Pleasantness Bias in Flashbulb Memories: Positive and Negative Flashbulb Memories of the Fall of the Berlin Wall Among East and West Germans
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Memory Category Fluency, Memory Specificity, And
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General © 2019 The Author(s) 2020, Vol. 149, No. 1, 198–206 0096-3445/20/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000617 BRIEF REPORT Memory Category Fluency, Memory Specificity, and the Fading Affect Bias for Positive and Negative Autobiographical Events: Performance on a Good Day–Bad Day Task in Healthy and Depressed Individuals Caitlin Hitchcock, Jill Newby, Emma Timm, Willem Kuyken Rachel M. Howard, and Ann-Marie Golden University of Oxford University of Cambridge Tim Dalgleish University of Cambridge and Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust In mentally healthy individuals, autobiographical memory is typically biased toward positive events, which may help to maintain psychological well-being. Our aim was to assess a range of important positive memory biases in the mentally healthy and explore the possibility that these biases are mitigated in those with mental health problems. We administered a novel recall paradigm that required recollection of multiple good and bad past events (the Good Day–Bad Day task) to healthy and depressed individuals. This allowed us to explore differences in memory category fluency (i.e., the ability to generate integrated sets of associated events) for positive and negative memories, along with memory specificity, and fading affect bias—a greater reduction in the intensity of memory-related affect over time for negative versus positive events. We found that healthy participants demonstrated superior category fluency for positive relative to negative events but that this effect was absent in depressed participants. Healthy participants exhibited a strong fading affect bias that was significantly mitigated, although still present, in depression. -
The Fading Affect Bias: Effects of Social Disclosure to an Interactive Versus Non-Responsive Listener
This is a repository copy of The fading affect bias: Effects of social disclosure to an interactive versus non-responsive listener. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/85286/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Muir, K, Brown, C and Madill, A (2015) The fading affect bias: Effects of social disclosure to an interactive versus non-responsive listener. Memory, 23 (6). 829 - 847. ISSN 0965-8211 https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2014.931435 Reuse Unless indicated otherwise, fulltext items are protected by copyright with all rights reserved. The copyright exception in section 29 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988 allows the making of a single copy solely for the purpose of non-commercial research or private study within the limits of fair dealing. The publisher or other rights-holder may allow further reproduction and re-use of this version - refer to the White Rose Research Online record for this item. Where records identify the publisher as the copyright holder, users can verify any specific terms of use on the publisher’s website. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ EFFECTS OF SOCIAL DISCLOSURE Running head: EFFECTS OF SOCIAL DISCLOSURE The Fading Affect Bias: Effects of Social Disclosure to an Interactive versus Non-Responsive Listener Kate Muir*1, Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT. -
Communication Science to the Public
David M. Berube North Carolina State University ▪ HOW WE COMMUNICATE. In The Age of American Unreason, Jacoby posited that it trickled down from the top, fueled by faux-populist politicians striving to make themselves sound approachable rather than smart. (Jacoby, 2008). EX: The average length of a sound bite by a presidential candidate in 1968 was 42.3 seconds. Two decades later, it was 9.8 seconds. Today, it’s just a touch over seven seconds and well on its way to being supplanted by 140/280- character Twitter bursts. ▪ DATA FRAMING. ▪ When asked if they truly believe what scientists tell them, NEW ANTI- only 36 percent of respondents said yes. Just 12 percent expressed strong confidence in the press to accurately INTELLECTUALISM: report scientific findings. ▪ ROLE OF THE PUBLIC. A study by two Princeton University researchers, Martin TRENDS Gilens and Benjamin Page, released Fall 2014, tracked 1,800 U.S. policy changes between 1981 and 2002, and compared the outcome with the expressed preferences of median- income Americans, the affluent, business interests and powerful lobbies. They concluded that average citizens “have little or no independent influence” on policy in the U.S., while the rich and their hired mouthpieces routinely get their way. “The majority does not rule,” they wrote. ▪ Anti-intellectualism and suspicion (trends). ▪ Trump world – outsiders/insiders. ▪ Erasing/re-writing history – damnatio memoriae. ▪ False news. ▪ Infoxication (CC) and infobesity. ▪ Aggregators and managed reality. ▪ Affirmation and confirmation bias. ▪ Negotiating reality. ▪ New tribalism is mostly ideational not political. ▪ Unspoken – guns, birth control, sexual harassment, race… “The amount of technical information is doubling every two years. -
How Personal Experience Modulates the Neural Circuitry of Memories of September 11
