the children,the commercial served asa re- example, the iepresentatne esperiments hy m nide!' I he PSA was apparmtly effective When given a candy bar after the commercial, only 2 per- programs) is almost always in the fantasy category. cent of the exposed group littered the wrapper, ver- The Impact of the violent portrayals varies accord- sus, 11 percent of the nonexposed groupa signifi- ing to the number of fantasy cues present in the cant difference, even though littering was evidently a portrayal: Cartoon violence generally has three cuts low-frequency, behavior for all the children. to indicate fantasy (animation, humor, and a remote setting); make - believe violence generally has two Stronger evidence would obviously be needed in cues (humor and a remote setting); and realistic, order to make a case for the effectiveness of repeti- acted violence generally has only one cue (the tion. In particular, more serious types of unsafe acts viewer's knowledge that the portrayal is fictional). should be studied, such as crossing streets carelessly Real-lik *violence (e.g., tootage of actual violence, or opening strange household containers. Special at- as in newsfilms or documentaries) has, of course, no tention should be given to acts which are not only cues to suggest fantasy. unsafe but attractive (rewarding) to children, such as "stunt-riding" with bicycles or playing with matches. There is evidence from one study that most These safety commercials would have to be carefully children as young as 4 years can distinguish these pretested, of course, to ensure that the effectiveness four levels of violence. However, about one-quarter of the portrayal, or the repetition of it, actually over- of 4 to 8 year olds define cartoon violence as depic- rides any prior attractions the behavior may have for tions of violence per se; 4-bout one-half of this age children. group also perceive make-believe violence in this way, and over one-half of 4 to 8 year olds see Unfortunately, such testing would involve an in- realistic (acted) violence as violence. Children ap- trinsic hazard, in that evidence of the effectiveness of pear to make these distinctions solely on the basis of a safety commercial would require that the unsafe the physical fantasy cues; there is no support for the behaviorbe performed as a baseline (or by a control idea that children, especially young children, can group of children), so that a reduction effect could differentiate types of violence on a more cognitive or be shown in the experimental group. In many cases, rational basisfor example, by a justification of the even a surrogate "prebehav 'oral" measure, such as a motives for the violent behavior or of the goodness child's knowledge or beliefs about the harmful of its consequences behavior, carries its own dangers. The Bandura line of research (e.g.. Masters, Gordon, and Clark, 1976) has shown that even a single exposure to a portrayal The key question is whether the differerit degrees of unsafe behavior may be remembered by children of fantasy in the various forms of television violence for substantial periods (two months or more) and make any difference in terms of effects on children may be imitated later. And as Wolf (1973) has and particularly in terms of Imitative aggression noted, portrayals of particularly deviant behavior fol lowing viewing. This question is especially Impor- may receive above-average attention and tant for evaluating the effects of children's commer- memorability. cials, since the type of violence used in the commer- cials is almost invariably of a fantasy nature (car- In short, pending the devising of assuredly harm- toon, make-believe, and occasionally realistic) and less testing procedures, safety commercials which in- is usually employed for a humorous or dramatic clude portrayals of unsafe acts are probably best left effect. out of children's television programingIndirect (televised) exposure of children to portrayed conse- The best available evidence suggel .s that less than quences of unsafe acts is likely to do more harm than about 5 percent of children's commercials depict any good. For the time being, safety training can be type of violence. However, both realistic and make- handled more safely in the home or school, where believe violence do appear in program promotions consequences of unsafe acts can be more directly and during children's viewing hours. Similarly, make- more frequently demonstrated to children. believe violence (sometimes) and cartoon violence (frequently) appearin about half of children's SUMMARY AND NEEDED RESEARCH programs. The question of the Impact of fantasy ver- sus realistic and real violence is therefore worth ex- Fantasyviolence. The type of violence employed amining in detail, aside from its specific application in children's commercials (and in most children's to children's commercials

90 3 Research on television violence involves many 1962; see also Wells' 1973 replication of Feshbach variables and multiple effects. Accordingly, a and Singer's experiment in which withdrawal of a classificatory framework was developed ( fable 5-6) violent diet of televisionare appeared to increase to aid the assessment of etfects and to serve as a aggression). Thus, in the long term, there is no con- possible guide for future research. Available studies vincing causal evidence of any cumulative instiga- indicate, first, that there is not much difference in tional effects, such as more aggressive or violent dis- the effects produced by different types offantasy positions in children. In fact, Ger bner and Gross violence. That is,cart -on, make-believe, and ( I 976)-have suggested that passivity is a more likely realistic, acted violence seem to function similarly as long-term result of heavy viewing of television stimuli for children Secondly, although the studies violence. Any instigation of deviant behavior by are few in number, they generally show that all children seems, therefore, to be confined to short- forms of fantasy violence have significantly weaker term circumstantial effects. effects on children than real portrayals of violence. All of this implies that an indictment of fantasy What are these effects? There is actually very lit- violence in children's programing must rest mainly tle evidence ofdirect imitationof television violence on a very slightriskthat the violent portrayal may be by children. There is evidence that fantasy violence, imitated and a somewhat greater risk that the as well as portrayals of real-life violence, caninsti- violence may have a short-term instigational effect gate or energizepreviously learned aggressive when situational circumstances suggest aggression as responses(e.g., interpersonalassaults during an appropriate response. In the first case, weshould children's play). It is by no means clear, however, perhaps weigh the remote risk of imitation against that the violence in a portrayal is solely responsible the much larger (but usually unacknowledged) prob- for this instigational effect. Rather, the evidence ability that the fantasy violence in children's televi- suggests ( I ) thatanyexcitingmaterial can be instiga- sion, though it does not lead to "catharsis,"provides tional of subsequent aggressive behavior; and (2) plenty of fun and entertainment for children. This thatitis theeffect of excitationrather than the probability seems self-evident; otherwise, children portrayal of violence which instigates or energizes would not spend so much time watching cartoons any subsequent aggression in children. Moreover, "saturated" with fantasy violence, nor action-adven- the type of "violent" behavior demonstrated in ex- ture programs which frequently contain some make- periments with children is, as has been noted by believe or realistic violence. Win-as (1969,1971), plausiblyinterpretable as 4-electing either novel play activities or, more A careful review of the evidence tends to confirm typidally,aloweringof previously learned play in- Kaplan and Singer's conclusion (in press) that the hibitions, rather than as an increase insocially debate about fantasy violence has often been slanted , threatening aggression. In short, "cold" imitation of by academic researchers; that the evidence does not violence by children is extremely rare, and the very warrant the strong conclusions warranted by many occasional evidence of direct, imitative associations reviewers; and that television violence is frequently between television violence and aggressive behavior held up as a scapegoat because it reminds us that the has been limited to extremely novel and violent acts real causes of violence are learned socially and by people (usually teenagers or adults) for whom de- culturally. viant behavior is already an established pattern.

The instigational effect means, in the short-term, More particularly, itis unlikely that children's that exposure to violent portrayals could be commercialscan be indicted as a cause of violence or dangerous to a childifshortly after the exposure aggression in children when (1) the types of violence (within 15 or 30 minutes), he or she happens to be in used in the commercials are rarely imitable, and (2) a situation which calls for interpersonalaggression the duration of the violence is much too short to sug- as anappropriateresponse e.g., an argument or gest that commeicials could have an instigational fight between siblings or peers. However, this same effect on the viewers. The prohibition of violent instigational effect could be produced byotherexcit- portrayals in children's commercial, therefore mg but nonviolent television content or by any other represents a value judgment about risk, rather than a excitational source, including, ironically enough, policy based on any scientific demonstration of ac- video failure or a parent turning the set off (Baer, tual harm to children

91 96 dla. Nevertheless, there are many improvemein ment conditions. These precautions have not research methods that could lead to more definitive always been followed. conclUSICVIS regarding television violence in both the programs and the commercials made for children. Most important, though, is the need to employ a wider range of measures One study has moved in Among them. this direction in examining "tolerance" effects. More I There is an immediate need for better descrip- study is needed of the distinction between pacifica- tions of violent portrayals in children's televi- tion and tolerance and, indeed, of the eight or so s:on, and especially of the physical stimulus different effects that violence may have, some of properties of violence, since these are most them harmless or even constructive. An unbiased readily understood by children. Future studies program of research must weigh any beneficial should systematically vary the dimensions of effects of violencee g., entertainment, in which animation (and "humanness"), of humor, of viewing but not aggression may be reinforced remoteness of settings, and of other fictional against socially undesirable outcomes, assigning cues in violent portrayals in order to determine probabilities and evaluative weights to each. how these cues interact in affecting children's Commercials in violent programs.'One recent ex- responses to the violence. periment suggested that violent programs may pro- 2. There is a need for better measurement of duce aggression only if they are shown with (I.e., In- possible mediating variables in the responses of terrupted by) commercials. The study has certain children to violent portrayals.1Vrost notably, limitations which left the findings somewhat am- there should be improved measures of the biguous, but the topic obviously deserves further in- child's cognitive labeling of violent content vestigation. Also Important is the contrary and of the arousal properties of this content possibility: that commercials may provide a socially Rarely haye children been asked to describe valuable function by toning down or defusing excite- the stimuli they are presumably reacting.to, ment generated by violent °Pother wise highly arous- and usually any mediating effects have been ing scenes in television shows. Given the current, merely assumed by researchers rather than ethical controversy over commercial sponsorship of directly tested. Similarly, direct physiological violent programsand also the empirical measi cements of arousal should be made. This possibilities that programs may affect commercials is essential if the overall excitational and in- and vice versathis issue should attract the interest stigational effects of violent programs are to be of advertisers as well as academic researchers and separated from any instigational or cognitive policymakers. effects directly associated with specific violent fi events in a program. Unsafe acts in safety messages. The danger with unsafe acts is that their depiction may lead to haz- 3. There is a need to identify a wider range of ardous imitation by children. The evidence indicates possible effects of violent portrayals and to that illustrations of unsafe behavior, even when provide better measurements of these effects punished or admonished in the commercial message, For obvious reasons, many so-called measures may make that behavior more salient and possibly of aggression are weak surrogates for actual more attractive to children than it would normally physical interpersonal aggression. The ques- be. The crucial controlling factor is not what hap- tionable validity of such measures has led pens to the model portrayed in the safety message, many to discount the evidence based on these but how the young viewer perceives the direct per- studies. In laboratory experiments, meas4res sonal consequences of the behavior if he or she were in which Interpersonal aggression is to Imitate it. PSA's on safety are generally produced realisticallysimulated shouldprovide the precisely because the unsafe behavior is so directly highest validity and would avoid the problem attractive to children. Only after repeated exposure of direct aggression measures being con- to such messages might the portrayed consequences founded by the degree of retaliatory action favorably alter a child's direct experience and judg-' shown by persons other than the experimental ment; and even then, such an effect is by no means subject. Field study measures are generally assured. Present indications are that safety commer- more convincing to policymakers than are cials should emphasize only the rewarding conse- laboratory experiments, in that the measure- quences of safe behavior, without showing unsafe ment criteria are clearer. However, inter-ob- behavior. Prevention of unsafe acts should he left to server reliability checks must be employed and direct in-home or in-school training and not to the observations should be blind as to treat- televisi44.(

92 Chapter 6

THE IMPACT OF PROPRIETARY MEDICINE ADVERTISINGON CHILDREN

In a 1975 petition before the FCC, the Attorneys did not address the impact of proprietary medicine General of 19 states requested a ban on medicine ad- advertising on children, since the studies reviewed vertising on television between 6 a.m. and 9 p m dealt with teenagers. Furthermore, the report Although the petition was rejected by the FCC in focused on drug abuse, in the sense of illicit drug 1976, this document and others filed in support of use, and did not respond to the broader concerns and in opposition to it, provide a useful summary of regarding the development of a general re ceptivity the questions at issue. The Attorneys General stated to proprietary medicine. that children are "particularly impressionable and susceptible to the influences of television advertis- In their petition, the Proprietary Association also ing," and that medicine commercials aim to create argued that OTC drug advertising serves as a useful "receptive attitudes towards pill taking" and to purpose in that "self-medication is an acceptedand "present drugs as the cure-all to the tension and essential element in the scheme of health care in this problems of everyday life." The petition suggested nation It is the consumer who must judge "whether that the long-term effect of such advertising would or not the symptoms are sufficiently discomforting be to create a "new and artificial demand for drugs to warrant the use of self-medication." In addition, It therefore requested that such commercials be the Association pointed out that "while it is true that restricted "to an audience which is equipped to young children do not possess the requisite ex- evaluate and digest sophisticated drug advertising." perience and judgment to properly evaluate OTC medicine advertising or to make judicious use of the Another petitionin support of the Attorneys products themselves, this same statement could be General request was subsequently filed with the FCC made of any advertising not specifically directed to by a public interest group, the Council on Children, children." Media, and Merchandising (1975). The Council asserted that "children who are repeatedly exposed CURRENT REGULATION to OTC drug advertisingiwhere adult role models are 'rewarded' for taking medicationarelikely to Television advertising of proprietary medicines learn a behavioral response from such ads and to act on "children's programs" is prohibited by the NAB on that learned behavior at somefuture time code: perhaps to their detriment." Nonprescription medicine, regardless of how Arguments against these petitions were set forth taken or administered, shall not be advertised in a communication to the FCC by the Proprietary in or adjacent to programs initially designed Association (1975), a nonprofit trade association of primarily for children under 12 years of age. OTC drug manufacturers. The association con- tended that medical and scientific evidence does not Similar provisions exist in the Children's Advertis- support a link between OTC drug advertising and ing Guidelines issued by the NAD: drug abuse, misuse, or overuse The Proprietary Association cited a report which it commissioned by Medications, drugs and supplemental vitamins Oxtoby-Smith, Inc. (1974) regarding youth and il- (liquid or pills) should not be advertised to licit drugs. This review of the literature foune no children. relationship between proprietary drug advertising and incidence of drug abuse. However, the report The NAB -guidelines apply to programs specifically designed for childrenthat is, those In this chapter, we use the terms nonprescription medicine, proprietary medicine. and OTC (over-the-counter) drugs in- shows which are concentrated on Saturday and Sun- terchangeably day mornings, Most children's viewing, however,

93 100 occurs during late afternoon and early evening The children The tendency has been to assume a simple NAD guidelines pertain to advertising in children's cause-and-effect relationship, although other programs and programs in which "audience patterns sources of information and attitudesparents, typically contain more than 50 percent children" or peers, siblings, and teacherswork in conjunction to advertising which is "clearly addressed to with television advertising Our analysis will seek to children II and under determinetherelativeeffectsof these several socializing agents on the child's conceptions of INCIDENCE medicte, as well as the function of such variables as the child's age, medical history, parent-child in- Although the existing sett - regulatory codes teraction style, and family background However, prohibit OTC drug advertising on programs the limited nature of the available evidence will not designed for children, children are nevertheless ex- allow us to be very definitive Figure 6 -I illustrates posed to such advertising Approximately 85 per- the analytic framework for our examination of OTC cent of all children's viewing is of nonchildren's drug advertising's role in the beliefs, attitudes, inten- programs (Nielsen, 1975) Among the- top 15 shows tions, and actual medicine use of children. viewed by children, only three are broadcast in time periods covered by the NAB children's codes.2 Beliefs and attitudes Atkin (1975e) examined the relationship between children's "medicine advertis- The Attorneys General petition asserted that "in ing exposure "1 and beliefs about ,nedicine and its the first six months of 1974, one out of eight televi- efficacy. Atkin's sample consisted of 256 children sion commercials was devoted to drugs." The peti- (5th, 6th, and 7th graders) :elected from schools in tion by the Council on Children. Media, and urban, suburban, and small town areas in Michigan. Merchandising further estimated that children see His results may be summarized in terms of the about 1,000 such commercials each year. following advertising exposure - belief relationships, all of which are based on sixth-order partial correla- These figures would have to be substantiated, but tions. (The results should be interpreted as tentative, they do provide at least a rough estimate of the inci- given the order of magnitude of the correlations.) dence of children's exposure to OTC drug advertis- ing. The question, then, is whether these commer- Perceptions of reality. Children with high ex- cials serve apositivefunctionas a source of posure to medicine advertising perceive that knowledge about illness and medicines, or a nega- people are more often sick (r = .14) and that tive function as a conditioning agent which fosters they more often take medicine (.14). receptivity to medicines and heightens demand for Belief in medicine. High exposure to medicine their use. advertising correlates with the child s belief in the quickness of relief after taking medicine RESEARCH EVIDENCE ( 10). ,Illness concern. Children with high exposure to There is little systematic research evidence on the medicine advertisir ,, worry more about getting effects of medicine advertising on children. Most sick (.14). research has focused on teenagers, rather than Approval of medicine. The relationship between children, and has been concerned with illicit drug exposure to medicine advertising and approval use, rather than with the use of proprietary of medicine is .12. medicines. Moreover, existing research has Efficacy of medicine Children with high ex- generally sought to document the harmful effects of posure to medicine advertising are more likely drug advertising and has ignored any potentially to feel better after taking medicine (.12). beneficial effects. Medicine usage. Tnere is a general lack of rela- tionship between medicine-advertising ex- There also is no useful conceptual framework posure and medicine usage (.03). concerning OTC drug advertising and its effects on 'An index constructed by multiplying the amount of viewing of evening television by the degree of attention to sample OTC 2See Chapter ti drug advertisements

94 101 Figure 6-1

Proposed Variables and Relationships Influencing the Effects of OTC Drug Advertising on Children

Independent Variables Dependent Variables

. Exposure to TVmedicine . Beliefsabout medicines advertising . Attitudes toward medicines . TV viewing . Intentions to use medicines

. Usage of medicines

I I MediatinVariables I I

I . Developmental I age medical history

I . Psychological

child's anxiety with respect to illness child's attitude toward TV adver- tising child's attitude toward sources of information

Social-Psychologial

Parent-child inter- action style

Parent-child (..o- viewing of TV Child's peer inte- gration

10 s..f) 95 In general, these results suggest that exposure to Campbell (1974) looked at the development of medicine advertising does, to a certain extent, in- illness concepts among children ages 6-12 as a :unc- fluence a child's conceptions of illness and medicine tion of their parents' concepts of illness He found These relationships tend to he accentuated some- that the transmission process is far from direct and what among the smarter children (as measured by that illness concepts are also related to the child's scholastic performance) and among thehigher health history and toage In other words, the social-status children Other variables, such as age Campbell results suggest that a parent's perspective and sex of the child, parental attitudes toward regarding illness is not necessarily therototype for medicine, or the child's frequency of illness, all show the child, and tha, other sources of information, par- inconsistent patterns ticularly the child's own experience with illness, contribute to the child's development of ideas about A study similar in «u ein to Atkin's is that of illness The role of proprietary medicine advertising Lewis and Lewis (1974). A total of 208 children (5th in the formation of the child's concepts of illness was and 6th graders) from both an experimental school not a concern of the Campbell study and a pi Ale school serving a disadvantaged popula- tion were asked to watch television and to describe Usage and intention to use medic we A child's commercials related to health From the children's usage of proprietary medicines is directly mediated reports,the authors inferredthatthe children by parents 4 As such, we can expect that a child's use believed 70 percent of the OTC drug commercials of OTC products would he a function of parental at- and that pe-sonal use as well as parental use of the tauo:s toward OTC medicines, the child's history' o advertised products increased the credibility of the illness, and the child's requests for OTC medicines advertising messages. This was particularly true Atkin (1975e) found a moderate correlation of 17 among children from lower socioeconomic back- between exposure to medicine advertising and usage grounds. The problems in conducting the study were of medicines, but this relationship disappeared when greatAs the authors themselves noted, the two frequency of illness was controlled Of course, it schools viewed television at different times, there may be argued that high exposure to medicine ad- was no validation of actual drug usage. the group vertising encourages more frequent feelings of being selected as a convenience sample, and the conduct of ill and in need of medication For example, research research in the context of a school assignment in- with adults has shown that "illness" is defined not troduced considerable possibility for asocial only by biological factors but also by psychological desirability bias and sociological factors (Fox, 1968)

In a multi-phase research effort, Kanter (1970) Research with teenagers by Milaysky. Pekowsky, found that students in 5th, 7th, and II th grades and Stipp (1975) found a positive relationship bet- reported their beliefs that advertising influences ween exposure to proprietary medicine advertising their feelings toward medicine No evidence was oh- and reported usage of OTC medicines This relation- tamed as to actual attitudinal effects Many of the ship was stronger in homes where there were many students also expressed the belief that other young OTC drugs around the house, but it did not vary ac- people were potentially capable of being influenced cording to whether the teenager took the drugs him- by OTC commercials. However, the drug commer- self or asked his mother for the drugs cials were not recalled more easily than other com- mercials, and they had a low salience to the students (I(t(u drug it 1/201.!('Most of the evidence on ex- (i.e., not talked about much) The youngest children posure to OTC drug advertising and drug usage con- (5th grade) were the most receptive and least criti- cerns illicit drugs, in fact this is the minor focus of cal, suggesting that the drug commercials may have the Milaysky etal research cited above Does greatest potential impact at this age 1,!vel This find- ing also indicate that skepticism increases with here is the special taw .0 Lhlitirtm Inge% mg ()I( prmithts age,afinding generally confirmedinprevious sin their mkn petip,n sited research by Ward (1971) and by Robertson and rImpning tr(m) () I (drug, and ..uggesteti telation..hip to Rossaer (1974) atherttsittg

ttO cumulative exposure to OTC medicine advertising Much of the research cited has been conducted ultimately encourage use of illicit drugs') Ot the with other objectives ,n mind There has been several studies to date, none has found a positive little effort to address systematically the issue of assclation Milaysky et al revealed a negativerela- proprietary medicine advertising's impact on tionship between exposure to drug advertising and children illicit drug usage Hulbert (1974), in research with Serious questions of research design can be college students, found no relationship between posed for many of the studiese g., small, television exposure andillicitdrug useAtkin nonrepresentative samples, lack of reliability (1975e), in research with 4th through 7th grade and validity checks, and the absence of replica- children, had similar results tion Most of the research attempts to trace short- In general, use of illicit drugs is associated with run effects, whereas the prevailing concern is peer-grouprelations and family background with the long-run cumulative effects of OTC (Hulbert, 1974, Kanter, 1970: Streit, HalsiAl, and drug advertising Pascale, 1974)As Hulbert (1974) notes,illicit Most of the research focuses on potential nega- drugs are generally tried first in small groups and tive effects of OTC advertising, whereas there use is heavily determined by friends' useHulbert may well be positive learning effects about also found that illicit drugs were used less by those medicines and their appropriate uses college students who lived at home The methods of analysis are largely cross-sec- tional (by age) and rely primarily on correla- All of these research projects examined current tion It these correlation analyses are to suggest drug advertising exposureIt could be argued that possible causal links, more care has to he taken what is of equal or greater concern is cumulative ex- in attempting to trace the cause-and-effect se- posure to drug advertising, since a long-termlearn- quence ing effect is at issue Critics of televisiondrug adver- tising have not focused on a direct short-termrela- Among the research questions that should be in- tionship between OTC advertising exposure and il- vestigated are the following licit drug usage. Furthermore, the relationship bet- ween current exposure to drugadvertising and illicit Exposure To what levels of proprietary drug us.:. might be expected to be negative, since il- medicine advertising are children actually ex- licit drug users are apparently less nome-oriented posed. and how does this exposure vary by age? and, therefore, less likely to he viewing television at How much of this exposure occurs while the all child is alone, how much in the presence of a parent') NEEDED RESEARCH AttentionDo children pay attentionto proprietary medicine commercials, or does

1 Current policy regarding the impact of proprie- "selective perception" operate to screen out tary wdicne advertising on children cannothe such advertising') What factors affect attention definitively addressed with existing research There levelage of the child, the child's health histo- are a number of problems ry, parents' in-home usage of OTC drugs? Understanding Do children understand OTC Only limited research to date has focused on commercials' What meanings do they take children Most of the research has been with from the commercial') How aware are they of teenagers and college students the product's value under specific conditions') The existing research has most frequently dealt What factors affect comprehension levels? withillicitdrugs and not with proprietary V geWIPW Lel'el Do heavy viewers of television medicines hold different attitudes toward OTC drugs A serious deficiency in the existing literature is than light viewers, when age and health history the failure to examine the relative importance are controlled') How does viewing level affect and interactionofthe various information receptivity to OTC drugs. realism of health sources, including media, advertising, parents, concepts. and awareness of advantages and dis- peers. and siblings advantages (e g , side effects) of OTC drugs')

10 4 Usage. To what extent do parents administer pact from the role of parents, peers, and OTC drugs to their children? To what extent teachers'? do children request OTC drugs, and is this associated with viewing level? At what age do children begin to self-administer OTC drugs? These questions are indicative of the lack of Multiple Sources. How do the various informa- knowledge concerning proprietary medicine adver- tion sources about OTC drugs interrelate, and tising and its effects on children. Such questions what specific roles do they play in a child's at- must be answered before any meaningful policy can titude toward and usage of the drugs? Can the be formulated in regard to the advertising of OTC role of OTC advertising be separated in its im- drug products on television.

10 98 Chapter 7

THE EFFECTS ON CHILDREN OF TELEVISIONFOOD ADVERTISING

What are the effects of television food advertising being urged on television to eat foods which neither on children? This question has been raised bythe present good health nor healthfullifetime food broadcast and advertising industries, by major cor- habits" (Gussow, 1972) porations that market and advertise food products to children, by consumer advocate groups' and by Critics of food advertising for children also take some members of the professional health com- issue with the presentation of food advertising For munities, by several congressional subcommittees,2 example, commercials typically promote confec- and by the Federal Trade Commission and the tions and snacks on the basis of their taste and flavor White House Conference on Food, Nutrition and and often associate food products such as breakfast ° Health (1970) cereals with toys and other premiums. According to the critics, the relationship of good balanced eating Criticism of food advertising on television has habits to health is rarely included or emphasized in been directed both at the quality of the food prod- children's food commercials, and nutritional infor- ucts and at their methods of presentation in televi- mation about the advertised food products and serv- sion commercials. in this report, we will not attempt ices is seldom provided (Choate, 1972) As a result to adjudicate the arguments about the quality of the of early exposure to television commercials, "it food products, since our focus is on the effects of ad- becomes very difficult (by the time the 6 year old en- vertising. However, questions about the nutriti9nal ters school) to reverse the whole process and explain value of advertised food products warrant serious that the first reason to eatis for the necessary consideration by food scientists, nutritionists, nutrients" (Mayer, 1973) federal agencies, and the food industries. Thus, questions have been raised about whether it has been claimed that a child's developing sense food advertising has an influence on children's of what our culture deems fit to eat is influence) by nutritional knowledge and attitudes toward foo I the foods that he or she sees in television comer- and good nutrition, their eating habits, and e%en cials (Jerome, 1975). Other important influencing their physical health. Critics have argued as well factors are also° involved, of course, such as that food commercials may be disruptive to parent- ethnicity, socio-economic status, and nutritrinal child relationships, in that conflicts may be precipi- education by parents (Goldblith, 1976)Critics tated when parents refuse children's requests for the claim that the mere presence in television advertis- advertised foods or when the parents' or teachers' in- ing of ready-to-eat cereals, candies, or other fluence over children's eating is contradicted or un- sweetened snacks suggests to children that these dermined by the advertising (ACT. Choate, 1972a, products are appropriate and desirable to consume Jerome, 1975). Furthermore, because these particular foods are heavily advertised to children, and because a full Members of the answer that in re- range of food products is notadvertised.1 cent years, rules and procedures have been adopted "childrenwhen they are still young enough to be to guide advertisers prom,,ting food products to forming their notions of what is good to eat--are children on television (NAB, 1975; NAD, 1975).4 With regard to the promotion of confections and 'Stith as the Council on Children. Media, and Merchandis- snacks, advertisers also note that taste and flavor are ing (('CMM) and Action for Children s;:!"vision (AC I the primary tunctions and attributes of these prod- 2For example, the Senate Subcommittee or, the Consumer. ucts; therefore, the advertising presentation of these the Senate Select Committee on Nutrition and Human Needs products is necessat< and appropriate. in addition, (1973), and the House Subcommittee on Communications food advertisers point out that the prime respon- (1975) sibility for nutritionally adequate and balanced diets 'The food categories commonly not included in television advertising to children arefish and meats, dairy products. legumes and beans, fruits. vegetables, and their Juices and fats ''See following swum for current industry regulations of and oils (Jerome, 1975) food advertising 1 6 for a child rests with the person who performs the the newborn infant and is independent of early ex- role of meal planner The types of foods consumed perience. However, itshould be noted that the in the home and served in the school expose children specific foods which come to satisfy a baby's taste to a variety of food products, and this ey?osure must preferences may be strongly influenced by the foods also be weighed in considering children's attitudes that are accessible and acceptable to families toward food, food advertising, and a balanced diet CURRENT AND PROPOSED REGULATION In testimony before a congressional committee, representatives of Kellogg and General Mills ex- Several forms of regulation and self-regulation pressed their conviction that their cereal advertising exist for televised food commercials directed at contributes to children's nutritional education by children.6 The NAB code (1974; 1975) includes the presenting cereal in the context of a full bteakfast, following references to the promotion of food prod- thereby encouraging them to eat a good breakfast. In ucts: this context, Kellogg cited thur "Good Breakfast Campaign," a series of commercials in which a Given the importance of sound health and variety of breakfast foods are depicted in orderto nutritional practices, advertisements for edi- educate children to the need for breakfast" (Senate, bles should be in accordance with commonly 1,975:5). Many companies also prepare and dis- accepted principles of good eating and s.uld tribute nutrition and health-related information in seek to establish the proper role of the adver- other forms, such as informational publications and tised product withinthe framework ofa related materials for schools, and nutritional label- balanced regimen. ing on product packages. Commercials for products, such as snacks, can- The food Industry has also raised the question of dies, gum, and soft drinks, should not suggest the feasibility of presenting meaningful statements or recommend indiscriminate or immoderate on the subject of nutrition within the format of a 30- use of the product. second commercial The Pillsbury Company states. Each commercial for a breakfast-type product It is our judgment . that the televis'on com- should include at !east one audio reference to mercial does not lend itself to a constructive and one video depiction of the role of the prod- learning process. So it is not in itself an effi- uct within the framework of a balanced regi- cient means of instructing viewers in a complex men In executing this reference to a balanced and extensive subject such as nutrition. regimen, it is permissible for the video to h, animated and for the audio to be delivered by General Mills also suggests that nutrition-education an animated character. However, a video title messages are best directed not to children but to superimposed on the screen may not by itself be parents, who have control over menu planning. used to describe a balanced regidin, as some viewers do not read yet. Finally, industry representatives justifiably argue that advertising is only one of many factors that With reference to message sources, real-life probably influenze children's food choices and diet. authority figures/celebrities are disallowed They point, for example. to studies' carried out at from being sl.own eating the advertised food, the Monell Chemical Senses Center, University of this constituting an endorsement or testimonial Pennsylvania. demonstrating that newborn babies situation. but cartoon characters created for respond to a wide variety of sugars and other taste and primarily associated with a specific stimuli. When tested within the first few days of life, children's food product ("presenter") can be these babies not only responded to sucrose at con- shown eating the product. centrations meaningful to adults, but they also dis- criminated among different sugars. This research Special enriched foods designed to serve as suggests that a drive for sweet stimulation exists in meal substitutes may be advertised as such,

6The history of regulationof televisionadvertisingto 'M R Kare Sweetener', limes and Umertaaairs National children is described by Choate (1975), spec,fically nongovern- Academy of Science, Washington, D C , 1975 mental forms of regulation are also identified by Banks (1975)

100 10 1 provided their purpose and nutritional value harmful." As a result, these "advertisements have are featured in the advertisement and are sup- the tendency or capacity to influence children to ported by adequate documentation. engage in behavior which is harmful or involvesthe risk of harm, and are unfair or deceptive acts or The NAD guidelines (1975) include these state- practices, in violation oc sections 12 and 5 of the ments regarding food advertising FTC Act General Foods agreed to cease and desist from these advertising practices. Particular control should be exercisedto assure that representations of food products Criticism of food commercials dir;cted to are made so as tv encourage sound usageof the children has often been accompanied by suggestions product, with a view toward healthy develop- for corrective action. For example, recommenda- ment of the child and the development of good tions from medical professionals and nutritionists nutritional practices have included (Senate, 1973.3)

Advertisements representing mealame in the Systematically incorporating nutritional infor- home should clearly and adequately depict the mation (e.g., major nutrients and the food's role of the product within the framework of a role in a balanced diet) in food commercials' balanced diet. Overconsumption, of food prod- (Jerome). ucts and beverages should be avoided, nor should it be implied that any one food provides Sha. L., curtailment (Shaw) or Lanning (Nizer) all the nutrients contained in a well-designed of childrei. stelevisionadvertisements for daily food plan heavily sugared products

The FTC, inits jurisdiction over unfair and Reevaluation by manufacture's of products deceptive acts or advertising practices, has taken an with high sugar content, toward reformulation active interest in the issue of tood advertising for with sugar substitutes (Navia). children. The following case illustrates the reg- ulatory ,efforts of the CommissionIn1975, the At the level of fundamental corporate policy, Commission reviewed a complaint of the Bureau of Mayer (1973) argued that "What we need is for food Consumer Protection concerning a series of televi-. companies to agree that they are selling taste, ap- soon advertisements for Post Grape-Nuts, arcady- pearance, maybe fashion, but first of all they are to-eat breakfast cereal marketed by GeneralFoods selling sources of nutrients, and one is by no means Corp. (FTC, 1975, #C-2733). In these commercials, exclusive of the other." an adult is shown picking wild growingvegetation, including cranbcrries, parts of pine trees, and cat- Robert Choatc, founderof the Council on tails, while observing that each is edible- ". I'm Children, Media and tvlerchandising, initiated sug- gathering part of my breakfast ... delicious with gestions for a Television Code for the Advertising of Grape-Nuts..." In some cases, the narrator put she Edibles, which would establish guidelines for the, picked berries into the cereal bowl. The consent frequency, grouping, and presentation of food acc order accepted by the FTC and General Foods vertisements directed to children (Choate, 1972b). stated that these advertisements might lead children Action for Children's Television sought corrective to eat harmful plants which they might findgrowing action by petitioning the FTC (1972) for a trade in natural surround-ings.7 The Commission'sallega- regulation rule to eliminate all food advertising to tion pointed out that "a substantialnumber of children on the grounds that it is "misleading and children do not have sufficient knowledge or ex- unfair." perience to distinguish between those plants ... which are harmful from those which are not More recently, the FTC has proposed a trade regulation rule on food advertising "... designed to eliminate deception and unfairness which may result 'A consent agreement is for settlement purposes onlyand from the making of certain affirmative claims with does not constitute an admission by respondents that they have respect to nutrition" (Federal Register, 1974, 39, violated the law The Poulos study (1975). described later in this chapter. was commissioned by the FTC to Investigate the effects 218; 1974, 41, 42). This proposed rule on food ad- of these commercials on children vertising has become the subject of considerable

101108

a controversy among food advertisers, their trade Another explanation may be the great variety of organizations and legal counsel, and members of the brands among these products. Ready-w-eat cereals health professions 8 Numerous Issues of fact, law, offer a case in point. In recent years, there has been a and opinion surround, the specific provisions of the dramatic increase in the h umber of cereal brands on proposed rule, and the real consequences of such the market. Presweetened ready-to-eat cereals, regulation are difficult to anticipate. marketed specifically as children's produCts, coin,

prise a significant portion of cereal product, , INCIDENCE estimated $470 million of the $1.5 billion _ (Advertising Age, March 8, 1976). Since each of the According to Broadcast Advertisers Reports, over major cereal manufacturers markets a number of $70 million was spent by advertisers in 1970 on net- different brands, product differentiation seems to be work weekend children's television shows. Eight ad- an economically viable for these vertisers accotinted for more than half of the adver- corporations. The differentiation among these essen- tising revenues from these programs, and five of tially similar products is achieved by varying such these eight were advertisers of food products to features as flavor, shape, color, and packaging and children Kellogg, General Mills, General Foods, promotion. Quaker Oats, and Mars (Pearce, 1971). (The other three were toy manufacturers.) Among the presweetened cereals, for example, in- dividual brand Images have been developed by associating the brand and its packaging with thepro- Information compiled more recently by Broadcast motional features of the commercials for the prod- ,4tivertisers Reports (1975) Indicates little change in uct. These include animated presenter characters the major food advertisers on network children's (e.g., the Sugar Frosted Flakes Tiger, Trix' Rabbit, programs 9 With the exception of Mars, which in Cap'n Crunch) and musical jingles and slogans (e.i., 1975 occupied a lower rank on the scale of advertis- "Sugar Frosted Flakes taste G RR R RR REAT!") In- ing revenues, the same four companies headed the pack and mail-order prizes and premiums are -1Iso listRevenues from cereal commercials appearing used to identify brands. For example, Ralston- on children's shows in1975 totaled over $24 Purina's new product entry, a fruit flax -)red break- million; those from candy and gum advertisingac- fast cereal called "Moonstones," was r escribedas counted for an additional $11 millioh.lo In com- follows: parison, promotional messages for toys, games, and hobby afts yielded a combined total of less than $22 m lion "Moonstones" isbuilt around moon-based characters called "Moonbeams"the good guys who work on the light side of the moon Of the wide range of edible products, why are and "Moonbums"the bad guys living cereals, candy, and other snacks so heavily pro- on the dark side of the moon who are continaully try- moted on television to young audiences? First, these ihg to their dirty hands on" the secret for- are products known to be consumed by children. mula for the cereal. The in-pack premium is a Also, while in most ca::s children are not the ?loon buggy and the package contains a T-shirt purchasers of these products, they are believed to ex- Self-liquidating 'offer (Advertising Age, March ert influence un their -parents to purchase the prod- 8, 1976). ucts (see Chapter 10 of this report). In contrast, "basic foods," such as fruit, vegetables, For some further general discussion of the proposed 'rule, fish, and meat are rarely advertised nationally, eyen sec the monograph by 1 Jacoby et al , "Affirmative Nutritional to general audiences. It is said that their undifferen- Disclosure in Advertising and Selective Alternatives. The Likely tiated, unbranded nature and well-established Impact on Consumers," Consumer Research Institute For a dis- familiarity to the consumer make them less suitable cussion of the proposed rule as it might affect children, see Choate, testimony submitted to the Federal Trade Commission items to promote profitably. in addition, with the byCouncil on Children, Media, and Merchandising, exception of certain trade organizations (e.g., the Washington, D C, Oct 1976 American Dairy Association), farmers and pro- The category of "network children's television" included ducers are not well-equipped to mount national ad- weekend morning programing, CBS's weekday morning show vertising campaigns. apta'n Kangarooand the monthly ABCAfter-School Special ,,Tast food restaurants also accounted for a large amount of There have been several content analyses of televi- advertiing revenues on the children's shows sion commercials tbat promote frod products or

102 10 (t) services specifically to child viewers One early level of child viewers; the income, television viewing analysis, cone'ssioned by the NAB, reported patterns, and media-related attitudes of families;. specifically oi c.iomotional thessages for foods dur- and intrafamily communications about food product ing network weekend morning lhildren's shows in choices and eating habits. Clancy-Hepburn con- the 1971 fall season (Winick et al , 1973). A subse- ducted two studies (1974) which examined the quent study, by Atkin (1975d), analyzed the content premise that children's responses to television food of network advertising on two comparable Saturday advertising affect their eating behavior through a mornings in 1973 and 1973 Although both of these variety of mediating factors, especially parent-child studies were conducted prior to the latest NAB and interaction. Fifty 8 to 13 year old middle-class boys NAD guidelines, they provide useful data for pre- and their mothers were interviewed in one study; the and post-code comparisons other included 55 lower-income boys and girls. The findings supported the correlation between parents' More recent data, commissioned by ACT, and children's attitudes and behavior toward food analyzes 1,975 weekend morning children's adverbs .advertising. Specifically; children of mothers with ing and program content on five commercial stations high knowledge of the validity of nutritional product in Boston (Barcus, 1975a). Three of the stations claims expressed significans'y fewer preferences and were network affiliated,the other two were requests for advertised foods and reported lower independent UHF stations A substantial majority consumption of these products. Children who fre- (68 percent) of the total sample of 400 commercial quently accompanied their mothers on grocery- annot, .rents were for food products or eating shopping trips made the most purchase demands, places. A more detailedlisting of food-product and there was a strong positive relationship between categories included 25 per4cent ready-to-eat cereals, children's purchase requests and mothers' yielding 25 percent candies and sweets, 10 percent eating to these requests, although the mothers with ...gh places and fast4od restaurants, 4 perCent "snack knowledge about advertising claims tended to yield foods," and another 4 percent miscellaneous (in- less to the requests for snack foods. cluding milk and dairy productt, fruits, fruit juices, and bread). Further evidence of children's requests for adver- tised foods and their parents' yielding to these re- Describing the specific content of the food com- quests was prescnted in a national, industry-sup- mercials, Barcus characterized 100 cereal messages ported survey of 6-14 year old children and their as follows. 50 percent show-ed the product in use and mothers in regard to food-related information, at- the other half just showed a picture of the product, titudes, and behavior (Gene Reilly Group, 1973b). 60 percent of the ads referred to taste/flavor, 25 per - A sample of 1,053 children were individually inter - centpade specific reference to sweetness. 43 percent 'viewed in their homes; 591 of their mothers com- mentioned nutritional value (e g ,by identifying pleted self-administered questionnaires. For the 2( names on vitamins); 91 percent represented the product categories examined in the study (including product as part of a balanced meal;'' only 3, percent presweetened cereals, cookies, fruit drinks, peanut specified ingredients or calories, and 47 percent butter, gum, and candy), at least 75 percent of the used a premium offer. In comparison-candy adver- mothers who purchased these products said that they tising also tended to mention taste/flavor (68 per- were influenced in brand and product selection by cent), but references to sweetness were rare (2 per- their children's requests. cent). The candy commercials also made infrequent references to nutritional value (8 percent) and rarely This study also attempted to account for the represented the candy product as being part of a operation of "passive dictation" as well as direct re- balanced meal (2 percent) They tended to show the quests in the interaction reported between parents product being eaten (76 percent) and 24 percent of and children. Wells (1966) described passive dicta- the ads referred to the canth as a snack tion as follows.

RESEARCH EVIDENCE If you ask a woman, "Who chooses the brand of dog food used?", she replies that she usually Reseurh on mechunny varulhhlMediating varia- does If you ask how she goes aboutit, bles include such factors as the -age and cognitive however, you will find she usually tries a dum- 'This practice it evidence of advertisers LinnplianCe with ber of brands and continues to buy the one the the halarq:ed breakfast di%Liosure required by the NAB Code dog likes best.

103.1 Thus, of the 82 percent of children who reported supermarket.12 The children's requests were found ever wanting ready-toat cereal, 56 percent said to be positively related to the amount of television their mothers knew which cereal they wanted with- they were reportedly exposed to at home Cereals out having to ask (passive dictation), while another and candy, the foods most frequently advertised in 40 percent reported having to ask their mothers to commercialS directed at children, were the most buy a specific kind for them The mothers tended to heavily requested items In addition, another phase corroborate the children's reports ot the study provided evidence that the harder a child worked (by pressing a button) to maintain The children also admitted to parental restric- commercials on a TV monitor, as compared to the tions on their eating habits For example, about halt programnarrative, the greaterthe number of the children interviewed indicated that their parents purchase requests he or she directed to the mother at restricted their intake of sweets, particularly candy, the supermar ket gum, and chocolate (although two-thirds said they were still allowed to buy candy either "ver' often" Research on effectsThe few studies in this catego- or "pretty often") Imposed limitations on the con- ry investigate such factors as children's learning of sumption of sweets are consistent with the mothers' infot matron from commercials, their acceptance ot expressed concerns about sugar generally (60 per- product claims made in food commercials, other tent) and candy specifically (54 percent) in their food-related attitudes, and their reported eating

children's diet. Mothers_of 6-7 year olds and behavior In terms of children's acquisition of infor- mothers at lower income levels reported the highest mation and attitudes from food commercials, the levels of concern Sixty-nine percent of the children Gene Reilly Group study (I 973b) found that the surveyed also reported that there were certain foods children demonstrated high awareness of nationally parents said they must eat, with vegetables (71 per- advertised brand names of such products as ready- cent) and meat (25 percent) rated most often to-eat cereals, candy, gum, and snack cakes and pies For example, 86 percent of cereal-eaters and 84 per- Atkin (19750 used unobtrusive observation in cent of candy-cons rers identified a specific brand supermarkets as a more direct way of studying as either their favorite or one they usually ask for, parent-child interaction in the selection of cereals The results indicated that in two-thirds of the 516 In general, the children's bred-name recall in- families observed, children initiated the selection of creased with age (this was also observed in a study a cereal, either by demanding (46 percent) or re- by Keiser,1975). Incontrast, the children's questin'g (20 percent) a specific cereal Parents were references to such products as ice cream and fruit twice as likely to approve than refuse the proposed juice were often made ingeneric terms(e.g., purchase, with demands resulting in slightly more "chocolate" ice cream and "orange" juice) than by acquiescence (65 percent) than requests (58 per- specific brands. cent).One-fourth o alltheinteractions were reported to result in parent-child conflict, usually as Questioned about more general information (e.g., a consequence of a parent's negative response to the "the kinds of things you call snacks"), the children child's initiative. Atkin speculated that the most often cited sweets (78 percent), such as cookies, children's observed behavior in the shopping situa- candy and , and ice cream The other foods tions was affected by their prior viewing of cereal identified as snacks were in order of frequency. salty commercials. However, he acknowledged that their chip-type products, fruit, sandwiches, and milk exposure to television commercials would need to be These responses indicate that the children's concepts experimentally controlled in order to conclude that of what constitutes an acceptabie snack usually in- their familiarity with the products was the result of cluded those products heavily advertised to them. advertising. It would also be useful in such a study to When the children were asked to valuate the nutri- provide comparative data by observing parent -child tional value of foods eaten at meals and for snacks, decision-making in regard to an unadvertised prod- uct. )2The study defined a purchase influence attempt as the child's making an independent request for an item (by asking, A subsequent .study (Galst and White, 1976) pointing, putting it in shopping bakket or grabbing), buying an :neasured preschool children's attempts to influence item with his'her own mune), making a det.ision when given the purchases while accompanying their mothers at the choice by the parent

104 1 1 1 sweets (notably candy and soft drinks) were consist- possibility that these children's enjoyment of break- ently described as being "not so good for you" in the fast was related to their liking ot cereals specifically context of mealtime foods. However, this evaluative advertised on ; hildren's programs distinction broke down somewhat for afterschool snacks, in that one out of f. ur of the children men- Atkin (1975f) attempted to record references by tioned sweet foods as "especially good for you or children to nutritional value or the presence of a healthy Following a similar line of questioning, premium offer as a reason for their selection of a another researcher, Disson (1974) asked 4th-7th particular cereal from the supermarket shelves graders about their sources of nutritional informa- Although almost none of the children eYplicitq tion. Less than half the children could identify a mentioned the general nutritional value of a prod- specific source for their information about nutrition. uct, nearly half of the children appeared to take ac- Those who did identify a source mentioned parents countof the premium in making their selection 11 most often and television only infrequently. However, this figure is based upon observer opinion as well'as actual mentions More recently, a series of studies commissioned by the Council on Children, Media and wierchandis- In other study, Atkin (1975b) varied the prod- ing have investigated the use of graphic materials to uct cliims presented in 10-second segments of a 30- convey nutritional information to children second food commercial and then compared the two (Feshback, et al., 1976). First, preliminary research different messages ininforming and persuading was carried out in order to test alternative graphic children about the same cereal product In one com- representations of a food's nutrient content (the pro- mercial, which Atkin characterized as a "traditional tein, vitamins, minerals, and calories contained in a emotional" claim, the assertion was made that the serving). On the basis of this pilot work, a spaceman cereal provides energy to become "a great swimmer" "Nutrition Computer" graphic, intended to be incor- and "to do great" in school. The other "rational in- porated into food advertisements directed to formation- oriented" message specified Pour vitamin children, was designed on whose chest area were bar ingredients in the cereal and made the claim that the graphs displaying the nutrient content for selected vitamins provide energy to "work hard" in school 14 advertised foods. A sample of 88 children ages 4 to 10 were exposed to this figure either with or without Each of 500 children (3 to 10 years old) was ran- a prior orientation-training session. The findings in- domly assigned to view one of the two messages and dicated that more than half of the Ist through 4th then interviewed No significant differences were graders (6 to 10 years) exposed to the graphic plus found between the children's overall recall of the orientation were able to reproduce correctly the two com,nercials (that is, the "subjects tended to nutritional information on a wooden spaceman learn the content presented in the version viewed") model with adjustable bar graphs. More interesting, The children were also equally less likely to express they were also able to evaluate the nutritional value preferences for tic prfiduct, regardless ot the version of hypothetical foods as depicted by means of the of the commercial they viewed According to Atkin, spaceman graphicHowever, it was not until 4th hisfindings demonstrate thatthe "information- grade that most of the children could reproduce and oriented" message strategy can be at least as effec- generalize information from the graphic without tive as the "conventional emotional" approach in prioi orientation (which could be provided, for ex- conveying product information and achieving ample, through school curricula or public service favorable responses from children announcements). Haefner, Leckenhy, and Goldman (1975) at- tempted to investigate the persuasiveness of four In terms of expressed attitudes toward meals, the Gene Reilly Group found that children mentioned "In regard to the salience ot the premiums incereal breakfast least often as their favorite meal (16 per- purchases, Romter ( I975) found that most children when asked cent) and dinner most often. The reasons given for to reproduce the back of a cereal box. include a premium in their drawings The ineidenix of this ,H.Lurrem..e increased with the liking breakfast less tended to involve not having age of the child. from 19 percent among fist graders to 75 per enough time or not being hungry For those children cent ot the fifth gra0_.rs Other studies on this issue are rex sewed who liked breakfast best, specific food preferences in Chapter 4 were usually responsible, with cereal being the pri- "Energy claims arc one of the issues under ,onsideration in mary "liked" food. The study did not pursue the the FTC s proposed trade regulation rule on toad aLkertising commercials against which complaints of possible needs to be extended and replicated, "the results do deception had been reviewed by the FTC. One of the suggest that the cereal commercials have the commercials, for Wonder Bread, was finally, judged capacity to lead children to engage in behavior that deceptive bythe Commission. A sample of ,34 increases risk to their physical being children in the 2d grade and 102 in 7th arldFith grades were interviewed before and after their view Atkin's survey (1975e) of 506 children (4th ing of a short film in which one of these four corn- through 7th graders) provides evidence of a positive m'ercials was inserted The advertising had no sig- relationship between children's reports of their ex- nificant effect on the children's reported liking of the posure to television advertising for cereal and candy products or their preference for the product in a and their consumption of these kinds of products. choice situation with three competing brands However, the relationship between reported ex- However, the 2d graders' acceptance of product posure and consumption is reduced when parents claims was influenced by all four advertisements, impose restrictions on their children's eating habits. and both the younger and older children exposed to the Wonder Bread commercial exhibited significant Finally, an experimental study by Gorn and change-, in their levels of acceptance of the specific Goldberg ( I 976b) assessed a variety of children's product claims made in that message (e.g, "Wonder responses to both single and multiple exposures to Bread is the best thing your mother can give you to commercials for a food product. The researchers grow fast") The Wonder Bread commercial was the used commercials for a brand of ice cream unknown only one of the four for which the older children to their 151 8 to 10 year old subjects thus permitting showed a shift (a lower level of disagreement) in a direct assessment of the extent of learning as a their belief of the prod! claims The authors con- function of exposure to TV commercials A control cluded that younger children are generally more group saw a program with no commercial inserts, likely than older children to be persuaded by prod- while other groups were randomly allocated to con- uct claims in commercials This tentative evidence ditions in which either one, three, or five commer- of the younger children's greater persuasibility sug- cials were inserted in the same program For some, gests the need for further investigation and special the three and five exposures consisted of the same consideration by both advertisers and regulators commercial repeated over again, for others, the The Poulos study (1975) commissioned by the three and five exposures consisted of different com- FTC, represents a pilot effort to examine whether mercials for the same product. the series of Post Grapc-Nuts commercials under review by the Commission had the tendency or The degree to which the children learned the capacity to lead children to pick and consume plants brand name and the number of flavors of ice cream which could he harmful As noted earlier in th;s available (featured prominently in each of the com- chapter, the commercials showed an adult picking me als) was first assessed The researchers found wild-growing vegetation while remarking that itis that any exposure to the commercials resulted in sig- edible In some of the advertising, the picked berries nificant proportions of the children recalling both were put into a bowl with the cereal Four of the the brand name and the number of flavors. commercials were showr to a small sample of 5 to 11 year old children (the average age was 6). Pre- and The commercials were not as eff,Nove inin- post-viewing interviews were .administered to fluencing the children's attitudes toward the adver- measure the children's beliefs about the edibility of a tised brand. Only one group (those exposed to three variety of plants depicted in color photographs different commercials for the ice cream )evaluated it more tavorably "relative to other ice creams they The plants included both familiar edibles (e g, knew torn and watermelon) and toxic plants, some of the latter resembling those pictured in the commercials It -was even more difficultto influencethe The children's ratings of edibility for the toxic plants childien's choice behavior They were told that one most closely resembling those in the commercials in of four snack foods might later he available for them creased much more than their edibility ratings tor and were asked to indicate their choice. None of the either the other toxic plants or the familiar plants groups who had been exposed to the ice cream com- The author concluded that, although the research mercials made more ice cream choices relative to the

