Bioethics, Substance Dualism and the Argument from Self-Awareness

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Bioethics, Substance Dualism and the Argument from Self-Awareness Bioethics, Substance Dualism and the Argument from Self-Awareness J. P. Moreland E.T.S. - 2011 Throughout history, most people have been substance and property dualists. Thus, regarding the mind/body problem, Jaegwon Kim's concession seems right: "We commonly think that we, as persons, have a mental and bodily dimension….Something like this dualism of personhood, I believe, is common lore shared across most cultures and religious traditions.…"i And regarding issues in personal identity, Frank Jackson acknowledges: “I take it that our folk conception of personal identity is Cartesian in character—in particular, we regard the question of whether I will be tortured tomorrow as separable from the question of whether someone with any amount of continuity— psychological, bodily, neurophysiological, and so on and so forth—with me today will be tortured.”ii People don’t have to be taught to be dualists like they must if they are to be physicalists. Indeed, little children are naturally dualists. Summing up research in developmental psychology, Henry Wellman states that “young children are dualists: knowledgeable of mental states and entities as ontologically different from physical objects and real [non-imaginary] events.”iii It seems to me that there are two tasks for any adequate philosophy of mind: (1) articulate one’s position and explain why dualism is the commonsense view (2) defend one’s position. I believe that there is an argument that simultaneously satisfies both desiderata in a non-ad-hoc way and, thus, the argument can thereby claim the virtue of theoretical simplicity in its favor. In what follows, I shall present the argument and defend its most crucial premise, and respond to two criticisms that have been raised against it. Before we examine the arguments, I want to offer some brief remarks about the soul and the sort of high, intrinsic value human persons possess which is of relevance to bioethical reflection. Now, one could believe in the soul and reject this sort of value, and one could reject the soul and embrace this sort of value. Whether or not each view can be justified is, of course, another matter. And, in any case, the existence of the soul, rightly in my view, factors into a good deal of bioethical argumentation. As I see it, physicalists of various stripes have a difficult task in attempting to justify this sort of value for human persons in so far as they are material objects, that is, without appealing to some sort of divine voluntarism or nonphysical emergent properties. In so far as we are physical objects, we are of little value, or so it seems to me. But, you may respond, what of the body? Are you being Platonistic here and devaluing the body’s worth? No, not at all. I just don’t think the body is of much value simply in so far as it is physical. As I see it, the body has value for these reasons: (1) As a Thomist, I take the body to be an ensouled, extended, physical structure; thus, the body includes the soul to be a body, and it is of value accordingly. By contrast, a corpse is of little intrinsic value. (2) It has secondary qualities, which serve as the metaphysical grounding of many of its aesthetic properties, and neither secondary qualities nor aesthetic properties are physical. (3) It exemplifies certain geometrical relations, e.g., shape and symmetry, but these are not physical, they are abstract (though I am open to the claim that their property-instances are physical). (4) It exemplifies a certain relational 2 complexity of arrangement, but since this complexity is an abstract object, it is not physical (though I am open to the claim that the relevant property-instance is physical). (5) It is owned by the person, it is the vehicle, perhaps by way of natural signs (as with Thomas Reid), in virtue of which the person is known, and it is intimately and causally related to the person. (6) It exists and, in so far as any existing thing has value, it does. (7) It is physical. (6) and (7) involve little value compared to the other factors. It must be kept in mind that the intrinsic value/beauty of the creation is due to factors like (2)-(4) above, not (7). This can be seen in the following observation: If Berkeley’s immaterialist ontology had been true, little of value of the created world would have been lost, metaphysically speaking. If I am correct about this, then while the body has incredible value, it is not largely due to its being physical. In as much as a physicalist identifies us with a physical object—e.g., an animal, composed material object, the brain or an atomic simple—to that extend he has a problem with accounting for the high, intrinsic value of human persons. An appeal to the emergent properties constitutive of personhood won’t help, because these are not physical and, thus, such an appeal fails to locate our value in our mere physicality. The Simple Argument Stewart Goetz has advanced the following type of argument for the non-physical nature of the self, which I have modified:iv (1) I am essentially an indivisible, simple spiritual substance. (2) Any physical body is essentially a divisible or complex entity (any physical body has spatial extension or separable parts). 