How personal experience modulates the neural circuitry of memories of September 11 Tali Sharot*, Elizabeth A. Martorella*, Mauricio R. Delgado*†, and Elizabeth A. Phelps*‡ *Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003; and †Department of Psychology, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Piscataway, NJ 08855 Communicated by Ulric Neisser, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, October 27, 2006 (received for review February 28, 2006) Brown and Kulik [Brown R, Kulik J (1977) Cognition 5:73–99] forgetting (3–8), perhaps at the same rate as other memories (5). introduced the term ‘‘flashbulb memory’’ to describe the recall of Although some features of the recollective experience associated shocking, consequential events such as hearing news of a presi- with flashbulb memories appeared to be distinct, such as their dential assassination. They proposed that the vivid detail of such vividness or the degree of confidence in which they are held, for memories results from the action of a unique neural mechanism. In many investigators the similarities in the rate of forgetting imply that the present study of personal recollections of the terrorist attacks it is unnecessary to posit a unique mechanism; ‘‘ordinary’’ mne- of September 11, 2001 (9/11) in New York City, we combine monic mechanisms will do. behavioral and brain imaging techniques, with two goals: (i)to Given the significant debate concerning the mechanisms that explore the neural basis of such memories and (ii) to clarify the produce flashbulb memories, it is surprising that no one to date has characteristics of the emotional events that may give rise to them. -
Ilidigital Master Anton 2.Indd
services are developed to be used by humans. Thus, understanding humans understanding Thus, humans. by used be to developed are services obvious than others but certainly not less complex. Most products bioengineering, and as shown in this magazine. Psychology mightbusiness world. beBe it more the comparison to relationships, game elements, or There are many non-business flieds which can betransfered to the COGNTIVE COGNTIVE is key to a succesfully develop a product orservice. is keytoasuccesfullydevelopproduct BIASES by ANTON KOGER The Power of Power The //PsychologistatILI.DIGITAL WE EDIT AND REINFORCE SOME WE DISCARD SPECIFICS TO WE REDUCE EVENTS AND LISTS WE STORE MEMORY DIFFERENTLY BASED WE NOTICE THINGS ALREADY PRIMED BIZARRE, FUNNY, OR VISUALLY WE NOTICE WHEN WE ARE DRAWN TO DETAILS THAT WE NOTICE FLAWS IN OTHERS WE FAVOR SIMPLE-LOOKING OPTIONS MEMORIES AFTER THE FACT FORM GENERALITIES TO THEIR KEY ELEMENTS ON HOW THEY WERE EXPERIENCED IN MEMORY OR REPEATED OFTEN STRIKING THINGS STICK OUT MORE SOMETHING HAS CHANGED CONFIRM OUR OWN EXISTING BELIEFS MORE EASILY THAN IN OURSELVES AND COMPLETE INFORMATION way we see situations but also the way we situationsbutalsotheway wesee way the biasesnotonlychange Furthermore, overload. cognitive avoid attention, ore situations, guide help todesign massively can This in. take people information of kind explainhowandwhat ofperception egory First,biasesinthecat andappraisal. ory, self,mem perception, into fourcategories: roughly bedivided Cognitive biasescan within thesesituations. forusers interaction andeasy in anatural situationswhichresults sible toimprove itpos and adaptingtothesebiasesmakes ingiven situations.Reacting ways certain act sively helpstounderstandwhypeople mas into consideration biases ing cognitive Tak humanbehavior. topredict likely less or andmore relevant illusionsare cognitive In each situation different every havior day. -
John Collins, President, Forensic Foundations Group
On Bias in Forensic Science National Commission on Forensic Science – May 12, 2014 56-year-old Vatsala Thakkar was a doctor in India but took a job as a convenience store cashier to help pay family expenses. She was stabbed to death outside her store trying to thwart a theft in November 2008. Bloody Footwear Impression Bloody Tire Impression What was the threat? 1. We failed to ask ourselves if this was a footwear impression. 2. The appearance of the impression combined with the investigator’s interpretation created prejudice. The accuracy of our analysis became threatened by our prejudice. Types of Cognitive Bias Available at: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_cognitive_biases | Accessed on April 14, 2014 Anchoring or focalism Hindsight bias Pseudocertainty effect Illusory superiority Levels-of-processing effect Attentional bias Hostile media effect Reactance Ingroup bias List-length effect Availability heuristic Hot-hand fallacy Reactive devaluation Just-world phenomenon Misinformation effect Availability cascade Hyperbolic discounting Recency illusion Moral luck Modality effect Backfire effect Identifiable victim effect Restraint bias Naive cynicism Mood-congruent memory bias Bandwagon effect Illusion of control Rhyme as reason effect Naïve realism Next-in-line effect Base rate fallacy or base rate neglect Illusion of validity Risk compensation / Peltzman effect Outgroup homogeneity bias Part-list cueing effect Belief bias Illusory correlation Selective perception Projection bias Peak-end rule Bias blind spot Impact bias Semmelweis -
The Flashbulb Memory of the Positive and Negative Events