106 11 A- control group who had not seen the ice cream com- that advertisers depict breakfast products like mercials Only when offered a second choice "in case cereals within the context of a "balanced meal". their first choice was not available" was one group Commercials for candy and cookie products (those who had viewed five different commercials) typically do not make any nutritional claims and significantly more likely to choose ice cream than generally refer to these foods as snacks rather than the control group as part of a meal

Lastly, Gorn and Goldberg provided each child Mediating variables Children respond differently with ice cream and subsequently measured the num- to food advertising according to their ages. A num ber of ounces each child had eaten (as they viewed a ber of studies report a predictable increase with age second TV program)No significant differences in the product information (e g., brand names) that were roted irt actual levels of consumption as a func- children retain from food commercials as well as in tion of the exposure to the ice cream commercials their knowledge about the validity of nutritional (this was true even when the child's weight was con- claims Younger children seem to express more ac- trolled) ceptance of food product claims than older children and to exhibit greater shifts in belief of product The researchers concluded that there appeared to claims subsequent to commercial viewing he a "hierarchy of effects- operating. Exposure to the commercials readily resulted in the learning of The parents' role in mediating the influence of the new brand name and a particular brand attribute television food advertising on their children is com- (number of flavors) Brand preference, however, ap- plex. Parents may intercede by imposing specific peared less susceptible to influence, and the restrictions on children's consumption of certain ad- children's choice behavior even less so Actual con- vertised sweets, such as candy. Children of mothers sumption behavior was not at all influenced by the with greater knowledge of the validity of nutritional ice cream commercials. claims were reported to make fewer requests for ad- vertised foods. In general, however, several studies Clearly, the impact of foodadvertising on present evidence of a positive relationship between children will vary as a function of the particular children's requests for advertised food products and food in question, the specific commercials utilized parental yielding to these requests, whether ex- and the age of the child, among other factors pressed directly or through "passive dictation." One Nevertheless, the "hierarchy of effects" hypothesis is- study also found children's requests for advertised

{ isistent with previous research using adult sub- foods to be positively related to the amount of itt..ts, and is a paradigm deserving of further con- television they were reportedly exposed to at home. sideration in future research Finally, conflicts between parents and children have been observed to ensue when children's food prod- SUMMARY uct requests were denied

Characteristics of food wrronercials. Food com- Effects. Children have been shown to acquire mercials appearing on children's programs (pri- specific product information presented in food com- marily weekend mornings) representnt limited range mercials. There is also preliminary evidence indicat- of products, including presweetened ready-to-eat ing that information about the nutritional content cereals, candy products, and cookiesIr. general, and value of food products can be effectively com- these commercial messages tend to associate the ad- municated to children both within commercials and vertised foods with specific brand name information, in brief (5 seconds) slide presentations. Studies have with taste flavor descriptions, with "fun,' and, par- also demonstrated shifts in children's beliefs about ticularly in the case of cereals, with premiums and advertised foods following their exposure to specific animated "presenter" characters Food advertise- commercial messages. These may include incorrect ments directed to child-en offer little nutritional in- as well as correct beliefs about promoted food prod- formation 'The nutritive content and value of foods ucts. As noted, children's consumption of advertised are rarely described or emphasized, their contribu- foods is mist often accomplished by influencing tion to a total balanced diet is seldom explained. A family purchase decisions While commercials are notable exception to this practice of not including not permitted to encourage children to make direct nutrition informa-is the NAB Code requirement purchase requests, it is generally assumed that such

10711 4 requests occur atleastin part as the !Ault of Finally, there is the question of using advertising children's development of desires for the foods they to provide nutritional information to young au- see advertised. diences

NEEDED RESEARCH 1. Should children be exposed to a wider range of advertised food products? There is clearly a gap between research evidence 2.Should advertisers include more nutritional which is considered "relevant" and that which is information to provide children with the op- "sufficient" to affect current advertising practices portunity to learn about nutrition as one fac- and regulatory policies.13 Future research can be tor among several in their food choices? more effectively applied to decisions about policy 3.Given the influence of parent-child interac and practice if it is specifically designed with that as tion on food selection, should food and nutri- its objective. For example, content analyses should tion-related television messages be directed be conducted with the specific intent of revealing the to children and to parents" extent to which food advertising presented informa- tion related to industry codes or government regula- Nutrition education has already been recognized tions. Measures could be designed to describe the as warranting some form of national public program ways in which nutrition disclosures were made (how (White House Conference on Food, Nutrition and central to the action or narrative content are they Health, 1970). Television's ability to serve as a ma- how much time or emphasis are they given aurally jor source of information for the American public and/or visually"), the ways in which the foods are marks it for a potentially important role in such a depicted, and the reasons offered for consuming ad- national program What seems to be necessary is the vertised foods. assumption of responsibility by various groups, in- cluding government, industry, and educators, for However, in order to determine what information determining ways to implement such a- program. is actually received by children and whether itis correctly understood,itis necessary for the Lesser (1974) describes some of the research steps Children's responses themselves to be studied and necessary to design and produce nutritionally infor- evaluated. For example, there has been little mative television material for children. First, research investigation of the extent to which children's existing knowledge and understanding of children perceive and comprehend the references to good nutritional patterns should be determined.16 "energy" and to the role of a cereal in a "balanced On that basis, the areas of "nutritional illiteracy" breakfast" which sometimes appear in food commer- most in need of correction can be identified.' / Then, cials. Both the FTC and the MAD (Griffin, 1976) as educational materials are developed, they must be have acknowledged the usefulness of research in tested with children to determine whether they hold which small samples of children are exposed to their attention, are understood, and produce desired questionable commercials and asked specific ques- as well as unintended changes in behavior. Pilot tions about their understanding of those messages. materials can be revised on the basis of this feed- back. Finally, any program of nutritional informa- The longer range effects of television food adver- tion must evaluate its long-term effectiveness for tising on children's food knowledge and eating representative members of the intended audience.I8 For example, do habits also needs to be studied 16A survey of adult consumers* understanding of certain food commercials have a cumulative influence on nutritional phrases and -laims used to food advertising was pre- children's conception of appropriate foods for meals pared for the FTC in the course of its work on the proposed and snacks? Does advertising affect the attitudes trade regulation rule (Response Analysis Corporation, 1975) upon which children base their developing food 17Sorenson and Hansen (1975) and Ulrich and Briggs (1973) preferences' pre' .!nt useful ideas for designing nutrition education curricula for children For example, ABT Associates (1974) evaluated "Mulligan Stew." a 4-H television series on nutrition targeted to 4th to 6th 15This problem is one of growing roncern among the scien- grade children, Cooper and Philp (19741-repoEted on an evalua- tific community (Comstock and Lindsey. 1975, Anderson. Com- tion by the Ontario Milk Marketing Board of its nutrition educa- stock. and Dennis, 1976) tion workshops ,(or elementary school teachers 11)

F08 Chapter 8

THE EFFECTS OF THE VOLUME AND REPETITION OF TELEVISION COMMERCIALS

This chapter reviews the research on effects of effects such as "irritation We shall refer to television advertising resulting from the frequency of this issue as repetition effects. children's exposure to commercials We will use the term volume to refer to frequency of exposure to CURRENT CODES. commercials in general. The term repetition refers to frequency of exposure to a particular commercial I. Long-term exposure effects. The NAB code Four main issues are apparent, three relating to now specifies that nonprogram material (of which volume and one to repetition commercial content is 80 percent or more) must be limited to 12 minutes per hour on weekdays and 9 5 I. That certain long-term effects may result from minutes per hour on weekends during programs "ini- children's exposure to commercials Alleged tially designed primarily for children." These limits effects include a greater susceptibility to per- went into effect on January i ,1976, and compare suasion', development of materialistic values with a limit in 1974 of 16 minutes per hour. During and, more positively, certain consumer adult programing, the limits are 9 5 minutes per socialization effects such as appreciation of the hour during prime time (any station-designated marketing and economic environment. Since it period of 3 5 consecutive hours between 6.00 p.m. is hypothesized that these effects increase with and midnight each day) and 16 minutes per hour at cumulative exposure to television commercials all other times. The NAD has no provisions pertain- as children grow older, we shall designate ing to volume of commercials, it covers specific them as long-term exposure effects commercial practices only 2 That certain effects may result from frequent exposure to commercials through "heavy view- 2. Heavy viewing effects The new NAB code pro- ing" within age groups Susceptibility to per- visions would automatically reduce children's likeli- suasion is the most common of these alleged hood of exposure to commercials for both heavy and effects. To distinguish these more immediate light viewersat least to the extent that they watch effects from the previous long-term category, children's programs This volume restriction applies we shall designate them as hears:viewing to time but not to the number of commercials. Pre- effects. dictably. neither the NAB nor the NAD codes 3 That volume effects are also relevant in the prohibit "heavy viewing" per se short-term via the "clustering" ot commercials in blocks between programs versus distributing 3 (lustering of fee tc The NAB code allows the them between and during programs. Propo- practice ot clustering commericials in blocks be- nents of clustering allege that it helps children tween programs In fact, the code prohibits the op- to discriminate between program content and posite, i e , too wide a of commercials advertising ctintentOpponents allegethat within programs For children's programs and also clustering leads to "clutter" and poorer in- prime time programs, the number of within-program clividual commercial performance, which un- interruptionsislimited to two per half-hour fairly penalizes the advertisers We shall refer program or four per one-hour program Again, this to this issue as clustering effect) volume-related matter is not covered in the NAD provisions. 44 That repetition ot the same commercial results in stronger effects than a single exposure. Most often, the allegation is that increased suscep- 4 Repetition effects. As fatas we can discern tibility to persuasion results from such repeti- from the NAB code there is no iimit on the number tion, but also included are other potential of times a particular commerci.il may he repeated

1011 Nor is there a limit on how rapidly it may be re- minutes.3 Thus, in 1975, children had the oppor- peated, apart from the restrictions on number of in- tunity to see as many commercials but a lower terruptions per hour and tots! nonprogram time per volume of advertising time than in 1971. hour already described. A second 'ype of incidence figure is represented by the extensive data on children's viewing patterns INCIDENCE described in chapter II of this report Note that the estimates of commercial exposure inthese data For the sake of continuity, we will retain in this should be regarded as incidence figures and not as section the "effects" headings used above. However, exposure figuresunless we define exposure as "po- it should be made clear that incidence here refers tential for exposure." The figures are based on merely to potential for effects. The effects themselves children's viewing of programs and are not adjusted are reviewed in the reibarch evidence section. for nonviewing (plus perhaps nonhearing) of com- mercials. Nonviewing can be substantial, as indi- 1. Long-term exposure effecta. Relevant incidence cated in Chapter I. As incidence figures, then, the figures for long-term exposure effects are tied into data show that an average child between the ages of the question of whether or not broadcasters adhere 2 and 11 is presented with about 19,000 to 20,000 to the NAB's restrictions on advertising volume. The commercials per year, or about 50 to 55 commer- indications are that, on average, broadcasters have cials per average viewing day. complied with theselimits. Although no 1976 figures are available for the 9.5-minute w". kend Some suppiementary statistics may be useful in rule or the12-minute weekday rule, a content placing these incidence figures in context. First of analysis was conducted the previous year when the all, the Nielsen data from which the incidence limits were 10 minutes arid 12 minutes, respectively figures are derived reveal a decline in viewing with (Barcus, 1975a; 1975b). The average (mean) per - age. However, the decline is slight-3 hours 47 hour times devoted to nonprogram material were minutes per day for 2 to 5 years olds down to 3 hours 9.5 minutes and 11.9 minutes respectively.' Barcus 41 minutes for 6 to 11 year olds. Thus, it is unlikely also noted that some stations consistently exceeded that the rate of cumulative exposure to commercials these limits slightly (usually by less than 60 seconds) declines significantly over the 2 to 11 age range. Sec- and that all stations monitored exceeded them occa- ond, it rii4 be recalled from Chapter ii that ampx- sionally. However, since approximately 20 percent imately 85 percent of the commercials children are of nonprogram time is occupied by noncommercial potentially exposed to are not shown during announcements,2 the tine figures for commercials children's programs and are therefore not subject to would almost invar..ibly be less than 9.5 and 11.9 the NAB children's code. On the other hand, it minutes. should be noted that noncode commercials are most likely to be on programs that are co-viewed by the Another finding in Barcus' research bears on the child and at least one parent. The overall co-viewing nature of "vol ume." Although the total time devoted incidence is estimated at 45 percent for both 2 to 5 to commercials in children's weekend programing year olds and 6 to IIyear olds.4 declined from 19 percent in 1971 to 16 percent in 1975, the number of commercials was hardly 2. Heavy viewing effectsThe program viewing reduced at all. In 1971, there was one commercial data (and, thus, potential commercial exposure every 2 8 minutes, and in1975, one every 2.9 data) referred to sd far have been based on means or

'Computed from Barcus' data in his Table 17 (1975a) and ',According to Barcus' figures. a tull 98 percent ut commer- Table 21 (1975b) cials in children's weekend programs are now 30 seconds in 2Fcr example, Barcus' weekend report recorded 79 9 percent duration, versus a mixture of 60-second and 10-second commer- programs, 13 Ipercent commercials, 2 8 percent program cials earlier promos (for a total of 15 9 percent "commercial time"), 3 2 per- ',Estimated from 1975 Nielsen data by taking the percent of cent noncommercial announcements, and 1 0 percent taken up viewing in each viewing category (see ( hapter it. Figure a -"1) and by station i d 's, dead air, and other miscellaneous material weighting these by co-viewing levels (see chap.. .)

4 110 1 1 averages. As noted in Chapter ii, there is considera- Barcus (1975a, o, c) happened to include as appen- ble variation around these average figures Although dices the number of times particular commercials the average viewing' figure is approximately 3 75 were aired during his content sampling periods One hours per day, it is likely that this ranges from about report (Barcus, 1975b) covered weekday program- one hour or less per day for light viewers to as much ing between 3 00 p.m and 6 00 p m on ten indepen- as six hours per day for heavy viewers s The range dent and network affiliate stations Over the week of for potential exposure to commercials is thus about afternoon programs the average (median and 5,400 to 32,600 per year or about 14 to 86 per day. modal) commercial was shown only once, only 4 6 Of course, the great majority of children will be well percent exceeded a once-a-day rate and only 0 8 within these ranges, but the estimates do indicate the percent exceeded the twice-a-day rate, with a max- difference between heavy and light viewing within imum of 14 showings of one commercial over the age groups in terms of volume incidence figures. five-day periodAnother report (BarcusI 975a) covered the period from 7 00 a m to1 30 p m. one 3. Clustering effects The normal practice is to Saturday and Sunday in April 1975 on five sta- distribute children's commercials in "pods" tionsthree network affiliates and two independent throughout children's programs rather thanto stations. Over the two-day period, the median com- cluster them at the beginning or end. Usually this mercial was shown twice, although the modal (most means that a maximum of three or four commercials frequent) figure was once; 40 percent of the commer- is presented during a single program interruption cials exceeded a once-a-day rate and 14 7 percent For example, if we assume a 9 5-minute limit per exceeded a twice-a-day rate, with a maximum of ten hour,,with all of those 9 5 minutes filled with 30-sec- showings for one commercial over the two-day ond commercials, plus the two breaks allowed dur- period. The third Barcus report covered two con- ing programs, a typical schedule might be 4, 3, 3, 3, secutive Saturday mornings in November 1975 the 3, 3, commercials over a one-hour period peak pre-Christmas period for children's adver- tiserson the three network affiliate stations Over The only stations to depart from this distributed the two-day period the average (median and modal) format are, as far as we know, the fourPost- commercial was shown once, 21.8 percent exceeded Newsweek Stations (PNS).6 On children's programs, a once-a-day rate and 8 2 percent exceeded a twice- PNS clusters the commercials between half-hour or a day rate, with a maximum of 17 showings. for a one-hour program segments with no in-program in- motion picture promotion, over the two-day tone terruptions.,A typical one-hour PNS schedule would week apart) period.8 be 6, 6six commercials prior to ahalf-hour program and sixatthe end of the half hour ' However, these frequency counts covered over- Although most unlikely, itis theoretically possible lapping or simultaneous programing by ten, five, within current NAB and FCC rules that a cluster of and three stations, respectively A child would have 19 consecutive 30-second commercials could be to be an almost impossibly avid "channel switcher" shown. to encounter this many repeats of a commercial More reasonable incidence estimates for potentially 4. Repetition effects. Repetition, in terms of inci- encountered repetitions are obtaine" II"' allowing for dence, refers to the frequency with which a particu- total possible individual viewing within the lar commercial is shown The content analyses by content periods and correcting them to daily rates. Based on these corrections the probable repetition This is a range estimate, precise variance figures are not rates are as follows. available. 6These stations are located in Hartford. Conn, Washington, D C , Jacksonville, Fla, and Miami, Fla 'PNS also limits commercial time to six minutes per hour in "Note stationi d 's aid plomottons for the stations' :hildren's programing timeslots programs were excluded trom our tabulations Table 8-1

Typical weekday Typical Saturday Typical Saturday afternoon (34 or Sunday morning morning In p.m.), 1975 M April 1975 November 1975

Maximum single com- 1.4 times 2.0 1.8 mercial rate assuming per day continuous viewing Typical (modal) single .1 times .2 .8 commercial repetition rate assuming continuous viewing

In other words, the average child is only likely to times a commercial is repeated but also by the num- be presentewith even the most frequently run com- ber of brand name repetitions within the cm'iercial mercial about twice in one day on a weekend or itself. A content analysis of commercials appearing seven times over a five-afternoon weekday period.9 during children's programs in 1972 and 1973 (Atkin A few commercials, then, might he encountered and Heald, in press) indicated that verbal (audio) about twice a day On the other hand, the average repetitions of the brand name averaged 3.65 per commercial would be encountered only once every commercial; most commercials used between two ten days on weekday afternoons and about once ev- and four repetitions, only a few used one, 'out about ery five weekend mornings. Thesefigures indicate one-fourth used five or more brand name repetitions thatmost commercials (60 to 80 percent) on per commercial. children's programs would be encountered approx- imately once a week for as long as they are run RESEARCH EVIDENCE

Note that the preceding estimates provide no data 1. Long-term Exposure Effects on repetitionrates during adult or prime time programs. They show only the estimated repetition In this section, we shall review studies which have rate for commercials appearing during programtime compared children's responsiveness to television set aside for children on weekend morningsand commercials across age groups. That is, we shall weekdays after school Moreover, the data do not in regard age as an index of cumulative exposure to dic.ate the total number of times that an average child commercials. Since the studies are cross-sectional is likely to encounter a particularcommercial. We cumulative, age-related effects are inferred rather can estimate daily or weekly frequency rates,but we than longitudinally observed within the same child cannot estimate total frequencies from theavailable or group of children.10 data. This would require a month-by-month analysis of advertising insertion schedules (for example from Various effects have been examined as a function Broadcast Advertisers Reports) and is beyond the of age, and there are different ways in which these resources of the present report. could be organized for discussion. We nave chosen to distinguish four categories of effects: (1) cognitive One final repetition incidence statistic is also ger- effects, which are relevant to the question of poten- mane. A fairly common measure ofcommercial tial deception or "deceivability";(2)affective effects, effectiveness is brand name recallBrand name recall might he influenced not only by the number of loThe widely heard criticism of the "lack of longitudinal studies. is not as serious as it sounds Longitudinal studies are of "These maxima could he exceeded if an advertiser were to more theoretical thav practical interest For practical purposes buy a concentrated time block On a single network Probability such as policymaking, cross-sectional studies are quite sufficient it exposure would still he close to these estimates. however, due Reasons for this are developed inthe section on Needed to channel switching between programs Research

112"9 which are relevant to the question of children's feel- cials, or their understanding of commercials, in- ings toward commercials and toward television ad- creases or decreases with cumulative exposure. vertising as an institution, (3) behavioral effects, which are perhaps most relevant to allegations cen- Children report that they trust commercials less tering on children's "susceptibility to persuasion," as they see more of them. This finding holds for com- and thus to fairness considerations; and (4) consumer mercials in general and seems also to be true for socialization effects. which are 'relevant to the assess- specific commercials The percentage of children ment of television advertising's contribution to who trust all commercials was shown to decline from children's general development as consumers 65 percent at the 1st grade level to 27 percent by 3d I grade and 7 percent by 5th grade (Robertson and Cognitive effects. Three types of cognitive effects Rossiter, 1974). For specific commercials, Atkin attributable to cumulative exposure to commercials ( I 975b) found a significant negative correlation of r have been studied. The first of these is attention = .51 (pL .001) between the age of the child and the Two major studies have shown that 8-11 year olds believability ratings of three claims in two commer- learn to pay less visual attention to commercials cials. Robertson and Rossiter (1974) hypothesized than 3-7 year oldsThis finding, was originally that the decline in trust is based on children's in- established by Ward, Lev inson, and Wackman creasing propensity to attribute "persuasive intent" (1972) in a natural observation setting and was to commercials. Note that the decline in trust or replicatechn laboratory settings by Atkin (1975b) believability with age, or an increase in attribution However, Atkin notes that the decline in attention of persuasive intent, does not necessarily mean that with age, although statistically significant, is oniy commercials are any less effectivea point which about two seconds less per 30-second commercial we will discuss later Also, the studies monitored only visual attention whereas auditory attention is also relevant. In fact, Perhaps the most important cognitive effect is brand name recall, a response that could be learned children's ability to understand commercials as a entirely through auditory attention, increases about function of cumulative experience. Rossiter and 100 percent with age (Atkin, I 975b). Robertson (1974, 1976b) examined children's un- derstanding of the conceptual basis of television Of particular interest is the additional finding by commercials interms of six variables which Atkin that recall of message elements in commer- measured children's ability(1) to define the cials also increases significantlyabout 50 per- difference between television commercials from centwith age Since message elements involv e both television programs, (2) to comprehend the exist- visual and verbal stimuli, auditory attention alone ence of an external message source or sponsor: (3) to could not account for this phenomenonIt seems perceive the existence of intended target audiences likely that older childrer, are capable of "process- for commercial messages; (4) to identify informative ing" commercials faster and consequently have less intent in commercials, (5) to identify persuasive in- need to pay much attention during subsequent ex- tent in commercials, and (6) to understand their posures.11 The attention question could he resolved symbolic representational characteristics T.ital - with a simple experiment in which the treatments nitive understanding of commercials was highly cor- consist of new versus familiar commercials and the related with age (r = 45. pi. 001) Further analysis measures comprise auditory ,is K el I as visual atten- (Rossiter and Robertson, 1976b) demonstrated that tion. age and cumulative experience accounted for 40 percent of the variance in cognitive understanding, From a policy standpoint. attention is not a very while differences in social background accounted for important response Concern about the cognitive only 9 percentUsing a similar but partial set of effects of commercials has rarely centered on how measures, Ward, Wackman, and Wartella (1975) attention-getting they areAttention is merely an also found a highly significant increase in children's assumed but necessary condition for more serious cognitive understanding of commercials as a func- effects, such as whether children's trustin commer- tion of age

liFasterprmessing L aid thec:n hetter acquisition and re- It is clear that, for the average child, cumulative tention of information of sImpl> better ahilits, to teL.all the infor- exposure to commercials has a definite positive rela- mation, or hoth tionship to his or her cognitive understanding of

113 what commercials are and what they are supposed to The evidence on intended behavior as a function do. This effect might be due, of course, not only to of age is dependent on the nature of the research experiential learning, but also, following Piaget's measure. An early study by Robertson and Rossiter theory, to age-related increases in children's cogni- (1974) asked children whether they wanted all prod- tive abilitiesIndeed, itis possible to regard these ucts they saw advertised on television Responses In- cognitive measurement results as "structural" evi- dicated the expected age-related decline. 53 percent dence for the proposition that children are less likely said yes at 1st grade, 27 percent at 3d grade, and to be susceptible to deceptive advertising practices only 6 percent answeied affirmatively at 5th grade. as they grow older However, a more moderate question used by Ward et al. (1975) asked children whether they wanted Affective

1975, Bever et at ,1975) For instance, the percent- with age affirmative answers by age group were kin- age of children who indicated that they liked all dergarten, 67 percent, 3d grade, 87 percent, and 6th commercials was shown in one study to decline from grade, 84 percent Consequently, the weight of evi- 69 percent at1st grade to 56 percent by 3d grade dence for a decline in advertising-induced intentions and25percentby5th grade (Robertson and with age 'and cumulative exposure is slight at best. Rossiter, 1974) This affective decline seems to hold for specific commercials as well as for commercials Evidence on the ensuing behavioral effects of re- in general. Atkin (1975b) measured children's over- quest frequenciesisalsofar from clear-cut all for three specific commercials and fcund a Robertson and Rossiter (1974) found a decrease in negative correlation of r = .35 (pi_ 001) with age. request frequency With age; however, the study Atkin's research did turn up one contrary result focused on pre-Christmas television advertising in Younger children are significantly more likely to which almost half of the advertised products were display irritation while watching commercials. relevant as children grow older.12 This criticism However, this finding stands in minor contrast to the may also be applied to the frequently heard in- overwhelming negativity of children's expressed terpretation of a classic study by Ward and Wack- affect toward commercials as children grow older. man (1972) to the effect that children's request fre- quencies decline with age. Studying a wide range of Behavioral effects. An °tit ious question, given the products, these authors actually found only a non- Increase in children's cognitive understanding of significanttendency forrequestfrequenciesto commercials with age and their increasingly nega- decline with age (r = 13, an r of ± .16 would have tive feelings toward them, is whether commercials been required for significance at the 5 percent level). have any less behavioral impact on children as they grow older This is not an easy question to -answer. Detailed inspection of the product-by-product One problem is the distinction between intended data in the Ward and Wackman study indicates behavior and actual behavior. Commercials may In- various trends by age depending on the product in still intentions or desire for an advertised product, questionFor example, requests for toys declined but the execution of these intentions in most cases re- with age, but requests for bicyclet increased. Re- quires the child to make requests to parents. This ob- quests for products which are prearnably relevant viously Introduces other variables pertinent to the requesting behavior, and these other variables may 12Atkin (197Sa1 also hound a decline in request frequency have little to do with the impact of the commercials with age His measure centered on toys, cereals, and two in- per se. Nevertheless, let us examine the evidence for dividual commercials and is almost certainly biased in the same both of these effects SA to children of all ages. such as snack foods and soft products only slightly and to produce a correspond- drinksboth heavily advertiseddid not showany ingly slight reduction in the frequency of advertis- age-related decline in -request frequency Thisnon- ing-induced requests to parents decline effect was substantiated in a later study by Ward et al (1975) in which requests for food prod- How is the slight deClin° in behavioral effects ucts were essentially constant across age groups (X2, n.s ), whereas requests for "child-relevant" products with age to be reconciled with the markedincrease such as toys and games, clothing, and record albums in children's cognitive understanding of commer- cials and with the equally marked decline in their actually increased significantly with age (X2,pL affective feelings toward commercials? In the 01) The earlier Ward and Wackman stud(1972) case of is mu,ch more comprehensive in terms of product increased cognitive understanding, the most.par- simonious explanation Would involve abandonment categories and should probably be favored over the Ward et al (1975) results. Neither study, however, of the implicit hypothesis that children who under- stand commercials better will be less affected by revealed any general decline in request frequencies as a function of age and cumulative exposure to them. This hypothesis assumes that commercialsare commercials in some way "bad," that they are not supposed to persuade children to want the advertised products, or that children become poorer judges of advertised Before interpreting request frequencies as dataon products as they grow older. All are questionable "effects," we must consider the problem referredto assumptions, and there is not necessarily any incom- earliernamel:', that this type of responseis under patibility in increased cognitive understanding not the control of factors other than advertising- induced intent. One obvious factor is the extent to which producing a decline in behavioral responsiveness. parents acquiesce to children's requests and thus reinforce this form of behavior. The Ward and In the case of children's increasingly negative Wackman study (1972) found that parental acquies- feelings toward commercials,itis possible that cence increases with the child's age (r = .20, pi_ .01) childrenare merelylearning an"institutional" To provide a couple of illustrative trends for prod- response from their parents or peers. Certainly, it is ucts relevant to children of all ages The percentage the ackepted thingtocriticize: commercials. of mothers who said they usually yield to children's Howeve, this criticism may be somewhat superficial requests for snack foods was 52 percent for 5 to 7 and m apply mainly to commercials executions year olds and rose linearly to 77 percent for I I to 12 rather 4han to the products advertised. Rossiter and year olds, comparable acquiescence figures for soft Robertson (1976) have .explored this and other drink requests were 38 percent and 54 percent reasons why children's attitudes toward commer- Another "extraneous variable" hypothesis might be cials in general do not necessarily bear any relation- that peer influence or other nonadvertisinges- ship to their attitudes toward advertised productsin periences might induce request behavior. However, particular. Ward and Wackman's results indicated that the role of television advertising in the requestingprocess In sum, long-term exposure effects (or, morecon- was relatively constant across age groups (r = .14, servatively, long-term exposure correlates) include n.s.). That is, television advertising did notseem to a marked increase in children's cognitive under- be displaced by peer influence or other factorsIt standing of televisioncotriercials,, an equally therefore appears that parental acquiescenceor rein- marked increaseinchildren's negative feelings forcement exerts a significant influence on children's toward televislo advertising as an iristitution, and' request frequencies, and that wide reinforcement in- only a slight decline in children's behavioral respon- creases with age, we should discount the earlier im- siveness to commercials. Note that these results im- plication of a constant advertising-inducedrequest ply that children do not become more susceptible to level A more accurate projection, adjusting for the persuasion as they accumulate experience with com- effects of extraneous reinforcement, would bea mercialsThis conclusion holds regardless of 4 slight decline in request levels. Thus, the safestcon- whether one chooses to attribute the slight decline in clusions from the 'available evidence are that the persuasibility with age to cognitive development or long-term exposure effect of commercialsis to to experiential learning Both factors are probably reduce children's intentions or desire for advertised involved.

1 4. ) 2 115 (ormumer social:zit/ioneffects We may alFo An alternative way of assessing the impact of whether, in the long term, television advertising c n- television advertising on children's satisfaction with tributes to children's broader understanding of the product choices is to examine children's disappoint- economic env ironment and to thedevelopment of ment or frustration when advertising-induced re- consumer knowledge and skills. Here,of course, it is quests are denied. The results of tvo studies suggest particularly difficult to isolate the role of television that, cumulatively, advertising does not increase dis- commercials from othersocializationforces. appOintment or frustration levels Robertsonand However there 's no doubt that television commer- Rossiter (1976) found that disappointment over cials play a large roleit only to initiate children's nonreceipt of reque, :d items declinedwith age consumer behavioratallagelevelsWe can although the actual relationship was slightly cur- therefore examine age-related trends with a- fair vilinear 37 percent at1st grade, :4) percent at 3d amount of confidence that we are monitoringlong- grade,-and 25 percent at 5-th. grade Thew findings term advertising effects, albeitin a multiple in- are subject to the earlier commentregarding televi- fluence context (The role of television commercials sion advertised products which decline as a propor- also ap- can he isolated with more confidence when examin- tion of choices after 3d grade The comment ing hens yandlight viewer differencesSee the plies to the study by Atkin (197`c) which found a following section nonsignificant correlation (r = 04) between age-and .reported "conflict and anger" over denial of re- One effect in which television commercials are quests for two products heavily ads ertiseu ontelevi- clearly implicatedischildren's satisfactionwith siontoys and cereals. It is not clear whether disap- choices they have made in favor of specificadver- pointment declines because fewer requests for these tised products Television commercials presumably products are made or whether children become generate expectations about theproduct and its at- more capable of coping with denial tributesTherefore, if children experience greater dissatisfaction with television advertised products as Consumer socialization effects has e been they grow older, it could he contended that commer- measured in a more ambitious manner b) Ward, cials contribute negatively to children's consumer Wackman. and Wartella 11975) Based on age as an socialization index of cumulative exposure to commercials, as well as other types of experience, various consumer Research findings conflict on this point Ward, skills were found to increase from kindergarten to artella, and Wackman (1975) reported data that 'tith grade. Predictably, for example, the number of seemed to indicate an age-related increase in dis- brands with Which children ware familiar was found satisfaction from 38 percentfor kindergarten to increase with age1 his held for tour product chddre" to 75 percent for 6th grade children categories, two of which (soft drinks and gum) are However, their research question was whether the fairly heavily advertised on television Interestingly, children had ever seen something on television that, children's perceptions of brand difterentiation with- when they got the item, was not as good as theyhad in product categories de( reused with ageAgain, this expected Clearly such a question provides no evi- held for television advertised products (e g , dence on the incidence of d. -atisfaction, it meicly toothpaste, peanut butter) Although the authors in- offers the unsurprising result that older children are terpreted this trend as reflecting a decrease with age could also be more 111-eiy to have had at least oneunsatisfactory in the strength of brand preference, it experience of this kind. Robertsonand Rossiter interpreted as reflecting children's increasing recog- (1976) measured aggregate satisfaction withprod- nition of "parity" status between closely' competing ucts received as Christmas presents Theyfound an brandswhich is prof- r-v a realistic asses,ment of increase in satisfact.an with agefrom 84 percent at many such products 1st grade to 95 percent at 5th grade (significant at (1975) also took another niecsure the 01level)However, these figures reflect Ward et al satisfaction with products that were not alwaysad- which mig it he affected b) television ads ertismg vertised on t evision (this w as especially true for the awareness of multiple sources of informationabout older children)Thus, findings on children's new products Ward et al 's d.scussion ofthese dasta satisfaction with product choices remain equivocal implied that children learn to use more sourt.es of at this juncture information as they grow older Howes er, then ktata

.3 116 actually indicated that older children rely more materialism apparently decreases as children grow heavily on television commercials as a source of new older. Despite the convergence of findings, hov ever, product information. To illtistrate, let us take one of the measures of materialism in both cases are such the product categories, snacks, which has wide age- that naive answers as well as materialistic ones could related appeal.'' Children nominated the following produce high scores14 as a source of information about new snack prod- ucts, with multiple responses allowed Some evidence that materialism may be at least temporarily instigated by commercials was provided Table 8-2' in an experiment by Goldberg and Gorn (1976). Children (ages 4 and 5) were randomly allocated to test conditions in which they diu or did not see corn- Information Kinder- 3d 6th ( mercials for the "Ruckus Raisers Barn" (a toy) in the Source garten Grade Grade contextof a10-minute- neutralprogram. The In-store observation 68% 78% 76% children were then shown separate pictures of two Interpersonal informa- boys, with one of the boys empty-handed and the tion 18 24 25 other holding the Ruckus Raisers Barn in front of him. As the experimenter showed a child the t.vo Mass media 32 65 71 pictures, he would say (TV's contribution) (28) (55) (60) I can bring one of these two boys to play with you. I can bring this boy who is not so nice and It is hard to interpret these ..ate simply as reflect- you can play with him and his Ruckus Raiser ing a more rational multi-source search strategy as Barn, orIcan bring this boy who is nice. children grow older Rather, the main trend appeal s - Would you like to play with the nice boy, or to be the increase in the importance of television as v. would you like to play with the boy who is not source of product information. / so nice-and his Ruckus Raisers Barn"

in any L._., it is far from evident that any substan- Some 70 percent of the control group (who had not tial consumer skills are attributable to children's seen the commercial) opted for the "nice boy," while cumulative experience with advertisingThisis only 35 percer of those who viewed the commer- partly because of lack of measurement and partly cials chose the "nice boyThis difference was still because of questionable measurement Consumer significant (although smaller) when subjects were skills develop with children's cumulative experience retested 24 hours later. with products As we shall see in the next section of the, discussion, there is no evidence that advertising The researchers speculated that_if commercials contributes to this process other than perhaps, in can encourage a willingness to disregard negative quite a few cases, to initiate it social values in the short term, they may do so in the 'Ong run as well. This study raises some hypotheses One anal consumer socializati in effect centers on that bear further examination with a wider range of the allegation that cumulative exposure to television operational constructs, a more varied population, commercials leads to materialism. "Materialism" is generally used to mean a preoccupation with money "Atkin 11975c) used a Lomposite measure consisting of and possessions For children this refers to a belief preference for a brand nume cereal (suspect on face validity or value-orientationratherthantoacquisitive grounds and probably biased toward younger children), belief behavior per se, since the latter is usually beyond a that toys produCe happiness and,emoyment in showing off prod- child's controlTwo studies have attempted' to ucts Ward et a1( 1 9 7 5 ) used an "average" of 4-point ratings of tmasure materialistic orientation as a function of agreement with three items (I) "Do you think people would be lots happier it they had more things like color TV's and blig age Atkin, 1975c, Ward et al,1975) Both found a Lars'. (2) -When I grow up the most important thing is to has' significant negative relationship indicating that lots of money (1)#"DO you think that to really he happy when you grow up you hate to have lots of money''" The last item ap- pears to he, redundant and might well encourage a child who I 'The two other categories n the study were to 'd clothes gave a "nonmaterialwicanswer to the second 'fem. to com- whic.41 probably have decreasing and increasing appeal resnec pound itin selflustitication The redder is left to evaluate tile Lively acn ss the kindergarten to 6th grade age spectrum probable validity of both of these indices of materialism

w '<- 1 c) 117 -.1- '--`1 and most importantly, a longer term measurement of cials Using the six-variable measure of cognitive un- a child's development At present, the materialism derstanding described previously, Rossiter and issueand especially its long-term implicationsis Robertson (1974) found a nonsignificant correlation best regarded as unresolved of r = 06 with television viewing Further analysis by the same authors (1976b) indicated that exposure 2. Heavy Viewing Effe(ts differences accounted for less than 4 percent of the variance in cognitive understanding compared with So far we have examined volume effects in terms 40 percent for age and 9 percent for social back- of the average child's responses at each age level ground Earlier we noted that there is considerable variation in the volume of exposure to television advertising Affective effects Heavy viewers tend to have more within each age level. We now turn to a considera- favorable attitudes toward television commercials tion of the effects of television commercials as a Children's attitudes- toward commercials become function of television exposure while holding age more negative as they grow older, but heavy viewers constant, to see whether the heavy viewers differ within age groups hold more positive attitudes than from light viewers in their responsiveness to corn - their peers. For example, Rossiter and Robertson mercials. Once again, we shall organize our analysis (1974) found a significant correlation of r = 10 (PL toterms of cognitiveeffects,affectiveeffects, 05) between attitudes and television exposure with behavioral effects, and consumer socialization, age held constant. (The measure of attitudes in this effects. study included trust, liking, and behavioral inten- tion scales.) Similarly, Atkin's research with specific Cognitive effects. la the previous section, we dis- commercials found a correlation of r = .30 (pL.01) cussed three cognitive effectsattention, trust or between liking and exposure For older children at believability, and overall c..gn:tive understanding of least, it seems improbable that liking of commercials commercialsIt seems ipso facto true that heavy "causes" televisionviewing Surely,interest-in television viewers pay more attention in total to programs leads to viewing. Therefore, itis a fairly commercials than light television viewers This does safe assumption that, for commercials, the direction not mean that heavy viewers necei-Wily pay more of cause and effect is from exposure to attitudes. attention to each commercial, but simply that heavy viewing is likely to result in a grcater aggregate Behimora/ effe( isHeavy viewers expressed volume of attention to commercials stronger behavioral intentions toward television ad- vertised products In November and December of Heavy child viewers tend to place more trust in each year, even children who are normal viewers commercials than lightviewers . Based on become "de fat to" heavy viewers of commercials for believabilityratings of two commercials, Atkin toys and games (Barcus, I 975c) A study focusing on (1975g) found a correlation of r = .22 (pL 01) wrth pre-Christmas advertising by Robertson and television exposure) c No corresponding finding has Rossiter (1976a) found that the proportion of such been reported for commercials in general, but such a toys and games in children's top-five Christmas re- finding4ts implied in correlations between television quests increased by 5 percent over the heavy adver- ,exposure andattitudemeasures whichinclude tising period. This increase occurred at all grade believability or trust scales levelsIst, 3d, and 5tha result significant at the .05 level, with no age x effect interaction.16-Correla- The third cognitive effect, children's understand- turns between choices and various information ing of the general concept of commercials, does not sources indicated that exposure to toy and game appear to wary with exposure to television commer- commercials was the dominant causal factor

The increase in children's behavioral intentions I'Ail correlations reported in tots section in comunction with as a function of pr:.-Christmas advertising exposure Atkin's research are tourth-order partials 1 hat is, they reflect waE paralleled by another behavioral variable re- the correlation between the Nariahle in question and television quest frequencies Atkin (1975g) found a similarly exposure «mtmllsm/ for or portioning out the effects of the following factors age, -,ex race. and school performance (on ap- proximate surrogate for intelligence)Atkins tele% isiones- l',A conservative formula vvasiosed to compute the c percent posure measure is based on reported viewing of %aturdov - orn- figure[he normal 'percentage increaseinterpretation would ir.g children's programs ( Atkin, 19758 l havc been about K to 10 percent grade level 125 II S significant correlation between television exposure fer that expectations were inordinately raised among and request frequencies for toys and cereals (r = 29, young children most heavily exposed to toy and PL .01).17 Both the Atkin study and the Robertson game advertising. That is, heavy viewing 1st graders and Rossiter study focused on products of more rele- had the highest incidence of "disappointment" or vance to younger children. This is not a confounding below expectancy ratings (24 percent). The data also factor, however, since with heavy exposure effects, suggest that by 3d grade, heavy viewers have learned we are dealing with differences within particular age to develop more realistic expectations about prod- groups uctsprobably due to greater experience with prod- ucts as well as greater cognitive understanding of In sum, the evidence from these various measures commercials. This is an example, therefore, of a of cognitive, affective, and behavioral elects sug- volume effect which seems to apply only to a par- gests that heavy viewers, to the main, respond more ticular age groupin this case, The youngest favorably to commercials than their light viewing children peers Results from the Robertson and Rossiter (1976a) Consumer socialization effec isEarlier, we looked study also indicated that heavy viewers experienced at children's satisfaction with advertised products as more disappointment over nonreceipt of advertised an indicator of whether exposure to commercials in products (pi_ .01)Again this effect was strongest volume affects their apparent ability to judge them among the younger age groups 4It was shown that satisfaction with adver tised prod- ucts increases with age.Aowever, the same research Table 8-4 study that provided the evidence for this conclusion (Robertson and Rossiter, 1976a) also found that; Disappointment over nonrecelpt within age groups, satisfaction was lower among of requested toys and games those children who were above average television viewers 1sAlthough the overallresults were Light viewers Heavy viewers statistically significant (p,05), a more detailed 1st grade 30% 44% breakdown indicates that the heavy viewing effects 3d grade 39 44 were primarily4 cated among the younger children 5th grade '24 27

Table 8-3 A contrary result was reported by Atkin (1975g). In Satisfaction with toys and games his study, he found a nonsignificant relationship be- received at Christmas among: tween children's viewing levels and "conflict and anger" reported by mothers following denial of re- Light viewers Heavy viewers quests for toys and cereals A likely explanation of 1st grade 92% 76% the discrepancy in the findings is that Robertson and 3d grade 92 90 Rossiter focused on 'Christmas present requests, whereas Atkin focused on requests throughout the 93 5th grade 95 year. One could argue that expectations of receipt are considerably higher at Christmas time. Also, Since the measure of satisfaction asked children to since many of the younger children's requests were state whether the products were better, the same, or made to "Santa" rather than to parents, there may worse than their expectations, it is reasonable to in- have been less reluctance to admit disappointment in theRobertson and Rossiter studyA contrary possibilityis that the reporters inAtkin's study 1-Unfortunately the maw study of children s request fre- (parents) may have been more reluctant to admit quencies (ward and Vvackman, 1972.) did not include telex iston disappointment by their own children exposure as a variable These investigators did find a very high correlation between 'perceived influence of commercials' and The safest conclusions from these results are that request frequencies as reported by mothers but as the authors under conditions of a very high volume of advertis- observed, the obvious circularity of cause and ettevt here renders this resiili spurious ing(i.e., commercio's for toys and games at Isbleavy and light viewers were do, ided at the me(11,,n in this Christmas), heavy 1 'ewers in the younger age groups study are more likely to have their e pectations about

' 't+ 1216 products raised by advertising. Accordingly, they decreases with age. However, holding abe constant, are more likely to experience less-satisfaction with further analysis by Atkin Indicated that materialism products they receive and more disappointment over was positively related to television exposure (r = those they do not receive. It is not known to what ex- .10, pi_ .05). As remarked earlier, much better in- tent these heavy viewing effects hold under normal struments are required for measuring consumer volume conditions values such as materialism, but the finding seems worthy of mention for future investigation, Turning now to the effects of heavy viewing on children's consumer knowledge and skills; we would Atkin (1975c) also reported another set of find- expect thatif television commercials contribute ings which cast a somewhat different light on the positively to this type of socialization, heavy viewers heavy exposure phenomenon We noted in the pre- would be standouts within each age group. In their vious section that children who are heavy viewers study of kindergarten, 3d grade and 6th grade express more favorable attitudes toward commer- children Ward, Wackrnan, and Wartella (1975) in- cialstrusting them more, liking them more, and cluded,televisIon exposure with 24 other variables in being more likely to respond favorably to them. an attempt to predict various consumer skills via These attitudes should work both ways. If might be regression analysis. Unfortunately, the investigators expected that heavy viewers should aiso respond employed some recombinations of the original Items more favorably toward "prosocial" commercials in their survey and It is impossible to determine from Atkin tested children's reactions toward three public their report exactly which items ended up in which service announcements which advocated, respec- particular new or "hybrid" variable. Even if we tively, use of seat belts, avoidance of littering, and assume that the recombinations are meaningful, the emphasis on nutrition rather than sugar in foods. results provide little evidence that frequent exposure Heavy viewers were no more likely thanlight to commercials facilitates acquisition of consumer viewers to endorse the positions advocated in the skills by children. Sixteen skills areas were used as commercials dependent variables and separate regressions were conducted for each of three age groups. Of the In sum, indications are that heavy exposure to resultant total of 48 regressions, television exposure television commercials (within age groups) tends to appeared in 16 of the equations as a significant pre- predispose children to respond favorably or more dictor variable.'' However, in half the cases, the strongly to "regular" commercials, but that this does relationships were negative, indicating that heavy not generalize to their responsiveness to "prosocial" exposure to commercials was associated with poorer commercials. Nor does heavy viewing appear to acquisition of consumer skills. Moreover, there was result in extended benefits such as fastcr acquisition no systematic pattern underlying the other half of of consumer knowledge and skills. the cases in which exposure showed positive rela- tionships with consumer skills. Because of the rela- tive failure of the television exposure variable for 3. Clustermg Effects two-thirds of the "consumer skills" equations, and because in the remaining third it showed contradic- Proponents of clustering, children's commercials tory results, it is impossible to conclude that heavy in blocks between programs argue that this practice exposure to commercials produces any consumer would help childrentodistinguishadvertising socialization benefits material from program material. Two studies have been conducted on clustering effects. Atkin (1975b) tested children's reactions TO seven commercials in a On the supposedly less beneficial side, letus briefly reexamine the issue of materialism We may single clustered presentation versus the same com- recall that, despite the questionable validity of the mercials distributed in four groups: once before, measures, both Atkin (19750 and Ward etal twice during, and once after a half-hour program. (1975) provided data suggesting that materialism Duffy and Rossiter (1975) used a somewhat more representative format in that the cluster condition was taped from an actual Post-Newsweek children's 19Ward et al (1975) tallied 21 instances ny which television program. Fourteen 30-second commercials ap- exposure entered the regression equation However inspeLtion sixin acluster of their data reveals that only 16 of these involved coefficients peared aroundthisprogram, significantly different from 0 at F(24,200) which are the approx- beforehand and eight in a cluster afterward. A dis- imately applicable degrees of freedom for thci4 study t ;fluted version of the presentation was constructed

12012 7 by distributing the corriiiici cials in a 3, 4, 4, 4 se- younger children are the main subject of concern, it quence before, after, and at two logical breaks with- seems that clustering does not affect attention and in the half-hour program, preserving the original may actually Increase it. order of the commercials The other measure of Interest to advertisers is Neither study can provide unequivocal evidence commercial recall. Adult research by Weili.(1974) for or against the hypothesis that clustering aids indicated in a labr'ratory study that the clustering of more than six commercials in a series leads to a sig- program-commercial separation (see Chapter1), because neither methodology employed in process" nificant drop in brand name recall for commercials discrimination measures The Duffy and Rossiter which follow the sixth. However, this finding'is con- study attempted to Infer discrimination based on tradicted by fieldresearch which Indicated no shifts in the children's visual attention between the differences in recall when up to eight commercials last two minutes of program precedirk each onset of (the maximum number studied) are clustered commercials and the commercials themselves. together (Ephron, 1975). The children's studies by Amon/ the youngest children in the study (1st grad- Atkin and 'Duffy and Rossiter support the no- ers) the clustered format, which included an audio difference contention. Neither study found any commercial warning, actually produced a smaller difference in brand name recall for the clustered attention shift between program and commercials versus 'distributed presentations. than the version with regular distribution. To the ex- tent that visual attentionis an indicator of dis- Ephron also presented a Gallup and Robinson criminadon, no evidence was obtained in favor of field recall study which showed a deleterious effect the hypothesis that clustering'aids children's if commercials for competing brands within the program-commercial discrimination ability. This same product category appeared within 30 minutes finding should not be accepted as conclusive, of one another. Since the closer the two commercials however, in the absence of corroborating evidence were together, the weaker the recall, it appears that from more direct measures of discrimination clusteringcould be detrimentalforcompeting brands, although not for noncompeting brands. Neither of the children's studies was in any way ex- Both studies were able to provide evidence rele- tensive enough to test this Ideaan important one .vantio the "advertisers' hypothesis" that clustering from the advertiser's standpoint. produces poorer commercial performance TwO measures frequently used by advertisers are au- Another problem in all of the recall data cited dience ratingsa rough, relative measure of atten- above is that none of the studies investigated cluster- tionand recall scores Atkin (1975b) found that ing effects for new commercials versus familiar ones. clustering actually produced significantly greater Itis quite possible that, at the time at which the visual attention than the normal distribution Duffy clustering effects were measured, familiar commer- and Rossiter (1975) found that clustering produced cials could already have attained their peak recall slightly, though not significantly, greater visual at- levels from previous exposures. In this respect, the tention among 1st graders but significantly less at- clustering manipulation, particularly in the experi- tention among 4th graders.20 Paradoxically, the mental studies might be quite superfluous since result for the older children seemed to be due not so recall may actually have beenattainedunder much to the commercials as to apparent irritation unclustered, or at least differently ordered condi- with the long stretch of program occasioned by the tions. A totally new commercial that appeared clusteredformat, perhaps older children have regularly in the middle position of an extended become used' to-commercial breaks. In any case, if cluster might well haveitsrecallperformance reduced. 2OThe Ward. Levinson. and Wackman (1972) study of children's visual attention provides a held validation of the In sum, the evidence on clustering as a (short- Duffy and Romer attention measure Ward et at 's study was a term) volume phenomenon is inconclusive. Cluster- naturalistic survey conducted during in-home viewing. Duffy ing does not appea: to help children to discriminate and Rossitees study was qonducted in a potentially "artificial" between commercialsand programs, but more valid classroom setting Yet. using identical rating systems. the two studies yielded highly similar attention scores (see Duffy and measurement is needed before this primary argu- Rossiter. 1975) ment. in support of clustering is settled. Similarly,

121 12 8 there seems to be little evidence to support adver- as the type of response relevant to commercials.22 tisers' fears that clustering will affect the perforin- Pending first-hand evidence from studies of ance of individual commercials, but neither can they children's commercials it therefore seems extremely put to rest sincenewcommercials and competing doubtful that differences in repetitionrate,at the fre- brands have not been studied in the clustered format quency that children are likely to encounter them, proposed for children's advertising would make any difference in :erms of lea, rung whatever is advocated by the commercial. 4.Repetition Effects A second parameter is the number of repetitions. Our final topic in the area oevolume and repeti- We could not estimate from the incidence data how' tion is repetition, which is defined as frequency of many times children see particular commerc.als. exposure to a single commercial rather than to com- However, we may guess that 10 times is a fairly typi- mercials en masse. There are two principal allega- cal figure and that three or four times might be a tions concerning repetiticn of individual commer- minimum figure unless the commercial is backed by cials. One is that repetition leads to stronger persua- an extremely light media expenditure. Secondary sion effects. This is an allegation that interests ad- research in learning (Hulse et al., 1975) again sug- vertisers as well as critics of advertising, and we can gests that the number of exposures may not make review the evidence for this contention without much differenceat least for learning. In fact, some regard to whether stronger persuasion is valued as theorists have recently argued that the effect of desirable or undesirable repetition is not so much to increase learning as to prevent forgetting.That is, many of the fairly simple The second allegation is that repetition produces responses associated with children's commercials "irritation." There has been no research on repeti- e.g., brand names and basic brand attributes such as tion and irritation with children's commercials, appearance, vitamin content, or premium offers although Greyser (1973) reported this to be a fre- may be learned in just one or two trials, and the quint complaint about commercials among adults. effect of further repetitions would be to ensure that We shall regard irritation as being a secondary effect the child "re-remembers" what was originally of repetition and one that is rather arbitrarily and learned and might otherwise forget. subjectively defined.2I In any case, as mentioned, there is no children's research to review. Of more in- This theory has interesting implications. Besides terest and importance is the first allegation: the rela- proposing that repetition may make a child less tionship between repetition and persuasion. likely to forget a brand name, it also suggests that repetition would not be likely to change a child's at- Incidence of repetition estimated from the Barcus titude toward the brand, since what would be data earlier suggested that the average child sees remembered would be the originally learned at- most commercials (60 to 80 percent) at a rate of titude Thus, unless we are willing to accept an argu- about once a week, but that some may be seen as ment to the effect that brand name recall is a suffi- often as twice a day or more Doesratemake a cient definition of persuasion rather than the com- difference? Secondary evidence from a review of monly understood definition involvingachange in learning 1 esearch indicates that "learning proceeds attitude or behavior, it is possible that repetition at just about the same rate regardless of the interval or at least beyand one or two exposuresactually of time that elapses between successive responses" has no effect on persuasion. In fact, this theory fits (Hulse, Deese, and Egeth, 1975). For "responses" in the available data remarkably well. Adult research our case, we may substitute "attentional exposures" in a number of studies by Ray, Sawyer, and Strong (see Ray, 1973) has shown that the effect of up to six

2IFor example. Greyser notes that people are far more likely to complain about corninercials fo; product categories that don't 22For the more theoretically inclined sve should note that this interest them and to like equally repetitive commercials for conclusion applies only to operant learning and not to classical product categories that do interest them People may also be conditioning The response of paying attention to commercials. more likely to remember commercials that irritate them despite atleast beyond initialattention,is clearly a voluntary or the amount of repetition "operant" response

122 129 repetitions of a commercial is mainly to increase In sum, there is no evidence that either the rate or brand name recall. Measures of liking and intent to number of repetitions affects children's tendencies to purchase the product are minimally affected by be persuaded by a commercial. Number of repeti- repetition. tions can affect brand name recall, but neither at- titudes nor desire for the product seem to be affected Most "laboratory" studies of children's commer- beyond one or two exposures. cials reviewed in this report have shown the test commercial only once. The exception is Goldberg Finally, there is tt e issue of brand name repeti- and Gorn (1974) who examined the effects of one tion within a commercial. Incidence figures put this versus three exposures to the same commercial. at 3.65, on average, with perhaps a quarter of all They found no increase in children's attitudes or children's commercials repeating the brand name choice behavior ieyond the single exposure results. five or more times (Atkin and Heald, in press). The Another laboratory study by Atkin (1975c) pro- Duffy and Rossiter (1975) study examined the rela- duced interesting evidence on the field effectiveness tionship between audio brand name mentions and of repetition. Approximately 100 4th and 5th grade brand name recall. The correlation was effectively children were shown a commercial for a well-known zero (r=0.06, n.s.). This could either be interpreted complexion remedy; half of this group saw it once, to mean that the recall aspect of the repetition theory the other half twice, a few minutes apart. Another does not hold for within-commercial repetition, only 100 or so children from the same sample base were for separate exposures to a commerZaal, or that with not shown the commercial. Atkin analyzed the familiar commercials, as these may have been, brand results in terms of those children who reported hav- name recall had reached a ceiling. Even if brand ing already seen the commercial at home, versus name repetition were effective, brand name recall, those for whom this was an initial exposure. Correla- as an effect, would not in our view constitute suffi- tions with the laboratory treatment variable (shown- cient evidence of persuasion. It is, of course, con- not shown) were as follows: ceivable that brand name repetition affects other responses such as attitudes or intentions; however, as Table 8 -5 discussed earlier, this seems unlikely. Also, repeti- tion as a phenomenon "within" commercials has not, Seen Wore First to our knowledge, been a really controversial issue Effect mossurel at ham Encounter compared with the more general phenomena of rate and number of individual commercial repetitions. Information .02 .22 recall SUMMARY AND NEEDED RESEARCH , Believeability .02 .27 Liking of brand -.07 .28 1. Long-term exposure effects. Perhaps the most Intent to buy -.08 .27 Impressive body of research on children's responses brand to television commercials has been that which docu- ments the dramatic age-related increase in their cog- ,Atkin's longer list of effects measures is summarized here nitive understanding of the nature and purpose of and appropriate correlations are averaged for convenience of commercials and the equally dramatic decline in presentation see Atkin. 1975c) their feelings toward television commercials as an institution. Paradoxically, however, their behavioral These reIts indicated that the "additional" lab ex- responsivenessto commercialsas reflected by posure Ifid essentially no impact on those children stated desires for advertised products and by fre- who had seen the commercial before. In contrast, quency of requests to pare -Its for these products "first time" viewers showed. significant effects.23 A declines only slightly over this childhood period. separate analysis was conducted on those who saw The paradox involves a real contradiction only if we one versus two lab exposures of the commercial. assume that commercials should not be responded Again, consistent with the theory, the only effect of to. If most commercials children see are nondecep- the multiple exposure was on brand name recall. tive and promote bona fide products, there is no reason why increased cognitive understanding 2,Estimating the degree of freedom for these correlations at should reduce children's preferences for the prbd- 50, with bivariate correlations an r of 27 would be significant at ucts advertised (nor why lack of cognitive under- the 05 level standing should increase them). To hold other than

123 1.30 this view is to assume that deceptive practices and tle evidence in current research to suggest that shoddy products are the rule in childreln's television longitudinal studies with individual children would advertising. Still, part of the paradox remains: why yield results that are significantly different from do children develop such negative attitudes toward those derived from representative figures for the advertising as they grow older? "average" child at various age levels.

