INFORMATION TO USERS

This material was produced from a microfilm copy of the original document. While the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the original submitted.

The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand markings or patterns which may appear on this reproduction.

1. The sign or "target" for pages apparently lacking from the document photographed is "Missing Page(s)". If it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting thru an image and duplicating adjacent pages to insure you complete continuity.

2. When an image on the film is obliterated with a large round black mark, it is an indication that the photographer suspected that the copy may have moved during exposure and thus cause a blurred image. You will find a good image of the page in the adjacent frame.

3. When a map, drawing or chart, etc., was part of the material being photographed the photographer followed a definite method in "sectioning" the material. It is customary to begin photoing at the upper left hand corner of a large sheet and to continue photoing from left to right in equal sections with a small overlap. If necessary, sectioning is continued again - beginning below the first row and continuing on until complete.

4. The majority of users indicate that the textual content is of greatest value, however, a somewhat higher quality reproduction could be made from "photographs" if essential to die understanding of the dissertation. Silver prints of "photographs" may be ordered at additional charge by writing the Order Department, giving the catalog number, title, author and specific pages you wish reproduced.

5. PLEASE NOTE: Some pages may have indistinct print. Filmed as received.

Xerox University Microfilms 300 North Zeeb Road Ann Arbor, Michigan 48106 ft 8k'1- SOSTARICH, Judith Mary, 1946- A STUDY OF THE READING BEHAVIOR OF SIXTH-GRADERS: COMPARISON OF ACTIVE AND OTHER READERS.

The Ohio State University, Ph.D., 1974 I Education, general §; I¥

1University Microfilms, A XEROX Com pany, Ann Arbor, Michigan

© 1974

JUDITH MARY SOSTARICH

ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

THIS DISSERTATION HAS BEEN MICROFILMED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED. A STUDY OP THE READING BEHAVIOR OF SIXTH-GRADERS :

COMPARISON OF ACTIVE AND OTHER READERS

DISSERTATION

Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for

the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate

School of The Ohio State University

By

Judith Mary Sostarich, B.S., M.A.

*****

. The Ohio State University 1974

Reading Committee: Approved by Dr. Alexander Frazier Dr. Charlotte Huck Dr. Paul Klohr

Adviser Department of Early and Middle Childhood Education ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

My sincere appreciation goes to the members of

my committee. Dr. Alexander Frazier, chairman, offered

unfailing support, encouragement, and counsel. Dr. Char­

lotte Huck's advice helped to direct the scope of this

study. Dr. Paul Klohr has supported me throughout my

graduate training.

My gratitude is extended to the sixth-grade tea­

chers, school librarians, principals, and administrators who graciously cooperated with this study. Finally, I would like to thank the sixth-grade children who took an

active and interested role in this project. VITA

July 3, 1946...... Born - Milwaukee, Wisconsin

1969...... B.S., University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, Wisconsin

1969...... Third-Grade Teacher, New Berlin Public Schools, New Berlin, Wisconsin

1969-1970...... Fifth-Grade Teacher, Greenwich Public Schools, Greenwich, Connecticut

1970-1971...... University Fellow, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio

1971...... M.A., The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio

1971-1973 ...... Teaching Associate, Department of Early and Middle Childhood Education, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio

1973-1974 ...... University Fellow, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio

FIELDS OF STUDY

Major Field: Early and Middle Childhood Education

Studies in Early and Middle Childhood Education. Professor Alexander Frazier

Studies in Children's Literature and Language Arts. Professor Charlotte Huck

Studies in Curriculum. Dr. Paul Klohr TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS...... ii

V I T A ...... iii

LIST OF TABLES ...... vii

Chapter

I. THE NATURE OF THE STUDY...... 1

Statement of the Problem ...... 5 Hypotheses of the Study...... 8 Conceptual Assumptions and Limitations . . 11 Definition of Terms...... 12 Summary...... 13

II. REVIEW OF LITERATURE ON MATURITY AND PURPOSE IN R E A D I N G ...... 14

Performance and Maturity in Reading. .. . 15 Aspects of Reading Behavior...... 22 Methods of Improving Reading Behavior. . . 67 Summary...... 71

III. METHODS AND PROCEDURES...... 72

S a m p l e ...... 72 Development of Instruments ...... 77 Procedure...... 80 Design of the Study...... 83 Treatment of the Data...... 85 Summary...... 86

iv Table of Contents (Continued) Page

Chapter

IV. ANALYSIS OF THE DATA

PART Is GENERAL FINDINGS...... 88

Sixth-Grade Readers...... 90 Active Readers Compared with Other Readers 95 Sex Difference in Reading Behavior of Active Readers ...... 104 Differences Among Eleven Classrooms. . . . 107 Summary...... Ill

PART II: CASE STUDIES OF ACTIVE READERS . . 114

Case Study 1: Mary...... 114 Case Study 2: Jane...... 118 Case Study 3: Dick...... 121 Case Study 4: Sally ...... 124 Case Study 5: Stan...... 127 Case Study 6: Pa t ...... 130 Summary...... 132

PART III: INVENTORY DATA...... 135

Reading vs. Television...... 135 Past Reading Experiences ...... 137 Other Sources of Reading Materials .... 140 Frequency of Library U s e ...... 142 Types of Materials Read...... 146 Reading Series Books ...... 148 Favorite Books ...... 151 Favorite School Subjects ...... 152 Summary...... • ...... 154

V. DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS...... 158

Restatement of Purposes...... 159 Procedures...... 159 Limitations of the Study ...... 162 Summary of the Study ...... 163 D i scussion...... 171 Possibilities for Further Study...... 179

v Table of Contents (Continued) Page

APPENDIX

A. LIBRARIAN-TEACHER NOMINATION FORM...... 187

B. STUDENT NOMINATION FORM...... 189

C. MY READING HABITS AND INTERESTS INVENTORY. . .191

D. READING REPORT RECORD...... 195

E. TEACHER ESTIMATION FORM...... 197

F. PARENTS' PERMISSION LETTER ...... 199

G. PARENTS' INTERVIEW LETTER...... 201

H. INTERVIEW QUESTIONNAIRES...... 203

I. REPORTS FROM THE SCHOOLS ...... 217

J. TABULATION OF FAVORITE BOOKS OR BOOKS REPORTED REREAD BY CHILDREN...... 223

BIBLIOGRAPHY . . 234 LIST OP TABLES

Table Page

1. Means and Standard Deviations of Quotient Scores by Classroom on the California Test of Mental Maturity...... 74

2. Means and Standard Deviations of Reading Scores by Classroom on the Iowa Test of Basic Skills...... 75

3. Means and Standard Deviations of Reading Scores by Classroom on the California Achievement Test...... 75

4. Number of Active and Other Readers...... 81

5. Summary of Materials Read on Weekdays and Weekends...... 89

6. Boys and Girls' Responses to Number of Pur­ poses and Number of Materials Read...... 91

7. Children's Responses to Weekend and Weekday R e a d i n g ...... 92

8. Comparison of Number of Girls and Boys in Active and Other Groups ...... 96

9. Active and Other Readers' Responses to Num­ ber of Purposes and Materials Read...... 97

10. Comparison of Mean Mental Age Scores of Active and Other Children in Nine Classrooms. 99

11. Comparison of Mean Reading Scores of Active and Other Readers ...... 101

12. Teachers' Estimate of Active and Other Readers' General Ability...... 102

13. Girl and Boy Active Readers' Responses to Number of Purposes and Materials Read . . . .105

vii List of Tables (Continued)

Table Page

14. Percent of Active Readers per Classroom. . . 107

15. Average Number of Purposes, Book, News­ papers, Magazines, and Comic Books Read by the Children in the Eleven Classrooms. . . . 109

16. Analysis of Variance on Purposes for Reading Among Classrooms...... 110

17. Average Time Spent Reading and Viewing Television in Hours...... 136

18. Percent of Children's Responses to Reading Experience Questions ...... 138

19. Percent of Children's Responses to Sources of Materials ...... 141

20. Children's Percent Responses to Number of School Library Visits...... 143

21. Children's Percent Responses to Number of Community Library Visits ...... 144

22. Materials Reported as Read Daily by C h i l d r e n ...... 147

23. Average Number of Series Books Read...... 149-

24. Children's Favorite Subjects Reported in P e r c e n t s ...... 153

viii CHAPTER I

THE NATURE OF THE STUDY

Reading has always played a significant role in the lives of the American people. Newspapers, political tracts, and books have been regarded as an important part of the American heritage, with our writers offering oppos­

ing opinions and attitudes on many social and political issues. A free people needs to be able to read to judge, question, and evaluate opposing viewpoints as well as to expand personal experience toward the attainment of rich and satisfying personal lives.

Although the merits of other media that supple­ ment reading have been offered as alternatives, many people still maintain there is a necessity and a specialty about reading. Dale (1952), who has carried on continuous stud­ ies of the merits of the cinema, television, radio, and reading, discusses two values of reading that are not present in other media: printed materials provide the most illuminating and varied records of human experience that are now available: and, at the reader's convenience, they can be examined and restudied time and again to

1 2

acquire clear understandings, develop rational attitudes,

and reach sound conclusions.

Early in American history, the task of teaching

children to read moved from the home to the school. Since

then schools have been expected to teach children to read well enough to weigh differing views, digest news, and

contemplate and enjoy the printed word. Reading is there­

fore considered a major part of the school's curriculum.

The public's interest in children's reading performance has become even more noticeable in recent years with the growing demand for accountability. Standardized reading achievement scores are published in the newspapers so that all concerned may check the reports to see whether children are reading at grade level.

However, in recent years, professional interest has moved beyond the teaching of basic reading skills to a growing concern for finding better ways to encourage voluntary reading activity by children. Nila Banton Smith

(1963) expresses the importance of a wider interpretation of the term "reading."

Of what value are all our efforts to establish proficiency in the basic skills of reading if children do not make the fullest use of these skills to enrich their lives, both as children at present and as adults later on? Permanent carryover interest in reading has long been stated as the ultimate goal of all reading instruction. This goal is now more urgent than ever before because of the many competing communication agencies with which reading is confronted (p. 387). 3

Similarly, Tinker and McCullough (1968) suggest

that "the acquisition of skill in reading can benefit a

person little unless he puts it to use in voluntary and

spontaneous reading" (p. 301). Bissett (1962) adds the

student's role to this discussion. " "The ends of reading

instruction are never in sight until individual learners

begin to develop the habit of reading out to reading to

satisfy inner needs and interests" (p. 8) .

A somewhat earlier study by Gray and Rogers (1956)

showed similar concern. When their scale of reading

maturity was applied to a sample community in the United

States, one of the most interesting findings was that no

subject emerged with superior reading habits. For the

sample as a whole, the authors concluded that "without doubt, the most impressive fact revealed relates to the predominately low rankings given to these cases" (p. 166) .

Only by searching the country were they able to identify a handful of exemplars to validate the upper limits of their scales.

Characteristic of the Gray and Rogers' criteria for reading maturity was that only one of the basic measures dealt with reading comprehension, that is, "the recogni­ tion and construction of meaning." The remaining criteria were concerned with the selection and use of the material read. Clearly the implication is that reading efficiency, 4

however necessary, is not enough in itself to insure read­

ing maturity.

Although reading authorities build strong argu­

ments for this position, the principal emphasis in today's

schools still goes to- efficiency. Examination of most

basal series shows that the importance of voluntary read­

ing is recognized, however slightly it may be actually

stressed in practice. The manual of instruction typically

includes suggested additional reading for nearly every

story. In their manual, Scott, Foresman, and Company

(1965) note that they hope to foster "attitudes and habits

that characterize a mature reader" (p. 14). Houghton

Mifflin (1968) offer a series that "leads a student to

sound critical judgment and a greater capacity for enjoy­

ing and profiting from his reading— now and in the years

to come" (p. I ). In practice, however, shill training

often dominates the curriculum as it does the pages of

the manual.

However preoccupied with skills teaching their

introduction to reading may have been, children in the elementary school exhibit mature or active reading habits by the time they are eleven or twelve years of age. By then, their level of skill development is such that they are able to select reading materials freely and read with little outside aid. Parents, teachers, and reading auth­ orities often refer to this age level as the "Golden Age 5

of Reading." Huck and Kuhn (1961) note that children

spend more time reading at this age than at any other.

Terman and Lima (1931) consider eleven- and twelve-year

old children to be in a "reading craze." Amount of time

spent reading and interest in reading sharply decreases

when children enter junior high schools.

Statement of the Problem

Children develop reading behavior that includes

habits and interests as well as skills. Some children

exhibit the motivation to read many types of materials

for varied reasons. Their reading behavior, or reading

pattern, constantly grows as they become increasingly

mature readers. Although they are a minority in most

classrooms, they are recognized by the teachers, librarians,

and students as active readers. They use the library

frequently, read many books, and are familiar with varied

types of reading materials. The majority of children

perceive reading in a more limited way; they see reading

as a necessity for school work but may not find varied uses for the skills in their everyday lives.

A comparison of these two groups of readers has

formed the basis of the present study. Reading behavior as studied here has included the reasons children perceive

for their reading as well as the number and types of mater­ ials they read. By comparing reports of active and other 6

readers, characteristics in the reading behavior of

active and other reader have been identified.

Further study of the active readers1 reading

behavior has included descriptions drawn from their read­

ing histories. Environmental and personal experiences

may have encouraged these children to read. What past

events or present activities have influenced the active

readers' mature perceptions of the numerous and varied possibilities for reading?

Finally, a number of general aspects of reading behavior that may help categorize children as active or

other readers have been explored. What aspects in the reading behavior of these children can be identified and may be used to show a more complete picture of active readers? What reading habits are in most general use by active readers? Does the amount and kind of material or specific reading experiences differ among active and other readers? In other words, what are the profiles of active and other readers?

Assuming that maturity in reading is a concept which includes aspects that can be described, the present study has first attempted to identify "active" or "mature" readers. Sixth-grade subjects in eleven classrooms were selected from suburban schools with school libraries and school librarians. Teachers, librarians, and students 7

were asked to nominate students who used printed matter

continuously and who had exhibited an individualistic

interest in reading- Those students receiving three or

more nominations were considered "active" readers. Com­

parisons between the active and other readers in the class­

rooms have been examined in an attempt to isolate unique

aspects of the active children's reading behavior.

The major characteristic of the reading behavior

under consideration was the range of purposes the children

perceived for their reading. Children were asked to record

every item they had read and select a purpose for that

reading. Measures were collected six times during a three-

week period on consecutive Mondays and Fridays. Purpose

for reading, as defined by Helen Smith (1966), is "the

reading 'set' used to determine that which the reader

intended to get from the selection." Five purposes for

reading, adapted from Smith's list, have been used in. this

study. They include having pleasure, meeting assignments,

seeking personal interest information, following daily events, and answering questions.

Further description of the active readers has come

from teacher estimates of their general performance as students and a standardized measure of reading ability and mental age.

Six readers reported to be "active" readers were interviewed to develop a more comprehensive view of active 8

child readers. Parents and teachers were also questioned

in an attempt to discover more detailed and unique infor­

mation about the home, school, and personal environments

of the readers.

Finally, in an attempt to secure a broader picture

of the children's reading behavior, an inventory entitled * "My Reading Habits and Interests" was given to all sub­

jects in the study. Questions concerned amount of time

spent reading, sources of reading materials, and favorite

books.

The purposes for this study may be identified as

follows:

1. To describe the relationship of factors in the reading behavior of sixth-grade children.

2. To describe the differences of active and other sixth-grade readers.

3. To describe the relationship of various fac­ tors in the reading behavior of active readers.

4. To describe the relationship of reading behav­ ior and classroom groupings of sixth-grade readers.

5. To describe the school, home, and personal environment of six active readers.

Hypotheses of the Study

The five problem statements have given rise to a series of hypotheses to be tested: 9

1. A purpose of this study is to describe the rela­ tionship of factors in the reading behavior of the entire group of sixth-grade readers.

H.: Boys will read for a greater range of pur­ poses than will girls.

H2: Girls will read more books than boys.

KL: The children will read for more purposes during the week than on weekends.

H^: The children will read more books on weekdays than on weekends.

H(.: The children will read more magazines, news­ papers, and comic books on weekends than on weekdays.

Hg: The boys will read more magazines, newspapers, and comic books than girls.

2. A purpose of this study is to describe the differences of active and other sixth-grade readers.

H^: There will be significantly more girls in the group selected as active readers than in the other group.

Hq : Children selected as active readers will report rereading more books than other read­ ers .

Hgi Active readers will read more books than other readers.

H^q : Active readers will read more magazines, news- . papers and comic books than other readers.

H.,: The mean mental age scores of the active readers will be higher than the mean of the other readers.

H.2: Children selected as active readers will have a higher mean reading achievement* score than will other readers. 10

H,o: Teachers will classify a significant number of active readers as above average in their general performance as students as compared to the other readers.

H. Children selected as active readers will read for a greater range of purposes than will children in the other group.

3. A purpose of this study is to describe the relationship of various factors in the reading behavior of active readers.

H. j- J Active boy readers will read for a wider range of purposes than active girl readers.

H.fi: Active girl readers will read a greater num­ ber of books than will active boy readers.

H.7: Active boy readers will read a greater num­ ber of comic books, magazines, and newspapers than will active girl readers.

H.g: Active girl readers will reread more books than active boy readers.

4. A purpose of this study is to describe the relationship of reading behavior and classroom groupings of sixth-grade readers.

H.q : Children in one classroom will read for a wider range of purposes than will children in other classrooms.

H2Q: Children in one classroom will read more books than children in the other classrooms.

H21: Children in one classroom will read more magazines, newspapers, or comic books than children in another classroom.

5. A purpose of this study is to describe the school, home, and personal environment of six active readers. 11

Case study descriptions will be given for the six active readers.

Conceptual Assumptions and Limitations

The schools selected for this study were in two suburban areas. Therefore, the population was select and findings are a reflection of that population.

Sixth-grade children have been assumed to have mastered sufficient reading skills to possess the ability to read independently. Their reports have been assumed to be an honest and detailed summary of their reading during the three-week period.

The teachers* perception of the children's general ability was recognized to involve subjective factors yet is the one usually relied upon in most grading situations in the elementary school.

The definition of "purpose for reading" as used in this study was an applied interpretation of the term, simplified for the understanding of the subjects. Per­ ceived purposes or reasons for reading were regarded as one aspect of the reading act that can help to identify a mature reading behavior.

The mental age and reading achievement scores of the active readers were compiled during an earlier period and could be subject to slight differences in interpreta­ tion. However, the scores were standardized and are 12

generally used in school systems as measures of children's

abilities.

Definition of Terms

Active readers: Students in the 11 sixth-grade

classrooms chosen by at least three of the nominating

groups as children who used ^printed matter continuously

and had exhibited an individualistic interest in reading.

Other readers; Students in the 11 sixth-grade

classrooms who were not so chosen.

Nominating groups; The school librarians, sixth-

grade teachers, and the children in the 11 sixth-grade

classrooms.

Purposes for reading; The reading "set" used to

determine that which the reader intended to get from the

selection as perceived by the children.

Range of purposes: The number of different pur­

poses for reading reported by the children during the

three-week period.

Mental age and reading achievement scores: Stand­

ardized test scores reported in the children's records.

Tests were previously administered by the school system.

General performance as a student; The teachers' perception of the children's abilities in the classroom; the grades the teachers would award to each child in ques­ tion based upon his ability. 13

Summary

This study has been an attempt to describe the

actual reading behavior of active readers in the elemen­

tary school- Since experts maintain that the child's

reading reaches a peak at the end of his elementary school

years, sixth-grade students have been used in the study.

Active readers were identified and considered in

relation to other readers of the same age. All children were asked to report their perceived reasons for reading

in an attempt to isolate unique characteristics in the

reading behavior of active readers.

A general description of other aspects of the children * s reading behavior has been based upon children's responses to an inventory that required them to describe their reading habits and interests. A more specific and in-depth description of the reading environment and moti­ vation of six active readers was sought through a case study approach.

In the following chapter, a review of related lit­ erature is presented. CHAPTER II

REVIEW OF LITERATURE ON MATURITY AND

PURPOSE IN READING

Theorists and researchers have studied the total reading performance of readers, specific and distinguish­ able aspects or characteristics of the individual's read­ ing behavior, and methods to improve and expand reading activity and performance. In attempting to identify distinguishable characteristics of a sixth-grade reader, the present study has relied primarily upon work that describes the individual’s total reading performance.

However, instruments used in the study to develop a more detailed picture of the children's reading environments were based upon studies of aspects of the individual's reading performance and methods to improve reading activity. Therefore, these three major approaches to the study of the reading experience are reviewed. They offer a varied look at reasons people express for reading.

14 15

Performance and Maturity in Reading

Many studies and analyses have been made of general

aspects of the adult reader's total performance. In a

review of research on adult reading, Asheim (1955) defined

"active" readers as thorse who are able to sustain an

interest in serious content as opposed to those who are

able merely to derive meaning from written symbols. He maintained that literacy is basic to an adult's ability to read but does not guarantee that the adult will become a self-committed reader. Gray and Rogers (1956), calling the successful readers a "mature" reader, contended that this person usually has a focus, or radix, of interest to which much of his reading relates and which serves as an inner drive or motivating force. Strang (1942), in describing adult reading patterns, noted a similar core or radix of interest that determines, more or less, the nature of the reading. Children were questioned in the case studies of the present research to determine if they had a special interest that motivated their reading. The central core theory of Gray and Rogers was expanded by

Gray (1958) when he defined aspects of efficient reading that help to build a center of interest. These include interest in. reading, purposes for reading, the recogni­ tion and construing of meaning, reaction to and use of ideas read, and the kinds of materials read. He saw read­ ing as a complex activity. One aspect of Gray's list, 16 purpose for reading, was used extensively in the present

study.

Many authors have described mature readers or characteristics of a mature reading behavior. Often maturity in reading and maturity in actions of individuals are assumed to be interrelated characteristics. Strang

(1942) found the following characteristics to be asso­ ciated with maturity in reading: a wide reading vocabu­ lary, superior comprehension of what was read, broad interest in social and world problems, an above average rate of reading, and a high abstract verbal ability.

After an extensive examination of levels of maturity in reading, Gray and Rogers (1956) concluded:

Maturity in reading as one aspect of total develop­ ment is distinguished by the attainment of those interests, attitudes, and skills which enable young people and adults to participate eagerly, independently, and effectively in all the reading activities essential to a full, rich, and produc­ tive life. It is assumed that, in the satisfac­ tion of interests and needs through reading, a mature reader will continue to grow in capacity to interpret broadly and deeply (p. 56).

This definition and the work of Gray and Rogers serves as the basis for the present study. Center (1952), in a discussion of the essential characteristics of a mature reader, talked about the habit of reading for "intelligent delight" and the "irresistible compulsion to read." In her judgment, a mature reader reads for many purposes, which include pleasure, information, understanding, 17

guidance, and inspiration. This study has attempted to

support this contention. Center contends that the mature,

critical reader comprehends not only surface meanings but

also implied meanings. The mature reader also brings wide

experiences to his interpretation of the materials read

and, in turn, greatly enriches his background of under­

standing through vicarious experience. Adler (1942) be­

lieved that maturity in reading includes the ability to use

reading to discover ways of participating effectively as

a member of society. In a discussion of elements of a

mature reader, Adler included reading as a tool for the

realization of one's own potentials, for knowing and under­

standing others and the society of which one is a part,

and for achieving the good society as the reader develops

for himself. Wollner (1949), in a study of sixth-grade

children's voluntary reading, noted that the method used

to study maturity must include more than an interest sur­

vey. She believed an individual's reading must be studied

with the same care, philosophy, and techniques that would

be used to study an individual's total behavior. According

* to Wollner, the teacher, librarian, or guidance worker

should seek to fill in the portrait of the child as a

reader. Accordingly the present research has relied on 0 the opinions of librarians and teachers as well as the

pupils to select the most active readers. In a brief 18

treatise on the social effects of reading, Waples (1940)

looked at reading and the total personality of the reader.

He noted that a marked social effect on individuals re­

sults from the reader's reinforcement of favorable pre­

dispositions to self, a desire to escape, and an instru­

mental need to solve problems.

Artley (1967) and Cleland (1969) placed more

emphasis upon the actual reading act when discussing

maturity in reading. Artley divided progress toward

maturity in reading into two parts, growth in reading and

growth through reading. Growth in reading refers to the

acquisition of skills and abilities that make for skill­

ful and effective reading as an interpretative act.

Growth through reading refers to desirable changes taking

place in the child as a result of having read, including

changes in attitudes, ways of thinking, overt behavior,

understandings, interests and tastes. Cleland defined

reading operationally as the cognitive process of per­

ceiving and ordering the environment. Reading becomes a means of attaining an ability to meet, understand, and deal with environment.

In their discussions of younger readers, Townsend

(1965) and Jacobs (1956) maintained that the material read is an important aspect of the mature reading act. Town­ send contended that the personality of the student and 19

the material he reads are interrelated and demand equal

attention. t The interplay of the learner with his subject matter— the impact, if you will, of the curri­ culum on the student— demands full attention. Both he who reads, and what is read, matter (Townsend, 1965, p. 303).

For this reason, pupils were ashed to report what they

read as well as why they read the materials in the pre­

sent study.

Jacobs (1956) listed three goals that may inspire

a child's desire to read: information, entertainment,

and self-understanding and self-enhancement. He regarded the reader and the material read as equal forces in the

successful reading experience. According to Jacobs, un­

less reading has done something to the reader, it probably has not been a very vital experience.

While asserting the importance of developing the reading habit, Frank (1960) described omnivorous child readers and children who read very little. She noted that avid readers find their way to books no matter what obstacles are put in their path. Presumably, to such children, reading is a preoccupation in itself. The children who read very little, according to Frank, are at least reading something. In other words, they have mastered the skills of reading, but reading itself has not become a pleasurable pursuit for them. In the 20 present study, six active or avid readers were questioned to ascertain whether reading was a preoccupation in it­ self for them.

A number of studies have attempted to describe levels of reading maturity in high school and elementary- age children. High school-age children were divided into three categories in a study by Allan and MacDonald (1971).

The "natural readers" develop independent areas of inter­ ests and have no difficulty in finding a book to their taste. The opposite extreme, children labeled as "non- readers," resist attempts to become interested in reading although a few may have mastered the mechanics of reading.

The "reluctant readers" are considered to be the majority of readers whose interest in or knowledge of books is limited, but whose reading skills are adequate. Against this conception of differences among readers, this pro­ ject attempted to stimulate greater interest in reading for pleasure among generally apathetic readers in a Great

Britain second-year high school class. The class was informed they were to read and review fiction with a

Scottish setting over a five-month period, the end pro­ duct of which was to be a booklet that might be a useful guide to other classes. They report an increase in book reading which carried over to the next year. Henderson and Long (1970), in a study of 150 achieving fifth-graders, directed their attention toward the quality and variety 21

of choices of boohs as well as to the number of books - « read. Indices of quantity, variety, and quality were

derived from (1) records of library usage, (2) the child's

own record of boohs read, and (3) time records of out-of­

school activities. The investigators decided that effi­

cient readers do not necessarily attain the broad pattern

of maturity conceptualized by Gray and Rogers (1956).

This finding would support Asheim's (1956) contention, in

his discussion of adult reading, the "'active' readers

seldom mahe up more than 25 to 30 percent of any group

that reflects, with a reasonable accuracy, a cross sec­

tion of the total population" (p. 8) .

The growth of a child toward becoming a mature

reader contains two developments, according to Margaret

Early (1960) in a review of research. She noted that

progress in "growing into wisdom" and maturing in liter­

ary appreciation parallel each other. Strang (1942) re­

ported three levels of development, which include uncon­

scious enjoyment, self-conscious appreciation, and con­

scious delight. The second stage is the pinnacle for

elementary students, according to Strang. At this stage,

the reader moves from simple interest in the story to peeper questioning of causality. He asks "why" when he

reads- Strang also noted that in this stage, the child takes a more active role in trying to select things that he likes. This stage is similar to the fifth level 22

of Wittick's (1960) pattern of development of taste in

reading. According to Wittick, the child at this level

begins to understand that reading can help him develop new

ideas and new experiences. An equal relationship between

book and reader develops. Strang and Wittick's theories

assert that there'is a noticeable movement from passive

acceptance to active participation on the part of the

reader.

