Kin Selection and the Evolutionary Theory of Aging

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Kin Selection and the Evolutionary Theory of Aging ANRV328-ES38-05 ARI 24 September 2007 8:51 Kin Selection and the Evolutionary Theory of Aging Andrew F.G. Bourke School of Biological Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich, Norfolk NR4 7TJ, United Kingdom; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2007. 38:103–28 Key Words First published online as a Review in Advance on kin conflict, life history, longevity, senescence, social evolution July 3, 2007 The Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Abstract Systematics is online at http://ecolsys.annualreviews.org Researchers are increasingly recognizing that social effects influence the evolution of aging. Kin selection theory provides a framework This article’s doi: 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.38.091206.095528 for analyzing such effects because an individual’s longevity and mor- tality schedule may alter its inclusive fitness via effects on the fitness Copyright c 2007 by Annual Reviews. All rights reserved of relatives. Kin-selected effects on aging have been demonstrated both by models of intergenerational transfers of investment by care- by UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS - CHICAGO on 06/05/08. For personal use only. 1543-592X/07/1201-0103$20.00 givers and by spatially explicit population models with limited dis- persal. They also underlie coevolution between the degree and form Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2007.38:103-128. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org of sociality and patterns of aging. In this review I critically examine and synthesize theory and data concerning these processes. I pro- pose a classification, stemming from kin selection theory, of social effects on aging and describe a hypothesis for kin-selected conflict over parental time of death in systems with resource inheritance. I conclude that systematically applying kin selection theory to the analysis of the evolution of aging adds considerably to our general understanding of aging. 103 ANRV328-ES38-05 ARI 24 September 2007 8:51 INTRODUCTION The evolutionary theory of aging provides an explanation for aging based on natural Aging (senescence): selection. The theory proposes that aging occurs because extrinsic mortality reduces decrease in an individual’s the relative size of cohorts of older individuals, causing the potential genetic contri- performance, survivorship, bution of older cohorts to future generations to fall. Therefore, any gene increasing or fecundity as age survival or fecundity is more strongly selected for when its phenotypic effects oc- increases, occurring as a result of intrinsic changes cur at younger ages, and conversely, any gene decreasing survival or fecundity is less strongly selected against when its phenotypic effects occur at greater ages. As a result, Kin selection: natural selection of genes for social age-specific phenotypic effects tend to evolve whereby genes for increased survival or actions via the sharing of fecundity have an effect at younger ages and genes for decreased survival or fecundity these genes between the have an effect at greater ages, so explaining the occurrence of aging. The evolutionary actor and its relatives theory of aging was initially formulated in the mid-twentieth century as an amalgam of contributions from R.A. Fisher, J.B.S. Haldane, P.B.Medawar, G.C. Williams, and W.D. Hamilton, with the last three authors having made the major contributions (reviewed in Charlesworth 2000, Rose 1991). The early theorists also identified two principal genetic routes to aging. The first, antagonistic pleiotropy, proposes that aging stems from selection of pleiotropic genes having a positive effect on survival and fecundity early in life and a negative effect late in life. The second, mutation ac- cumulation, proposes that aging stems from lack of selection against genes of purely negative effect, where these effects occur only late in life (e.g., Hughes & Reynolds 2005, Kirkwood & Austad 2000). A major area of research has developed on the basis of the evolutionary theory of aging (e.g., Arking 2006, Austad 1997a, Carey 2003, Charlesworth 1980, Finch 1990, Rose 1991), with more derived versions of the original theory having been con- structed to account for various complicating factors (e.g., for the case when extrinsic mortality reduces population density and so increases resource availability for older age classes; Abrams 1993). The theory has also been integrated with the general study of life history evolution because finite resources entail the occurrence of trade-offs between somatic maintenance and reproduction (Barnes & Partridge 2003, Kirkwood 1977), and because the age at first reproduction represents the critical period beyond which aging is predicted to occur (Charlesworth 1980, Rose 1997, Stearns 