The answer is probably just what it appears to 2.Heavy viewing effects.Heavy viewing effects bethe acquisition of attitudes toward advertising are somewhat different from long-term exposure which are currently prevalent in society. There is no effects in that they focus on the effects of volume evidence that children generalize this attitude to the within age groups rather than on the cumulative products promoted in children's commercials. volume across different age groups. Perhaps the most Important conclusioninconjunction with Still unresolved is the question of whether long- heavy television viewingand, by implication heavy term exposure to television commercials increases exposure to commercials=is that such viewing does children's consumer socialization. The studies in this not retard children's cognitive understanding of ad- areaallsuffered from either inconclusive vertising. On the other hand, heavy viewing does not demonstrations the role of advertising in the accelerate it either. The evidence on cognitive un- socialization process and/or poor dependent derstar,ding seems to be clearly in favor of some sort measures of consumer socialization phenomena. The of Piagetian explanation in which developmental solution to the first limitation Involves more precise processes are dominant and stimulus experience, "tagging" of advertised versus nonadvertised Items beyond a minimally typical level, does not seem to as well as comprehensive measurement of other matter much. variables thataffect consumer learning besides television advertising. The role of television adver- However, heavy television viewers at each age tising could then be assessed through experimental level do tend to hold more favorable attitudes or statistical controls. toward commercials and toward products they see advertised. This is especially pronounced at younger The solutiontothe secondlimitationisto age levels, at least until 1st grade, and there is some develop more valid and reliable measures of con- evidence that this heavy viewer difference persists at sumer socialization effects. The validity problem is a noticeable but diminished magnitude thereafter. complex, but we could proceed well beyond the cur- But we cannot conclude that this apparent tendency rent state of measurement by excluding measures for heavy viewers to be persuaded somewhat more with questionable facevalidity. The reliability often than their lighter viewing peers has anything to problem is simple by comparison, since it Involves do with poorercognitivecapacity; this simply does basic procedures of an Item analysis and measure- not differ as a function of individual viewing levels ment replication (see Rossiter, in press). within age groups.

A type of research thatis needed mainly for A possible explanation is that heavy viewers tend theoretical reasons is longitudinal measurement of to see more products, and more products that they commercial exposure effects on a single child like, so that their aggregate liking of commercials Longitudinal measures could then be replicated with and aggregate total of expectations based on com- many children to provide a more generalizable pic- mercials is higher because of differential product- ture. The predominant alternative, of course, is the generated reinforcement. This explanation is sup- reliance on cross-sectional methodology to infer ported by the supplementary finding that heavy longitudinal effects. viewers are no more favorably predisposed than light viewers toward commercials such as public Indeed, cross-sectional research is quite satisfac- service announcements which do not involve "lika- tory for most decisions faced by children's advertis- ble" products. We must conclude from existing evi- ing policymakers. No one has yet mounted a con- dence that heavy viewers may be more 'ften per- vincing argument as to how or why longitudinal suaded, since they see more commercials, but that research would greatly improve our knowledge of this does not involve notions of deception or advertising's effects on children. There is, in fact, lit- deceivability.

131 124 Robertson and Rossiter's research has offered the the advertisers' hypothesi., that clustering is an un- best measures of children's cognitive understanding fair imposition on the right of individual advertisers of commercials to date because they are based on a to reach their potential audiences, we reemphasize comprehensive model of cognitive criteria the importance of employing commercials never developed from work by Paiget and Guilford. seen before. Otherwise, cluster effects will be con- However, further refinement and testing would be a fused with previously established responses to the worthwhile endeavor because so many policy deci- commercials. This research should be particularly sions in children's advertising have to do with decep- careful to control for serial position or order effects tionand, specifically, withthe capacityto be since these are critical to the contention about clut- deceived or misled What we need is a valid and ter. reliable test to establish this capacity. As yet untested is a corollary to the advertisers' The firststudy that should be conducted to hypothesis that commercials for competing brands "norm" such a test should involve girls as well as are more detrimentally affected by the clustering boys the latter were the subjects of the original format than commercials for noncompeting brands. Robertson and Rossiter research)Other studies This corollary introduces an area of research that (e.g.,Forguson,1975) suggestthatacross age goes beyond the clustering issue. Competitive brand groups, girls will not substantially differ from boys, choice has rarely been investigated with children, consequentry, the preceding conclusions did not especially with the predominance of single-commer- allude to whit, would otherwise appear to be a defi- cial samples in children's advertising research. cient sampling base. However, this should be cor- roborated by studies in which the cognitive measures 4 Repetition effects We can be more conclusive are taken with children of both sexes regarding the effects of repeated exposure to in- dividual commercials There seems to be little basis 3. Clustering effects A third aspect of the volume for concern that repetition leads to greater persua- issue is posed by the question ofwhether clustering sionunless by persuasion we mean no more than of children's commercials would benefit children the ability to remember the brand name mentioned or perhaps penalize advertisers Theevidence on in the commercial. A considerable amount of sec- both questions is inconclusive. The separation issue ondary research in learning theory and the available isunresolved because appropriate measures of primary research indicates that neither the rate at children's discriminatory ability have notbeen which children encounter a commercial e ,_ fre- employed in studies to date. The clutter issue is quency per program or per week) nor the total num- unresolved because experiments on clustering have ber of tunes they encounter it, beyond the first one or used nonnovel commercials whose effects g , two expcisures, has any incremental effect on either brand name recall, brand attitudes) could have been their liking of the brand or their intention to request attained prior to the clustering manipulations in the or buy it. The effect of repetition seems to be mainly studies. A further controversyarises overthe to prevent children from forgetting theiroriginally possibility that commercials for competing brands learned reactions to the product within the same product category may suffer most from the greater likelihood of juxtaposition under a However, these conclusions about the rate of clustered format repetition stem from secondary research. Although the theory and findings are substantial, a replication The program-commercial discriminationissue with children's commercials as the stimuli would received detailed attention in Chapter Iof this establish whether or not the absence of a rate effect report. Recommended Improvements in generalizes to our specific domain of interest. Con- methodology such as the use of signal-stopping tech- clusions about the number of repetitions derives niques are equally applicable to research oncluster- fromboth primary and secondary research. ing which seeks to test the hypothesis thatthis pro- Therefore, further research in this area is not a prac- posed format for children's commercials offers a tical priority, although repetition poses a fascinating policy-relevant benefit Regarding future studies on pursuit for the theoretician

1 '3 ') 125 Chapter 9

THE EFFECTS OF TELEVISION ADVERTISING ON CONSUMERSOCIALIZATION

The term 'consumer socialization" refers to the multiple factors that play a role in the process of a continuous, on-going process by which children child's consumer socialization learn skills, knowledge, and attitudes relevant to theirpresent and future hehas ior as consumers RESEARCH EVIDENCE (Ward, 1974) Although there islittle agrsement about what skills, knowledge. and attitudes com- In this, section, we will examine as adahle e\ prise "consumer socialization,-itisunderstood dence on, three major polio questions relating to various agents have a role in this processparents, consumer socialization peers, schools, the communit\ , as well as television advertising I the prose titflow does consumer socialization occur')

In some form, ads ertising surely affects children's 2lin fluent e% un the pro( e%5V% hat is the role of learning about the marketplace Parents report that ads erti-sing relatis e to other influences upon children readily learn brand names and Jingles from the child' advertisingResearch has ins estigated various is the impact on adult aspects of children's learning from commercials The Inatome%IA hat hehas tor of childhood consumer-related ex- e.g sources of product information (Caron and Ward, 1975), consumer processing skills (Ward et periences' al 1976), and learning about products and pre- cha«miet ut cont urner tot itilizatum lhree studies miums (Rubin, 1972, Shimp et al, 1975) pertaining to the impact of early experience on later hehas tor are examined below 1 he illustrate the But what is the nature of advertising's contribu- ways in which the outcomes of consumersocializa- Does ads ertising' tion to ,consumer socialization tion have been consideredGuest (1942, 1955) a broader understanding of the contribute to simply examined consistent:\in brand references marketplace, and does it help to develop "good" or between childhood and adult years. Arndt (1971) "effective- consumer skills during childhood and attempted to examine parental impact on Older later years'' Or, does advertising interfere with the offspring, andV\ ard, Wackman. andV\ artella development of such skills' 119762,detmed consumer socialization outcomes in terms of the development of information-processing A prior question which must he answered is shat skills particular skills, know ledgeind attitude,: comprise "effective'' or "good- consumer hehas orAlso, do In one of the Less longitudinal studies in consumer thelong-termeffectsof consumer socialization research, Guest inters iest, cd suhiects regarding superSede the short-term effects of specific ads cuis- brand loyalty and then reintersiewed them atter a ine In other words, if children aremisled" by in- I 2 -sear utters alAt the time of the original inter- dividual commercials, or do not fully understand s sews (1942),the suhiects(3dto12th grade some of them, is there evidence of cumulative, long- children) reported their 'favorite brands I he sub- term harm'' How do se define harm' Finally, how sequent inters sews to 1954 indicated a strongdegree do we compare consumer hehas ior for those ex- of 'brand loyalty," since about one-third of the sub - posed to television advertising and those not ex- jects maintained their preference for earlier-named posed, since sortually all children grossing up in this brands, es en when present use age at original inter- country are exposed to television from An early age' s few, IQ, and socioeconomic skutus werecontrolled How es er, Guest's studies suffer from the common Sonic of these questions Acre addressed in the difficulties of longitudinal studies, in that onls 20 preceding chapter of this report, in terms of the percent of the original subjects could he located effects of children's cumulative exposure to telesi- and or returned use mail reinters few questionnaire, shin advertising user time Here, sse A ill examine and brand availabilit \could not he controlled the issues in terms of tele\ ision ads ertising as H:u: of herefore, his data cannot be considered((Oil usis c

1 :3 3 12/ Arndt examined parental influence on ottspring's Cross-sectional data do exist. however, to descrihe consumer behavior, finding significant agreement the average development of consumer skills during between college students and their parents regarding the age span of approximately 5 to 12 sears favoritestores, brandloyalty, and suchsocial characteristics as opinion leadership and ulnas at IV e onsumer sic mil:atom o« ors Data arc scant ness Arndt reported that college students and their and indirect with regard to consumer socialization parents differed concerning their perceptions ot the Bandura (1971) points u the importance ot an im- importance of brand differences for carious pro- itation theory ot learning for studies of consumer ducts, but it is not clear whether different kinds of socialization, and some recent research pros ides evi- items were purchased by college students and their dence that parents expect their children to learn parents or whether they purchased the same items consumer skills through imitation rather than but used them differentlyArndt also reported through directed training by parents or schools parent-offspring similarity in "behavioral variables' (Ware, et al , 19761 (favorite stores. opinion leadership, and willingness to try new products). but these tindings may correl- The dominant theoryinstudies at consumer ate with other variables, since the students Ilsed at socialization concentrates on cognitive develop- home Moreover, the small sample (55 students and ment A study hs Ward. ackman, and Wartella parents) does nut - permit generalizations to the large 11975) attempted to integrate ideas about cognitive population at college-aged students who do nut Its e development and tamely interaction Atter review- with their parents ing research on parental influences un children, the authors identified tour classes ot family influences Ward, Wackman. and NA, artetla sought to define which were expected to attest consumer socializa- consumer socialization in terms at the des elopment tionatvarious stages otchildren scognitive of certain intormation-processing skillsI heex- des elopment amined data from 615 mother-child pairs. including equal numbers of children in kindergarten, 3d, and I Goats mothers has e for their children's con- 6th grader. "Higher' andlower- lock ot infor- sumer learning, mation-processing skills in the children were con- ceptualized by the authors aLLot ding to the cognins e 2Mothers' behas tar as msumers, focusing on abilities required For example, higher loci skills (heir use ot intormation in consumer decisions, included awareness of the purpose ot ads ertising 1,1(010 -tlnld mrevuen.arl (Tull dill ((111114111'r and use of multiple and abstract teatures ot products tit 111(MS in bath mother-initiated and Lhild- in brand comparisons. while lower les el skills in- initiated learning situations. cluded brand comparisons on the basis at perceptual attributes and awareness ot a few sources ot product 3 Children's (Tportumues tar mdepend, ru( informationThe authors found that consumer in- uue he/rattar formanon-proLessing skills were strongly related to the children's age and les el of Lagnaise abilityan the specitiL s ambles included in each category are indication that such skills do indeed des clop from listed in table 9-1. along with the soLioeconomtc early to later childhood However, the relationship status ot the suhieLts of television ads ertising to the acquisition , these skills was not at all clear ur consistent (see chapter Ditterent farnik influences attested higher and Illsser level information-processing skills, depending on the stage at cognitise development ot the child In summary, there arc no firm data to demon- For sounger children.mother-childinteraction strate that childhood Lonsumet experiences, includ- .triableabase) was most strongly related to co ing exposure to ads ertising, has e a direct impact on sumer intormation-processing development. whit, later adult behaviorI here have been no extensis e tot older children, the learning seemed to proceed longitudinal studies, and we do not know which more through ohsers anon and imitation ot mothers' early es ents have a lasting unpaLt shish do not helms tor as consumers (sariablr. 2 ahos ci

128 134 Table 9-1

Family influences Affecting Consumer Socialization

Mother's Consumer Education Goals

1 Number of money goals 2. Number of quality shopping goals 3 Number of bargain goals i 4 Degree of opposition to children's commercials

Mother's Own Consumer Behavior

1 Frequency of using contextual attributes in purchases 2 Frequency of using price/appearance attributes inpurchases 3 Frequency of using advertising attributes in purchases 4 Relative efficiency of information use in purchases 5 Total information use in purchases 6 Number of sources consulted in major purchases 7. Budget planning 8 Budget accounting

Mother-Child interaction About Consumption

1 Frequency of negotiating purchase requests 2 Frepuenc-y of refusal with explanation 3. Frequency of not yielding to purchase requests 4 Frequency of discussing consumption generally 5 Number of comments about commercials 6 Flexibility in responding to purchase requests

Children's Opportunities Variables

1 Number of different sources for money 2. Total child income 3 Frequency of taking child shopping 4 Child power in making purchases 5 Frequency of exposure to television commercials

ek Other

1. Family socioeconomic status

Source Ward. Wackrnen, and Wartella, 1976

performance The authors also found evidence of three ways in 3 By directly influencing the child's abilities. which parents influence consumer socialization as a consumer, regardless of cognitive finding is that By directly influencing the rate of cognitive A somewhat surprising, but tentative 1 some kindergarten children canidentify the selling development of the child, which in turn affects purposes of advertising and displayother consumer- consumer information-processing skills, related skills beyond what would he expectedfrom 2 By influencing the child's use of existing cogni- theoretical descriptions of their cognitive -stage predicting tive skills in consumer situations, abilitiesThe most important variable

129 1 ) 1.a j this higher level skill was the frequency of con- Younger adolescents talked more with parents sumer-related parent-child interaction (variable 3 about specific consumption practices and acts, but above). It this finding is replicated, it would suggest such intrafamily communication was operative for that parents of even very young children may havea all adolescents in mediating betweenexposure to ad- major impact on relatively advanced consumer- vertising and purchases In a partial replication of tnformation processing skills this study, Stephens and Moore (1974) and Moore and Stephens (1974) reported findings similar to Theories of learning, cognitive development, and Ward and Wackman However, in contrast to the farnil; interactiod all may help to increaseour un- earlier study, the researchers found only marginal derstanding of how consumer socialization pro- differenceS between younger and older adolescents ceeds. But actual data do not yet exist The limited in parent-child communication about consumption, research to date suggests that the family plays a cru- and also found this communication to be infrequent. cial role and that the process appears to change over This may be due to different family characteristics in the course of childhood, since parents treat their semirural sample (Ward and Wackman's sam- different ;aged children differently and children's ple was suburban) cognitive abilities change It also appears that early parent-child interaction is a key _element, followed Using self-reports, Fauman (1966) studied the by the increasing importance of observation and im- relative influence ot parents and peers on brand itation in later childhood preferences and brand loyalty among 250 boys (grades 10, H, and 12) from predominantly work- Influences on consumer socialization, Several ing-class families. Both peer and parental influence on brand preferences were found to decrease with studies have attempted to assess the relativein- children's intelligence, but were not related to media fluences of family. peers, and mass mediaon con- exposure time While parental influence decreased sumer socialization. A tew have used self-reports to with age, peer influence remained constant with provide estimates of family. peer, and mediain- age, suggesting that this source ot influence is established fluences on responses to advertising (James, 1971 ), on purchase acts (Teter, 1966), and on drug at- by early adolescence, or about 10th grade. However, titudes and use (Kanter. 1970) The general finding the direction of influence is not clear. If a son is cor- is that parental influence decreases, and peer in- rect in reporting that he and his father use the sa- .e brand, it may he that the son has influenced the fluences increases. with age. Mass media influences are reported to be low and constant over later child- father's brand preferences, rather than viceversa. hood and early adolescent years However, these Brand loyalty was found to increase withage but decrease with intelligence and with increasing media self-reporting measures of influences are of suspect validity exposure

The results with respect to intelligence ere not im- Ward and Wackman (1971) examined the rela- mediately clear Perhaps higher intelligenceis rel- tive influence of parents and media on four aspects ated to lower respect for peers' opinions, or within- of "consumer learning" among junior and senior creased knowledge of consumption alternatives,or high school students (1) recali of TV commercial perhaps given the concurrent effects of lowerIn- slogans.(2)attitudes towardcommercials, (3) tel ligen and higher media exposure, certain materialistic attitudes, and (4) self-reported effects adolescents are more easily persuaded by different of advertising on specific purchases They reported brand appeals Fauman did not explore these in- that learning of slogans is more a function of in- teresting possibilities telligence than of TV exposure time However, at mucks toward advertising andmaterialistic at- Cateora (1963) analyzed teenage consumption titudes were both found to be related to the reasons patterns and relative influences of peers and parents given by the subjects for watching commercials Par- on some of these patterns Self-administered ques- ticularly important were "social utility" reasons tionnaires were completed by189 juniors and i e., the motivation to watch commercials as a means seniors in one high school in a "small" city. Cateora of gathering informationaboutlifestyles and found considerable homogeneity among adolescents behaviors associated with uses of specific consumer concerning various "consumer values and goals," in- products cluding attitudes toward credit and saving, attitudes

130 I 3 to and comparison shopping, relationships among ing contributes to "effective" or "good" consumer quality, quantity, and price, and attitudes toward behavior patterns, whet' er advertising merely pro- merchants and advertisirr These values were vides consulter- related stimuli which provides a relatively independent of social class. catalyst in the process; or whether television adver- tising'sinfluences contribute to any tong-range However, inspection of the data r, several socialization effects. curvilinear relationships between social class and consumer attitudes and practices. Lower classand Data do not exist to support thecontention that upper class students often expressed similar at- long-term consumer socialization obviates short- titudes, but these differed from those of middle-class term issues pertaining to children'scomprehension, adolescents Such results it ay indicate that different of individual advertisements In any case,if one experiences may lead to expression of the same at- oelieves that children have a right to be able to fairly titudeFor example, regarding attitudes toward evaluate advertising messages, it seems advisable to quality vs quantity in buying apparel, the responses separate short-term and long-term effects.regard- of middle-class adolescents who said they preferred less of how long the effects of commercials maylast. There are also no longitudinal clan. to assess the to buy one pair. 1 good-quality shoes rather than several pairs of !,ser quality shoes may reflect their long-range impact of exposure to advertising during Having experienced this choice, whereas the same at- childhood Data do support the contention that ad- titudes expressed by lower class adolescents may vertising contributes to some elements of "consumer reflect thy it desire to be able to make this kind of learning " However, the research emphasizes the advertising's purchase selection primary role of the family in mediating effects and in contrtbuting directly to consumer It appears that the studies of consumer socializa- socialization. tion examined here reflect the conventionalwisdom that parents become less important and peers more NEEDED RESEARCH anportant as children enter adolescence. However, studies have examined only a few aspects of con- The most compelling research need is indicated sumer socialization, and advertising is often not by the long-term nature of consi ter socialization even consideredMore bra are needed before effects. It is clear that further research is needed on definitive statements can be mat . about the relative the extent to which early learning affects later con- influence of different socializing agentsIn these sumer behavior One view is that as pear.'e get older, tuture studies, a major lt,-robleto will be to identify, role changes make early learning unimportant The measure, and compare the degrees ofinfluence of other view holds that t a ly learning remains a prim- different socialization agents. It rr ay he less impor- ary determinant of later patternsof cognition and tant to try to specify how much various influences behavior. Both of these simple views tray be affect behavior than to understand how socializing misleadingIt may be that the question is not how agents combine to affect children. We mightfind, for much early learning experiences influence later con- example, that television advertising provides infor- sumer patterns, but which aspects of earlylearning mation anda sho;t-term stimulus for product are important and which are not dzsires, but parents mediate these outputs in their ottspring over much longer periods of time Such a Perhaps the most vexing problem is.specifying the behaviors possibilityis considered in our next chapter. on outcomes_ must what skills, attitudes, and parent-child interaction. comprise consumer socialization, and :low canthese reflect something of the"'quality" of consumer SUMMARY behavior`' A promising approach may be an infor- mation-processing framework, since it focuses on It consumer socialization is defined in terms of such complei cognitive skills as selection and uSr of development of ski!ls, attitudes, and knowledge information relevant to purchase decisions. Such relevant to consumer behav ,,,r, then itis probably cognitive erations are fundamental to purchasing safe to conclude that television advertising is one of behavior and are mote important and explanatory many influences on the process. Whatis not clear than the simple cataloging of purchases over ti or from existing research is whether television advertis- the measurement of changes in consumer attitiirles.

131 1 3 4I Extremely long-term longitudinal research does "naturalistickinds of data-gathering procedures; not seem necessary or desirable., Rather, research e.g., using purchase diaries followeeby retrospec- could combine longitudinal and cross-sectional tive interviews, rather than simple surveys which design features (see chapter 12) Finally, research on rely only on verbal-report data. consumer soc:llization would seem to requite ,

132 Chapter 10

TELEVISION ADVERTISING AND PARENT-CHILDRELATIONS

There has been great concern about the effects of These concerns are addressed by the following advertising on family relations. This concern takes research questions: several forms. What attitudes do parents held toward Consumption requests by children, par- child' en's advertising? How much do they re- ticularly those attributed to advertising, may sent its intrusion on family Interaction proc- strainarent-child relations. esses? This strain may be greatest among What mediation do parents exercise regarding economically disadvantaged families, who children's viewing behavior? How much parent- do presumably must deny most requests. child co-viewing occurs and how much parents control the amount of televian view- Denial may be frustrating to both parent and ing by their children and the types of programs child, leading to guilt and resentment. watched? Children's requests may complicate family- How much parental mediation of children's consumption priorities, leadingto maladaptive product requests occurs? What level of requests practices, especially among the poor. do children initiate?

Action for Children's Television (1971) summar- How do parents handle children's requests? izes tbese concerns as follows. What is the incidence of parental yielding vs. denial? In the case of children's advertising, the pur- What are the outcomes of parental mediation? pose is to use the child as a surrogate salesman How much conflict or disappointment results? to p essure the parent into buying the product. What learning occurs from these interactions? This is unfair to the c'.ild ... to the parent and can be damaging to the parent-child rela- tionship. CURRENT REGULATION Green (1971) of HEW's Office,of Child Develop- ment offers a similar view The purpose of advertising to children is, of course, to encourage sale of the product or, asstated The child ..is put in the position of an inex- by WAD (1975), " .. to encourage trial and repeat periel.-ed solicitor; and the parent, an ex- purchase." This Inevitably involved parent-child in- perienced though unsolicited buyer. When the teraction of some sort, since parents are in the parent denies th- child's request for an advet- pivotal role of mediators between advertising in- tising product he may feel guilty or 1-8entful at fluences andtrial or repeat purchases. being repeatedly placed in the position of hav- ing to say 'no,' Nevertheless, advertising on children's programs Some me.hbers of the markeingtommunity hold must not exhort children to pressuretheir parents to a contrary view, claiming that advertising cantesult buy. The NAB code specifically prohibits advertis- In pisitive interaction' between parents and-child, -ing-induced pressure. teachmg he child intelligent habits of purchase and 5 consu,r2tion. Fo; example, Bank:, (1971) suggests that the exchange between parents and child "may Children shall not be directed to purchase or actually facilitate the child's ability to cope with the to ask a parent crit other adult to buy aproduct realities of independent living." or service for them.

133 13 The NAD policy is essentially the same. not widely publicized. Thus, we must assume that the frequency of registered complaints may con- Children should not be urged to ask parents or siderably understate the actual level of concern. In others to buy any product contrast, it may well be that surveys of parental at- titudes regarding advertising and children may con- INCIDENCE siderably overstate the amount and strength of nega- tivesentiment. When asked,parents may feel Consistent with the NAB code, national adver- obliged to comment negatively on advertising tisers may no longer explicitly ercourage children to especially advertising to children, although such at- ask their parents to buy a product However, even titudes may be relatively unimportant to them. before enforcement of this code, the incidence of such direct urging was very low. Winick etal. This issue is addressed in research by Bauer and (1973), in a 1971 content analysis of children's com- Greyser (1968). In general, they find that adults do mercials, found that only 1.3 percent of commercials not regard advertising as particularly salient to encouraged children to ask their parents to buy. them. Although adults express definite opinions However, advertisers are free to encourage children when asked about advertising, it is not a topic which to request products from parents by means other they spontaneously diScuss or feel strongly about. In than direct exhortation terms of overall attitudes, Bauer and Greyser report that 41 percent of the public is basically favorable RESEARCH EVIDENCE toward advertising, 14 percent unfavorable, 34 per- cent mixed, and 8 percent indifferent. About 15 per- Parental Attitudes Toward Children's Commercials cent of the sample cites advertising as needing im- mediate attention and change. There are two sources of data regarding parental attitudes towards children's commercials: (1) When parents are asked to express their opinions parents' complaints about children's commercials; ..bout advertising to children, their reported at- and (2) survey research evidence on parental at- titudes are often considerably more negativ^. In a titudes toward children's commercials. study conducted on behalf of Action for Children's Television, Yanexlovich (1970) found that many Statistics on adult complaints about advertising mothers complained about commercials, especial!) come from the FCC and the NAD. In 1973, the FCC misrepresentation of the product, manipulation of received some 55,000 letters commenting on or the child, and strains on low-income families. complaining about television. Of these letters, only Another survey, by Ward, Wackman, and Wartella 34 specifically complained about children's com (1975) find the following profile of responses to mercials. However, in1974 the Commission children's commercials among 615 parents: received over 100,000 letters commenting on adver- tising practices in children's television, of which 90 Strongly negative 23% percent were negative. The impetus for this outpour- Negative 50% ing of letters was the efforts of Action for Children's Neutral 23% Television Positive 4% 100% The second source of data on parental complaints in the NAD,Ghich in toe first year of operation, These negative attitudes are most pronounced 1974, logged about 125 letters from the public con- among parents of younger, kindergarten-age cerning children and advertising. On the basis of children, but do not seem to be associated with these complaints, the NAD opened six formal inqui- social class. ries. The unit also opened 20 inquiries based on its own monitoring of children's commercials Adult attitudes toward chiiren's commercials (Campbell, 1975). ! are negative, but how strongly held are they? A i survey by Atkin (1975g) suggests th majority of For parents to complain about advertising to parents are willing to pay the price of commercial children requires considerable initiative on their Interruptions on children's programs in order to part, since the means of registering complaints are have these programs continue. With a sample of 301

12414 0 mothers of school-age children, Atkin finds the However, as the authors note, the phenoinenon is following more complex since parents have multiple goals for their children, including fostering responsibility, Most mothers are opposed to banning of com- education, and exposure to-different value systems. mercials on Saturday morning television (53 Parents may feel -that the regulation of television percent opposed, 28 percent in favor, 19 per- viewing is in conflict with other socialization objec- cent maybe or don't know). tives. As Feldman, Wolf, and Warmouth conclude:. "Only with an appreciation of the conflicts facing TV-advertised children's products a, c judged parents and an appreciation of the choices available to be neither a better nor a worse value than to them can we assess what should be asked of them unadvertised products. and what must be asked of the larger society." Only 49 percent of mothers report having seen any Saturday morning commercials Roper (1975) conducted a study of parental at- titudes for the Television Information Office of the Only 22 percent of mothers can cite a particu- NAB. The data shown in table 10 -I Indicate, in lar commercial which they feel is especially general] that resistance to commercials on children's bad for their children. programs Is much less than acceptance;' that resist- ance declined somewhat between 1972 and 1974; The ,mothers' opinions would not seem to be and that resistance was somewhat greater among based upon much direct observations, since one-half parents of the youngest children. We could dispute of the mothers have not seen any Saturday morning the wording of the questions asked as being perhaps commercials This raises the issue of salience of less than neutral, as well as the validity of single parental attitudes toward children's commercials. question attitude scales. These results are, however, consistent with the Atkin (1975g) survey cited Research by Feldman, Wolf, and Warmouth earlier (1976) also probes attitudes of a, randomly selected sample of 150 parents of children between 2 and 12 Finally, Culley, Lazer, and Atkin (1976) ex- years old These conclusions can be drawn amined attitudes toward children's commercials across a range of publics, including "townspeople," The average level of concern about children's " executives," "Actiott for commercials is not high Children's Televisuin spokesperson," and "govern- Nevertheless, 65 percent of the sample felt ment" (regulatory agency) personnel. The.. sampling some sort of regulation is needed in this research is far from ideal, exclpt for the "townspeople" respondents who were randohtly Thereis no apparent relationship between selected. The methodology also varies in that the parents' expressed dissatisfaction with com- "townspeople" interviews were conducted in-per- mercials and their monitoring of children's son, whereas the other Interviews were secured by televisiol. viewing mail. We should also be aware that the "townspeo- Level of concern about child-directed com- ple" are not n..cessarily parents of children. mercials is not predictable from demographic Nevertheless, the comparison of attitudes among characteristics or from family characteristics these groups is Interesting, :is shown in table 10-2. such as family size or age of children. Among the two-thirds of the sample preferring In regard to the perceived effects of televised ad- regulation, 65 percent %fluid prefer regulation vertising on children, the "townspeople" and by an Independent ciAl's group, such as "government" samples express surprisingly similar ACT: 20 percent wou,prefer industry self- opinions, while the "advertising agency" sample is regulation, 10 percent would prefer govern-' considerably more positive and the "ACT" sample ment regulation; and 5 percent would prefer an considerably more negative. In general, about one- electronic device that could be attachedio the quarter of the.wnspeople feel that . advertising television set. helps develop good consumer skills (item I in the ta- ble); about one-half feel that commercials may Thus, parentsseem to wish to abdicate the respon- arouse anxieties in children (Item 4), two-thirdsfeel sibility for regulation of to other sources that commercials Increase parent-childconflict

135141 Table 10-1

Parental Attitudes Toward Children's Television Advertising:

"Now I'd like to ask you about commercials on children's television programsand I mean all kinds of children's programs. Some people think there should be no commercials in any kind of children's programs because they feel children can be too easily influenced. Other people, while perhaps objecting to certain commercials, by and large see no harm in them and think children learn from some of them. How do you feelthat there should be no commercials on any children's programs, or that it is all right to have them? People who have children: Both under Total Under 6 years and over 6 6-16 years Sample old only years old old only On Children's programs 11/72 11/74 11/7- 11/74 11/72 11/74 11/72 11/74 % 0, % % % % % % /0

Should be no commercials 32 27 39 34 37 31 31 27 All right to have them 60 63 58 65 62 66 64 67

Don't know/No answer 8 10 3 1 1 3 5 6

"If eliminating commercials on children's TV programs meant considerably reducing the number of children's programs, which would you favor.." %...

11/71 11/72 11/74

Eliminating the commercials and considerably reducing the number of children's programs, or 43 38 35 % Keeping the commercials to keep the children's programs 47 53 54

Don't know/No answer 10 9 11 Source Roper Organization (1975)

(item3), and almost all the townspeople fed that sion advertising, but, for the most part, this concern commercials often persuade children to want things is not so intense as to favor banning commercials they don't need (item 2). altogether. Parents do favor increased regulation, with a preference for industry self-regulation and As to level of regulation, the townspeople favor some sentiment for more government regulation. an increase (item5) but,as reported in other Parental opinions about children's commercials may research, do not favor an outright ban on children's not be strongly held or highly salient. Attitudes. commercials (item 7). The ACT sample was the toward children's commercials obviously vary most regulation-oriented of all, by a substantial across publics, from advertising agency executives margin over the government sample (items 5, 6, 7). on the positive side of the spectrum to Action for Given the regulatory alternatives, the townspeople Children's Television representatives on the nega- are more inclined toward industry self-regulation tive side than toward government lation (items 8, 9), whereas the ACT sample p,..little confidence in self-regulation (item8) Parental Mediation of Television Viewing Behavior

In summary, parents do appear to be concerned Here we are concerned with iwo main issues: the and to have some complaints about children's televi- extent of parent-child co-viewing, and the level of

1361 4 2 Table 10-2 Attitudes of Consumer, Advertising, ACT, and Government Regulatory Agency Group Samples Towards Children's TV Advertising

TownspeopleAgency ACT Government Sample Sample Sample Sample (N -= 21) Item (N = 455) (N = 71) (N = 51) A. TV Advertising Effects on Children (Percent Agreement)

1 Advertising helps develop a child's ability to make good consumer deci- sions 27 65 2 23 2.Commercials often persuade children to want things they do not really need '95 66 200 95 3.Tolevision commercials lead to an in- crease in parent-child conflict 67 7 93 64 4.Television commercials often arouse anxieties and feelings of insecurity in children 51 3 76 41 B. Level of Regulation 5.Television advertising to children should be more regulated than it already is 77 23 100 91' 6.Children's television advertising re- quires special regulation because of the nature of the viewing audience 89 61 98 86f 7.Advertising on children's television programs shouid be banned completely 32 3 89 36 C Mode of Regulation. 8.Commercials to children should be regulated by advertisers themselves 54 63 14 23 9.Commercials to children should be regulated by the government 30 11 70 50 10. Itis up to the parents to regulate children's television viewing behavior 93 97 81 67

Source Gulley, Lazer, and Atkin (1976)

parental control over the amount and type of of the childNielsen data are useful in examining programs viewed by their children A related factor such co-viewing levels, since the distortions of sell- is the presence of multiple-television sets within a reporting are avoided and the Nielsen sampling plan household and the resulting effects on family view- is well developed ing patterns. Nielsen data (1975) reveal considerable Parental control over the amount and type of differences in adult-child co- viewing levels by time programs viewed is a direct torn. of mediation The of day, reaching a high of 70 percent for prime time presence of a parent as a viewing partner provides a programing (7 30 -1 100 p m ), falling to 57 percent further opportunity for mediation of program and , weekdays (10 00 a m - 4 30 p m Month!, to Fri- advertising content Parents and children may ex- day), and showing a low of 20 percent for Saturday change comments, and instruction may take place, mornings Unfortunately, the level of co-viewing even if it is not deliberately studied during the late afternoon (4 30 - 7 30 p m ) on week- Co-viewing levelsLevels of co-viewing vary by days, a time when children view heavily, is not time of day, number of sets in a household, anciage reportedNielsen data also reveal that co-v iewing

137 143 va, ies by specific program, but does not seem to the child in 33 percent of the cases. In 27 percent of have been affected by the rise in multiple set owner- the cases a joint decision is made ship from 1971 to 1975 Co-viewing is also found to ft. be most pronounced for younger children. Parental control Regarding parental restrictions ot television viewing, Bower reports definite rules Lyle and Hoffman (1972) examinee co-viewing about children's viewing among 46 percent of col- patterns within a working-class community in a lege-educated parents and 25 percent of parents with small town outside Los Angeles There are obvious a grade school education. Surprisingly, the amount limitations to these data, including the nonrepresen- of viewing control is net related to children's age tativeness of the sample, reliance on child reports, (see table 10-3). However, the type of viewing con- and failure to examine patterns by time of day. trol does differ according to age. For example, Nevertheless, the data do reveal a broader range of parents of older children are more likely to forbid co-viewing patterns, since they include siblings The watching certain programs whereas parents of mot complete data, tor 1st graders, are as follows' younger children are more likely to change the chan- nel when they considered a program to be objec- Watch with siblings 37% tionable. Children from higher-education house- Watch with parents 8% holds have some influence upon which program' the Watch with siblings and parents 27% famil; is to watch but children in households with Watch with friends 8% less education mole often decide program selection. Half alone, half with someone 14% In these latter households watching televisionis Mostly alone I I% more likely to he encouraged to occupy the child's timethe "pacifier" role. Total 100% Consensus of parent-chu'd reports. There are two These data indicate that sibling co-viewing is the problems in evaluating research on television view- dominant pattern (37 percent) andthat ing patterns. The first is that television use is often parent children co-viewing is next most frequent (35 accompanied by'a variety of other activities, such as percent) The study also examined parental co-view- eating, reading, and studying (see Bechtel etal., ing by age and found a declining pattern as the child 1971, Lyle and Hoffman, 1971). Thus, the term grows older "viewing" can ke on a variety of different in- terpretations, and consensus as to what constitutes Co-viewing was also studied by Bower (1973) parent-child co-viewing becomes problematic. For with a national probability sample of about 1,900 example, does joint viewing occur if a parent is in the households. Itis subject to the same limitations ot same room but engaged in another activity? The sec- ond problem is discrepancies between parents and self-reporting and lack of time-segment breakdowns, and the data do not isolate the incident of child-only children in their descriptions of viewing patterns. Greenberg et al. (1971) found a significant lack of viewing, since the questions all refer to joint-viewing agreement between reporting by mothers and patterns, However, Bower found that in multiset children on householdssibling co-viewing is the predominant rules regarding television viewing. pattern (in 43 percent of cases), followed by hus- There was best agreement on the frequency of parents being present, good consensus on the amount band-wife viewing (33 percent), entire family view- ing (12 percent), and parent-child viewing (7 per- of viewing with friends, but a relative lack of consen- sus as to the level of viewing by children alone. cent) For sinrde-set households, the most common joint viewing pattern is the entire family (55 percent) followed by husband -wife (17 percent), sibling (13 Research by Rossiter and Robertson (1975) ex- percent), and parent-child (13 percent), Mother- amined 253 mother-child dyads regarding television child co-viewing is more common among blacks (20 control and found that parents claimed less total percent ot co-v iewing) than whites (8 percent) and is viewing, more co-viewing,stricter control, and slightly more common among white collar families greater parent-child interaction than their children (10 percent of co-viewing) than among blue collar reported. Examining social class, the study found 'families,(7 percent), Bower also found that when that upper class parents consistently seem to give the mothers and children view together, the mother more socially desirable response. Therefore, actual determines the program in 37 percent of the cases, parental control by better educated parents may be

1314 1 Table 10-3 Parental Control of Viewing Age of child:

4-6 yrs. 7.9 yrs. 10-12 yrs. (N = 197) (N = 2)7) (N = 189)

Rules about viewing time Restrict amount of viewing

Often _ 30% 39% 34% Occasionally 27% 25% 27% . Never 43% 36% 39% Set special hours Often 41% 48% 46% Occasionally 26% 18% 22% Never 32% 34% 32% Rules about program content Decide on programs Often 45% 37% 46% Occasionally 28% 35% 38% Never 27% 27% 11% Change channel when program is objectionable Often 40% 27% 30% Occasionally 30% 36% 40% Never 31% 29% 31% Forbid watching certain programs Often 39% 39% 52% Occasionally 27% 29% 22% Never 35% 32% 25%

Source Bower (1973) as low as that among the poorly educated, and we Our concern here is primarily with request rather cannot necessarily accept the Bower data,Since his than independent purchases by children, since our figures are based only on parental reporting. focus is on direct parent-child interaction regarding consumption. This section examines the incidence of Mediation of Guying Behaior Request Levels request, to parents, the next section focuses on the process of parental mediation. Children act as consumers in several ways. Wells (1965) posits the following In general, the research evidence indicates that the incidence of children's in-family purchase re- by making personal purchasesspending quests varies with the age of the child and the prod- small amounts of nic,ney on their own, uct category There is also some tendency for social classes to relate to the incidence of purchase re- by making direct requests at home; quests by making direct requests in the store; Age of child.Requests generally decrease by "passive dictation"i.e., parents buying somewhat among older children (Wells, 1965, what they know their children willingly con- Atkin, 1975a; Ward and Wackman, 1972; sume and avoiding what they resist consuming Caron and Ward, 1975; Robertson and

139145 Rossiter, 1976, Clancy-Hepburn, 1974) This amount of Saturday morning television viewing may be due to a number of factors, including However, the author cautions that "this could be a the child's increasing sophistication in dealing function of age." A positive correlation was also with his parents in less direct ways than ex- found between the child's attitudes toward advertis- plicitly asking ing and the number of requests.made. Product category. Children are more likely to Atkin (1975g) assessed children's self reports of make requests for products which are fre- requestpreferences among 738 children, from quently consumed by them, such as breakfast preschoolers to 5th graders. Parallel data were also cereals, or of particular interest to them, such derived from 301 motherS of these children. Consist- as toys. This is intuitively obvious but has been ent with our earlier discussion, Atkin finds that the substantiated in a number of studies, including younger children are considerably more likely than Wells (1965), Ward, and Wackman (1972), the older children to report requesting cereals and and Ward, Wackman, and Wartella (1975) toys after seeing commercials. Requests for cereals Social class. There is some tentative indication and toys appear to be roughly equal. In general, of increasing requestlevels the higher the thereisa reasonably strong consensus between social class (Atkin, I975a; Caron and Ward, mothers and children about the level of requests. 1975, Wells and Losciuto, 1966) As reported in table 10-4, requests are signifi- It is, of course, difficult to demonstrate a cause- cantly greater among heavy viewers of Saturday and-effect relationship in regard to the role of televi- morning television than among light viewers. The sion advertising in encouraging purchase requests to correlation between viewing and requests is +.41, parents. Nevertheless, there is evidence suggestingand drops to+ 29 whenage,sex,race, and that television advertising's impact, relative to other scholastic performance are controlled. Atkin also information sources, can be fairly great under cer- correlates the amount of cfildren's viewing, based tain conchtions. on a combined mother/child measure, with requests for specific products, reporting a + 28 correlation Caron and Ward (1975), in a study of 84 third between viewing and advertising-simulated cereal and fifth grade child-mother pairs, reportthat request, and a +.17 correlation for toy requests. children cite television as the source of Christmas- Again, when age, sex, race, and school performance gift requests in 27 percent of the cases, fcllowed are controlled, the correlations fall somewhat, to closely by friends in 26 percent of the cases. Televi- + 22 for cereals and + 10 for toys sion advertising as an information source was also found to increase in importance with the age of the child. In another assessment of television advertis- In summary, Atkin concludes "There is a clear ing's impact on Christmas-present requests, pattern of evidence showing that Saturd4 morning Robertson and Rossiter. (I 976a) found it to be the televition advertising has an important influence on dominant information source among a sample of 289 children's asking for cereal and toy products." We 1st, 3d, and 5th graders. This study also examined share Atkin's conclusion. Television advertising is the "magnitude" of advertising effects by assessing an important information source for child-oriented the proportion of toy and game requests versus re- products, primarily foods and toys. Such advertising quests for bother generally nonadvertised presents encourages requests to parents, especially among during the period of concentrated toy and gamtbad- younger children. vertising preceding Christmas. During thispre- Christmas period, there was an approximate 5 per- Proc ess of Parental Alediat tonYielding Denial cent increase in the proportion of toy and game re- quests across all grade levels. Furthermore, heavy What are the levels of yielding or denial when viewers were significantly more likely to request ad- children Inmate purchaserequests')In general, vertised toys and games than light viewers. yielding varies by product category and seems to in- crease with the age of the child. The relationship be- Two exploratorystudies by Clancy-Hepburn tween social class and yielding iss

Light Heavy viewers viewers (N = 444) (N = 294) Many of the TV commercials are for toysthings like and racing cars. After you see these toys on TV, how much do you ask your mother to buy them for you? A Lot 16% 40% Sometimes 64% 46% Never 20% 14%

After you see commercials for breakfast cereals on TV, how much do you ask your mother to buy the cereal for you? A Lot 24% 41% Sometimes 50% 39% Never 26% 20%

Sou.c.e Atkin (1975g)

Product category. Yielding seems to depend on the Examining yielding patterns for toys, Caron and product and whether it is primarily for the child's Ward (1975) found that parents yield to 31 percent consumption. For example, Ward and Wackman of children'srequests Robertson andRossiter (1972) report yielding levels of 87 percent for (1976b) found that parents yield to 43 percent of cereals, 63 percent for snack foods, 54 percent for children's Christmas-gift requests games and toys, 42 percent for candy, 39 percent for toothpaste, 16 percent for shampoo and 7 percent Thus, yielding levels seem substantial for child- for pet foodall based on the report of mothers relevant productsFor cereals, the available from middle-class families research indicates parental yielding to roughly two- thirds of requests. Further research, however, should document yielding levels according to price In'an observational study ot 516 tamilies. Atkin and other relevant variables (19750 found that 62 percent of parents yield to the child's cereal "request" or "demandIn another ob- Age of childPositive associations between the servational study, Wells and Loscuito( 19661 child's age and parental yielding levels have been reported that parents acquiesce to 69 percent of the found in research by Ward and Wackman (1972), children's requests for cereal and 57 percent or le- Atkin (1975a), Wells (1965), and Ward, Wackman, quests for candy and Wane Ila(1975)Other research, however, failed to find this relationship, including that of Berey and Pollay (1968) also examined the rela- Caron and Ward (1975), and Berey and Pollay tions between mothers and children in the purchase (1968)Thelatterconclude thatthemother's of ready-to-eat cereals They found that the mother purchases ot cereals are independent of the child s is a strong "gatekeeper" in children's -dections, a age, as well as the number of siblings, the mother's finding that seems contrary to the Ward and Wack- outside employment, and the number of trips to the man and Atkin reports that parents acquiesce to store by the child most purchase requests by children for breaktast cereals However, the Berey and Po llay data must be The strength of the relationship between 4 4 regarded as highly preliminary due to the limited child and parental yielding may logically vary as a sample size and measurement procedures. tunc ion of yet unspecified variables, such as social