Aspects of Reading Behavior

Separate and distinguishable aspects of the

reader's performance have been discussed and studied by-

various theorists and researchers.

Purposes

One set of factors used to describe the reading behavior of adults and children includes awareness of

purposes leading to reading and the number of purposes

actually leading to reading. These two purposes were

developed in depth in the present study. Gray and Rogers

(1956) included both items as segments of a reading pro­

file. Summarizing earlier research, Huus (1963a) reported

that not many adults chose to read in their spare time.

She proposed that a possible factor contributing to this

situation might be that little is done in schools to pro­

mote a lifetime interest in recreation reading. In this 23

study, teachers were questioned as to methods they use to promote reading. Huss (1963a) also has identified fiye purposes for reading: to satisfy curiosity, to check in­

formation, to retreat into a fanciful or exciting world, to use time, and to enjoy the story. Children read less

for prestige than do adults, she believed,- although even youngsters might be heard comparing the number of books they read. Although Stauffer (1963) was primarily address­ ing himself to reading instruction in a discussion of children's reading in basal textbooks, he emphasized the importance of setting purposes for reading. According to

Stauffer, selecting, appraising, questioning appropriately, and pursuing answers are the hallmarks of a good reader.

It is apparent that both reading and thinking start with a state of doubt or of desire. It is apparent also that the process of recon­ structing goes on as inquiry or discovery, until the doubt is resolved, the perplexity settled, or the pleasure attained (Stauffer, 1963, p. 542).

Dublin (1971) coined the term "professionally oriented reader" to apply to an adult who is actively aware of purpose for reading. Dulin described an instructional program in reading for the professional level adult in terms of five criteria: who he is, what his goals are, what particular needs he has in reading, what particular reading materials he works with, and how teaching method­ ology must.be adapted to him. There are two main goals 24

for the professionally oriented reader. The first is how

to meet the reading needs of his daily work faster, more

efficiently, and with as little waste motion as possible.

The second is a broadened range of general reading skills

with concentration on a more expressive vocabulary. Huus,

Stauffer, and Dulin would agree on the importance of set­

ting purposes in the individual's total reading pattern.

Research concerning the ability of students to set

purposes for reading has yielded varying results. Emans

and Patyk (1967) used a forced-choice type of question­

naire to study the reading motives of high school stu­ dents, relating the effect of intelligence, reading level,

sex, age, and social class to informational, identifica-

tional, esthetic, and recreational motives. Definitions of each motive and a list of reasons for reading were included in a questionnaire given to 324 students in grades nine through twelve. Results showed that the recreational motive ranks highest for all students.

Henderson (1965) compared the extent to which 24 good and 24 poor fifth-grade readers could set and achieve purposes for reading. The students were interviewed individually. Each child was asked to read four stories silently. The responses to the stories were tape re­ corded and rated. Purpose-setting achievement was mea­ sured by the summed scores of four subordinate rating 25

scales: number of conjectures, number of purposes, use

of evidence, and oral expression. Tbe conclusion was

that good readers are more effective than poor readers in

setting purposes for reading and that setting purposes is

positively related to their attainment.

After a thorough study of the literature relating

to purposes for reading, Helen Smith (1965) noted that

authorities, for the most part, have not defined "pur­

pose" as they use the term in their writing. In lieu of

a definition, they have presented lists or examples

through which the reader must infer the definitions.

Smith found at least two kinds of purposes for reading

included in most lists: (1) the life purposes or motives

for reading, which she considered primary purposes, and

(2) desired behaviors and/or instructional objectives

related to comprehension, which she termed secondary purposes. The primary purposes included enjoyment,

intellectual demands, utilitarian purposes, socioeconomic demands, vocational or avocational interests, personal or social needs, problem solving, and spiritual or religious needs. The present researcher developed applied interpretations of Smith's primary purposes for this study. Smith (1966) defined purpose as the "set used to determine that which the reader intends to get from the selection." On the basis of this analysis, she 26

studied differences between 30 twelfth-grade students

divided into two groups, good readers and poor readers,

on the basis of reading, English, and intelligence tests.

A case study method involving a structured interview was

developed to secure evidence of the success of the sub-

. , jects in answering product questions and of the processes

involved when the students read for two different purposes,

details and general impressions. The evidence secured

supported the hypothesis that good readers read for the

purposes of details and general impressions with equal

success. The poor readers read with more facility for

the purpose of getting details than for the purpose of

getting general impressions. Finally, Smith's data sup­

ported the hypothesis that good readers use different pro­

cedures when they read for widely divergent purposes such

as for details and for general impressions; poor readers

do not differentiate these two purposes in reading.

Asheim (1956) maintained that purpose setting for

reading is in part socially motivated. "For even when a

man can read and has reading materials readily available,

he does not necessarily read if the rewards of reading

are not apparent to him" (p. 27) . 27

Response to Literature

A second set of factors often discussed in an

evaluation of reading patterns includes a reader's re- <> sponse to literature. This response includes interpre­

tation, taste, appreciation, attitude, and interest var­

iables. Rosenblatt (1938) discussed response to litera­

ture and maintained that the interaction of book and

reader is essential if reading is actually to become an experience for an individual. "In the molding of any

specific literary experience, what the student brings to literature is as important as the literary text itself"

(Rosenblatt, 1938, p. 82). In the present study, the researcher has sought to discover aspects of the readers' personalities in the case study approach. In a review of related research, Huus (1964a) reported difficulty in classifying-studies in this field since the development of such intangibles has not been effectively measured.

Also, levels of response have not been experimentally defined.

Purves and Beach (1972), in their review of the literature, defined response to literature as a complex process that consists of interrelated parts: understand­ ing, the possession of information and the ability to grasp verbal and human complexities, psychological readi­ ness to become as objective as one can, the concomitant 28

psychological ability to enter into the world of the

work, the use of various evaluative criteria, and the

ability to articulate critical statements. They also

assumed that understanding and liking are associated and

that readers are influenced by what they read. Research­

ers have found a relationship between response to litera­

ture and reading ability. In an analysis of children's

responses to humorous situations in literature, Monson

(1966) was concerned with the following problems: (1) a

comparison of two methods for eliciting children's

response to humor in literature; (2) an investigation

of children's judgment of humor in excerpts from chil­ dren's books; and (3) an investigation of children's

choices of specific types of humor. The investigator developed pencil and paper instruments for assessing the responses of 635 fifth-graders to selections of five types of humor: character humor, humor of surprise, humor of the impossible, humor of words, and humor of the ridiculous situation. Her conclusion was that chil­ dren with high intelligence often judge excerpts as more humorous than do children of middle or low intelligence.

In the Burgdorf study (1966), 432 children in grades four, five, and six were divided into two groups. One group read a story silently while the other heard the story read orally. All children were interviewed individually 29

on the questions about the selections and rated on their

answers. Questions pertaining to each of the literary

selections were designed to elicit inferences relating

to eight different categories of information: (1) pur­

pose or intent of the author, (2) setting of the story,

(3) elements of style, (4) plot development, (5) char­

acterization, (6) realism, (7) reader identification,

and (8) value judgments. Burgdorf concluded that chil­

dren with higher reading scores are better able to draw

inferences when asked to read the stories themselves.

However, when the stories are read aloud to all the

children, there is no significant difference in response patterns between or among the intelligence groups. She

reported that scores on drawing inferences from literary

selections are significantly higher when children listen to stories read to them than when they read the stories.

In Peltola's (1963) study of first-grade chil­ dren's responses to art in picture books, 194 first- grade children were interviewed. Each child was shown eight pairs of books and asked to decide which book in the pair he would like to have a chance to read. The chil­ dren were free to handle and examine the books; never more than two books were available at one time; no child saw any book more than once. Sex differences were not­ able in the middle of the continuum of rated books, but 30

both boys and girls agreed upon the top four choices.

Burgdorf (1966) and Monson (1966) also reported no sex

difference in the children's responses to stories.

Weekes (1929), Ring (1968), Morris (1970), and

Cooper (1969) rioted the importance of the child's inter­

action with the work. I-ri a study of figurative language

in poetry, Weekes concluded that the child's actual exper­

iences are an important element in the child's ability to

interpret poetry and as such influence children's choices.

Weekes' study was an inquiry into the effect of certain

factors of meaning on children's choices of poetry. It was devised to test (1) the extent to which figurative

language and involved sentence structure affect children's choices of poetry and (2) the extent to which actual exper­ ience as a factor of meaning affects children's choices of poetry. In the preliminary study, a sampling was made of the intermediate grade courses of 35 school systems.

The themes of all poems found listed were classified to discover any significant trends and the results served as a guide in the selection of test material. Forty-one poems in the original and 17 simplified versions of these poems constituted the test material. Four tests and one ques­ tionnaire were administered to 412 students in three cities and six small communities. The collected data supported the assertion that actual experience has an 31

influence on children's choices of poetry. Children

tend to select poetry that embodies a known experience.

Ring (1968) noted that adolescent readers need

assistance in overcoming the problem of responding ego-

centrically to literature. Four characteristic reading

and interpreting difficulties were identified in a review

of related research: failure to maintain an adequate dis­

tance from the works, failure to perceive the focus of

the stories, invention, and egocentrism. Subjects were

62 pupils from two twelfth-grade, college preparatory

English classes. The data analyzed were free written

responses to two short stories. Two groups of 12 sub­

jects were selected for comparison: group A consisted of

subjects who described an interpretive process they usually employ; group B consisted of subjects who stated they were unaware of any such process. Ring found for both groups that free responses were characteristically limited in scope and confined to a basic pattern of evaluation in terms of personal preference, description and reiteration of details, and, to a lesser extent, interpretation. The subjects typically dealt with the stories as objective reports of human behavior rather than literary works. Ring implied that direct teaching of a sound interpretive process should be considered important in the teaching of adolescents. Morris (1970) 32

sought to secure insight into the nature of response

patterns of experience readers when reading a difficult

poem. He interviewed 15 high school upper classmen volun­

teers. Their free oral introspective comments given in

response to a difficult poem were taped as were their

retrospective answers to 32 questions concerning response

strategy. The responses were typed, divided into thought units, and coded according to a 12 category classification

scheme. Individual response profiles varied widely in each category of interpretation. Morris hypothesized that a developmental trend develops with immature, or younger readers, tending to place emphasis on general response and association to other activities while mature readers place increasing emphasis upon interpretation when reading a difficult poem. Only the most advanced readers move toward a more analytical response to poems.

Cooper (1969) sought to discover whether high school jun­ iors might have a relatively consistent way of responding to short stories. "Way of responding" was limited to four response categories: engagement, perception, interpreta­ tion, and evaluation. Finding that most of his young readers attempted to relate the world they perceived in the text to the world they already knew, Cooper concluded that interpretation is the most popular mode of response for high school juniors. 33

The interpretation aspect of response to literature has been studied by Rogers (1965) and Piekarz (1956).

Using eleventh-grade students as subjects, Rogers inves­ tigated individual differences in the interpretive pro­ cess. A sample of 28 high school juniors, half low level and half high level readers, furnished the data. Each student was interviewed in terms of structured and unstruc­ tured questions about a selected short story and completed a questionnaire which explored the integration of the short story with the personal life of the reader within a ten-day period after the story had been read. Other questionnaires explored factors related to the inter­ pretive process, familiarity with short story writers, attitudes toward reading, and attitudes toward reading short stories. Rogers reported differences between the high and low groups of readers, as measured by an intelli­ gence test, occur in their comparative familiarity with the short story and with authors, in their ability to grasp literal meanings, and in their ability to grasp implied meanings. Piedarz (1956) secured recordings of interviews with sixth-grade high level readers following silent and oral reading of selected passages. During the interviews, each pupil verbalized his thought and feeling concerning what he had read and answered 30 questions, the answers to which involved nine aspects of interpreta­ tion. Using a rating scale similar to the Gray and Rogers' 34

(1956) profile, Piedarz analyzed the responses and found

marked differences between high and low level readers.

High level readers are able to produce greater variety

and number of responses, implying a greater penetration

i:han their peers. Low level readers limit their responses

to literal meanings and give only passing attention to

implied meanings and critical reactions. High level

readers remain objective and impersonal in their inter­ pretation while low level readers have difficulty in main­ taining an objective attitude and in distinguishing be­ tween their own and the author's ideas. Even though high level readers possess strong feelings toward the ideas presented in a story, they are able to control their reac­ tions and to distinguish clearly between their own opinion and those of the author. Low level readers' evaluations expressed stem from an emotional rather than an intellec­ tual base and are highly personal in nature.

Developing Taste and Appreciation in Literature

Although taste is a component in the total reading response, little recent research has been reported. During the last 20 years, several studies have been conducted in regard to the newspaper, magazine, and book reading inter­ ests of children, but the topic of developing taste is rarely found discussed. However, theorists have discussed the issue. Seoasta (1968) defined good taste as 35

that quality which is the province of critic and teacher, that quality which each child ought to develop. It is needed, always, but especially now in a transition age when old values become strong in prescribing new ones (p. 22).

Dora Smith (1952) proposed putting boohs on reading ladders

that would go upward from a poor literary level to the heights of boohs by great authors.

Some researchers and theorists have maintained that quality booh selection is an indicator of improved taste in reading.

Booh reading rather than newspaper or magazine reading is a barometer . . . of taste. We tend to associate booh reading with those individuals who desire more substance, depth, and breadth them can be found in magazines and newspapers (Nila Banton Smith, 1963, p. 4).

Worh in the area of appreciation has often included discussions about taste in selecting or responding to lit­ erature. Carroll (1933) included three categories in a definition of appreciation: sensitivity to style, the ability to appreciate intellectually the deeper meanings of the article or booh being read, and the emotional capa­ city to respond to the fine shades of feeling found in the worhs of the best writers. He believed that some type of measurement of levels of appreciation is possible for teenage children, using short selections of varying merit. From her discussion of characteristics of sixth- grade readers, Early (1968) defined five different stages of literary appreciation for children and noted that 36

teachers should be able to identify the individual child's

progression through these stages. Early listed descriptors

of the five stages of reading. In the first stage, children

observe that authors often have certain specialties and

recognizable styles of writing. Children in the second

stage can recognize some stereotypes in characterization

and plot development. In the third stage, children have

discovered series books and read many of them. After more

experience with books, children begin to recognize differ­

ences in quality among books. At this fourth stage, they

are beginning to develop a set of literary criteria for

evaluating books. Finally, at the fifth stage, children

are able to recognize different forms of literature. Using

Early's stages, an inventory question in the present re­

search was developed to study the number and types of

series books read by the children.

According to Cooper (1971), research in the field of appreciation may be thought of as having three foci:

(1) aspects of the literature itself, (2) methods of teaching literature in the classroom, and (3) the charac­ teristics of the reader. Claiming both research and expert opinion as sources of validity, Carroll (1938) devised a test of prose appreciation for high school students con­ sisting of 12 sets of four prose extracts— one from a recognized author, one from a book generally considered 37

to be of poor quality, one from escapist fiction such as

found in romance or movie magazines, and one a mutilation

of a good story. All the extracts in one set were on the

same subject. Carroll gave the test to 3,000 Minnesota

high school students. Later versions of the test, now

out of print, were standardized on junior high and college

populations.

For their study of literary appreciation, Williams,

Winter, and Woods (1938) constructed a variety of tests.

On the Age Scale Test, the high school age subjects were

asked to rank a set of 15 compositions on the subject of

"school." In the prose part of the Ranking Method Test,

subjects sorted short prose extracts of a wide variety of

merit according to preference and then resorted according

to favorites. In the prose part of the Paired Comparison

Tests, subjects chose the better of two sentences. In

the Triple Comparison Tests, they were to choose the best

of three sentences in terms of sound of the sentence, logi­ cal construction, and aptness of particular words. In the connected prose part of the test, subjects were asked to choose the best from among three short prose extracts— the best usually from the Oxford Book of Prose. the middle selection from "an author of an intermediate type," and the worst from popular magazines. Even though the experi­ menters did not place much faith in the reliability coef­ ficients because the separate sections of the tests were 38

too short and "the alternative forms too imperfect," they

did find reliabilities for the various tests that ranged

from .36 to .94.

A second approach to assessing appreciation of

literature, the use of objective tests to measure specific

objectives, is exemplified most recently by the large-

scale National Assessment of Educational Progress (1971).

Achievement in literature, one of ten subjects and skills

included in the national assessment, was assessed in 1970-

1971. Those tested were students of nine, thirteen., and

seventeen years of age and young adults between 26 and 35

from all parts of the country. The objectives were written by specialists in literary study, evaluated by school peo­ ple and laymen, and then returned to the specialists for revision. The broad goals for literary study agreed upon were that the students should be able to (1) read litera­ ture of excellence, (2) become engaged in, find meanings

in, and evaluate a work of literature, and (3) develop a continuing interest and participation in literature and the literary experience. These then became the basis for the various "exercises" or tests.

More than 40 years ago, Broening (1929) reported evidence of the immediate value of direct teaching of specific elements of appreciation for the literary appre­ ciation of pupils in grades four, five, and six. Broening 39

devised measures of literary appreciation, using interest

studies and her own background in selecting content. After

teaching literary appreciation so defined to fourth-,

fifth-, and sixth-graders, she compared and interpreted the students' scores on an initial and final application

of the Broening-Literary Appreciation Test as an indica­

tor of growth in literary appreciation. LaBrant and Heller

(1939) reported the value of the opportunity afforded in

free reading periods for self-selection and appraisal in promoting more mature interests and tastes among pupils in grades seven through twelve. Their descriptive study was based on a seventh- and tenth-grade class at the Ohio

State University School. Records of their reading kept by the students for three years furnished the data.

A third way to get at appreciation of literature is by content analysis of the oral or written response.

The response can be free or it can be structured in reply to a set of specific questions.

Richards (1929) analyzed the difficulties, stumb­ lings, and misreadings of college students' readings of poetry. Richards made a practice of asking his own stu­ dents to write down their responses to a variety of poems without benefit of knowing the authors of the poems. As he reviewed the responses, he found the following to be the main problems of his readers: (1) inability to under­ stand the poem as a statement or expression, (2) inability 40

to perceive the form of the poem and the movement and rhythm of the lines, (3) inability to respond fully to

imagery, (4) tendency to be misled by erratic associations,

(5) reliance on stock responses, (6) proneness to senti­ mentality and inhibition, (7) unwillingness to judge the worth of the poetry alone apart from the worth of the views and beliefs about the world stated in the poetry, and

(8) unwillingness to judge a poem for its own merit. These deficiencies were the categories of his analysis.

Using the content analysis approach, Taba (1955) identified four categories of responses to literature: projection, generalizations, self-references, and irrele- vancies. The categories were devised from the objective state by the class in group meetings rather than from an expert analysis of the data later. Taba's study of high school students suggested a way to analyze a large amount of verbal response material to literature. Squire (1964) refined this approach. After recording and studying responses of 52 ninth- and tenth-grade students to four short stories, he then devised seven categories by which to code the response elements: literary judgments, inter- pretational responses, narrational reactions, associational responses, self-involvement, prescriptive judgments, and miscellaneous.

Studies that focus on the mechanics of literary appreciation include those by Choppin (1969) and Broom (1934). 41

Choppin looked at the difference between multiple choice

and open-ended test questions in measuring the understand­

ing and appreciation of literature. Two short poems, each

dealing with the subject of death, were chosen; and two

tests, one in multiple-choice format and the other with

open-ended questions, were constructed for each poem. Sub­

jects were 275 seventeen- and eighteen-year old students... .

Upon analyzing the results, Choppin concluded there is no

evidence that multiple-choice questions per se measure

anything different than open-ended questions per se. Broom was looking for a physical manifestation of appreciation by using a galvonometer to measure esthetic response and

found no significant physical manifestation of response when high school and college students read selections that had varying degrees of emotional content.

Attitude and Interest

The difficulty in much of the educational research concerned with attitude and interest is a failure to ade­ quately define and isolate distinguishing characteristics of the two terms. Getzels (1956) defined attitude as a

readiness to react in a particular direction with respect to the given object. We do not ordinarily speak of being driven by an attitude; we are neces­ sarily driven by our interests (p. 7). 42

According to Getzels, an interest is a

characteristic disposition, organized through experience, which impeled an individual to seek out particular objects, activities, understand­ ings, skills, or goals for attention to acquisi­ tion (p- 7) .

Gray and Rogers (1956) looked at the term, interest, from two perspectives. They defined an interest area as one about which a person was sufficiently concerned that he sought related material to read. The term "depth of inter­ est" was used to define the extent and degree of serious­ ness of the reader's penetration of a given topic or area.

Purves and Beach (1972) pointed out that the reasons for distinguishing between attitude and interest is that posi­ tive attitudes toward books may exist without the reader's ever really developing interest in active reading. For example, students may have a positive attitude toward con­ temporary fiction but never read it. The present study considered the time children spent reading and the inter­ ests they expressed in school subjects.

Zais (1969) contended that students' attitudes toward reading could not be isolated from personality factors and interests. In his study of sophistication of reading interests as related to selected personality factors and certain other characteristics of high school seniors, Zais contended that the investigator is rarely able to determine exactly which factors in the preferred 43

books appeal to the reader.

The evaluation of growth in sophistication of reading interests has most often taken the form of observing the reader's progression through 'freely chosen' materials. The limited effec­ tiveness of this technique is generally conceded, however, because the 'free choice' of the reader is subject to such contaminating influences as availability, adult influence, and readability (Zais, 1969, p. 273).

The present research sought to determine the extent of some

of the "contaminating influences" mentioned by Zais through the use of questions in the interest inventory. Zais developed the Sophistication of Reading Interests Scale as a measure of all the variables affecting appreciation.

On the other hand, Peifer (1962) maintained that a scale could be developed to measure students' attitudes toward reading. Using steps patterned after the methods of

Likert, Peifer developed an attitude scale to measure objectively students' attitudes toward reading in the secondary schools. The procedures used to develop the scale included collecting responses of 75 high school students, scoring the items, checking the test out by item analysis, checking on test reliability and validity, and constructing local norms. Using the scale as a measuring device, Peifer reported both low and high attitudes in reading are found in high school students in accelerated, average, and low reading ability classes at all grade levels. 44

Generally, it appears that there is agreement among

authorities in the reading field that attitude and amount

and hind of reading are closely related. For example,

the San Diego, California County Department of Education

(1961) developed an inventory attempting to measure the

reading attitude of elementary school pupils with the idea

that such information would be useful to teachers. Dauter-

man (1970) and McCaul (1949) studied the effects of prior

attitudes upon reading. Two purposes underly Dauterman's

study of dogmatism and reading: to determine the relation­

ship between closed mindedness and reading comprehension and

to test out a particular rationale for the teaching of

literature. Twenty high school students, from similar

socioeconomic backgrounds and with similar intellectual

abilities, were administered the Davis Reading Test, after which they read and recorded their spontaneous responses

to a short story. They also responded to a test on dogma­

tism and were ranked as open- or closed-minded, using the

Rokeach Dogmatism Scale, with open-minded students being

found to have higher reading comprehension scores than close-minded students. McCaul (1949) tested the hypo­ thesis that attitudes affect a reader's interpretation of printed matter. Two hundred and forty-one students read one of four short stories that were identical except for the name of the main protagonist. The four names used,

Tom, Roosevelt, Wilkie, and Hitler, were chosen as possible 45

indicators of preconceived attitudes. The students re­

sponded to a series of questions about the stories. McCaul

reported that pupils' attitudes tend to affect their inter­

pretation of the experimental material in at least one

respect: the motives that they ascribe to the persons

about whom they read. McCaul also noted that the stronger

the pupils' attitudes, the greater is the tendency for

their attitudes to affect their interpretation.

The literature on types of interests is vast. For

the purposes of the present study, the review has been cen­

tered on the number of interests and the depth of involve­ ment as defined by Gray and Rogers (1956). Range of

interests has often been studied in relationship to varied

aspects of the reader. Waples and Tyler (1931) found that

factors which make for differences in reading interests

could be ordered as sex, amount of schooling, occupation, environment, age, and time spent in reading. Summarizing studies done before 1930, Walby (1956) reported that differ-, ing rates of physical and mental growth, school environment, social or economic status, home training, and past exper­ ience had all been found to affect individual reading preferences. Wittick (1960) proposed that the content of what people read was probably most influenced by the period in which they lived. Huck and Kuhn (1961) noted that

"children's reading interests reflect the pattern of their general interests" (p. 28), with interests changing as the 46

child grows and being reflected in the type of book

selected and the kind of content preferred. Wittick (1960),

Norvel (1958), and Rankin (1944) noted differences or

changes of interests by age and sex. Individual differ­

ences in reading interests are great and tend to crystal­

lize between the ages of 12 and 16, Gray and Monroe (1929)

concluded from their study of the reading interests and

habits of adults. The present study considered interests

in relation to children's purposes for reading. Generally

the sixth-grade students responded that the reasons they

chose to read a specific book or article was that they were

interested in it. In the case studies, children reported

a specific interest that motivated much of their reading.

Studies that seek to identify and classify child­

ren's interests include those by Shores (1954), Shulte

(1969), Ashley (1970), Amatora and Edith (1951), and

Cornell (1941). Shores sought to determine the informa­

tional needs of children and what they read about. Pac­ kets of questionnaires were sent to 270 communities for

children in grades four through eight and to their parents

and teachers. Another 270 questionnaires were sent to

librarians in or near these communities. Communities were selected equally from the nine census regions; from

rural, urban, and metropolitan areas; and from various

socioeconomic groups. Shores noted some sharp sex 47 differences in the data so collected. Boys are more inter­ ested in astronomy, geology, physical geography, space travel, Indians, science, planes and jets, rochets, boats, sports, and baseball. Girls are more interested in animals, horses, dogs, literature, fairy tales and mythology, mys­ tery, teenagers and children, famous people, and school.

Shores also reported that pupil interests are not identi­ cal to their informational needs. Shulte (1969) described the independent reading interests of 6,568 children in grades four, five, and six in 243 classrooms in four states.

Data were gathered by means of questionnaires, a reading interest preference test, and a teacher questionnaire.

Shulte reported a decrease in independent reading interests as grade level increases. Boys are more interested in historical fiction, history, social studies, health, and science. Girls prefer realistic fiction, fanciful tales, biography, recreational interests, and poetry.

Ashley (1970) asked a representative sample of 900

Canadian children in grades four through seven to indicate their likes and dislikes in relationship to 40 reading topics. Mystery and adventure stories were identified as

"peaks of interest" by the subjects, with the least popular topics being grade readers, fables, people of other lands, newspapers, and magazines. Amatora and Edith (1951) reviewed four-year reading records kept by 724 fifth-grade 48 pupils. The children in the study had frequent individual

conferences with their teachers during the period. No

lists of required reading were supplied, but titles were

frequently suggested to arouse interest. After classify­

ing the books read as to specific type, the investigators identified hero worship as the main interest area in the intermediate grades. Cornell (1941) conducted a study of the voluntary reading of high school pupils in the Cleve­ land libraries on the basis of records kept by librarians and analyzed by experts. Cornell reported that the num­ ber of interests increases with an increase in the amount of reading done. The present study was concerned with the way children perceived their interests as influencing their reading habits.

The relationship between interest and reading comprehension of ninth-grade students was examined by

Berstein (1955). Two selections judged to be widely dif­ ferent in inherent interest were rewritten so as to make them exactly equivalent in readability. A five-point interest scale and a comprehension test were provided for each page of the six-page stories. The tests were admini­ stered to 100 ninth-grade pupils in a large metropolitan junior high school. Berstein's results support the hypo­ thesis that high interest is associated with better reading comprehension. Roderick (1968) looked at interest in terms 49

of whether or not a relationship exists between creativity

and literary experiences of sixth-grade children. Subjects

were 100 students in three central Pennsylvania schools

divided into high, middle, and low creative groups on the

basis of their scores on the Minnesota Test of Creative

Thinking. During a three-month period, children read

from a set of 30 titles provided by the examiner and kept

a log indicating whether or not they liked a book and why.

A panel of authorities in children's literature created

the 30 titles provided by the examiner in terms of its

appeal to the most or least creative child. Roderick

reported that children differ in the choices of books

they make and this is related to their level of creativity.