World Academy of Science, Engineering and Technology International Journal of Psychological and Behavioral Sciences The Flashbulb Me Vol:6,mo No:5,ry 2012 o f the Positive and Negative Events: Wenchuan Earthquake and Acceptance to College Aiping Liu, Xiaoping Ying, Jing Luo Abstract—53 college students answered questions regarding the However, whether the positive personal event could bring circumstances in which they first heard about the news of Wenchuan flashbulb memory? Scott and Ponsoda (1996) selected 10 earthquake or the news of their acceptance to college which took place positive and 10 negative public events (one for each year approximately one year ago, and answered again two years later. The between 1982 and 1991). They found that the recent events number of details recalled about their circumstances for both events was high and didn’t decline two years later. However, consistency in remembered better than those events earlier and there was no reported details over two years was low. Participants were more likely difference in memory for the details of personal circumstances to construct central (e.g., Where were you?) than peripheral for the positive and negative events [17]. As Wright and information (What were you wearing?), and the confidence of the Anderson (1996) pointed out, however, these events may differ central information was higher than peripheral information, which on characteristics (such as the intensity of emotion or surprise) indicated that they constructed more when they were more confident. that may influence the formation of these memories [18], for which Scott and Ponsoda (1996) did not collect information. Keywords—flashbulb memory; consistency; reconstructive error; confidence For instance, some events labeled as positive by Scott and Ponsoda (1996) may be irrelevant to some participants. -
ORDINARY MEMORY PROCESSES SHAPE FLASHBULB MEMORIES of EXTRAORDINARY EVENTS a Review of 40 Years of Research
4 ORDINARY MEMORY PROCESSES SHAPE FLASHBULB MEMORIES OF EXTRAORDINARY EVENTS A review of 40 years of research Jennifer M. Talarico and David C. Rubin We review the shifting definition of “Flashbulb memory” in the 40 years since Brown and Kulik coined the term. We evaluate evidence for veridical, phenomeno- logical, and metacognitive features that have been proposed to differentiate Flashbulb from ordinary autobiographical memories. We further consider the event conditions thought to be necessary to produce Flashbulb memories and discuss how post-event processing may distinguish Flashbulb memories. We conclude that a categorical dis- sociation between flashbulb and other autobiographical memories is untenable, but that Flashbulb memories still pose important, as yet unanswered, questions. Brown and Kulik (1977) observed a phenomenon that captured the public’s attention – seemingly indelible memory for important, emotional events. They dubbed it “Flashbulb memory (FBM)” and conducted the first modern empirical study on the topic. The concept was equally effective in capturing the attention of memory researchers, and in the 40 years following their seminal publication, the topic has been investigated almost as often as the events that lead to such memo- ries allow. During this time, the description of the phenomenon has undergone an interesting and important transformation. The initial “special mechanism” hypothesis was that FBMs were a permanent, veridical (though not necessarily complete) memory record that resulted from a unique memorial process involving automatic encoding of all aspects of an impor- tant (emotional) event as it happened. However, this strong hypothesis did not last long, as evidence of both errors of omission and commission in the recall of FBMs were soon identified (Christianson, 1989; Neisser & Harsch, 1992; Neisser et al., 1996). -
Memory Processes
6 CHAPTER Memory Processes CHAPTER OUTLINE Encoding and Transfer of Information The Constructive Nature of Memory Forms of Encoding Autobiographical Memory Short-Term Storage Memory Distortions Long-Term Storage The Eyewitness Testimony Paradigm Transfer of Information from Short-Term Memory Repressed Memories to Long-Term Memory The Effect of Context on Memory Rehearsal Key Themes Organization of Information Summary Retrieval Retrieval from Short-Term Memory Thinking about Thinking: Analytical, Parallel or Serial Processing? Creative, and Practical Questions Exhaustive or Self-Terminating Processing? Key Terms The Winner—a Serial Exhaustive Model—with Some Qualifications Media Resources Retrieval from Long-Term Memory Intelligence and Retrieval Processes of Forgetting and Memory Distortion Interference Theory Decay Theory 228 CHAPTER 6 • Memory Processes 229 Here are some of the questions we will explore in this chapter: 1. What have cognitive psychologists discovered regarding how we encode information for storing it in memory? 2. What affects our ability to retrieve information from memory? 3. How does what we know or what we learn affect what we remember? n BELIEVE IT OR NOT THERE’SAREASON YOU REMEMBER THOSE ANNOYING SONGS that strengthens the connections associated with that Having a song or part of a song stuck in your head is phrase. In turn, this increases the likelihood that you will incredibly frustrating. We’ve all had the experience of the recall it, which leads to more reinforcement. song from a commercial repeatedly running through our You could break this unending cycle of repeated recall minds, even though we wanted to forget it. But sequence and reinforcement—even though this is a necessary and recall—remembering episodes or information in sequen- normal process for the strengthening and cementing of tial order (like the notes to a song)—has a special and memories—by introducing other sequences. -
The Fading Affect Bias: but What the Hell Is It For?