3 (3) Principle of Indiscernibility of Identicals (4) Therefore, I am not identical with my (or any) physical body. The premise most likely to come into dispute is (1), and I will offer a defense of it shortly. Here, I want to make a few brief comments about (2), especially the role that extension and separable parts play in it, along with the associated notion of simplicity in (1). Part/whole relations are important for treatments of substances, and there are two kinds of parts relevant to our discussion—separable and inseparable (aka modes). Setting aside properties, there are two ways something can be simple in the sense relevant to our discussion: by being uncomposed of separable parts or by being metaphysically indivisible. I use “metaphysically indivisible” to mean what many philosophers say by “indivisible in thought.” Something could be metaphysically divisible but not physically divisible (if, say, such division annihilated the whole), but not conversely. Moreover, all particulars that are metaphysically indivisible are uncomposed, but not conversely (an continuously extended whole with no separable parts could still be divided). According to our usage, a substance with inseparable parts is simple. Any relevant entity to which I am identical on a physicalist view is (most likely to be) composed of separable parts (e.g., the living body, brain, or a standard sub-region of the brain) or (less likely) at least divisible (a physical simple if such there be).v I want to advance two arguments for premise (1). Both arguments support the claim that we are directly aware of ourselves as indivisible, simple spiritual substances. Here is the first one. It often happens in science that a range of apparently unrelated data can be unified if a theoretical entity is postulated as that which is causally responsible for 4 that range. The postulation of electrons unified a wide range of phenomena by depicting them as effects of the electron’s causal powers. Sometimes a range of apparently unrelated items of knowledge can be unified if one has knowledge by acquaintance with some relevant object. For example, there are many things I know about a certain spatial region R in the philosophy classroom in which I usually lecture. For example, I know that everyone walks around R and not through it. R is rectangular in shape, about four feet tall and two feet in width and breadth. R does not contain metal in it, R contains something that is darker than yellow, if a book is placed near the top of R it will hover in stable suspension off the floor. I know all these things. But rather than being a set of isolated pieces of knowledge, there is a unifying pattern to them. Indeed, I know each of them in virtue of knowledge by acquaintance of the podium that overlaps R. It is on the basis of that acquaintance that I know the items in question. There are many apparently unrelated items of knowledge people commonsensically have of themselves. At the very least, these are strong intuitions that are widely embraced (sometimes expressed in the first person): (i) I am an indivisible, uncomposed thing that cannot exist in degrees. If I lose, say, half of my body or brain, I am not thereby a half of a person. I am an all or nothing kind of thing—either I am present or I am not. In some brain operations and cases of Dandy Walker Syndrome, well over half of the brain is absent. But while the person may lose functioning, we have strong intuitions that a whole person is present. 5 (ii) Pairs of people are not themselves conscious subjects of experience. Siamese twins involve two, not three subjects of experience. When two people shake hands, a new subject of experience does not come-to-be. We are inclined to think that the reason for this is that subjects of experience cannot be composites. Thus, pairs or larger groups of anything—conscious or unconscious—in whatever relational structure cannot be a conscious subject of experience. My knowledge of this fact is a species of the following genus: For any x and y, the union of x and y is not itself a thinker.
Recommended publications
  • Dualism Vs. Materialism: a Response to Paul Churchland
    Dualism vs. Materialism: A Response to Paul Churchland by M. D. Robertson Paul M. Churchland, in his book, Matter and Consciousness, provides a survey of the issues and positions associated with the mind-body problem. This problem has many facets, and Churchland addresses several of them, including the metaphysical, epistemological, semantic, and methodological aspects of the debate. Churchland, of course, has very strong views on the subject, and does not hide his biases on the matter. In this paper I shall reexamine the metaphysical aspect of the mind-body problem. The metaphysical question concerns the existential status of the mind and the body, and the nature of the relationship between them. Like Churchland, I shall not hide my biases on the matter. What follows may be thought of as a rewriting of the second chapter of Churchland's book ("The Ontological Issue") from a non-naturalistic perspective. Substance Dualism René Descartes argued that the defining characteristic of minds was cogitation in a broad 2 sense, while that of bodies was spatial extension. Descartes also claimed that minds were not spatially extended, nor did bodies as such think. Thus minds and bodies were separate substances. This view has come to be called substance dualism. Descartes's argument for substance dualism can be summarized as follows: (1) Minds exist. (2) Bodies exist. (3) The defining feature of minds is cogitation. (4) The defining feature of bodies is extension. (5) That which cogitates is not extended. (6) That which is extended does not cogitate. Therefore, (7) Minds are not bodies, and bodies are not minds.