141 i class and family structure Berey and Pollay, for ex- analysis is badly needed to understand their relative ample, found that highly child-centered mothers impact. yield less often to cereal requests, prompting these authors to speculate that " . given her overriding Outcomes of ParentalMediation Learning/Conflict concern for the child's well-being, she tends to ig- nore the child and to purchase what she thinks will Parental mediation of children's purchase re- do the child the most good." (Berey and Pollay do quests is both an opportunity for parental instruction not, however, examine the amount of conflict cre- regarding consumption and a source of possible con- ated by such overruling of child preferences.) Ac- flict. Most existing research on children and adver- cording the presence of an age/yielding relationship, tising has focused on intrafamily conflict and disap- Wells (1965) offers an interesting hypothesis, pointment. Almost no studies focus on possible posi- "Older children are more selective and more cir- tive learning outcomes. cumspect .especially when the product is one they are going to consume themselves Conflict. In research cited previously, Robertson and Rossiter (1976b) probed children's disappoint- Social classMixed results are obtained across ment upon not receiving Christmas-present requests. studies when socialclassis related to yielding Disappointment, measured 2 weeks after Christmas, (Atkin, 1975a, Caron and Wal ',1975; Lyle and is not as high as might be anticipated. Parents Hoffman, 1971; Ward and Wackman, 1972; Ward, "refused" 57 percent of chilgtren's requests, and only Wackman, and Wartella, 1975; Wells and Lose nit°, 35 percent of the children icated disappointment 1966). On balance, however, the weight of the evi- after denial. Of course, h er levels of disappoint- dence favors the hypothesis of a positive relationship ment might have been served if measures were between the two. However, it may well be that social taken immediately after d nial. Disappointment was classis not a particularly meaningful or robust most pronounced among nger children, children variable in this context. with high television exposurand, contrary to ex- pectation, among children from homes with a high Parental knowledge and attitudes. Research on level of parent-child interaction. Regarding this last adolescents by Ward and Robertson (1970), suggests finding, the authors suggest that children from these that television advertising may complement ultra- homes may feel more "let down" if they have dis- family communication about consumption Com- cussed presents but did not receive them mercials sometimes do provoke family communica- tions. Ward and Robertson found that adolescents in Atkin (1975g) focuses more on parent-child argu- families with high levels of communication about ments and anger than on disappointment One-sixth consumption are more favorable toward advertising, of the children report arguing with their mothers "a and ale more materialistic in orientation. Prelimin- lot" and another one-third arguing "sometimes" ary research by Clancy-Hepburn (1974) further sup- after denial of toy requests One-fifth of the children ports the interaction between parental attitudes and become angry "a lot" about toy denials, and two- children's attitudesin this case, toward food ad- fifths become angry "sometimes." For denials of vertising. Children whose mothers have a good un- cereal requests, the degree of argument and anger is derstanding of the validity of nutritional claims ex- basically similar but somewhat lower press significantly lower preferences and fewer re- quests for advertised foods and report lower con- Atkin reports a tendency for arguments and anger sumption of these products. Furthermore, mothers to increase as children grow older, and a slight cor- with a good understanding of advertising claims relation between Saturday morning television ex- tend to yield less to children's requests for snack posure lnd a combined conflict/anger index foods. However, based on path analysis, Atkin concludes that television exposure has no directlink on con- Thus, parental yielding may be a function of flict/anger, but works indirectly through increasing product, age of the child, social class level, and the frequency of children's product requestsIn parental knowledge and attitudes. These few rela- parallel interviews with mothers of these children. tionships certainly do not fully explain yielding or Atkin finds that mothers detect disappointment in denial Other variables, especially family interaction 21 percent of cereal denials and 29 percent of toy style, are undoubtedly involved. and multivariate denials. Overt al.6eris reported in5 percent of 14D 142 cereal denials and 10 percent of toy denials The nificant, the evidence appears to suggest that, at least correlation between television exposure and anger is in this experimental setting, exposure to TV com- .11 for cereals and18 for toys mercials for toys can contribute directly to negative feelings on the part of the child toward his parent. 'II another study based on unobtrusive in-store ooservations. Atkin (19750 recorded conflict and In order to examine the relationship between the disappointment over parental denials of cereal re- child's level of disappointment upon nonreceipt of a quests. In the instances of denial, conflict occurr:d toy and exposure to the commercial, each child was in 65 percent of the cases and unhappiness resulted shown a picture of a boy watching television, with 48 percent of the time There is some tendency for only the back of his head visible in the picture. The conflict and unhappiness to be highest among 6-8 experimenter then asked year olds. Atkin notes that "conflict is seldom in- tense or persistent Displays of child anger or sad- ness are also short-livedinmostcases." This You see this boy; he didn't get the Ruckus research does not attempt to relate levels of conflict Raisers Barn, so he went to watch television in- and disappointment to television advertising ex- stead. Do you thank he was sad that he didn't posure. get the Ruckus Raisers Barn, or do you think he was still happy because he could watch However, an experimental study by Goldberg television`" and Gorn (1976) assessed the cAtent to which ex- Close to two-thirds of the control. group thought the posure to a TV commercial for a toy affects a child's boy was "still happy" compared to about one-third feelings toward a parent who denies a rea- -et for the of those in the experimental groups who gave the advertised toy as well as the level of disc .ntment same response, (24 hours later the responses were on nonreceipt of the toy. One hundred and sixty-six slightly but not significantly close to those of the 4-5 year old children were. randomly allocated to control group). It thus appears that direct exposure control and experimental groups Children in the to a TV commercial for a toincreased the likeli- experimental groups (n=112) viewed commercials hood that children would consider failure to receive for a toy (labeled "Ruckus Raisers Barn") in the the toy as inducing "sadness." This approach to context of a 10-minute neutral program A control measuring the direct relationship between exposure group (n=54) viewed the program without any com- to television commercials and various emotional mercials. andsocialoutcomes would appear worthy of replication and generalization Following exposure tO the commercials and or program material, children were shown pictures of Learning.Children's requests provide an oppor- the same boy either happily embracing his father or tunity for parental teaching about consumption, This walking glumly away from him. The experimenter focus, however, has not been pursued in research. It then asked. would seem, for example, that parents' responses to children's requests could help to teach the child I know a boy whose daddy didn't get him the about the realities of the marketplacewhether ex- Ruckus Raisers Barn, when his daddy didn't plicitly or implicitly. In the Atkin (1975g) survey, give him the Ruckus Raisers Barn do you think the most frequent reason for parental denial of toy he wanted to play with his daddy like this requests was "expense" followed by "poor value or do you think he wanted to go away from his Very few parents simply said "no" without furthg.1 daddy like this .9 explanation (The chapter in this volume on "Con- sumer Socialization"isa fuller account of this While aboutthree-fifths of the control group perspective ) thought the boy would "still want to play with his Daddy," only about two-fifths of those in the experi- SUMMARY AND NEEDED RESEARCH mental groups thought so. The responses remained consistent 24 hours later. While some of the com- Since the foregoing analysis is based on such scat- parisons with the control group were not quite sig- tered and incomplete research, we must consider it

141143 re.1=1110.1 only a source of hypothesis for future studies These and seems to increase with the age of the child. propositions seem worthy of further research. Yielding may also be associated with pare tal attitudes toward television advertising Parental concernParental attitudes toward children's television advertising are generally Outcomes: learning and conflict. Disappoint- negative, bat are probably not very strongly ment, conflict, and even anger are found when held or Important in their lives. Parents seem parents deny requests. Television exposure willing to pay tne price of children's advertis- seems linked to these outcomes How parents ingif the alternative were discontinuing teach good consumer habits when responding children's programing However, they favor to child-initiated requests needs systematic increased regulation study. Parental lediation of viewing behavior Levels All of these themes, parental mediation of the of parent-child co-viewing vary by time of day and are lowest, as might be expected, for effects of telbvision advertising is certainly one of the most important topics for future research. The children's programs. Co-viewing is considera- existing tentative findings badly need replication. bly less prevalent than children viewing alone Viewing rules for children appear to be more New studies must be initiated to describe the nature common among college-educated parents. of parent-child exchanges about advertised products However, parents from higher social-class and children's purchase requests. levels may be giving what they consider to be socially-desirable responses to the questions Describing and analyzing interactions within asked in research, thus, the acival exercise of families is a difficult research task. Because of the parental viewing rules needs better documen- distortions of self-reports, greater use should be tation made of direct observation and unobtrusive research methods. Studies must also observe family interac- Children's ream sts to parents Purchase requests tions over longer periods of time than in current generally decrease ameng older children They studies. Parent-child relations are hardly limited to vary by product and are highest for products those surrounding the influence of television adver- frequently consumed by children (such as tising on children's purchase requests; future studies cereals) or of particular interest to them (such must place family interaction about consumption as toys) Exposure to television advertising is into the larger context Jf ongoing family exchanges. found to be associated with children's request Such studies of the mediation of television advertis- for both toys and cereals ing by parent-child mteractions will be among our Parental yielding dental Yielding to children's most difficult research undertakings. They will also purchase requests varies by product category, be among the mast significant.

144 Chapter 11

SUMMARY OF RESEARCH FINDINGS

No one disputes the proposition that television broader background for our overall recommenda- advertisements ha.e some euect on those who view tions for future research, presented in the next them, it only in persiading :leviers to try a sponsor's chapter. products Given the variety and pervasiveness television advertisements and the many specific Thus, on the basis of cur analysis of issues and ex- sues we have identified,it seems likely that the istingresearch, we have derive,'tilefollowing - "effects" of television-,-,'vertisingare multipie. schematic list of stimulus properties, proposed out- Mot cover, even among the special audiei- with comes, and mediatingariablesI he list is almost -filch we are concerned in this reportchildren 2 to certainly incomplete, but it provides a means for 12 the range of vat 'talon among individual viewe. s identifying the most important variables and the is great Children differ in tastes and interests, in possible links among them television viewing patterns, in levels of cognitive ahility and emot on:41 maturity, in family c :cum- 1. Stimulus Properties stances. in school experiences, and in peer group and A. Amount and placement of advertising community influencesA'1 of these variables may B.Program-commercial separation practices affectthe impact of tr.lev:Aon advertising upon C.Nature of products ad ertised children children D. Commercia:s not directed at cnildren (drugs, etc ) Thus, we are dealing with a c,,mplex setof E Content and techniques of commercials

stimuli, with a variety of possible effects, and with a 1 Format and audio-visual techniques diversified populatton. Our major effort, therefore, (animation, music, pacing, special has bean to identify the specific issues which seem to effects, etc ) be mt. .menable tr_.searcn. We have attempted to 2 Characters and spokespersons identity the hypoth,__ which link specific. definable 3 Product claims, disclosures, and dis- stimuli wit" specific, measurable outer ,nes, and claimers which , where appropriate,relevant mediat- 4. Premium offers ing variables. We have also tried to evaluate the sig- nificance of these possible outcomes in terms of the 11 Possible Outcomes h. principles already established for regulating A. Intended effects of advertising advertising to childrenthat is, the FTC's require- 1. Attention to and recall of product ment tiro auvertising not he "deci.ptive" or "unfair" brands and attributes and the NAD's mandate "tensure that advertising 2. Desire for advertised products directed to children is truthful, accurate, and fair to 3. "Trial and repeat purchases" of prod- children's perceptions." ucts or purchase requests to parents B.Unintended effects of advertising In the preceding chapt..rs, we have reviewed the Short-range existing research in terms of the 10 key issues which I.Confusion between program and com- were selected as the framework for this rep-ort. In mercial materials- failure to under- this chapter, we .t:1 step back from tilt: :ram-work, stand selling intent of commercials. which is orientel to currently outstanding is -,,es, in 2. Failure to comprehend product at- order to summarize the present state of knowledge in tributes or disclaimers (complexity of terms of what it. own about the links between the assembly or operation, role of a food stimulus prop, I advertising, the possible out- product in a "balanced diet," etc.) comes of childt exposure to a4vertising, and the 3. incorrect (exaggerated) assessment of role of mediating variables in affecting these out- product performance or of sa tsfaction comes We present this alternative perspective in provided by produc oler to identify areas of relative strength and weak 4 Encouragement of unsafe behavior ness in the body of existing rest .-ch and to provide a through imitation

145 151 t11-range the researcher sometimes neglects the .ifective 5 Encouragement of "inappropriate" dimension of each commercial as a wholeeven standards for consumer choices though this may well he the most salient aspect of a (sweetness or "fun" rather than nutri- commercial's impact on viewers Nevertheless, con- tionals al ue emiums rather than tent analyses are useful in defiing the nature of the product .ttribies. etc advertiser: Isms viewed by children, and there is a 6 Promotion)f parent-child com- continuing need for periodic analyses to keep the munication and or conflict knowledgef actual practices up to date 7 Learning about workings ot marketplace and advertising (e.g POSSIBLE OUTCOMES

comparing advertisingch-TI)ns with ac- a tual product) Virtually all of the existing research is based on Lon 1?-range the assumption that advertising does have an impact 8 Encouragement or reinforcement of on children Thus. studies in this area have generally unhealthy or hazardous behavior (e g 6 .designed to test the relative effectiveness of poo., nutritional habits, drug abuse) specific techniques (e gpremium vs no-premium 9 Encouragement or reinforcement of offers, male vs. female actors, clustered vs. dis- social values (e g , sex role or other persed placements of ads) Because of the specificity stereotyping, distrust or cynicism of these studies, and the small sample of sfkjects toward society, unselfishness) they typically employ. their results can be 10 Development ot consumer skills generalized only to a',MIRAdegree. On the other hand. the broader survey-type studies, whkh at- III Mediating Variables tempt to assess the impact of advertising on actual I Child's chararcerstics (age. cognitive attitudes and behavior, raise the. question of the development, sex, socioeconomic validity of self-reported data In addition, the exist- status, intelligence, etc.) ing survey-type studies do not always examine their 2 Child's viewing patternsvolume of results in terms of specific measures of children's ex exposure to advertising posur° to television advertising 3 Parent-child interactions (parental co: trol of viewing, co- viewing, con- With these qualifications in mind, we can trol of consumption) nevertheless reach some tentative conclusions about 4 Other sources of consumer informa- the effects of television advertising for children. We oon (print, peers, school, stores, othe.!,r will look first at what r known about the intended TV content, etc ) effects of television advertising, then at evidence of unintended effects

Sl iMULUS PROPERTIES Intended effe(tsIt is clear that children pay an-1.- 00n to television commercials, th(,Jgh the amount of The existing literature provides tairly detailed attention seems to he dependent upoi, d number of J extensive descriptions of the stimulus properties variables. Research indicates that a child's ot television advertisiug directed achildren. The is likely to be somewhat greater to (I) commercials two princilal sources of this information are self- for products relevant to children (Wartella and Et- regulatory codes and content analyses. The former tema, 1974), (2) commercials with a greater degree the NAB and NAD codes, specify what is an what is of audio complexity (Wartella and Ettema, 1974); not permitted in childten's advertising to children, (3) commercials with a high level of physical action but, of course, these guidelines areprescriptive (Rust and Watkins, 1975), and (4) commercials rather than descriptive of the actual content of shown in a clustered format (Atkin, I975b). Other children's commercials. Content analyses (e.g., research, based on television programing rather than Winick et al., 1973; Atkin, 1975d; Barcus 1975 a & advertising, suggests that achild'sattentionis b) a-e more analytical, typically descrikfing a sample enhanced by the presence of both auditory elements, of commercials according to such categories as including lively music, tonging, rhyming, and sound products, audio-visual techniques, characters and effects; and visual elements, including active move- frequency. However, in the process of breaking ment, animation, and visual changes in general down and coding specific attributes of commercials, (Levin and Anderson, 1976)

146 1 5 There is also evidence from a number of studies likely to litter than subjects who did not see the an- about children's learning from television commer- nouncement cials. In a survey of mothers of preschool children, a majority of respondents said that their children sang In other experiments, few significant differences commercial jingles learned from' television (Lyle have been found in children's desire for or requests and Hoffman, 1972) Another survey of children (2 for products which were advertised using different to 6 years) and their mothers found that television techniques. In the Atkin experiments, for example, advertising was the most important source of new- "rational" vs. "emotional" appeals for a cereal, male product information for cereals and toys (Howard, vsfemale announce'voices, and dispersedvs. Hulbert, and Lehmann, n d.) A series of experi- clustered formats all red about equally effec- ments by Atkin ( I975b & c) found that a majority of tiveintheir persu, ness, While repeated ex- the children successfully recalled products and posure to the same co,amercial increased children's product attributes after being exposed to commer- recall, it did not lead to an increase in their liking or cials for those products. desire for the advertised product (Atkin, 1975c, Gorn and Goldberg, I976b). However, the presence The Atkin experments also tested the relative of the "batteries not included" disclaimer in both effect' _ness of dtlerent strategies and formats of audio and video tracks (vs. audio only) decreased commercials (e.g, premium offers vs. no premiums, the children's desire for the product at the-same time "rational" vs "emotional" presentations of a prod- thatit increased their recall of the qualification uct, video disclaimers vs both audio and video dis- (Atkin, 1975b). claimers)In most cases, the alternatives did not produce sip iificant differences in learning, although In terms of "soe.rce effects," these is evidence recall was greater when'an ad male "modest" rather- from a number of studies (not all conducted with than "exaggerated" claims for product performance, television advertising) that associating a product when the children were exposed to an ad twice in- with an endorser can significantly change children's stead of once, and when a disclaimer ("batteries not attitudes tow-rd that product (Hyams et al., 1975, included") was presented both auditorially and Iskoe, 1976) An experiment by Atkin ( I975b) sug- visually, rather than just visually. In another experi- gests that endorsement of a product by a figure not ment, comprehension and recall ofa disclaimer were liked by children can actually diminish the product's shown to beincreased whenitswording was appeal Adults, in general, seem to be more effective simplified (Liebert et al, 1976) than children as product presenters, although idults shown with children are the most effective o.-:1 The impact of advertising on children's attitudes (Robertson et al., 1975; Gardner, 1975) id behavior are of particular interest, since the most basic defining characteristic of advertising is its Finally, the evidence regarding the effectiveness persuasive intent.t ne existing evidence indicates of premiurt, offers is mixed Shimp et al(1975) that advet using is at least moderately successful in reported no differences in children's attitudes creating positive attitudes toward a product and in toward a cereal advertised with and without a pre- stimulating requests for the productfr) Lyle and mium offer Atkin (1975b) found amore positive Hoffman's survey (1972) of mothers of preschoolers, response to a breakfast cereal advertised with a pre- 87 percer reported that their children asked for mium offer, but he reported no differences in the food items they saw advertised on television, and 91 children's expressedintention to request the percent reported requests for toys advertised on _purchase of the product. On the other hand, two sur- television. A field study conducted during the pre- veys of mothers indicated that children's actual Christmas period found that requests for heavily ad- cereal requests are frequently based on advertised vertised toys and games increased by 5 percent premiums ard, Wackman, and Wartella, 1975, (Robertson and Ross ter, 1976) Several laboratory Atkin, 1975g) Because of the inconclusiveness of studies (Goldberg and Gorn, 1974, Liefield and these results and the continuing debate over pre- Norsworthy, 1974) have found modest correlations mium offers in children's advertising, this is a topic between children's exposure to specific television that calls for further study. advertisements and increased short-range desire for the advertised products An experiment by Atkin Untrue, .ed effects'Certainly, most advertisers do (1975b) demonstrated that children exposed to an not deliberatelysetoutto confuse or mislead antilittering public set vice announcement were less children, nor to promote unsafe, unhealthy, or 15 3 147 socially undesirable behavior. Nevertheless, critics possibility by prohibiting the use of violence and en- have claimed that such outcomes may in fact be the couraging the portrayal of positive social values result of children's exposure to televisionadvertis- Even so, the potentially harmful messages actually ing, and most of the policy issues wehave ithritified beim, conveyed to children by individual commer- Ire in this area of incidental learning and other unin- cials'are not always obvious For example, a cereal tended effects We will first consider the evidence commercia, which associated the product with wild- regarding the short-term effects -If individual com- grow ng edible plants led some children to label mercials or specific techniques, then we will review similar-appearing poisonous plantsasedible the evidence concerning the cumulative impact of (Poulos, 1975) The incidence of such potentially television advertising over time hazardous messages is almost certainly quite small, but these results suggest that careful attention needs Perhaps the most fundamental short-term issues is to be given to the totality of information conveyed to the ability of children to distinguish programfrom children by comn ercials commercial material, and the implicationsif children are unable to do so. There is evidence from The issue of the impact of adult-oriented adver- a number of studies that youngerchildren are very tising on children is entu ely an issue of unintended likely to be confused about the distinctions between effects, since these messages concern products programs and commercials Theevidence also indi- presumably not of interest to children Nevertheless, cates that as children become older,they often begin we know that children do most of their viewing at to distinguish between programs andcommeicials times other than those when children's programing is merely on the basis of superficial characteristics, broadcast Research evidence on the consequences such as duration or animation Apparently, develop- 01 the exposure of children to adult-orientedadver- ment of anunderstanding of the most salient tising is extremely limited and has focused primarily differencesthe selling intent of commercials is a on the impact of advertising for nonprescription process that takes time. Several studieshave at- drugs The single experimental study on this topic tempted to trace the stages this process and to tested children's responses to a commercial for an deHeate how children understand and respond to o 'er-the-counter decongestant medicine (Atkin, television advertising at each stage (Rubin, 1972, 1975) Subjects shown this ad, along with a number Ward and Wackman, 1973, Robertson and Rossiter, of other child-oriented ads. were reported to have 1974). paid "substantially less" attention to the medicine commercial Nevertheless, the children were found The possibility of children being confused or to have been affected by the ad twice as many of the misled by specific features of commercial messages children exposed to this ad said "they would take a has also been the subject of a few studies For exam- pill to combat a stuffed-up noseBecause of the ar- ple, one aspect of the issue of premium offers is the tificiality imposed by the experimental design for possibility that inclusion of such ffers makes It this studyimmediate responses to a single eA- more difficult for children todetermine what is posure to one commercial-message in a laboratory being sold, and that these offers distract attention settingthe implications of the study, and of similar from other more important features of the product studies by Atkin and others, are not easily deter- One study (Rubin, 1972) suggests that premium mined offers create such confusion, at least among young children Two more recent studies do not confirm this finding (Stump etal, 1975, Atkin,1975) A final area of possible unintendet.: effects on However, each of these studies employs different children's advertising concerns the role of commer- measures, and so they are not directlycomparable cials in promoi:Ag conflict between parentsand Moreover, their usefulness is hawed by their small children We have already reviewed evidence that sample:: Given these mixed results, more research parents receive requests for the purchase ofproducts will be needed before this issue can be resolved advertised on television for children Surveys and direct observation of parent-child interactionsindi- Another area of unintended effectsisthe cate that parents (usually mothers) denyfrom 31 possibility that children will learn unsafe or un- percent (Wells and Losciuto, 1966) to38 percent desirable Sehavior from commercial messages The (Atkin, 19751) of children's requests for cereals, and industry's self-regulatory codes recognize this from 57 percent (Robertson and Rossiter, I976h) to 1 5

' S 69 percent (Caron and Ward, 1975) of children's re- "high exposure" to such advertising arc somewhat quests for toys Robertson and Rossiter report that more likely to worry about getting sick, to feel better slightly more than one-third of the children they in- after taking medication, and to perceive higher inci- terviewed indicated disappointment over denial of dences of illness in people generally (Atkin, 1975e). their toy requests The expressions of disappoint- No positive correlation has been found between ment were most frequent among younger children. children's exposure to over - the - counter drug adver- tising and their attitudes toward usage of illicit drugs Atkin (1975e) found that one-sixth of ciiildren he (Milayskyetal ,1975; Hulbert,1974; Atkin, surveyed said that they argue with their mothers "a 1975e). Evidence 941 children's usage of proprietary lot" after denial of tay requests, while another one- medicints is mixei Atkin found no link with ex- third say they argue "sometimesAtkin concluded posure to'adwrtising, while Milaysky et al reported that exposure to television advertising is indirectly a moderately positive relationship linked to conflict by increasing the frequency of pirthase requests and subsequent denials In a study Knowledge about patterns of food consumption based on direct observations ma supermarket, Atkin and nutritional knowledge and attitudes isalso (1975f) found that conflict followed in two-thirds of limited. Surveys by Atkin (1975e & g) provide the cases of mothers' denying children's cereal re- preliminary indications that children's exposure to quests The children expressed unhappiness one-half food advertising "affects general .. nutritional of the time, although Atkin described such conflict orientation." He found that "heavy" viewers of as being "seldom intense or persistent." In an experi- Saturday morning television were more likely to eat mental study, Goldberg and Gorn (1976) found that the types of foods advertised on television. They children exposed to a toy commercial responded were also more likely to'isk parents to buy adver- more negatively to a parental denial of a request for tised cereal brands and to eat at advertised fast-food the toy than children who had not seen the commer- restaurants. Finally, these children were less disap- cial. Children who had seen the commercial also proving of sugar, gave a higher nutritional rating to projected a greater level of disappointment to the sweetened cereals, and believed more strongly in the possibility of denial rf a request for the toy More value of children's vitamin supplements These sur- needs to he known about the role of advertising in veys also indicatedthat children who watched providing disappointment to the possibility of denial messages on good nutrition (both public service an- of a request for the toy. More needs to be knoWn nouncements and segments within cereal commer- about the role of advertising in providing disap- cials) were more likely to give high nutritional rat- pointment and- family conflict, and whether such ings to the specific foods emphasizedinthese conflict places significant strains on the relationship messages andto believe thatanutritious and between parents and children balanced breakfast is important.

In regard to the possible longer range, cumulative MEDIATING VARIABLES consequences of children's television advertising, Relatively extensiveinformationisavailable most of the questions concern the impact of commer- regarding the influence of the various mediating cials upon children's development of consumer variables on the effects of television advertising on skills and their formation of behavioral patterns and children. In fact, more of the existing research deals attitudes in drug and food usage, as well as the with the role of the mediating variables than with development of broader social values. Once again, direct links between Independent and dependent these are issues for which existing researci. offers lit- variables. tle guidance A body of research is available on the process of' children's consumer socialization, but lit- The mediating variable,'wnich most clearly tte of it focuses explicitly on the contribution of emerges among the many studies is the c is age. television advertising. Itis clear that parents still For example, the research we have re, !d indi- play the dominant role in training their children for cates that young children (typically, belt) ige 5 or an adult consumer role. Whether television aids this 6) are more likely to believe claims they hear in process, or confl :cts with it, remains to he deter- commercials (War'd and Wackman,1973),less mined likely to recall specific features and information from ,:ommercials they have seen (Rubin, 1972), Research on the long-term effects of nonprescrip- and more likely to he confused or to tail to under- tion drug advertising indicates that children with a stand disclaimers (Atkin, 1975h, Licher(etal,

14'9155 1976). Perhaps most striking is finding that young number of experiments, Atkin ( I 975b) found "sig-

children frequently do not understand the selling in- nificant . although modest" differences between tention of advertising (Rubin, 1972, Robertson and the responses of white and black children: The white Rossiter, 1974) Taken as a whole these studies subjects paid more attention to Commercials and had demonstrate thatas children grow older,they slightly higher levels of recall and information ac- become more sophisticated in dealing with television quisitionAtkin acknowledged, however, that un- advertising. This finding is hardly surprising in light controlled differences in socioeconomic status may of the considerable body of psychological literature account, at least in part, for these results. Evidence documenting stages of child development in other on sex-based differences is mixed, but is manifested areas. In fact, much of the research on age-related most clearly in the attraction of boys and girls to differences in relation to television advertising has products (primarily toys, such as dolls or racing been based on the prior work of Paiget and other cars) designed to appeal primarily to one sex (Atkin, child-development psychologists. I 975c).

While existing research firmly establishes that age Another mediating variable is thelevel of a of viewers is an important mediating variable, the child's exposure to television advertisingHeavy significance of this finding for policymaking is much television viewing seems neither to accelerate nor to less clear. Prom cross-sectional studies of different retard children's understanding of commercials, age groups, it appears likely that children's difficulty although it does appear, in general, to be correlated in understanding and assessing television advertising with snore favorable attitudes toward advertising is primarily a short-term phenomenon. However, and advertised products. There is little evidence that from a policy standpoint, the question is whether repeated exposure to individual commercials has negative effects, even in the short term, are tolera- any incremental etfect on either children's attitude ble. Industry spokesmen take a positive view, sug- toward desire for the advertised product. EXperi- gesting that trial and error learning from television mental studies"' (Gorn and Goldberg, 1974, 1976b; advertising provides children with experiences Atkin, 1975b) indicate that the primary effect of necessary for developing sophisticated adult con- repetition is to prevent children from forgetting a sumer skills. commercial's content rather than greater persuasion.

Even if exposure of young children to television The influence of parents clearly emerges as a final advertising ultimately judged to he a significant important mediating variable As the recipients of problem, itis not easy to envision practical and children's purchase requests, parents directly medi- effective remedies Action for Children's Television ate most of their children's c-msumer behavior. The has proposed that all advertising be removed from existing research on this topic has focused on paren- children's programs, but we have seen that children's tal compliance with or denial of purchase request programing accounts for only about 15 percent of (Wells,1965; Atkin,1975h; Caron and Ward children's viewing time. Thus, banning commercials 1975) Littleattentionhas been givento the on children's programs would not prevent children possibility that positive learning is provided by tncse from being exposed to advertising The Canadian interactions. Parents (especially mothers) also play Association of Broadcasters has banned all advertis- key roles in controlling children's television view- ingtochildren during weekday hours, onthe ing, and there is some evidence of a disci epancy be- assumption that' only preschool children would he tween mothers' reports of the degree of this control viewing television during those times and the level of parental control reported by their children (Rossiter and Robertson, 1975) This find- Race ar.:1 sex are two other mediating variables ing suggests the need for further study of how which apparently affectchildren's responsesto parents actually monitor and regulate children's television advertising For example, summarirting a viewing

1501 5 i; Part III

Chapter 12

RECOMMENDATIONS FOR FUTURERESEARCH

As we have noted, scientific iaquiry into the MIDLEVEL RESEARCH effects of television advertising on children is still in its infancy. The number of studies specifically ad- As we have noted, one of the factors contributing dressed to this topic is quite small, and a mere to the gap between research and policymaking i3 the handful of investigators are responsible for a ma- fact that much policy deliberation (and many actual jority of these. Moreover, more than three-quarters code provisions) deals with, the prohibition or re- of these studies have been published since 1974. quirement of specific advertising practices, without Considering these facts, as well as the complexity of identifying the specific outcomes presumed to be the issues and the limitations of current methods, It linked with those practices. However, many out- is not surprising that few firm policy-relevant con- standing policy questions can be usefully addressed clusions can be reached at this point. using existing research methods. Each of the preced- ing literature review chapters contains suggestions An even more fundamental reason for the lack of for such research. The following listing simply il- policy-relevant findings is that the questions which lustrates the kinds of specific studies which seem have been asked by researchers have often borne lit- likely to yield practical guidance on current issues: tle relation to those asked by policymakers. As we indicated. in the preceding chapter, many of the What constitutes an "appropriate device" to sep- studies we reviewed attempted simply to document the intended effects of advertising. However, a arc.e programs and commercials? demonstration that a particular commercial or ad- vertising technique is persuasive, even irresistibly The NAB code requires that commercials persuasive, does not necessarily lead to any directly directed at children "shall be clearly sep- policy-relevant conclusions. Similarly, what are arated from program material by an ap- policymakers to do with research findings which propriate device." In the absence of any confirm age-relateddifferences as a mediating more specific definition, each network variable in the effects of television advertising? This has created a different separation device, is not to say that such research is not useful in as have the independent stations. No test- developing a better understanding of how advertis- ing has been done to determine whether ing works. However, future investigations must go any of these devices do in factassist beyond the general lines laid down by the studies children in distinguishing between reported to date if they are to be helpful in clarifying programs and commercials. If some or and resolving the important issues in this. debate. all of the devices are proved to be effec- tive, there are also the questions of how We are proposing that new research efforts be effective they are and how the various directed in three areas more closely linked to ongo- current devices compare. Finally, alter- ing policy concerns. We conceive of these three areas native devices, such as placing commer- as "levels" o_ f generality: (1) "midlevel" research to cials within a visual "frame" (a technique test specific hypotheses or premises upon which ex- now employed by some stations toiden- isting or proposed regulations are based, (2) tify so-called "free speech messages"), "macrolevel" research on the role of television adver- could be tested and compared. This data in tisinginchildren'slives; and (3)"microlevel" w "uld be of nmediate use to the NAB research to examine how children perceive specific, refining itdefinition of an appropriate individual commercials. separation device.

I 151 ..x. k../ How effective are commercial food messages in existing evidenceissparse and in- communicating nutritional information to conclusive. 'A single experiment indi- children' cated that although children's attention to an adult drug commercial was con- How much responsibility advertisers siderably less than to child-oriented com- should bear for a nutritional education mercials, the exposure produced modest has been the subject of a continuing con- attitudinal changesinthe children troversy. Currently, the FTC is consider- (Atkin, 1975b). ing adoption of a complex trade rule regulation requiring disclosure of a con- One specific focus of concern about the siderable amount of nutritional informa- effects of adult advertising on children is tion in television and other advertising advertising for over-the-counter drugs. for fdodproducts. Althoughnot However, the issue of children's specifically aimed at children'srelevision responses toadult advertisingisa advertising, the proposed regulation broader one. Studies of children's atten- wouldaffect food commercials for tiontothese commercials, and their children. Industry spokesmen argue that effects on children's knowledge, at- the brevity of televrSion commercials titudes, and behavior are much needed. make them inappropriate vehicles for Descriptive studies of the extent and such disclosures, and they suggest that nature of adult-child co-viewing and its the addition of more nutritional informa- influences as a mediating process would tion could conceivably produce more also provide important data for evaivat- confusion than education among ing the significance of children's exposure children. Testing children's perceptions to adult advertising. of food commercials containing the pro- posed disclosures couldestablishthe MACROLEVEL RESEARCH validity of these industry assertions. Ex- periments using a variety of audio and The research we are proposing heie is intended to visual techniques could also determine determine, for the first time, the importance of the most effective means for com- television as an influence on childrenin com- municating nutritionalinformationto parison with other major s,,alizing factors, such as children. parents, relatives, peers, school, church, and other media. There is a pressing n, d for studies which How do children respond to adult-oriented ad- could help settle the prolonged controversy between vertising? those who believe television is simply an innocuous source of leisure-time entertainment, which children While the NAB and NAD advertising quickly learn to treat with a casual and healthy skep- guidelines are intendedtoprovide ticism, and others who believe that the medium has special protection to children, they apply become a primary shaping force in children's lives, to no more than 15 percent of all the competing strongly with the traditional roles of television advertising that children see. parents, school, and church. The research we are Defenders of the guidelines argue that proposing would not be focused on television, but chlgren are interested only in products rather on children and ihe shaping forces in their en- relevant to them and therefore disregard vironment, including television and television adver- obviously adult-oriented advertising tising. They also argue that most viewing by children of adult commercials takes Such a program of research would be neither sim- place in the company of adults (parents), ple to organize nor inexpensive to conduct It would who mediate the effects of such advertis- undoubtedly require the extension of current ing, research methods and the development of new tech- niques of measurement. Particular attention would Neither of these assertions has been have to be given to the validity of observational and subjected to systematic testing, and the survey measures. Despite these considerable

152 1 56 difficulties, we believe that the effort would he the NAD's a0m.rable principle that children's com- justified by the importance of the results, not only to mercials be "truthful, accurate, and fair to children's issues relevant to television and television advertis- perceptionsThis research would resemble the type ing, but also to a variety of other issues related to the of studies routinely conducted by advertising agen- quality of children's lives in this country cies in preparing commercialsIt would focus on commercials as whole entities as well as on the We are not proposing a multiyear, longitudinal specific techniques or component elements which studyFollowing the development of a group of were the concern of most of the academicstudies c' idren over an extended period oltime might well reviewed in this report produce the most revealing data, but the cost of carrying out such research, and the long delay in However, the research we are proposing differs reporting results, make this a less attractive alterna- from that conducted by advertising agenciesin tive Rather, we envision a program of research with several important ways.It would be conducted a relatively large and heterogeneous population of systematically, it would employ randomized sam- children (and other members of their families). It ples of commercials as well as of children, it w,uld would employ a variety of techniques and measures examine unintended as well as intended effects; and, to determine the relative importance of (and the in- of course, it would be publicly available While the teractions between) the sources from which cL.Idren research would concentrate on commercials in- acquire the information that influences their values tended for children, it should also include, adult - and attitudes and shapes their behavior Among the oriented advertisements This research would not be project's objectives would he an examination of the costly, nor would it require elaborate facilities to role of television within tne context of family ex- conduct it perienceand an examination of family interactions within the context provided by televisionWhile The purposes of this research would he twofold. some time would be required for planning this First, it would allow the development of methods to research project, for solicitation of proposals, for identify specific commercials which are confusing or site selection, and for startup, it should he possible misleading to children. Second, it would lead to the for this research to he carried out and reported with- accumulation of data about children's perceptions of in a 2- to 3-year period commercials (based on children's responses to a range of actual commercials), which could he used MICROLEVEL RESEARCH to determine the comprehensibility and fairness of 2rtising fora particular product or product The research we are proposing here is at the op- tategory or the effectiveness of a particular tech- posite end of the spectrum from the preceding nique. Ultimately, it may he possible to develop a recommendation. The oblective of this microlevel standardized instrument to test commercials prior to research would be to determine how well actual broadcast to ensure that they are, in fact, "truthful, television advertisements for children comply with accurate, and fair to children's perceptions

153 15 Appendix A

EVALUATION OF INDIVIDUAL STUDIES

The following 21 articles and papers are included 10. J. R. Rossiter and T. S. Robertson, Children's in this analysis' Television Viewing: An Examination of Parent-Child Consensus.' Sociometry, 1975, in I. E. Wartella andJ.Ettema, A Cognitive press. Developmental Study of Children's Attention I I. T. S. Robertson and J. R. Rossiter,`Children to Television Commercials. Communication and Commercial Pers'asion: An Attribution Research, Volume 1, Number1, January Theory Analysis. Journal of Consumer 1974. Research, Volume 1, June 1974. 2. C. E. Lewis and M. A. Lewis, The Impact of 12. S. Ward and D. Wackman, Children's Infor- Television Commercials' on Health-Related mation Processing of Television Advertising, Beliefs and Behaviors of Children. Pediatrics, in P. Clark., (ed.), New Models for Mass Com- Volume 53, Number 3, March 1974. munication Research, Beverly Hills: Sage, 3. J. P. Liefeld, et. al., Television Advertising pages 119-146. 1973. and Children: An Experimental Study. 13. M. Goldberg and G. Gorn, Children's Reac- Working paper, University of Guelph, tions to Television Advertising: An Experi- Guelph, Ontario. ., mental Approach. Journal of Consumer 4. R. S. Rubin, An Exploratory Investigation of Research,Volume I, September 1974. Children's Responses to Commercial Content 14. C. Atkin, Effects of Television Advertising of Television Advertising in Relation to their on Children. First Year Experimental Evi- Stages of Cognitive Development. Ph. D. dis- dence. Report #1, TV Advertising and sertation, University of Massachusetts, 1972. Children Project, Final Report, June 1975. 5. S. Ward, D. Wackman, and E. Wartella, 15. C. Atkin, Effects of Television Advertising Children Learning to Buy: The Development on Children, Second Year Experimental Evi- of Consumer Information Processing Skills. dence. Report #2, TV Advertising and Marketing Science Institute Report, Novem- Children Project, June 1975. ber 1975. 16. C. Atkin, Effects of Television Advertising 6. T. A. Shimp, R. F. Dyer, and S. F. Divita, on Children. Survey of Pre- Ndolescent's Advertising of Children's Premiums on Responses to Television Commercials. Television: An Experimental Evaluation of Report #6, TV Advertising and Children Proj- the FTC's Proposed Guide. Unpublished ect, July 1975. manuscript. George Washington University, 17. J. R. Milaysky, B. Pekowsky, and H. Stipp, 1975. TV Drug Advertising and Proprietary and Il- 7. A. Caron and S. Ward, gift L.icisions by licit Drug Use Among Teenage Boys. Public Kid.nd Parents.Jourtkl of Advertising Opinion Quarterly, Winter 1975-76. R, ..h, Volume 15, Number 4, August 18. Howard, Hulbert, and Lehmann, An Explor- I atory Analysis of the Effect of Television Ad- 8. S. d and D. Wr man, Family and vertising on Children. Unpublished. Mewd Influences on ,adolescent Consumer 19. T. S. Robertson and J. R. Rossiter, Short- Learning. Amer' .Behavioral Scientist, Run Advertising Effects on Children: A Field Volume la:.v,.per3, (January/February Study. Journal of , 1971), pages 41.",-427. Volume XIII (February 1976), pages 68-70. 9. T. G. Bever, M. L. Smith, B. Bengen, and T. 20. J. R. Rossiter and T. S. Robertson, Children's G. Johnson, Young Viewers' Troubling TV Commercials: Testingthe Defenses.Jour- Response to TV Ads. Harvard Business nal of Communications, Volume 24, Autumn Review, November-December 1975. 1974.

-19.1Igeniym,...... 21 R Faber and S Ward, Validation of Mother- in one case (17) and unclear in the other (5) Child Purchase Influence Frequency Reports Study 8 randomly selected classrooms within by the Multitrait-Multimemod Matrix Tech- schools which were not randomly selected nical Report, Marketing Science Institute, Several of the studies (2, 14, 15, Rossiter and April 1975 Robertson studies) attempted to include all subjects within participating schools, in all in- All of the studies included in these reviews are stances the method of selecting schools was not concerned with the relationship between TV adver random or was unspecifiedIn eight of the using and some child-oriented outcome, such as studies (1, 3, 4, 6, 9, 13, 16, 18) the selection of purchase behavior or cognitive learning Since these subjects was not random or was unspecified studies investigated a variety of questions and used Thus, it is unfortunate that the findings of most different outcome measures, tney will he considered of these studies cannot he generalized beyond independently. For the purposes of this overview, those students actually participating One in- however, it may also be informative to examine tilt. vestigator (Ward) participated in four of the area as a whole, so that some tentative "state of the five studies which used random selection to art" generalizations about the research methods some degree, other investigators in this area used may be made, discussing the following aspects could increase the generalizability of their of the studiespopulation, sample selection pro- findings by incorporating random selection cedures, sample size, outcome measures, unit of procedures into their studies analysis, statistical tests, and experimental design C. Sample Size A. Populations The sample sizes used in these studies were The populations investigated by these generally quite large The sample sizes ranged studies are fairly diverse The age or grade from 30 (3) to 1, 094 (8) All but five had more level of the subjects was specified in all studies than 100 subjects, and eight included at least Ages of the children ranged from as young as 2 250 subjectsIt should he noted that several years (study 18) to12th graders (study 8), studies (2, 5, 7, 12) had difficulty in getting however, most of the subyct populations were subjects to participate or suffered from high at- attending preschools or elementary schools tritionrates,thusreducing thesize (and The investigators often mentioned the social perhaps reducing the representativeness) of the class of the subjects, and a range of SES levels intended sample represented in at least a few of the studies The choice of areas of the country from which to D. Outcome Measures select the population fog study appears usually to have been determined by t'e investigators -This area is characterized by the existence of location, as a result, several areas are under- a wide variety of measures of the effects of TV represented or excluded (e.g , the West Coast) advertising on childrenIt is appropriate here to comment upon some general characteristics B. Sample Selection Procedures of the instruments used in these studies

(Note Since all four studies by Rossiter and All but two of the studies (I,II) utilized Robertson-10, II,19, and 20apparently nonstandard outcome measures, that is, the in- used the same subject population, they will he vestigator designed the instrument specifically considered as a single study in this and the for the purposes of the particular study Of the following section on sample size ) other two, one (11 ), used a modified version of a previously reported interview procedure, Five of the 17 separate studies (5, 7, 8, 12, and the other (I ), used a previously reported 17) utilized some form of random selection attention measure. The absence of standard procedure. Studies 7 and 12 randomly selected testinstruments isstriking, and presents individual subjects. Studies 5 and 17 randomly serious problems forconsumers of this selected individuals within schools, however, research(a)it makes direct comparisons of the method of selecting schools was deliberate finding, ,cross studies difficult, since it is often 1G1 156 vertising on children, it seems to be a necessary notpossible to determineitthe various measures are actually tapping the same out- outgrowth' of the realization that TV's in- comes, and (b) the tact that many ofthese in- fluence pervades many aspects of the child's vestigators unsigned the outcome measures life.In order to accurately gage its effects, specitically for the purposes of their studies various outcomes related to cognitivedevelop- opens the possibility that their expectations ment and consumer behavior must beinvesti- may have been reflected in the designof the gated. Short-term outcome measures predomi- test instruments or procedures and thus in- nate, long-term outcome measures are rare. fluenced the findings For example, in attempt- ing to determine how for whether)attitudes E. Unit of Analysis toward TV advertising change with age, there 4401 is an unlimited number of interview questions There was complete homogeneity with which the child can be asked evenif the respect to the choice of the unit ofanalysis in different investigators all adhered to a struc- these studies All 21 studies considered the in- tured, closed-ended interview format. It is ob- dividual subject as the unit of analysis, vious that the choice of which specific ques- although severalof theinvestigatorsper- tions are asked may dramatically alien the formed additional analyses on other units (e.g outcome of a studyIt would seem to be of products requested). Thus, the problem of at- foremost importance to develop valid and tempting to compare the resultsof studies reliable measures of the outcomes investigated which utilize different analytical units is not an in these studies which can be meaningfullyad- issue in this area ministered in different experimental situations More content-specific measures may also he an F. Statistical Tests essential component of these studies, but they should ideally be supplemented with more All but two of these studies (9, 18) report general indices statistical tests of the significance of their find- ings, although' in a few instances(2, 7, 8, 12) all In addition to content specific differences possible tests are not specified or carried out. between measures tapping similar outcomes. The choice and use of tests generally appears the investigators adopted various formats for appropriate, with a few exceptions(multiple gathering information open-ended vsclose- statistical comparisons (3,14), use of ended questions, written vs verbal interviews, parametric tests when nonparametric tests ap- behavioral vs less "active indices, mother vs pear more appropriate (6)) child reports. etc Choice of form may in- fluence the findings, and efforts should be G. Experimental Designs made to determine the comparability of the various research strategiesAn example of Seven of the studies (I. 3, 4, 6, 13, 14, 15) such an effort is provided by study 21, in which utilized truly experimental designs to investi- All of the authors investigated the agreement between gate the effects of TV ads on children mothers' and children's reports of child these studies used randomization to some purchase influence frequencyThe tact that degree. In studies I, 3, 4, and 13, individual their data indicate convergent validity for the subjects were randomly assigned to treatment two measures increases the confidence that can conditions Subjects in study 6 were randomly vs control condi- he placed in comparisons between studies using assigned to experimental these different indices tions, however, it is unclear how subjects were assigned to the different experimentaV varia- In additio6 to differences in specific content tions. While treatments wererandomly and format among measures designed to tap assigned to groups of four subjects in study14, formed; in similar outcomes, the studies demonstrate con- itis unclear how the groups were slierable diversity with respect to the out- addition, the procedure for assigning subjects conditions is comes which they attempt to investigateWhile to several measurement this diversity may hinder efforts to make unspecified Instudy15, subjects in each general statements about the effects of TV ad- classroom were randomly divided into two

157 164. groups, and one ot tour treatments was 'Ian very short-term effects of TV advertising It is domly assigned to each group It is commenda- possible that these effects may, wash out over ble that all of these experimental studies incor- longer periods of time g , even in the time it porated randomizationintotheirresearch actually lakes to get to the store') design, thus minimizing the possibility 6f pre- treatment between group differences biasing In contrast, the 13 descriptive studies the outcome sacrifice experimental controls for closer ap- proximations to real consumer- activities To The remaining 13 studies can oe clas 'fled the degree to which questionnaire and inter- as "descriptive'', the investigator d ot ex- view responses accurately reflectsubject pose subjects to an experimental treatment, but behavior, these studies tirovide valid evidence simply-obtained information about subjects' as to TV advertising's effects on children in existing behaviors. While these studies offer in- natural settings. Unfortunately, the validity of teresting descriptio.is of the relationship be- the data collected in this manner is difficult to tween TV advertising and children's assess behaviors, the lack of experimental controls does not permit causal inferences to be drawn' There appears to be two reasonable from these findings. Thos, a principal function strategies for attempting to integrate the evi- of these studies may be to generate hypotheses dence provided by both types'of research. The for future experimentation first involves comparing the results of surveys .i_ and experimentalresearch. Agreement be- Considei ing that only about one-third of the iween the findings should increase our confi- TV advertising studies are experimental, one dence in the validity of both types ot research may be tempted to conclude that these studies Lack of agreement would pinpoint particular constitute a methodologically weak area Such research topics on which to concentrate future a cm/elusion may not be justified, however etforts Ses,eral of the questions which the experiment- ers chose to investigate (e.g , the relationship The second strategy invokes es designing between attitudes toward TV and age) may not "naturalistic experiments" which attempt Co be amenable to experimental methods In ad- impose experimental controls and yet maintain dition, experimental research in this area is a realistic atmosphere as wellStudies which plagued by serious limitations It is not clear at successfully meet these requirements would this time whether the experimental etforts pro- provide the strongest evidence concerning TV vide theNst or most important evidence as to advertising's effects Two ot the studies in- thek_d\al-likeffects of TV advertising on cluded in this review, one experimental (3) and childre While experimental controls allow one descriptive (19), have made efforts in this causal inferences to be drawn about the treat- directionStudy 3investigatedsubjects' ment effects, there is reason for concern as to purchase request behavior following experi- whether the artificiality of the experimental mental viewing of commercial advertising conditions accurately reflect actual viewing Subjects accompanied their mothers to a local and reacting behavior. For example, most of grocery store, where investigators posing as these experiments were conducted in viewing employees or shoppers observed and recorded situations away from the home, which were their behavior. Study 19 attempted to assess generally free from distractions These condi- the effects of TV toy and game advertising on tions undoubtedly result in increased attention children's toy and game preterences by survey- to commercials. The authenticity of other ing subjects before and after the pre-Christmas aspects of the experimentai settings, such as advertising peakWhile these studies have opportunities for choosing desired products, serious limitations (e g , study 19 is largely un- the actual material viewed, and length of view- controlled), they represent important attempts ing time, is also questionable. A very impor- to combine the benefits of descriptive and ex- tant qualification of the experimental studies perimental studies into a single research involves the tact that they generally focused on paradigm

151.63 A striking difference between the experi-_ publication of early descrjaLive work en- mental and descriptive studies should be men- couraged later efforts whichMilt directly on tioned. Most (5/7) of the experimental studies the precedirig studies, and thus gained easy ac- are unpublished (3, 4,'6, 14, 15),while a ma- cess into the published literature.Whatever the 1 jority (10/13) of the descriptive studies are reason, it is unfortunate that many consumers published (all but 5, 16, 18). Therefore, if only of TV advertising research, whose main access publica- published sources had been considered in this to this literature is through journal review, descriptive studies would have out- tions, must rely almost exclusively ondescrip- numbered,experimental studies by 10 to 2 The tive studies for information about theeffects of predominance of descriptive studies inthe TV advertising on children In the future, in- published literature may be a result of the creased emphasis on experimental studies in research preferences of the leading investiga- thepublishedliterature would provide a tors in the field (e.g., 7 of the 12 published arti- valuable supplement to the regularly appear cles were authored by Ward etal., or by mg descriptive studies Rossiter and Robertson), or, it may be that the

Study 1

A Cognitive Developmental Study of Children's At- Experimental Design: Within each of the three age tention to Television Commercials. E Wartella and levels, 10 children were randomly assigned toview J. Ettema Communication Research, Vol. 1, No I, each of the four versions of the program.Subjects blocks were not January 1974 viewed the program in pairs. Since rotated, commercial content was not used as anin- Purpose: To test the relationship between stimulus dQpendent variable (due to possible fatiguefactors). complexity of television commercials (with content controlled) and children's attention to the stimuli. Outcome Measures: Attention was measuredby a scheme devised by Ward, Levinson, andWackman Population: Nursery school, kindergarten, and sec- (1972) Attention was coded as full, partial, or none ond grade students in an upper middle class subur- at given observation intervals Fullattention indi- position wit ban St. Paul, Minnesota, school. cated that the child was in a viewing eyes on 'cite screen; partial attentionindicated that Sample Selection Procedure: Unspecified the child was in a viewing position with eyesoff the screen, "apparently not listering," orverbally or Sample Size: 120 (40 at each grade level) physically reacting to the television content; no at: tension indicated that the child was not in a viewing Interob- Specific Tre: Alt:Subjects were free to watch position and eyes were not on the screen (or not watch) a (situation comedy) server reliability on a subsample ofsubjects was The original commercials were deleted and com- 90 6 7( mercials manipulated in terms of stimulus complex- ity were inserted Twelve commercials were used, Unit of Analysis: Individuals grouped in three blocks, 1) irrelevant commercials, composed of four commercials concerning products Statistical Tests: Yes (ANOVA) of low relevance for children, 2) three relevant com- mercials concerning foods, and 3) five relevant corn Results: Analysis employed theobservations of mercials also concerning foods The commercials only the first two blocks. The authors predicated of the corn -, varied as to visual and auditory complexity, as rated that differences in stimulus complexity by a measure developed by Watt and Krult (1972) mercials should produce differences in attention Four versions of the program were used, with the with the most complex commercial (high onboth commercials rotated within the blocks so that each visual and auditory) receiving the most attention It appeared as the first commercial in the block in one and the least complex (low-low) receiving least. version (except for one of the block 3 commercials) was alsopredictedthatthis difference should Blocks were not rotated within the program decrease with age An attention score (the subject's