There are many studies that seek to determine what

is of interest to boys and girls at various ages in their

development. Among the early investigations, Jordon's

(1921) study found that boys prefer adventure stories and

that girls like stories of home and school, love, history, mild adventure, and fairy stories. Terraan and Lima's

(1925) survey of the reading interests and amount of read­

ing of boys and girls from ages 11 to 16 was conducted in

connection with the Stanford research on gifted children

in which 1,000 gifted children, ages six to sixteen, with

I.Q. scores over 135, were compared to approximately 1,000 unselected children of the same ages. Intelligence ranges 50

of the unselected group were not noted. Five instruments

were used to collect the data: (1) a questionnaire for

parents on the amount of time the child spent in reading

and the kinds of books and magazines read; (2) a teacher

questionnaire on amount of reading done by children; (3) a

reading record booklet kept by the child reporting biblio­

graphic information and opinions of books; (4) an interest

blank used by children to report topics of most interest;

and (5) a questionnaire for 100 graduate students concern­

ing their childhood interests in reading. Terraan and Lima

noted that from age nine and up, the breach between the

reading interests of boys and girls widens, with boys

choosing stories of adventure and vigorous action, non­

fiction, and animal stories, while girls like fairy tales,

poetry, sentimental fiction, and animals. The amount of

reading increases from age six until about 12 or 13, with

a gradual decrease later; girls also read more books than boys. Lazar (1937) administered a Pupil Background Inven­

tory to 4,300 pupils in seventh through twelfth grades in

13 New York City public schools. The items from the in­

ventory selected for the specific study of the pupils'

reading interests and activities were school subject liked,

choice of reading as compared with other subjects, kinds of books liked, kind of books liked best, possession of

library cards, number of books read during a month, parts 51

of the newspaper liked best, magazines read, inventory of

titles of books enjoyed most, and inventory of books recently read. She found that girls show a greater inter­ est in owning library cards and report reading more books than boys. However, she also noted that boys read maga­ zines more frequently than girls.

Rankin's (1944) findings were used as a basis for later studies. Rankin reviewed the circulation cards of books of contemporary fiction and noted few sex differences in book selection. Rankin also identified five reasons children in grades seven through nine give for selecting books: (1) theme or specific topic; (2) recommendations of other people concerning books; (3) previous knowledge of the author's name or his works; (4) format, pictures, and clear print; and (5) style.

In a later study of reading interests, Norvell

(1959) asked 24,000 children in grades three through eight in New York State to indicate their interest in selected works. In all, 1,576 literary selections were used. The data were similar to those obtained in previous studies: boys prefer prose and girls poetry; boys like adventure and physical struggle, human characters, animals, humor, courage and heroism, and patriotism; girls prefer lively adventure, home and school, human characters, domestic animals and pets, romantic love, sentiment, mystery, super­ natural, and patriotism. Looking specifically at boys' 52 reading interests, Stanchfield (1962) interviewed 153 boys

in grades four, six, and eight in the Los Angeles City

Schools to ascertain their reading preferences. Each inter­ view extended over an hour and covered 50 categories of reading interests. Stanchfield reported that "unusual experiences" is the characteristic most highly preferred, followed by "excitement," "suspense," "liveliness and ac­ tion," and "surprise and unexpectedness."

Cleary (1939) and Link and Hoff (1946) looked at other aspects of the interest question. Cleary's study had three objectives: (1) to ascertain what a large and varied group of seventh- and eighth-grade boys and girls had read during a definite period of time in books, maga­ zines, and newspapers; (2) to discover their preferences and appraisals of books; (3) to determine the effect that various factors such as intelligence, sex, age, racial and nationality differences, social interests, and educational background may have on their reading. The children kept records of their reading and answered a questionnaire on their preferences. Cleary reported that children who attend movies frequently read more, both books and maga­ zines, than the average; she also found that those children whose median radio-listening time exceeded three hours per day read fewer books but more magazines. In a similar manner, the present study reported magazine, newspaper, and comic books reading as well as book reading by children. 53

Link and Hoff (1948) used a survey approach with

interviews to ask questions of 4,000 teenagers and young

adults. In a preliminary field test, Link and Hoff found

that the formal education of the respondent is an important

factor in determining his book-reading habits. The higher the education, the greater the frequency with which books are read. In their final study, Link and Hoff reported that people with hobbies tend to read significantly more than those who have no hobbies.

Some studies have been reported where researchers try to increase and broaden reading interests. Fox (1947)

0 sought to broaden interest and develop children's awareness of quality material. She conducted an experiment to pro­ mote interest in reading with 21 pupils in grades four and five. Tests constructed by the experimenter were given to locate reading level, interest and activity, to evaluate traits, and to diagnose reading habits. The program in­ volved workbook lessons, free reading lessons, and story time where the teacher read aloud to the class. Fox reported that children's interests expand to more fields and types of literature as a result of a reading program.

Miller (1969) compared children's interests and teacher's knov/ledge of children's literature. Four basic teacher competencies for fostering interest in books were distilled from recommendations by authorities in reading. They 54

included: (1) a knowledge of a wide range of children's

books, (2) a knowledge of children's interests. (3) a

knowledge of how to set the stage for the love of reading,

and (4) a sensitivity to what distinguishes a good book

from a mediocre one. The investigation involved nine

fifth-grade and seven sixth-grade teachers and their 414

students in five elementary schools. Instruments were

selected and constructed to measure 17 teacher performances

and practices associated with the four recommended com­ petencies. The students responded to a questionnaire that

included questions on number of books read and special read­

ing interests. After comparing the teacher and pupil forms,

Miller maintained that a gap appeared between the sugges­ tions <5f authorities and actual teacher practices in fos­ tering interest in reading. Bissett (1959) developed a program that increased accessibility to books for children and taught library skills in an effort to broaden children's interest patterns. One hundred and ninety children in seven fifth-grade classrooms were trained in recording all books read outside of class. Classrooms were assigned randomly to three treatment groups:• (1) simulating normal procedures, (2) increasing accessibility to books but not offering specific programs to give children knowledge about books, and (3) devoting 90 minutes of language arts instruction time to teacher and peer recommendations of 55

the books in the classroom libraries. Bissett reported a

significant rise in the number of books read with increased

accessibility and recommendations. The teacher question­

naire developed for this study attempted to distinguish

aspects of the teachers' methods that encouraged reading

in the classroom.

The depth of interest, according to Gray and Rogers

(1955), helps indicate the extent and degree of seriousness

of the reader1s penetration of a given area or topic. In

their study of maturity in adult reading, three factors

were found to be identifiable as indicative of depth of

interest: (1) the reader's knowledge of the field; (2) the

nature of the materials read on the given subject; and

(3) the nature of the reaction to the materials read. In

his study of children's reading behavior, Clark (1959)

examined the family context of boys1 reading and the rela­

tionships among boys' print use. Subjects were 137 Seattle

families of tenth-grade boys. The boys and their parents were interviewed in an attempt to describe the boys' read­

ing patterns as they relate to adult reading. Clark found that boys who are heavy book readers do not exhibit any distinctive pattern of book interest. 56

In reviewing the research in interest-in-reading,

Weintraub (1969), Jan-Tausch (1970), and Purves and Beach

(1972) criticized the research techniques employed in many

of the studies. Weintraub noted that as each researcher

has developed categories of reading interests, the same

topic has sometimes been placed under different categories.

He reported a second problem concerning survey and struc­

tured multiple-choice questionnaires is that the adult in­

vestigator might not be drawn by the same element in a cate­

gory, story, or illustration that attracts a child. Jan-

Tausch concluded that many investigations have been made

to discover the group interests of children according to

intelligence levels, age and grade groupings, sex, socio­ economic, and nationality differences. However, there has been a scarcity of studies designed to determine the basic

interests of the individual child and how they relate to the child's choice of reading material. In his own study,

Jan-Tausch had 100 children in seventh- and eighth-grades express their major theme preferences by selecting illus­ trations. The test instrument devised for this study con­ sisted of 36 illustrations involving 12 categories or themes. He concluded that children's interests vary and that their responses to pictures tend to be pluralistic rather than singular. 57

Purves and Beach (1972) listed another bias of many

studies. They contended that the researchers never quite • * free themselves from the notions of what students' interests

"should be." Thus, certain subject matter, for example sex

and drugs, has been totally ignored. They propose that

interests reported from studies are "group interests" and

not individual interests since most studies look at the

student population as a whole.

Theorists or generalists have also written widely

about children's reading interests. For example, Huus

(1963a) listed children’s interests by their grade level

in school. After discussing the developmental growth in

interests, Frank (1960) maintained that an 11 or 12 year-

old child may find pleasure in certain books that run in a

series. She listed three values of series books: (1) gen­

uine pleasure in meeting familiar characters; (2) the

child's interest in the characters and what happens to the

characters; and (3) the enjoyable experience for the child

of reading on his own. The lists of series books supplied by Frank were modified and used in the interest inventory

of the present study. Betts (1961) listed characteristics

thought to apply to the 11 year old reader. According -_o

Betts, at age 11 most children are reading ten or more books a year on their own. Girls' interests in fiction reaches a high point and boys enjoy a heavy diet of books 58

and articles on science and invention. Boys and girls have

a high interest in tales of adventure and in mystery stories.

Other Aspects of Reading Behavior

Relationships between reading and intelligence,

reading ability, time spent reading, and sex have also been

much studied. These factors were used to compare children

in the present study.

Findings on the relationship of intelligence and

amount of reading vary. Terman and Lima (1931) noted that

their exceptionally bright children read on an average of

three to four times as many boohs as children of average

intelligence. As already reviewed, their findings were

based on a study of 2,000 students of six to 16 years.

Carroll (1934) gave 600 junior high students and 270 senior

high students the Carroll Prose Appreciation test and com­

pared the scores to the students' I.Q. scores. He reported

the correlations indicate a marked degree of relationship between intelligence and the ability to appreciate litera­

ture. Lazar (1937) found the amount of reading done by dull children is measurably less than that done by bright

children in her study of 4,300 high school pupils. She obtained correlations between the number of books read and the intelligence quotient of .40 for boys and .36 for girls. 59

Strang (1942) noted positive correlations between interpretive reading skills and intelligence and suggested that growth in appreciation is affected by mental maturity.

She interviewed 112 adolescents and adults using a pro­ cedure "somewhere between the standardized reading test procedure and the flexible social or psychiatric case- study method" (Strang, 1942, p. 5). However, Wollner (1949) found a correlation of less than .44 between intelligence, reading skills, and interest in reading and thus called into question the adequacy of generalizing a relationship between intelligence and voluntary reading. In analyzing her data, she noted that voluntary reading is related to intelligence, reading ability, sex, factors in home and school environment, factors in adolescent development, other leisure time pursuits, and individual emotional adjustment. Wollner collected data on eighth-grade pupils at the Horace Mann-Lincoln School of Teachers College over a one-year period. The data were collected from three major sources: standardized tests, school records, and special procedures.

Norvell (1958) claimed that superior, average, and slow pupils usually enjoy the same kinds of reading mater­ ials. His report is based on 960,000 expressions of opin­ ion on report forms from 2,400 children in grades three to six in schools in New York State. The report form provided 60

the child's name, grade, age, and sex to which the I.Q.

score was later added. The children listed what they had

read and rated it as "very interesting," "fairly interest­

ing," and "uninteresting." Other studies have shown that

* children of various intellectual levels are at least inter­ ested in the same subjects though often at different

chronological ages.

Clarke (1969) studied the amount and kinds of

reading done by 137 tenth-grade boys. The boys were inter­ viewed and asked to respond to a questionnaire. In analyz­

ing the data, Clarke maintained that the correlations be­ tween skills and print use are too modest for predictive use in the classroom. Among school populations, as Gray

(1960) reported in a review of research, correlations between intelligence and reading achievement tests tend to cluster between .40 and .60. While this substantial, positive relationship would be considerably higher if the full range of intelligence were sampled, the fact that it was not higher suggests that other factors than intelli­ gence per se affect reading achievement, that specific aspects of intelligence may be more highly correlated with reading achievement than others, or both.

Henderson and Long (1970) found a correlation be­ tween reading skills measured by achievement tests and quality of reading, in their.study of the independent 61

reading behavior of 150 achieving fifth-graders. However,

they contended that the relationship between intelligence

and reading achievement is suspect since the two standard­

ized tests may measure the same variable. Their study

directed attention toward the quality and variety of chil­

dren's choices of boohs as well as to the number of boohs

read. These variables were intercorrelated with standardized

achievement, X.Q., and a number of noncognitive and demo­

graphic variables. Indices of quantity, variety, and

quality were derived from (1) records of library usage,

(2) the child's own record of boohs read, and (3) time

records of out-of-school activities.

Factors other than reading achievement test scores

and intelligence that contribute to the development of a mature reader, according to Gray and Rogers (1956), in­

clude amount of time spent reading, number of interests, depth of interests, awareness of purposes for reading, number of purposes leading to reading, and the type of materials read.

According to most studies, girls spend more time reading and are more interested in reading. Lehman and

Witty (1928) reported girls frequently select "reading boohs" as one of their favorite activities and more fre­ quently judge reading to consume more time than any other activity. Five thousand children were ashed to chech from 62

a comprehensive list of 200 activities only those in which

they had voluntarily engaged in during the preceding week.

One of the items included in the list was "reading books."

The pupils were, directed to mark this activity if they had

engaged in it "just for fun" during the preceding week.

In addition to checking the activities in which they had

participated, the children were also asked to designate

the three activities which they liked best and the one

activity which in their judgment had consumed the largest

amount of time. Lehman and Witty concluded the sex dif­

ference is clearly marked. However, Wollner's (1949) find­

ings question these assumptions. In her study, Wollner noted that a difference between eighth-grade boys and girls appeared only in the measure of self-rating, not in actual reading behavior. Wollner found that certain boys do more reading and withdraw more books than girls of the same age, reading ability, and intelligence.

Asheim (1956) contended that time spent reading is a factor which makes for differences in reading interest.

Gray and Rogers (1956) also maintained that time devoted to reading did not bear a consistent relation to other aspects of the reading behavior. Other forms of media compete for a child's reading time. Nila Banton Smith

(1963a), in a review of related research, noted that data revealed children on an average were devoting about one 63

hour per day to voluntary reading and three hours per day

to viewing television.

Home and School Environment

External factors help mold an individual's reading

pattern. There is considerable evidence that parental

attitudes and the nature of the home environment are impor­

tant determinants of a child's reading success. In the present study, the home environment factor was used as a basis for questions on the interest inventory and as a main purpose of the parent interviews. Eddings (1956) examined the cumulative and reading records of sixth-grade children and ashed parents to respond to a questionnaire. Eddings noted that the educational background of the parents tends to be related to the reading growth of pupils rather than occupation statue. Carrello (1957) interviewed good and poor fifth-grade readers and analyzed the responses of parents to a questionnaire on the home literary environ­ ment. He stated that home environment is the genesis of reading ability, especially as it is concerned with paren­ tal attitudes of personal enjoyment in reading, acceptance of the school, and pride in their child's accomplishments.

Jefferson (1958) investigated and analyzed the relationship between the reading preferences of elementary school children and parental estimates of those choices.

He asked the fifth-grade students and their parents to 64

respond to questionnaires about reading interests and

preferences. Jefferson found that parents often estimate

their child's reading preferences accurately.

In his parent interviews, Clarke (1965) attempted

to discover what parental behaviors are effective in

socializing children's print use, in increasing their use,

and in fostering parent-child similarities in print use.

He found evidence that parental socialization values and

the nature of the child-parent relationship are important

$ determiners of children's interest in serious newspaper

content. Hughes and Willis (1965) invited 1000 seventh-

grade children in the Salt Lake City area to nominate

children they thought were "extended readers." This pro­

cedure identified 58 readers who were matched on sex,

intelligence, and reading placement to 58 other children.

The selected "extended readers" and their parents were

interviewed. The study indicated that parents who are

professed "wide-readers" tend to have offsprings who have developed the same trait.

The family position of the learner and its rela­ tionship to reading achievement for 300 good and 300 poor readers was investigated by Otto (1965). At the sixth- grade level, a significant difference was found to favor the oldest child or an only child. However, when all readers were considered, only the oldest child could be 65

used as a reliable guide for the prediction of reading

achievement.

Ketcham (1966) found that the following home back­

ground factors are related to reading achievement for 582

students in grade ten: (1) mother's use of the library,

(2) the number of newspapers and the quality of magazines

in the home, and (3) the nature of the father's job.

Powell's (1966) study sought to discover the amount of

extensive reading done by children from different supply

sources. Pupils in grades four, five, and six in two dif­

ferent elementary schools were interviewed. One school had

classroom collections? the other school was adjacent to a

public library. Neither school had a school library.

Powell reported that the more immediate the access to the

library material, the greater the amount of pupil recrea-

tory reading. Findings of these studies support the con­

tention that outside environmental activities and institu­

tions affect the child's individual reading patterns.

Hansen (1967) gave 48 fourth-grade children an

attitude scale to assess their self-commitment to indepen­ dent reading. From these scores, students were grouped

into high and low groups. Each home was then visited to gather information on the home literary environment. Upon analyzing the data, Hansen concluded that factors in the home environment are better predictors of reading attitude 66

than either social class characteristics or test

I,Q.

Studies of children's reading and amount of time

spent watching television were conducted hy Witty and

Kinsella (1958). -They concluded that children are reading

somewhat more than they did before the advent of television.

However; there are some who read less now; these children

are considered a real problem by their parents and teachers

who often look on television as a threat to reading. In a

later study, Witty (1961) reported that the average amount

of reading had probably increased a little since the advent

of television. However, the small amount of time devoted

to reading, which averages about an hour each day, was in

sharp contrast with the three hours a day daily given to television by younger pupils. Time spent reading and time

spent watching television by sixth-grade children was com­ pared in the present study.

Another environmental influence on children's read­ ing that has been studied is that of school and community libraries. As part of a study on library usage, Gaver

(1963) classified school libraries into classroom collec­ tions only, centralized collections, and school libraries.

The purposes of Gaver's study were twofold: first, to develop instruments which would evaluate the programs of library services available in elementary schools; second, to study the resulting scores and rating in terms of 67

(a) their relationship to measures of educational achieve­

ment and community position and (b) their ability to dif­

ferentiate between schools having degrees of library pro­

vision. The study primarily covered traditional printed

materials such as books and magazines. Two basic groups

who responded to this study were (1) the teaching and ad­

ministrative staffs of six schools representing one of

three different library categories and (2) sixth-grade

pupils of these schools. Gaver reported that the amount of

reading done is related to the nature of the library pro­ vision in the school.

In a discussion of the school reading program and

the library, Cianciolo (1968) maintained that

extensive school library holdings enable the read­ ing teacher to implement an individualized reading program effectively, for, as children may pursue their own interests and needs independently, make their own selections, and read at their own rates, the teacher may incorporate the concepts of self­ selection, seeking behavior, and pacing to the teaching of developmental reading (p. 44).

For these reasons, schools with libraries were selected

for the present study.

Methods of Improving Readincr Behavior The teacher's role in stimulating voluntary reading has been discussed by theorists. Stauffer (1959) gave four processes for developing lifetime habits of reading:

(1) teachers need to have a clear understanding of each 68

pupil's personal and social growth and adjustment; (2) tea­

chers need to be thoroughly familiar with books; (3) tea­

chers need to have a wealth of books available for chil­ dren; and (4) teachers need to organize their programs so as to focus attention on the interests and needs of the children.

Huck (1962) maintained that schools are teaching children the skill of reading but are not helping children develop the habit of reading. "In many instances we have developed an illiterate group of literates— adults who know how to read but do not read" (p. 307) . Arbuthnot (1962) listed four characteristics of a good literature teacher:

(1) the teacher reads stories aloud; (2) the teacher reads poetry aloud; (3) the teacher talks about books to her children as informally as she might talk about baseball; and (4) the teacher is not afraid to share some of her adult enthusiasm with her children. Arbuthnot believed that only a teacher who knows and likes to read and talk about reading will develop children who also know books and will grow through reading. Appreciation for literature needs to be fostered, according to a later article by Huck

(1965), as through a daily story hour, for example.

Iverson (1967) emphasized that the enjoyment of literature does not just grow as the child matures; the teacher has to encourage it and has to teach for it. He 69

distinguished between skill building and the study of lit­

erature in the reading program. In skill building, a child

has to know "what" a word means. In literature study, a

child must be more concerned with ".how" a word means.

In a discussion of the book, Ginger Pyle. Tyler

(1965) suggested that it might be helpful to use psycholo­

gical theory to understand the appeal of children's books.

She believed children's books should be analyzed in terms

of psychological theory.

Conclusive research is lacking in regard to the

types of school experiences which affect voluntary reading.

However, an investigation by Hunnicut (1943) supported the

belief that amounts of reading, variety of choices, and

outcomes in satisfaction are more highly related to an

"activity" program that places emphasis on wide reading

opportunities and freedom of choice than to the traditional

educational program. Hunnicut sought to determine the

differences in reading patterns which might develop when

children were exposed to divergent patterns of teaching in

a six-year study involving two teaching programs in two

schools, one program an activity based program and the other following a basal-skill approach.

Other researchers have tried comparing methods of

instruction in an attempt to stimulate voluntary reading.

Lawson (1964) compared four methods of instruction: the 70

conventional based on the basal reader, the individualized

approach, a combination of conventional and free reading

approach, and a free reading approach with limited basal

use. Lawson found a procedure that included an equal

amount of time spent in conventional instruction and free

'reading is the most likely to be a positive factor in

encouraging children's free reading. Pfou (1967) estab­

lished two groups of 170 first-grade readers and followed

their progress for two years. The experimental group was

exposed to new methods of teaching and many supplementary

books while the control group relied on the traditional basal method of reading instruction. Pfou reported chil­

dren who participated in a planned program of recreational

reading did significantly better on measures of interest in

reading.

Fader (1968) encouraged high school age boys to

read by supplying them with numerous paperbacks. He con­ cluded that the more accessible the books, the more boys will read. Bissett (1969) established three treatment groups using 190 children from seven fifth-grade class­ rooms. Classrooms were randomly assigned to treatment A which simulated normal classroom procedures, treatment B which increased accessibility of books, but in which no program was instituted to give children knowledge about the books or to recommend titles to be read, and treatment C 71

in which the teachers devoted 90 minutes of language arts

instruction time to teacher and peer recommendation of the

books in the classroom library. The third treatment group

read more books than the other groups. Bissett concluded

that an increased accessibility and knowledge of books

increases the number of books children read.

Summary

Theorists and researchers have looked at various

aspects of children's reading behavior. Characteristics

of concern have included purpose, response to literature,

age, sex, interest, appreciation level, and teacher motiva­

tion techniques. An understanding of parts of children's

reading behavior is needed before an attempt can be made

to describe the total reading profile of active child readers.

Chapter III will describe the methods and proced­ ures used in this study. CHAPTER III

METHODS AND PROCEDURES

Chapter III contains a description of the sample population, instruments, and procedures used in this study.

Statistical treatment for the 21 hypotheses is presented.

Sample

Sixth-grade children in two suburban Columbus school systems reported their reading habits for this study. These systems both have school libraries staffed by professional full-time librarians. The library programs in all participating schools have been in operation two years or more. Two hundred seventy-six children in 11 classrooms selected by school officials participated.

These classes were all heterogeneous in pupil population.

Experience of the teachers ranged from three to 20 years, and teaching may be characterized as basically traditional although some teachers were moving toward a more informal approach. . .

Community A is a city of 40,000 people and ranks second highest in income per family in Ohio for cities of more than 25,000 people, with 85 percent of the homes

72 73 owner-occupied. There are seven elementary schools, two junior high schools, and one senior high school in this district that serves a 9.7 square mile area. All schools have libraries and full-time librarians. The community also supports a public library. One hundred ninety-four sixth-grade student in nine classrooms and three schools in this system participated in the study.

Community B is located on the far north side of the

Columbus metropolitan area and is also known as well to do, with more than 85 percent of its homes owner-occupied.

The public school system serves a 20 square mile area with six elementary schools, two junior high schools, and one high school. All schools have staff librarians and school libraries. The residents also support a public library that is considered one of the most complete in northern

Franklin County. Eighty-two children in three classrooms of one school in this system participated in the study.

Mental maturity scores for children in Community A were available and are reported in Table 1. Mental matur­ ity scores were not available for children in Community B.

The reading section of the Iowa Test of Basic

Skills was administered in fifth grade to children in

Community A. Scores are reported in Table 2. Community B administered the California Achievement Tests to children in the fourth grade. Reading scores are reported in Table 3. 74

Table 1

Means and Standard Deviations of Quotient Scores by Classroom on the California Test of Mental Maturity

(Approximately 182 Students)

Classroom Mean Standard Deviation

1 117.652 11.904

2 121.040 12.351

3 123.826 12.525

4 113.391 11.252

5 116.250 9.646

6 115.318 7.631

7 113.208 11.348

8 115.818 9.738

Note: The California Test of Mental Maturity was administered in Grade 4. 75

Table 2

Means and Standard Deviations of Reading Scores by Classroom on the Iowa Test of Basic Skills

(Approximately 182 Students)

Classroom Mean Standard Deviation

1 6.83 1.182

2 6.99 1.212

3 6.99 1.141

4 6.31 1.265

5 6.36 .762

6 6.26 .653

7 6.74 1.093

8 6.92 1.330

Note: The Iowa Test of Basic Skills was administered in Grade 5. 76

Table 3

Means and Standard Deviations of Reading Scores by Classroom on the California Achievement Tests

(Approximately 80 Students)

Classroom Mean Standard Deviation

9 5.73 1.092

10 5.84 2.111

11 5.71 1.855

Note: The California Achievement Tests were administered in Grade 4. 77

Development of the Instruments

The instruments used in this study were developed

by the experimenter after a review of the related research.

Two professors in early and middle childhood education

advised on the development. As part, of the process, a two-

week pilot study was run in one fifth-grade class during

which children offered suggestions that were used to clar­

ify the wording of directions and simplify the format of

the instruments.

These are the seven instruments used in the study:

Librarian-Teacher Nomination Form. On this form

(see Appendix A ) , teachers and school librarians were asked

to identify the most active readers among the sixth-grade

children in their schools; they were asked to supply at

least five names.

Student Nomination Form. On this form (see Appen­

dix B), students were asked to nominate classmates whom

they believed read a great deal. An operational defini­

tion of active readers was supplied. Children were invited

to nominate themselves if they believed they met the descrip­ tion of an active reader.

Interest Inventory. Children filled out a form entitled "My Reading Habits and Interests" (see Appendix

C). Seven questions required a closed response and were selected to gain quantifiable descriptors of the sixth- 78

grade readers. Two open-response questions concerning

series kooks and favorite books were also included.

Reading- Record. The reading record was developed

to collect information that would identify children's rea­

sons for reading (see Appendix D). The form was based on

the definition of primary purpose by Helen Smith: "the

reading 'set' used to determine that which the reader in­

tended to get from the selection as perceived by the chil­

dren" (H. Smith, 1966, p. 10). Subjects were instructed

to record what they had read under the purpose that they

felt best described why they read the material. The chil­

dren also were asked to classify the material in terms of

type of publication.

Teacher Estimation Form. Teachers rated each child

in their class as above average or average in his general performance as a student (see Appendix E) . General per­

formance was defined as the child's general attitudes,

abilities, and actions as demonstrated by his ability to

adjust and to succeed at assigned and independent tasks.

While the teacher's perception of a child's general ability may sometimes be biased, it is the measure relied upon in most grading situations in the elementary school.

Standardized Test Scores. The researcher was allowed access to the mental age and reading achievement test scores of the children in the study that were on file 79

in the school systems. Community A had on record the

scores from the California Test of Mental Maturity, which

had been given in the fourth grade, and the Iowa Test of

Basic Skills. which had been administered in the fifth

grade. Community B did not have a mental age measure but

allowed the researcher to record the scores on the Cali­

fornia Achievement Test, which had been administered in the

fourth grade. Parental approval was needed to release the

scores and a form was developed for that purpose (see Appen

dix F).