    [Show full text]
  • The Mystery of David Chalmers
    Daniel C. Dennett The Mystery of David Chalmers 1. Sounding the Alarm ‘The Singularity’ is a remarkable text, in ways that many readers may not appreciate. It is written in an admirably forthright and clear style, and is beautifully organized, gradually introducing its readers to the issues, sorting them carefully, dealing with them all fairly and with impressive scholarship, and presenting the whole as an exercise of sweet reasonableness, which in fact it is. But it is also a mystery story of sorts, a cunningly devised intellectual trap, a baffling puzzle that yields its solution — if that is what it is (and that is part of the mystery) — only at the very end. It is like a ‘well made play’ in which every word by every character counts, retrospectively, for something. Agatha Christie never concocted a tighter funnel of implications and suggestions. Bravo, Dave. Copyright (c) Imprint Academic 2013 So what is going on in this essay? It purports to be about the pros- pects of the Singularity, and since I can count on readers of my essay For personal use only -- not for reproduction to have read Chalmers, I needn’t waste so much as a sentence on what that is or might be. See Chalmers (2010). I confess that I was initially repelled by the prospect of writing a commentary on this essay since I have heretofore viewed the Singularity as a dismal topic, involving reflections on a technological fantasy so far removed from actuality as to be an indulgence best resisted. Life is short, and there are many serious problems to think about.
    [Show full text]
  • Richard Swinburne's Arguments for Substance Dualism
    Richard Swinburne’s arguments for substance dualism. MA by Research in Theology and Religion David Horner September 2018 Richard Swinburne’s arguments for substance dualism. Submitted by David Horner to the University of Exeter as a dissertation for the degree of MA by Research in Theology and Religion in September 2018 This dissertation is available for Library use on the understanding that it is copyright material and that no quotation from the dissertation may be published without proper acknowledgement. I certify that all material in this dissertation which is not my own work has been identified and that no material has previously been submitted and approved for the award of a degree by this or any other University. 1 Acknowledgements. I would like to thank my supervisors, Dr Jonathan Hill and Dr Joel Krueger for their support and encouragement in the writing of this dissertation and for their patience in trying to keep me on the straight and narrow. I want to acknowledge the many conversations, on this and other topics, I have had with my friend and philosopher, Dr Chris Boyne, who sadly died in June of this year. I thank all my other chums at The Bull, Ditchling, for listening to my metaphysical ramblings. And finally, I thank my wife, Linda, for once more putting up with this kind of thing. 2 Abstract This dissertation is a contribution to debates in the philosophy of mind and of personal identity. It presents a critical account of arguments for substance dualism to be found in Richard Swinburne’s Mind, Brain, and Free Will (2013).
    [Show full text]
  • General MT09 4 Scepticism M
    GeneralGeneral PhilosophyPhilosophy DrDr PeterPeter MillicanMillican,, HertfordHertford CollegeCollege LectureLecture 4:4: TwoTwo CartesianCartesian TopicsTopics Scepticism,Scepticism, andand thethe MindMind LastLast TimeTime …… … we looked at scepticism about INDUCTION. ThisThis LectureLecture …… … will move on to SCEPTICISM concerning the external world, most famously exemplified in Descartes’ first Meditation, and his related claims about the nature of MIND AND BODY. TheThe NextNext LectureLecture …… … will say more about modern responses to SCEPTICISM, and focus on KNOWLEDGE. 2 TwoTwo KindsKinds ofof ScepticismScepticism VerticalVertical ScepticismScepticism – Inferring from one kind of thing to a different kind (e.g. inferring from one’s sensations or appearances, to the existence of real physical objects that cause them). HorizontalHorizontal ScepticismScepticism – Inferring things of the same kind as one has experienced (e.g. inferring from one’s sensations or appearances, to expect similar sensations or appearances in the future). 3 ExternalExternal WorldWorld ScepticismScepticism ItIt cancan seemseem thatthat ((““verticalvertical””)) externalexternal worldworld scepticismscepticism isis farfar moremore worryingworrying thanthan ((““horizontalhorizontal””)) inductiveinductive scepticism:scepticism: – Maybe I am just dreaming, and there is no external world at all. – Maybe an evil demon is causing me to have illusions of an external world. – Maybe a wicked scientist has my brain in a vat, and is creating these illusions. 4 DescartesDescartes’’
    [Show full text]
  • A a a How to Achieve the Physicalist Dream Theory of Consciousness