1992). Overall, empirical work has provided strong support for the evolutionary theory of aging from several sources. These include comparative analyses of life histories as by UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS - CHICAGO on 06/05/08. For personal use only. a function of the strength of extrinsic mortality and experimental manipulations of schedules of reproduction (e.g., Hughes & Reynolds 2005, Kirkwood & Austad 2000, Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2007.38:103-128. Downloaded from arjournals.annualreviews.org Rose 1991, Stearns 1992), although results from some studies have proved more con- sistent with derived versions of the theory than with the original theory (Reznick et al. 2004, Williams et al. 2006). Related studies have provided better support for antagonistic pleiotropy than for mutation accumulation as genetic routes to aging (e.g., Campisi 2005, Hughes & Reynolds 2005, Kirkwood & Austad 2000, Leroi et al. 2005, Partridge & Barton 1993, Partridge & Gems 2002a, Rose 1991), with some phenomena not readily reconcilable with either route (Mitteldorf 2004). Another major body of evolutionary theory, kin-selection theory (Hamilton 1964, Lehmann & Keller 2006, Michod 1982, West-Eberhard 1975), has sought to explain 104 Bourke ANRV328-ES38-05 ARI 24 September 2007 8:51 the evolution of social behavior as a function of the genetic relatedness of interacting individuals. Kin selection theory is based on Hamilton’s rule (Grafen 1985, Hamilton 1964), which states that a social action is naturally selected when the sum of rb and c Relatedness: probability exceeds zero, where r is the relatedness of the interactants, b is the change in offspring that two individuals share a number of the recipient of the act, and c is the change in offspring number of the gene as the result of kinship social actor. Although controversial in some of its applications (Alonso & Schuck- Social action: an action by Paim 2002, Wilson & Holldobler¨ 2005), kin selection is widely accepted as a powerful one individual (the actor) and robust explanation for various forms of social behavior across many taxa (e.g., affecting the survivorship or Bourke 2005, Emlen 1995, Foster et al. 2006, Trivers1985). These forms include kin- offspring output of another selected conflict, whereby individuals within social groups differ in their relatedness individual (the recipient) to the group’s progeny, and hence in their fitness optima, leading to potential within- Inclusive fitness: in kin group conflict (Ratnieks & Reeve 1992, Trivers 1974). A central tenet of the theory is selection theory, the fitness of a focal individual that an individual’s fitness (its inclusive fitness) depends upon its own offspring output incorporating effects of its plus its effects (through social actions) on the offspring output of relatives (Hamilton social actions toward 1964). A key prediction is that altruism (entailing lifetime reproductive costs) cannot relatives evolve unless interactants are related (Hamilton 1964, Lehmann & Keller 2006). Altruism: social action in The evolutionary theory of aging, including its derived versions, has traditionally which the actor experiences considered patterns of aging with reference to an individual’s fitness evaluated in the a loss in survivorship or absence of social effects (e.g., Hughes & Reynolds 2005, Rose 1991). However, kin offspring output and the recipient a gain selection theory predicts that social effects will influence patterns of aging because a focal individual’s longevity and mortality schedule may affect the fitness of other individuals. If the interacting individuals are relatives, the focal individual’s inclusive fitness will be altered, and hence patterns of aging in the focal individual will, in principle, be subject to kin selection. Recently, researchers have incorporated social and kin-selected effects into evolutionary models of aging (e.g., Lee 2003, Travis 2004), albeit implicitly in some cases. In a related development, researchers have argued that social organisms exhibit a complex, coevolutionary relationship between the trajectory of social evolution and patterns of aging (e.g., Alexander et al. 1991, Carey & Judge 2001). Nonetheless, patterns of aging stemming from social effects have not been systematically considered in the synthetic, overarching framework provided by kin selection theory. Applying this framework is an important exercise because it allows social hypotheses of aging to be classified and rigorously assessed. It also reveals hitherto underappreciated possibilities, such as the occurrence of potential kin-selected conflict over the timing of death and aging. In addition, explorations of by UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS - CHICAGO on 06/05/08. For personal use only. coevolution between sociality and aging have tended to proceed independently of one another. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2007.38:103-128.