APPLIED COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY Appl. Cognit. Psychol. 23: 1122–1136 (2009) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/acp.1614 The Fading Affect Bias: But What the Hell Is It For? W. RICHARD WALKER1* and JOHN J. SKOWRONSKI2 1Department of Behavioral Sciences and Social Work, Winston-Salem State University, Winston-Salem, NC, USA 2Department of Psychology, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL, USA SUMMARY This article reviews research examining the fading affect bias (FAB): The finding that the intensity of affect associated with negative autobiographical memories fades faster than affect associated with positive autobiographical memories. The FAB is a robust effect in autobiographical memory that has been replicated using a variety of methods and populations. The FAB is linked to both cognitive and social processes that support a positive view of the self. Accordingly, we speculate that one function of the FAB may be to induce individuals to be positive and action-oriented so that they may better face and master life challenges. Copyright # 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Baddeley (1988) challenged researchers in memory and cognition to explain any given phenomenon by answering a simple question: What the hell is for? The present article poses this question in relation to the fading affect bias (FAB). The FAB shows that the emotion associated with negative event memories generally fades faster than the emotion associated with positive event memories. This phenomenon has been well documented in several studies over the years, receiving the most intense scrutiny in the last decade. To better understand the possible functions of the FAB, this article will first review some of the classic perspectives on memory and emotion. -
Investor Memory of Past Performance Is Positively Biased and Predicts Overconfidence
Investor memory of past performance is positively biased and predicts overconfidence Daniel J. Waltersa,1 and Philip M. Fernbachb aINSEAD, Singapore 138676; and bLeeds School of Business, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309 Edited by Henry L. Roediger III, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, and approved July 13, 2021 (received for review December 28, 2020) We document a memory-based mechanism associated with investor recalled their cholesterol scores and cardiovascular risk categories overconfidence. In Studies 1 and 2, investors were asked to recall their as more favorable than shown on a recently viewed test (26). most important trades in the recent past and then reported investing Selective forgetting was found in study participants who ten- confidence and trading frequency. After the study, they looked up ded to more readily forget details about negative feedback than and reported the actual returns of these trades. In both studies, in- equivalent positive feedback on a performance evaluation (27). vestors were biased to recall returns as higher than achieved, and Researchers have suggested this bias occurs because people larger memory biases were associated with greater overconfidence reminisce more on positive information than on negative infor- and trading frequency. The design of Study 2 allowed us to separately mation following ego-enhancement motivations (28, 29). Selec- investigate the effects of two types of memory biases: distortion and tive forgetting becomes stronger with age (30) but also occurs in selective forgetting. Both types of bias were present and were inde- children as young as 5 y old (31). pendently associated with overconfidence and trading frequency. -
An Exploration of the Relationship Among Valence, Fading Affect, Rehearsal Frequency, And
AN EXPLORATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP 1 Abstract The affect associated with negative (or unpleasant) memories typically tends to fade faster than the affect associated with positive (or pleasant) memories, a phenomenon called the fading affect bias (FAB). We conducted a study to explore the mechanisms related to the FAB. A retrospective recall procedure was used to obtain three self-report measures (memory vividness, rehearsal frequency, affective fading) for both positive events and negative events. Affect for positive events faded less than affect for negative events, and positive events were recalled more vividly than negative events. The perceived vividness of an event (memory vividness) and the extent to which an event has been rehearsed (rehearsal frequency) were explored as possible mediators of the relation between event valence and affect fading. Additional models conceived of affect fading and rehearsal frequency as contributors to a memory’s vividness. Results suggested that memory vividness was a plausible mediator of the relation between an event’s valence and affect fading. Rehearsal frequency was also a plausible mediator of this relation, but only via its effects on memory vividness. Additional modeling results suggested that affect fading and rehearsal frequency were both plausible mediators of the relation between an event’s valence and the event’s rated memory vividness. Keywords : self, memory, rehearsal, vividness, fading affect bias AN EXPLORATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP 2 An Exploration of the Relationship among Valence, Fading Affect, Rehearsal Frequency, and Memory Vividness for Past Personal Events A large corpus of research results shows that the affect associated with negative (or unpleasant) memories typically tends to fade faster than the affect associated with positive (or pleasant) memories (see Kim & Jang, 2014).