    Draft May 2020 For G. Rabin Grounding and Consciousness (Oxford) How to Achieve the Physicalist Dream: Identity or Grounding?* Adam Pautz Brown Imagine [a picture] with a million tiny pixels. The picture and the properties reduce to the arrangement of light and dark pixels. The supervenience of mind and all else upon the arrangement of atoms in the void — or whatever replaces atoms in the void in true physics — is another case of reduction. David Lewis (1995) The reader is welcome to label ground physicalism a form of “dualism” or “emer- gentism” (or perhaps a new position entirely), so long as she recognizes that ground physicalism is built around the thesis that the mental is not fundamental but rather grounded in the physical. Jonathan Schaffer (2020) Most of nature is pretty is boring: just different arrangements of atoms in the void. But consciousness seems special. When brains reached a certain complexity, a miracle hap- pened. There appeared properties of a wholly novel type: conscious experiences. To explain this, we may have no choice but to posit special “psychophysical laws”. There are possi- ble worlds where these laws don’t obtain, the miracle doesn’t happen, and we are all zombies. This is property dualism. In “Sensations and Brain Processes”, J. C. Smart articulated just how unappealing property dualism is. It provides a complex and nonuniform picture of reality. And he put forward an alternative physicalist dream picture of reality. In sentient as well as in- sentient nature, “there is nothing in the world but increasingly complex arrangements of physical constituents” (1959: 142).
    [Show full text]
  • Emergence Theories and Pragmatic Realism Charbel Niño El-Hani Federal University of Bahia, Brazil
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by CommonKnowledge Essays in Philosophy Volume 3 Article 3 Issue 2 Pragmatism and Neopragmatism 6-2002 Emergence Theories and Pragmatic Realism Charbel Niño El-Hani Federal University of Bahia, Brazil Sami Pihlström University of Helsinki, Finland Follow this and additional works at: http://commons.pacificu.edu/eip Part of the Philosophy Commons Recommended Citation El-Hani, Charbel Niño and Pihlström, Sami (2002) "Emergence Theories and Pragmatic Realism," Essays in Philosophy: Vol. 3: Iss. 2, Article 3. Essays in Philosophy is a biannual journal published by Pacific nivU ersity Library | ISSN 1526-0569 | http://commons.pacificu.edu/eip/ Emergence Theories Essays in Philosophy A Biannual Journal Volume 3, Number 2 Emergence Theories and Pragmatic Realism Abstract The tradition of pragmatism has, especially since Dewey, been characterized by a commitment to non- reductive naturalism. The notion of emergence, popular in the early decades of the twentieth century and currently re-emerging as a central concept in metaphysics and the philosophy of mind, may be useful in explicating that commitment. The present paper discusses the issue of the reality of emergent properties, drawing particular attention to a pragmatic way of approaching this issue. The reality of emergents can be defended as a pragmatically-useful ontological commitment; hence, pragmatism can be employed as a tool in the debate over the structure and reality of emergence. This strategy of justifying ontological commitments is examined through historical and systematic discussions of the pragmatist tradition. It turns out, among other things, that while classical pragmatists did not specify any technical notion of emergence in the contemporary sense, their non-reductively naturalist views are relevant to the more recent emergence discussions -- especially because they rejected the metaphysical realism typical of today’s ontologically-oriented emergence theories.
    [Show full text]
  • Today •Brief Point About Nagel •Mind-Body Problem(S) •Mind-World
    Today •Brief point about Nagel •Mind-body problem(s) •Mind-world relationship •Tips on the second reading a comment about Nagel mind body world Questions from folk psychology How do thoughts happen? propositional attitudes How do qualia happen? consciousness Can thinking cause action? free will Does our spirit die when we do? immortality two views of philosophy determining the truth uncovering possibilities “Philosophy is useful when you are the stage where you can learn a lot about a problem just by thinking about it.” John Perry Theories of mind and brain Dualism Behaviorism Materialism Dualism substance dualism radical skepticism Descartes’ arguments mind is apparent mind is indivisible can’t perceive wax, but can understand it Cartesian dualism - mind has no “extent” Popular dualism - mind is different from body, but is inside it “ghost in the machine” argument from religion parapsychological phenomena a knock down argument against substance dualism? Property dualism epiphenominalism - mental properties have no causal effects interactionist property dualism mental properties are emergent mental properties are irreducibility Jackson’s neuroscientist Chalmers proposal Arguments against property dualism minds are embodied panpsychism? Ockham’s razor pragmatism evolutionary continuum Behaviorism privileged access - unobservable verification zombies? inverted spectrum argument Materialism I: Reductionism Leibniz’s law: A and B are identical iff they share all properties Is the brain knowable? Materialism II: Functionalism Mind as software Psychology independent of the brain Uploading the brain? Materialism III: Eliminativism mental states don’t exist analogies witches phlogiston elan vital spirit diseases Failures of folk psychology introspectionism experiments split brains Differ from reductionism? gamma synchrony DFPLC Mind and world Idealism dreams, other minds Realism “God is no deceiver” Transcendentalism .