Teacher Interview Form. The researcher asked the

teachers of the six children interviewed in the case stud­

ies questions concerning the teaching methods used in their

classrooms (see Appendix H). Questions were included that

dealt with the number and variety of books and printed matter available in the classroom. The teachers also

reported books read to the class. The interviews were held during the school day.

Home Interview Form. The researcher interviewed the parents of the six children selected for case studies

(see Appendix G) to gain more information about the reading history and home environment of the children. Among the questions asked during the interview were those concerned with the amount of reading materials in the home as well as those designed to discover the parents1 attitude toward children's reading in general (see Appendix H). 80

Student Interview Form. Six children, selected as

active readers, were interviewed using a case study

approach. The questions (see Appendix H) were designed to

gain more information about the reading history and social

background of the readers. Many of the questions were

open-ended to allow the children to respond freely. The

children were interviewed individually.

Procedure

The researcher first met with the teachers, librar­

ians, and principals of the selected schools to present a

summary of the study and a copy of the instruments. A discussion of the intent and procedures of the study

followed.

Each of the 11 classrooms was visited by the re­ searcher six times during a three-week period. After analyzing the results of the pilot study, the researcher had developed a lesson plan (see Appendix I) to introduce the study to the classes. During the first visit, various aspects of the study were discussed with the students.

Although the specific hypotheses were not mentioned, the students were told that the study was an attempt to describe average sixth-grade readers. Care was taken to enlist cooperation since it was believed that a favorable attitude would be reflected in the quality and quantity of the response to the instruments. 81

At this first meeting, the researcher also pre­

sented three forms: the student nomination form, the

reading record, and the reading inventory. Children and

teachers filled out their respective nomination forms at

the same time. The reading record was discussed in some

detail since this form was to he used at each subsequent

visit. Finally, the children answered questions on the

reading inventory. Each initial visit lasted 30 minutes.

The researcher tabulated the number of nominations

received by each child. A maximum of four nominations was

possible: those of the teacher, the librarian, other stu­

dents, and the student himself. Children receiving nomina­

tions from three sources were identified as active readers

for purposes of this study. From the total sample of 276

children, sixty-three were identified as active readers

through this method (see Table 4).

Table 4

Number of Active and Other Readers

Active Other

Boys 22 118

Girls 41 95

Total 63 213 82

The children responded to the reading record on

Mondays and Fridays over a three-week period in late aut­

umn. On Mondays, the children reported reading they had

done over the weekend. On Fridays, they reported their

weekday reading, which included class assignments. Al­

though active readers were identified early in the data

collection process, they received the same directions and

forms as the other readers in their classes. The teacher

estimation form was presented during the last classroom

visit. The researcher distributed and collected all forms

during the series of meetings with each class.

The parent, student, and teacher interviews were

scheduled for the two weeks following the last presentation

of the reading record. After the first week of the study,

a list of children nominated as active readers who had

reported reading for the maximum number of purposes was prepared by the researcher. From this list, six children were selected for the case study interviews by the teachers,

librarians, and researcher. Factors that influenced the decision were teachers* opinions of the children's ability

and possible parent cooperation. Parents were interviewed

(see Appendix H) in their homes. Except in one case where both parents were present, the mothers of the students were those questioned. The teachers and students were interviewed at school. The principals also responded to 83 general questions about the six children. The case studies are reported in Chapter IV, Part II.

Design of the Study

The intent of the study was to look at possible descriptors of active readers. The data collected from the reading record were analyzed to test for significance be­ tween the responses of various groups of readers. Children were grouped by sex, mental ability, classroom, and nomina­ tion to the active reader group. The hypotheses presented in Chapter I were rewritten in the null form for statisti­ cal analysis purposes. The null hypotheses follow:

1. A purpose of this study is to describe the relationship of factors in the reading behavior of the entire group of sixth-grade readers.

H^: Boys will not read for a greater range of purposes than will girls.

E^: Girls will not read more books than boys.

H^: The children will not read for more purposes during the week than on weekends.

H^: The children will not read more books on weekdays than on weekends.

H(-: The children will not read more magazines, newspapers, and comic books on weekends than on weekdays.

Hgj The boys will not read more magazines, news­ papers, and comic books than girls. 84

2. A purpose of this study is to describe the

differences of active and other sixth-grade readers.

H_: There will be no significant difference in the number of girls in the active group compared to the other group.

Hft: Children selected as active readers will not report rereading more books than other read­ ers.

Hg: Active readers will not read more books than other readers.

H1n: Active readers will not read more magazines, newspapers} and comic books than other readers.

H. ..: The mean mental age scores of the active read- ers will not be higher than the mean of the other readers.

H19: Children selected as active readers will not have a higher mean reading achievement score than other readers.

H,-: Teachers will not classify a significant num­ ber of active readers as above average in their general performance as students as compared to other readers.

H, .: Children selected as active readers will not read for a greater range of purposes than will children in the other group.

3. A purpose of this study is to describe the re­ lationship of various factors in the reading behavior of active readers.

H.-: Active boy readers will not read for a wider range of purposes than active girl readers.

H.fi: Active girl readers will not read a greater number of books than will active boy readers. 85

H,„: Active boy readers will not read a greater number of comic books, magazines, and news­ papers than will active girl readers.

ELr: Active girl readers will not reread more books than active boy readers.

4. A purpose of this study is to describe the relationship of reading behavior and classroom groupings of sixth-grade readers.

Hiq: Children in one classroom will not read for a wider range of purposes than will children in other classrooms.

H__: Children in one classroom will not read more books than children in the other classrooms.

H?1: Children in one classroom will not read more magazines, newspapers, or comic books than children in another classroom.

5. A purpose of this study is to describe the school, home and personal environment of six active readers.

Cast study reports are found in Chapter IV, Part II.

Treatment of the Data

Four statistical tests were used to analyze the data. On hypotheses concerning a comparison between boys and girls or between active and other readers (H^, Hg, Hg,

Hg, Hg, H11, H12, H14), a two-sample t-test was used.

To analyze questions concerning types and amounts of read­ ing reported on weekdays and on weekends (Hg, H^, H,-), the paired jt-test was used. To analyze questions concerning relationships between classrooms (H-^g, Hgg, **21^ * 86

analysis of variance was run. Finally, on the hypotheses

concerning teachers' classification of students (H^g) and number of girls in the active reading group (H^), the

chi-square test was run.

The responses to questions on the interest inven­ tory were compiled and are reported in Chapter IV, Part III.

Children were grouped by sex and by nomination to the active or other group in this section.

The responses of parents, teachers, and children are reported in the six case studies that appear in

Chapter IV, Part II.

Summary

In the present chapter, the procedures and design of this study were outlined in detail. Seven instruments were developed by the researcher to identify active and other sixth-grade readers and to elicit responses that would help to describe reading behavior and habits of children.

Two hundred seventy-eight children in 11 class­ rooms participated in this study. Teachers, librarians, and students nominated children they considered active readers. Active and other readers reported the type and reasons for their reading over a three-week period. Six children, selected from the active reader group by 87

teachers, librarians, and the researcher, were inter­

viewed in a case study approach.

The following chapter will present the data gathered and discuss appropriate analysis of such data. CHAPTER IV

ANALYSIS OF THE DATA

PART Is GENERAL FINDINGS

Two hundred seventy-six children used the reading

record form (see Appendix D) six times over a three-week

period to report on their weekend and weekday reading

activities. The children noted the materials they read

and selected the purposes that best described their read­

ing.

Before turning to a discussion of the hypotheses,

it may be of interest to note the amount and variety of

reading reported by the group as a whole. Children read

an average of 13-53 books over the three-week period. This

number included information and pleasure books. The week­

day count of books read was 9.63 per child which was more

than two times the number of books reported read by the

children on weekends. The weekend book reading average per child was 4.48 books. Children read an average of

5.09 magazines over the three-week period. The number of magazines reported read on weekdays was 2.67 magazines per

child compared to a weekend average of 2.49 magazines per

88 89

child. The number of newspapers read on weekdays was 2.67

per child and did not differ much from the average of 2.49

newspapers read on weekends. The total average number of

newspapers read was 5.09 per child. Finally, comic book

reading averages were much less than the other averages

with 1.12 comic books reported read per child. Weekday

comic book reading of .58 comic books per child was similar

to the weekend averages of .61 comic books per child. A

summary of the materials read by the children is reported

on Table 5.

Table 5

Summary of Materials Read on Weekdays and Weekends

Mean Standard Deviation

Books Read Total 13.53 6.71 Weekdays 9.66 7.48 Weekends 4.48 3.42

Macrazines Read Total 3.41 2.76 Weekdays 2.05 1.80 Weekends 1.36 1.50

Newspapers Read Total 5.09 2.74 Weekdays 2.67 2.05 Weekends 2.49 1.50

Comic Books Read Total 1.12 2.63 Weekdays .58 1.66 Weekends .61 1.84 Note: 276 children reporting. 90

The descriptors of active and other readers that

are drawn from the data are reported in terms of the five purposes of the study set forth in Chapter III. In all

cases, the .05 level of significance is considered grounds

for acceptance of the 21 hypotheses to be examined.

Sixth-Grade Readers as a Group

One purpose of this study was to describe the rela­ tionship of factors in the reading behavior of the entire group of sixth-grade readers. In this section, hypotheses

1-6 are restated and examined. Hypothesis 1 stated in the null form follows:

"Boys will not read for a greater range of purposes than will girls." The number of purposes reported by boys and girls were compared, using the two sample jb-test. The average number of purposes reported by boys was 2.78 and the average for girls was 2.92 (see Table 5). The t-value of -1.86 approached significance at the .05 level but did not reach it. Therefore, the null form of Hypothesis 1 was not rejected. Boys and girls read for about the same range of purposes.

The second null hypothesis was "Girls will not read more books than boys." Girls read an average of 15.34 books compared to an average of 11.78 books reported by the boys (Table 6) . The number of books read by boys and girls was tested for significance using the two-sample jt-test. 91

The t-value of -4.56 was strongly significant at the .05

and .01 levels. Therefore, null Hypothesis 2 was rejected.

Girls did read more boohs than boys.

Table 6

Boys and Girls1 Responses to Number of Purposes and Number of Materials Read

Bovs Girls S tandard S tandard 2-Sam- Mean Deviation Mean Deviation pie t-value

Purposes 2.78 .82 2.92 .87 -1.86 Books 11.78 6.36 15.34 6.61 -4.56* Magazines 3.56 2.94 3.26 2.56 .88 Newspapers 4.78 2.71 5.41 2.74 -1.95 Comic Books 1.64 3.29 .59 1.54 3.43* *Significant at .05 and .01 levels •

For Hypotheses 3, 4, and 5, the paired t-test was

used to compare the children's weekend and weekday reading.

The null form of Hypothesis 3 was "The children will not

read for more purposes during the week than on weekends."

Children read for an average of 2.56 purposes on weekends

compared to an average of 3.06 purposes on weekdays (see

Table 6). The paired t-value of -10.91 was significant at

the .05 and the .01 levels. This allowed for the rejection

of Hypothesis 3 in its null form. The children read for more purposes during the week than on weekends. The re­

sults of Hypothesis 4 were similar. Hypothesis 4 stated

that "The children will not read more books on weekdays 92 than on weekends." The average number of books reported read by the 276 children on weekdays was 9.66 books com­ pared to an average of 4.48 books per child on weekends

(Table 7). The paired _t-value of -12.89 was significant at the .05 and .01 levels. Therefore the null was rejected.

The children did read more books on weekdays than on week­ ends .

Table 7

Children's Responses to Weekend and Weekday Reading

Standard Paired Mean Deviation t-Value

Purposes Weekend 2.56 .97 -10.91* Weekday 3.06 .92

Books Read

Weekend 4.48 3.43 -12.89* Weekday 9.66 7.48

Magazines Read Weekend 1.80 2.05 6.34 Weekday 1.36 1.50

Newspapers Read Weekend 2.49 1.50 -1.50 Weekday 2.68 2.10

Comic Books Read Weekend .61 1.84 .26 Weekday .58 1.67 *Significant at .05 and .01 levels. 93

I The third comparison of materials read on weekdays

and weekends was stated in Hypothesis 5. The null form

of Hypothesis 5 was "The children will not read more maga­

zines, newspapers, and comic books on weekends than on

weekdays." To analyze the data on this multiple hypo­

thesis, the number of magazines, newspapers, and comic

books read were compared separately. Three sub-hypotheses

were generated. They follow:

Hr : The children will not read more magazines a on weekends than on weekdays.

H_, : The children will not read more newspapers on weekends than on weekdays.

Hr : The children will not read more comic books c on weekends than on weekdays.

The children reported reading an average of 2.05 magazines

on weekends and an average of 1.36 magazines on weekdays

(Table 7). The paired t-value of 6.34 was significant at the .05 and .01 levels. Therefore, null Hypothesis 5a was rejected. The children read more magazines on weekends than on weekdays. The number of newspapers read on weekends and weekdays did not differ significantly. The children reported reading an average of 2.49 newspapers on weekends and an average of 2.68 newspapers on weekdays. The paired t-value of -1.50 was not significant and the null was retained for Hypothesis 5b. A similar finding was reported for Hypothesis 5c. Children reported reading an average of .61 comic books on weekends compared to an average of 94

.58 comic books on weekdays. The paired t-value of .26 was almost significant at the .05 level but did not reach significance. Therefore, the null form of Hypothesis 5c could not be rejected.

Hypothesis 6 was the final hypothesis that concerned the entire group of sixth-grade readers. The null form of

Hypothesis 6 was "The boys will not read more magazines, newspapers, and comic books than girls." Again, as in

Hypothesis 5, the three parts of this hypothesis were treated separately. The null forms follow:

Hga : Boys will not read more magazines than girls.

H^: Boys will not read more newspapers than girls.

Hgc : Boys will not read more comic books than girls.

Boys reported reading an average of 3.56 magazines compared to an average of 3.26 magazines for girls (see Table 6).

The two-sample t-value of .88 was not significant at the

.05 level so the null form of H^c was rejected: Boys read more comic books than girls.

In summary, treatment of the data in terms of Hypo­ theses 1-6 leads to the identification of several significant characteristics of the reading behavior of this sample of sixth-grade children.

These children read for significantly more purposes during the week than on weekends. They also reported read­ ing significantly more books on weekdays and more magazines 95

on weekends. No difference was revealed in the number of

newspapers and comic books read on weekends and weekdays.

Boys and girls read for the same number of purposes.

However, girls reported reading a significantly greater number of books than boys. Boys read more comic books than girls. There was no difference in the amount of maga­

zine and newspaper reading done by the boys and girls.

Active Readers Compared with Other Readers

Another and second purpose of this study was to describe the differences between active and other readers.

Active readers were nominated by their teachers, librarians, classmates, and themselves. Children receiving three or more nominations were considered active readers for pur­ poses of this study. There were 63 children in the active reader group and 213 in the other group (Table 4). Hypo­ theses 7-14 are concerned with the differences between these groups.

The null form of Hypothesis 7 follows: "There will be no significant difference in the number of girls in the active group compared to the other group." Forty of the

136 girls participating in the study were nominated as active readers. A chi-square test of independence was run to determine if there was a significant difference in the number of girls nominated to the active group compared to the number of girls in the other group. A corrected 96

chi-square of 6.67 was calculated with one degree of free­

dom (see Table 8). It was significant at the .01 and .05

levels, therefore, the null form was rejected: There was

a significant difference in the number of girls in the

active group compared to the number of girls in the other

group.

Table 8

Comparison of Number of Girls and Boys in Active and Other Groups

Girls Bovs Totals

Active 40 23 63

Others 96 117 213

Totals 136 141

Hypotheses 8-10 dealt with the materials read by

active and other readers. The two-sample jt-test was used

in analysis of the data relevant to these hypotheses.

The null form of Hypothesis 8 follows: "Children selected as active readers will not report rereading more books than other readers." An average of 4.27 books was reported reread by active readers compared to an average of 2.08 books by other readers. The active readers' number was double that reported by the other readers. The t-value of 2.84 was strongly significant at the .05 level and 97

approached significance at the .01 level. Thus, the null was rejected: Children selected as active readers report

rereading more books than other readers.

Hypothesis 9 dealt with the number of books read by active and other readers. This number included informa­ tion and pleasure books. The null form of Hypothesis 9

follows: "Active readers will not read more books than other readers." An average of 17.11 books were reported read by active readers; the average for other readers was

12.50, or about five books less per pupil (Table 9).

Active readers read more books than other readers.

Table 9

Active and Other Readers' Responses to Number of Purposes and Materials Read

Active Others 2-Sam­ Standard Standard ple Deviation Mean Deviation t-ValueMean

Purposes 3.12 .84 2.74 .83 3.15** Books 17.11 6.80 12.50 6.32 4.78** Magazines 3.90 2.81 3.27 2.74 1.57 Newspapers 5.76 2.83 4.90 2.67 2.12* Comic Books 1.18 2.10 1.11 2.77 .21 Books Reread 4.27 5.80 2.08 3.35 2.84** *Significant at .05 level. **Significant at .05 and .01 levels. 98

There were three sections to Hypothesis 10. The

null forms of the three sections follow:

H10a: Active readers will not read more magazines a than other readers.

H._. : Active readers will not read more newspapers than other readers.

H10c: Active readers will not read more comic boohs than other readers.

Active readers reported reading an average of 3.90 maga­ zines compared to an average of 3.27 magazines for other readers (Table 9) . The jt-value was not significant and the null for H^0a Was . Active readers reported read­ ing an average of 5.76 newspapers compared to 4.90 news­ papers for other readers. The t-value of 2.12 was signifi­ cant at the .05 level but not at the .01 level. The .05 level has been assumed acceptable in this study; therefore, the null form of Hypothesis 10b was rejected. The average active reader reported reading 5.76 comic books compared to an average of 4.90 for other readers - The t-value was not significant and the null of hypothesis 10c was retained..

In short, active and other readers differed significantly in their reading of newspapers but not in reading of maga­ zines and comic books.

Hypotheses 11 and 12 both dealt with a comparison of the two groups of readers on standardized test scores.

A two-sample t-test was used to analyze the data for both hypotheses. The null form of Hypothesis 11 follows: "The mean mental age scores of the active readers will not be

higher than the mean of the other readers." Mental matur­

ity scores were available only for children from Community

A. Forty-four active readers and 138 other readers were

found in this sample. The mean mental age of the active

readers was 123.82 (see Table 10). The,mean of the other

readers was 114.98. The jt-value of 5.41 was significant

at the .05 and .01 levels. The null was rejected: The

mean mental age scores of active readers was found to be

higher than the mean for other readers.

Table 10

Comparison of Mean Mental Age Scores of Active and Other Children in Nine Classrooms

Mean Standard Deviation t-Value

Active 123.82 8.79 5.41* Others 114.98 11.20 *Significant at .05 and .01 levels. Note: Approximately 182 children reporting.

Hypothesis 12 was tested twice since the reading

tests available differed in Communities A and-B. Community

A had on record the scores from the Iowa Test of Basic

Skills, which had been administered in the fifth-grade.

Community B allowed the researcher to use the scores on the

California Achievement Test, which had been administered 100 in the fourth-grade. The t-test was run separately for children in the two communities. There were 138 children in Community A and 94 in Community B. Since some pupils' scores were not available, the sample size for this test differed from the total sample size.

The null of Hypothesis 12 follows: "Children selected as active readers will not have a higher mean reading achievement score than other readers." For Comm­ unity A, the active readers' mean reading achievement score was 7.48 while the other readers' mean score was 6.44 (see

Table 11). The t-value of 6.34 was significant at both the

.05 and .01 levels. For Community B, the active readers' mean reading score was 6.61 compared to a mean score of

5.50 for other readers. The t-value of 2.27 was signifi­ cant at the .05 level only. However, the .05 level has been considered acceptable for purposes of this study, so the null was rejected: Children selected as active readers have a higher mean reading achievement score than other readers.

In another approach to searching out possible differences among active and other readers, teachers were asked to classify their students as average or above aver­ age performers in classroom activities. These ratings were then compared in terms of membership in the active or other group of readers, with the chi-square test of 101

Table 11

Comparison of Mean Reading Scores of Active and Other Readers

Community A: The Iowa Basic Skills Reading Test

Mean Standard Deviation t-Value

Active 7.48 .90 6.34** Others 6.44 1.08 **S ignificant at . 05 and .01 levels. Note: Test was administered in grade five. Scores were recorded for approximately 182 children.

Community B: California Achievement Test

Mean Standard Deviation t-Value

Active 6.61 1.87 2.27* Others 5.50 1.59 ^Significant at .05 level. Note: Test was administered in grade four. Scores were recorded for approximately 27 children. 102

independence administered on the data. This design was

used to test the null form of Hypothesis 13: "Teachers

will not classify a significant number of active readers

as above average in their general performance as students

as compared to other readers." The teachers classified

15 active readers as average students and 48 as above

average students (see Table 12). Out of the total of 213

other readers, teachers classified 142 as average readers

and 72 as above average readers. The corrected chi-square

of 33.144 with one degree of freedom was significant at

the .05 and .01 levels. The null form of Hypothesis 13 was

rejected: Teachers classify a significant number of active

readers as above average in their general performance as

students as compared to other readers.

Table 12

Teachers' Estimation of Active and Other Readers' General Ability

Above Aver acre Aver acre Totals

Active 15 48 63 Others 142 71 213 Totals 157 119 276 Corrected Chi-Square: 33.14* One degree of freedom. *Significant at .01 and .05 levels. 103

The final hypothesis comparing active and other

readers dealt with the difference in the range of purposes

reported by members of the two groups. The null form of

Hypothesis 14 stated that "Children selected as active

readers will not read for a greater range of purposes than

will children in the other group." The two-sample t-test

was used to test the data. Active readers reported reading

for an average of 3.12 purposes while other readers reported

a mean of 2.74 purposes (see Table 9). The jt-value of

3.15 was significant at the .05'and .01 levels so the null

form was rejected. Hypothesis 14 was accepted: Children

selected as active readers read for a greater range of

purposes than other readers.

In summary, several differences between active and

other readers were noted. More girls were nominated as

active readers than boys. Children selected as active readers read for more purposes than did other readers.

Active readers also read more books, reread more books, and read more newspapers than other readers. There was no difference between the two groups in the amount of maga­ zine and comic books read. The mental age scores and reading achievement scores of the active readers were significantly higher than for the other readers. Finally, teachers classified active readers as above average stu­ dents in comparison to the performance of other students in the classroom. 104

Sex Differences in Reading Behavior of Active Readers

The third purpose of this study was to describe the relationship of sex differences in the reading behavior of active readers. Of the 63 active readers, 41 were girls and 22 were boys. Hypotheses 15-18, which dealt with this topic, are discussed in this section. The two-sample t- test was used to test the hypotheses.

Hypothesis 15 dealt with the range of purposes reported by active boy and girl readers. The null form states that "Active boy readers will not read for a wider range of purposes than active girl readers." The mean num­ ber of purposes reported read by active boys was 3.08 com­ pared to a mean of 3.14 purposes reported by the girls (see

Table 13) . The _t-value was not significant at the .05 or .01 levels and the null was retained for Hypothesis 15.

Hypothesis 16 concerned the amount of book reading reported: "Active boy readers will not read a greater num­ ber of books than will active girl readers." Active boy readers read an average of 17.14 information and pleasure books over the recording period compared to an average of

17.10 books for girls (Table 13) . The jt-value was not significant at the .05 level and the null form of hypo­ thesis 16 was retained. 105

Table 13

Girl and Boy Active Readers1 Responses to Number of Purposes and Materials Read

Girls Bovs Standard Standard 2-Sample Deviation Mean Deviation jt-ValueMean

Purposes 3.14 .82 3.08 .90 -0.25 Books 17.10 6.53 17.14 7.43 0.02 Magazines 3.90 2.46 3.91 3.42 0.01 Newspapers 5.75 2.74 5.77 3.04 0.03 Comic Books .70 .97 2.05 3.12 0.03 Books Reread 4.08 5.95 4.64 5.65 .37

The null of Hypothesis 17 had three parts to be

tested:

H17a: Active boy readers will not read a greater number of comic books than active girl readers.

H17b: Active boy readers will not read a greater number of magazines than active girl readers.

H,_ : Active boy readers will not read a greater ° number of newspapers than active girl readers.

Active boy readers reported reading an average of 2.05

comic books compared to the girls' average of .70 comic books (Table 13). The _t-value of 1.97 was very close to

significance at the .05 level but the null of must be

retained. Boys read an average of 3.91 magazines while the girls' average was 3.90 magazines. The t-value was not significant at the .05 level, and the null of was retained. Finally, the active boys and active girls 106

newspaper reading was very similar. Active boys read an

average of 5.77 newspapers compared to the girls' average

of 5.75 newspapers. The t-value was not significant at

the .05 level. The null of Hypothesis 16c was retained.

In short, no significant differences were found between

active boy and girl readers in their reading of comic

boohs, magazines, and newspapers.

The last hypothesis that dealt with sex differences

in the active reader group was Hypothesis 17. The null

form states that: "Active girl readers will not reread

more books than active boy readers." Active girl readers

reported rereading an average of 4.08 books; the average

for the boys was 4.64 books (Table 13). The t-value was

not significant at the .05 level, and, therefore, the null

was retained.

In summary, the boys and girls in the group selected

as active readers reported similar types and amounts of

reading. There was no difference in their perceived pur­

poses for reading or in the amount of material they read.

Also boys and girls in the active group reread a similar

number of books. 107

Differences Among Eleven Classrooms

A fourth purpose of this study was to examine read­

ing behavior in relationship to the classroom groupings of the sixth-grade readers. Children in 11 classrooms par­ ticipated in the study. Of the 213 children, sixty-three were identified as active readers by the nomination process.

There was an average of 5.72 active readers per classroom.

The percent of active readers in each classroom is reported on Table 14.

Table 14

Percent of Active Readers per Classroom

Active Total Classroom Readers Readers Percent

1 4 23 17.39 2 5 25 20.00 3 3 24 12.50 4 4 25 16.00 5 6 22 27.27 6 9 23 39.13 7 7 24 29.16 8 7 25 28.00 9 5 28 17.85 10 6 28 21.42 11 7 29 24.13

Three hypotheses, 19-21, were generated concerning classroom groupings. The analysis of variance statistic was used to test for significance between the 11 groups.

Equal variances were assumed at the beginning to run the test. The null form of Hypothesis 19 stated that "Children 108

in one classroom will not read for a wider range of pur­ poses than will children in other classrooms.1' Although many of the mean scores of number of purposes reported read were similar among the 11 classrooms (see Table 15), notably higher scores were reported in classrooms 2 and 10 where the means were 3.16 and 3.18. The lowest mean was

2.02 purposes, reported in classroom 4. The anova_F-ratio was 4.46 which was significant at the .05 and .01 levels

(see Table 16). The null form of Hypothesis 19 was re­ jected: Children in one classroom read for a wider range of purposes than children in another classroom.

Hypotheses 20 and 21 will be discussed together.

The null forms follow:

H2n: Children in one classroom will not read more books than children in the other class­ rooms .

H21: Children in one classroom will not read more magazines, newspapers, or comic books than children in another classroom.

The nature of the data collected for Hypotheses 20 and 21 did not meet the requirements of the anova statistic. The variances in the scores reported by the children in the classrooms were too great to be assumed similar. The

Bartlett-Box F tests for homogeneity of variance on the data indicated a variance range too great for proper use of the anova statistic. Therefore, tests were not run on the two final hypotheses. 109

Table 15

Average Number of Purposes, Books, Newspapers, Magazines, and Comic Books Reported Read by the Children in the Eleven Classrooms

Classroom Purposes Books Maas. News. Comic Books

1 2.68 13.13 2.43 5.48 0.48

2 3.16 15.48 2.84 5.60 0.12

3 3.05 14.71 3.38 6.83 2.79

4 2.02 9.10 2.88 4.12 0.68

5 2.99 18.60 2.23 5.73 1.73

6 2.75 11.13 3.35 4.96 1.04

7 2.82 13.38 4.13 3.83 0.58

8 2.91 17.16 4.04 5.12 1.04

9 2.97 13.71 3.68 4.46 1.18

10 3.18 13.29 5.21 6.39 1.46

11 2.53 10.17 3.00 3.76 1.24

Averages 2.82 13.53 3.41 5.09 1.12 110

Table 16

Analysis of Variance on Purposes £or Reading Among Classrooms

Source______Sum of Squares_____ df Mean Squares_____ F

Between groups 28.32 10 2.83 4.456* Within groups 168.44 266 0.64

Total______196.76______276______*Significant at .05 and .01 levels.