1 5.16 4 average attention across all observations during a Success: The treatment effects partially supported single commercial) for each subject on eachc the author's hypothes,of the effects of stimulus mercial was computed The attentionscores were complexity on children's attention analyzed by a t.-way ANOVA (age by stimulus complexityYith repeated measures on the first tac- Criticisms: The authors point out two weaknesses tor A separate analysis was computed for each of of the study I) the older subjects had a uniformly the two blocks For the irrelevant product block, high attention mean, whichmay have produced both main effects were significant, while theage x ceiling effect depressing the differentscores of these stimulus complexity interaction wa not significant childr:nthus, "the-issue of whether or not thela- A comparison (not statistically tested) of themeans fluence of perceptual attributes declines withage re- indicated that kindergarteners had the highestmean mains open to debate," and 2) stimulus complexity attention and nursery schoolers the lowest mean at- only accounted for 2r4 of the variancesin attention tention, and that attention was highest for the high- Thus, the absolute effect of this variable (whilesig- visual'high auditory commercial and lowest for the nificant) was small The attention measure appeared high visual low auditory commercial Theinterac- to he more sensitive to the child's visual reaction tion was in the predicted &rectums (although not than his auditory reactions For example,a child significant) the difference in attention to the hi'h- who was not :n a viewing position andwriose eyes high (h-h) and low-low ( 1-1 ) is greatest fornursery were not on the screen would be rated "no attention" schoolers, smallest for second graders For the rele- although he could nave hi 11 listening quite atten- vant prouuct block, both main effects and the age x tively rtimulus complexity interaction were signiticant At- ten ion to these commercialt was not orderedac- Ihe Hoek 2 commercials differedin lengththis cording to stimulus complexity ( I-visual, h-audito- lac ,a1) have had an effect on children's attention ry highest, I-I lowest) As predicted, the (h-h) ..,-:ores in addition to complexity Also, the commer- ( II) difference is largest for the youngest group cials within blocks differed in specific content(e g I he authors note that the tact that the irrelevant vsGatorade) % hich may have in- block came tirst may have influences, the insignifi- fluenced attention to themchildren's familiarity or cant interaction for that block, since ..he subjects inheres'in the products apparently was not con- may have been adjusting to the env ironment during trolled It would seem to he more effective to create that period different versions, ith different complexity levels) of one commercial and to present the differentver- Further analyses were andertaken withan atten- sions to different groups of subjects tion change measure, whichisthe sum of the changes in attention from one observation to the next for 12 I he authors, state why the third block data observations marking transitions from commercials (representing intermediate ratings of complexity) to programs and vice versa A one-way ANOVA for were not reportedIt would he informattve to know age was signiticantNursery school children ap- it finer gradation, of complexity did not demon- peared to change the most in their attention from oh- strate a signhcant effect ,,n attention ser% ation to observation The tuthors feel that this finding supports the hypothesis thatyounger It would has.. been intormative to fest the specific children are more sensitive to shifts vont programto contrasts within faclors w with a post hoc test or, better commercial and vice versa Fora ,r data analyses yet, to hip"! built planned ..-ontrasts into the analysis demonstrated that, for the irrelev t block, mean at- 1 his would have been possible since the authors had tention scores of all children were higher for high a priori hypotheses about the outeome auditory than low-auditory his sug- gests that variation in auditory complexiti may be Sponsorship: Office otheld 1)0, elopment more important than % dilation in visual complexity (differences not tested statistically iIn addition, Published: \ es high auditory commercials exhibit moremovement toward bull :Attention than low auditory,commer- Summary:1 his study suegest, that the stimulus cials and the decline it attention for the tormeiis complexity ofonimereiab, and age ar... related to generally more gradual children's attention,ld thattheeffectsof

1{',)) 160 and the differencesinstimulus complexity on attention small percent of the variance in attention', and decrease wit'h,age. The study's principal weaknesses potential biasing effects of differences in length are a possible ceiling effect for the oldersubjects, the specific content of coirmercia;s w..r ..uocks fact that stimulus complexity only accounted tor a

Study 2

The Impact of Television Commercials on Health- ment" inthe strict sense, the investigators were Related Beliefs and Behaviors of Children. C E simply interested in the reactions of two naturally- Lewis and M. A. Lewis Pedtatri, s, Vol 53, No 3, occurring groups ts,) one condition (watching health- .4nrch 1974 related messages on TV )

Purpose. To study the impact of health-related Unit of Analysis: Commercial messages ;advertised television messages on children products) and individuals

Populatior: There were tviopopulations under Statistical Ttatts: Yes (X2) and descriptive data; study: 1) miaversity lab school Auden:s (5th and 6th tests specified grades) presumably of middle to upper class and mostly white (group A), 2) public elementary selfool inboth 5th and 6th grades classified as disadvantaged and Result.: Most of the messages viewed groups were commercials. There were differences mostly no'white (group B) (not tested statistically) in the types of commercials Sample Selection Procedure: Nil students were reported by the two groups A higher percent of asked to participme However, only 90% of group A school B students believed the messages, used the subjects and 54% of group B subjects actually par- duci, and parents used the product There were ticipated .The method of selecting the two Par- otner specific ditfeiences with respect to these fac- 1 here was a ticipating schools is unclear tors fin -various types of pr:;duct statisticallysignificantassociation (X2) between Sample Size: School A 117 (out of 130) actually children's beliefs and parents' use of the products completed reports School B 91 (out of 170) ac- advertised the child is more likely to believe if the parent uses or vice versa The authors report that tually completed reports there were statistically significant positive associa- tions between children's beliefs and their use of the Outcome Meastires: Written student respOnses to products, and parent use and child use, but the several questions concerning health messages they analyses are not presented for these latter findings viewed on TV Pcicentages seem to indicate that parental use was more influential with respect to chilaten's beliefs Experimental Design: Written instructions were than children's useStudents attending school A distributed to children and parents explaining that were classified as to the number of commercials they asocialstudies the "'assignment" was partof by their teachers as to their withhut believed and' were rat( program and students were to complete it "critical thinking ability" (rated fromI -9) There The forms requested data about six assistance was little apparent dift2rence between thecritical students messages concerning health or illness which th iking ability ratirgs of "believers," and "inabe viewed onI V in their home The students ap- tuieeners,- and "skeptics" the authors report that no parently could choose which messages to reportI he association between these variabl i( idtest students supplied information about time of viewing, -unspecified) Finally, over 9tri- of stutits in both they nature of the program, what the message was, it schools made inferences from their descriptions ex- believed the message, and (it they reported on a actly as intended by- the sponsors, and the viewing commercial) whether or not they had ever tried the oatterns 01 the two groups were similar product and it their parents had ever used itSub- jects had week to complete the assignment This study was not experimental since the researchers did Success: Not appocable, since a prolam cfr treat- not as4ign subjects to groups and there was no -treat- ment was not being evaluated Criticisms: The authors point out 3 weah.resses incomplete and thus were eliminated. An effort television programs were viewed by groups A and B should have been made to determine if those subjects at different points in time (10 weeks apart) In addi- not participating were different from those included tion, what shows (and commercials) were actually watched and reported c_*. was not controlled, itis Published: Yes possible that some subjects reported only certain types of commercial messages--e.g., those that they believed, 2) the data on drug taking behaviors of Sponsorship: Public Health Service both children and parents are unvalidated; and 3) no attempt was made to determine what types of errors Summary: This essentially descriptive study indi- in judgment the children made (disbelieving a true cated that children generally believe TV health-re- message or belie-mg a false one) lated messages, and that associations exist between children's beliefs and parent's use, children's beliefs As was mentioned above, the response rates were and their own use, and parent use and child use Its low, especially for the disadvantaged group. In ad- major weaknesses are high attrition rate and ques- dition, the authors report that certain responses were tionable validity ot data

Study 3

Television Advertising and Children An Experi- jests in both groups were ld that *hey could play mental Study. John P. Liefeld et al., working paper, with some toys Eight out of the 17 toys were the U. of Guelph, Guelph, Ontario, 1974 products advertised in the commercial seen by the subjects in the second group Observers recorded the Purpose: To investigate the effects of TN adv ertise- physical and verbal behavior of the subjects for a 10- ments on selected physical and verbal behavior of minute period, and their observations were checked year-old male children by viewing video tape records During the toy play period, the subjects exposed t,toy commercials Population: Mother and son pairs who were from were the experimental (E) grout-, while the children whitecollar or managerialiprofessional families exposed to the breakfast cereal commercials made who were at least second generation, or Anglo-Sax- up the control (C) group on Canadians Following the toy play session, mothers collected Sample Selection Procedure: 50 subjects meeting their sons and pro-eeded to a large grocery store. the above specifications were identified through ads Each mother entered the supermarket and began in newspapers, letters to mothers of children in shopping "normally No more than one mother and public school kindergarten classes, and through son pair entered the breakfast cereal aisle at one "personal references Questionnaire responses timeChildren'sreactionstothecereals -were allowed the investigator to eliminate those subjects recorded by observers dressed in clerk uniforms, who consumed the two cereals or owned the two toys and verbalizations were tape recorded (the recorder to be advertised in the experiment. was buried in a cart full 01 groceries which was "unobtrusively" pushed by a woman shopper) In the Sample Size:32 mother-son pairs met the above shopping condition the subjects exposed to cereal requirements and agreed to participate. ot which 30 commercials were the E subjects, while the subjects pairs completed the experiment. exposed to tov commercials acted as C subjects Two days after the xperiment a sa,aple of subjects (selec- Experimental Design: Subjects were randomly tionprocedt.. unspecified )was interviewed to assigned to two treatments. Both groups ot subjects determine if tht_ child had developed awareness of viewed cartoons. Subjects in the first gr,r_ip were the experiment none of the subjects indicated such shown four commercials advertising each if two awareness cereals "cereal subjects", subjectsinthe secon.: group viewed four commercials for each of two toys Outcome Measures: Background [Mtn:nation pro- "toy subjects" After the viewing sessions, the sub- vided by mother, and observations ot the child's TV

I watching behavior, shopping behavior, and toy play- Sponsorship: Unspecified. ing behavior. Published: No Statistical Tests: Yes (t tests, z tests) Criticisms: The authors are aware of the study's multiple Unit of Ana!;:! :: Individuals major weaknesses small sample sizes, using statistical comparisons. failurp to control for Results: There were few differences between the differences in initial attractiveness between the E groups in 'background, TV viewing,playing, and and other products, and measuring only the short- shopping behaviors not directly related to the ex- term effects of TV advertising. The authorsadmira- perimental hypothesesthus, the groups appeared bly emphasize that as a result, these findings areonly comparable. The results demonstrate that E subjects tentative "These procedures were deemedjustifia- were more likely to direct theirbehavior toward the ble given the exploratory nature of this study. They advertised toys. Also, E subjects in the shopping ex- are useful for developing 'hypothesesfor further periment showed more "approach" behaviortoward study and for providing tentative evidence on tlic the advertised cereals than C subjects (she group effects of television advertising on children'." differences were not dramatic, however). There was no difference between the groupsin purchase in- Summary: This study suggests that TV advertising fluence attempts Differences in behavior betwe' C increases approach behavior of subjectstoward ad- subjects or between E subjects on two clitfer,w,est- vertised toys and cereal. Subjects were randomly ing days (I week apart) were inter preted as :-esulting assigned to trea"ient groups, and the investigators from differences in background variables however, attempt-alto observe children's behaviorin a n's were so small for these comparisons that little natural setting (grocery store). The study's principal confidence can be placed in them weaknesses are small sample size, use of multiplz statistical comparisons, failure to control for the ini- Success: If success is defined or the E subjects ap- tial attractiven ss of the E and other products,and proaching the advertised products more than C sub- measuring only short-term advertising effects. 44. jects. there was only slight indicationsthat is oc- curred

Study 4

An Exploratory Investigation of Children's levels were determined by, three levels of cognitive Responses to Commercial Content of Television development (the three grade levels) and twofor- Advertising in Relation to their Stages of Cognitive mats of ,commercial contentSubjects were ran- Development. Ronald S Rubin, Ph D dissertation, domly assigned to the two formats ofcommercial the two U of Mass , 1972 content Subjects were randomly assigned to commercial content formats (12 subjects percell, of a Purpose: To explore TV advertising stewing as it 6M, 6F) Subjects viewed two different versions version was affects the "consumer learning process" of the child new breakfast cereal commercialone product-oriented, while the other emphasized a toy racing car premium which comes inthe cereal box Population: First, third, and sixth graders attending The commercials had not been previously viewedby Bondsville Elementary School within the Palmer subjects. The outcome measure was recall ofselected School district in Western Massachusetts elements of the commercials and understandingof the commercial message. as measurt:d by aflexible Sample Selection Procedure: Not random, The questionnaire interview, designed for this s:udy. youngest subjects (and subjects not repeating agrade Subjects were interviewed immediately following level) at each grade level were selected the presentation of the commercial

Sample Size: 72 Outcome Measure: Flexible questionr.ure inter- view, designed for this studs Experimental Design: The experiment tested for the effect of six experimental conditions The six Statistical Tests: Yes (X2 )

163 1 6

9 Unit of Analysis: Individual Success: If "success- is arbitrarily definedas the existence of a relationship between children's ability' Results: There was a significant association be- to recall and understand commercial messages and tween the specific elements recalled and stages of stages of development or form of commercial con- development. older children were more able to tent presentation, the study was generally successful recall detailed information The recall of specific for stage of development but mixed for commercial elements was independent of level of commercial content presentation. content presentation The amount of elements recalled was significantlyrelatedtostages of Sponsorship: Unspecified development (older subjects recalledmore informa- Published: No. tion) and to the form of commercial presentation (children viewing the premium-oriented commercial Criticisms: Differences (e g, use of amm anon) recalled more elements). The action sequence recall other than premium presentation existed between of the subjects was significo.ttly associated with developmental stage (trend toward sequence recall the two commercial versions which may have in- fluenced children's responses. with increasing age), and with commercial content presentation (improved sequence recall with prod- Coding cf responses into categories, such as level uct-oriented commercial) The child's understand- of recall of action sequences, appeared to depend ing of the use of the product was independent of his somewhat on the coders judgment and thus may developmental levels (although there was a trend reflect an unconscious bias in the dgection of the ex- toward increased understanding with age), but was perimental expectations, especially if the ages or related to commercial content (children viewing the commercial content data were available while cod- product-oriented commercial were more often ing classified in the "understanding" category) There was a relationship between awareness that the., were In classifying subjects as to their understanding of viewing a commercial and stage of cognitive the use of the product, responses about the premium development (increased awareness with 4r.e) while were classified as reflecting lower level understand- children's awareness was independent of commer- ing. However, Lite child may view the premium as cial content presentation Understanding of why part of the product, failure to comment oil the prod- commercials Are shown (selling aspects) was related uct does not necessarily indicate a lack of under- to stage of development (increased understanding standing of its use. with age), but this variable was not related tocom- mercial content presentation. Similar findings ex- While the authors refer to grade levels as "stages isted for the child's understanding of why cominer- of cognitive development," "age" would be a more cials are made. There was a relationship betv.'een the accurate label for that variable. child's understanding of "what is supposed to be This study s subject to the usual qualifications ac- wanted" and stage of development (older under- companying the 'Ise of flexible questionnaire inter- stand better) and also with commercial content pre- view techniques with children. sentation (product children more often answered "cereal- while premium children mentioned the pre- Summary: This study indicated thatthere is mium). The reasons subjects gave as to wh; they generally an association between children's ability to were supposed to want the product were related to recall and understand commercial messages and developmental level and commercial content pre- their cognitive level (determined by age), and that at sentation (older and product presentation subjects times commercial content presentation (product were more aware of buying motive) versus premium emphasis) is related to children's responses, but to alesser extent than stage of The author presents additional analyses concern- development. Subjects were randomly assigned to ing results using aided recall and breaking down the types of commercial presentation. The study's prin- stage of development comparisons into comparing cipl weaknesses are differences between commer- two consecutive stages of development (instead of cial types other than premium emphasis, possible analyses involvingill three levels). These results bias due to using flexible interview techniques, and will not be presented here possible coding biases.

164 Study 5

Children Learning to BuyThe Development of I. Children's consumer i.formt.tion processing. Consumer Information Processing SkillsS. Ward, "There are consistent age-related changes in D. Wackman, and E Wartella MarketingScience the kinds of information children attend to, Institute Report, Nov 1975 select, and use to describe and conceptualize the consumer environment. This change ap- Purpose: To investigate children's acquisition of pears to reflect basic developmentalgrowth in buyin, skills children's cognitive capabilities toward in- creased awareness and use of more abstract, Population: Kindergarteners, third, ansixth g, functional kinds of information in consumer ders in Boston (Sommerville area) and M inneapolis- information processing." StPaul (Mounds View area). The Boston subjects were from a predominantly workingclass com- '7,Children's money use and purchase requests: munity, while the Minneapolis subjects werefrom "Children's money use skills (e g., saving) in- the middle class suburbs crease as children grow older" however, aut' ors did not find age- relato:d changes in Sample Selection Procedure: It is unclear if all nonskilled money use (es ,frequency of available schools were included; the authors ran- purchase requests) except forchild relevant domly sampled subjects from the participating pl.(.ct s." -schools, but the response rates were far below 100% (55% in Boston, 87% in Minneapolis) 3 The family cc,nte..t: of children's consumer socialization: "The family context for consumer Sample Size: 615 child-motherpairs (301in learning varies in rather consistent ways for Boston, 314inMinneapolis)The sample was different SES levels" (in particular, Interaction divided almost equally among grades, cities, sexes, with the child about consumption increases as and SES levels the mother's socialstatus increases, while lower status paresis appear to give their children more opportunities to operate as inde- Experimental Design: This study was descriptive. . Also, "The family con- Children and mothers were interviewed for1 hour pendent consumers) . substan- In addition, mothers completed a self-ad- text for consumer socialization differs each tially for children of different ages"... (e.g , ministered questionnaire. Children's responses to open-ended questions were coded as to "theoretical mothers of older children are more likely to negotiate with their child about purchases, and categories of interest to r.tovide them with greater opportunitiesf(ir independence as consumer). Outcome Measures: Nonstandard interviews and parent questionnaires (se7. Table, Variables). 4 the relationship between the family context and children's consumer, behuvior.In regression Unit of Analysis: Indivtdrrals and individual analyses with all age groups combined, "age responses (an individual r..ay respond morethan was the b...t predictor for nearlyall child once on certain items). behavior variables and the family support variables did not increase explanatory power Statistical Tests: Yes (X2, correlations, multiple to any major extent In the subsequent analyses regression). for the three separate grade revels, family con- text variables did increase ourexplanatory Results: The major independent variable in this power, but the importance of specific support study is the child's grade in school, sex and SES variables changed, between kindergarten and differences are also considered. There are many third grade. In particular, motherchild in- results reported in this book-length study, thus, they teraction var iables were consistently important will not he reported in detail here. The following for the development of kindergartener's con- summary comments describe the generalfindings sumer skills. On the other hand, mothers' own

165 1 70 consumer behavior appeared to he cons!stently Foundation of the American Association of Adver- unimportant for older children's skill develop- tising Agencies. ment. Published: In press 5. Additional finding: The relatronship between exposure to commercials and children's con- Criticisms: This study is based solelyon survey sumer learning is mixed and of rather limited data, and thus is subject to the usual qualifications importance. Also, exposure to commercials accompanying survey research. "does not appear to motivate children consist- ently toward increased spending or asking for Summary: This descriptive study presents several products relationships between children's consumer behavior, family context, and [eve of cognitive development. Success: Not applicable (no treatment). (See above summaries in results section.) Sponsorship:Office of Child Development, Marketing Sciences Instttute, and the Educational

Study 6

Advertising of Child 'en's Premiums on Television formation. The three experimental ads differed only An Experimental Evaluation of the FTC's Proposed as to the length of time devoted to premiurti presen- Guide. T. A. Shimp, R. F. Dyer, and S. F Divita tation (10, 15, or 20 seconds). The premium object Unpublished Manuscript, George Washington was a football team patch. Subjects were presented University 1975. with a 5-minute cartoon, the 30-second commercial, and a 1-minute announcement aboutpet care. Purpose: To empirically test the Federal Trade Commission's position that television advertising of Students within the four groups (viewing different premium offers to children are harmful and should commercial versions) were watched on be discontinued. Two specific )research questions age and sex and "balanced" on "cognitive ability" (memory and were investigated. ( 1) does the inclusion of a pre- verbal comprehension measures). Subjects mium portionin a TV commercial distract the were ran- domly assigned to the control condition, butitis child's attention from merits ofe principal prod unclear whether subjects were randomly assigned to uct?, and (2) is the necessary effe .t of the premium the different E treatments offer to cause children' to pure ase or want to purchase the advertised product`? Immediately postexposure,subjects'recallof Population: First t.) sixth grade chili.-1 attending specific features of the commercial and attitudes a Washington, D. suburbanpart. .nialschool towards the product and thepremium were which volunteered to participate in the study measured. In addition, a "simulated purchaseset- ting" was used to test the children's cereal preference Sample Selection Procedure: The school volun- (the advertised product versus two well-known teered to participate, it is unclear if all of its students brands) actually participated Outcome Measures: Sample Size: 197 recall subjects completed a pencd-and -paper multiple choice test (yes, no, not sure) Therewere Experimental Design: TV commercials serecon- 15 "product" questionsIn addition, E subjects structed for a hypothetical cereal product. Fourver- answered ar. additional II premium orientedques- sions of a 30-second TV commert ill were prepared tions a control commercial which only presented informa- tion concerning the cereal ?rid three experimental attitude children selected one of five faces ads w1lich included both premium and productin- (ranging from an extreme smiling face toan extreme _I 7/ 166 frowning face) corresponding to their feelings about mium recall decreased from the I 5-second to the 20- the premium and the experimental product second premium presentation )

brand choice preference the child selected the The second set of findings pertain to the hy- cereal he would most and second most prefer from pothesis that the premium presentations willin- three cereals (E brand and 2 others). fluence subject reactions to the product

Unit of Analysis: Individuals The E groups' responses to the "happy face" at- titude measures toward the premium and the adver- Statistical Tests: Yes tised product were correlated (Pearson) The cor- relations for both sevgroups were positive, they Results: The first set of findings pertain to the hy- were quite small and nonsignificant. Thus, "it ap- pothesis that including premium offers distracts the pears that greater liking oe a premium objectdoes child from relevant information about the quality of not necessarily create greater liking of the product the product containing the premium" (p. 24). In addition, the C group actually displayed a more favorable attitude A two-way ANOVA (cognitive level I by length of towaid the advertised product than the E subjects (t- commercial time devoted to presenting information test comparing mean attitudes) even though the E about premium) demonstrated significant main subjects were very favorably disposed to the pre- effectsHigher cognitive level subjects exhibited mium. greater recall of product information than lower cognitive level subjects. Scheffe's post hoc com- In the brand choice experiment, the majority of par. sons indicated that the "10-second premium'E subjects chose the experimental product as their group had significantly hiCier product recallthan least preferred cereal. While there is a tendency for the other two # groups However, the performance subjects to become more favorable to.le experimen- of the C group was not significantly different from tal product as the proportion of time devoted to pre- the E groups- "children exposed to aprod- senting the premium increases, the results (X2) were uct/premium ad did not have significantly less prod- not statistically significant Correlations between the uct recall than the product version only subjects" (p. respondents' brand chotce preferences for the ex- 18) perimental product with their attitudes toward it were significant, as were the correlations between at- A two-way ANOVA (cognitive development x titudes toward the premium object and preferences treatment) was performed on the amount of product for the experimental product. Although the latter recall (this was taken to he a measure of distraction) correlation is modest, it suggests thatthe more a The treatment differences were again significant in premium is liked, the more appealing is be adver- the same fashionasinthe previous analysis- tised product containing the premium" (p 28) However, the cognitive development factor was not significant Sponsorship: The School of Government and Busi- ness Administration, George Washington Univ..r- The E groups' accuracy in recalling product in- say formation was compared to their premium recall ac- curacy using t-tests for differences between propor- Published: No tions These results suggest that as the length of pre- mium presentation increases, the proportion of -c- Success: If "success" is arbitrarily defined as the curate product information recall decreases and pre- different treatments differentially affecting subject's mium recall increasesEven when equal timeis recall of relevant commercial information and, devoted to product and premium information, sub- brand-choice preferences, the support is mixed, and jects were able to more accurately recall 1, -emium appears to lean toward the "not successful" conclu- information (It should be noted, however, that pre- sion

Criticisms: The authors' decisionto divide the 'The authors arbitrarily labeled children less than 8 years sample according to age (less than 8 years versus 8 "preoperational and childreng or older "concrete-opera- or older) and to claim that this division corresponds tional to "level of cognitive development" is questionable. The fact that two variables are correlated (in this uct and premium recall should have been for corre- case age and cognitive level) is not sufficient reason lated samples, whether or not this is the case is to substitute one for the other It would have been unspecified. more accurate to refer to the variable as "age Summary: The results indicate that product recall It is possible that in selecting brand preferences accuracy may decline as greater proportions ot com- the child responded according to his desire for the mercial time are devoted to presenting premiums product, but that in an actual purchase situation, he However, it does not appear that devoting relatively would purchase the product one time to obtain the short periods of time to premium presentations dis- premium tracts from the child's product recall ability in ad- dition, liking:a premium object does not necessarily The authors compute Pearson correlations when insure that children will desire the product contain- one of the variables is the five "face valuesWhile ing the premium. The study was experimental, with these data are ordinal, they may not be interval scale subjects randomly assigned at least to the control and thus a nonparametric analysis may ha, c been condition The use of the "cognitive level" label ap- more appropriate In addition, the t-tests computed pears inappropriate, and the use of certain statistical for differences between proportions of correct prod- procedures is questionable

Study

Gift Decisions by Kids and Parents Andre Caron Statistical Tests: Yes, but not all necessary tests and Scott Ward, J of Advertising, Vol15, No 4. were carried out (or perhaps. simply not reported) Aug 1975.

Outcome Measures: Children's requests. mothers' Purpose: To examine certain aspects of the relative notes of children's requests and their responses, and influences of mass media and interpersonal sources parent's buying behavior not standard) on children's product desires and parental decision- making regarding their children's product desires, Unit of Analysis:Gift requests, gifts received, and individual subjects Population: Middle and upper class mother-child pairs in tv1( iitreal The children were third and fifth graders Type of Analysis: Descriptive and X:

Sample Selection Procedure: Random Results: Children requested "much the same kinds ot items (to Santa, parents. or both ) regardless ofage Sample Size: 84 mother-child pairs actually pro- or social classChildren most often cited television duced data (initial random ir.:IUded 54 third and 52 as the source ot gilt ideas. tollowed by friends Older fifth graders) children were considerably more likely to cite TV and less likely to cite friends thanyounger children, Experimental Design: 4 weeks before Christmas. and older children were more likely to cue catalogs subjects (children) were asked to write a letter to (differences not tested statistically ) Santa, telling him what they wanted for Christmas Children weie asked where they got the idea for each Gilt requestsYounger children asked for more gift requested "Mothers were trained to obtrusively gifts than older children Howe% er, they were less record each Christmas gift request dui mg a 7-day likely to receive specifically requested gifts Middle period and to note their verbal response (ii any) to class children requested more gifts than upper class the childA content analysis ot television commer- children, and they requested more gifts from "Santa cials directed to children in these age groups was only Middle classkidsreceivedmore gift,. also conducted although not used inthis study although the receipt of specifically requested gilts Following C'hi istmas vacation, the spe,iticgifts (conversion rate) was "%ery S111111,11.- ri both which children recmed were noted econon'ic groups There were significant differences (X2) in the Since there was a high attrition rate, there should types of gifts requested by children of different have been an effort to determine it those not produc- social classes, and there were also significant age ing data were atypical differences Sex differences were not significant The authors state thatthe finding that older Parents most often responded verbally in neutral chodren more often cite TV as a source of prodpct terms (e.g , "we'll see") to children's gift requests information, and that younger children are more The age and social class differences in parental ver- likely to find out about products hrough seeing bal responses were not significant them in stores However, their data indicate that the store is as often an information source for fifth gra- There were significant age, social class, and sex ders as for third graders differences in types of gifts received. Fifty percent of the gifts received had been explicitly requested Sponsorship: Grants from Radio Canada and either in the letter to Santa or to the parents. The Marketing Sciences Institute percent of specific gift requests that were fulfiller: is "somewhat higher" for upper class children. Re- quests made both to Santa and to parents were most Published: Yes likely to be fulfilled. Middle class families were more likely to yield to requests for certain typesof Success: The authors did not initiate an actual toys, while uppei class parents were more likely to program or treatment, thus itis not possible to yield to requests for other types of toys evaluate the success of the study

Criticisms: This was not an experimental studytIt is possible that mothers did not accurately record all Summary: This nonexperimental study demon- of the children's gift requests and their own strated several age and social class differences with responses to.the requests In addition, participation respect to sources of gilt ideas. gift requests, and gifts in the research study may have affected parents'gift received, although the differences were generally buying not dramatic

Study 8

Family and Media Influences on Adolescent Con- Outcome Measures:I he four criterion sal tables sumer Learning S Ward and D Wackman, recall of commercial content, attitudes toward TV andself American Behavioral Sc ientist, Vol14. No 3 advertiging,materialisticattitudes. (Jan ,Feb 19711 pp 415-427 reported effects of commercials on buying hehavior These and other variables %Am e measured by the Purpose: ho investigate the development of con- questionnaire responses except sumer learning in adolescents SES (measured bv Duncan soc mecum( index) and Population: Eighth through twelfth graders in the 10 (measured by'teach'' in school Prince Georges County, Maryland. school district Statistical Tests: Yes for rr s(mme analyses Sample Selection Procedure: Classrooms in12 schools were randomly selectedThe 12 schools Unit of Analysis:I p:i were not randomly selected (e g black schools refused to participate) Results: Subjects were Ws tiled into (At) age groups or the analyses. eighth and ninth graders versus Sample Size: 1,094 tentheleventh, and twelfth graders ( omparisons between the age groups indicateno significant Experimental Design: This sok::is descriptive differences on the four learning sritcriarecall. at- The subjects completed self-administered question- titudes, materialism, and busing helms tor I here naires were significant (test unspecified i age dif eren ces on

169 almost all of the "communication" variables utility reasons for viewing commercials is also a pre- younger subjects watch more TV, talk more with dictorforyoungersubjects Accordingtothe their parents about consumption, and are more authors, these results suggest that "simple exposure likely to watch commercials for "social utility" and to advertising is not a sufficient condition for buying "communication utility" reasons According to the behavior other variables involving the process- author's, "Their results suggest thatalth ugh ing of information about consumption intervent be- younger and older adolescents may be at the same tween exposure to the commercial and purchase level in terms of consumer learning, the processes of learning may differ for the two age groups Success: Not applicable

Correlations (product-moment) among the cri- Sponsorship: National Institute of Mental Health, terion variables for both younger and older adoles- and the Marketing Science Institute cents are "nearly all essentially zero," indicating that "several criteria of consumer learning are Published: Yes quite independent of each other Criticisms: Since theudy was not experimental, it The investigators conducted "step-up" regression isnot possible to make inferences about "direc- analyses to study differences in "consumer learning tionality" of relationships between variables, or processes" across age groups Each criterion varia- "causalityThus, the authors' claims that certain in- ble was prodicted by three sets of independent varia- dependent variables whichpredict the criterion bles (demographic, communication, and reasons for ariables are involved in the "learning process" for watching TV commercials) The authors discuss the criterion variables are not justified For exam- those independent variables which when added to ple, it is possible that consumer learning as indicated the regression equation increased the proportion of by attitudes toward TV advertising influences the variance accounted for by at least Intelligence amount of time spent watching TV, and not the was the major predictor of TV recall for older and rev erse younger subjects Two variables social utility reasons for viewing commercials and time spent I his study is subject to the usual qualifications watching TVaccount for much of the younger associatedwithobtaining data withself-ad- group variance in attitudes toward TV advertising ministered questionnaires In contrast, three different variables vicarious con- sumption reasons for viewing commercials, family Summary: This descriptivestudysuggeststhat communication about consumption, and SESac- older and younger adolescents do not differ with counted for much of the variance for older subjects respect to consumer learning (as measured by the Social utility and vicarious consumption are major tour criterion variables), but that tifactors in- predictors of materialismforboth age groups fluencing the learning processes may differ across However, the amount of money the adolescent has age groups Particularly interesting was the fact that available is a predictor for younger subjects, while several factors were better predictors of the eftects IQ was a predictor for older suhjeos 1-1,ree varia- of TV advertising on buying behavior than TV ex- bles are important predictors of the effects of [V ad posure, in tact, table 3 indicates that there is small ertismg on buying behavior for both age groups but negative relationship between TV exposure and family communication about consumption, social effects of TV advertising on buying behavior, While utility reasons for viewing IV commercials, and ex- this finding is not discussed by the Juniors, it would posure to magazinesIn addition, communicatory seem to warrant further investigati5n

Study 9

uung 'ewersI roubling Response to Ads Population: 5 12 year old children from middle T 0Bever, M LSmithifBengen, and I and working class families in northern New Jersey Johnson Harvard Hatuiett Retie tt, Nov -Dec 1975 Purpose: Ti examine trends in children's attitudes toward advertising during the vears 5-12 Sample Selection Procedure: rnspecdied 1 75 Sample Size: There were 6 boys and 6 girls in each tising in the socialization of children." The only data of four 2-year interval age groups (5-6, 7-8, 9-10, supplied by the authors are two histograms which 11-12) for a total of 48 subjects. show the average number of children in each age group who: 1) comprehend questions about Experimental Design: Subjects were interviewed morality, fantasy, and economics, and 2) are able to about morality, fantasy, economics, and TV com- respond "figuratively" and/or "operationally" to ad- mercials, and the responses were tabulated vertising. Both histograms demonstrate increases with age. Outcome Measure: Nonstandard interviews Statistical Tests: None reported (although the Success: Not applicable (no treatment). authors state in the introduction that some were per- formed). Published: Yes

Unit of Analysis: Individuals (frequencies). Sponsorship: Unspecified. Results:_ The authors report in an anecdotal man- ner the ability of different age subjects to make judg- Criticisms: This study informally presents ments about fantasy, morality, and economics and children's responses which support the authors' in- their reactions to TV advertising. Their reported terpretation of their data. The reader needs more in- results generally suggest that "children between 5 formation to fairly assess the authors' conclusions. and 12 gradually learn to interrelate their under- standing of fantasy, morality, and economics. This No statistical tests are reported in this study and integration appears to coincide with an increased the method of selecting subjects for participation is ability to deal with advertising..." Six to five year unspecified olds "largely ignored advertising as being irrel7aftt to their lives," 7-9 year olds "attempt with great While the authors cite the 7-10 year olds' "limited difficulty and little success to integrate advertising operations powers" as a major reason for their into their lives," and at age 10, "they resolve the problems in dealing with misleading advertising, conflict temporarily through an overgeneralization one of the histograms indicates that approximately thatalladvertisingismisleading." Eleven and half of 7-8 year olds were able to respond "opera- twelve year olds "resolve conflicts more satisfac- tionally" to advertising torily thus they can identify both the good and the had aspects of advertising The authors con- Summary: This descriptive study suggested that clude by statingthat "the 10 year olds' anger children's sophistication with respect to their at- towards misleading advertising as well as the 11 and titudes toward Tv advertising uicreases with age 12 year olds' increased tolerance of social hypocrisy The data which are presented in support of many of raise serious questions about the role of TV adver- the authors' contentions is informal

Study 10

Children's Telet,ision Viewing An Examination of subjects within schools were selected are unspecified Parent-Child ConsensusJohn R.Rossiter and in this article However, this sample appears tobe Thomas S Robertson, So urnetry, 1975 identical to the one used in Robertson and Rossiter (1974), in which all of the boys in four of the schools Purpose: lo compare parent and child reports with were inclu'ded, and all the boys in oneclass at each respect to TV viewing and television advertising in- grade level in the fifth school were included. The fluence, and to examine response patterns ,a relation method of selecting schools and the classes in the to children's ages and parental social class fifth school is unspecified in the 1974 article Inter- views were completed with the mothers in87 percent Population: First, third, tod fifth grade hoys from of the caes, thus eliminating 13 percent of the sub- Philadelphia area Cathoilc school, jects from this study

Sample Selection Procedure: How schools and Sample Size: N-253 mother-child dyads

171 176 Experimental Design: This study was essentially difterf ntly, parent and child resinse distributions correlational in nature Children's responses were were significantly different rall but one obtained through personal interviews at school by ,believability) of the response variablesParents trained graduate studentsParents responses were reported exciting more viewing supervision than the obtained through telephone interviews conducted by children reported expciencing In addition, parents the same interviewing team appeared to underestimate their children's commer- cial susceptability Specific Outcome Measures: Parent and child reports vbith respect to two sets of variables were ob- An analysis, of parert and child consensus by tained The first set ( exposure, viewing grade level indicated that the age variable can not supervision, co-viewing, and parent-child interac- account for the parent-child reporting discrepancy tion) assessed parentally imposed television controls Analysis of parent and child responses by parent s w:th closed-end questionsThe second set(per- education and occupation indicated that parents suasive intent recognition, liking, belie :abtiiy, report stronger social-class effects than their motivation) assessed the perceived susceptibility ot children "The added bias by social class means that children to TV commercials with closed-end ques- the sound practices of more enlightened patents may tionsforparents, and open-end questionsfor not he as prevalent as they appear" (r)21) children The response sets for all ot the variables were trichotomized either by -utilizing closed-end Success: Nut applicable questions with three categories, inspecting the total resp se distribution for each variable and dividing the responses on this basis, or by coding open-ended Sponsorship: Leo Burnett, Inc, Mattel, Kellogg, responses into three categories Nestle, Management and Behavioral Science Center of the University of Pennsylvania, and National Statistical Tests: Yes (parametric andnon- Science Foundation parametrc) Published: Yes Unit of Analysis: Inds [duals Criticisms: The principal criticismt.f this study Results: Only twootthetelevision control concerns the questionable validity ot parent and measures and two of the commercial susceptiblity child reports without observation of children's a..;- measures show significant parent-child correlations tualloving patterns, etcAlso, itis difficult to (Pearson r) The TV control variables producing a determine if parents' and children's responses are significant (positive) correlation between parents equivalentFor exampl..., when both a child and and children wereere television exposure and parent- parent report that iite child "likes" commercials (or childinteractionThe commercial susceptability report differently),itis questionable whether the variables producing significant (positive) correla- concept of "likes commercials' means the same tions were persuasiveintentrecognition and thing for children and adults believability The reported r's tor all of the ari,11,,Ls are tow (all under 18) Summary: This Lorrelat,Tia1 study suggests that parents' and children's perceptions ot TV control x2 analyses for aggregate response sun daritv in- exerted by parents' and children's susceptability to dicated thatparents and children respond commercials may dater

Study 11

Children and Commercial Persuasion An Attribl- Sion commercials and the effects ot such understand- tion Theory Analysis, T human SRobertson and ing on attitudes and purchase requests John R Rossiter Jouraal ()1 ((mstoner Research 1, June 1974 Population: First, third, and filth grade boys in five Purpose: Jo examine the extent to which children schools within, the Philadelphia area Catholic school are capable ot understanding the purposes ot Sample Selection Procedure: All ot the boys in Children who are capabre of recognizing com- four of the schools were included (census), whi'in mercials as persuasive 1) can distinguish commei - the fifth school all the boys in one class Lach grade culls from programing, 2) can recognize the exist- level were included The procedure for selecting the ence of an external source or a commercial sponsor, classes included in this latter school is unspecified 3) perceive the idea of intended audience, 4) are In addition, the procedure for selecting the tie aware of the symbolic nature of commercials, and 5' schools is unspecified Only two students did not cite' instances of negative discrepancies where tne participate product did not meet their expectations based on the commercial message (correlations between each of Sample Size: N=289 these 5 variables and recognition ot persuasive in- tent were all significant, however, all arc probably Outcome Measures: Interviewers used open-ended highly correlated with age) Stepwise discriminant questions adapted from Ward (1972) "and nrioditied weights suggested that symbolic perception was the in line with our conceptual trannework and question- primary determinant ot pet suasive intent recogni- naire pretests"thus, the measures vv ere not stand- tion Only two ot the correlations between recOgruz- ard ing assistive intent and these five variables were sig- nificantr:ognition of an external source or spon- sor and perltion of an intended audience Experimental Design: This was a descriptive study Children were interviewed using open-ended The two intentvariables (assistive and per- questions by trained graduate studentsSubject suasive) were also related to attitudes (trust, liking, responses were coded blind by three judges, : the and consumption motivation) There are several sig- few cases (less than 5 percent) where the ludges did nificant results Children holding assistive intent at- not agree were eliminated from the study Several tributions tend to trust commercials more (positive variatil:s were coded in this mannerLfor example, cori,:iations), whereas if he sees them as persuasive whether or not a child could discriminate between he tends not to like them (negative correlation) programs and commercials, or cold recognize the While abdity to recognize either type of intent is intent ot commercials Parent-child interaction was negatively related to consumption motivation, only based on child reports. and re: interaction was -the correlation between persuasr. e intent recogni- based on t:,e child's designation by other students in tion and diminished desire for advertisedproducts is the sample as a "best triend significantDis_ immant analyses indicated that persuasive intent is the dominant factor (ovLr assis- Unit of Analysis: Ind iv iduals tive intent) in predicting trust, liking. and consump- tion motivation when both tares are liffisidered to Statistical Tests: 1 es (Kendall correlation L(etti- combination cents and multivariate dis..rinimant analyses)

Results: An oven iew ot the results suggestsin- Success: Not applicable intreatment) creasingly sophisticated cognitions and less positiv e attitudinal structures" toward oiimiercials with age Sponsorship: Leo Burnett Inc ,Mattel, Kellogg, (increasing gr4de levels) Nestle, Management and Behavioral Science Center of the Universityof Pennsylvania and National Children's attribution of persuasive intentto Science Foundation commercials was significantly and positively related to age and parental education Discriminant analysis Published: Yes suggested that age was the most signiticant taLior The child's interaction level with parents. the pres- Criticisms:fAs the authors point out, the sample ence or absence of older siblings, and ILv el ot peer was limited to Catholic boys Thus the results are integration were all unrelated to the perception of not generalizable to other groups The authors men- persuasiveintent"I he onlyfactorsignificantly tion that a pilot study in a public schoo, found dia. asFaciated with attributing assistive (het Will or in- religious ditterences "tended to complicate tree formational) intent to commercials was absence of view content and measurement," v. hich suggests that older siblingsthe older or only children tend to see Lintel ences between v drums p.ligious groups Im the advertising as designed to assist measured variables may well exist

1 173 .41

I his studis subject to all 'he criticisms accom- attribution of persuasive intent, as were the cogni- panying the 4`_.e of open-ended questionnaire tech- tive variables Children who attributed persuasive niqu.s--especiallythequestionof whether the intent to commercials, tended to like them less, trust iesponse,, accurately represent reality Thy tz,e of them less, and were less likely to express a desire for three judges adds confidence to the sco. mg of the in- the products advertisedChildren who attributed terviews, although the subjects who were eliminated issistive intent tended ;like and trust the commer- due to lack of rater agreement may have been atypi- cials, but not necess, ily to expre,desire for the cal, turther limiting the generalizabiliti, of the prodactsRecognitionofpersuasiveintentwas result dominant over recognition of assistivc intent in pre ting liking, trust, and commercial motivation Summary: This descriptive studysuggested the The study's principal weakness are limited tollowing relationships Children appear to develop generalizability of the results and the weaknesses increasingly sophisticated cognitions about comn sociated with the use of a nonstandard open-ended including attribution of persua a intent, with interview formatSince the study was nonexperi- age Parent education was also positive:1/ rela'ed to mental, causal nue: ences are not justified

Study 12

s Inform on Processing of Television Ad- Experimental Design:I his was completely vertisingScott Wm I and Daniel Wackman in P descriptive Clarke. (Ed ),Newt Models for Mass. CoryzMunicanon Reieanh,Beverly HillsSage, pp 119-146 Statistical Tests: Test (X: )for intormation proc- essing data, but not for selecoon of intormation Purpose: lo examine two as:cts t,f children's in- data formation processing of TV commercialsselectiori of information a.-d cognitive processing of informa- Unit of Analysis: Indiv 'duals andindi.i(lual tion responses

Population: Children of mothers participating in r asults: Boston area serve e clubs IttlOrMatIon Pro. esssk;ofon/met( tais Most 5-8 Sample Selecilon Procedure: A rands n sample of year olds exhibited a low ley el understanding of the service clubs was initially contacted From each what a commercial is, whine ;he n ,iority of 9-12 of these clubs, "approximately" equal numbers of year olds exhibited a medium level understanding anther; of 5-12 year olds were randomly selected (significant X2 age by level of awa eness) Of 108 mothet, initially contacted, 90 agreed to par- ticipate The final sample, for which ail of the col- Most 5-8 year olds exhibited hni. level (Interco- lected data are available, numbered 67 nation between commercials and programs, whereas three-toerths of the 9-12 year olds exhibited high Sarlpilt N =6i level differentiation (significant X=) For both o: thesevariables (commercialunderstanding and Outcome Measures: Children's Jrm.i on proc- program-commercial differentiation) lowley, (-sing was investigated throug "direct interviews responses indicated a reliance -7n perceptual Luc v,ith the children The intervi transcripts were while higher level responses indicated a greater un- c Sued by two research assistan Children's selec- derstanding of the meaning of the message oon of information was exam. ed through training mothers to code their child( enz§ attentional behavior The s, mple was (' ded into three "cognitiv e while watching TV Mothers completed viewing poets" low (children who gave low 'eve' responses logs which indicated when the child was likely to be /fo both of the above two questions), medium wat 'ling TV Specific times for observanm' were (children who gave a medium or high level response sampled from these logs Eve., 10th commercia. ;e- to the commercr.i understanding question and a q.,:nce that the child watched was coded and in- highlevelresponse to the program-commercial cluded in the study. diffe.entiation ouestion), and high (the performance 1 T./ 174 characterizing tnese subjects is unspecified) These Sponsorship: National Institute of M,:otAl Health, classiLcatiorre related to the subjects' ages The Marketing Science Institute, and the American Ass( -imam of Advertising Agencies Children's awareness of the purpose of commer- cials was shov n to increase with cognitit e ley el A Published: N es similar result was demonstrated for the relationship between complexity of recall ot liked and disliked commercials and cognitive level The reasons given Criticisms: A large proportion of the subjects ini- for liking or disliking commercials differed some- tiallycontacted were not includedinthefinal vvi,at across cognitiv: levels, in particular, a thir, of analysis An attempt should have been made to low and medium level children based their feelings determine if these subjects were atypical toward the commercial on their response to the product advertised, compared to only 5 percent of is unclear if the coders of tie information proc- high level children Lower cognitive level children essing interviews wet e aware of the subjects' ages It are more likely to perceive commercials as truthful, this were the case, the ratings of subject responses judge the commercial's truthfulness on a percep- may have been unintentionally blase('Also, one ,aal (as opposed to a reality-testing) basis, and to may question the accuracy and comparability of the give trusting resprnses (not aware of the selling mo- mothers' codings of their children's viewing patterns tive) when asked why commercials do or don't tell (e g ,mothers of children of different let els may the truth has c differed iii their coding behav 'ors) Children's Selection of IntOrmationAttention to commercials decreased from the first to later com- The authors neglect to soecity the criteria for a mercials in a sequence, lower cognitit e level sublects "high cognitive level- classification showed the smallest decrease in attention frisn the program to the first commercial, while the high level Summary: Tim desci iptis e studyindicatedthat children showed the greatest differentiation between "children's information selection and processing is attention to the program and the first commercial influenced by theincogrotive development' (p 143) Low cognitive level children also demonstrated the Lower cognitive level children tended to focus on most stability in their attentional behavior toward perceptual features of commercials, while higher commercials appearing at the beginning or end ver- Jet el subjects focused on more abstraij features In cus the middle of a program, or at different \ iew ing addition, lower level ,,r.ildren were more likely to times rThildren's attention to commc,cials was re- trust commercials than higher level children The at- lated to the types of products advertised, how et er, tentional patterns of the lower level children were the low level subjects did not appear to demonstrate stable across different conditions, while the higher greater stability across different product types th, n level subjects showed more differentiation in their the higher level subjei.is attentional behavior The study's princip.al weakness is the possibility ot bias in the coiling of subject Success: Not applicable ises

Study 13

Children's Reactions to T,:levision Advertising'\n Popullon: 8 to 10 year old boys associated with a Experimental Approach M Goldberg and cecreation department in an English speaking upp-r Gorn, Journal of Consumer Research, VolI, Sept middle class suburb of Montreal 1974 Sample Selection Procedure: Unspecied Purpose: To determine the extent to which1 V commercials motivate children to try to obtain ad- vertised products Sample Size: N = 1 3 3

175 '1ti Experimenta: Design: Subjects\Acre introduced to Results:The manipulation of expectancy was sue- two new toss(ItOf WI (inthe market)Baseline LeSSfUl in that the posttest results revealed a signifi- me,,-w.s of attitudes toward each eel the two toss emu main effect A Neuman-Kayls test revealed that were ohtamed on a s punt bipolar scale (apparently the perceised chances of winning the ('C were higher not standard) On another s point hipolar scale with in the high expectancy group tnan in the other two the first to`. ) representing one pole and the other groups, however, the moderate and loss groups sse-e t\IIIVs )representingtheother,thesuhjeet- alniost identical ,peeitied which et the two toss he would rather get Prior to exposure to the commercials ( C and HA' I he I \PectancN "Penmen-talk mahlPalatedby \Acre seen as approximately equally attractive the experimenter ;idling the children that he had program was s revved as fairly interesting The punk only I(low expectant!,) (moderate expectancy was pserceised asery hard and hurls interesting or 14 (high expeeiahes ) s for the 15 boss in each 1 here vtere nti signiticfint differences among treat- group Subjects were told that to win the (0), they ment levels on any ot these measures would has e to sore a pestleIt they were the first, wil,flig the first 8, of the first 14 boys (depending on two -\\ ,yANOA.A's were performed onat- lexcl of exNetanes \+'')-1id win the .osIt they titudinal and hchas untildatato determine the quit heroic the \sinners in their group solved the effects of expectancy and commercial exposure pu/tle :hey would reeeise HW s but it they were still Analyses with time spent on the puzile hefore quit- kiin!, atter the winners soled the pritile they ting (persistance measure) were meaningful for those would win nothingMtwas chosen as the alterna- su..nects who could not sole e the task In 122 Thus, ,ise pate because pilot tests ( eported ) indicated the subjects solving the task in-1!) were excluded that the children had a slight preterenee for the CC froma!!analysisThe a.uthors performeda ( fuleiren were asked toi their perceised ehonees logarithmic transformation to normalite the tunit \sinning on a S)oint scale (croups of children were task data AN( ()VA's on the attitudinal data were subsequently shoy.n a program with either 0, 1, or 3 performed, cos drying on preprogram scoresthe crimmere ialsinsertedAtterthe program cos ariance analysis was essential since there were children rated their attitudes toward the program on su' .t antral reprogram differences on the at- a 5-point scalyI he preprogram measures of the titudinal measures (despite randodl assignment) desnabilit, ot the to\ s the perceised probability The two attitudinal measures were attitude toward of winning (I were readministered Children were the CC and comparati\ e attitude ICC ts HW) then reminded of the number of toys as ailahle and I here was a significant main effect of expectancy' the pei eels ed probability or winning was measured a third tine Children then worked in separate euhi- on both attitude measure In both cases, the( nly sig- nificant difference occurred between subjects in the cies on a difficult experimental put/le Vs hen a child deerded to stop he lett the euhiele, and the other high and moderate expectancy groups (the attitudes tor low expectancy groups tell in between) Thus. children were un,mare of his departureF. noted the high expectancy of receiving the toenhanced its time each child worked On the puttle Finally sub alue in comparison to moderate expectancy There beets rated the task's interest and dittyrIty on two 5- was also a srgnificant expectancy effect on the point scales, and were given a set e t W' p,)r amount of time spent working on the putzle High tiopating expectancy subtect, worked significantly longer than those in the loss expectancy group (with moderate ):1 summary, the design was a 3x3 raetorial (3 expectancyinthe middle)I hus, persistance les els of c())nniercial exposure and 3 les els (4 expce- hehas lot was related to exr ctagc! taaey ) with approximately 15 subjects cellSubjects werer indonils ass..c.tnedto eelsthe dependent ommercial exposure measures Acne time Tent oft the task and the at- There was asignificant effect on the comparative attitude measure but not titudinal data on the attitude toward the CC measure The in - crea5es iron] 0 to noth I and 1 exposure, on coin- Unit of Analysis: 11-1,11ideals par, ive measure were significant. but there `.as no differeru e hetwetn the means in the I and 3 ex- Statistical Tests: Nes. AMA' A, AN( r tS A. and posure groups The itumher of commercials also sig- post hoe c.)mparisons nificantly e`fected time worked at the tasks to win the CC (Ink the difference between o and I ex Criticisms: The experimental manipulation of ex- posui es was significant, with I Limn] pectancy was onlypartially suceesstul, while the in increased time worked at the task (I he difterence high expectancy group differed sigmlw.antly trom between theand 3 conditions was -lus' below the c the moderate and low expectancy groups these lat- percent loci of sign ihs:once '') ter two groups had similar perceived chances of win- ning (expectant:\ 1Thus, it may have been more ap- I herewere nosignificantinteract neffects propriate to combine these two groups for the pur- (commercial exposure x expectancy )I hus, children poses of analysis, since differences between these two with low exneetancs0{- nhtai ning a "tos_,..arestill groups on the dependent eanah;es would appear not affected hs commercials Alternane el the com- to he caused by expectancy difterences mercials did not create enough desire for a to\ to eliminate the differential affects tit expectance The -exclusion of the IIsubjects who actually sole ed the puzzle may indicate that the brightest stu- Success: Although sonic suhhypotheses were not dents were elim mat ed, thus, limiting the generaliza- supported re g, the authors predicted an interaction tion of the findings In addition, excluding these sub: Meet, and the pied mid posinee relationship be- wets may have contributed to preprogram attitude tween expectancy and attitude toward the product differences between groups despite random assign- was only partially supported), the results gem:rail\ ment supported the hypotheses that expectancy and cc - merLial.expnsure atteL.1 attit des and hehas tor in the Summary: this studs ssas well executed Subleets predicted directionsI hus the studs should he cin- were randomly assigned i treatment combinations, sidered sui-sstul and the data analysis appeared careful and ,,ppropri- ate The results suggest that commercial exposure to a to\ and expectancy of receleing the toinfluence Sponsorship: ( ,ffi,t(1,1 ( (Irani the'- ,totude toward the toy (in comparison to another to and his hehas ior in attempting to obtain Published: 1 es it