    [Show full text]
  • Precis of Psychoneural Reduction: the New Wa Ve
    Grazer Philosophische Studien 61 (2001) 249-255 PRECIS OF PSYCHONEURAL REDUCTION: THE NEW WA VE John BICKLE University of Cincinnati Although her plumb for eliminative materialism generated the most sound and fury, three other projects in Patricia Churchland's (1986) Neurophilosophy were of greater philosophical significance: Contributing to a new theory of cross-levels intertheoretic reduc- tion for science in general and reformulating the traditional mind-body problem using these resources; Replying to dualists' arguments based on features of conscious ex- perience (qualia, subjectivity); Replying to multiple realizability arguments for both functionalism and the methodological autonomy of cognitive psychology from the neurosciences. Churchland did not invent any of these projects, but her book gave them a unified focus grounded on a post-logical empiricist philosophy of science and a wealth of empirical neuroscientific detail. Unfortunately, these three projects and the progress Church land and other reductionists made got lost in the bombast over eliminativism. By the late-1990s interest in eliminativism had waned, but recent de- velopments in philosophy of mind had also eclipsed these specific pro- jects. Some flaws in the theory of intertheoretic reduction remained unad- dressed. (Clifford Hooker 1981 had first noticed these in the very paper that remained the benchmark for the theory.) New "dualistic" arguments based on more sophisticated metaphysics emerged (Chalmers 1996), along with "New Mysterian" worries about humans' capabilites of solv- ing the consciousness-brain problem (McGinn, 1989). Nonreductive physicalism replaced functionalism, and supervenience replaced realiza- tion as the preferred alternative to reduction. Clearly, if "neurophiloso- phy" was to remain viable, these three projects and their unity within a philosophy of science and the empirical neurosciences required reformu- 250 lation and a new focus.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Why I Am Not a Property Dualist by John R. Searle I Have Argued in a Number of Writings1 That the Philosophical Part (Though
    1 Why I Am Not a Property Dualist By John R. Searle I have argued in a number of writings1 that the philosophical part (though not the neurobiological part) of the traditional mind-body problem has a fairly simple and obvious solution: All of our mental phenomena are caused by lower level neuronal processes in the brain and are themselves realized in the brain as higher level, or system, features. The form of causation is “bottom up,” whereby the behavior of lower level elements, presumably neurons and synapses, causes the higher level or system features of consciousness and intentionality. (This form of causation, by the way, is common in nature; for example, the higher level feature of solidity is causally explained by the behavior of the lower level elements, the molecules.) Because this view emphasizes the biological character of the mental, and because it treats mental phenomena as ordinary parts of nature, I have labeled it “biological naturalism.” To many people biological naturalism looks a lot like property dualism. Because I believe property dualism is mistaken, I would like to try to clarify the differences between the two accounts and try to expose the weaknesses in property dualism. This short paper then has the two subjects expressed by the double meanings in its title: why my views are not the same as property dualism, and why I find property dualism unacceptable. There are, of course, several different “mind-body” problems. The one that most concerns me in this article is the relationship between consciousness and brain processes. I think that the conclusions of the discussion will extend to other features of the mind-body problem, such as, for example, the relationship between intentionality and brain processes, but for the sake of simplicity I will concentrate on consciousness.