Hypothesis 20 dealt with the number of boohs read by children in each classroom. The range of books reported read during the three-week period was from an average of

9.10 books (Classroom 4) to an average of 18.60 books

(Classroom 5) (see Table 15). The mean number was 13.52.

The totals included pleasure and information books and both weekday and weekend reading.

Hypothesis 21 concerned other reading materials.

These materials included magazines, newspapers, and comic books. Children in Classroom 10 reported the highest average of 5.21 magazines. The lowest average was reported in Classroom 5, where the number was 2.23 magazines. The average number of magazines read per classroom was 3.41.

The average newspaper number per classroom was 5.09 news­ papers, with a range from 4.12 (Classroom 4) to 6.39

(Classroom 10). The range of comic book reading involved smaller numbers. Comic book reading ranged from -12 Ill

(Classroom 2) to 2.79 (Classroom 3). The average number of comic books read by the children was 1.12.

In summary, there was a significant difference in the number of purposes reported read by the children in the separate classrooms. The analysis of variance test showed a significant F at the .05 level. The greatest difference in the reported purposes for reading occurs between classrooms 3 and 5. The amounts of materials read by children in the various classrooms could not be tested using the analysis of variance statistic. This statistic assumes equal variances among groups and the variances re­ ported were too great. It was, therefore, not appropriate to run the test under these conditions.

Summary

For this study, two hundred seventy-six children reported their reading over a three-week period. The children noted the materials they read and selected the purposes that best described their reading. Comparisons were made between boys and girls and between active and other readers. The 21 hypotheses were discussed in terms of four purposes of the study.

One purpose of the study was to describe the rela­ tionship of factors in the reading behavior of the entire group of sixth-grade readers. The data included some significant differences in the reading behavior of the 112

children. These children read for significantly more pur­ poses during the week than on weekends. They also reported reading significantly more books on weekdays and more maga­ zines on weekends. No difference was revealed in the num­ ber of newspapers and comic books read on weekends and weekdays.

Boys and girls read for the same number of purposes.

However, girls reported reading a significantly greater num­ ber of books than boys. Boys read more comic books than girls. There was no difference in the amount of magazine and newspaper reading done by the boys and girls.

A second purpose of this study was to describe the differences between active and other readers. Several differences were noted. More girls were nominated as active readers than boys. Children selected as active readers read for more purposes than did other readers.

Active readers also read more books, reread more books, and read more newspapers than other readers. There was no difference between the two groups in the amount of magazine and comic books read. The mental age scores and reading achievement scores of the active readers were significantly higher than for the other readers. Finally, teachers classified active readers as above average students in comparison to the performance of other students in the classroom. 113

Another purpose of the study was to describe the

relationship of sex differences in the reading behavior

of active readers. The boys and girls in the group

selected as active readers reported similar types and

amounts of reading. There was no difference in their

perceived purposes for reading or in the amount of mater­

ial they read. Also boys and girls in the active group

reread a similar number of books.

The fourth purpose of this study was to examine

reading behavior in relation to the classroom grouping of the sixth-graders. One hypothesis was tested and there was a significant difference in the number of purposes reported read by the children in the separate classrooms.

The amounts of materials read by children in the various classrooms could not be tested.

Chapter IV, Part II, presents data on the fifth purpose of the study, which is to describe selected active readers in greater depth. PART XI: CASE STUDIES OF ACTIVE READERS

The fifth purpose of this study was to describe the school, home, and personal environments of selected active readers. During the first week of the study, the researcher listed children who were nominated as active readers and who reported reading for the maximum number of purposes. From this list, librarians, teachers, and re­ searcher selected six readers to study individually. The four girls and two boys chosen were considered to be repre­ sentative of the other active readers. To collect informa­ tion about these six readers, parents and present teachers were interviewed as were the children themselves. In the case studies that follow, names have been changed to insure anonymity.

Case Study 1; Mary

Mary is the oldest of four sisters, all of whom enjoy reading. She often shares what she is reading with her ten-year old sister. Their mother, a former English teacher, believes that her next daughter, age seven, is finding the same joy in reading that the older girls have displayed. The pre-school youngest is captivated by

114 115

Mother Goose and Dr. Seuss rhymes. Mary's parents enjoy

reading and often discuss their reading with the girls.

Reading History

Mary is an enthusiastic reader who has enjoyed

independent re.ading since she was in first grade. She

remembers disliking the "Dick and Jane" readers and going

to the library for books with more plot and action. In

the second grade, Mary developed an interest in biographies

and has read most of those available in her school library.

A fourth-grade teacher who was "spectacular” with books

recommended many new authors and stories to Mary. Mary

still enjoys talking with this teacher about her reading.

Present Reading Interests

At the present time, Mary is involved with the

Louise May Alcott books. She has read Little Women three

times. She has also just finished The Diary of Anne Frank.

She enjoyed the Diary because she believes she "really

found out about Anne's life." Mary has recommended the book to others and plans to reread it herself. According

to Mary, she gets a great deal more from a book the second or third time through it. Her interest in biographies has

continued and includes a desire to find out about people who lived in earlier times. Mary has read all the Laura

Ingalls Wilder books. Mary shares her reading enthusiasms with her best friend, Jane; her three sisters; and her mother. 116

Mary reads other materials besides books. She has

her own subscription to Seventeen magazines and reads ar­

ticles in the women’s and news magazines to which her par­

ents subscribe. Like most of her friends, she scans the

newspaper for current events for school and enjoys the

daily comics. Often she will read a newspaper article if

the headline strikes her as interesting or if her mother

recommends it. Mary's family shares interesting news

articles. Mary also reads picture books to her youngest

sister and recommends books to the others.

Her teacher considers Mary an excellent student

with a keen mind and a love for the language arts. Mary

seems to enjoy writing her own stories and poems and is

often busy in the creative writing area of the classroom.

Home Environment

Mary's mother considers her oldest daughter to be

extremely reliable, mature, and well organized. Mary likes

to be challenged in daily activities as well as in what

she reads. She is a great help around the house and has

a variety of interests, including a love of baseball. Mary

relates to characters in stories and is often absorbed with her reading. She also likes to be read to, a job

that Mary's father enjoys.

According to her mother, when Mary was in third grade, her avid interest in reading became very noticeable. 117

At that time, Mary was less selective about what she read

and often substituted quantity for quality. As she grew

older, she became more selective. Her mother reports that

she considered herself a "minor expert" on Louise May Alcott

until Mary introduced her to some Alcott boohs of which she

had never heard.

Although the television serves as background noise

most of the day in Mary's home, Mary is not much interested

in the programs. She often retires to her room to read.

Mary's parents control night viewing since they believe

the girls need to spend time on other activities.

Parents1 Attitude Toward Readincr

Mary1s parents have a highly supportive attitude

toward reading. Both enjoy reading and find it a neces­

sary and important part of their daily lives. Her mother believes reading helps to develop communication skills and

spark creative ideas. She encourages quality reading to help the girls develop taste in literature. She believes parents should start reading to their children as early as possible, supply them with books when they are young, and share the excitement of new books with them. She encourages books as gifts for the children to increase their personal libraries. 118

Case Study 2: Jane

Jane is an independent sixth-grader. She has her

own paper route and is in charge of starting the evening

meals for her parents, both of whom work. Jane and her

brother, who is younger, share household chores and enjoy

playing games with their friends. Often Jane is the leader

in organized games.

Reading History

Jane considers reading an enjoyable hobby. She

started reading independently in first grade as soon as she

had mastered the beginning skills. An all time favorite book, Understood Betsy by Dorothy Canfield, was a gift from her grandmother and Jane has reread it many times. Jane

considers her second-grade teacher to be the teacher who

liked books the most. This teacher devoted many hours to reading and sharing books with her classes.

Present Reading Interests

For the past two years, Mary and Jane have been best friends and have shared books. Jane's current favorite book, The House of Sixty Fathers by DeJong, was one that

Mary had suggested she read. Like Mary, Jane enjoys bio­ graphies . The DeJong book reminds her of a biography and also "it's exciting." Jane enjoys stories about people from different cultures and earlier periods of time. 119

Jane reads the paper she delivers, finding most of the newspaper sections of interest. She enjoys finding out ahout current news. However, her favorite section is the comic section, with the sports section next. Jane reads several magazines regularly. Her family subscribe to Reader's Digest and National Geographic, both of which

Jane enjoys reading. She also reads the women's and news magazines available in her home. She keeps up with current sports events by reading Sports Illustrated at school. She and her younger brother read a few comic books; Superman is their current favorite.

Jane's teacher reports that Jane has read all the books in the classroom collection and is a frequent visitor to the library. Jane is an above average student who excels in most subject areas although she favors language arts and reading.

Home Environment

Jane's mother is very proud of her daughter's per­ formance at home and at school. In play activities outside of school, Jane is a good organizer. She is a very respon­ sible child who helps around the house. She is interested in many activities, including gymnastics, piano lessons, and the Pioneer Girls Club. She spends a great deal of time reading; she reads quickly and comprehends easily. Her mother reports that "reading is a part of Jane." Jane 120

feels free to talk about what she reads with her parents,

although no special time is set aside for such discussions.

Her parents often read stories to Jane and her brother when

they were younger. Jane's father especially enjoyed read­

ing the Uncle Remus stories to them.

Television viewing is restricted to special hours;

Jane's parents consider reading and other activities more beneficial. Jane and her brother have their own special places to read and both enjoy their privacy.

Parents1 Attitude Toward Reading

Jane's mother believes that a love of reading will be beneficial to Jane as she grows older. Books are con­ sidered an important part of Jane's home. Her mother en­ courages books as gifts for her children because she be­ lieves it is important for them to develop their private collections. Jane's grandmother cooperates by sending books for birthday and Christmas gifts. Jane's parents consider reading a necessary and vital part of their family lives. As they view it, children are encouraged to read when books are easily accessible, when the children are read to as youngsters, and when they see their parents reading. 121

Case Study 3: Dick

Dick is the oldest of three children. His ten-year

old sister is becoming an avid reader, while his sister in

kindergarten still enjoys having her parents read to her.

Dick's mother, a former remedial reading tea,cher, is famil­

iar with children's books and has advised her children on

some of their book selections. Bedtime is a favorite tirfie

of day for Dick and his sisters because their mother always

reads to them. They all enjoy the story time.

Reading History

Dick learned to read when he was four years old.

His mother spent a good deal of time reading to him, and

he soon began to pick out words from the storybooks. Before

long he was reading books on his own, especially the

Dr. Seuss books. Dick's reading models have always been

his parents, who have shared his enthusiasm for books. He

does not have a favorite book, although he has read a num­ ber of the Matt Christopher sports books and the Jan Wil­

liams ' books more than once.

Present Reading Interests

Sports now compete with reading for Dick1s free time. He plays baseball and volleyball and hopes to par­ ticipate in organized sports activities in junior high school. The interest in sports has motivated Dick to read 122 sports-related materials. He has his own subscription to

Sports Illustrated and Boy's Life. He also reads the sports section of the daily paper.

However, Dick reads many other materials on differ­ ent subjects. He enjoyed the stories in the basic reader,

Reading Power, because each story was "different and inter­ esting." He enjoys Reader's Digest because of the varied material that is presented in the magazine. He also buys comic books and avidly reads the comics, front page, and television sections of the daily papers. Dick selects stories to share with his younger sister and spends time reading to her. Dick's teacher reports that Dick is very bright and does extremely well in all classroom activities.

He is a patrol cadet and a library volunteer as well as an excellent student.

Home Environment

Dick says his mother sometimes wishes he would spend as much time completing his chores as he does reading.

His mother agrees and calls him her "resident absent-minded professor." She feels he is a typical boy, who will help around the house when asked but would rather by playing or reading. He has been known to go to bed with a flashlight in order to finish an exciting book. Dick's mother con­ siders her son to be a well-rounded boy. He enjoys sports, 123

Boy Scouts, playing the piano, and mathematics as well as

reading.

All the members of Dick's family enjoy reading.

The house is filled with books, and the family makes fre­ quent trips to the public library. Dick's, father is an active reader, who enjoys reading to his children; and

Dick enjoys sharing what he has read with his father.

Dick's sister will often share her books with the family.

Relatives give the children books as gifts. Dick's parents buy copies of favorite books for the children but encour­ age using the library for most books.

Dick enjoys watching television on Friday and Satur­ day evenings. His parents do not watch television and con­ sider two evenings a week of television viewing enough for

Dick and his sisters.

Parents' Attitude Toward Reading

Dick's parents believe his interest in reading will help him develop into a well informed adult. They see reading as an important and necessary part of their lives and encourage their children to read. Dick's mother be­ lieves that children should be read to at an early age and should be encouraged to develop reading interests of their own. She believes that if the parents are consistent in reading to their children and in reading themselves, their children will develop an interest in and a love for reading. 124

Case Study 4; Sally

Sally is the youngest of four children. Her inter­ est in reading is sparked by a desire to be part of the family discussions. Both parents are avid readers and are presently enrolled in a Great Books Course. They enjoy reading aloud to each other and sharing an interesting passage or thought with their children. At the present time, Sally's brother and oldest sister are away at college.

Reading History

Sally's interest in reading was sparked by her next oldest sister who kept talking about the interesting books she was reading. The first book Sally read on her own was

The Middle Sister because it was her sister's favorite and

Sally was "determined" to read it. Sally does not have favorite books and does not enjoy rereading stories because

"there are so many and it's always fun to try something new." She remembers enjoying the freedom of being able to read on her own in first grade and has read widely ever since. Sally thinks of her fourth-grade teacher, who shared

Misty of Chinncoteaque. Rabbit Hill, and Brightv of the

Grand Canyon with the class, as the teacher who enjoyed books the most. Her sixth-grade teacher reads to the class daily. This year's favorites have been Chancy and the Grand 125

Rascal and A Wrinkle in Time. The teacher also includes

a fifteen-minute free reading time in the daily schedule.

Sally is a good student who is active in many of

the class projects, especially current events, social stud­

ies, and arithmetic. Her teacher reports that Sally is a very verbal child with an above-average vocabulary.

Present Reading Interests

Sally's reading time is divided among magazines, newspapers, •and'books. She reads two weekly news magazines that are delivered to the house and still subscribes to

Jack and Jill. Sally enjoys the comics and horoscopes in the newspapers and reads the rest of the daily papers to keep up with the news in general. She has a large library of children's books, some of which have been handed down to her by her brother and sisters.

Sally has many outside interests, including embroid­ ery, bead work, painting, and animals. Her love of animals has developed into an interest in veterinary medicine, which she hopes to pursue in college. She reads information books about dogs and cats and has already sent for college cata­ logues. Sally and her sister enjoy painting and their paintings are hung in their bedrooms and in the living room.

Sally likes reading and enjoys sharing what she has read with her sister and her friends. Sally says, "Reading is fun." 126

Home Environment

Television viewing is of minor importance in Sally's

home. The family is usually too busy to watch, although

Sally has a' favorite show, The Waltons, and watches about

four hours of television per week. Sally's parents do not

encourage television viewing, since they, see it as too

passive an activity for children. Anyway, their children

are usually too busy doing other things.

Sally's parents love to read. Dinner time is an

important event in Sally's home because family members

share their current reading enthusiasms. The children have been known to search through books or magazines to

find something interesting to bring to these discussions.

Parents' Attitude Toward Reading

Sally's mother has fond memories of spending long afternoons sitting under a tree and reading. She sees

Sally's reading as an extension of her own personal love for reading. Both parents value reading highly and feel their children will be better rounded because of their- love of books. They believe the best way to encourage children to read is by setting an adult example. 127

Case Study 5; Stan

Stan is the youngest of three children and the only

child at home. His married sister lives in the East and his brother, a member of the varsity swimming team, is a

junior in college. Stan's interest in swimming is partially motivated by a desire to emulate his older brother.

Present Reading Interests

Stan's life revolves around swimming. Every week­ day morning at six-thirty, Stan attends a swimming class at the local high school. His reading interests are a reflec­ tion of his sports activities. He subscribes to Swimming

World and avidly reads all the swimming news in his brother's

Sports Illustrated and in the local newspapers.

Stan is also interested in other sports and has read the Matt Christopher books’three times. His interest in sports, which began to develop in the second grade when he played on a basketball team, was encouraged by his brother.

Stan like to read "to find out about different people and different things." After a class discussion of

Guy Fawkes, Stan decided to read Plot Night to find out more about who Fawkes was and what he had done. He enjoys the Alfred Hitchcock Mystery series of books and reads them all at least twice. He also reads some Hardy Bovs mysteries, since he enjoys the excitement and adventure in these tales. 128

Last year, Tie was disappointed to find out that he had

read all the children's mystery stories available at the

local library.

Stan reads the newspaper and especially enjoys the

comics and the crossword puzzles. He also has a subscrip­ tion to the Children's Digest and the National Geographic

(School Edition). He reads the family's Reader's Digest. enjoying the humor sections in particular.

Reading History

His interest in reading became noticeable when he was in the first grade, by which time he was recognized as possessing an advanced vocabulary. Learning to read was easy for Stan and by second grade he was reading indepen­ dently. His parents encouraged his interest in reading by taking him on weekly trips to the library. He remembers his fourth-grade teacher as a person who seemed to like books and who often read to the class. After hearing

Charlie and the Chocolate Factory read aloud, Stan checked the book out to read himself. His sixth-grade teacher con­ siders Stan an above-average student who capably completes his work regularly. Stan takes an active part in class discussions and has many friends in the classroom. 129

Home Environment

His mother believes that Stan read more books when

he was in the fourth and fifth grades than he does now.

She attributes this to his new swimming activity, which has

eliminated some of his free time. She reports that while

the time Stan spends reading books has decreased, he is

spending more time reading magazines and newspapers now.

Stan is an "average" boy around the house, who often has

to be reminded of his duties. He takes an active interest

in the family's discussions of current events and books.

Stan's television viewing is limited to a half-

hour after dinner. His parents do not encourage television

• viewing during the week since Stan has busy days with his

swimming lessons and school.

Parents' Attitude Toward Reading

Stan's parents are avid readers who make weekly

trips to the library. They often discuss what they are

reading with their children. They believe reading will be

an important part of their children's future and encourage

reading by giving books as gifts. Stan's parents and older brother and sister used to read to Stan when he was younger.

Stan's mother believes that reading to children when they

are young encourages them to become independent readers. 130

Case Study 6: Pat

Pat is an only child who has developed an interest

in reading and in gymnastics. She will often lie on her

practice mats while reading. Her parents work. Pat's

mother is a social worker and Pat's father, who is blind,

has his own business.

Reading History

Pat has liked to read since she started picking out

words in her picture storybooks when she was four years old.

She remembers completing her first "Dick and Jane" reader

and being very happy because she had read it all the way

through independently. Pat has developed an interest in

Nancy Drew mysteries sparked by her mother. As a child,

her mother had read a great number of these books and was

able to recommend specific titles. Pat has reread many of

the 40 books she owns as well as the Caroline

Haywood series books. She remembers enjoying the books

her fifth-grade teacher read to the class and considers

that teacher a friend of books. Pat's sixth-grade teacher

has read Peter Potts and It's Like This, Cat to the class

and is planning to read Freaky Friday. The teacher also

enjoys reading poetry to the class.

Present Interests

Pat's teacher considers Pat a social leader in the class. Pat is capable of verbalizing her opinions and takes 131

an active part in class discussions. She is an above-

average student, who enjoys creative writing. Pat writes

her own stories and poems. This interest was sparked by a

fifth-: grade teacher who encouraged Pat's early writing

attempts.

Pat's interests include gymnastics,, baseball, and

reading. She takes ballet, acrobatics, and gymnastics

lessons and hopes to compete on an Olympic team someday.

She spends a lot of time reading. Pat keeps up with sports

news by reading the daily paper and Sports Illustrated, to

which she has her own subscription.

Home Environment

Pat's mother believes Pat does a lot of reading

and is an adequate help around the house. Pat goes to the

library regularly and has learned to find most materials

in the public and school libraries. Pat has had a library

card since she was in first grade and her parents encourage her to use the library. She works around the house when asked and completes the jobs she is assigned.

Parents' Attitude Toward Reading

Her mother considers reading an important force bearing on Pat's future and encourages her to read. They often share what Pat has read. Pat likes to recommend books to her mother, who reads mostly magazines and newspapers. 132

Her father often listens to talking hooks. Pat's parents believe that children will be more likely to become inde­ pendent readers if they are read to as youngsters and if books are readily accessible. They believe parents should give children the opportunity to become familiar with printed matter.

Summary

Six children, four girls and two boys, identified by the researcher, librarians, and teachers as active readers were interviewed. Parents and teachers of the children were questioned also to develop a comprehensive view of their home and school environment.

There were many similarities in the reading his­ tories of the six children. All of the children could remember their first reading successes. Two of the chil­ dren learned to read at home by picking words out of story­ books while the others remembered mastering early reading skills in the first grade. By the time the children were in the second grade, they were independent and interested readers. The six children remembered names of teachers who seemed to like books. In some cases, the children could remember the books these teachers shared with their classes. 133

These readers had special interests around which

much of their reading was centered. Two girls were inter­

ested in a type of literature, biography, while the others

reported interests’ in sports and animals. The children

read books and magazines to gain more information about

their interests. Although all the children expressed a

special interest, they also read many materials in diverse

subject areas. They had personal libraries of books, maga­

zine subscriptions, and access to family newspapers and

magazines. The children were regarded as good students

and took active roles in the classroom activities.

The home environments of the children had many

similarities, including easy access to many reading mater­

ials. All the mothers had read to their children when

they were younger and provided a time and place for their

children to read as they grew older. The parents also bought books as gifts and planned trips to the libraries.

They reported that the children were good helpers at home

and were busy in many outside activities.

The parents encouraged their children's interest in

reading. They had a positive attitude toward reading and believed success in reading would benefit their children

in the future. The parents liked to read themselves and

shared their reading with their children. They felt read­

ing was an important and necessary part of their families

lives. 134

Despite these similarities, there were great dif­ ferences among the children in terms of personal charac­ teristics .

Chapter IV, Part III, will include a discussion of the inventory data. PART III: INVENTORY DATA

Data from the inventory entitled "My Reading Habits

and Interests” (see Appendix C) are summarized in the follow­

ing discussion. Responses of the children have been tab­

ulated and examined in terms of (1) characteristics of the

group as a whole,. (2) differences between boys and girls,

and (3) differences between active and other readers. The

data so derived are presented topically according to the major sections of the inventory.

Reading vs. Television

The 276 children reported reading a total of 412.5 hours a day (see Table 17), with an average time of 1.49 hours per day. They reported a total time spent viewing television of 808.5 hours or 2.92 hours per day. Thus, children reported themselves as spending nearly twice as much time each day viewing television as they spent read­ ing.

The 136 girls read for about 25 minutes more a day than the 140 boys. Girls averaged 1.69 hours on read­ ing while boys averaged 1.28 hours. The boys and girls spent almost the same amount of time viewing television.

135 136

The boys averaged 3.04 hours of television viewing com­ pared to 2.90 hours for the girls. Recorded in minutes, the boys averaged eight minutes more a day watching tele­ vision than the girls.

Table 17

Average Time Spent Reading and Viewing Television in Hours

Television Number of Book Reading Viewincr Students Total Aver acre Total Averaqe

Girls 136 225.5 1.69 381.5 2.90 Boys 140 187.0 1.28 427.0 3.04

Active 63 118.5 1.88 159.0 2.52 Others 213 290.5 1.36 662.0 3.11

Total 276 412.5 1.49 808.5 2.93

Both boys and girls spent more time watching tele­ vision than they spent reading. Boys averaged 1.76 hours or one hour and 45 minutes more time daily viewing tele­ vision than reading. The difference between television viewing and reading for the girls was 1.21 hours or one hour and 12 minutes.

The average time spent on reading reported by the

63 active readers was 1.88 hours. The 213 other readers reported an average of 1.36 hours. Active readers read for

.52 hours, approximately 31 minutes, more each day than other readers. Active readers averaged 2.52 hours per day of television viewing time compared to a 3.11 hour average 137 for other readers. Other readers viewed television for

.59 hours or 35 minutes more than active readers. Expressed in percentages, active readers reported spending 38 percent more time reading than did other readers, while other read­ ers reported watching television for 23 percent more time than active readers.

Past Reading Experiences

In reporting on a variety of past reading exper­ iences, children named receiving books as gifts more often than any of the other four choices: using the school library; being taken to the library; being read to by par­ ents or brothers and sisters; or belonging to a bookclub

(see Table 18). In all cases, more than 50 percent of the students reported having the experience. Seventy-two per­ cent of the total group reported receiving books as gifts.

Sixty-six percent of the sample reported that they were read to by their parents or brothers and sisters, which was not much above the 63 percent response on "was taken to the library." Sixty-one percent of the children re­ ported using the school library. Finally, the lowest frequency of response was to membership in paperback book clubs. Even then, 55 percent of the children reported having that experience. Table 18

Percent of Children’s Responses to Reading Experience Questions

Girls Boys Active Others Total n % n % n % n % n %

Used the school 89 65.4 81 57.9 40 63.5 130 61.0 170 61.6 library

Received books 95 69.9 106 75.7 53 84.1 148 69.5 201 72.8 as gifts

Was taken to 89 65.4 85 60.7 48 76.2 126 59.2 174 63.0 library

Belonged to book­ 82 60.3 70 50.0 43 68.3 109 51.2 152 55.1 club or ordered paperbacks

Was read to by 97 71.3 85 60.7 46 73.0 136 63.8 182 66.0 parents or brothers and sisters 138 139

Girls reported a greater amount of past experience

than boys except in receiving books as gifts. Seventy-five

percent of the boys reported receiving books as gifts com­

pared to 69 percent of the girls. Seventy-one percent of

the total girl sample said that they had been read to by

family members as against 60 percent of the boys. Sixty-

five percent of the girls reported using the school library

and taking trips to the community library. More boys re­

ported previous library trips (60.7%) than use of the school

library (57.9%). Girls and boys reported book club member­

ship less frequently than any of the other choices, but

girls outdid boys here also, 60.3 to 50 percent.

Active readers reported more past experience with

books than other readers. Eighty-four percent of the active

readers noted they received books as gifts as compared

with 70 percent of the other readers. Active and other

readers were about equal in trips to the school library

(63.5 and 61 percent). The greatest difference was in book

club membership. Sixty-eight percent of the active readers

reported having belonged to book clubs as against 51 per­

cent of the other readers. The active readers also reported being taken to the library more often (76 percent as against

59 percent) and being read to more frequently at home (73 percent against 64 percent). 140

Other Sources of Reading Materials

When children were asked to note the places they

went to get books to read, they reported using many differ­

ent sources. Six choices were listed: school library,

classroom, book club, home, friends, and community libraries.

Almost 90 percent of the children reported using

the school library as a source of materials (Table 19).

The second two most popular sources were the home (81.5%)

and community libraries (73.9%). Less than half the chil­

dren (41%) reported book club membership and only a third

(32.6%) reported getting reading materials from their

friends.

Boys and girls reported little difference in sources used. Ninety-two percent of the girls and 86 percent of the boys reported using the school library to obtain reading materials. Eighty-one percent of both groups, boys and girls, used books from their home libraries. Seventy-five percent of the girls and 72 percent of the boys reported using the community library. The frequency of responses of boys and girls to use of classroom collections was simi­ lar, sixty-three percent of the girls and 66 percent of the boys. The greatest difference in the responses of girls and boys occurred in sharing books with friends. Fifty percent of the girls reported obtaining reading materials from their friends and only 18 percent of the boys noted Table 19

Percent of Children's Responses to Sources of Materials

Girls Boys Active Others Total n % n % n % n % n %

School library 126 92.6 121 86.4 62 98.4 185 86.9 247 89.5

Classroom 87 63.9 93 66.4 50 79.4 130 61.0 180 65.2

Bookclub 67 49.2 48 34.2 32 50.8 83 39.0 115 41.7

Home 111 81.6 114 81.4 55 87.4 170 79.8 225 81.5

Friends 65 47.8 25 17.9 32 50.8 58 27.2 90 32.6

Community library 103 75.7 101 72.1 53 84.1 151 70.9 204 73.9

H H 142

this. Also, nearly half of the girls (49%) and only a

third of the boys (34%) reported book club membership.