Recommended publications
  • Kin Selection Definition Otherwise Known As Inclusive Fitness Theory
    KinSelection Definition Otherwiseknownasinclusivefitnesstheory,kinselectionreferstothetheorythatpeople haveevolvedtofavorotherswhoaregeneticallyrelatedtothem.Thelogicofthetheoryisthata genecanpropagateitselfthroughtworoutes.Thefirstisbyincreasingthelikelihoodthatbody inwhichitresides(theself)willsurviveandreproduce(e.g.,byleadingtotheselectionof nutritiousfoodsandfertilemates).Thesecondisbyincreasingthereproductionofclose relatives(kin)whoalsopossesscopiesofthesamegene(e.g.,byleadingtheselftohelpkinin waysthatincreasethechancesthattheywillreproduceandthegenewillbepassedon).Someof yourkinaremorecloselyrelatedtoyouthanothersandthereforearemorelikelytocarryyour genes.Thus,becauseyoushare50%ofyourgeneswithyoursiblingsbutonly12.5%withyour cousins,youshouldbemuchmorelikelytohelpsiblingsthancousins.Accordingtothetheory ofinclusivefitness,parentalcareforoffspringisaspecialcaseofkinselection,asitisyet anothercaseofpeople(oranimals)providingcareforcloselyrelatedkinwhocarryshared geneticmaterial. HistoryandModernUsage Thetheoryofkinselectioniswidelyregardedasthemostimportanttheoreticaldevelopment inevolutionarythinkingsinceDarwin,asitproposesamechanismthatexplainswhyindividuals wouldaltruisticallyhelpothers(thatis,whytheywouldprovideresourcestosomeoneelseata costtothemselves).Theideaofaltruismseemscounter-intuitivefromaDarwinianperspective, asanybehaviorthatincreasesthelikelihoodthatanotherindividualwillsurviveorreproduceat acosttoone’sownsurvivalorreproductionshouldbeselectedagainst.Butifthisaltruistic behaviorenhancesthesurvivalorreproductionofarelatedindividualtoagreaterdegreethanit
    [Show full text]
  • Gene-Culture Coevolution, Group Selection, and the Evolution of Cooperation
    EC_2018_A12 Gene-Culture coevolution, group selection, and the evolution of Cooperation The Evolution of Cooperation How can altruism / cooperation evolve? 1 EC_2018_A12 Levels of Selection "although a high standard of morality gives but a slight or no advantage to each individual man and his children over the other men of the same tribe (...) an advancement in the standard of morality will certainly give an immense advantage to one tribe over another.” (C. Darwin, Descent of Man, 1871) Levels of Selection Individuals (“basic” [Neo]Darwinism) Genes (“Selfish-gene” Sociobiology) Groups? Multilevel selection? Higher-level adaptations? Genetic Group Selection? “Naïve group selectionism”: The probability of survival of individual living things, or of populations, increases with the degree with which they harmoniously adjust themselves to each other and to their environment. This principle is basic to the concept of the balance of nature, orders the subject matter of ecology and evolution, underlies organismic and developmental biology, and is the foundation for all sociology. (Allee et al. 1949) “The good of the species” (Wynne-Edwards) 2 EC_2018_A12 Levels of Selection Migration, genetic drift, etc: Intergroup effects weaker than intragroup, interindividual selection. Intra x intergroup differences X Wilson DS & Wilson EO (2007) Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology Multi-level selection/ limits in kin selection theory/ “major transitions” Eusociality: Kin Selection X Individual selection + preadaptations. (communal nests) Nowak, Tarnita & Wilson, “The Evolution of Eusociality”, Nature 2010 (X Abbot et al [+100!], Nature 2011) “Major Transitions” in Evolution Maynard Smith & Szathmáry 1997 “Apart from the evolution of the genetic code, all these transitions involve the coming together of previously independent replicators, to cooperate in a higher-level assembly that reproduces as a single unit.” 3 EC_2018_A12 Natural selection & the evolution of cooperation Cooperation is needed for evolution to construct new levels of organization.
    [Show full text]
  • |||GET||| Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Development 2Nd Edition
    EVOLUTIONARY PERSPECTIVES ON HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 2ND EDITION DOWNLOAD FREE Robert L Burgess | 9780761927907 | | | | | Evolutionary developmental psychology In David M. His research interests are in social and cultural factors affecting human growth and Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Development 2nd edition evolution of the human growth pattern. Debra Lieberman similarly objected to the characterization of evolutionary psychology as ignorant of developmental principles. Kevin MacDonald. Pub date Apr O'Rourke Editor. DeWitte, and James W. Already purchased in store? Biological Reviews. Undetected location. Stress and Human Biology Gillian H. Developmental Psychology. Leonard 8. Adaptation Altruism Coevolution Cultural group selection Kin selection Sexual selection Evolutionarily stable strategy Social selection. By continuing to use this site you consent to receive cookies. Comparing and integrating approaches Further Reading References. Some authors argue that childhood environment and early life experiences are highly influential in determining an individual's life history strategy. Undetected location. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Theodore Steegmann, Jr. Book Add to list Added to list. Nature versus nurture Morphogenetic field. More information Less information icon angle. Would you like to change to the site? Thomas; Ellis, Bruce J Evolutionary Psychology. Human Nutritional Evolution William R. Edition: 2 Edited by: Robert L. Retrieved Evolutionary Why a species evolved the structures adaptations it has. Developmental plasticity and evolution. Clark Description Index About the author Evolutionary theory is Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Development 2nd edition of the most wide- ranging and inspiring of scientific ideas. Clark Barrett have refuted claims that mainstream evolutionary psychology neglects development, arguing that their discipline is, in reality, exceptionally interested in and highly considerate of development.