Study 14 liftects 1 cies ision Ads 'sing on Childrc..t rest Sample Size: Nsot) Neart xpei.inentarEeic,cnce, Charles\t .in, Report Ade erasing and Children Project, Experimental Design: Subjects were exposed to Him'Report,Department of Communications, one of the eight 20- minute stimulus tapes containing Michigan State [me ersny, June 19 entertainment mats:F.1W ads erosing, and news here were nine experimental hypotheses being Purpose: To es amme the impat..1 OAP, ads ei tested The basic design tot investigating each of the (using prActices on the knowledgeattitudesand pin hof-s invoked comparing half of the total suh- hehas u,r tri'ung children mos csposito one eirsion of the stimulus and the r.her halt e cc% tng on alternate version (while there Population: I-- lementary and presLti-,11 duldr(, were eight stimulus tapes, there were not eight different E treatments the Lansing area rather, there v re nine hy- potheses for Alneli there were two conditions)

Sample Selection Procedure:. crientars schools and two preschools 'e ere caret ulle selected I he stimulus,t apes were played to groups of tour to provide a substantial prortrtion hlack stu- children at a timeItis unLleat 'loss these groups dents Thusthey- were not rondoml \selected 'sere iormed One of the eight stimulus tapes was -Most studentsineach-cicoolparticipated andiunly selected for slit tee ing to each group One (parent s permission fiquired) It hile the saniplt n halt of the subjects responded to postvtcw mg int,1- eluded sultieLts of all SES lock, it was"purposie ele 1C1s,.while the one, halt underwent a product skewed to oeerrepresent children ti,m less as'..in selecting procedure addition, one-halt of all sub- taged backgrounds jects were monitored tor responses while stewing

177 1 he method for assigning subjects to these condi- (5) MEDICINE ADVERTISINGChildren tions is unspecthed who viewed a r`ristan commercial more often inch- catcd that they would take medicine for a cold, Outcome Measures: Coders rated the attentional thought pills were more effective, and perceived and affective responses of halt of the subjects 'tering higher levels of dines- in society, compared to those the tapesAttention was rated on a 5-point scale who did not see the ad (amount of eye contact) Degt of enjoyment and irritation wen: rated on 3-step scales (high, moder- (6) DISCLAIMERSA toy commercial with an ate, low) Verbalizations were codedaccording to Ludy] as well as a video superimposed disclaimer of topic and valence Change in interest was rated on a noninc.uded batteries produced greater awareness 3-point scale (increase, no change, decrease) of this qualification but created less product desire than a video-tidy' disclaimer After viewing the tapes, halt' of the subjects re- sponded to' nonstandard interviews The other halt (7) CLUSTERED I/8 DISPERSED STRUC- participated in a product selection and play condi- TURE OF PRESENTATIONSIightly'greater tionBehavioral and verbal preferences, expecta- levels of commernialsttention, enjoyment, learning, tions, and aggressive behaviors were re,cied by an and desire were obtained when commercials were assistant hunched together rather than conventionally dis- persed Statistical Tests: es (t-tests, X2) Success: Itis difficult to determine it the treat- Results: Mks study of the impact of variousAver- ments were generally successtul, since a large rum In general, attention thing practices on the knowledge,attitudes, and her of hypotheses were tested to and learning from commercials were rtrongly re- hehas or of young children reports a large number lated to the age ot the subjects, the effects of the of findings Some are selected here various treatments or 'eat rung and other behaviors ,rated, and lepends strongly on the specific aspect (I) : MIUM OFFFR STR EC.Or Children of the prodill which is ')eing considered more often desired a breakfastfood when the com- mercial teaturt'I a premium tothan when a pre- mium was r meutioned however, they were not Criticisms: The method of assigning subjects to the more likely to anticipate asking t ,eirmother to buy v minus measurement conditions is unclearIn addi- it for them tion, while stimulus tapes were randomly assigned to groups in four subjects, the method ot assigningsub- (2) PROGRAM CHARACTERSAPPEARING jects to groups is un tear IN COMMERCIALSThose viewing a Flintstones cereal ad more often desired the productwhen the Ratings of subject responses (e g , degree of en- ad was shown in the context of a Flintstones cartoon joyment) were based on observer judgments It is oh- than in a Bugs Bunny cartoon, this did not seem to ious that these ,atings may not accurately represent be due to contusing the commercial with the content thesublect's underlying condition Inaddition, of the program, but rather a heightened identifica- coder expectations may have unconsciously biased tion with the program characters in thecommercial the results

(3) RA TONAL MESSAGE STRAllG \rA -This study focused short-term effects (nI V rationalvitan in-orierstedcerealad was readily advertisingItwould he ,nfortnahve to conduct learned and equ illy successful in terms of recalland Itniger term studies to supplement these findings desire, compared to a standard emotional presenta- tion of the cereal The authors tested the significance of their find- ings with multiple ttests, thus increasing the proba- (4) LEARNING FROM PUBLICSERy ICS bility of a ditte retie e appearing due to chan,:e ANNiOUNCEMEN rs-Th,,,,, who viewed an anti- .ng PSA less often exh:hitedlittering hehas tor Published: No afterwards, compared to those k h ) did not seethis ad Sponsorship: ott,Lc of (hifd f)ey elopm nt I Summary: This study tested severalhypotheses depended on the specific hypothesis beingcon- concerning theeffectsof TV commercials on sidered. The study's major weaknesses are failure to children's learning and other behaviors Learning specify how subjects were assigned to groups and and attention were strongly related to the age of the measurement conditons, and the questionable subjects, while the effects of the various treatment validity of coder's ratings of subject behaviors manipulations on subject behaviors were mixed and

Study 15

Effects of Television Advertising on Children ary eyeglass advertisement was varied one group Second Year Experimental EvidenceCharles saw her dressed as a s. tun judge, another as a com- Atkin, Report #2, TV Advertising and Children puter programer, a third group saw her as a techni- Project, June 175 cian repairing TV sets, and the control group saw no eyeglass commercial Ss who were exposed to a par- Purpose: To test children's intentional and inciden- ticular occupational model were more likely to tal learning from television ,:ommercials select that occupation as appropriate for women.

Population: Elementarys, hoolstudents (grades (2) RECREATKA AL SEX ROLESTis. sex of 2-5) in Lansing and East Lansing, Michigan children shown playing with a traditionally male- oriented racing car set was varied one group saw Sample Selection Procedure: While the author two girls playing with t1te racing cars and the other states that the schools represented "varying socio- group saw two boys. Not all Ss perceived the sex of economic neighborhoods," how these schools were the m9dels, but those Ss who did perceive the actors selected is unspecified "Almost every student" in to be girls were far more likely to tees that girls the selected grades in each school participated in the should appropriately play with racing cars and were experiment (with parents' permission) slightly more desirous ot playing with the tcy them- selves Sample Size: N =400 (3) LEARNING FROM PUBLIC SERVICE Experimental Design: Subjects viewed one of four ANNOUNCEMENTSOne group viewed a half- versions or a15-minute videotape containing minute cartoon message emphasising that sugar con- children'sitews, entertainment, and advertising sumption produces cavities and may eventually contentThe content ot the commercials was cause teeth 'o fall rut, the control group did not see manipulated across the tour experimental tapes. to this PSA Exposed subjects tended to believe that allow the testing of nine experimental questions The sugar causes cavities and makes teeth fall out, were students from each classroom were randomly more worried about getting cavities, and more often assigned to two groups One of the four stimulus telt that sugar was not goad for them Viewers were tapes was randomly selected for showing to each slightly more likely to say that theyould eat less group Atter viewing the tape, subjects responded to sugar in the future a 10-page multiple choice questiannaire (4) MEDICINE USAGEThe verbal script of a '.----- utcome Measure: Ten page multiple-choice standard headache remedy commercial was altered q istionnaire not standard) to emphasize moderation in usage Half heard con- ventional claims of speedy headache relief, while the Statistical Tests: Yes (X2 and .:orrelationsi others also heard the qualification that the meiicine should he taken only "when you really need it" and Unit of Analysis: Individual that one shouldn't ta'ce "too many" pills for a headache. The qualified ad was lust as effective in Results: Many findings are reported, some are terms of and acceptance of relief selected here Maims There was a slight tendency for Ss hear..tg the qualified message to say that people shouldn t (I) OCCUPATIONA!. SEX ROLESThe oc- take pills for mild headaches and that they would cupational ro'e portrayed by a woman in an ordin- personally take less pills tot headache relief

179 1 (5) HERO-FIGURE ENDORSEMENTIn a Success: Depends upon specific hiothesis being commercial promoting a cookie product, the spokes- considered man was dressed either in ordinary streetcrothes or in an astronaut uniform The group viewing the or- Sponsorship: Office ofhilcl Development dinary spokesman were slight's/ more likely to want to cat the cookies than Ss exposed to the astronaut Published: N. Vero figure, there was no difference in the intention to, ask parents to purchase the product Criticisms: As always, the validity of results ob- tained with nonstandard multiple-choice question- ( 6) COMPARATIVE MESSAGE STRATEGYOne version of a chocolate bar com- naires is open to question For this reason. the inclu- mercial emp'oyed a conventional "one-sided- sion of behavioral indices would seem advisable strategy of desdibing only positive attributes of the product, while a "two-sided- vcrsion included com- The authors do not state the exact number et sub- parisons with a competing brand along ditnesions of jects at each age level viewing the &Herm ta:Je ver- size and nutrition Ss in the two-sided condition were sions somewhat more likely to learn about the two at- tributes of the advertised product. Although many This study focused only on short-tel.ir effects two-sided subjects mistakenly thought that they had Also, the experimental setting clack of diversions in seen an ad sponsored by the competing brand, they experimental room) may have artificall heightened were no more likely to prefer the competitor. Ss attention to commercials receiving the oneded message liked each candy brand about as well as the two-sided Ss Summary: This study used a partially randomized (7) MESSAGE REPETITIONOne group saw a design in investigating nine experimental hypotheses blemish-cream commercial once and half savit pre- concerning theettects of TV advertising on sented twice several minutes apart on the tape children's sex-role attitudes and personal health, Those in the double exposure condition were more practices, and the persuasive impact ct certain ad- likely to remember the brand name of the product vertising message strategies and source attributes and to worry about blemishes than those exposed The results differ according to the specific hy- once, but it did not express any greater liking to:the pothesis under considerationTire study's major product, intention to buy it. or belief in effective- weaknesses are questionable validity of the outcome ness measure, ari,", tne artificiality of the test situation

Study 16

Effects of Television Advertising on Children Michigan,- the method used to select these schools is Survey of Pre-Adolescent's Responses to Television unspecifiedItis unclear. if all available subjects Commercials Charles Atkin Report #6 TV Ad- within the chosen schools participated in the study vertising and Children Project, Department of Com- munications, Michigan State University, July 1975 Sample Size: N =775

Purpose: To describe patterns of advertising ex- Experimental Design: This study was purelk posure and evaluation its the naturalistic setting and descriptive An "omnibus survey instrument- con- to examen' the role of commercials in late childhood taining multiple choice and open-ended questions socialization. designed to measure children's responses to TV ad- vertising was administered to all subiects In addi- Population: Fourth through seventh graders from tion, each questionnaire eontaine0 a supplementary Michigan schools set of items 256 filth, sixth, and seventh grade stu- dents responded to a supplement ecntaming ques- Sample Selection Procedure: While the author tions about medicine, while 506 children (fourth- states that subvis,ts came trom "a number of schools seventh grades) received a food and nutrition sup- :r. urban,s, , urban, and smalitown areas of plementItis unclear how the supplements were 1S1 assigned to subject (but assignment apparently was (5) CEREAL ADVERTISINGChildren wno not random) watch the most cereal ads on Saturday television ire much more likely to ask parents to buy cereale and OUtCOMS Wafture: Nonstandard questionnaire to eat advertised brands, those from families with no snack rules are most strongly affect ed Arguing with Statistical Tests: Yes (correlation coefficients). parents and becoming angry when requests are denied are mediated by increased request frequency. Unit of Analysis: Individual Advertising does not significantly affect beliefs of th; value of sugar or the incidence of tooth cavities Results: Selected findings are' (6) CANDY ADVERTISINGAdvertising has (I) ADVERTISING EXPOSUREPreadoles- a modest impact on children's eating of advertised cence is a period of heavy television consumption, candy brands and quantity of candy bars consumed. with respondents reporting more than 2 hours of There are negligible effects on beliefs about sugar prime time viewing each evening They still view and development of cavities many Saturday morning programs (particularly fourth and fifth graders) and also watch teen - (7) ADVERTISING AND NUTRITION oriented music programs. These viewing patterns in- Children most exposedto informationalcereal dicate that youngsters encounter a large number of messages stressing nutritional breakfast habits tend commercials for a wide variety of product types to recognize the importance of eating a good break- Across 26 specific ads, children report being fast and to give higher nutritional ratings for the moderately attentive when commercials appear. cereal,toast,and orange Juice foods thatare emphasized in these ads. (2) E"ALUATION OF ADVERTISINGMost children report being irritated by commercial inter- &ICONS: Not applftabie, since there was no treat- ruptions, the sample is divided on the question of ment. banning Sarday n-irning commercials, with younger chilen and those who are highly irritated tending to Ivor removal. Preadolescents are Sponsorship: Office of Child Development generally skeptical of the trnstworthmess of TV ads, less than one-fourth think that commercials always Published: No WI the truth. Childrei. who disbelieve commercials tend to disbelieve authority figures such as adults Criticisms: The study's major weakness is its total and salesmen, but attention and liking of ads are not reliance on survey research, the degree to which related to either form of distrust. questionnaire responses represent the subjects' true behavior is oren to question The author feels that (3) ADVERTISING AND HYGIENE There "the field setting allows more confident generaliza- are substantial positive associations bet'veen ex- tion of the findings to the re 'world in which the posure to dieodoranti,douthwash/acne cream com- children liveThis study underscores the recurrent mercials and worry about personal hygiene, using problem of hoosing between controlled (and possi- hygiene products, perceiving the importance akid bly artificial) experimental conditions and surveys societal usage of these 1...oducts, and believing that the products work effectively The author includes specific commercials and TV shows in their indicesIt is unclear how they were (4) MEDICINE ADVERTISING Exposure to chosen, and it may he that these specific choices in- ads for headacheistomach ache/sleeplessness fluenced the outcome, (e gin assessing reported at- remedies is moderately related to children's percep- tention to medico-- commercials, a subject may at- tions that people often become illand rely on tend more closely to those included in the question- medicine, and to their personal concern about get- naire than to medicinecoMmercials in general) In ting sick Personal usage 8 Id approval of medicine is addition, m comparing some indices the author used only weakly affected by advertising There is no evi- a "multiplicative technique" which combined degree dence thatads contribute topositiveattitudes of attention and frequency of viewingIt is unclear toward illicit drugs why these relationships are multiplicative

181 1S5

111110111W_ Study 17

TV Drug Advertising and Proprietary and Illicit Results: Drug advertising exposure and proprie- Drug Use Among Teenage Boys.1 R. Milaysky, B tary drug use: Pekowsky, and H Stipp. Public Opinion Quarterly, There is a small but significant relationship sub- Winter 1975-76 jects with high exposure use more proprietary drugs than those with lower exposure Purpose: To empirically test the charge that drug advertising may be related to use of proprietary and illicit drugs Drug advertising exposure and illicit drug use A negative relationship was found between exposure Population: 7th, 8th and 9th grade boys in low and and drug use. the higher the TV advertising ex- middle SES midwestern schools The precise loca- posure, the less are illicit drugs used. This relation- tion of these schools is unspecified ship remained negatwe even when the amount of total TV exposure was partialed out, ar.-I was nega- Sample Selection Procedure: Schools were ap- tive or not significant when low, medium, and high parently selected in an attempt to obtain a sample viewers were considered separately The authors "balanced with respect to SES and grade and, among "searched" for subgroups in which the relationship the low-SES boys, balanced by race." Target boys between drug advertising exposure and illicit drug were randomly selected from enrollment lists. The use was positive by controlling for many variables, target boys were then asked for names of up to eight such as age, race, IQ, SES, etc. No subgroups were neighborhood friends, who were also recruited Ad- found in which the relationship was positive. The ditional subjects (friends and subjects participating negative relationship appeared in part to be a result in a separate study) were recruited in later phases of t- the fact that eventual users of illicit drugs watch the study less TV (and experience less exposure to drug adver- tising) before experimentation with drugs begins. Sampl*SIze: 822 beiys were present for at least one 'hese results suggest that "television is not a factor phase (wave) of the study leading directly to illicit drug use

Experimental Design: The subjects wereinter- Indirect links between drug ad- i:rtising exposure viewed with questionnaires five separate times over and illicit drug useSubjects' use of proprietary \a period of 31/z years (May 1970-Dec. 1973), while drugs was not related to use of illicit drugs Subjects' their parents were interviewed twice The subject readiness to take proprietary drugs was signiftr:_'_ntly questionnaires tapped the subjects' proprietary and related to their use of illicit drugs: however, 0 ere illicit drug use and drug advertising exposure (note- was no relationship between exposure to drug aut er- the authors followed an elaborate orocedure for tising and the subjects' readiness to take proprietar} computing exposure to drug commercials from sub- drugsthus, TV was not indirectly related to use of jects' reports of TV exposure The exposure measure illicit drugs tapped the amount of drug advertising the subject could have watched ). The authors made special Published: Yes efforts to insure that their measures were valid, and they randomly selected TV shows for inclusion in theinitial (Wave I) questionnaire The question- Success: Not applicabif naires differed across the different measurement times ./ Sponsorship: National Broadcasting Company

Outcome Measure:'onstandard queviormaires Criticisms: There were differences in the descrip- tions of some drugs and the number of drugs listed Unit of Analysis: Individuals between the !ast administered and the earlier ques- tionnaires A., the authors point out, 0 eretore, a cer- Statistical Tests: Yes (nonparametric Tau since, tam amount of caution should be exercised in ir- data is only ordinal scale) terpreting changes over time

1S3 .I. 0 IL) A lair percent of subjects responded to euestions Summary:I his correlational study suggests that about nonexistent "dummy" shows, or reported their exposure to TV drug advertising does not directly or drug use inconsistently While the authors took pre- indirectly lead teenage boys to take illicit thugs "On cautions to insure that these errors did not bias the the contrary, the data indicate that itis the lighter results, this fact is indicative of the questionable viewer of drug advertising on TV who is more likely validity of survey research to use illicit drugsDrug advertising exposure was positively related to proprietary di ug use Since the It is difficult to determine why the N's for the study was not experimental, causal inferences are different analyses nary unwarranted.

Study 18

An Exploratory Analysis of the Ettct of They ision 4 Most mothers feelthat TV causes their Advertising on ChildrenHoward, Hulbert, and children to ask them to buy things Lehmann (unpublished) 5 Mothers had mixed opinions as to their feel- Purpose: To determine how children between the ings about the value. of watching TV ads for ages of 2 and 6 responded to teleyksion advertising their children

Population: "Relatively well -to-do" 2 to 6 year old children from rural Nev. Hampshire and urban New Unit of Analysis: Individuals, orindividual Jersey. responses, frequencies

Sample Selection Procedure: Unspecified Criticisms: This study was entirely descriptive The authors make several statements (rot listed above) Sample Size: N =96 wnich are not based on data presented

Outcome Measure: Nonstandard interview I here is a y high incidence of "no response," which weakensiese findings Experimental Design: Children and mothers were interviewed The findings of tills study would be interpreted cautiously e g, the fact that children did not men- Statistical Tests: No (frequencies arc presented) tion advertising as an important reason for buying products does not indicate that it has no effect on Results: The authors collected data on alarge purchase requestsAdvertising may influence the number of questions thus, interesting findings will other reasons listed (e g, taste, special feature) he highlighted

Published: No 1 Ch.ldren almost never mentioned advertising as the important reason for busing toys or cereals Success: Not applicable

2 TV seems to he the most important source of Sponsorship: Faculty Research Fund of the Grad- new-product information for cereals and toys uate School of Business, Columbia Universdy

3 Most mothers feel that children distinguish be- tween cor,imercials and programs, and most Summary: This purely descriptive study presents claim to discuss TV-advertised products with several findings concerning TV advertising, their children children, and mothers (See results section above )

1 ,' ,es4 Study 19

Snort-Run Advertising Effects on Children A Field Outcome Measure: Survey (not standard) Study* Thomas S Robertson and John R Rossiter Journal of Marketing Research, VolXIII (Feb Results: Overall toy and game preferencesin- 1976), pp 68-70 creased slightly from 45 percent to 48 percent dur- ing the 4-week measurement period The preptrit Purpose: To investigate the effects of TV advertis- choice changes were statistically significant ingonchildren'stoy and game choicesin a ',ANOVA) The authors state that the increase in toy "naturalistic setting and game choices may be conservative since the ad- vertising campaign had already begun by the time of Population: First, third, and fifth grade boys in the first measurement In addition, the mean number Philadelphia area schools of toy and game category requests to parents in- creased from 2 2 to 2 7 during the measurement Sample Selection Procedure: The procedure for period The correlation between TV as an informa- selecting schools and subjects within schoolsis tion source and children's toy and ganochoices was unspecifiedThe study was conductedinfive 24 (p 001), larger thirifor several other sources "diverse- Philadelphia area schoolsto provide a wide range of socioeconomic backgrounds", thus, it Sponsorship: National Science Foundation, Leo appears likely that the schools were not randomly Burnett, Inc,Mattel, Kellogg, Nestle selected Published: Yes Sample Size: N=289 Success: Not applicable Experimental Design: A two-wave survey was ad- .ministered in which children were asked to nominate Criticisms: The authorsfeelthatthisstudy their five most strongly preterred Christmas present demonstrates the short-term effects of TV advertis- choicesat two time periods5 weeks before ing on children's toy and game choices and purchase Christmas and Iweek before Christmas Children requests However, the study sutfers from several were also asked how often they had actually re- weaknessesFirst, the two different measurement quested each item from their parents, and where they times were supposed to reflect prepeak and post- had seen or heard about each item mentioned peak TV toy . advertising efforts. However, no foi-ingti the differences in percentage of Children's item ctoices were ass:ened to one of cc inrcial.,'ea;ing with toys and games at the four categories. (1) toys and games, (2) educatio times tic me.sv :rid between measurements is and craft items, (3) I,nsure and personal items, and providedSezonu, the;.; re many other factors (4) sports equipment The authors then traced brand beside TV overusing my have influenced name itemsin TV advertisinglogs. this effort e inc ease in toy and game choices and purchase revealed that (-Ay the toy and game group received is(e g,increased converst.tion about "substantial" T V advertising support, while items in Christ s by r...ers and family, increased awareness the other three groups were advertised only "occa- that rnuests for toys were likeL, to be received sionallyOn this basis, the authors conclude that favorably, storefront displays, etc ). The authors' "any change in the proportion of toy and game re- assumption that "the major difference in product in- quests (versus requests for items in the other catego- formation input over the survey period was televi- ries) from the first wave, which preceded the peak of sion advertising, which was concentrated on the toy TV advertising for toys and gaiiies, to the second and game category,- is not supported with data wave, which followed the peak of TV advertising tot The increase in toy and game requests was modes toys ant, games, could be taken as prima facie evi- -s'and not tested statistically dence for TV advertising effects Summary: This study suggests that pre-Christmas Unit of Analysis: Individual TV advertising increased children's toy and game choices and purchase requests However, these find- Statistical Tests: Yes (ANOVi-t, Kendall's tau) ings must he viewed with caution

185 1 Study 20

Children's TV Commercials Testing the Defenses Results:Predictors of children's cognitive and at- John R Rossiter and Thomas S Robertson Journal titudinal defenses to television advertising Matura- of Cor,munuation, Vol 24, Autumn 1974 tional development (age and grade) is the most sig- nificant detei minant of children's cognitive and at- Purpose:To investigate "persuasion processes" in titudinal defenses to television advertising (matura- children's susceptibility to television advertising tion results in a greater understanding of and a more defensivF attitude toward commercials), The other Population:Primary schoolboys (1st,3d, th correlations which follow were computed with age grade) frcn i'hiladelphia Catholic schools (same as and grade partialled ou. other articles by these authors) TV exposure was unrelated to cognitive defense

Sample Selection Procedure:The area V.#as level, but was significa-tly related to the child's at- selected in an effort to provide a "broad social class titude toward commercials (heavy viewers are more profile." Selection of schools and subjects within favorably disposed toward TV advertising) schools is unspecified Children of better Mt. ated parents exhibited Sample Size;N=289 stronger cognitive and attitudinal defenses to com- mercials, although parent-child interaction was not Outcome Measure:Open-ended interviews (not a significant variable Children with no older sib- standard) were used to measure each child's level of lings were found to belore cognit.vely sophisti- understanding of commercials (cognition) and his catedPeers do not pia,a significant role in the associated belief, affect, and motivational disposi- child's acquisition of defenses to advertising tion with respect to them (attitude)

Commercial-instigated choice behavior was CogniAve and attitudinal defense effectiveness measured as follows child; en listed Christmas pres- over the peak toy and game television advertising ent selection-in early November and againin period (the correlations between children's cognitive December, The measure was the proportion of toy and attitudinal defenses and their preference levels and game requests (for which there was concentrated for television advertised toys and games. both before TV advertising) in relation to total requests (See and after the peak of the toy and game TV ad period, Study 19 ) were computed separately by grade level)

Experimental Design:This study ispart of the Children's cognitive and attitudinal defenses are 1976 study (Study 19) which investigated the effects strongest at the beginning of the peak ad period, but of pre-Christmasay and game advertisingThis are "neutralized" atits conclusion (children with study focuses on cognitive and attitudinal defenses strong defenses to commercials selected fewer TV with respect to commercialsCognitive defenses promoted toys and games on the premeasure than were indicated by the child's level of understanding children with weaker defenses, however, on the post- of commercials, while attitudinal defenses were indi- measure, defenses are generally ineffective predic- cated by his beliefs, affect, ar 1 motivational disposi- tors of preference). This is a result of increased TV tion toward commercialsChildren were inter- item preference among children with the initially viewed, and the interview protocols were scored strongest defenses The cognitive defenses of the 5th anonymously with respect to the child's school and graders appeared to be most resistant to advertising grade level by three independent coders into response dimensions developed a priori Cognitive There is a shift in importance from attitudinal and attitudinal defenses were related to a number of defense to cognitive defense with increasing grade factors level (cognit ve defenses are minimally important for1st grade. as are attitudinal defenses for 5th Statistical Twits:Yes (Kendall's tau) graders)

Unit of Analysis:Individual Success:Not applicable

186 Sponsorship: Leo Burnett. Inc., Mattel, Kellogg, Summary: This correlational study suggests that Nestle, National Science Foundation, maturation is the most signiticant determinantof children's detenses to TV commercials, TV ex- Published: Yes posure was significantly related toattitudinal detenses to commercials There is some evidence the con- Criticisms: The authors attribute effects to that detenses were effective at the prepeak measure- centrated toy and game TV advertisingduring the ment time, but were less effective at postpeak,the do not pre-Christmas weeks However, the authors cognitive defenses of 5th graders were most resistant such adver- provide information about the level ot to to effects of concentrated advertising. There was tising at the two measurement timesIn addition, a shift from attitudinal to cognitivedefenses with there were no controls to insure thatTV advertising, age The study lacks sufficient controls toconfi- responsible for the and not some other factor, was dently attribute deterioration of defenses to concen- "neutralization" of cognitive andattitudinal trated TV advertising defenses with respect to TV advertised toysand games

Study 21

Validation of Mother-Child Purchase Influence Results: The authors use Campbell and Fisher's Frequency Reports by the Multitrait- Mult'method (1959) multitran-multimethod matrix approach to Matrix. Ronald Faber and Scott Ward. Technical test convergent and discriminant validity of Report, Marketing Science Institute, April 1975. mothers' and children's ratings of children's purchase requests The data support the existence of Purpose: To assessthevalidityotdata from both types of validity mothers and children concerning the frequency of children's attempts at purchase influence When the data was analyzed by children's age groups, the most highly correlatedreports of Population: Mother and child (kindergarten, 1st, purchase influence attempts appeared to he those of and 3d graders) pairs in the Boston and Minneapolis kindergarteners and their mothers metropolitan areas

Sample Selection Procedure: Unspecified Convergent validity existedfor allthree age tips (mother's and clutch en's reportscorrelate Sample Size: N =615 highly) However, data for the younger children in- cheated, considerably less discriminant validity than Outcome Measures: Children indicated on a 4- the data for older children point scale how often they attempt to influence their mothers to buy each of different products Mothers responded to the same scale as to their per- Summary: This article simply tests the validity of ceptions of their children's purchase requests child and mother reports concerning children's purchase influence attempts. The results indicate Experimental Design: Interviews were conducted convergent and discriminant validity, although dis- with each mott. and child separately criminant validity is weaker for young children Appendix B

RESEARCH RESOURV ROSTER ON TELEVISION ADVERTISING AND CHILDREN

As a segment of the comprehensive project to A coin pirhemive ettort sAas made to disseminate study the effects of TV advertising to children, the the InfOrmatIon Nsidely regarding the rosterAn- National Research Roster vvas compiled with tour nouncements of the organuation of the roster were minor objectives in mind sent to 156 protessional journals, newsletters, and other publications in numerous disciplines and oc- (I) to identity professionals who have qualdica- cupational specialt.associated s ith child develop- tions or have demonstrated interest in con- ment, mass communications, marketing and , her- ducting research studie,i in the area of child using, education, childcare, and socialpolicy development and television advertising studv-s Also, an ex_ensive literature rev irvv was con- (2) to sample the breachand depth of the na- ducted to canvass names of investigators and com- tional research resources currently available mentators active in this held Roster questionnaires forrelevantstudiesinterms ofspecial %Acre sent to more than 6110 prospective candidates populations, facilities, research modes, and professional orientation of investigators At the tune the main roster closed, 348 completed (3) to provide investigators a means for making questionnaires had been rec.cied from members of theirinterestandavailability knownto numerous professional sr_IaltiesA statistical prospective research-sponsoring agencies profile of the respondents and their research (4) to solicit investigator's vievvs or. Important s( °claIties tollovvsThe complete roster of areas for nevv research researchers has been issued separately

NATIONAL SCIENCE FOUNDATION RESEARCH RE- JURCE ROSTER ON TELEVISION ADVERTISING AND CHILDREN

Summary Summary

Respondents; 333 n Professional Discipline-

Psychology 20 36% Professional Status. Mass Communication 93 28% PhD 230 69°. Education 30 9' MS orMA 53 16's Marketing 26 8% Ed D 17 5% Sociology 17 5% BS orBA 10 3% Educational Research 7 2% MD 3 1% Librarianship 7 2% MBA 3 130 Advertising 3 1% Others 10 3% Others 30 9% Not reported 7 2% Research Specialty-

Social effects of mass c-mm 67 20% Professions; Affillatior Audience research 50 15% Colieges of universities 246 74% Cognition 43 13% Business organizations 57 17% Consumer research 23 7% Schools 10 3% Verbal learning processes 17 5% None 7 2% Survey research 13 4% Libraries 7 Information processing 10 3% Radio/TV Stations 3 1 '0 Program analysis and evaluation 10 3% Others 3 1', Others 100 30%

18') Summary Summary

Research Facilities n New Project Lead Time: n

a Population-Age 1 month 103 31% 3 months 100 30% 9 -12 yrs 246 74% None 60 18% 5 - 8 yrs 243 73% Varies 27 8% Adolescents 223 67% 6 months 17 5% Adults 210 63% Longer than 6 months 3 1% Intent - 4 yrs 150 45% Not reported 23 7% Minorities I 137 41% Women 117 35% Open Dissemination of Findings: Elderly 117 35% Yes Poor 103 31% 260 18% Maybe 43 13% Men 9O 27% No 17 5% Not reported b Population-Source 13 4%

Elementary schools 226 68% Findings: -Colleges, universities 220 66% In the published literature 196 59% Junior high schools 180 54% Upon request only 77 23% High schools 186 53% Not available, confidential 23 7% Preschools 173 52% Not reported 37 11% Day care centers I67 50% Private homes 137 41% Length of Professional Service: Community centers 97 29% Libraries 97 29% Since 197 180 54% Business organizations 73 22% Since 1965 77 23% Private clubs 60 18% Since 1960 33 10% Since 1950 30 9% c Research Mode Since 1940 7 2% Since before 1940 3 1% Field experimental 270 81% Not reported 3 1% Survey 256 77% ki? Literature review & analysis 236 71% Length of Experience In Child/TV Research: Laboratory study -, 230 69% Case study 183 55% 1 - 5 yrs 160 48% Community participation 1 70 51% 6 -10 yrs 103 31% 11 -15 yrs 27 8% d Facilities 16 yrs + 23 7% Not reported 20 6% Schools 246 74% Library 210 63% Prior Professional interest: Laboratory 1 70 51% Studio 130 39% Social or Dev Psychology 50 15% Home 117 35% Social effects of TV 47 14% Education 26 8% e Equipment Radio/TV Broadcasting & Production 26 8% AdVertising 17 5% Standard audio-visual recorders 210 63% Consumerism 17 5% Standard audio-v 'us' receivers 200 60% Audience Research 13 4% Psychophysical testing equipment 23 7% Verbs , learning 10 3% Paper end pencil type 7 2% Others and not reported 127 38%

102 190 Interest in Research Topics: Considerable Some interest No Interest Interest

Advertising Practices Separating programs from commercials 93 28 53 16 187 56 Exaggeration or distortion 153 46 27 8 153 46 Misleading comparisons 113 34 37. 11 183 55 Celebrity endorsements 53 16 53 16 227 68 Host selling 23 7 50 15 260 78 Premiums, contests 43 13 50 15 240 72 Direct pressure to purchase 90 27 40 12 203 60

Perceptual/CepnItNe Issues Visual-verbal integration 117 35 50 15 166 50 Awareness of special effects 93 28 50 15 190 57 Vulnerability to broadcasting techniques 177 53 23 7 133 40 Information processing 196 59 30 9 107 32

Child Development Issues

Safety 83 25 63 19 187 56 Medicines, vitamins 57 17 57 17 220 66 Nutrition 96 29 57 17 180 54 Sex roles/sex differences 153 46 43 13 137 41 Health information 83 25 60 18 190 57 Values orientation 226 68 20 6 87 26

Consumer Socialization Appeals to self-image 173 52 20 6 140 42 Sexual connotations 100 30 47 14 186 56 Social status appeals 157 47 26 8 150 45 Cost/value emphasis 93 28 50 15 190 57

Social Policy Issues Policy statements 183 55 7 2 143 43 Guidelines/codes 167 50 0 0 167 50

191 193 Appendix C

National Association of BroadcastersCode News, Vol. 8, No. 10, October 1975 Children's Television Advertising Guidelines

PREAMBLE I General

Children, especially preschoolers, are highly de- A. Documentation adequate to support the pendent on the guidance and direction of the adult truthfulness and accuracy of all claims and world around themtelevision includedfor their representations containedin the audio or individual development. Since children, especially video of the advertisement must be made when unsupervised by adults, may not in all situa- available upon request to broadcasters and/or tions be able to discern the credibility of what they the Code Authority. watch, they pose an ethical responsibility for others to protect them from their own potential suscep- tibilities. However, broadcasters believe that adver- The disclosure of information on the charac- tising of products or services normally usedby teristics and functional aspects of a prod - children can serve to inform children not only of the, uctlserviceis strongly encouraged. This in- attributes of products/services but also of many cludes, where applicable, relevant ingredient aspects of the society and world in which theylive. and nutritionalinformation.In order to Everyone Involved in the creation, production, and reduce the possibility of rritsimpressions being presentation of such advertising to children has a created, all such information shall be pre- responsibility to assure that such material avoids sented in a straightforward manner devoid of being exploitative oforinappropriate to a child's language or production techniques which may stilldeveloping cognitive abilities and sense of exaggerate or distort the characteristics or value. functions of the product.

Recognizing these special considerations the C Television, advertisements shall not include NAB Code Authority issues these Children's Televi- presumptions that a product or service requir- sion Advertising Guidelines de.igned to assist ing material investment canbe had for the manufacturers, their agencies, and broadcasters in asking. Children sha' Inot be directed to the preparation and evaluation of television com- purchase or to ask a parent or otheradult to mercials. buy a product or service for them.

Except where hereinafter stated, theseguidelines D In order to help assure that advertising is non- apply to advertising ot. products designed primarily for exploitative in manner, style, and tone, such children or to adlefising which istelecast during advertising shall avoid using exhortative programs designed primarily for children orwithin sta- language. It shall also avoid employing ir- tion breaks between such consecutive programs. ritating,obtrusive or strident audio tech- designed prunarils for children niques, or video devices such as cuts of less than 1 second in length, a series of fast cuts, In addition to the following special guidelines, all special effects of a psychedelic nature (e.g., such advertising is subject to review, where applica- flashing colors, flashinglights, flashing ble, under the standards contained in the Television superecl copy, or other effects whichcould Code overglamorize or mislead).

'194 E Any representation of a child's concept of M. Advertisements shall not Include dramatiza- himself/herself or of his /her relationship to tions of any product in a realistic war at- others must he constructively handled. When mosphere. self-concept claims are employed, the role of the product/service in affecting such promised N Oversimplification orminimization of price benefits as strength, growth, physical prowess, such as "only" or "just" shall not be used. and growing up must accurately reflect docu- Price may be employed in advertising only mented evidence if It can be supported as the usual and custom- ary price in a substantial number of out- F. Advertisements shall not portray attitudes lets in the given trade area or areas where the and practices inconsistent with generally advertising is scheduled. recognized social values and customs. 0. Advertisements shall include audio and video G. Appeals shall not be used which directly or by disclosure when Items such as batteries needed implication contend that if children have a to operate a product as demonstrated in the product they are better than their peels or advertising are not Included. lacking it will not be accepted by their peers. P. Positive exposition of a product's own at- H. Material shall not be used which can reasona- tributes are acceptable. However, because of bly be expected to frighten children or pro- their potential to encourage dissatisfaction on voke anxiety, nor shall material be used which a child's part, competitive/comparison/ contains a portrayal of or appeal to violent, superiority claims or techniques aredis- dangerous, or otherwise antisocial behavior. allowed.

I. Advertisements and products advertised shall Q. Advertising shall positively and clearly dis- be consistent with generally recognized stand- close a product's method of operation and ards of safety. Advertisements shall not In- source of power, where applicable. clude demonstrations of any product in a manner that encourages harmful use or dram- R. Advertising shall disclose when a product re- atizations of actions inconsistent with quires assembly. There shall be no demonstra- generally recognized standards of safety. tion which creates the impression that a prod- uct comes fully assembled when such is not the J. The use of real-life authority case. figures/celebrities as product presenters shall not include their personal testimonials or en- II. Products/Special Categories. dorsements. A Toys K Persons who are recognized as being iden- tified, specifically or generically, with an ad- In addition to the foregoing I. General pre- vertised product's counterpart in real-life may cepts the following guidelines are applicable not be used as spokespeople or'endorsers. This to all television toy advertising and to other prohibition also applies to actors representing advertising designed primarily for children such persons. which emphasizes a product's play value. Ex- cepted are those commercials primarily L. Nonprescription medications and supplemen- designed for and directed to adults. tal vitamin products, regardless of how taken or administered, shall not be advertised in or I. Advertising shall present the toy on its ac- adjacent to program: initially designed pri- tual merit as a play thing. It shall neither marily for children under 12 years of age. exaggerate nor distort play value. (This prohibition does not apply to products which have been vitamin enriched or fortified 2. Audio and visual production techniques in accordance with accepted nutritional prin- shall not misrepresent the appearance and ciples ) performance of toys Any view of a toy or

194 195 any demonstration of its performance shall and used with the product advertised, its be limited to that which a child is reasona- incidental appearance in the product seg- bly capable of reproducing. ment of the commercial will be permitted on a case-by-case basis. 3. When a toy is presented in the context of a play environment, the setting and situatiJn 2. The premium/offer shall at some time be shall be that which a child is reasonably displayed in a still visual presentation, so capable or reproducing. that it is clearly depicted.

4. The use of stock film footage, real-life 3. In the premium/offer segment, the use of counterparts' Of toys, fantasy, and anima- stock footage, real-life counterparts, fan- tion are acceptable if; (a) they are confined tasy, or animation is not permitted. In to the first one-third of the commercial, (b) order to maintain continuity, the product no child or toy appears within them and, spokesperson may deliver a lead-in to the (c) the commercial as a whole conforms to premium/offer segment, provided it con- the Children's Television Advertising tains no endorsement or sell copy for the Guidelines. premium or offer. Also for continuity, the voice-over used in the premium/offer seg- Any other use of stock film footage, ment may be that of the product real-life counterparts, fantasy, and anima- spokesperson. tion and any overglamorization (e.g., large displays, dazzling visual effects) is not per- 4. The number of items shown in a play situa- mitted. tion shall not exceed two per child, or a 5. The original purchase must be clearly dis- mahimum of four with two or more closed in the body of the commercial. children, unless the possession of more by There shall not be any implication that op- .one child can be reasonably supported by tional extras, additional units or items that the advertiser. are not available with the toy, accompany 5. Positive disclosure of special information, the toy's original purchase. such as the price or separate purchase In the closing 5 seconds of the commer- nature of the items offered, shall be made cial the original purchase must be dis- in the audio. As deemed appropriate, sup- closed by video with audio disclosure porting disclosure simultaneously in the where necessary for clarification. video will be required. 6. Advertising shall not employ costumes and 6. If any conditions are attached to obtaining props which are not available with the toy a "free"prembm or offer, all the condi- as sold or are not reasonably accessib e to tions must be clearly and conspicuously the child without additional cost. disclosed simultaneouslyinaudio and video. The appearance of the word "free" Premiums and Offers in a video super shall not exceed in size that of the conditions disclosed. The Advertising Guidelines for Children's Premiums and Offers shall, be applied to all 7. Toy Advertising Guidelines andall advertising designed primarily for children Children's Television Advertising which promotes premiums or offers. Excepted Guidelines shall apply where applicable to are those commercials which are pr:marily premiums and offers. designed for and directed to adults. C. Food 1 The amount of time devoted to a premium or offer shall be a continuous segment and All Children's Television Advertising shall not exceed one-half of the commer- Guidelines under I. General in addition to the cial or 20 seconds, whichever is less in following specific guidelines will apply to length. If the premium/offer is related to food advertising.

95 196 I. Given the importance of sound health and reference to and one video depiction of the nutritional practices, advertisements for role of the product within the framework edibles shall be in accord with the com- of a balanced regimen monly accepted principles of good eating and seek to establish the proper role of the 3 Special, enriched foods designed to serve advertised product within the framework as a substitute for a meal may be adver- of a balanced regimen. Any representation tised as such prov idea their purpose and of the relationship between an edible and nutritional value are featured in the adver- energy must be documented and ac- tising and supported by adequate docu- curately depicted. mentation.

2. Each commercial for breakfast-type prod- uctsshallincludeatleastone audio

EDITOR'S NOTE 4 The second paragraph of Guideline I-N is not Intended to cover price/cost references During discussion of the new Children's Televi- in children's premiums/offers advertising. sion Advertising Guidelines, the board offered the following interpretive comments: 5. In the implementation of Guideline I-P, a distinction should be made between non- 1 An adult product may be advertised in or durable (transient/consumable) products adjacent to a children's program if the and durable products. A nondurable prod- commercial for the product meets all ap- uct (product designed to be used up and plicable Children's Television Advertising replacedinrelatively short periods of Guidelines and other Code standards and time, e.g., foods, drinks, crayons, bat- policies. teries) may be compared in a television commercial to the previous version of th,:. 2. The word "buy" in Guideline I-C is to be same product. Advertising for all durable understood initsbroadest sense. Such productsissubjectto review under words as "get," "obtain," are to be con- Guideline I-P. sidered synonymous with "buy." 6. In certain limited situations, criterion #2 3. Th,... word "adjacent" in Guidelines I-L under III-Adult Interpretation may be and 111-3 is to be understood as referring waived. An example of such a situation is to station breaks that occur immediately the use of a child actor/actress in advertis- before and after programs designed pri- ing for a fast-food company that is adult marily for children. oriented and scheduled in adult programs.

D. Snacks, Candy, Gum, and Soft Drinks mg references the play value of clothing, the All Children's Television Advertising Toy Advertising Guidelines II. A. will also be applicable. Guidelines under I. General in addition to the following guideline will apply to advertising F. School/Educational Supplies for snacks, candy, gum, and soft drinks. All Children's Television Advertising Guidelines,li. I. Commercials for products such as snacks, General, will apply to candies, gums, and soft drinks shall not school/educational supply advertising. In ad- suggest or recommend indiscriminate dition where such advertising references the play value of the product, the Toy Advertising and/or Immoderate use of the product Guidelines II. A. will also be applicable. E. Clothing Feature Film Trailers All Children's Television Advertising Feature films, other than those appropriate Guidelines, I. General, will apply to clothing for a general family audience shall not be ad- advertising. In addition where such advertis- vertised in or adjacent to programs initially

196 197 designed primarily for children under 12 the parent /adult -childrelationship is years of age. established and the parent/adult remains the dominant character. III. Adult Interpretation 3. The broadcast schedule does not include Advertising of any product covered by the the placing of the commercial in or adja- foregoing categories if designed for and cent to programs designed primarily for directed to adults, will be exempted from the children. application of the provisions of thi.. Children's Television Advertising Guidelines provided: 4. The commercial complies with all applica- ble Television Code standards. I The creative concept and execution of the commercial, both the audio and video, are With the exception of the section gol,ernirl clearly and unequivocally designed to ap- feature film trailers which takes effect January 1, peal to adults and not primarily to 1976, the guidelines become effective September 1, children under 12 1976. Until that time current policies in each of the applicable areas addressed by the new guides will 2. Any use of a child is limited to a real-life apply. situation and, if OP- child is used other than as an incidental. background character, National Asso:Iation of Broadcasters Television such use is confined to a situation in which Code - 19th Edition, June 1976

Selected Sections Relevant to Children

II. Responsibility Toward Children Because children are allowed to watch programs designed primarily for adults, Broadcasters have a special responsibility broadcasters should take this practice into ac- to children. Programs designed primarily for count in the presentation of material in such children should take into account the range of programs when children may constitute a sub- Interests and needs of children from instruc- stantial segment of the audience,. tional and cultural material to a wide variety of entertainment material. In their totality, All the standards set forth in this section programs should contribute to the sound, apply to both program and commercial balanced development of children to help material designed and Intended for viewing by them achieve a sense of the world at large and children. informed adjustments to their society.

In the ccurse of a child's development, ADVERTISING STANDARDS numerous social factors and forces, including television, affect the ability of the child to IX General Advertisi,,g Standards make the transition to adult society. 1. This Code establishes basic standards for The child's training and experience during all . The principles the formative years should include positive of acceptability and good taste within the sets of values which will allow the child to Program Standards section govern the become a responsible adult, capable of coping presentation of advertising where applica- with the challenges of maturity. ble. In addition, the Code establishes in this section special standards which apply Children should also be exposed, at the ap- to television advertising. propriate times, to a reasonable ran^e of the realities which exist in the world sufficient to 2. Commercial television broadcasters make help them make the transition to adulthood. their facilities available for the advertising

197 196 of products and services and accept com- 8. Advertising by institutions or enterprises mercial presentations for such advertising. which in their offers of instruction imply However, television broadcasters should, promises of employment or make exagger- in recognition of their responsibility to the awd claims for the opportunities awaiting public, refuse the facilities of their :.cations those who enroll for courses is generally to an advertiser where they have good unacceptable. reason to doubt the integrity of the adver- tiser, the truth of the advertising represen- 9. The advertising of firearms/ammunition is tations, or the compliance of the advertiser acceptable provided it promotes the prod- with the spirit and purpose of all applica- uct only as sporting equipment and con- ble legal requirements forms to recognized standards of safety as well as all applicable laws and regulations. 3. Identification of sponsorship must be Advertisements of firearms/ammunition made it all sponsored programs in accord- by mail order are unacceptable. The ad- ance with the requirements of the Com- vertising of fireworks is unacceptable. munications Act of 1934, as amended, and the Rules and Regulations of the Federal 10. The advertising of fortune-telling, occult- Communications Commission. ism, astrology, phrenology, palm-reading, numerology, nand- reading, character- 4. Representations which disregard normal reading, or subjects of a like nature is not safety precautions shall be avoided. permitted. Children shall not be represented, ex- 1 I. Because all products of a personal nature cept under proper adult supervision, as create special problems, acceptability of being in contac. with or demonstrating a product recognized as potentially such products should be determined with especial emphasis on ethics and the canons dangerous to them. of good taste. Such advertising of personal 5. In consideration of the customs and at- products as is accepted must be presented titudes of the communities served, each in a restrained and obviously inoffensive television broadcaster should refuse manner. his/het facilities to the advertisement of products and services, or the use of adver- 12. The advertising of tip sheets and other tising scripts, which the station has good publications seeking to advertise for the reason to believe would be objectionable purpose of giving odds or promoting bet- o a substantial and responsible segment of ting is unacceptable. e community. These standards should be The lawful advertising of government applied with judgment and flexibility, tak- organizations which conduct legalized lot- ing into consideration the characteristics teries is acceptable provided such advertis- of the medium, its home and family au- ing does not unduly exhort the public to dience, and the form and content of the bet. particular presentation. The advertising of private or govern- mental organizations which conduct 6. The advertising of hard liquor (distilled legalized betting on sporting contests is ac- spirits) is not acceptable. ceptable provided such advertising is limited to institutional type announce- ments which do not exhort the public to 7. The advertising of beer and winesis bet. acceptable only when presented in the best of good taste and discretion, and is accept- 13. An advertiser who markets more than one able only subject to federal and local laws. product should not be permitted to use ad- (See Television Code Interpretation No. 4 ) vertising copy devoted to an acceptable

198 199 product for purposes of publicizing the Commercials, whether live,film, or brand name or other identification of a tape, within programs initially designed product which is not acceptable. primarily for children under 12 years of age shall be clearly separated from 14. "Bait-switch" advertising, whereby goods program material by an appropriate or services which the advertiser has no in- device tention of selling are offered merely to lure the customer Into purchasing higher priced Trade name identificationor other substitutes, is not acceptable. merchandising practices involving the gra- tuitous naming of products is discoul-i-iri 15 Personal endorsements (testimonials) shall in programs designed primarily tor be genuine and reflect personal ex- children. perience. They shall contain no statement Appeals involving matters of tizalt,0 that cannot be supported if presented in which should be determined by physicians the advertiser's (,%vn words. should not be directed primarily to children.