    [Show full text]
  • Substance and Property Dualism Michael Lacewing Introduction
    [email protected] © Michael Lacewing Substance and property dualism Michael Lacewing Introduction Substance dualism is much better known than property dualism. A substance is traditionally understood an entity that does not depend on another entity in order to exist. Substances are also those things that have properties. Substance dualism holds that there are two fundamentally different types of such entities – material substances, or bodies, and mental substances, or minds. If minds do not depend on bodies in order to exist, e.g. if minds can exist separated from any body, then minds are separate substances. Substance monism is the view that there is only one sort of substance, either mental (idealism) or matter (materialism). According to materialism, everything that exists must be dependent on some material thing to exist. For example, mental properties (including mental states, such as holding beliefs and mental events, such as having a thought) are properties of a person, and a person is necessarily a material object. If you think that matter is the only form of substance, and all properties are properties of something that is material, you may still believe that mental properties are quite different sorts of properties from physical ones. Materialism, as a theory about substance, does not need to be committed to the claim that mental properties are physical properties. If you hold that mental properties can’t be completely accounted for in terms of, or ‘reduced to’, physical ones, you maintain ‘property dualism’. You may claim that mental properties are properties of a person, not a brain; or you may even claim that they are properties of the brain, but not physical properties.
    [Show full text]
  • Introductory Lectures: the Nature and History of Cognitive Science Chapter 4: Philosophy’S Convergence Towards Cognitive Science by Dr
    Introductory Lectures: The Nature and History of Cognitive Science Chapter 4: Philosophy’s Convergence Towards Cognitive Science By Dr. Charles Wallis Last revision: 9/24/2018 Chapter Outline 4.1 Introduction 4.2 The Scientific Explosion of the Early 20th Century 4.3 The Logical and Mathematical Explosion of the Early 20th Century 4.4 The Rise Logical Empiricism 4.5 Logical Behaviorism 4.6 Three Problems for Logical Behaviorism 4.7 Identity Theories: Reduction Through Reference Equivalence Rather Than Meaning Equivalence 4.8 Identity Theories: Type-Type Identity 4.9 Two Arguments For Type-Type Identity Theory 4.10 Identity Theories: Token-Token Identity 4.11 Functionalism 4.12 Computationalism 4.13 Property Dualism 4.14 Eliminative Materialism 4.15 Glossary of Key Terms 4.16 Bibliography The 20th Century and the Semantic Twist 4.1 Introduction Recall that ontological frameworks provide a general framework within which theorists specify domains of inquiry and construct theories to predict, manipulate, and explain phenomena within the domain. Once researchers articulate an ontological framework with sufficient clarity they begin to formulate and test theories. Chapter two ends with the suggestion that oppositional substance dualists face two major challenges in their attempt to transition from the articulation of an ontological framework to the formulation and testing of theories purporting to predict, manipulate, and explain mental phenomena. On the one hand, oppositional substance dualists have problems formulating theories providing explanations, predictions, and manipulations of the continual, seamless interaction between the mental and the physical. Philosophers often call this the interaction problem. On the other hand, the very nature of a mental substance--substance defined so as to share no properties with physical substance--gives rise to additional challenges.
    [Show full text]
  • Property Dualism and the Merits of Solutions to the Mind-Body Problem a Reply to Strawson
    Fiona Macpherson Property Dualism and the Merits of Solutions to the Mind-Body Problem A Reply to Strawson 1. Introduction This paper is divided into two main sections. The first articulates what I believe Strawson’s position to be. I first contrast Strawson’s usage of ‘physicalism’ with the mainstream use. I then explain why I think that Strawson’s position is one of property dualism and substance monism. In doing this, I outline his view and Locke’s view on the nature of sub- stance. I argue that they are similar in many respects and thus it is no surprise that Strawson actually holds a view on the mind much like one plausible interpretation of Locke’s position. Strawson’s use of ter- minology cloaks this fact and he does not himself explicitly recognize it in his paper. In the second section, I outline some of Strawson’s assumptions that he uses in arguing for his position. I comment on the plausibility of his position concerning the relation of the mind to the body compared with mainstream physicalism and various forms of dualism. Before embarking on the two main sections, in the remainder of this introduction, I very briefly sketch Strawson’s view. Strawson claims that he is a physicalist and panpsychist. These two views are not obvious bedfellows, indeed, as typically conceived, they are incompatible positions. However, Strawson’s use of the term ‘physicalism’is not the mainstream one. Strawson, clearly, recognizes this and takes some pains to distinguish how the way in which he con- ceives of physicalism is different from mainstream physicalism.
    [Show full text]