    [Show full text]
  • Persistent Misunderstandings of Inclusive Fitness and Kin Selection Park, Justin H
    University of Groningen Persistent misunderstandings of inclusive fitness and kin selection Park, Justin H. Published in: Evolutionary Psychology DOI: 10.1177/147470490700500414 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2007 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Park, J. H. (2007). Persistent misunderstandings of inclusive fitness and kin selection: Their ubiquitous appearance in social psychology textbooks. Evolutionary Psychology, 5(4), 860 - 873. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470490700500414 Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). The publication may also be distributed here under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license. More information can be found on the University of Groningen website: https://www.rug.nl/library/open-access/self-archiving-pure/taverne- amendment. Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.
    [Show full text]
  • Altruism Researchers Must Cooperate Biologists Studying the Evolution of Social Behaviour Are at Loggerheads
    COMMENT PHYSICS How the media COLLECTIVES Leadership EXHIBITION New show EVOLUTION Responses to misconstrued Steven tips learned from house- highlights 300 years of recent reappraisal of kin Hawking’s latest book p.657 hunting bees p.658 science in Berlin p.660 selection p.661 Altruism researchers must cooperate Biologists studying the evolution of social behaviour are at loggerheads. The disputes — mainly over methods — are holding back the field, says Samir Okasha. ast month, 30 leading evolutionary now calling for a radical rethink, arguing that I contend that there is little to argue about. biologists met in Amsterdam to discuss kin selection is theoretically problematic, and Much of the current antagonism stems a burgeoning controversy. The question has insufficient empirical support, and that from the fact that different researchers are PARKINS D. Lof how altruistic behaviour can arise through alternative models better account for the evo- focusing on different aspects of the same phe- natural selection, once regarded as settled, is lution of social behaviour2. Others regard kin nomenon, and are using different methods. In again the subject of heated debate. selection as solid, and the rethink as unneces- allowing a plurality of approaches — a healthy The question dividing biologists is the sary and potentially retrograde. thing in science — to descend into tribal- degree to which inclusive fitness theory, or kin Rival camps have emerged, each endors- ism, biologists risk causing serious damage selection, explains the evolution of altruism ing a different approach to social evolution. to the field of social evolution, and potentially — in which an animal provides a benefit to Heated exchanges have occurred at confer- to evolutionary biology in general.
    [Show full text]
  • Kin Selection Is the Key to Altruism
    Update TRENDS in Ecology and Evolution Vol.21 No.2 February 2006 57 but is necessary, given the fact that ants, bees and wasps 10 Tallmon, D.A. et al. (2004) The alluring simplicity and complex reality include some of the most beneficial insects on Earth and of genetic rescue. Trends Ecol. Evol. 19, 489–496 provide crucial ecosystem services, such as pollination. 11 Beye, M. et al. (1999) Unusually high recombination rate detected in the sex locus region of the honey bee (Apis mellifera). Genetics 153, Thus, their extinction could have a widespread impact. 1701–1708 12 Woyke, J. (1963) What happens to diploid drone larvae in a honeybee References colony? J. Apic. Res. 2, 73–75 1 Hedrick, P.W. et al. (1996) Directions in conservation biology: 13 Castric, V. and Vekemans, X. (2004) Plant self-incompatibility in comments on Caughley. Conserv. Biol. 10, 1312–1320 natural populations: a critical assessment of recent theoretical and 2 Hedrick, P.W. (2001) Conservation genetics: where are we now? Trends empirical advances. Mol. Ecol. 13, 2873–2889 Ecol. Evol. 16, 629–636 14 Young, A.G. et al. (2000) Genetic erosion, restricted mating and 3 Caughley, G. (1994) Directions in conservation biology. J. Anim. Ecol. reduced viability in fragmented populations of the endangered 63, 215–244 grassland herb: Rutidosis leptorrhynchoides.InGenetics, Demo- 4 DeMauro, M.M. (1993) Relationship of breeding system to rarity in the graphy and Viability of Fragmented Populations (Young, A.G. and lakeside daisy (Hymenoxys acaulis var. glabra). Conserv. Biol. 7, Clarke, G.M., eds), pp. 335–359, Cambridge University Press 542–550 15 Garrigan, D.