X Presentation of Advertising 4. No children's pi ogram personality or car- toon character shall be utilized to deliver I. Advertising messages should be presented commercial messages within or adjacent to with courtesy and good taste; disturbing or the programs in which such a personality annoying material should be avoided; ev- or cartoon character regularly appears. ery effort should be made to keep the ad- This provision shall also apply to lead-ins vertising message in harmony with the con- to commercials when such lead-ins contain tent and general tone of the program in sell copy or imply endorsement of the which it appears. product by program personalities or car- toon characters. 2. The role and capability of television to market sponsors' products are well recog- 5. Appeals to help fictitious characters in nized. In turn, this fact dictates that great television programs by purchasing the ad- care be exercised by the broadcaster to vertiser's product or service or sending for prevent the presentation of false, mislead- a premium should not be permitted, and ing or deceptive advertising. While it is en- such fictitious characters should not be in- tirely appropriate to present a product in a troduced into the advertising message for favorable light and atmosphere, the pres- such purposes. entation must not, by copy or demonstra- tion, involve a material deception as to the 6. Commercials for services or over -the- characteristics, performance, oi. ap- counter products involving health con- pearance of the product. siderations are of intimate and far-reach- ing importance to the consumer. The Broadcast advertisers are responsible following principles should apply to such for making available, at the request of the advertising: Code Authority, documentation adequate to support the validity and truthfulness of claims, demonstrations, and testimonials a. Physicians, dentists, or nurses or actors contained in their commercial messages. representing physicians, dentists, or nurses, shall not be employed directly 3. The broadcaster and the advertiser should or by implication. These restrictions exercise special caution with the content also apply to persons professionally and presentation of television commercials engaged in medical services (e.g., physi- placed in or near programs designed for caltherapists, pharmacists, dental children. Exploitation of children should assistants, nurses' aides). be avoided. Commercials directed to children should in no way mislead as to the b. Visual representations of laboratory product's performance and usefulness. settings may be employed, provided

199 200 they bear a affect relationship to bona viewer, the following standards are set forth ir fide research which has been conducted accordance with sound television practice: for the product or service.(See Televi- sionCod, X, IT) In such cases, 1. Non-Program Material Definition: laboratory technicians shall be iden- Nonprogram material, in both prime tified as such and shall not be employed time andallother time, Includes as spokespersons or in any other way , commercials, promotional an- speak on behalf of the product ,..., nouncements, and all credits in excess of 30 seconds per program, except in feature films. In no event should credits exceed 40 XIII. Premiums and Offers seconds per program. The 40-second limitation on creditsshallnotapply, 1. Full details of proposed offers should be however, in my situation governed by a required by the television broadcaster for contract entered into before October 1, investigation and approved before the first 1971. Public service announcements and announcement of the offer is made to the promotional announcements for the same public. program are excluded from this definition.

2. A final date for the termination of an offer 2. 'Allowable Time for Non-Program should be announced as far in advance as Material- possible.

3. Before accepting for telecast offers involv- a. In prime time on network affliated sta- ing a monetary consideration, a television tions, nonprogram material shall not broadcaster should be satisfied as to the in- exceed 9 minutes 30 seconds in any 60- tegrity of the advertiser and the adver- minute period. tiser's willingness to honor complaints in- Prime time is a continuous period of dicating dissatisfaction with the premium not less than 3 consecutive hours per by returning the monetary consideration. broad ..ast day as designated by the sta- tion between the hours of 6:00 p.m. and 4. There should be no misleading descrip- midnight. tions or visual representations of any pre- miums or gifts which would distort or b. In all other time, nonprogram material enlarge their value in the minds of the shall not exceed 16 minutes in any 60- viewers. minute period.

5. AssUrances should be obtained from the advertiser that premiums offered are not c. Children's Programing TimeDefined harmful to person or property. as those hours other than prime time in which programs initially designed pri- 6. Premiums should not be approved whit.'-- marily for children under 12 years of appeal to superstition on the basis of age are scheduled "luck-bearing" powers or otherwise. Within this time period on Saturday and Sunday, nonprogram material shall not exceed 9 minutes 30 seconds in any XIV Time Standards for Non-Program Material* 60-minute period. In order that the time for nonprogram material and its placement shall best serve the Within this time period on Monday through Friday, nonprogram material shall not exceed 12 minutes in any 60- 'See Time Standards for Independent Stations p 20 minute period

200 2 01 3 Program Interruptions: thereof inprime time (prime timeis defined as any three contiguous hours be- a. Definition. A program interruption is tween 6 00 p.m and midnight, local time), any occurrence of nonprogram material or 8 minutes in a 30-minute period or within the main body of the program. multiples thereof during all other times.

b. In prime time, the number of program 3. Where a station does not carry a commer- interruptions shall not exceed two with- cial in a station break between prOgrams, in any 30-minute program, or four with- the number of program interruptions shall in any 60-minute'program. not exceed 4within any 30-minute program, or 7 within any 60-minute Programs longer than 60 minutes program, or10 within ary 90-minute sh'all be prorated at two interruptions program, or 13 in any 120-minute per half-hour program. Stations which do carry commer- The'number of interruptions in 60- cials in station breaks between programs minute variety shows shall not exceed shall limit the number of program inter- five. ruptions to3 within any 30-minute ogram, or 6 within any 60-minute cIn all other time, the number of inter- program, or 9 with,- any 90-minute ruptions shall not exceed four within program, or 12 in any 120-minute any 30-minute program period. programNews, weather, sports, and special events are exempted because of for d In children's weekend programing time, mat. as above defined in 2c, the number of programeterruptions shall not exceed 4. Not more than foul nonprogram material two within any 30-minute program or announcements as defined above shall be four within any 60-minute program. scheduled consecutively. An exception may be made only in the case of a program e. In both prime time and all other time, the following interrupticn standard 60 minutes or more in length, when no more than seven nonprogram elements sh411apply within programs of15 may be scheduled consecutively by stations minutes or less in length: who wish to reduce the number of program 5-minute programI interruption, interruptions 10-minute program-2 interruptions; 5. The conditions of paragraphs thr,:e and 15-minute program-2 interruptions fourshallnot apply , tolivesports 41Pprograms where the program format dic- tates and limits the number of program In- TIME STANDARDS FOR INDEPENDENT terruptions. STATIONS / --CHILDREN'S REVIEW UNIT BilB National Advertising Division I. Nonprogram elements shall be considered --T Council of Better Business Bureaus, Inc as all-inclusive, with the exception of re- quired credits, legally required station ------1845 Third Avenue. New York, N.Y 10022 identifications, and "bumpers." Promotion Spots and public service announcements, CHILDREN'S ADVERTISING GUIDELINES as well as commercials, are to be con- sidered nonprogram elements PREAMBLE

2. The allowed time for nonprogram ele- These Guidelines have been developed for the use ments, as defined above, shall not exceed 7 of advertisers and advertising agencies and for the minutes in a 30-minute period or multiples self-regulatory mechanism which they have

201 202 established, the National Advertising Division, to honesty, justice, generosity, and respect for help ensure that advertising directed to children is others truthful, accurate, and fair t(, children's perceptions

Because trends inadve-tising are Lontinuaily V Although many influences affect a child's per- changing and because research and study are con- sonai and social development, it remains the stantly shedding new light on children's develop- prime responsibility of the parents to provide ment and understanding of advertising, these guidance for children, and to exert necessary Guidelines will continu?, to be revised and modified and proper influences in children's exposure to on an on-going basis, as circumstances dictate he worldAdvertisers should contribute to this parent-child relationship in a constructive PRINCIPLES manner.

Five basic Principles underlie these Guidelines A. Interpretation for advertising directed t(children Advertisers are remindedthatthein- sy terpretation of these' Gindehnes should con- form to and implement the Principles stated Advertisers should always take into account above. The intent in all cases should be to ueal the level of knowledge, sophistication, and fairly and honestly with children, fulfilling the maturity of the audience to w ,co the message spirit as well al the letter of the Guidelines is primarily directed Since younger children Each individual commercial or advertis.ment have a limited capability for discerning the should be considered in that context credibility of what they watch, they place a Differences in the nature of broadcast media special responsibtlity upon advertisers to pro- and print media should he taken into account tect them from their on susceptibilities The Guidelines should not be regarded' as prescribing rigid or inflexible rules which may deprive children and advertisers of the II Realizing that children are imaginative and benefits of innovations and new approaches that mal:e-believe play constitutes an impor- B. Scope , tant part of the growing up process, advertisers should exercise care not to exploit that imag- The clauses in these Guidelines embrace inative quality of children Unreasonable ex- advertising designed to appeakto children II pectations of product quality or performance years of age and under. This includes should not be stimulated either directly or in- children's advertising which is broadcast in directly by advertising children's programs and programs in which audience patterns typically contain more than 50% children. Commercials appearing in shows in which children are a substantial au- III Recognizing that advertising may play an im- dience segment, but less than 50%, will he portant part in educating the child, rntorma- regarded as subject to these Guidel.nes only tion should be communicated in a truthful and when they are clearly addressed to children. II accurate manner, with full recognition on the and under. Print advertising is subject tv these part of the advertiser that the child may learn Guidelines when it is primarily directed to or practices from advertising which can affect his primarily read by ail Iren or her health and well-being C Social Values Advertising should emphasize positive IV \ dvertisers are urged to capitalize on the po- social and moral values and enrich the dignity tential of advertising to influence social of human life, as opposed to portrayals of behavior by developing advertising that, violence, appeals to tear, or prejudice of any whenever possible, addresses itself to social kind To this end

standards generally regarded as positive and I Advertisements should Never portray as beneficial, such as friendship, kindness, desirable any practices which are generally considered unacceptable from the stand- form of communication to any audienc6, in- point of social, legal, moral, institutional, cluding the very young. or family values. Social stereotyping which is demeaning or derogatory to any group However, the use of special situations and should be avoided. fantasy in advertising should not suggest unat- tainable expectations of performance. 2. Advertisements should not reflect disdain for parents or -parental- judgment, nor presentations should not reflectunfavorably on other genetally orallo exploit the child'3 difficulty in distinguishing recognized sources of child guidance. ". brtween the real and the fanciful 3. Advertisements should never portray un- . desirable living habits Advertising should Particular control should be exercised to 4 convey respect for others and the world in assure that: which the child lives. Civility and good Copy, sound and visual presentationsas manners should be encouraged. well as the advettisemenrin its totality 4 Advertisements should encourage good do not mislead on performance charac- use of language. This does not preclude in -. teristics such as speed, method of opera- formal usage. tion, size,-color, durability, nutrition, 4 noise, etc.; on perceived benefits s. .:ii as 5. Advertisements should avoid the conten- the acquisition of strength, popularity, tion that, by posse.ssing a product, a child growth, proficiency, intelligence, and the will be more accepted by his peers, or by like; or, on the expectation of price range lacking it, he or she will be less accepted by Or cost of the product. his peers. 2.The advertisement clearly establishes what 6. Advertisements should avoid the implica- is inc.aded in the original purchase of the tion that a parent or adult who purchases a advertised product, employing where given product or service for a child is bet- necessary positive disclosure on what items ter or more generous than one who does are to be, purchased separately in a way not. that will be understood by the child au- 7. Advertisements should not falsely imply dience to which the advertisement is pri- that purchase and use of a product or serv- marily addressed. All --advertising for ice' will confer upon the user the prestige, products sold unassembled should indicate skills,r other special qualities of charac- that assembly is required. If any other ters appring in the commercial or ad. product is essential inorder.to use the ad- Material nefits attributed to the product vertised product- -uch as batteries=this or service be inherent in the use should be disclosed. thereof. 3. A clearly depicted presentatiOn of the complete advertised product is shown in D. PI esentation the advertisement. When appropriate in Children have vivid imaginations. Use Gf helping toidentify the product, the imagination enables a child to project himself package may also be depicted, provided it beyond his immediate capacities and reach for does not mislead as to product charac- his future potential. Advertisers should, teristics, content, or the price range to be therefore, always respect a child's imagina- expected. tion. 4 Advertising demonstrations showing the use of a product or premium can be readily The use of imaginative situations relevant duplicated by the average child for whom to the audience concerned is an acceptable the product is intended and normal communications practice. Im- plicit in the foregoing is the concept that fan- 5 Representations of food products should tasy, including animation, is an appropriate be made so as to encourage sound usage of

203 204 the product with aley.' toward healthy with or identified with the productmay -development of the child and the develop- be used as presenters for the advertised ment of good nutritional practices Adver- product or service, provided they do not tisements representing mealtimeinthe do or say anything to mislead children as home should clearly and adequately depict to the product or service concerned. the role of The product within the frame- 4 Nat ional4S, known persons may not be used work of a balanced diet Overconsumption to attobute a characteristic or quality to a of food products and beverages should he product (including a premium) unless they avoided, nor should it he implied that any are generally recognized as qualified to one food provides all the nutrients con- speak on,the subject. All personal endorse- tained in a well-designed daily food plan ments should reflect the real experience 6 To ensure accurate and truthful represen- and beliefs of he endorser tation of playthings, they should he shown in normal play envirOnments And situa- Comparative Claims tions It is recognized that advertising which com- 7 A fair and equitable number of products pares the advertised product to another prod- should he featurqd, consistent with the uct may he difficult for children to understand number of children shown in the play set- and, evaluate and may therefore he misun- ting. Should a whole line of toys or more derstood Therefore, advertisers are urged to toys than might he reasonably owned by represent products on their merits without the average child be featured, limbo set- reference to competition tings (which are defined as nonrepresenta- tional settings with a plain background) or In the event that a true and significant ad- in-storesettings, are suggested,as they vantage may exist in a product which can he might provide a better context for fair readily understood by children, this advan- *demonstration of these products to tage should be clearly explainedIf adver- children. tisers should develop comparative advertising to children this should he done with the Promotion by Program Character, Editorial following cautions in mind Character, or Personal Endorsement I Comparative statements should be infor- Itis recognized that very young children mational and not demeaning to. other may not fully recognize (inferences between products or to previous versions of the editorial and'program content and advertising same product content Hence, endorsement by characters on 2 Comparative statements should not sug- ,the programs or in the editorial content of a gest that the advertised product is superior publication may confuse children Therefore to another in individual attributes or over- all characteristics unless such statements I Program personalities or, program charac- ters -(liye or animated) on children's can bc, documented programs should not be used to promote 3 Comparative statements implying overall products, premiums, or services in or adja- superiority should be avoided when such cent to any program where the personality statements are based on attributes in which or chardcter appears the advertised product excels, and where the competitor's product excels in other at- 2 Inpritm dia, characters and per-. tributes not mentioned sonaliti s ()elatedwiththe editorial contena publication should not he used 4 Comparative price statements should be to promote products, premiums. or serv- based on the usual and customary price ices in the same publication paid in a substantial number of sales in the trade area where the advertising is carried 3 Subject to paragraph (I) of this section, Price comparisons should he understand- "product charters--personafitics !Re able to the average child for whom the o. animated who. are closely associated product is intended

204 r I,

J 4

G. Pressure to Purchase 2.Advertisements should avoid demonstra- tions or portrayals that encourage misuse, The purpose of advertising to children is to or dangerous or inappropriate use of the encourage trial and repeat purchase product which isinconsistent with However, children are not as prepared to generally accepted standards of safety. make independent decisionsor contribute to family decisionsas are adults. Accordingly, 3.Medications, drugs, and supplemental to, avoid undue pressure to purchase: vitamins (liquid or pills) should not be ad- vertised to children. I.Children should not be urged to ask ' parents or others to buy any product. I Claim Substantiation 2. Products which by Iheir very nature are In accordance with the basic principle of not primarily intended for use by children dealing fairly and honestly with children: should' not employ advertising directed to children; nor should such products be pro- 1. Advertising to children should not claim moted by premiums or other means or imply any product or performance directly to children. characteristics which are not supportable by factual data or research which conforms 3. All price representations should be clearly to sound professional practices and concisely set forth m a manner ,so as not to exert undue pressure to purchase, 2. Puffery (defined as "flattering " and price minimizations such as "only" or or "extravagant commendation") is not ac- "just" should not be used in any adverts- ceptable support for an objective poduct 0 ing directed to childrer, claim. Advertising claims which might be construed as literally true must be literally 4.When toys or any other product can be true. purchased either individually or as a col-

lection of related items, price representa- J. Additional Guidelines for Premium Advertis- tion should clearly Indicate to the child ing that the cost of the collection is greater than the cost of the individual item. The use of premiums in advertising has po- tential to enhance the appeal of a product to a H. Safety child. Special attention should, therefore, be paid to the use of premiums in advertising. To For the child, imitation, explorationand guard against premiums exploiting the experimentation are important facets of the children's immaturity: learning processThe various media can enhance this process, as can advertisingin I Care should be taken that the child's atten- each of the media which the child encounters. tion is focused primarily on the product Recognizing this, advertisers shouldguide rather than the premium. Therefore, major .their advertising to contribute to theestablish- emphasis should be given to the product ment of safe and sound habits inchildren. and its benefits. Emphasis on the premium should be clearly secondary.

Moreover, children, and occasionally 2. It is recognized that limitation of the time parents, may not be cognizant of hazards that devoted to a premium offer within a com- may exist through use or abuse ofproducts. mercial may not be sufficient to ensure pri-,. Therefore. maryattention to the product offer. Therefore, advertisers are urged to weigh I Advertisements, except specific safety all factors, including time, to ensure that messages, should not portray adults or the product message is primary. children in any unsafe acts, situations, or conditions or in acts which are harmful to 3 When a premium offer is used, the condi- others. tions of the offer should be stated simply,

205 2 0 G in terms which a child can understand. Ev- CHILDREN'S REVIEW UNIT ery effort should be made to communicate ' National Advertising Division so-called "mandatory" statements and dis- B-- BB Council of Better Business Bureaus, Inc. claimers in terms which will be understood -1 845 Third Avenue, New York, N.Y. 10022 by a child audience.

-.4 206 A., BIBLIOGRAPHY

Abel, John D., "Television and Children A Selec- .advertising Age, "Proposal to Ban Premium Ads Im- tive Bibliography of Use and Effects,"Journal of periled by New Research Data," May 19,1975, p. Broadcasting, Vol. XIII, I (written 1968-69) I. Abt Associates, Inc. An Evaluation of the Mulligan Advertising Age, "Sharper Line of Action Toys Stew 4-H Television Series (Cambridge, Mass Readied for Fall," Vol. 47 (September 13, 1976). Abt Associates, Inc., 1974). Advertising Age, "Anti-TV Violence Mood Getting Action for Children's Television. Newton. Worse," Vol 47 (October 18, 1976), pp. 1, 110. Massachusetts, Brief II, Toys advertised decep- Advertising Age, news column, Vol. 47 (November tively. A filing to the Federal Communications 15, 1976). Commission, Dec. I I, 1970. "Advertising Guidelines for Children's Pre- Action for Chiidren's Television, Newton, miums/Offers," National Association of Broad- Massachusetts. Petitionto prohibit advertise- casters Code Authority Interpretation, February ments for children's vitamins on children's and 1972. family television programs. Petition to Federal Trade Commission, November 10, 1971. Albaum, Gerald, "Measuring Attitudes of School- Age Children A Note on Effects of Place of Action for Children's Television, Newton, Measurement," Journal of Consumer Research, Massachusetts. General comments on television January 1975 (in press). advertising to children, testimony before the Federal Trade Commission, November 10, 1971. Aldous, Joan and Jack M. McLeod, "Commentaries on Ward 'Consumer Socialization,' "Journal. of Action for Children's Television, Newton, Consumer Research, I (September 1974). Massachusetts. Testimony before the U.S. Senate Select Committee onNutritionand Human Ambrosino, L., "Do Children Believe in. TV?", Needs, March 6, 1973. Children Today, I , No 6, 18, 1972. Advertising Age. ".68 Teen Spending Hit $20 Billion Anderson, Kristin, George Comstock, and Nancy Rand Bureau Says," Vol. 40 (March 10, 1969), p. Dennis "Priorities and Recommendations," Jour- 39. nal of Communication, 1976, 26, 2. Advertising Age, "Psychologist Urges Moratorium Arndt, "A Research Note on Intergenerational on TV Ads for Kids," Vol. 42 (November15, Overlap of Selected Consumer Variables," 1971), pp. 88. Markeds Kommunikasion, 1971. Advertising Age, "TV's Effect of Kids Unknown, Associates for Research in Behavior, Inc , "The FTC is Told," Vol 42 (November 15, 1971), pp. Effects of Child-Directed Television Advertising I and 92 on Children: An Analysis of theCharges and a Conceptualization," Washington, D.0 , Con- Advertising Age, "Many Gripe at Proposed Ban on Premiums; Quaker likes it." June 10, 1974, p. I sumer Research T.:6titute, Inc., September 1972. Advertising Age. "See Fast Showdown on Englman's Association of National Advertisers, Children's Call for Premium Ad Ban," June 10, 1974, p.I Television Advertising Guidelines, adopted May 31, 1972. Advertising Age, "Engman Softens Demand for Children's Premium Ad Ban," June 24, 1974, p. Atkin, Charles, "Brief Summary of Major Findings

I from 1973 Children's Advertising Experiments," "Summary of Major Findings from 1974 Advertising Age, news column, Vol 45 (December Children's Advertising Experiments," "Prelimi- 16, 1974) nary Findings from 1973 Children's Advertising Advertising Age, "Advertising Portraying or Survey," "Mother's Advertising Survey," (1973), Directed Toward Women," Vol. 46 (April 21, "Learning from Television Commercials," "1974 1975) pp. 72-76 TV Advertising Survey," unpublished.

2 0U 207 Atkin, Charles, "The Impact of Premium Offersin presented to the Advertising Division, Associa- Children'sTelevision Commercials," Depart- tion for Education in Journalism, August 1972. ment of Communications, Michigan State Univer- sity, 1974 Baer, Donald M. "Laboratory Control of Thumbsucking by Withdrawal and Representa- Atkin, Charles "Children's Social Learning from tion of Reinforcement," Journal of the Experimen- Television Research Evidence on Observational tal Analysis of Behavior, 5, (1962), pp. 525-528. Modeling of Product Consumption," presented at conference of Associationfor Consumer Ball, Samuel and Gerry Ann Bogatz. The First Year Research, Cincinnati, I975a. of Sesame Street. An Evaluation. Princeton, New Jersey. Educational Testing Service, 1970 Atkin, Charles, "Effects of Television Advertising on ChildrenFirst Year Experimental Evi- Bandura, Albert, "Influence of Models' Reinforce- dence," Report #1, Michigan State University, ment Contingencies on the Acquisition of Imita- June I975b tive Responses," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, (1965), pp. 589-595 Atkin, Charles, "Effects of Television Advertising on ChildrenSecond Year Experimental Evi- Bandura, Albert, Social Learning Theory (Morris- dence," Report #2, Michigan State University, town, New JerseyGeneral Learning Press, June I975c 1971) Atkin, Charles, "Effects of Television Advertising Bandur,a, Albert, "Modeling Influences on on ChildrenContent Analysis of Children's Children," testimony to the Federal Trade Com- Television Commercials," Report #5, Michigan mission, November 1971. State University, June I975d Bandura, Albert. Aggression. A Social Learning Atkin, Charles, "Effects of Television Advertising Analysis (Englewood Cliffs, N..1Prentice-Hall, on ChildrenSurvey of Pre-Adolescent's 1973) Responses to Television Commercials," Report #6, Michigan State University, June 1975e Bandura, Albert, D Ross, and S Ross, "Imitation Atkin, Charles, "Effects of Television Advertising of Film-mediated Aggressive Models," Journal of on ChildrenParent-Child Communication in Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1963, 66 Supermarket Breakfast Selection," Report #7, Bandura, Albert, D. Ross, and S.A. Ross, Michigan State University, June 1975f. "Vicarious Reinforcement and Imitative Learn- Atkin, Charles, "Effects of Television Advertising ing," Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. on ChildrenSurvey of Children's and Mother's 67 (1963), pp 601-607 Res'ponses to Television Commercials," Report Bandura, Albert and R. H. Walters, Social Learning #8, Michigan State University, June 1975g. and Personality Development (New York Atkin, Char lesand J Culley, "Effects of Television Rinehart and , 1963) Advertisingon ChildrenAttitudes of Industry Banks, Seymour, Summary of Research on Child Executives, Government Officials, and Consumer Development and the Effect of Mass Media on Critics Cowards Children's Advertising," Report Television Advertising on Children (Chicago Leo #4, Michigan State University, June 1975. Burnett Co., 1971). Atkin, Charles, and Garry Heald, "Content Dimen- sions of Children's Television Commercials," Banks, Seymour, "Statement inBehalf of Joint Journal of Communications, (in press). ANA-AAAA Committee before the Federal Trade Commission," October 28, 1971, Atkin, Charles, J. P. Murray, and 0. B. Naymen (eds.), "Television and Social Behavior An An- Banks, Seymour, Statement before theFederal notated Bibliography of Research Focusing on Trade Commission, October 28, 1972 Television's Impact on Children," Washington, National Institute of Mental Health, 1971. Banks, Seymour, "Children and Television," remarks at the 15th Annual Conference of the Atkin, Charles and Charles Reinhold, "The Impact American Advertising Federation, Washington, of Television Advertising on Children,"paper D C., January 1973

208 " 9 Advertising to Banks, Seymour, Remarks prepared for the "Semi- Barcus, F Earle, Pre-Christmas nar on Consumer Socializationof Children," Children, Newton, Mass.. Action for Children's Chicago, January 11, 1973 Television, (September 1976). Banks, Seymour, "Advertising and Children," talk Barry, Thomas E., "Children's TV Commercials, presented to the AMA Public Interest Workshop, Importance of the Race Factor," paper presented Association, Washington, D C , May 1973. to the American Psychological August 30, 1975. Banks, SeymOur, Statement to the Federal Com- munications Commission, Washington,D.C., Barry, Thomas F and R. W. Hansen,"How Race of 1973 Affects Children's TV Commercials," Journal Advertising Research,13 (October1973), pp. Banks, Seymour, "Advertising and Children," in 63-67 S F Myna (ed ), Advertising and the Public In- terest (Chicago American Marketing Associa- Bauer, Raymond A. and Stephen A. Greyser,Adver- Boston: tion, 1974). tising in America. The Consumer View Harvard Business School ,1968. Banks, Seymour, Remarks to the NAB National Conference on Children's Television, Bechtel, Robert B., Clark Achelpohl, and Roger Washington, D C , June 3, 1975. Akers, "Correlates Between Observed Behavior and Questionnaire Responses onTelevision Banks, Seymour, "PublicPolicy on Ads to Viewing," in Television and Social Behavior, Vol. Children," Journal of Advertising Research Vol. IV, (ed.) Eli A. Rubinstein, et al., pp. 274-344 15, 4, (August 1975), pp. 7-12. Belton', Fiancis X., Testimony before theSubcom- Banks, Seymour, "The Policy Issues Involved in a mittee on Communications of theHouse Inter- Discussion of Children's Commerciais," paper state and Foreign Commerce Committee,July 15, presented at the American Psychological Associa- 1975. tion, August 30, 1975. Bellow, Francis X petition before the F.C.C. of the Barcus, F. Earle, "Advertising in the Sunday Com- Attorneys General of Massachusetts, Alaska, ics," Journalism Quarterly, X XXIX (Spring Colorado, Delaware, Hawaii, Illinois, Maryland, 1962), pp 196-202 Nebraska, New Hampshire, North Carolina, Barcus, F. Earle, "Saturday Children's Television," Maine, Pennsylvania, Rhode Island, and Wyom- Action for Children's Television, Newton. Mass , ing to promulgate a rule restrictingthe advertis- July 1971 ing of over-the-counter drugs, July 1975. Barcus, F. Earle, "Description of Children's Televi- Berkowitz, Leonard, Aggression: A Social Psychologi- sion Advertising," statement before theFederal cal Analysis (New YorkMcGraw-Hill, 1962). Trade Commission, hearings on modern advertis- Berkowitz, Leonard, The Developmentof Motives ing practices, November 10, 1971. and Values in the Child (New York- BasicBooks, Barcus, F Earle, "Network Programming and Ad- Inc., 1964). vertising in the Saturday Children's Hours-A June and November Comparison," Action for Berry, Lewis A. and R. W. Pollay, "The Influencing Children's Television, Newton, Mass., lama ry Role of the Child in Family Decision-Making," Journal of Marketing Research, 5 (February 19 1972. 1968), pp. 70-72. Barcus, F. Earle, "Concerned Parents Speak Out on Children's Television," ActionforChildren's Beuf, Ann H., "Television Commercials asSocializ- Television, Newton, Mass., March 1973. ing Agents," in J Kernan (ed.)Advances in Con- sumer Research, Vol. III,Proceedings of the Barcus, F. Earle, Weekend Commercial Children's Association for Consumer Research,1975, Television, Newton. Mass Action for Children's Television (October 1975)a. Bever, J. G., M L. Smith, B. Bengen, and T. G. Barcus, F. Earle, Television in the Afternoon Hours, Johnson, "Young Viewers Troubling Response to TV Ads," Harvard Business Review, Vol. 53, 6 Newton, Mass.. Action for Children's Television, (November-December 1975), pp. 119-21. (October 1975)b

209 210 Blatt, Joan, L. Spencer, and S Ward. "ACognitive Association for Childhood Education Interna- Developmental Study of Children's Reactionsto tional, 1954) Television Advertising," workingpaper, Market- ing Science Institute. Cambridge, Mass., 1971 Bryant, Jennings, Jr, "Formative Research in Ad- vertising," paper prepared for Children's Televi- Bluestone, Stephen L, "Inquiry into Modern Ad- sion Workshop, 1972. vertising Practices," statement before the Federal Trade Commission, November 12, 1971 Buck, Ross, "Nonverbal Communication of Affect in Children," Journal of Personality and Social Bogatz, Gerry Ann and Samuel Ball, The Second Psychology, 31, 1975, pp. 644-53. Year of Sesame Street A Continuing Evaluation, V o IsI & 2 (Princeton, N JEducational Test- Bureau of Advertising, American Newspaper ing Service, 1971) Publishers Assn, "The Adult Toy Buyer A Further Exploration of How Parents and Non- Bond, E L, Jr , "New Young and Rubicam Study Parents Buy Toys," New York: American shows Pragmatic Youth," Advertising Age,35 Newspaper Publishers Association, September (April 13, 1964), p. 8 1967.

Brower, Robert T., Television and the Public(New Burrall, Sarah and John Rossiter Perceived Meaning York. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston,Inc., 1973) of Product Claims in F A 0.i Schwartz Toy Catalog Brazelton, T Berry (M D ), "Children andTelevi- Advertising Paper presented at the Conterenceon sion," unpublished talk Culture and Communications, Temple Univer- sity, March 1975. Breckenridge, M.E., "Food Attitudes of 5-12Year Old Children," Journal of the AmericanDietetic Bushy, Linda J. "Sex-Role Research in We Mass Association, 35.704, 1955 Media," Journal ofCommunication, Vol. 25 (Autumn 1975), 107-131. Breen, Miles P. and J. T. Powell, "The RelationBe- tween Attractiveness and Credibility of Televi- Cagley, James W, "Advertising Preference Testing sion Commercials as Perceived byChildrenA in Middle Childhood," paper prtesented at South- Replication," paper presentedto Central State west Social Science Association, March 1973. Speech Association, Minneapoiis, 1973. Cagley, James W., "Children's Preferences of Brim, Orville, G., Jr and S. Wheeler, Socialization Selected Print Appeals," Journal of Advertising, 3(4), 1974, pp. 34-39 After Childhood Two Essays (New YorkJohn Wiley & Sons, Inc, 1966). Calder, Bobby J , Tom Robertson and John ,Broadcast Advertisers Reports Daypcirt SalesForce Rossiter, "Children's Consumer Information Report, New York 1975 Processing," Communications Research. Vol2, (July 1975) pp. 307-316 Broadcasting, "Television for Children:There's Campbell, John D More than May Meet the Eye," November20, , "Illness Is a Point of View: The 1972, p. 33. Development of Children's Concepts of Illness," Child Development, Vol 46 (March 1974),pp BroadcastingYearbook,1974. Washington, D C. 92-100. Broadcasting Publications, Inc., 1974. Campbell, Roland P., statement on the self-regula- Brodbeck, A. J., "The Mass-Mediaas a Socializing tion of children's advertising, presented to the Agency," paper presented at the AmericanPsy- Subcommittee on Communications of the Com- chological Association Symposiumon Children mittee on Interstate and Foreign Commerce, U.S and the Mass-Media, San Francisco,1955. House of Representatives, July 15, 1975. Bronfenbrenner, Urie, "DevelopmentalResearch, Canadian Advertising Advisory Board, "The Public Policy and the Ecology of Childhood,"un- Child's World and Television Advertising," published paper, Cornell University. November 1971 Brumbaugh, Florence N., "What EffectDoes TV Canadian Association of Broadcasters, "Broadcast Advertising Have on Children9", in Constance Code forAdvertising to Children," October Carr (ed.), Children and TV (Washington,D.C. 1971, revised 1973. p 210 211 Canadian Broadcasting League, "Quebec Code for Choate, Robert B ,"Proposed Public Television Ads to Children," Telenation Vol.I, January Code Authority for Advertising Edibles, 1973 Beverages, Pills and Other Internally Consumed Products." Presented to the Code Authority, Na- Canadian Broadcasting League, "A Brief to the tional Association of Broadcasters, 1972b Parliamentary Standing Committee on Broad- casting, Film, and Assistance to the Arts," June4, Choate, Robert B., "The Selling of the Child The 1973 Role of Motivational Research Houses in Children's TV Advertising," statement before the Caron, Andre H., "The Effects of Advertising on Consumer Subcommittee of the Committee on Children," Le Pub licite-Club de Montreal, Inc., Commerce, U.S. Senate, New York City. July 23, Montreal, 1971. 1973. Caron, Andre and S. Ward, "Gift Decisions by Kids Choate, Robert B., "The Eleventh Corn 1 andment and Parents," Journal of Advertising Research, Thou Shalt Not Covet My Child's Purse," Vol 15, 4, August 1975, pp. 12-50. (Washington D.C. Council on Children, Media Cateora, Philip R., An Analysis of the Teen-Age and Merchandising, 1973) Market (Austin, Texas: University of Texas Choate, Robertl , Petition of the Council on Bureau of Business Research, 1963). Children, Media and Merchandising to issue a Cateora, Philip R., "Teenagers as Consumer trade regulation rule governing the private Buyers," Chapter 6 in An Analysis of the Teen-Age regulation of children's televisionadvertising, Market, st, lies in Marketing No. 7. Bureau of filedbeforetheFederal Trade Commission, Business Research, University of Texas at Austin, Washington, D.C., March 1975. 1963, pp. 86-103. Choate, Robert, B., Petition for the Council on Chatfee, Steven H. and J.M McLeod, "Adolescent Children, Media and Merchandising for recon- Television Use in the Family Context." in E.A. sideration and redrafting of Amendments to Child Re- Rubinsteinet al. (eds.),Television and Social Television License Renewal Form on Eqsalization Behavior, Vol. 4 (Washington, D.C.: Government lated Topics and for Extebsion and Printing Office, 1972), pp. 149- I 72. of Restraints on Television Advertising to Children, before the Federal Communications Charren, Peggy, "Roundtable f.. Children's TV," Commission, March 1975. group discussion attended by PeggyCharren, Bill Claggett, George Heinemann, Jack Jacobson, Choate, Robert B., Statement presented at Hearings of Gerald Thain, Seymour Banks, Media Decisions, before the Subcommittee on Communications Com- August 1973 the Committee on Interstate and Foreign merce, House of Representatives(Serial No. Charren, Peggy, testimony before the House Com- 94-53) Washington, D.0 Government Printing munications Subcommittee (Newton, Mass. Ac- Office, 1975 tion for Children's Television, July 14, 1975). Choate, Robert B Testimony to the FederalTrade Cheies-Miller, Pamela, "Reactions to M ait...1 Roles Commission in the matter of trade regdlafior rule in Commercials," Journal of AdvertisingResearch, on food/nutritional advertisingOctober 1976 Vol. 15, 4 (August 1975), pp 45-49. Choate, Robert B and N. Dehevoise, "Presentation Children's Review Unit (see National Advertising to the Child DevelopmentConsultants of the Division) Council of Better Business Bureaus, Inc ,"Janu- Choate, Robert B., testimony before the Subcommit- ary 1975 tee on the Consumer Comnintee of Commerce, Clancy, K. J., "One Nutritionist's View ofFood Ad- U.S Senate, July 23, 1970. vertising. The Proposed TRR's of the FTC,"talc Choate, Robert B , "Researchers Air Views on Kids' presented at American Marketing Association Ads." Advertising Age. November 15, 1971, pp Educator's Conference, August 1975

86-87. Clancy. K . J and S. Sharaga, "Some Effects of Choate, Robert B , "The Sugar-Coated Children's Television Advertising on Children's Nutrition Eating Hour," The Nation, January 31, 1972a Attitude, Nutrition Knowledge and

211 212 Habits," paper presented at the meeting of the Consumer Advisors to Children's Television Adver- Society for Nutrition Education, Boston, August tising Project, "Proposed additions and altera- 1974 tions to present code standards governing televi- Clancy, K J, S Sharaga, A Hickey, 6 Neville, H sion advertising to children." memo to Federal Goldbert, and B. Bibby "The Effects of Televi- Trade Commission Chairman Lewis Engman. sion Advertising on Children's Food Habits," January 14, 1974 Cornell University, in progress Cooper, Barbara and Murray Philip "Evaluation of Clancy-Hepburn, Katherine. A A Hichey, and G Nutrition Education in Everyday Teaching En- Neville, "Children's Behavior Responses to TV vironmentJournal of Nutrition Education, 1974, Food Advertisements," Journal of Nutrition 6, 3 Education, Vol b 3 (July-September 1974),pp Costley, Gary E, "Novel Approaches to Nutrition 93-96 Education." remarks before the 1973 Newspaper Clausen, John A, Sawn:ann and Society (Boston Food Editor Conference. Little, Brown, and Co, 1968) Coulson, John S.. "Buying Decisions Within the Cline, V. B, R G Croft, and S Courier, "Desen- Family and the Consumer-Brand Relationship," sitization of Children to Television Violence," in J W Newman (ed.'), On knowing the Consumer Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, (New York John Wiley and Sons, Inc.. 1966). (1973), 360-365 Council on Children. Media and Merchandising, -Collins, W Andrew, "The Developing Childas a "Comments Before the Federal Communications V iewer,"Journal of COMMUMCatIOPI. 25, (Autumn CommissionSeptember 25, 1975. 1975), 35.44 Cul ley, James D., "Perceptions of Children's Televi- Collins, W Andrew and C A Houten, "Relative sion Advertising An Empirical Investigation of Effects of Communication and Messageon Opin- the Beliefs and Attitudes of Consumer, Industry, ion Change by Children and Adolescents," un- and Government Respondents," preliminary published paper. Institute of Child Development, report, Bureau of Economic and Business University of Minnesota, 1975 Research, January 1975 Culley, James D Comstock, George, "Effects of Televisionon , William Lazer, and C. K Atkin, Children. What is the Evidence",paper pre- "The Experts Look at Children's Television," sentedat Telecommunication Policy Research Journal of Broad«Lsting. Volume 20 (Winter Conference, Airlie, Virginia, April 1975 1976), pp 3-21 Comstock, George, Television and Human Behavior Cuozzo, Peter F., "An Inquiry into the Image of Food and Food Habits as Presented by Television The key Studies (Santa Monica, Calif Rand Corporation, June 1975). Food Commercials," unpublished masters thesis, Annenberg School of Communications, Univer- Comstock, George, "Television and the Teacher," sity of Pennsylvania, 1971 working paper. Santa Monica, Calif The Rand 1) Corporation, 1976 7.nger, K., "Children's Earliest Conceptions of comic Relationships (Australia)," Journal of Comstock, George and M Fisher, Television and Social Psyrhology, 47 (1958),pp 231-240 Human Behavior A Guide to the Pertinent Scien- Deering, B. Jand J. Jacoby, "The Effects of 'Alter- tific Literature (Santa Monica. Calif Rand Cor- native Relationships' and 'Relative Resources' on poration, June 1975) Consumer Decisions Between Mother and Child." in Proceeds of the 2nd Annual Conference Comstock, George and G Lindsey, lelevision and of the Association for Consumer Research (Col- Human BehaviorThe Research Horizon, future lege Park, Maryland Association for Consumer and Present (Santa Monica, Calif. Rand Corpora- tion, June 1975) Research, 1971). pp 135-142

Diamond, StevenI.. "Consumer Education Condry, J., "Psychologist Urges Moratorium on TV Perspectives on the State of the Art," Harvard Ads for Kids," Advertising Age, November 15, University, Graduate School of Business Ad- 1971 ministration, unpublished paper, May 23. 1972.

212 1)4..1:3 Dickins, Dorothy and U Gerguson, "Practices and Engman, Lewis, address before Young Lawyer's Attitudes of Rural White Children and Parents Section of American Bar Association, August Concerning Money," MississippiAgricultural 1973. Experiment Station,Technical Bulletin #43, Engman, Lewis, address before the 1974Annual State College, Mississippi, June 1957 Convention, American Advertising Federation, Dickins, Dorothy and A. Johnston, "Children's In- Washington, D.C. June 3, 1974 fluence on Family Food Purchase Decision," Engman, Lewis, statement before the Subcommittee Bulletin #671, State College,Mississippi. on Communications,Committee on Interstate Agricultural Experiment Station, Mississippi Commerce, House of Representatives,United State University, 1963, p. 15. States Congress, July 16, 1975. Disson, Stephen, The Effects of Television Advertising Ephron, Erwin, "How (and How Not) to SolveTV on a Child's Eating Habits andControl Over the Clutter," Advertising Age, Vol 46 (April21, Family Food Purchase. Unpublished Manuscript, 1975), 57-58. University of Pennsylvania, 1974. Eppright, E and PSwanson, "Distribution of Donohue, Thomas R.., "Effects of Commercials on Nutrients Among Meals and Snacks of Iowa Black Children," Journal of Advertising Research, School Children," Journal of the American Vol 15 (December 1975), 41-47 Dieic Association, 31 256, 195. Doolittle, John and Robert Pepper, "Children's TV Erickson, E., Childhood and Society (NewYork: Ad Content 1974," Journal of Broadcasting, 19.2 Norton, 1950) (Spring 1975), pp. 131-142 Faber, Ronald, "Children's Television Viewing Pat- Drabman, Ronald S. and Margaret H Thomas, terns," unpublished paper, Marketing ScienceIn- "Does TV Violence Breed Indifference," Journal sti,ute, September 10, 1975. of Communication, 25 (Autumn 1976), 86-89, Faber, Ronald and S. Ward, "Validation of Mother- puffy, John and John R. Rossiter, "The Hartford Child Purchase Influence Frequency Reportsby Experiment. Children's Reactions to TV Com- the Multitrait-Multimethod Matrix,"Marketing mercials in Blocks at the Beginning and the End Science Institute Technical Report, April 1975. of the Program," paper presented at the 1975 Conference on Culture and Communications, Fauman, Bruce C., "Determinants of Adolescents' Philadelphia: Temple University (March 1975). Brand Preferences," unpublished thesis,Sloan School of Management, M I T., June 1966. Dunsing, Marilyn, "Spending Money of Adoles- cents," Journal of Home Economics, LII (1960), Federal Corn m un [cations Commission, Federal pp. 756-759 Communications Commission Annual Report, 1973. Dunsing, Marilyn, "Money Management Ex- perience of High School Students,Part I Obtain- Federal Communications Commission, "Children's ing Spending Money," Journalof Home Television ProgramsReport and Policy State- Economics, L1I (1960), pp. 756-759. ment," Federal Register, Vol 39, 215 (November Editor and Publisher, "Teenage Spenders Rely on 6, 1974). Ads in Newspapers," 99 (January 22, 1966), p Federal Communications Commission, "Public 17. Notice: February 23, 1976." Elias, Marilyn, "How to Win Friends and Influence Federal Register, Vol 39, No 134, July II, 1974, April Kids on Television," Human Behavior, pp 25505-25510 1974, pp 16-23. Elias, 'Marilyn, "Pied Pipers of Television The Art Federal Trade Commission, "FTC Explores Children and Advertising," Broadcast Advertising, of Influencing Children on What to Buy," Human November 15, 1971 Behavior Eliot, Thomas, "Money and the Child's Own Stand- Federal Trade Commission, "Food Advertising ard of Living," Journal of Home Economics, Proposed Trade Regulation Rule and Staff State- XXIV (1932), pp 1-9 ment tederal Register, 1974. 39, 218 213 214 Federal Trade Commission, "Final Notice Regard- Flavell, 1 H,The Development Psychology of Jean ing Food Advertising Proposed Trade Regulation Piaget(New York D. Van Nostrand Co., 1963) Rule."Federal Register, 41,42 Fowles, Barbara R., testimony submitted to Sub- Federal Trade Commission,In the Matter of General committee on Communications, House of Repre- Foods Corporation. Complaint Decision and Order sentatives, July 17, 1975. (Docket No. C-2733). 1975. Fowles, Barbara R. Statement presented at Hearings Federal Trade Commission, "Hudson Pharmaceuti- before the Subcommittee on Communications of cal Corp.," FTC Consent Order File No. 762 the Committee on Interstate and Foreign Co-i- 3054, Washington, D.C. (1976). merce, House of Representatives (Serial No. Feldman, Shei and Abraham Wolf, "Parental 94-53). Washington, D.C., Government Printing Preferences Among Regulatory Approaches, and Office, 1975 their Correlates," paper presented at the Fowles, Barbara R., "Moppets in the Marketplace. American Psychological Association, August 30, Evaluating Children's Responses to Television 1975. Advertising," paper presented to the Association Feldman, Shel and Abraham Wolf, "What's Wrong for Consumer Research, Cincinnati, Ohio, with Children's Commercials,"Journal of Adver- November I, 1975 tising Research,Vol14: 9 (February 1974),pp. Fox, Renee C "Illness,"International Encyclopedia 39-43. of the Social Sciences,New York. Macmillan, Feldman, Shel, Abraham Wolf, and Doris War- 1968, Volume 7, pp 90-97 mouth, "Parental Concern about Child-Directed Frideres, James S, "Advertising. Buying Patterns, Commercials,"Journal of Communication, Winter and Children,"Journal of Advertising Research, 1977, Vol. 27, pp. 125-137. 13.1 (February 1973), pp. 34-36. Ferguson, Clara P., "An Investigation of Preadoles- cent Children's Attitudes Toward Television Friedlander, Bernard and HarrietWetstone. "Effects of Informational and Cartoon Program Commercials," unpublished dissertation at North Texas State University, August 1973 Format, MusicalDistractors and Age on Children's Listening to Television Soundtracks." Ferguson, Clara P., "Preadolescent Children's At- New England Instructional Television Research titudes Toward Television Commercials" Center, 1974 (Austin, Texas: Bureau of Business Research, The University of Texas, 1975). Friedlander, Bernard, Harriet Wetstone, and Christopher Scott "Suburban Preschool Feshbach, Norman, Tricia Jordan, and Arline ()In- Children's Comprehension of an Age-appropriate man. "The Nutritional Graphic Preliminary In- Informational Television Program Child vestigation submitted by the Council on Development,1974,45,561-565 Children, Media and Merchandising to the Federal Trade Commission, Wash, D.0 , Octo- Friedrich, Lynette L., "Implications from Research ber 1976 on Prosocial Behavior for Programming and Ad- vertising Directed to Children," remarks to the Feshbach, Norman, T. S Jordan, A. S. Dillman, R Children's Advertising Review Unit Seminar, Choate, S. Feshbach, and M. Zolotow, "The New York City, June 20, 1975 Design of a Graphic to Convey Nutritional Infor- mation to ChildrenPilot Studies." Council on Friedrich, Lynette and Aletha Stein. "Aggressive Children, Media and Merchandising. October andProsocialTelevision Programs and the 1976. NaturalBehavior of Preschool Children." Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Feshbach, Seymour, "Reality and Fantasyin Filmed Development,1973,38, Violence," in John P. Murray. Eli A. Rubinstein 4 and George A. Comstock (Eds ),Television and Galst, Joann and Mary White, "The Unhealthy Per- Social BehaviorVol 2. Television and &Ala! suader. The Reinforcing Value of Television and Learning,Washington, D C U S. Government Children's Purchase-Influencing Attempts at the Printing Office, (1972), pp. 318-345 Supermarket,"Child Development,1976, 47.

214' fl 1 7 A, 1 t.) Gardiner, M., "Mothers' Attitudes Toward Meeting of the Associationfor Consumer Children's Advertising," Sales. Management, July Research, Boston, Mass., November 8-11, 1973. 1971. Goldberg, Marvin E.and GeraldJ.Gorn, Gardner, Robert K., "Replication of 'Children's "Children's Reactions to Television Advertising: Responses to Commercials. A Conjoint An Experimental Approach," Journalof Con- Analysis'," student paper, The Wharton School, sumer Research,1 (September 1974), pp. 69-75. University of Pennsylvania, (1975). Goldberg, Marvin E. and Gerald J. Gorn, "Material Gavian, Ruth, "Children's Experiences Moth vs. Social Preference,Parent-Child Relations, Money," Social Education, II (1938), pp. 166-168. and the Child's Emotional Response:Three Dimensions of Response to Children's TV Adver- Gerbner, George, "Violence in Television Drama. tising," unpublished paper, McGill University, Trends and Symbolic Functions," in George A. Faculty of Management, Montreal, 197¢. Comstock and Eli A. Rubinstein (Eds ), Televi- the sion and Social Behavior VolI. Media Content Goldblith, Samuel, Factors WhiLh Will Influence and Control, Washington, D.C.: U,S. Government Future of the American Food Industry,presented at Printing Office 04972), pp. 28-187. IndustrialLiaison Program Symposium at M I.T., 1976. Gerbner, George and Larry P. Gross, "Violence Profile No. 7: Trends in Network Television and Gordon, Thomas F , "Mass Media and Socializa- Viewer Conceptions of Social Reality tion: Theoretical Approaches," paperpresented 1967-1974," unpublished manuscript, The An- at the International CommunicationAssociation, nenberg School of Communications, University of New Orleans, April 1974 Pennsylvania (1975). Gorn, Gerald J and Marvin E Goldberg,"Televi- Gerbner, George and Larry P Gross; "Living With sion Advertising and Lower IncomeChildren," Television: The Violence Profile," Journal of unpublished paper, McGill University Faculty of, Communication, 26, (Spring 1976), 173-199 Management, Montreal, I 976a. Gertenbach, Robert, AddresS to Children's Review Gorn, Gerald J and Marvin E.Goldberg, Unit Seminar, New York City, June 20, 1975 "Children's TV Commercials and the Hierarchy of Effects Hypothesis," unpublished paper, gibbs, Mary, "Decision-Making Procedures by McGill University, Faculty of Management, Young Consumers," Journal of Home Economics, Montreal, 1976b. 55 (May 1963), pp 359-360 Gorn, Gerald J. and Marvin E. Goldberg, "A Gilbert, Eugen.?, Advertising and Marketing to Young Research Program Investigating the Effects of People (Pleasantville, New YorkPrinters' Ink Television Commercials on Children in Canada," Books, 1957). working paper presented to the Standing Commit- Gilbert, Eugene, "Why Today's Teenagers Seem So tee on Broadcasting Films, and Assistance tothe Different," Harper's Magazine, 219 (November Arts, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada, June 12, 1973. 1959), pp. 76-79 Goslin, David A., Handbook of SccializationTheory Gilkison, Paul "What Influences the Buying Deci- and Research (Chicago Rand McNally and Co , sion of Teenagers9", Journal ofRetailing, 41 (Fall 1971) 1965.), pp. 33-41. Goulart, Ron, The Assault of Childhood (Los Goldberg, Marvin E and Gerald J Gorn, "An Ex- Angeles, CalitSherbourne Press, Inc ,1969). perimental Approach to the Effects of Television Advertising on Children," in S. F Divita, Adver- Green, Frederick C . testimony betore the Federal tising and the Public Interest(Chicago Trail* Commission, Nov ember 12, 1971 American Marketing Association,1974), pp Greenberg, Bradley S ,Philip M Ericson, and 149-159 Mantha Vlahos. ''Children s Television Behaviors as Perceived by Mother and Child," in Goldberg, Marvin Eand Gerald J Gorn, "Children's Reactions to Television Advertising lelevision and So( ral Behavior, Volume IV, ed. for Toys," paper presented to the Fourth Annual Eli A Rubinstein, et al, pp395-409

215 216 Greenberg, Bradley S. and B. Reeves, "Children prepared for the Ford Foundation Seminar, Dec and the Perceived Reality of Television," Journal 17-19, 1973. of Social Issues. (in press). Gussow, Joan, "The Empty Calories and Greyser, Stephen A, "Imtation in Advertising," Boom," Madison Avenue, March 1975. Journal of Advertising Research, Vol 13 (Febru- ary 1973), 1-8 Haefner, James E, John B. Leckenby, and Steven L Goldman, "The Measurement of Advertising Im- Griffin,Emilie, "The Children's Advertising pact on Children," paper presented at the Review Unit," remarks before the National American Psychological Association, August 30 Association of Broadcasters, Children's ,Televi- 1975. sion Workshop, Washington, D.C., June 3, 1975. Haefner, James E., and Steven E Permut, "An Ap- Griffin, Emilie, "What's Fair to Children? The proach to the Evaluation of Deception in Televi- Policy Need for New Research on Children's Per- sion Adikertising," Journal of Advertising, 3, (4), ceptions of Advertising Content." Jourial of Ad- 1974. vertising, 1976, 5, 2. Hagen,). W., "The Effect of Distraction on Selec- Grojean, Patricia Jo and A. Johnston. "Children's tive Attention," Child Development, Vol. 38 Influence on Family Food Purchase Decision," (1967), pp. 685-694. Bulletin 671, Agricultural Experiment Station, Mississippi State College, Mississippi, September Hagen, J. W. and C. A. Hale, **The Development of 1963. Attention in Children," in A. D. Pick (ed.), Min- nesota Symposium on Child Psychology, Vol. 7, Grojean, Patricia Jo, "Monetary Experiences and Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press Consumer Practices of Your Children," Masters (1973). Thesis, Oklahoma State University, May 1972. Hanson, Rose L., "A Study of Children's Use of Gronhaug, Mjell, "Advertising and Children in a Money," University of Iowa Studies Culture Without TV Commercials," paper pre- inChild Welfare, VIII (1933), pp. 221-247. sented atthe Third Annual Conference in Research in Marketing, Brussels, June 1974. Hawkins, Del I. and C. A. Coney, "Peer Group In- fluences on Children's Product Preferences," Gruenberg, Sidonie M. and B.C.Gruenberg, Parents, Children. trod Money (New York. Viking Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, II Press, 1933) (Spring 1974), pp. 322-331. Guest, Lester P, "The Genesis of Brand Aware- Helitzer, Melvin L. and Carl Heyel, The Youth ness," Journal of Applied Psychology, 2& (1942), Market (Media Books, 1970). pp 800-808. Heller, Melvin S. and Samuel Polsky, Studiesin Guest, Lester P., "Brand LoyaltyTwelve Years Violence and Television, New York. American Later," Journal of Applied Psychology, Vol. 39 Broadcasting Company (1976). (1955), pp 405-408. Hendon, Donald W., "The Effects of TV Commer- Guest, Lester P ; "A Longitudinal Study of Attitude cials on 'Normal' and Mentally Retarded Development and Some Correlates," Chile children:: paper presented at American Development, 35 (1964), pp 770-784. Academy of Advertising Conference, Tempe, Gussowt Joan, "It Makes Even Milk a DessertA Arizona, March 1973. Report on the Counter Nutritional Messages bf Hermann, Robert C. (ed.), The Consumer Behavior of Children's Television Advertising," Clinical Children and Teenagers: An Annotated Bibliogra- Pediatrics, No, 12, pp 68-71 phy (Chicago, Ill.: American Marketing Associa- Gussow, Joari, "Counternutritional Messages of TV tion, 1969). Ads Aimed at Children," Journal of Nutrition Education, 4 2 (1972), pp 48-52 Reslop, Louise, "A Laboratory Experimentto Study the Effects of Premium Advertising on Children's Gussow, Joan, "Children and Television. What do Desire for a Cereal and on Purchase Outcome," a' We Know? What Do We Need to Know?",paper research proposal, October 21, 1974.