Active and other readers used sources in much the

same ’fashion. Ninety-eight percent of the active readers

and 87 percent of the other readers reported using the school library. The second highest percent- response rate was in reading materials selected from the home, with 87 percent of the active readers and 80 percent of the other

readers getting reading materials there. Similar percent responses were recorded for use of the community libraries, eighty-four percent of the active and 70 percent of the other readers so reporting. Active readers reported using the classroom collections more frequently than other read­ ers, eighty percent compared to 61 percent. Fifty percent of the active readers and 40 percent of other readers re­ ported book club membership. The greatest difference in the active and other readers responses was in sharing books with friends, with half the active readers so reporting as against 27 percent of other readers.

Frequency of Library Use

When the children were asked to specify the number of times per week they used school and community libraries, they had five responses available: twice a week, once a week, every two weeks, once a month, or not very often

(Tables 20 and 21). Thirty-seven percent of the children Table 20

Children's Percent Responses to Number of School Library Visits

Twice a We eh Once a Week Every 2 Weeks Once a Month Never O/ n /O n % n % n % n %

Girls 65 47.7 36 26.5 28 20.6 3 2.2 4 2.

Boys 46 32.9 42 330.0 38 27.1 4 2.8 10 7.:

Active 29 46.0 20 31.7 13 20.6 1 1.5 0

Others 82 38.5 58 27.2 53 24.9 6 2.8 14 6.'

Total 111 37.3 78 24.2 66 23.9 7 2.5 14 5.. 143 Table 21

Children’s Percent Responses to Number of Community Library Visits

Twice a Week Once a Week Every 2 Weeks Once a Month Never n % n % n % n % n %

Girls 5 3.7 18 13.2 31 15.4 45 33.1 47 34.6

Boys 4 2.9 11 7.9 25 17.9 47 33.8 53 37.9

Active 2 3.2 10 15.9 9 14.2 27 42.6 15 23.8

Others 7 3.3 19 8.9 37 17.4 119 55.9 85 40.0

Total 9 3.0 29 10.5 46 13.0 92 33.3 100 36.2

H 145

reported using the school library twice a week while 24

percent reported visiting the school library once a week.

Twenty-three percent made bi-weekly visits. In all, more

than 84 percent of the children reported using the school

library at least twice a month. The figures were reversed

for reported visiting to the community libraries. Almost

70 percent of the children reported visiting the community

libraries once a month or never. Thirteen percent of the

children reported bi-weekly visits while ten percent re­ ported weekly trips. Few reported going as often as twice a week.

More girls than boys reported two trips per week to the school library, forty-seven percent of the girls com­ pared to 32 percent of the boys. Twenty-six percent of the girls and 30 percent of the boys reported weekly visits.

In all, more than 80 percent of both girls and boys reported visiting the school library at least twice a month. As with the total group, the reported frequencies of visits to the community libraries were much less. More than 65 percent of the girls and boys reported visiting the community libraries once a month or never.

More than 90 percent of the active and other readers used the school library at least twice a month. Forty-six percent of the active readers and 38 percent of the other readers visited the school libraries twice a week. Twenty 146

percent of the active readers and more than 58 percent

of the other readers made weekly trips. Visits to the

community libraries were less frequent although nearly 16 percent of the active readers and nine percent of the other

readers reported weekly visits. More than 66 percent of the

active readers and 96 percent of the other readers reported visiting community libraries once a month or never.

Types of Materials Read

In reporting on types of materials read every day, children were asked to choose from the following list: magazines, books, comic books, information book, newspapers, textbooks, and basic readers. Children reported using books

(74%), newspapers (69%) , and magazines (63%) more than other materials (see Table 22). Textbook use was reported at 49 percent followed by information books (39%), comic books

(38%), and basic readers (22%) .

Books were most chosen by girls (80%) as their chief choice of daily reading materials. Boys put magazines first

(70%). In order of responses, girls reported reading news­ papers (69%), magazines (55.9%), textbooks (54.4%), informa­ tion books (38.2%) , comic books (35.3%) and basic readers

(23.5%) . The order for boys was magazines (70.7%), news­ papers (69.3%), books (69.3%), textbooks (45%), comic books

(41.4%) , information books (40.7%), and basic readers (20.7%) . Table 22

Materials Reported as Read Daily by Children

Girls Boys Active Others Total n % n % n % n % n %

Magazines 76 55.9 99 70.7 38 60.3 137 64.3 175 63.4

Books 109 80.1 97 69.3 58 92.1 148 69.5 206 74.6

Comic Books 48 35.3 58 41.4 22 34.9 84 39.4 106 38.4

Information 52 38.2 57 40.7 23 36.5 ' 86 40.4 109 39.5 Books

Newspapers 94 69.1 97 69.3 52 82.5 139 65.3 191 69.2

Text Books 74 54.4 63 45.0 37 58.7 100 46.9 .137 49.6

Basic Readers 32 23.5 29 20.7 18 28.6 43 20.2 61 22.1

H 148

Ninety-two percent of the active readers reported

reading books on a daily basis compared to 69 percent of the

other readers. Although active and other readers chose books

and newspapers as the most common daily reading materials,

active readers reported greater frequencies--92 percent for books as against 69.5 percent and 82.5 percent for news­ papers as against 65.3 percent. After books and newspapers, the active readers' ordered responses were magazines (60.3%), textbooks (58%), information books (36.5%) ,-.and-basic read­ ers (28.6%) . The order for other readers was the same as active readers although the percent of responses differed somewhat: magazines (64.3%), textbooks (46.9%), information books (40.3%) , comic books (39.4%), and basic readers (20.2%) .

Reading Series Books

Presented with a list of 14 series books, the chil­ dren were asked to write the number of books that they had read from each series. Children read an average of 14.3 series books per child (see Table 23). Nancy Drew Mysteries was the series reported read most often with an average of

3.11 Nancy Drew books reported per child. This was an aver­ age of two more books read than any other series listed. Table 23

Average Number of Series Books Read

Girls Boys Active Others Total n % n % n______% _____ n______% ______n_____%

Alfred Hitchcock 161 1.18 273 1.96 107 1.70 327 1.53 434 1.57 Mysteries

Bobbsey Twins 224 1.65 30 .21 124 1.97 130 .61 254 .92

Caroline Haywood 193 1.42 117 .84 127 2.01 183 .86 310 1.12 Eddie Books

14 .10 0 2 .03 12 .06 14 .05

The Hardy Boys 71 .52 386 2.76 98 1.56 359 1.69 457 1.66

Laura Ingalls 368 2.71 62 .44 159 2.52 271 1.27 430 1.55 Wilder Series

Matt Christopher 48 .35 316 2.26 65 1.03 299 1.40 364 1.32 Sports Stories

Maud Lovelace 76 .56 3 .02 22 .35 57 .27 79 .29 Betsy-Tacy Books

Nancy Drew 764 5.62 96 .69 435 6.90 425 2.00 860 3.11 Mysteries

Paddington 138 1.01 64 .46 60 .95 142 .67 202 .73 Adventures Table 23 (Continued)

Girls Boys Active Others Total n % n °A n % n % n %

Sue Barton, Nurse 25 .20 13 .09 1 .11 31 .15 38 .14

Tarzan 37 .27 103 .74 15 .24 125 .59 140 .50

Walter Brooks 1 71 .52 58 .41 31 .49 98 .46 129 .47 Freddy

Wizard of Oz 147 1.08 87 .62 68 1.08 166 .78 234 .85 151

Among the choices provided, girls read more Nancy

Drew Mystery boohs than books from any other series. On an

average, each girl reported reading at least five Nancy

Drew stories. The most widely read series for boys was the

Hardy Bovs stories followed closely by the Matt Christopher

sports books. Boys read an average of 2.8 Hardy Bovs stories

and 2.2 Matt Christopher stories.

Active readers read more Nancv Drew and Laura Ingalls

Wilder books than other readers. Again, Nancv Drew books were most popular with an average of over six books read by each active reader and two books read by other readers.

Active readers read an average of at least two Caroline Hay­ wood Eddie books and Wilder books. In general, active

readers read more series books than other readers.

Favorite Books

A question on the inventory requested children's

free responses. They were asked to record the names of their favorite books or books that they had read more than once. The most frequently mentioned books were Charlie and the Chocolate Factory by Ronald Dahl and Charlotte1s Web by E. B. White (Appendix J). Books receiving at least seven votes included Follow My Leader by James Garfield,

James and the Giant Peach by Ronald Dahl, the Laura Ingalls

Wilder Little House Books, Little Women by Louise May Alcott, and A Wrinkle in Time by Madeleine L'Engle. 152

Charlie and the Chocolate Factory received the most

mention by boys and girls. Charlotte1s Web received the

second highest number of votes for boys and girls. Three

selections, Charlie and the Chocolate Factory, the Laura

Ingalls Wilder Little House books, and A Wrinkle in Time were mentioned most often by active readers..

Favorite School Subjects

Finally, the last inventory question called for

children to select their favorite school subject. Physi­

cal education received the highest percent of responses for boys and girls (see Table 24). The girls' second choice was about equally divided between reading and art while the boys favored arithmetic.

The active readers were more evenly divided in their choice of favorite subjects. They chose reading

(25.4%) almost as often as they chose physical education

(28.6%). Seventeen percent of the active readers chose arithmetic. Much lower percentages were recorded for art

(11.1%), science (4.8%), and music (1.1%), while no active reader chose language. The percentages of active and other readers who chose arithmetic, seventeen percent for active and 16 percent for others, were similar as were the responses for art. Eleven percent of both groups chose art as a favorite subject. Almost 20 percent more active read­ ers chose reading than other readers. Only four percent Table 24

Children's Favorite Subjects Reported in Percents

Girls Boys Active Others Total n % n % n % n % n %

Arithmetic 22 16.1 24 17.1 11 17.5 35 16.4 46 16.7

Reading 26 19.1 7 5.0 16 25.4 17 8.0 33 12.0

Music 5 3.7 2 1.4 3 1.1 4 1.9 7 2.5

Language 1 .7 2 1.4 0 3 1.4 3 1.1

Science 7 5.1 18 12.9 3 4.8 22 10.3 25 9.1

Phy. Ed. 43 31.6 75 53.6 18 28.6 100 46.9 118 42.8

Social Studies 7 5.1 5 3.6 5 7.9 7 3.3 12 4.3

Art 25 18.4 7 5.0 7 11.1 25 11.7 32 18.2

H w 10 of the active readers chose science compared to ten per­

cent of the other readers.

Summary

The intent of the inventory was to gain more infor­

mation about the reading habits of the sixth-grade children

in this study. Responses were reported for the children

as a whole, by sex, and by membership in the active reader

and other groups. The order of reporting has followed the

inventory sections.

The children reported spending nearly twice as much

time each day watching television as they spent reading.

Boys watched slightly more television than girls. The

average television viewing time for boys was a little over

three hours, while the girls' average was about eight min- 'v utes less. Other readers reported spending art>average of

35 minutes more each day watching television than active

readers.

The children read for an average of one and one-half hours per day. This was an average of one hour and 20 min­ utes less time spent on reading than on television viewing.

Girls reported reading an average of 25 minutes more each day than boys. Active readers read for approximately 30 minutes more each day than other readers. 155

In reporting on a variety of past reading exper­

iences, children named receiving books as gifts more often

than any of the other four choices: using the school

library; being taken to the library; being read to by par­

ents or brothers and sisters; or belonging to a bookclub.

In all cases, more than 50- percent of the students reported

having the experiences. Girls reported a greater amount of

past experience than boys except in receiving books as gifts.

Active readers reported more past experience with books

than other readers. The greatest difference was in book

club membership. Sixty-eight percent of the active read­

ers reported having belonged to book clubs against 51 per­

cent of the other readers.

Children used many different sources to acquire and

select reading materials. Six choices were listed: school

library, classroom, book club, home, friends, and community

libraries. Almost 90 percent of the children reported using

the school library as their source of materials. Boys and

girls reported little difference in sources used. The most

frequently used sources for boys and girls were the school

libraries, the children's homes, and the community librar­

ies. Active and other readers used sources in much the

same fashion. The greatest difference in the active and

other readers' responses was in sharing books with friends, with half the active readers so reporting against 27 per­

cent of other readers. 156

When the children were asked to specify the number

of times per week they used school and community libraries,

they had five responses available: twice a week, once a

week, every two weeks, once a month, or not very often.

In all, more than 84 percent of the children reported using

the school library at least twice a month. The figures

were reversed for reported visits to the community libraries.

More girls than boys reported two trips per week to the

school library. More than 90 percent of the active and

other readers reported using the school library at least

twice a month. The frequencies of visits to the community

libraries for active and other readers were much less.

When asked to note types of materials read every

day, children reported using books, newspapers, and maga­

zines more often than other materials. Books were most

chosen by girls as their chief choice of daily reading

materials; boys put magazines first. Although active and

other readers chose books and newspapers as the most common

daily reading materials, the percentage of active readers

so reporting was higher than the others.

Presented with a list of fourteen series books, the

children wrote the number of books they had read from each

series. Nancv Drew Mysteries was the series reported read

most often. Girls read more Nancy Drew Mysteries than books

from any other series. The most widely read series for boys was stories. Active readers read more Nancy 157

Drew and Laura Ingalls Wilder books than other readers.

Active readers read more series books than other readers.

Children were asked to name their favorite book.

The most frequently mentioned books were Charlie and the

Chocolate Factory and Charlotte's Web. Active readers also mentioned the Laura Ingalls Wilder Little House books, and

A Wrinkle in Time.

When asked to choose their favorite subject, most children selected physical education. However, active readers chose reading as often as physical education.

The following chapter will present a brief restate­ ment of the purposes, procedures, and summaries of the study. In addition, limitations and conclusions of the observed findings will be stated, along with suggestions for further study and research. CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

The primary purpose of this study was to examine

descriptors of the active sixth-grade readers to describe

their reading behavior. The major characteristic of the

reading behavior under consideration was the range of pur­

poses the children perceived for their reading. The chil­

dren, grouped as active or other readers, were compared to

discover similarities and differences in their reading be­

havior. Boys and girls identified as active readers were

compared in an attempt to describe unique factors in their

reading. The home, school, and personal environments of

six active readers were examined to develop a more compre­ hensive picture of the reading background and habits of

active readers. Finally, an inventory was given to all students to secure a broader picture of the children's reading behavior. Questions concerned amount of time spent reading, sources of reading materials, and favorite books.

158 159

Restatement of Purposes

The purposes for this study follow:

1. To describe the relationship of factors in the

reading behavior of sixth-grade children.

2. To describe the differences of active and other

sixth-grade readers.

3. To describe the relationship of various factors

in the reading behavior of active readers.

4. To describe the relationship of reading behavior

and classroom grouping of sixth-grade readers.

5. To describe the school, home, and personal

environment of six active readers.

Procedures

The procedure for this investigation fell into three parts. The first was the identification of the active or mature readers. The second was the development of the recording instruments. The third was the collection and analysis of the data.

The children were selected from four schools where a school library staffed with a full-time librarian had been in existence for three years. The schools were in similar suburban communities and assumed to have similar socioeconomic samplings. Children in 11 sixth-grade class­ rooms participated in the study. 160

Subjects for the selected sample of active readers

were identified through the use of four sources. The school

librarians and the sixth-grade teachers in the schools were

given a list of characteristics deemed descriptive of active

readers. They named the sixth-grade children they believed best met these criteria. The third and fourth sources of nominations for the sample were the children's nominations

of themselves or their classmates. After the names were tabulated, children whose name appeared on three or more of the lists were considered "active" readers for purposes of this study.

The major characteristic of the reading behavior under consideration was the range of purposes the children perceived for their reading. Children were asked to record every item they had read and select a purpose for that read­ ing. Measures were collected six times during a three-week period on consecutive Mondays and Fridays. Purpose for reading, as defined by Helen Smith (1966), was "the reading

'set' used to determine that which the reader intended to get from the selection." Five purposes for reading, adapted from Smith's list, were used: having pleasure, meeting assignments, seeking personal interest information, follow­ ing daily events, and answering questions.

Further description of the children readers included a teacher estimate of the children's general performance as 161

students and a standardized measure of reading ability

and mental age.

Six readers reported to be "active" readers were

interviewed to develop a more comprehensive view of active

child readers. Parents and teachers were also questioned

in an attempt to discover .more detailed and unique informa­

tion about the home, school, and personal environments of

the readers.

Finally, a short reading inventory was given to all

the children to secure a broader picture of the sixth-

graders' reading behavior. Questions concerned the amount

of time spent reading, sources of reading materials, and

favorite books.

Four statistical tests were used to analyze the data.

On hypotheses concerning a comparison between boys and girls

or between active and other readers, a two sample jt-test was

used. To analyze questions concerning types and amounts of

reading reported on weekdays and on weekends, the paired

t-test was used. To analyze questions concerning relation­

ships between classrooms, the analysis of variance was run.

Finally, on the hypotheses concerning teachers’ classifica­

tion of students, the chi-square statistic was used.

The responses of parents, teachers, and children were compiled and reported in six case studies. 162

The responses to questions on the interest inventory

were compiled and reported in percentage figures. Children

were grouped by sex and by nomination to the active or other

group.

Limitations of the Study

Intact classroom groupings were used to explain and

to collect the data. The children did not have a choice as

to whether they wished to participate in the study although

every effort was made to secure their cooperation. The

passage of time and repetition of the form may have dis­

couraged complete reporting by the children. The students'

responses were assumed complete, honest diaries of their

weekly reading. During the three-week period, teachers

might have encouraged more reading since the study empha­

sized that curriculum subject.

The definition of "purpose for reading" as used in

this study was an applied interpretation of the term, sim­

plified for the understanding of the subjects. Purposes

or reasons for reading were one aspect of the reading act

that helped to identify a mature reading pattern. The forms

used in this study were developed by the experimenter and had not been subjected to validity and reliability tests.

Children were given a closed response form to report read­

ing purposes. Some children might not have been able to

select a purpose that explained their reasons for reading. 163

The mental age and reading achievement scores of the

active readers were compiled during an earlier period and

could be subject to slight differences in interpretation.

However, the scores were standardized and are generally used

in school systems as measures of children's abilities.

One has to be highly cautious in generalizing these

findings to other populations. This sample included children

from four suburban schools. Caution must be used in attempt­

ing to generalize to other grade levels or populations who

differ from the sample used in this study. Realization by

students and teachers that they were participating in a

research project may have made them react differently than

they might have under normal conditions.

Summary of the Study

For this study, two hundred seventy-six children reported their reading over a three-week period. The collect­ ed data were presented in three sections: general findings, case studies, and inventory data. In the first section, the

21 hypotheses were examined in light of four purposes of the

* • study. Children noted the materials they read and selected the purposes that best described their reading. Comparisons were made between boys and girls and between active and other readers. In the case study section, six children identified as active readers were interviewed. Parents and teachers of the children were questioned also to develop 164

a comprehensive view of their home and school environments.

The gathered data were presented in six case studies. In

the final section, children's responses to an interest in­

ventory were presented. The inventory was given to all 276

children to gain more information about their reading habits.

Responses were reported for the group as a whole, by sex,

and by membership in the active or other group.

General Findings

Four purposes and the 21 hypotheses'*' of the study

were examined in the first section of the analysis of the

data.

One purpose of the study was to describe the rela­

tionship of factors in the reading patterns of the entire

group of sixth-grade readers. This included tests of sig­

nificance for Hypotheses 1-6. There were some significant

differences in the reading patterns of the children. Re­

sults of the paired t-test on Children's weekend and weekday

reading were reported for Hypotheses 3, 4, and 5. The chil­

dren read for significantly more purposes during the week

than on weekends (H^). They also reported reading signifi­

cantly more books (B^) on weekdays and more magazines on weekends (5a). No difference was revealed in the number of

newspapers and comic books read on weekends and weekdays

^H5b and H5c ^ ’

■*The hypotheses appear in Chapter I, pp. 8-10. 165

Differences in boys and girls' reading were tested

for Hypotheses 1, 2, and 6 using the two-sample t-test.

Boys and girls read for the same number of purposes ( H ^ .

However, girls reported reading a significantly greater num­ ber of books than boys (HL,) . Boys read more comic books than girls (Hgc). There was no difference in the amount of magazine and newspaper reading done by the boys and girls

*H 6a and H6b^ * A second purpose of the study was to describe the differences between active and other readers. This section included test of significance for Hypotheses 7-14. Several differences were noted. More girls were nominated to the active reader group than boys (H^). Other comparisons of active and other readers were tested for significance using the two-sample t-test. Children selected as active readers read for more purposes than did other readers (H^)- Active readers also read more books (Hg), reread more books (Hg) , and read more newspapers (H10b^ tlian other readers. There was no difference between the two groups in the amount of

age scores and reading achievement scores of the active readers were significantly higher than for the other readers

(H11 and H12). Finally, teachers classified active readers as above average students in comparison to the performance of other students in the classroom (H^g). 166

Another and third purpose of the study was to

describe the relationship of sex differences in the reading

behavior of active readers. Of the'63 active readers,

forty-one were girls and 22 were boys. Hypotheses 15-18

dealt with this topic.

The boys and girls in the group selected as active

readers reported similar types and amounts of reading. There

was no difference in their perceived purpose for reading

(H15) or in the amount of material they read * Also boys and girls in the active group reread a similar number

of books (H^g).

The fourth purpose of this study was to examine

reading behavior in relation to the classroom grouping of

the sixth-graders. Three hypotheses, 19-21, were generated

concerning classroom groupings. One hypothesis was tested

and there was a significant difference in the number of pur­

poses reported read by the children in the separate class­

rooms (H,„). The amounts of materials read by children in

the various classrooms could not be tested (E^q and EL>^)•

Case Studies

Case studies of six active readers were presented in

the second section of the analysis of the data.

0 There were many similarities in the reading his­ tories of the six children. All of the children could remember their first reading successes. Two of the children 167

learned to read at home hy picking words out of storybooks while the others remembered mastering early reading skills in the first grade. By the time the children were in sec­ ond grade, they were independent and interested readers.

The six children remembered names of teachers who seemed to like books. In some cases, the children could remember the books these teachers had shared with their classes.

These readers had special interests around which much of their reading was centered. Two girls were inter­ ested in a type of literature, biography, while the others reported interests in sports and animals. The children read books and magazines to gain more information about their interests. Although all the children expressed a special interest, they also read many materials in diverse subject areas. They had personal libraries of books, magazine sub­ scriptions, and access to family newspapers and magazines.

The children were regarded as good students and took active roles in the classroom activities.

The home environments of the children had many sim­ ilarities, including easy access to many reading materials.

The mothers and fathers had read to their children when they were younger and provided a time and place for their children to read as they grew older. The parents also bought books as gifts and planned trips to the libraries.

They reported that the children were good helpers at home and were busy with many outside activities. 168

The parents encouraged their children's interest in

reading. They had a positive attitude toward reading and

believed success in reading would benefit their children

in the future. The parents liked to read themselves and

shared their reading with their children. They felt reading

was an important and necessary part of their families lives.

Despite these similarities, there were great differ­

ences among the children in terms of personal characteristics.

Inventory Data

The final section of the analysis of the data

included a report of the 276 children's responses to an

interest inventory. The intent of the inventory was to gain more information about the reading habits of the sixth-

grade children.

The children reported spending nearly twice as much time each day watching television as they spent reading.

Boys watched slightly more television than girls. The average television viewing time for boys was a little over three hours, while the average for the girls was about eight minutes less. Other readers reported spending an average of

35 minutes more each day watching television than active readers.

The children reported reading for an average of one and one-half hours per day. This was one hour and 20 min­ utes less time spent on reading than on television viewing. 169

Girls reported reading an average of 25 minutes more each

day than boys. Active readers read for approximately 30 minutes more each day than did other readers.

In reporting on a variety of past reading experiences,

children named receiving books as gifts more often than any

other of the four choices: using the school library; being taken to the library; being read to by parents or brothers and sisters; or belonging to a bookclub. In all cases, more than 50 percent of the students reported having the exper­ iences . Girls reported a greater amount of past experience than boys except in receiving books as gifts. Active readers reported more past experience with books than other readers.

The greatest difference was in book club membership. Sixty- eight percent of the active readers reported having belonged to book clubs against 51 percent of the other readers.

Children used many different sources to acquire and select reading materials. Six choices were listed: school library, classroom, book club, home, friends, and community libraries. Almost 90 percent of the children reported using the school library as their source of mater­ ials. Boys and girls reported little difference in sources used. The most frequently used sources for boys and girls were the school libraries, the children's homes, and the community libraries. Active and other readers used sources in much the same fashion. The greatest difference in the active and other readers' responses was in sharing books 170 with friends, with half the active readers so reporting

against 27 percent of other readers.

When the children were asked to specify the number of times per week they used school and community libraries, they had five responses available: twice a week, once a week, every two weeks, once a month, or not very often.

In all, more than 84 percent of the children reported using the school library at least twice a month. The figures were reversed for reported visits to the community libraries.

More girls than boys reported two trips per week to the school library. More than 90 percent of the active and other readers reported using the school library at least twice a month. The frequencies of visits to the community librar­ ies for active and other readers were much less.

When asked to note types of materials read every day, children reported using books, newspapers, and maga­ zines more often than other materials. Books were most chosen by girls as their chief choice of daily reading materials; boys put magazines first. Although active and other readers chose books and newspapers as the most common daily reading materials, more active readers than other readers reported this activity.

Presented with a list of 14 series books, the chil­ dren wrote the number of books they had read from each series.

Nancy Drew Mysteries was the series reported read most often. 171

Girls read more Nancy Drew Mysteries than books from any other series. The most widely read series for boys was the

Hardy Boys stories. Active readers read more Nancy Drew and Laura Ingalls Wilder books than other readers. In general, active readers read more series books than other readers.

Children were asked to name their favorite books.

The most frequently mentioned books were Charlie and the

Chocolate Factory and Charlotte's Web. Active raaders also mentioned the Laura Ingalls Wilder Little House books and

A Wrinkle in Time.

When asked to choose their favorite subject, most children selected physical education. However, active readers chose reading as often as physical education.

Discussion

The major purpose of this study was to describe the reading behavior of active, or mature, sixth-grade readers in comparison to other readers in the classroom. Active readers read for a greater range of purposes than other readers. They also read more books and newspapers than their peers. Generally, the active readers were also the better students in the class, having scored significantly higher on standardized mental age and reading achievement tests as well as having received above average ratings from their teachers. 172

Information secured from the case studies and the

inventory showed that active readers spend more time read­

ing and less time watching television than their peers. \ Book club memberships and sharing books with friends were

popular with active readers. Also, these children reported

liking reading as a school subject and going to printed mat­

ter for pleasure and for information.

The findings from this study support earlier research.

On an average, there were five active readers per class­ room, a figure that agrees with the studies of Henderson and Long (1970) and Asheim (1956). According to their find­ ings, active readers seldom make up more than 25 to 30 per­ cent of any group that reflects a cross-section of the total population.

The boys and girls in the active reading group re­ ported similar reading behavior, although there were more girls in this group than boys. Wollner (1949) reported that certain boys read a great deal and this remained true in this study; however, the majority of the active readers were girls. Perhaps as was evidenced in the case study of

Stan's reading, some boys have passed the "reading craze" period suggested by Terman and Lima (1931) and have already become involved in other activities. As the earlier re­ searchers noted, the amount of time spent reading and inter­ est in reading sharply decreases when children enter junior 173

v high schools. The active readers' higher mental age scores reflected earlier findings, by Terman and Lima (1931) and

Lazar (1937) that bright children read more. This conten­ tion could include the higher reading achievement scores of active readers.

According to Gray and Rogers (1956) , a mature read­ er may be described as having a special purpose or special motivation for reading. More importantly, this reader is able to recognize the unique purpose for each item or ar­ ticle that he reads. Helen Smith (1967) noted that good high school readers were also able to recognize purposes for reading. Active sixth-grade readers in this study per­ ceived their reading for a wider range of purposes than other readers. They had a better understanding of the rea­ sons for reading and the possible benefits they could ac­ quire from reading. They saw their reading as more than a limited school subject and were able to relate reading to many of their daily activities. In the case study reports, the two boys noted a special interest, sports, that was motivating their reading. Stan read anything he could find on swimming and Dick kept rereading sports books because he had trouble finding new ones. Two girls, Mary and Jane, had developed an interest in people of other times and other cultures and were trying to relate to these times and places. These subjects had become a focus or radix of 174

interest to which most of their reading related, an aspect

of behavior which is parallel to the pattern of mature

readers discussed by Gray and Rogers (1956).

In comparison to other readers, active readers read

more books and newspapers and reread more books than other

readers. They enjoyed reading and spent mare time reading

than their peers. Asheim (1956) and Gray and Rogers (1956)

noted that time spent reading was a factor in interest in

reading. These children were very highly interested in

reading.

Some of the reading time was spent reading series books. Frank (1960) noted that children find pleasure in

certain books that run in a series. Active readers also

shared books more frequently with their friends and be­ longed to book clubs more often than their peers. They

felt that owning books and sharing books were important parts of their daily activities. The reading interests reported by the children in the case studies were similar to those listed by Betts (1961), with a decided interest

t in series mysteries and series sports stories.

The home and school environments of the active readers reflected an adult interest in printed matter.

Clarke (1956) noted that parents' interests in reading was a factor in children's reading, while Hansen (1967) pointed out that the home environment was a good predictor of reading success. In the home of the six active readers 175 who were interviewed, the parents had a highly positive attitude toward reading and believed tlat reading would help their children in the future. In these homes, books were easily accessible and magazines arrived weekly. The parents read and shared their reading with each other and with their children. In all cases, they had read to the children in earlier years; some mothers were reading to their sixth-grade children.

As with all the active readers in this study, these children received many books as gifts and had been taken to the community libraries at a young age. The parents took an interest in their child's reading and could accurately judge their child's reading ability, a fact that Jefferson

(1958) noted earlier. The six children interviewed were the oldest, youngest, or only children in their families.

Otto (1965) found the oldest or the only child in a family was the better reader but he did not find it to be the same for the youngest member of the family. Perhaps having lived in a very literary home, the youngest child read to keep up with the family interests and discussions.

The children that were interviewed displayed an

"intelligent delight" and an "irresistible compulsion to read" similar to that described by Center (1952). Their response to literature was maturing from a compulsion to read many books to a selective desire to learn more about 176

the world around them. The children were moving through

the reading levels described by Strang (1942) and Wittick

(1960). They were taking an active role in choosing what

they read. These children were bringing an interest in

people and topics to their reading and were interacting with

their reading. This interaction of reader and books is

essential for a response to literature as discussed by

Rosenblatt (1938).

Early (1968) listed descriptors of five stages of

reading. Most active readers appeared to be at the third

stage where they had discovered and read many series books.

Mary moved to Early's fourth stage where she recognized the difference in the quality of books. Also, the active read­ ers remembered teachers who liked reading and encouraged their reading and writing.

The reading patterns of the entire group of sixth- graders varied on weekends and weekdays. Children read for more purposes during the week than on weekends possibly because they were engaged in teacher directed reading during the week. Boys and girls reported similar reading patterns.

The number of books read by the children was greater during the week than on weekends with girls reporting reading more books than boys. The accessibility of books in the school and class assignments were no doubt factors in the number of books read on weekdays. Pader (1968) and Bissett

(1969) reported that easy accessibility to books encouraged 177

reading. Waples and Tyler (1931) included the environment,

sex, and time spent reading as factors that influence read­

ing. In the present study, more girls than boys reported

reading boohs and girls spent more time reading. Both boys

and girls* reported receiving books as gifts although girls

shared books with their friends more often than boys. The

time spent watching television by the boys and girls was

similar to the amount of time reported by Witty (1961)—

about three hours per day. Children reported spending about one and a half hour reading, which was slightly higher than the averages reported by Witty. Since the present study was conducted in a classroom atmosphere, students may have considered more class time in their estimate of daily reading time.

Girls read more series books than boys. Some of this interest in series books doubtless came from the children's parents, who purchased the books as gifts. Also, the girls shared their books with their friends and in this way helped to distribute more series books among their peers. The girls and active readers read a greater number of these books than other readers.

Children did not read more newspapers on weekdays than weekends. The amount of newspaper reading done did not vary with a change in class assignments. Most chil­ dren reported reading the newspaper to keep up with news and sports. A possible explanation was that children saw 178 their parents read the papers on a routine basis and were

following'that pattern. This would be similar to the con­ clusions reached by Waples and Tyler (1931), who believed the environment had an effect upon the amount of reading.

Since there was a small response by the children to comic book reading, the results were not conclusive, although boys reported reading more comic books than girls.

Children used the school libraries as their main source of books, with active readers reporting more than two trips a week to the libraries. This would support

Gaver's (1963) finding that school library programs are important. However, some active readers felt they had to go to other sources for reference materials because they believed the reference section was limited in their school libraries. Children used community libraries less often.

A factor that might have influenced this response was that the community libraries were a great distance from the homes of the children and had to be reached by car.

When the range of purpose category was analyzed by classroom, students in certain classrooms reported reading for a significantly wider range of purposes than children in other classrooms. This finding would be more dependent upon the school than the home environment. The teachers’ methods of helping children discover many reasons for their reading might influence this result. Perhaps the teachers’ 179

presentation of the language arts curriculum or her expec­

tations of the class as a whole encouraged the students to

view their reading as a skill that may be used in many ways.

These teachers offered a variety of activities related to

literature.

Possibilities for Further Study

The major purpose of this study was to describe the

reading behavior of active readers. Comparisons between

active and other readers were cited to discover similar­

ities and differences in the reading behavior of the two

groups. Case study interviews offered more information

about the reading history of six active readers. Finally,

data from an inventory provided more descriptors of the

active readers as well as information concerning favorite

books and series books reported read by the children.

To develop a clear and precise picture of the active

elementary school reader, more research is necessary. The

instruments used in this study may be modified for further

research; the study could be replicated using a different

sample; aspects of the results of this study may be re­

searched more comprehensively; and further research may be

developed from omissions in the procedures or data collect­

ing instruments of the present study. Finally, analysis of

the results of this study provide a basis for general re­ marks concerning children's reading behavior in the elemen­ tary school. 180

Testing and Development of Instruments

The investigative instruments developed for this

study offer a means of identifying and describing active

readers. First, the method developed for selecting active

readers (see Appendix B) is a simplified way of identifying

the more mature readers in the classroom. For this study,

the significant differences between the active and other

groups suggests that this simplified method of nominating

active readers is a viable tool.

Second, the use of the reading record (see Appendix

D) allows for collection of information about the purpose

and variety of reading materials reported read by active

and other readers. This study demonstrated that differences between the responses of the two groups to the reading

record can be measured. Third, the use of the reading

record enabled the researcher to distinguish descriptors of

a group of active readers which may have teaching implica­ tions. Active readers in this sample read for more purposes than other readers. They also read more books and news­ papers than other readers. A list of characteristics of the active readers' reading behavior may aid teachers in enrich­ ing and enlarging the school reading program. xai

Fourth, the descriptions of active readers, with all

their limitations, offer substantiation to earlier attempts

to theorize and to define characteristics of active readers.

The comparisons of responses on the reading record, the

case studies (see Appendix H), and the reading inventory

(see Appendix C) include general and specific aspects of the

profile of active readers' reading behavior. Fifth, the

comprehensive pictures of six active readers developed

through the teacher, parent, and pupil questions and reported

in the six case studies (see Appendix H) provide a framework

for describing the school, home, and personal development

of active readers. Responses to the interviews offer a more

complete picture of the reading history and total environment

of active readers. Finally, the reading inventory (see

Appendix C) may have something to offer for further research

in the study of favorite books and number of series books

read. Although some questions for the inventory instrument

were drawn from conventional sections of such instruments,

the questions concerning favorite books and series books

provide a way to collect information that distinguishes

active and other readers.

In summary, the instruments developed for this study may be used in further research as means to identify and describe characteristics of active readers. The selection procedure, reading record, case study interview questions, 182

reading inventory, as well as combinations of the forms

are workable instruments for identifying and describing different reading behaviors.

Replications of the Study

This study should be replicated with children in different schools and at various grade levels so -the find­ ings may be generalized to a larger population. The repre­ sentative sample should include children from different parts of the city as well as children from suburban comm­ unities. The responses to the reading record could be collected for a longer period of time than was reported in this study. What are the differences in the reading behavior of sixth-grade children in the inner city compared to those of children in suburban communities? How does the reading behavior of active readers change as they grow older? At what grade can a group of active readers be identified? What changes in amount or type of responses are recorded by active and other readers over a six-week or eight-week period? What is the reading profile of an active fourth-grade reader and how does it compare to the reading behavior of a sixth-grader? 183

Extensions of Aspects of the Study

This study presents a way of identifying children's

purposes or reasons for reading. It allows for a more

definitive discussion and investigation of the nature of pur­

pose -for reading. Also, the studj- includes characteristics

from the personal history of active readers that offer poss­

ibilities for further research.

The method used to identify children's purposes for

reading is a way of getting at children's reasons for read­

ing. It is a basic approach and could be modified to include

a more sophisticated interpretation of "purpose for reading."

Children may have more reasons for reading than those sup­

plied on the reading record (see Appendix D). An open-

ended response possibility might encourage more students

to respond.

The personal histories of the active readers reported

in the case studies include information that could be

expanded to offer more detailed information about the chil­

dren' s past school experiences and their past reasons for

reading as well as their current reading behavior. The sim­

ilarities and differences among the active readers could be

researched using a combination of the case study and interest

inventory responses. More information could be gathered

from teachers, parents, and classmates to develop a better and more complete picture of the profile of active readers and their reasons for reading. 184

Sidelights

Other topics of interest that developed during the

present investigation include alternative methods for iden­

tifying active readers; identification of specific types of

books; the function of non-book material; teacher actions

and book use; and parents' attitudes toward reading.

A combination of two instruments could be used to

identify active readers. After the nomination procedure is

completed, one list of active readers would be identified.

A second list could be made from the total number of re­

sponses on the reading record. The list of children who

reported reading for the greatest number of purposes could be compared to the list developed from the nomination pro­

cedure. A comparison of the two lists provides a more

selective way of identifying children for the active reader group.

In the present study, information and pleasure books were considered in one category. Specific differences be­ tween the two types of books could be identified and sup­ plied to the children, so they could categorize their book reading. The number of pleasure and information books read by the children could be compared.

Children read a great deal of non-book material including magazines, newspapers, and comic books. Further research could be done to discover the function of non-book material in the lives of the children. For example, active 185

readers reported having their own magazine subscriptions,

reading family magazines, and finding others in the school

libraries. What part does magazine reading play in the total

reading environment of active readers?

In two classrooms, children read for more purposes

than children in the other classrooms. The teacher influ­

ence may have had a direct bearing on the type and amount of

reading done by the children. Further research is needed to

identify selected teacher behaviors that encourage children

to read.

Finally, parents' attitudes toward reading could be

examined in much greater depth. All the parents of the chil­

dren described in the case studies had a positive attitude

toward reading. It would be interesting to compare the

attitudes of parents of other readers with those of active

readers.

New Foci of Concern

The literature on children's interests is vast and

useful in most descriptive situations. However, it has a

limiting function since it is often interpreted as a defini­

tion of what children should like to read. The interpreta­

tion of the literature on interests often is demonstrated

by giving children what they are supposed to like to read

instead of by encouraging them to investigate reading materials to find out what they want to read. In other 186 words, this method supplies the children with a response before they have time to explore the question.

An investigation into the children's-purposes for

reading supplies more pertinent information since it begins by asking the children to describe what they want to read and their reasons for reading. Purposes for reading can be

^expanded as children are taught the various reasons for and benefits of reading. Reasons for reading can be developed at any age as long as children become aware of the many ways their reading can be adapted for pleasure and for in­ formation.

The need to help children understand the many pur­ poses for reading should be an important part of pre-service education for teachers. They should be continually aware that reading covers the total school program and is not only a skill that has to be taught. Teachers need to realize that children will read individually and independently only when they are aware of the many ways reading can benefit their lives. They need a purpose or reason for reading. APPENDIX A

LIBRARIAN TEACHER NOMINATION FORM

187 APPENDIX A

LIBRARIAN - TEACHER NOMINATION FORM

This study is attempting to identify and describe

the habits of "active" sixth-grade readers.

Such a child . . .

1. Uses the library frequently 2. Has demonstrated the ability to use reference materials 3. Spends spare time reading 4. Chooses reading as an extra curricular event 5. Can suggest good books for his peers 6. Can recite incidents from printed matter or quote passages from books.

In short, these are the children who do a good

deal more than the usual amount of reading.

Would you please list the names of at least five

sixth-grade children who you believe could be described by this definition?

I believe the following sixth-grade children are "active

readers:

188 APPENDIX B

STUDENT NOMINATION FORM

189 APPENDIX B

STUDENT NOMINATION FORM

Here is a list of activities that may describe you or some­

one you know in your classroom. As you read the list, try

to think of students in your classroom who do these things.

This boy or girl . . .

1. Goes to the library a great deal 2. Can help his friends find things they need in the library 3. Spends lots of time reading 4. Sometimes wants to read rather than play 5. Can suggest a good book for other children to read 6. Has read a lot of books 7. Can tell about the stories in books, maga­ zines, and newspapers.

Do you know boys or girls in this room who sound like this

(they read a lot)? If so, list the name(s) here:

Are you such a reader yourself? If you think so, list i your name here:

190 APPENDIX C

MY READING HABITS AND INTERESTS INVENTORY

191 Name

Teacher______

S chool______

INVENTORY: MY READING HABITS AND INTERESTS

1. How much time a day would you say you spend reading?

_____ less than 30 minutes _____ ahout an hour _____ about two hours _____ about three hours _____ more than three hours

How much time a day would you say you spend watching TV?

_____ less than 30 minutes _____ about an hour about two hours about three hours more than three hours

Here are some reading experiences that could happen to children. Check those that would have applied to you when you were younger.

_____ used the school library received books as gifts was taken to the library was read to by parents or brothers and sisters belonged to a book club or ordered paperbacks

4. Would you check the places where you get books to read?

_____ school library _____ classroom _____ book club _____ home _____ from your friends _____ community library

192 193

5. How often do you go to the school library?

once a week twice a week every two weeks once a month __ not very often or never

6. How often do you go to the community library?

, once a week twice a week _ every two weeks once a month not very often or never

7. People read many different kinds of things every day Do you read any of the following kinds of material at least once a day?

magazines books comic books — information books (like encyclopedias) newspapers text books _____ basic readers

8. Her® is a list of series books. Please put down the number of books you have read from each series.

The Bobsey Twins Nancy Drew Mysteries The Hardy Boys Alfred Hitchcock Mysterv Series ____ Sue Barton, Nurse , Nurse Wizard of Oz Paddington Adventures Tarzan ----- Laura Ingalles Wilder's Little House Books Sports Stories by Matt Christopher Maud Lovelace 1s Betsy Tacy Books Caroline Haywood's Eddie Books ----- Walter Brooks' Freddy Books 194

9. Sometimes people like to read books for a second or third time. You may have found that you especially like a book and have read it more than once. If. you have, write the title or titles on the following lines.

10. What is your favorite subject in school?

_____ arithmetic _____ reading _____ music _____ art _____ language _____ science _____ physical education _____ social studies APPENDIX D

READING REPORT RECORD

195 READING REPORT RECORD

Name______Directions: Write down everything you have read School______since the last record. Put the title Teacher______of the article or book in the column Date______that best describes why you read that particular item. Then check the box Girl Boy (Circle One) at the bottom of the column that best describes the type of material you read.

To enjoy the To get infor­ To learn more To keep up To answer a book or mation for about a hobby with news question of article school or special and sports your own or to interest have a new experience

Book Maqazine Newspaper Comic Book Information Book Basic Readers

H vo cr> APPENDIX E

TEACHER ESTIMATION FORM

197 APPENDIX E

TEACHER ESTIMATION FORM

This study is an attempt to describe the various characteristics of a child who reads a lot, an "active" reader, and other children in your classroom. One aspect of the child's performance is relative to this study. It is the child's general performance in the classroom.

Would you please consider each child in your class­ room in relation to this variable? Check whether you be­ lieve the child's performance is average or above average in relation to the other children in your classroom.

The following definition may help you.

General Performance as a Student: The child's general attitudes, abilities, and actions as demonstrated by his ability to adjust and suc­ ceed at assigned and independent tasks.

Average Above Average Names of Children Students Students_____

198 APPENDIX F

PARENTS’ PERMISSION LETTER

199 Dear Parents,

As part of a research project at Ohio State Univer­ sity, I am collecting information to describe the reading patterns and interests of sixth-grade children. The pur­ pose of this study is to present a description of avid and other readers in the sixth-grade. Your child is taking part in this project at school.

To complete the data collection process, it is necessary to record your child’s mental age and reading achievement scores. Your permission is required before such information may be released.

All data are confidential and will be reported anonymously in the final report. By signing this form, you will allow your child's scores to be released only for the purposes of this research.

I would appreciate your cooperation.

Sincerely yours,

Judith M. Sostarich

Parent's Signature

200 APPENDIX G

PARENTS' INTERVIEW LETTER

201 Dear

As part of a research project at Ohio State Univer­

sity, I am collecting information about the reading behav­

ior and interests of sixth-grade students. Your child has

been identified as an active and avid reader.

To secure a complete description of your child's

reading outside of school, I would like to personally ask

you some questions about your child's reading history. The

interview will take no more than twenty minutes and may be

conducted at a time and place most suitable to you.

I would like to assure you that all information is

confidential and will be reported anonymously in the final

report.

I would appreciate your cooperation and will tele­

phone you in the near future to discuss further arrange­ ments .

Sincerely yours,

Judith M. Sostarich

202 APPENDIX H

INTERVIEW QUESTIONNAIRES

203 TEACHER INTERVIEW

Name______Grade Currently Teaching______Last Grade Taught______Number of Years Experience______Highest Degree Held______Did you ever have a Children's Literature course? _____ Yes _____ No If you did, about how many years ago?______

A. Books That You Have Read Aloud This Year

1. What books have you read to your class this year?

2. What books are you planning to read to your class this year?

3. What stories or chapters from books have you read aloud to your students this year?

4. Have you read any children's books to yourself for the first time this year? What were they?

5. Do you have a favorite children's book? What is it?

6 . Do you read poetry aloud to your students?

7. Do you have a favorite poem? What is it?

8. What poetry anthologies do you have in your class­ room?

B . Selection Aids

1. Do you have a classroom collection of books? About how many books are in the collection?

2. In selecting books for purchase and use for class, where did you go to get help and suggestions? Check the one or ones that you used the most.

_____ book displays _____ ideas from basal readers _____ another teacher's recommendation 204 205

_____ librarian _____ Principal _____ Supervisor _____ Special selection aids (Arbuthnot1s Children1s Boobs Too Good to Miss, etc.) If checked, please list: 1. 2 . 3. 4.

_____ Reviews in periodicals. If checked, list name of periodical that you use for book reviews: 1. 2 . 3. 4.

_____ Other sources (please explain)

3. Does your classroom collection change during the year? About how often?

C . Sharing Books and Printed Matter

1. Facing a busy day and a full schedule, do you find time to read aloud to your class? Which statement below describes best your situation on reading aloud to your students?

_____ I try to read daily. I read about _____ min­ utes per day.

_____ I read occasionally, sometimes fairly regu­ larly, sometimes not. I average about _____ minutes a week reading aloud.

_____ I seldom read aloud. Just when there is extra time.

_____ I never read aloud to the class.

2. Below are listed some other techniques that might be used with students. Check the statements that best describe your situation.

Storytelling (Having a repertoire of complete stories for telling to students)

_____ Often, I have an interest in this area. _____ Occasionally, 3 or 4 times a year. 206

_____ Seldom, 1 or 2 times a year. _____ I have not used this particular method.

Book Talks (Taking several books that you are well acquainted with and introducing them to your class)

_____ Often, I try to give book talks on a regular basis. _____ Occasionally, but at least 4-6 times per year. _____ Seldom, 1-3 times per year. _____ I have not used this particular method.

Audio-visual materials (Films, records, etc. for literature purposes)

_____ Often, at least once a month. _____ Occasionally, but at least 4-6 times per year. _____ Seldom, 1-3 times per year. _____ I have not used this particular method.

Other methods

Are there any other ways you share books with children? Please list them.

3. Is there time during the school day or school week for the students to read their own library books? Which statement best describes your situation?

_____ A regular time is provided each day. Approx. ______. _____ Time is provided on a certain day or several days a week. _____ Students may read at free moments and after assignments are completed. _____ Students usually are expected to read the book at home. 207

4. Which statement best describes the student use of the school library?

_____ Students have daily access to the central library. _____ Students may go as a group once a week and at other times when there is a need, and permis­ sion is given. _____ Students may go to the library once a week or once every two weeks. _____ Students cannot go to the central library, the teacher or librarian brings the books to the room.

5. List the ways in which books are made available to your present class.

_____ book club _____ classroom library _____ school library _____ community library

6 . What are some techniques and ways you have used for your pupils to report, to share, and to enjoy creatively their books and stories after they have finished reading them?

7. How do you teach reading skills in your classroom?

D. Use of Books in Instruction

1. Are class assignments given from one or more texts? Please explain.

2. Curriculum in social studies or science often can be taught using different materials. Which approach describes your method? _____ use one basic text as a major reference _____ use more than one text book as a major source _____ use library books as major sources _____ use a majority of pamphlets, magazines, or newspapers as a source of materials no books; experienced based. Many of the subject areas in elementary school can be taught using one of the three following methods. Which method do you believe is most effective in your classroom?

_____ unit based _____ textbook based _____ discrete interest areas

Some teachers believe current events can be used effectively in the classroom. What materials and methods do you use to talk about current events in your classroom? Please explain:

Are major news stories discussed in your class­ room? _____ Yes No If they are, what materials do you believe are essential to this discussion?

Do your children subscribe to a children's news­ paper (example: Weekly Reader) or a magazines as a class? _____ Yes _____ No If yes: What methods do you believe are most successful in using the newspaper in your class­ room? Please explain. 209

What subject areas do you believe lend themselves to the most varied amount of materials?

_____ arithmetic______music _____ physical education _ social studies _____ language _____ reading _____ science _____ art

8. What subject do you enjoy teaching the most?

_____ arithmetic _____ science reading _____ music _____ physical education _____ art _____ social studies _____ language

9. Do you have interest areas in your classroom? _____ Yes _____ No If yes: What areas do you have?

If yes: What are the most popular areas? HOME INTERVIEW

Your child has shown an active interest in reading. The purpose of this study is to describe the reader's purposes and motivations for reading. Part of this purpose includes a description of the reader's home environment as it re­ lates to his reading.

All information will remain anonymous in the final report­ ing and none of it will become part of your child’s record.

Could you please answer the following inventory to help describe your child's home reading environment?

Name of child______Father's Occupation______Father's highest grade attended in school______Mother's highest grade attended in school______Ages of all children in the family______

Of the children we have, just mentioned, we are going to talk about ______. We will probably be referring to the others on occasion but our discussion will be mostly about______.

1. How does______usually do in school?

2. How much does read outside of required schoolwork? How many hours does he/she read for recreation during the week? Do you consider this adequate? Do you encourage more reading? Is there a regular time set aside for his/her reading? How regularly is this followed?

3. Do you expect him/her to read any number of books monthly? How many would you be satisfied with? Do you force him to read? What kinds of books do you encourage him to read?

210 211

The following questions are aimed at getting an assessment of the materials available in the household related to children's reading.

4. What newspapers or magazines do you subscribe to? Do you encourage your child to read them? If so, how? Do you discuss articles or stories in his presence? Does he participate or mainly listen?

5. Does your child have a library card? For what library? When did he get it? Who git it for him or did he get it for himself? Did anyone go with him on his first visit? Who? Where else does he obtain reading material?

6 . How is your child at completing jobs on time, helping around the house, or picking up after him? Does he do these on his own?

7. How would you describe him/her as a reader? What do you think about his reading?

8 . Has your child always spent a lot of time reading? When did his reading become noticeable? Has he read more at one time of the year than another? Has he read more at one age than at another? How would you describe the history of your child's reading?

9. Has your child shown a prolonged interest in some topic or some type of reading materials? If so, what are they?

10. How do your other children like independent reading?

11. Did you read books to your child when he was younger? At what age did you stop? Do you still read to him? How regularly did you read to him? Does he read to you? Who else read to the child? Can you remember any of his favorite stories? What do you think is your child's favorite book?

12. Does your child bring homework from school? How much time is spent on homework daily? Do you help him? How often? Have you always helped him? 212

13. Is there any time when you talk about what your child reads? Is there any time when your child initiates a discus­ sion about his reading? Do you ever initiate these discussions? How often? For what purpose? . How do you follow-up on discussions?

14. About how many hours on weekdays does your child watch television? What are his favorite programs? Do you approve of ;the number; of hours? What do you think of.his selections? When .you do not approve, what do you do?

15. Did you like to read as a child? Can you remember any.books you read as a child? What were they?

16. What is different about the way your child reads com­ pared to the way you read?

17. What are your favorite television programs? Have you recommended any programs to your child in the past two weeks? If so, which ones? Do you discuss any programs with your child after watching?

18. How often does your child read comic books? Do you see any value in them?

19. Do you give books as gifts to your children? How often? Do you give books as gifts to persons outside of your family? Are the children involved in the selection of these gifts? Do you encourage giving books as gifts?

20. What effect do you think liking to read will have on your child's future career? What are you doing to encourage this?

21. Do you think reading has been important to you and your husband's life and work? How?

22. Can you name any books your child has read in the past month? 213

23. How do you describe yourself as a reader?

24. How do you describe your husband as a reader?

25. Who reads more, you or your husband?

26. Is there any advice you would give other parents who want to encourage their children to read? STUDENT INTERVIEW

The purpose of this talk is to try to find out more about you and your reading. Many people here at school think you are a good reader. I'd like to know more about you and more about your interest in reading. Would you please answer these questions as honestly as you can?

1. Do you like to read? Why?

2. Can you remember when you started to like to read?

3. Can you remember what grade you were in when you really started to read a lot on your own?

4. Do you remember the first book you read all by your­ self? What was the name of the book? Did you like the story? Why do you think you remembered it?

5. Do you have a favorite book? What is it? Why is it your favorite? How did you find that book to read?

6 . What is the last book you read? Why did you choose to read it? What did you think about it? Would you read it again? Would you suggest it to a friend?

7. What magazines do you read? Where do you get the magazines to read? Do you have a favorite magazine? What is it? Why is it your favorite?

8 . What comic books do you read? Where do you get the comic books you read? Do you have a favorite comic book hero? Who is it? Do your friends read comic books? What is their favorite comic book hero?

214 215

9. Do you read the newspaper? What parts of it do you read? Why do you read the paper? Do your friends read it?

10. Have you ever read a book more than once? What was it? Why do you think you read it again? Would you recommend it to a friend?

11. Can you remember any teacher who really seemed to like books and read a lot to you? What was her name? What grade did she teach?

12. Can you remember the names of any books teachers have read to you?

13. Did your parents read to you when you were younger? Do you have a favorite story you share with your parents? Did your brothers or sisters ever read to you? Are you sharing a book with your family right now?

14. Are there any topics that really interest you, like space, baseball, knitting, etc.? What have you read on that topic?

15. Do you have a friend that likes to read? Do you have a brother or sister that likes to read? Do you know anybody who likes to read as much as you do?

16. Have you had an interest in a topic that has lasted a long time? What is it? When did you first get interested in it? What have you read about it?