216 "1,, 1"" Hess, Robert 0. and H Goldman, "Parent's Views Jacoby,J.and D B.Kyner, "Experimentally of the Effects of Television on Their Children," Validating a Conceptualization of Brand Child Development, 33 (1962), pp. 411-426 Loyalty," paper in Consumer PsycholOgy, P due University, paper #115, 1972. Himmelweit, Hilde T., "The Social Cost of Adver- tising," 'a proposal to study the effects of TV com- Jacoby, J. and David B. Lymer, "Brand Lo ty vs. ,met,cials and children's programming on Repeat Purchase Behavior," Journal of Marketing children, preparedforthe Ford Foundation Research, 10'(February 1973), pp. 1-9. Seminar, December 18019, 1973. Ja by, J., Jerry Olson, George Szybillo, and Ed- Hollander, S. W. and JJacoby, "Recall of 4._:azy ward Hart. Affirmative Nutritional Disclosure id_ Mixed-Up Commercials," Journal of Advertising Advertising and Selective Alternatives: The Likely Research, Vol. 13, 3(1973), pp. 39-42 Impact on Consumers. Consumer Research In- stitute, Inc., 1975. Horn, Thomas D. and L. Miller, "Children's Con cepts Regarding Debt," Elementary School Jour- James, Don L., "Youth, Media, and Advertising," nal, 55 (March 1955), pp. 406-412. Studies. in Marketing, No. 15, Bureau of Business Research, University of Texas at Austin, 1971. H.R 8613, Bill introduced to House of Representa- tives 7/14/75 to create a National Council on Jennings, Ralph, "Programming and Advertising Children and Advertising by Representative Practices in Television Directed to Children," - Wirth(D-Colorado). Action for Children's Television (Newton, Mass.: 1972). _Howard, John A. and James Hulbert, "Advertising and the Public Interest," a staff report to the Jennings, Ralph and r Jennings, "Programming Fed Trade Commission, Washington, D.C., and Advertising Practices in Television Directed 19*. to ChildrenArother Look," Action for Children's Television (Newton, Mass.). Howaid, John A., James Hulbert, and Donald R. Lehmann, "An Exploratory Analysis of the Effect Jerome, Norge W., "Children's Television Advertis- of Television Advertising on Children,' working ing: A Vehicle for Pre-Social Nutritional paper (n.d.). Messages," remarks to the Children's Advertising Review Unit Seminar, June 20, 1975, New York Hulbet, James, "Applying Elteer Behavior Analysis City. to Social Problejny. The Case of Drug Use," in Proceedings of tO American Marketing Associa- Johnson, Nicholas, "Bringing Home the Bacon i 1 tion, ed. Ronal C. Curhan, 1974, pp. 289-292. Chlosterol-Conscious Age," speech to the G-o- cery Manufacturers of America, Washington, Hulse, Stewart ft, James Deese, and Howard Egeth, D.C., September 17, 1971. The Psychology of Learning 4th edition, New, York: McGraw-Hill (1975). Johnson, Nicholas, Testimony before Senate Sub- committee on the Effect of Promotion and Adver- Hyams, Larry. Shari Tanner, and John R. Rossiter, tising of Over-the-Counter Drugs, September 22, "Effects of Race Models on Children's Product 1971. Preferences," paper presented at the 1975 Con- ference on Culture and Communications, Johnson, Nicholas, "Rx for Children's Television," Philadelphia, Temple University (March 1975). speech to second national Action for Children's Television Symposium on children and television, Instructor, "Kids Don't Buy Ads," 82 (August - ".-, - Chicago, III., October 1971. tember 1972), p. 28. Maturity. A Study Irwin, Mac, 'il,s.'our Right to Communicate Being Kagen, J. and 14. A. Moss, Birth to in Psychological Development, New York: John Threatened?", speech presented to the Associa- tion of Canadian Advertisers, May 5,975. Wiley and Sons, Inc., 1962. Iskoe, Andrew, "Advertising Via Famous Per- Kalman, Herbert C.' "Compliance, Identification, sonalities and the Effects on Children,' student and Internalization: T)ree Processes of Opinion paper, The Wharton School, University of Penn- Change," Journal of-Conflict Resolution, Vol. 2 sylvania (March 1976). (1958), 51 -60. 2

217 2 1 b Kane, Thomas R,Joanne M Joseph and J. T. Kohlberg, L, "The Cognitive-Developmental Ap- Tedeschi, "Person Perception and the Berkowitz proach to Socialization," in D. Goslin (ed.), Paradigm for the Study of Aggression,"Journal of Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research - Personality and Social Psycholovv,33,(1976), (Chicago Rand McNally and Co, 1971). 666-673. I Kolman, A. S,"Cereal Advertising A Study in Kanter, Donald L, :Pharmaceutical Advertising Family Policy," paper presented at the meeting of and Youth," unpublished paper, Coronado the Midwest Sdciological Society, Omaha, Neb., Unified School District, Coronado, California, April 1974. 1970. Krugman, Herbert, "The 'Draw a Super Market' Katzman, Nathan, "King Audience," in George Technique,"Public Opinion Quarterly,24 (Spring Comstock (ed.),Television and Human Behavior 1960), pp. 148-149. (forthcoming). Krugman, Herbert, "The Impact of Television Ad- Kay, Herbert, "Children's Responses to Advertising. vertising: Learning Without Involvement,"Public Who's Really to Blame?",Journal of Advertising, Opinion Quarterly,29 (Fall 1965), pp. 349-356. 1,1 (1974). Krugman, Herbert, "Processes Underlying Ex- Kaye, Evelyn,The Family Guide to Children's Televi- posure to Advertising."American Psychologist, sion(New York: Panthenoil 1974). 1968,23 Keiser, Stephen K, "Awareness of Brands and Kyner, David, Jacob Jacoby, and Robert Chestnut, Slogans,"Journal of Advertising Research,15, 4 "Dissonance Resolution by Grade School Con- (August 1974), pp. 37-43. sumer," Purdue University, 1974. Keiser, Stephen K. and P. G. Kuehl, "Social Class LaMothe, William E., testimony of the Kellogg and Income Influeneet on External Search Proc- Company before the United States Senate Select esses of Adolescents,"Proceedings,Third Annual Committee on Nutrition and Human Needs, Conference, Association for Consumer Research, March 12, 1973. 1973, pp. 602-631. Laurendeau, M. and A. Pinard,Causal Thinking in Keniston, K "Youth: A 'New' Stage of Life," the Child(New York. International University American Scholar,39 (Autumn19' 0),pp. Press, 1962). 631-654, Laurie, Liz, "Measuring Commercial Impact,"Jour- Kenkel, W. F., "Family Interaction in Decision- nal of Advertising Research,15, 4 (August 1975), Making on Spending," in N. Foote (ed.),House- pp. 23-25. hold Decision-Making,4 (New York: New York University Press, 1961). Lefkowitz, M. M., L. D. Eron, L. 0. Warder, and L. R. Huesmann. "Television Violence and Child King, Martha B.,Money-Management: Children's Aggression: A Follow-Up Study," in Com- Spending(Chicago: Household Finance Corpora- stock and E. Rubinstein (e&.).,Television and 1 1946}. Social Behavior, Volume III.Televiston and Klapper, Hope L., "k2hildren's Perceptions of Adolescent Aggressiveness.Washington.,J.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, Television as A Function of Cognitive Stage. A 1972, 35-135. Preliminary Inquiry," unpublished paper, March 10, 1974. Lehmann, Dona:d R., "An Exploratory Analysis of .the Effect of Television Advertisingon Klemensberg, Peter, "Advertising to Children and Children," working paper, Columbia University, Information Processing," MBA dissertation, Graduate School of Business, 1971. McGill University, April 1974. Leifer, Aimee 0., "An Examination of the Socializ- Knowles, William S., "The Effectiveness of Ter.: vi- ing Influence of Television in the United States," sion Advertising on the Child Consumer," un- paper presented to the Prix .leunesse Seminar, published paper, Southern Methodist University, "Television and Processes of Socialization Within Fall 1971. the Family," Germany, June 1975.

218 a Liefer, Aimee, Neal J. Gordon, and Sherry! Browne Liebert, Robert M. and R W Poulos, "Television Graves, "Children's Televisrin More than Mere and Personality Development The Socializing Entertainment,"Harvard Educational Review.44 Effects of An Entertainment Medium," in A. (1974), pp. 213-245 Davids (ed.),Child Personality and Psy- gi chopathology. Current Topics,Vol 11 (New York Liefer, Aimee and Donald F Roberts "Children's Wiley, 1975). Responses to Television Violence" in John P. Murray, Eli A. Rubinstein, and George A. Com- Liebert, Robert, Stephen Scheutz, and Joyce stock (eds.),Television and Social Behavior, Vol 2, Sprafkin.Spot Messages Appearing Within Satur- Television and Social Learning,Washington, D C.: day MorningTelevisionPrograms. A Content U.S. Government Printing Offic, (1972), pp Analysis.Written record presented at Hearings 43-180. before the Subcommittee on Communications of the Committee on Interstate and Foreign Com- Lesser, GeraldChildren and TelevisionLessons merce, House of Representatives (Serial No. from Sesame Street ,:ewYork: Random House, 94-53). Washington, D.C., Government Printing 1974. Office, 1975. Lesser, Gerald S., "Nutrition in Children's Televi- Liebert, Robert M., J. N. Sprafkin, and R. W, sion Advertising," remarks to the.Children's Ad- Poulos, "Selling Cooperation to Children," in W. vertising Review Unit Seminar, June 20, 1975, S. Hald (ed.),Proceedings of the 20th Annual Ad- New York City. vertising Research Foundation Conference(New Levin, Eric, "The Violence Dropouts,"TV Guide, York: Advertising Research Foundation, 1975), (January I, 1977), 4, 6-8. pp. 54-57. Levin, Stephen andDaniel Anderson. "The Liefield, John P. and F. Norsworthy, "The Influence Development of Attention."Journal of Com- of Television Advertising on Children Compared munication,1976,26,2. to Peer and Parental Influence," unpublished paper, University of Guelph, Ontario, Canada, Lewis, Charles E. (M.D.) and Mary Ann Lewis, 1974. "The Impact of Television Comrn'ercials on Health-Related Beliefs and Behaviors of Liefield, John P. et al. "Television Advertising and Children,"Pediatrics,3 (March 1974). Children- An ExperimentalStudy," working paper, University of Guelph, Ontario, Canada, Liebert, Robert M., testimony before the subcom- 1974. mittee on Communications of the House Commit- tee on Interstate and Foreign Commerce, July 16, Lather, Ned, "Television Advertising and Its Psy- 1975. chological Effects on Children," statement ap- yended to Seymour Banks' statement before the Liebert, Robert M. and Robert A. Baron, "Short - Federal Trade Commission, October 28, 1971. Term Effects of TelevisedAggression on Children's Aggressive Behavior," in John P Mur- Locker, Aaron M., statement to the House Com- ray, Eli A. Rubinstein and George A Comstock munications Subcommittee from the Toy (eds.),Television and Social Learning, Manufacturers of America, Inc., July 16, 1975. Washington, D.C., U.S. Government Printing Lovett, Robert, E. Barker, and B. Marcus, "The Office, (1972), pp. 181-201. Effects ora Nutrition Education Program at the Journal of Nutrition Educa- Liebert, Robert M., D. E. Liebert, J. N. Sprafkin, Second Grade Level," tion,Vol. 2, No. 2, Supplement 1 (Fall 1970), pp. and E. A. Rubinstein, "Effects of Tele,,ision Commercial Disclaimers on the Product Expecta- 81-95. tions of Children," unpublished paper, Brookdale Lyle, Jack, "Research on Television Advertising 'International Institute for Applied Studies in thr- and Children," paper prepared for the Ford- Mental Health Sciences, Stony Brook, N.Y., Foundation, August 1973. 1976. Lyle, Jack and Heidi R Hoffman, "Children's Use Liebert, Robert M., J M. Neale, and E. S. David- of Television and Other Media," inEliA son,The Early Window. Effects of Television on '' Rubinstein etal.(eds.),Television and Social Children aid Youth(New York' Pergamcr Press, BehaviorVol. 4 (Washington, D.C.: Government Inc., 1973). Printing Office, 1972), pp 129-256. 219 2204 Lyle, Jack and Heidi R Hoffman, "Explorations in Mayer, Jean (ed ) US Nutrition Policies in the Sev- Patterns of Television Viewing by Preschool-age enties San Francisco. W H. Freeman and Co.. Children," inEli A Rubinstein etal. (eds ), 1973 Television and Social Behavior. Vol4, pp 257-273 McArthur, Leslie Z and Susan V Eisen, "Televi- sion and Sex-Role Stereotyping," unpublished Lubin, Joanne S, "From Bugs to 6atman, paper, Waltham, MassBrandeis University Children's TV Shows Produce Adult Anxiety," (n d ). The Wall Street Journal (0cLober 19, 1976), 1,37 McCarthy, Charlotte, J, "An Exploratory Study of Maccoby, Eleanor, "Role-Taking in Childhood and the Monetary Concepts of Preschool Children," its Consequences for Social Learning," Child unpublishedmaster'sthesis, Oklahoma State Devel. .17t, 30 (1959), pp 210-252 University Maccoby, Eleanor, The Effects of Television on McCarthy, Elizabeth D, Thomas SLangner, Children," in W Schramm (ed.), The Science of Joanne C. Gersten, Jeanne G. Eisenberg. and Human Communu tion (Nev. York Basic Books, Lida Orzack, "Violence and Behavior Disor- 1963), pp. 116-128 ders," Journal of Communication, 25, (Autumn 1975 ), 71-85. Maccoby, E E and JW. Hagen, "Effects of Dis- traction upon Central versus Incidental Recall McGannon, Donald M, Statement before the Sub- Developmental Trends," Journal of Experimental committee on CoLimunications, House Commit- Child Psychology, Vol 2, (1965), pp. 280-289 tee on Interstate and Foreign Commerce, July 14, Marshall, Hplen R., "Pifferences in Parent and 1975 Child Reports of the Child's Experience in the McGuire, William J"Persuasion, Resistance, and Use of Money," Journal of Educational Psy- Attitude Change," inI SPool etal. (Eds ) chology, 54 (1963), pp 132-137 Handbook of Commu. .cation, Chicago. Rand-

Marshall, Helen R, "The Relation of Giving McNally, (1973), 216-252 Children An Allowance to Children's Money McIntyre, Jennie and J. JTeevan, Jr., "Television Knowledge and Responsibility to Other Practices Violence andDeviant of Parents," Journal of Genetic Psychology, 104 Behavioi-,"inE A. Rubinstein et (1964), pp 35-51 al.(eds.),Television and Social Behavior, Vol 3 (Washington, D.C. Government Marshall, Helen R and L. Magruder, "Relations Printing Office, 1972), pp. 383-435. Between Parent Money Education Practices and Children's Knowledge and Use of Money," Child McLeod, Jack J. and Steven R Chaffee, "The Con- Development, 31 (1960), pp 253-284. struction of Social Reality," in J. T Tedeschi, ed, The Social Influence ProcessChicagoAldine Martin, Charles H, "The Berne Model and Adver- P Atherton Publishing, 1972, pp. 50-99. tising Messages A Psychographic' Study," paper presented at Association for Education In Jour- McLuhan, Marshall, Understanding Media. The Ex- nalism conventioninOttawa, August16-19, tensions of Man New York McGraw-Hill Book 1975 Company, 1964 Martin, Cora and L Benson, "Parental Perceptions McNeal, James U, "Childrenas Consumers," of the Role of Television in Parent-Child Interac- Bureau of Business Research, University of Texas tion," Journal of Marriage and the Family. 32 at Austin, 1964 (August 1970). pp 410-414 McNeal, James U, "The Child Consumer: A New Masters, John C , Robert F C.ordon, and Lawrence Market," Journal of Retailing, 40 (1969), pp. V Clark, "Eftects of Self-Dispensed and Exter- 15-22. nally Dispensed Model Consequences on Ac- quisition, Spontaneous and Oppositional Imita- McNeal, James U, "An Exploratory Study of the tion, and Long-Term Retention," Journal of Per- Consumer Behavior of Children" in McNeal sonality and Social Psychology,33, (19'6), (ed.), Dimensions of Consumer Behavior (New 421-430. York Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1969)

C) , ) 220 l, vr...../-4- Media Decorous, "Children's TV, Calm Before the Mori.,, Norman S lelevoton's (held (Toronto, ,,Storml" (August 1974), p 48 Canada. Little, Brown, and Co , 1971) Melody, William H "Children's Television Moskin, Robert (Ed ). The Case for Advertising Economics and Public Policy," Action for firviitrKittsofthe Industrl 'Prekrutationto the Children's Television, Newton, Mass , 1973 federal Irade (ommrssion New \ ork\A AA Melody, William H , Cioldren's 1 the E«momics 1973 of Esplortation (New Haven, Conn Yale Univer- Munn, Mark, "The Effect of Parental Busing Habits sity Press, 1973) on Children Exposed to Children'sTelevision Melody, William Hand Wendy Ehrlich, Programs," Journal of &or:drawing 2 (1958), pp "Children's TV Commercials The Vanishing 253-258 Policy Options," Journal of Communication, 24 4 (Autumn 1974), pp 113-125 Murray, John P , "Television in Inner-City Homes Viewing Behavior of Young Boys,-in F A Milaysky, JRonald , Berton Pekowsky and Hurst Rubinstein et al(eds ),leletoion and Human Stipp, "TV Drug Advertising and Proprietary Behavior, Vol 4 (Washington, D C eminent and Illicit Drug Use Among Teenage Boys." Printing:01u, 1972). pp 345-394 PublicOpinion Quarterly, Volume 39, Winter 1975-7'6, pp 457-481 Myers, John G , "Communication Models and Ad- vertising Regulation" Berkeley Institute of Busi- Milkovich, Mark and M Miller, "Effects of Televi- Universityof sion Advertising on ChildrenExploringthe ness and Economic Research, Relationship Between Television Viewing and California, May 1972) Language Development," Report #3, Michigan Myers, John G"Advertising and Socialization, "* State University, July 1975 draftpreparedforRussell Sage Foundation Miller, Donald, "Remarks Regarding Children's Social Science Frontiers Series, September 1973 Advertising," testimony before the Federal Trade Commission, November 10, 1971 Myers, L , Jr,"Plan 12Relation of Personality to Perception of Television Ads ertising Messages, Moleski, Leo, "Children's TV Advertisements A in L Arons and M A May (eds ),Ielevoion and Brief Review," paper presented at the American Human Behaviorlomorrme' Res ear, h m %hos Psychological Association, August 30, 1975 Copununuritions (New York Appleton-('entury Moore, Denise F ,"Sharing in Family Financial Crofts, 1963), pp 202-218 Management by High School Students," Marriage Murray, John P()gut B Nay man, and ( harks K 15 (November 1953), pp and Family Living, Atkin, "Television and the C hild A Comprehen- 319-321 sive Research Bibliography," Journal ofBroad- Moore, Roy L , "A Cross-Sectional Analysis of casting, XVI, 1 (Winter 1971-72) Consumer Learning Among Younger vs `Older Adverto- Adolescents," unpublished Ph.D' dissertation, National Advertising Division Ch ldren' Better Busi- University of Wisconsin, 1974 mg Guidelines, New York, Council of ness Bureaus, June 1975 Moore, Roy L and L. F Stephens, "Some Com- munication and Demographic Determinants of National Association of Broadcasters, Thefe/ Consumer Learning Among Older and Younger sum Ode, 17th edition, April 1973,18th c Adolescents," paper presented to the Mass Com- June 1975 munications Division of the International Com- National Association of Broadcasters.Advertising municationAssociation, New Orleans,April GuidelinesChildren's TV Advertising New 1974 York NAB, January 1, 1976 Moore, Roy L G P Moschis, and L F Stephens, "An 'Exploratory Study of Consumer Role Per- National Association of Broadcasters, 'Children's ceptions in Adolescent Consumer Socialization," TV Advertising Statement of Principles." (ode paper presented to International Communication Nem, The National Association of Broadcasters. Association, Chicago, April 23-26, 1975 Volume 7, Ns6, June 1974

221 222 Nelson, Bardin H, "Role of Money in Rural Family Olsen, C. N P J. Tichenor, and G. A. Donohue, "A Living," Bulletin 979, texas Agricultural Ex- Systems Evaluation of a Purposive Message The periment Station. Texas A&M College, College 'Mulligan Stew' ETC Protect," report submitted Station. Texas, June 1961 to Agricultural Extension Service, Institute of Nev lilt, G ,The Impact of Television Advertising Agriculture, Foi estry, and Home Economics, on Children's Food Opinions and Practices An University of Minnesota, March 1975 Exploratory Study," unpublished maste-'s thesis, Oppenhelm, Irene G., The Teen-Age Consumer," Cornell University. 1973 in Educating the Teen-Ager in Human Relations A C Nielsen Co, "The Eftects of Telev ision Upon and Management of Resources (Washington, Children," confidential report, 1974. D C.. American Home Economies Association, 1965),-pp 17-35 A C Nielsen Co, The Television Audience197.1 Chicago A'C Nielsen Company, 1975 (series of Oppenheim, Irene, The Family as Consumers (New annual volumes): York The Macmillan Co.. 1969). Nissenson, Marilyn, "Report on the Seminar on Oxtoby-Smith, Inc, Why Do American Youth Use II- Children and Television Ads ertising," report to lui' Drugs'', Supplement No 4 to the April 1971 the Ford Foundation, February 1974 Report New York, 1974 . Grant. "Eftects of Different Forms of Filmed Palmer*: Edward. "Formative Research in Educa- Aggression on Children's Constructive and tional Television Production The Experience of DestructivePlay," Journal of Personality and the Children's Television Workshop." inW. Social Psychologs. 26. (1973), 54-59 Schramm (ed.), Quality in Instructional Television. University Press of Hawaii, 1972. Norman. D A, Memors and Attention An Introduc- tion to Human Information Pun essing, (New York Palmer, Edward L, "Formative Research lo the Production of Television for Children," offprint John Wiley and Sons. Inc.1967) from the seventy-third yearbook of the National Odom, R D"Effects of Perceptual Salience on a Society for the Study of Education, 1974 Recall of Relevant and Incidental Dimensional V alues A Developmental Study" Journal of Es- Parsons, Talcott and R F Bales, family, Socializa- perimental Psw Vol92, (1972). pp tion, and Interaction Process (Glencoe, Ill.The 285-291 Free Press, 1955) Odom. R. Dand D W Corbin, "Perceptual Parsons. T, R. F Bales. and E A Stills, Working Salience and Children's Multi-Dimensional Papo in the Theory of Action. (Glencoe, III.. the Problem Solving," Child Development. Vol 44 Free Press, 1953) (1973). pp 425-432 Pearce. Alan, "The Economics of Network Odom, R. D and R D Guzman, "Development of Children's Television Programming," staff Hierarchies of Dimensional Salience," Develop- reports submitted to Federal Communications mental Psychology, Vol 6 (1972), pp 271-287, Commission, July 1972. Odom, R D and D. C MuiWoz,,ier, "Dimensional Pearce, Alan, "The Economics of Children's Televi- Salience and Identification of the Relevant sion An Assessment of the Impact of a Reduction Dimension in Problem Solving A Developmental in the Amount of Advertising," staff report for Study." Developmental Psychology, Vol 1., the Federal Communications Commission, June (1971), pp 135-140 1974. Oiemann. Ralph,"Vi,hatMoney Means to the Phelan, Gladys K and J. Schuaneveldt, "Spending Child," University oflotta StudiesinChild and Saving Patterns of Adolescent Siblings," Welfare, XX':1933 Journal of Home Economics, 61(1969),pp. 104-109. O'Kelly, Charlotte G "Sexism in Children's. Televi- sion." Journalism Quarterly. Winter 1974, pp Philip, J. M., the Child's World and Television Ad-

722-724- vertising (Toronto. Canada AIM Ltd,1971). Piaget, Jean, The Psychology of Intelligence (London. Rao, T., "Television Advertising and its Effects on Rauthledge and Kegan Paul Ltd., 1950). Children," University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, in progress. Piaget, lean, The Origins of Intelligence in Children (New YorkInternational University Press, Ray, Michael L. "Marketing Communication and 1952) the Hierarchy of Effects," in P Clarke (ed.) New Models for Mass CommunicationResearch, Piaget, Jean, The Child's Conception of the World Beverly Hills: Sage (1973). (Totowa, N.J Littlefield, Adams, and Co., .1965). Reilly Group, Inc., The Gene, "Assumption by the Child of the Role of Consumer," The Child, Piaget, Jean, The Psychology of the Child (New York. Volume IJuly I973a. Basic Books, 1969). Reilly Group, Inc., The Gene, "Meals and Snacking Piaget, Jean and B I nhelder, The Growth of Logical The Child and What He Eats,"The Child, Thinking from Childhood to Adolescence, trans- Volume 2, December 1972b lated by A Parsons and S Seagrin (New York Basic Books, 1958) Reilly Group, Inc., The Gene, "The Child, the Media, and the Message," The Child, Volume 3, Pick, Ann D ,D. G Frankel, and V. LHess, September 1974. "Children's AttentionThe Development of Selectivity," Review of Child Development Reilly Group, Inc., The Gene "The Child's Private Research, 5 (1974), pp 82-89. World of Leisure," The Child, Volume 4, April 1975 Pierce, F. N , L J Hooper, and J D Culley, "Per- ceptions of Television Advertising Directed at Reisman, David and H. Roseborough, "Careers and Children An Investigation of the Views of an En- Consumer Behavior," in L. H Clark, ed., Con- tire Community," paper presented at the meeting sumer Behavior, Vol II: The Life Cycle and Con- of the Association for Education in Journalism, sumer Behavior (New York New York Univer- San Diego, Calif , August 1914 sity Press, 1955), pp. 1-18.

Potter, David M , People of Plenty, Economic' Abun- Roberts, Donald F., "Communications and dance and the American Character (Chicago, Ill Children. A Developmental Approach," in I. de University of Chicago Press, 1954). Sola Pool and W. Schramm, edsHandbook of Communication (ChicagoRand-McNally Col- Poulos, Rita Wicks, "Unintentional Negative Effects lege Publishing Co., 1973), pp 174-215 of Food Commercials on ChildrenA Case Public Study," Media Action Research Center, 1975 Roberts, Donald F , "One Highly Attracted Young Persons,"inGeorge Comstock (ed.), Powell, Kathryn S. and D A Goi.er, "The Adoles- Television and Human Behavior (forthcoming) cent as a ConsumerFacts and Implications," Robertson, Thomas S , "The Impact of Televlsion Viiirricige and Family Li mg, 25 (August 1963), pp. Advertising on Children," Wharton Quarterly, 7 359-364 (Fall 1972), pp. 38-42. Preston, (van L., The Great American Blow-Up Puff- Robertson, Thomas S and Shel Feldman, "C'hildren ery inAdvertising and Selling, Madison, Wis as Consumers. The Need for Multitheoretical University of Wisconsin Press (1975) Perspectives," in Beverlee B Anderson (ed ), Ad- vances in Consumer Research, Vol. III, Proceed- Prevney, Esther E., "A QuantitativeStudy of Family Practices in Train;ng Children in the Use of ings, Association for Consumer Research, (1975), Money," Journal of Educ at tonal Psychology, 36 pp 508-512. (1945), pp 411-428 Robertson, Thomas S.and John R. Rossiter, "Children and Commercial Persuasion. An At- Proprietary Association. "Comments of the Proprie- tribution Theory Analysis," Journal of Consumer tary Association in Opposition to the Proposed Research,I (June 1974), pp. 13-20. Rule to Prohibit Advertising of Drugs on Televi- sion Before 9 P M Before the Federal Com- Robertson, Thomas S. and John R Rossiter, "The munications Commission, Washington, D.0 Impact of Advertising on Children Long-Term August 19,) and Campaign Effects," draft, July 16, 1974.

223 22 4 Robertson. Thomas S and John R. Rossiter, "The Cognition and Attitude," doctoral dissertatioa, Effects of Concentrated Television Advertising Annenberg School of Communications, Univer- on Children's Toy and Game Choices," un- sity of Pennsylvania, 1974 published paper, February 1975. Rossiter, John.R , "Visual and Verbal Memory in Robertson, Thomas Sand John RRossiter, Children's Product Information Utilization," "Children's Consumer Disappointment," working paper presented at the Association for Consumer paper, Center for Research on Media and Research Conference, Cincinnati, Ohio, October Children, University of Pennsylvania, April 1975. 1975. Rossiter, John R., "Reliability of a Short Test Robertson, Thomas Sand John RRossiter, Measuring Children's Attitudes Toward TV "Maturational and Social Factors in Children's Commercials," Journal of Consumer Research, (in Understanding of TV Commercials," paper pre- press). sentee at the American Psychological Associa- tion, August 30. ;975 Rossiter, John R. and Thomas S Robertson, "The ReceptionYielding Paradigm in Children's Robertson, Thomas S and John RRossiter, "At- Susceptibility to Television Advertising," un- titude Theory and Children's Consumer published manuscript, Center for Research on Behavior." draft for AMA Attitude Research Media and Children, University of Pennsylvania, Conference, Hilton Head, South Carolina, Febru- 1974 ary 1976. Rossiter, John Rand Thomas Robertson, Robertson. Thomas S and John R Rossiter, "Short- "Children's TV CommercialsTesting the Run Advertising Effects on Children A Field Defenses," Journal of Communications (Autumn Study," Journal of Marketing Research, 13 (Febru- 1974), pp 137-145. ary I976a), 68, 70 Rossiter, John R and Thomas Robertson. "Canoni- Robertson, Thomas Sand John R Rossiter, "Children's Consumer Satisfaction," A working cal Analysis of Develcpmental, Social, and Ex- paper, Center for Research on Media and perimental Factors in Children's Comprehension Children, University of Pennsylvania, I976b of Television Advertising," Journal of Genetic Psychology 129 (1976), 317-327. Robertson. Thomas R. and John R.Rossiter, "Stimulus and Dispositional Variables in Rossiter, John Rand Thomas Robertson, Children's Responses to Advertising," Journal of "Children's Television Viewing An Examination Communication, in press, 1976c. of Parent-Child Consensus," So( unnetry, 39 (Sep- tember 1975), 308-326. Robertson, ThomasS.,JohnR.Rossiter, and Deborah Brenner, "Children's Responses to Rubin, Ronald S , "An Exploratory Investigation of Commercials. A Conjoint Analysis," working Children's Responses to Commercial Content of paper, The Wharton School, University of Penn- Television Advertising in Relaticto their Stages sylvania (1975) of Cognitive Development,' unpublished doc- toral dissertation submitted to the University of Robinson, Helen F, "Money Concepts in the Kin- Massachusetts, 1972 dergarten," Journal of Nursery Education, XIX

( I 964), pp 73-77 Rubenstein, Eli A, "Remarks on Pro-Social Rogerson, Lonnie C. and E B Whitford, "Money Research in Children's Television," remarks to Experience of Ninth-Grade Pupils," Journal 01 the Children's Advertising Review Unit Seminar, Home Economic 8, 52 (1960), pp. 44-45. June 20, 1975, New York City

Roper Organization, Inc, "Trends in Public At- Rust, Langhourne and Thomas Watkins, titudes Toward Television and Other Mass "Children's Commercials Creative Develop- Media. 1959-1974 New York. Television Infor- ment," Journal of Advertising Research, 1975, 15, mation Office, April 1975. 5 Rossiter, John R ,"Children's Susceptibilityto Salomon, Gavr lel, "Cognitive Skill Learning Across Television Advertising A Behavioral Test of Cultures," Journal of Ccmrunuation, 1976, 26, 2,

224 2 Sarson, Evelyn (ed.), Action for Children's Televi- Shimp, Terenace, R Dyer, and S. Divita, "Advertis- sion, The First National Symposium on the Effects ing of Children's Premiums on Television An Ex- of Television Progitimming and Advertising on perimental Evaluation of the F.T.C.'s Proposed Children (New York: Avon Books, 1971) Guide," 1975. Schneider, John A., statement in behalf of the CBS Siegel, Alberta E., "Film-Mediated Fantasy Aggres- Broadcast Group before the Subcommittee on sion and Strength of Aggressive Drive," Child Communications, House Interstate and Foreign Development, 27 (1956), 365-378. Commerce Committee, July 17, 1975 Singer, J. L. -The- Influence of Violence Portrayed Schramm, Wilbur, "What the Research Says," in W. in Television or Motion Pictures on Aggressive Schramm (ed.), Quality in Ingitictional Television. Behanor," in J. L Singer (ed.), The Control of University Press of Hawaii, 1972. Aggression and Violence Cognitive and Physiologi- cal Factors (New York Academic Press, 1971) Schramm, W., J. Lyle, and E. B. Parker, Television in the Lives of Our Children (Stanford, Calif. Singer, Jerome L., "Imaginative Play and Normal Stanford University Press, 1961). Child Development," remarks to the Children's Advertising Review Unit Seminar, July 20, 1975, Scheutz, Stephen and J. N Sprafkin, "Spot Messages New York City. Appearing Within Saturday Morning Television Programs. A Content Analysis," Media Action Smarden, Eddie, "The True Problem with Kids Research Center, 1975. Televisionand a No Nonsense Methodology for Improving the Medium," paper written for Senate Select Committee. Hearings on Nutrition and Ogilvy and Mather, Inc . February 20, 1973. Human Needs. Television Advertising of Food to Children (Series 73/NE 3, 4, 5, 8) Washington, Snow, Robert P., "How Children Interpret TV D.0 , Government Printing Office, 1973. Violence in a Play Context," Journalism Qua:-

terly, 5 I,( 1974 ), 13-21. Shaak, Bruce, Lori Annes, and John R. Rossiter, 1 "Effects of the SocialSuccess Theme on Sobel, Gwenn and John R. Rossiter, "Effect f Sex Children's Product Preference," paper presented Stereotyping on Children's Product P ence at the 01975 Conference on Culture and Com- and Advertising Recall," paper presented at the munications, Philadelphia Temple University, 1975 Conference on Culture and Communica- (March 1975). tions, Philadelphia, Temple University (March 1975) Shaffer, Helen B, "Youth Market," Editorial Research Reports, August 25, 1965, pp 625-641 Snyder, Joyce, "New NAD Children's Unit Will Per- form by Precedent, Not Push," Te/evisin 'Radio Shapiro, Sydelle S,R L Bale, V Scardino, and T Age, July 22, 1974. Cerva, "An Evaluation of the Mulligan Stew 4-H

Television Series," VolI, Abt Associates, Inc, Sorenson, Ann and R. Gaurth Hansen, "Index of December 20, 1974 Food Quality," Journal of Nutrition Education, 197S, 7, 2 Sharaga, S., "The Effects of Television Advertising on Children's Nutrition Knowledge, Nutrition Spratlon, 1 N, R M Liebert, and R W Poulos, Attitudes, and Eating Habits," unpublished doc- "Effects of a Prosocial Televised Example on toral dissertation, Cornell University, 1974 (Ann Children's H e I ping ,-Journal ofErpertmenta! Arbor, Mich University Microfilms Number Child Psyhology. 1975 75-1451). Sponsor, "Always on Saturday and Occasionally on Sheikh, Anpes, Prasad Kant, and Rao Tannuri, scnday, Kids Don't Take Their Quarters Around "Children's TV Commercials A Review of the Corner to Loews Like They Used to Admen Research," Journal of Com mun !cat ions.24 art.Learning," XII (January23,1967), pp (August 1974), pp. 126-135 27-34

Sheth, Jagdish N, "A Theory of Family Buying Stein, A P, "Mass Media and Young Children's Decisions,': paper read to American Psychologi- Development," 71st Yearbook of the National calAssociation,Division of ConsumerPsy- Society for the Study of Education, 1972, pp chology, September 5, 1970 191-202. 22k; 225 Stein, Aletha H and Lynette K. Friedrich, "Tekvi- tions and of the Consequences Received," Journal sion Content and Young Children's Behavior," in of Personality, 44, (1976), 149-162. John P. Murray, Eli A. Rubinstein, and George Summers, JohnB.,statement of theNational A. Comstock (eds.), Television and Social Behavior Association of Broadcasters before the Subcom- Vol.2. Television and Social Learning, mittee on Communications of the House Inter- Washington, D C U.S. Government Printing state and Foreign Commerce Committee, July 15, Office, (1972), pp. 202-317. 1975. Stein, G. and J. Bryan, "The Effect of a Television Model upon Rule Adoption Behavior of Surgeon General's Scientific Advisory Committee on Television and Social Behavior, Television and Children," Child Development, 43 (1972), pp. Growing Up. The Impact of Televised Violence 268-273. Washington, D.C.U.S. Government Printing Steiner, Gary A., "The People Look at Commer- Office (1972) cials: A Study of Audience Behavior," Journal of Survey of Consumer Responses to Nutrition Claims Business, 9 (April 1966), pp. 272-304. Summary Report,A,Princeton, New Jersey. Steinfeld,J.,untitled testimony, U.SCongress, Response Analysis Corporation. 1975 Senate, hearings before the Subcommittee on Tannenbaum, Percy H. and Dolf Zillman, "Emo- Communications of the Commerce Committee, tional ,Arousal in the Facilitation of Aggression March 1972. through Communication," in Leonard Berkowitz Stephens, Lowndes F. and R. L. Moore, "Consumer (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Socialization. A Communication Perspective, (New York Academic Press, 1975), pp 149-192. paper presented to the International Communica- tion Association Student Summer Conference, TV Guide Magazine, "TV and Your Child In Search Athens, Ohio, 1973. of an Answer," M. Parini (ed ), New York TV Guide, 1969 Stephens, Lowndes F. and Roy L. Moore, "Price Accuracy as a Consumer Skill Among Younger Television/Radio Age, "NARB and Kid Ads New and Older Adolescents," submitted to Associa- Code in Works''" (April 1, 1974), p 21 tion of Education in Journalism, August 1974 Television/Radio Age. "Advertising in a Restrictive Stephens, Lowndes F. and Roy L. Moore, "Price Environment New Codes Return Ad Concepts Accuracy Asa Consumer Skill," Journal of Adver- Back to Basics," (November 26, 1973), p 39 tising Research, 14, 4 (August 1975), pp. 27-34. Teter, John W , "The Family...Peers, and Media In Steuer, F.B.,J.M. Applefield and R. Smith, fluences on Youth's Present and Future Con- "Televised Aggression and the Interpersonal Ag- sumption Preferences," unpublishedthesis, a gression of Preschool Children," Journal of Er- Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, perimental Child Psychology, I I, (1971), 442-447 Oklahoma, 1966 Strauss, AnselmL., "The Development and Thompson, Glenn, W., "Children's Acceptance of Transformation of Monetary Meanings in the TelevisionAdvertising andtheReaction of Child," American Sociological Review, 17 (1952), Televiewing to School Achievement," Journal of pp. 275-286. Education Research, 58 (December 1964), pp Streicher, Helen W. and H. L. Bonney, "Children 171-174. Talk About Television," paper presented at the Treisman, A. M ,"Verbal Cues. Language and 1973 meeting, Eastern Sociological Society. Meaning in Selective Attention," American Jour- Streit, Fred, Donald L.Halsted, and PietroJ. nal of Psychology, Vol. 77. 1964, pp 206-219 Pascale, "Differences among Youthful Users and Nonusers of Drugs Based on Their Perceptions of Turner, Ralph, H , Family Interaction (New York Parental Behavior," International Journal of the John Wiley and Sons, Inc , 1970) Addictions, Vol. 9, 1974, pp 749-755. Twomey, John E., Some Views on IV Advertising to Suls, Jerry M. and Daniel C. Gutkin, "Children's Children, Radio and Television Ai ts Department, Reactions to an Actor as a Function of Expecta- Ryerson Polytechnical Institute. June 1973

226 -,. "...... 2-I Ullrich, Helen and George Briggs, "The General Walters, Richard H and R D. Parke, "Influences of Public," in J. Mayer (ed.) L' S. Nutrition Policies in Response Consequences to a Social Model on the Seventies, San Francisco W H Freeman and Resistance to Deviation," Joutnal of Experimental Co., 1973. Child Psychology, I, (1964), 269-280 United States Congress, Hearing before Senate Ward, Scott, "Effects of Television Advertising on Select Committee onNutrition and Human Children and Adolescents," Research supported Needs. Nutrition Education-1973Parts 3-5. by National Institute for Mental Health and by TV Advertising of food to children 93d Congress, the Marketing Science Institute, June 1971. 1st session. March 5-12, 1973 (Washington, D C Ward, Scott,"Advsrtising and YouthTwo Government Printing Office, 1973). Studies," Sloan Management Review, 14 (1972), United States Congress, Hearings before Senate pp. 2-382. Select Committee onNutrition and Human Ward, Scott, "Children's Reactions to Commer- Needs. Nutrition EducationI973. Part8 cials," Journal of Advertising Research,12,2 Broadcast Industry's Response to TV Ads 93d (April 1972), pp 37-45. Congress, 1st session. June II, 1973 (Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office, 1973). Ward, Scott, "Consumer Socialization," Journal of Consumer Research, I (September 1974), pp. 1-14 United States Congress, Hearing before House Sub- committee on Communications on Broadcast Ad- Ward, Scott, "Children and Television: Impressions, vertising and Children. 94th Congress, 1st Ses- Impact, andIssues'," speech to Quaker Oats sion. July 14, 15, 16, and 17, 1975. (Washington. Management Conference, October 19, 1971 D.C.: Government Printing Office, 1975. Ward, Scott, "Kids TVMarketers on the Hot Vaillancourt, Pauline Marie, "Stability of Seat,"Harvard Business Review (July-August Children's Survey Responses," Public Opinion 1972). . Quarterly, XXXVII, 3 (1973), pp 373-387 Ward, Scott, "Can Pragmatic Marketing and Virtue Verna, Mary Ellen, "The Female Image in Mardi Together? Ask the Children! ", remarks to Children's TV Commercials,'' Journal of Broad- the Children's Advertising Review Unit Seminar( casting, 19:3 (Summer 1975), pp 301-309. New York City, June 20, 1975. Vogl, A. J., "The Changing Face of the Children's Ward, Scott and Ronald Faber, "Validation of Market," Sales Management, 93 (December 18, Mother-Child Purcl.ase Influence Frequency 1964), pp. 35-36, 38. Reports by the Multitrait- Multimethod Matrix." A working paper, Marketing Science Institute, Wackman, Daniel B., statement to the Federal 1975. Trade Commission (November II, 1971). Wackman, Daniel B. and Scott Ward, "Children's Ward, Scott, and T. S. Robertson, "Family In- Information Processing of Television Commer- fluences on Adolescent Consumer Behavior," cial Messages," manuscript based on a Sym- presented at American Psychological Associa- posium at the American Psychological Associa- tion, September 5, 1970. tion Convention, Montreal, 1973. Ward, Scott and T. S. Robertson, "Family In- Wackman, DanielB. and EllenWartella, "A fluences on Adolescent Consumer Behavior," Review of Cognitive Development Theory and paper presented to the First Annual Conference, Research and Discussion of Implications for Association of Consumer Research, Amherst, Research on Children's Responses to Television Mass., 1970. Advertising," prepared for Ford Foundation, Ward, Scott and T. S. Robertson, "Adolescent At- December 1973. titudes Toward Television Advertising: Prelimin- Wackman, DanielB. and EllenWartella, "A ary Findings" in E. A. Rubinstein et al. (eds.), Review of Research Related to Suggested Television and Social Behavior, Vol 4 Guidelinesfor Premium Advertising,"April (Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office, 1975 1972), pp. 526-542.

IS

NO 227 228 Ward, Scott and Daniel Wackman, "Family and Theory to Consumer Socialization Research." Media Influences on Adolescent Consumer paper presented to the Association for Consumer Learning," American Behavioral Scientist,14, 3 Research, Cincinnati, Ohio, October 1975. (January 1971). Wartella, Ellen, "A Research Model for Examining Ward, Scott, G. Reale, and D. Levinson, Consumer. Socialization from a Cognitive "Children's Perceptions, Explanation, and Judg- Developmental Perspective," paper presented at ments of Television Advertising A Further Ex- International Communications Association meet- ploration," working paper, Marketing Science In- ing, Chicago, April 1975. stitute, Cambridge, Mass., 1971. Wartella, Ellen and James Ettema, "The Role of Ward, Scott, T R Robertson, and D. B. Wackman, Stimulus Complexity in Children's Attention to "Children's Attention to Television Advertising," Television Commercials: A Developmental D M Gardner (ed ), Proceedings, Association Study," paper presented to Association for leer Consumer Research, 1971, pp. 143-156 Education in Journalism, August 1973. Ward, Scott,D.Levinson, and D Wackman, Wartella, Ellen and James Ettema, "A Cognitive "Children's Attention to Television Advertising," Developmental Study of Children's Attention to E A Rubinstein, et al (eds ), Television and Television Commercials," Communications SocialBehavior, Vol4,(Washington, D.C. Research, I,I (January 1974). Government Printing Office, 1972), pp 491-516. Weik, James L., "Advertising Recall as Influenced Ward, Scott and D. B Wackman, "Effects of Televi- by the Number, Length, and Position of Commer- sion Advertising on Consumer Socialization," cials, in Ronald C. Curhan (ed.), 1974 Proceed- Marketing Science Institute, Cambridge, Mass., ings: Series No. 36, Chicago. American Marketing September 1973. Association, (1974), 406-410. Ward, Scott, and D B. Wackman, "Children's In- Wells, William D., "Children as Consumers," in J. formation Processing of Television Advertising," W Newman (ed.), On Knowing the Consumer reprinted from New Models for Mass Communica- (New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1966) tion Research, Vol. II, Sage Annual Reviews of Wells, William D., "Communicating with Communication Research,1973, edited by F. Children," Journal of Advertising Research,5 Gerald Kline and P. Clark, pp. 119-146. (June 1965), pp. 2-14. Ward. Scott and D. B. Wackman, "Children's Wells, William D:, "Television and Aggression: Purchase Influence Attempts and Parental Yield- Replication of an Experimental Field Study," un- ing," Journal of Marketing Research, 9 (August published manuscript, Graduate School of Busi- 1972), pp 316-319 ness, University of Chicago, ()973). Ward, Scott, D. B. Wackman, and Ellen Wartella, Weiss, Walter, "Effects of the Mass Media of Com- Gerald Lesser, "Effects of Television Advertising munication,"in Gardner Lindzey andElliot on Consumer Socialization," working paper, Aronson (eds.), The Handbook of Social Psy- Cambridge, MassMarketing Science Institute, chology (2d ed.), Vol. 5, (1969), pp. 77-195. (1974). Weiss, Walter, "Mass Communication," Annual Ward, Scott, D. B. Wackman and Ellen Wartella, Review of Psychology, 22, (1971), 309-336. Children Learning to Buy. The Development of Consumer Information Processing Skills. White House Conference on Food, Nutrition and Cambridge, Mass. Marketing Science Institute HealthFinal Report, Washington_ D.C., (1976) Government Pr ling Office, 1970. Ward, Scott, D. B. Wackman, and Ellen Wartella, Wiley, Richard E., statement on Children's Televi- Consumer Socialization: An Information Processing sion Advertising before the Subcommittee on Anproach to Consumer Learning, (Beverly Hills, Communications, House Committee on interstate Calif : Sage Publications, Inc., forthcoming). and Foreign Commerce, July 17, 1975. Ward, Scott, D. B. Wackman, and Ellen Wartella, Wilkie, William L., "Assessment of Consumer In- "Contributions of Cognitive Development formation Processing Research in Relation to of)ri 4, 228 Public Policy Needs," Washington, D C . Office of Violent and Nonviolent Films on Viewers' of Exploratory Research and Problem Assess- Subsequent Aggression," Journal of Experimental ment, National Science Foundation, 1974 Social Psychology, 12, (1976), 220-232. Williams, Joyce W , "A Gradient of the Economic Wrightei, Carl P., 1 Can Sell You Anything. New Concepts of Elementary School Children and York Ballantine Books (1972). Factors Associated with Cognition," Journal of Consumer Affairs. 4 (Winter 1970), pp 113-125 Yankelovich, D., "Mother's Attitudes Toward Winick, Charles, L G Williamson, S. F. Chuzmir. i Chi: !ren's Programs and Commercials," and M. P. Winick Children's 'television Commer- (Newton Center, MassAction for Children's cialsA Content Analysis (New YorkPraeger Television, 1970) Publishers. 1973). Zillman, Dolf, Roland C. Johnson, and J. Witryol, S.. L Lowden, and 1Fagan (1967), "In- Hanrahan, "Pacifying Effect of Happy Ending of centive Effects upon Attention in Children's Dis- Communications Involving Aggression," Psy- crimination Learning Journal of Experimental chological Reports, 32, (1973), 967-970. Child Psychology, Vol 5, pp 94-108 Wolf, T. M. "Effects of Televised Modeled Ver- Zillman, Dolf, James L. Hoyt, and K D Day, balizations on Resistance to Deviation," Develop- "Strength and Duration of the Effect of Ag- mental Psychology, 8, (1973), 51-56. gressive, Violence, and Erotic Communications on Subsequent Aggressive Behavior," Com- Wohlner, Grace, "How Children Learn to Handle munication Research, I, (1974), 286-306 Money," Parents Magazine. X LV I (1971). 46, pp 84-86. Zunich, Michael, "Teenagers' Influence on Personal Worchel, Stephen. Thomas W Hardy, and Ritchard and Family Purchases." Journal of Home Hurley, "The Effects of Commercial Interruption Economics, 58 (June 1966), pp 483-484

229 440 t'i 0 * US. GOVERN' MEW PT OFFICE 1977 0-244-412 *