17. What do your brothers and sisters read? Do they share what they read with you? How?

18. Do you talk about what you read at home? When?

19. Do you have a special place you go when you want to read? Where is it?

20. Where would you look if you had to do a report on something special in science (example: insects) for school? Would you have information like that at home? Where would you look at school? 216

21. Do you have a library card? Do you use the library? Why?

22. What is your favorite television show? What would you guess would be the favorite television show of your friends?

23. What is your favorite subject in school? Has it always been your favorite? APPENDIX I

REPORTS FROM THE SCHOOLS

217 REPORTS FROM THE SCHOOLS

I. Introduction

Name - Sostarich

Favorite Subject?

I'm going to talk about the one many people often ask about— reading.

This is different. Let's not just talk about books. What else do you read? What else could you read?

MAKE LIST ON BOARD.

II. Explanation of Study

What does research mean? (Investigation) We're looking into or investigating things that children read. This is not any kind of test because there are no right or wrong answers. In fact, one reason for doing research is to try to find or discover answers.

Your teachers have said that you are pretty good readers. They know you and have said that you will help us find some of these research answers.

What we need to know is what you read. Simple isn't it? Everything you write down on these forms will be kept confidential. Does anybody know what that word means?

For example, I may tell the teachers that five of you read Sports Illustrated, but I won't say which five or how many times you read it. I hope that by the time this is through, we'll be friends. You are really helping me out by being honest in your answers to these forms.

218 219

III. Reading Record

We'll do this form a few times in the next few weeks. Maybe you could be thinking more about the times you read. READ DIRECTIONS ALOUD. Next time, why don't you try to think up a really different or unusual thing you could read. We'll talk about it next time. Perhaps you could tell us about something funny you read.

IV. Student Nomination Form This form is a little different. You already know all the people in this class. Some are good ath­ letes, some are good spellers, or actors, or math­ ematicians. Since we're talking about reading, who do you think are good readers. READ FORM ALOUD. BE SURE THEY UNDERSTAND. Look around the room, maybe it'll remind you of someone else who you think reads a lot. If you think you read a great deal, be sure to write your own name on the last line.

V. Interest Inventory

This is the last form for today. In this part of the research, we are just trying to find out what an average sixth-grade reader is like. Put down what you read— SOME ANSWERS YOU CAN CHECK MORE THAN ONE BE SURE TO LOOK. Be sure your name^is on the paper and that you have done all four pa^es.

Thank you— I'll see you later this week. 220

READING RECORD REPORTS November 2 , 1973

I 'm backI

Name - Miss Sostarich.

Reading, any reading.

What is the usual reading you do during a school week? What are some of the things you read this week?

Did anyone read anything unusual or different?

Think about the reading you have done this week as you fill out this form.

READ DIRECTIONS ALOUD.

Remember: this is a confidential form, so please be as complete and honest as you can.

COLLECT.

We'll do this a few more times; maybe you can think of the kinds of reading you do this weekend.

READING RECORDS November 5, 1973 Monday

Good morning (afternoon)1

Did you have a nice weekend? play ball? listen to football games? rake leaves? do some sewing or cooking?

I'm back because I'd like you to record what you read over the weekend. It should be easier to do this time since you know the form and since you have only two days to remember.

Just put down what you read--even a cereal box or a score card; I realize since it was a weekend and only two days you might not have as much to write as you had on Friday, Remember this is confidential.

Thanks for doing such a good job on the forms last week. I appreciate your help on this research. 221

READING RECORDS November 9, 1973

Good Morning!

I hope you have had an interesting week in school. Have any of you done something you thought was really different or interesting?

As you know, I'm especially interested in what you read this week. I'm sure you'll have more information than on Monday since you were here in school this week.

The form we're going to fill out today is the same one. Please try to remember everything you have read. I appreciate your cooperation with this research.

READ DIRECTIONS ALOUD

COLLECT

READING RECORDS November 12, 1973 Monday

Good Morning (afternoon) i

I hope that you all had a nice weekend. Did anyone do something that they would like to tell us all about? Any camping trips this weekend? I know I enjoyed the football game. Did you see it?

Like last week, I'd like you to record what you read over the weekend. You only have to remember what you read for the last two days.

I realize since it was a weekend and only two days since the last report that some of you might not have as much to write down as you had on Friday. Just write down what you read.

Thank you for your help with this research. 222

READING RECORDS November 16, 1973

Good Morning (afternoon)I

I bave come for the last time j find out about your read­ ing. Like I said last week, I know that you do different kinds of reading during the week than you do at home. What have you been studying at school that you need to read to do? Do you have a favorite topic that you have been study­ ing lately?

Please fill out the forms completely. DON'T FORGET TO PUT YOUR NAMES ON THE PAPERS I

I want to thank you all for your help with this study. I appreciate you taking the time to do this. I'll be back sometime later to tell you, in general, how the study came out. Have a nice day. A P P E N D I X J

TABULATION OP FAVORITE BOOKS OR BOOKS

REPORTED REREAD BY CHILDREN

223 224

Bov Girl Active Other Total

AQC's Wide World of Sports

Abe Lincoln

Adventures in Forgotten Valley

Adventures of Huck Finn

All of a Kind Family

All of a Kind Family Uptown

Alvin's Secret Code

Amy and Laura

Andy Buckrem's Tin Men

Angie

Annie Oakley

April's Witches

Arabian Knights

Are You There God, It's Me Margaret

Arrow Book of Sports

Babe Ruth

Baked Beans for Breakfast

Bambi

The Baseball Trick

The Bear's House

Best Friend

Betsy-Tacy

Black Beauty 225

Bov Girl Active Other Total

The Black Stallion Series 2 2 2

Bob Turley 1 1 1

Born Free 2 1 3 3

The Box Car Children 1 1 1

Bread and Butter Journey 1 1 1

Brian's Song 2 2 2

Buffalo Knife 1 1 1

By the Banks of Plum Creek 1 1 1

Call it Courage 2 1 3 3

Call of the Wild 1 1 1

The Cay 1 1 1

Casey Stengal 1 1 1

Catcher with a Glass Arm 2 2 2

Charles Darwin’s Origin of 1 1 1 the Species (abridged)

Charlie and the Chocolate 7 13 5 15 20 Factory

Charlie and the Glass 1 1 1 Elevator

Charlotte's Web 3 16 3 16 19

Chitty Chitty Bang Bang 2 2 2

The City of Lead and Gold 1 1 1

Clifford the Dog 2 2 2

Clue in the Crumbling Wall 1 1 1

The Cronicles of Narnia 1 1 1

David Livingston 1 1 1 226

Bov Girl Active Other Total

Deadline at Spook Cabin 1 1 1

Dr. Jeckle 1 1 1

Don't Take Teddy 1 1 1

Ellen Tebbits 3 3 3

Encyclopedia Brown 1 1 1

Escape to Witch Mountain 1 1 1

Freddie Plays Football 1 1 2 2

Freddie the Detective 1 1 2 2

Fifteen 2 1 1 2

The First and the Last 1 1 1

The First Four Years 2 2 2

Five Chinese Brothers 2 2 2

Flower Treasure 1 1 1

Follow My Leader 3 6 2 7 9

Football Boys 1 1 1

From the Mixed Up Files 1 1 1 of Mrs. Basil E. Frank- weiler

Ghosts, Ghostsj Ghosts 1 1 1

Ghost Rock Mystery 1 1 1

Ghost Town Treasure 2 2 2

Go Ask Alice 1 2 1 2 3

The Great Airport Mystery 1 1 1

Great Moments in Pro Foot­ 1 1 1 1 ball

Green Ghost 1 1 1 227

Bov Girl Active Other Total

The Hamster Hunt 1 1 1

Hard Drive to Short 1 1 1

Harriet the Spy 1 1 1

Heidi 2 2 2

Heidi Grows Up 1 1 1

Henry Huggins 2 2 2

Henry Reed 2 2 2

H e ’s Your Dog, Charlie 2 2 2 Brown

Hidden Staircase 1 1 1

Higglity, Pigglity, Pop! 1 1 1

The Hollisters 1 1 1

Homer Price 1 1 1

The House Mother 1 1 1

The Incredible Journey 1 1 1

Inventors of Medicine 1 1 1

Irma's Big Lie 1 1 1

Island of the Blue Dolphins 2 2 2

It's For You, Snoopy 1 1 1

J .T. 1 1 1

James and the Giant Peach 3 6 4 5 9

Japan 1 1 1

John Paul Jones 1 1 1

John Williams, Printer 1 1 1 and Comic 228

Bov Girl Active Other Total

Julie of the Wolves 1 1 1

Junk Day on Jupiter Street 1 1 1

Katy John 1 1 1

King of the Grizzlies 1 1 1

Knight's Castle 1. 1 1

Lassie 1 1 _ 1

Laura Ingalls Wilder 2 5 5 2 7 Series

Laura's Luck 1 1 1

Life Among Savages 1 1 1

Lincoln's Birthday 1 1 1

Little House in the Big 3 1 2 3 Woods

The Little Leftover Witch 1 1 1

Little Men 2 1 1 2

The Little Seashore 1 1 1

Little Turtle 1 1 1

Little White Foot 1 1 1

Little Women 7 3 4 7

Lisa and Lottie 1 1 1

Lisa Bright and Dark 1 1 1

The Long Winter 1 1 1

Lorna Doone 1 1 1

Lou Gehrig, Boy of 1 1 1 Sandlots

Louis Braille, Seeing 1 1 1 Fingers 229

Boy Girl Active Other Total

Love Bug

The Mad Adventures of Capt. Klutz

The Magic Finger

The Magic Tunnel

Man in the Box

The Man Who Never Was

Matt Christopher Series

Margurite Henry Horse Books

The Melted Coins

Mickey Mantle

Mischievous Meg

Miss Pipa

Misty of Chincoteague

More All of a Kind Family

The Mouse and the Motor­ cycle

Mr. Fox

Mr. Mysterious and Company

Mr. Popper's Penguins

My Side of the Mountain

Mysterious Bender Bones

Mystery at Black Rock Cave

Mystery of the Hidden Treasure 230

...Boy, GirlActive Other Total

Mystery of the Tower Treasure

Mystery of the Velvet Room

Mystery Over the Brick Wall

North to Freedom

Old Yeller

On the Banks of Plum Creek

One Hundred Dresses

One Hundred Pounds of Popcorn

One Hundred and One Dalmations

Panda Bears

Peanuts Treasure

Phio Rizzoto

Pippi Longstockings

Planet of the Apes

Planets, Stars, and Space

A Pony Called Lightning

Rabbit Hill

Red Trailer Mystery

Reptiles of North America

Return to Hiroshima

Ribsy 231

Bov Girl Active Other Total

Right Wing 1 1 1

Robin Hood 1 1 1

Scrub Dog of Alaska 1 1 1

Scott Corbet Trick Book 1 1 1

Sea Star 1 1 1

Secret Mirror Bay- 1 1 1

Secret of the Lost Tunnel 1 1 1

Seven Heroes 1 1 1

Shadow of a Bull 1 1 1

Sherm Bloody Murder 1 1 1

A Short Season 1 1 1

Signpost to Switzerland 1 1 1

Sinister Signpost 1 1 1

Sitting Bull 1 1 1

Snakes 1 1 1

The Snake That Went to 1 1 1 School

Snow Dog 1 1 1

Sounder 2 2 4 4

Star Quarterbacks of 1 1 1 the NFL

Star Runningbacks of 1 1 1 the NFL

Steal the Way Home 1 1 1

Strange But True Foot­ 1 1 1 ball Stories 232

Boy Girl Active Other Total

Strike From the Sky 1 1 1

Striped Ice Cream 1 1 1

Strongbox Mystery 1 1 1

Stuart Little 4 2 1 5 6

Sue Barton, Senior Nurse 1 1 1

Summer of the Swans 2 2 2

Swamp Fox 1 1 1

Swiss Family Robinson 1 2 3 3

Tarzan of the Apes 1 1 1

This Dear Bought Land 1 1 1

This Stranger, My Son 2 1 1 2

The Three Firecrackers 1 1 1

Thursday's Child 1 1 1

Tomboy 1 1 1

Tom Swift Jr. and His 2 2 2 Outpost in Space

Touchdown for Tommy 1 1 1

Tower Treasure 1 1 1

Trouble River 1 1 1

The Trouble with Jenny's 1 1 1 Ear

Trumpet of the Swans 4 2 2 4

Turtles 1 1 1

Twelve Ghostly Stories 1 1 1

Twenty-one Balloons 1 1 1 233

Bov Girl Active Other Total

Two Weeks to Winning Chess 1 1 1

Underground Railroad 1 1 1

Understanding Betsy- 1 1 1

War in the Pacific 1 1 1

Whales 1 1 1

What the Witch Left 1 1 1

What's New Lincoln? 1 1 1

When the Dike Broke 1 1 1

White Mountains 1 1 1

The White Stallion 1 1 1

Wild Country 1 1 1

Wind in the Willows 2 2 2

Wizard of Oz 1 3 1 3 4

The World's Greatest 1 1 2 2 Athlete

Wright Brothers 1 1 1

A Wrinkle in Time 3 6 5 4 9

Yellow Eyes 1 1 1

Your Heart and How it Works 1 1 1

Zero the Pox in the City 1 1 1 BIBLIOGRAPHY

Adler, Mortimer J. How to Read a Book: The Art of Getting A Liberal Education. New York: Macmillan Co., 1942.

Allan, T. L., and MacDonald, Catherine. "Recreational Reading and the Reluctant Reader." Teaching Eng­ lish. IV (May, 1971), 3-5.

Amatora, S. M., and Edith, S. M. "Children's Interest in Free Reading." School and Society. LXXIII (March, 1951), 134-37.

Arbuthnot, May Hill. "When Teachers Read, Children Read." Instructor. LXXII (November, 1952), 82-84.

Artley, A. Sterl. "Literature in the Language Arts Pro­ gram." Readings on Contemporary English in the Elementary School. Edited by Iris M. Tiedt and Sidney W. Tiedt. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc., 1957, pp. 214-19.

Ashley, L. F. "Children's Reading Interests and Individ­ ualized Reading." Elementary English. XLVII, No. 8 (December, 1970), 1088-95.

Asheim, Lester. "What Do Adults Read?" Adult Reading. 55th Yearbook, Part II, NSSE. Chicago: Univer­ sity of Chicago Press, 1956, pp. 5-28.

Berstein, Margery R. "Relationship Between Interest and Reading Comprehension." Journal of Educational Research. XLIX (December, 1955) , 283-88.

Betts, Emmett A., and Welch, Carolyn M. The ABC Teacher's Guide: Adventures Mow and Then; Betts Basic Readers. New York: G. C. Merrian Co., 1961.

234 235

Bissett, Donald J. "The Amount and Effect of Recreational Reading in Selected Fifth Grade Classes." Unpub­ lished Ph.D. Dissertation, Syracuse University, 1969.

. "The Usefulness of Children's Boohs in the Read­ ing Program." Paper presented at IRA Conference, Kansas City, Missouri, April 30-May 3, 1969.

Bfoening, Angela Marie. Developing Appreciation Through Teaching Literature. Boston, Massachusetts: John Hopkins University, 1929.

Broom, M. E. "A Study of Literary Appreciation." Journal of Applied Psychology. XVIII (1934), 357-63.

Burgdorff, Arlene B. "A Study of the Ability of Inter- mediate-Grade Children to Draw Inferences from Selections of Children's Literature." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Ball State University, 1966.

Carrello, Lawrence W. "The Relation of Certain Environ­ mental and Developmental Factors to Reading Ability in Children." Dissertation Abstracts. 17: 1252.

Carroll, Herbert A. "Appreciation of Literature and Ab­ stract Intelligence." Journal of Educational Psychology. XXV (January, 1934), 54-57.

______. "A Method of Measuring Prose Appreciation." English Journal. XXLL (1933), 184-89.

Center, Stella S. The Art of Book Reading. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons, 1952.

Choppin, Bruce. "Can Literary Appreciation Be Measured Objectively?" International Review of Education. XV (1969), 241-47.

Cianciolo, Patricia Jean. "The Library and the Reading Program." Education Digest. XXXIII (January, 1968), 43-45.

Clarke, Peter. A Study of Children's Reading Behavior. Washington, D.C.: Office of Education (DHEW) Bur­ eau of Research, March, 1969.

Cleary, Florence. "Why Children Read." Wilson Library Bulletin. XIV (October, 1939), 119-26. 236

Cleland, Donald. "Why Should I Read?" Paper presented at the College Reading Association Conference, Boston, Massachusetts, March 13-15, 1969.

Cooper, Charles R. "Measuring Appreciation of Literature: A Review of Attempts." Research in the Teaching . of English. V (1971), 5-23.

______. "Preferred Modes of Literary Response: The Characteristics of High School Juniors in Relation to the Consistency of their Reactions to Three Dissimilar-Shopt Stories." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, University of California at Berkeley, 1969.

Cornell, Ethel L. "The Voluntary Reading of High School Pupils." American Library Association Bulletin. XXXV (May, 194]) 295-300.

Dale, Edgar. "Reading-One Way to Communicate." Bureau of Educational Research Newsletter, XVIII, Novem­ ber, 1952. Columbus, Ohio: The Ohio State Univer­ sity.

Dulin, Kenneth. "The Professionally-Oriented Reader." Paper presented at National Reading Conference, Tampa, Florida, December, 1971.

Early, Margaret J. "Stages of Growth in Literary Appre­ ciation." English Journal. XLIX (March, 1960), 161-66-

______, jet jil. Teacher's Edition of First Splendor. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, Inc., 1968.

Eddings, Inez Clark. "Patterns of Reading Growth: A Longitudinal Study of Patterns of Reading Growth Throughout the Six Grades in Two Elementary Schools." Dissertation Abstracts. 17 (06): 1253.

Emans, Robert, and Patyk, Gloriav "Why Do High School Students Read?" Journal of Reading. X (1967), 300-04.

Fader, Daniel, and McNeil, Elton B. Hooked on Books: Pro­ gram and Proof. New York: Berkeley Publishing Corporation, 1968.

Fox, Maude Greene. "An Experiment in Promotion Interest in Reading." Elementary School Journal. XLVII (April, 1947), 451-60. 237

Frank, Josette. Your Child's Reading Today. New York: Doubleday and Company, 1960.

Gaver, M. Effectiveness of Centralized Library Service in Elementary Schools. New Brunswick, New Jersey: Rutgers University Press, 1963.

Getzels, Jacob W. "The Nature of Reading Interests, Psy­ chological Aspects." Developing Permanent Interest in Reading. Edited by Helen Robinson. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1956, pp. 5-9.

Gray, William S. . "Reading." Encyclopedia of Educational Research. Edited by Chester W. Harris. New York: MacMillan Company, 1960, 00. 1086-1135.

______, and Munroe, Ruth. The Reading Interests and Habits of Adults. New York: MacMillan, 1939.

______, and Rogers, Bernice. Maturity in Reading: Its Nature and Appraisal. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1956.

Hansen, Harlan Stuart. "Relationship Between the Home Literary Environment and Self-Commitment to Inde­ pendent Reading." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1967.

Havighurst, Walter, ,et jal. Teacher's Notes for Exploring Literature. Boston, Massachusetts: Houghton Mifflin Company, 1968.

Henderson, Edmund H. "A Study of Individually Formulated Purposes for Reading." Journal of Educational Research. LVII (1965), 438-41.

______, and Long, Barbara H. A Study of the Independent Reading of Achieving Fifth Graders. Virginia University, Charlottesville. Washington, D.C.: Office of Education (DHEW) Bureau of Research, 1970.

Huck, Charlotte. "Literature's Role in Language Develop­ ment." Childhood Education. XLII (1965), 147-50.

______. "Planning the Literature Program for the Ele­ mentary School." Elementary English. XXXIX (April, 1962), 307-09.

_, and Kuhn, Doris. Children's Literature in the Elementary School. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 1961. 238

Hughes, M., and Willis, P. "Personal Reading. A Study of the Seventh Grade." On Becoming a Reader. Pro­ ceedings of the Claremont Reading Conference, XXIX, 1965, pp. 90-101.

Hunnicutt, C. W. "Reading of Children in Activity and' Regular Schools in New York City." Elementary School Journal. XLIII (May, 1943), 530-38.

Huus, Helen. a. "Developing Interest and Taste in Liter­ ature in the Elementary Grades." Reading as an Intellectual Activity. Edited by J. Allen Figurel. IRA Conference Proceedings, VII, 1963, pp. 46-50.

______. b. "Interpreting Research in Children's Liter­ ature, Children, Books, and Reading." Perspectives in Reading #3. Newark, Delaware: IRA, 1964, pp. 123-45.

______. c. "Reading Interests." Readings on Reading Instruction. Edited by Albert J. Harris and Edward R. Sipay. New York: David McKay Company, Inc., 1963. "An Inventory of Reading Attitude." Improving Reading Instruction. Monograph 4 . San Diego County Depart­ ment of Education, California, 1961.

Iverson, William J. The Role of the Teacher in Developing Lifetime Readers. New York: MacMillan, 1967.

Jacobs, Leland B. "Goals in Promoting Permanent Reading Interests." Developing Permanent Interest in Reading. Edited by Helen Robinson. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1956, pp. 20-25.

Jan-Tausch, E. "Discovery and Measurement of Interests in Reading." IRA Conference Proceedings. XIII, 1970, pp. 116-25.

Jefferson, Benjamin F. "Some Relatiohships Between Parents and Children's Preferences in Juvenile Literature." Elementary School Journal. LVIII (January, 1958), 212-18.

Jordon, Arthur M. Children's Interests in Reading. Con­ tributions to Education, No. 107. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1921.

Kennedy, John J. The Chi-Square Statistic: Training Paper. Columbus, Ohio: The Ohio State University, 1970. 239

Ketcham, C. A. "Factors in the Home Background and Reader Self-Concept Which Relate to Reading Achievement." Proceedings of the College Reading Association. Edited by C. A. Ketcham, VII, 1966, pp. 66-68.

LaBrant, Lou, and Heller, Frieda M. An Evaluation of Free Reading in Grades 7 to 12 Inclusive. Columbus, Ohio: Ohio State University Press, 1939.

Lawson, Hoyle D. "Effects of Free Reading on Reading Achievement of Sixth-Grade Pupils." Dissertation Abstracts, 25 (11): 6340-41.

Lazar, Mary. Reading Interests. Activities, and Opportun­ ities of Bright. Average, and Dull Children. Contributions to Education, No. 707. New York: Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University, 1937.

Lehman, Harvey, and Witty, Paul. "Sex Differences in Ref­ erence to Reading Books, Just for Fun." Education. LXVIII (June, 1928), 602-12.

Link, Henry C., and Hopf, Harry A. People and Books. A Study of Reading and Book-Buying Habits. New York: Book Manufacturers' Institute, 1946.

McCaul, Robert. "The Effects of Attitudes Upon Reading Interpretation," Journal of Educational Research. XXXVII (February, 1949), 451-57.

Miller, Lewis Jerold. "Fostering Interest in Children's Literature: Selected Teachers Practices and Com­ petencies." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Indiana University, 1969.

Monson, Dianne L. "Children's Responses to Humorous Sit­ uations in Literature." Unpublished Ph.D. Disser­ tation, University of Minnesota, 1966.

Morris, William P. "Unstructured Oral Responses of Exper­ ienced Readers Reacting to a Given Poem." Disser­ tation Abstracts, 31: 5573-6767.

"NAEP: Literature Objectives." National Assessment of Educational Progress. Denver, Colorado, -1971.

Norvell, George W. What Boys and Girls Like to Read. New York: Silver, 1958. 240

Otto, W. "Sibbling Patterns of Good and Poor Readers." Psychology in the Schools. I (1965), 53-57.

Peifer, John Edgar. "The Development of an Attitude Scale to Measure Students1 Attitudes Toward Reading in the Secondary Schools." Dissertation Abstracts. 23 (10): 3696.

Peltola, Bette J. "A Study of Children's Booh Choices." Elementary English, XL (1963), 690-95.

Pfou, Donaly. "Effects of Planned Recreational Reading Programs." The Reading Teacher. XXI (October, 1967), 34-39.

Piekarz, Josephine A. "Getting Meaning From Reading." Elementary School Journal. LVI (March, 1956), 303-09.

Powell, William R. "Classroom Libraries: Their Frequency of Use." Elementary English. XLIII (April, 1966), 395-97.

Purves, Alan C., and Beach, Richard. Literature and the Readers: Research in Response to Literature, Reading Interests, and the Teaching of Literature. Urbana, Illinois: NCTE, 1972.

Rankin, Marie. Children's Interests in Library Books of Fiction. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University, 1944.

Richards, I. A. Practical Criticism. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World, 1929.

Ring, Jerry W. "A Study of the Interpretive Processes Employed by Selected Adolescent Readers of Three Short Stories." Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, The Ohio State University, 1968.

Robinson, Helen M., .et _al. Vistas Guidebook Book 5 . Glenview, Illinois: Scott, Foresman and Company, 1965.

Roderick, J. "Some Relationships Between Creativity and the Reading Preferences and Choices of a Group of Sixth Graders." Educational Leadership. XXVI (October, 1968), 49-52. 241

Rogers, Charlotte D. "Individual Differences in Interpre­ tive Responses to Reading the Short Story at the Eleventh Grade Level.” Dissertation Abstracts. 25 (11): 6318.

Rosenblatt, Louise M. Literature as Exploration. New York: Noble and Noble, 1938.

Sebasta, Sam Leaton. "Developmental, Interest, and Read­ ing Levels." Evaluating Books for Children and Young People. Edited by Helen Huus. Newark, Dela­ ware: IRA., 1968, pp. 16-26.

Shores, J. Harlan. "Reading Interests and Informational Needs of Children in Grades Four to Eight." Ele^> mentary English,XXXI (December, 1954), 493-500.

Shulte, E. S. "Independent Reading Interests of Children in Grades 4, 5, and 6." International Reading Association Conference Proceedings. I, 1969, pp. 728-32.

Smith, Dora. "The Goals of the Literature Period and the Grade Sequences of Desirable Experiences." Improv­ ing Reading in All Curriculum Areas. Supplementary Educational Monographs No. 76. Chicago: Univer­ sity of Chicago Press, 1952.

Smith, Helen K. Instruction of High School Students in Reading for Different Purposes. University of Chicago, Chicago, Illinois. Washington, D.C.: Cooperative Research Program of U. S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, 1966.

______. "The Response of Good and Poor Readers When Asked to Read for Different Purposes." Reading Research Quarterly. Ill (Fall, 1967), 421-32.

Walker, Clare Connoly. "An Evaluation of Two Programs of Reading in Grades IV, V, and VI of the Elementary School: A Study of the Basal-Text Program in the Intermediate Grades to Determine Comparative Growth in Reading Attitudes, Habits, and Skills." Disser­ tation Abstracts, 19 (02): 256.

Waples, Douglas, and Tyler, Ruth. What People Want to Read About. Chicago: American Library Association and The University of Chicago Press, 1931.

, et al. What Reading Does to People. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1940. 242

Weekes, Blanche E. The Influence of Meaning on Children's Choices of Poetry. Contributions to Education, No. 354. New York: Bureau of Publications, Tea­ chers College, Columbia University, 1929.

Weintraub, Samuel. "Children's Reading Interests." The Reading Teacher. XXII (April, 1969), 655-59.

Williams, E. D., et al. "Tests of Literary Appreciation." British Journal of Educational Psychology. VIII (1938), 265-84.

Wittick, Mildred Letton. "Conclusions Concerning Sequen­ tial Development of Reading Abilities in Grades Four Through Six.” Sequential Development of Reading Abilities. Supplementary Educational Mono­ graphs No. 90. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960, pp. 225-27.

______. "Sequential Development in Reading Interests and Tastes." Sequential Development of Reading Abilities. Supplementary Educational Monographs No. 90. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960, pp. 150-56.

Witty, Paul. "Studies of Children's Interest— A Brief Summary III.” Elementary English, XXXVIII (January, 1961), 33-36.

______, and Kinsella, Paul. "Children and TV— A Ninth Report." Elementary English. XXXV (November, 1958), 450-56.

Wollner, Mary H„ B. Children's Voluntary Reading as an Expression of Individuality. Contributions to Education, No. 944. New York: Bureau of Publica­ tions, Teachers College, Columbia University, 1949.

Zais, Robert. "A Scale to Measure Sophistication of Reading Interests." Journal of Reading. XII (1969), 273-76.