    [Show full text]
  • Kin Selection Raymond Hames University of Nebraska - Lincoln, [email protected]
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Anthropology Faculty Publications Anthropology, Department of Summer 8-2015 Kin Selection Raymond Hames University of Nebraska - Lincoln, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/anthropologyfacpub Part of the Archaeological Anthropology Commons, Biological Psychology Commons, Social and Cultural Anthropology Commons, and the Social Psychology Commons Hames, Raymond, "Kin Selection" (2015). Anthropology Faculty Publications. 128. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/anthropologyfacpub/128 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Anthropology, Department of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Anthropology Faculty Publications by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. Published (as Chapter 19) in Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, Second Edition, edited by David M. Buss, pp 505-523. Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Used by permission. digitalcommons.unl.edu Kin Selection Raymond Hames University of Nebraska-Lincoln Introduction When Hamilton (1964) published his theory of inclusive fitness it had no immediate im- pact in the social and behavioral sciences, even though ethnographers knew kinship to be a universally fundamental factor in human social organization, especially in egalitarian so- cieties in which humans have spent nearly all their evolutionary history. In many ways, it was a theory that perhaps anthropologists should have devised: Anthropologists knew kinship fundamentally structured cooperation, identity, coalition formation, resource ex- change, marriage, and group membership in traditional societies.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Sociobiology: Inclusive Fitness and the Core Genome Herbert Gintis
    An Introduction to Sociobiology: Inclusive Fitness and the Core Genome Herbert Gintis June 29, 2013 The besetting danger is ...mistaking part of the truth for the whole...in every one of the leading controversies...both sides were in the right in what they affirmed, though wrong in what they denied John Stuart Mill, On Coleridge, 1867 A Mendelian populationhas a common gene pool, whichis itscollective or corporate genotype. Theodosius Dobzhansky, Cold Springs Harbor Symposium, 1953. The interaction between regulator and structural genes... [reinforces] the concept that the genotype of the individual is a whole. Ernst Mayr, Populations, Species and Evolution, 1970 Abstract This paper develops inclusive fitness theory with the aim of clarifying its appropriate place in sociobiological theory and specifying the associated principles that render it powerful. The paper introduces one new concept, that of the core genome. Treating the core genome as a unit of selection solves problems concerning levels of selection in evolution. 1 Summary Sociobiology is the study of biological interaction, both intragenomic, among loci in the genome, and intergenomic, among individuals in a reproductive popula- tion (Gardner et al. 2007). William Hamilton (1964) extended the theory of gene frequencies developed in the first half of the Twentieth century (Crow and I would like to thank Samuel Bowles, Eric Charnov, Steven Frank, Michael Ghiselin, Peter Godfrey-Smith, David Haig, David Queller, Laurent Lehmann, Samir Okasha, Peter Richerson, Joan Roughgarden, Elliot Sober, David Van Dyken, Mattijs van Veelen and Edward O. Wilson for advice in preparing this paper. 1 Kimura 1970, B¨urger 2000, Provine 2001) to deal with such behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • The Natures of Universal Moralities, 75 Brook
    Brooklyn Law Review Volume 75 Issue 2 SYMPOSIUM: Article 4 Is Morality Universal, and Should the Law Care? 2009 The aN tures of Universal Moralities Bailey Kuklin Follow this and additional works at: https://brooklynworks.brooklaw.edu/blr Recommended Citation Bailey Kuklin, The Natures of Universal Moralities, 75 Brook. L. Rev. (2009). Available at: https://brooklynworks.brooklaw.edu/blr/vol75/iss2/4 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Law Journals at BrooklynWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Brooklyn Law Review by an authorized editor of BrooklynWorks. The Natures of Universal Moralities Bailey Kuklin† One of the abiding lessons from postmodernism is that reason does not go all the way down.1 In the context of this symposium, one cannot deductively derive a universal morality from incontestible moral primitives,2 or practical reason alone.3 Instead, even reasoned moral systems must ultimately be grounded on intuition,4 a sense of justice. The question then † Professor of Law, Brooklyn Law School. I wish to thank the presenters and participants of the Brooklyn Law School Symposium entitled “Is Morality Universal, and Should the Law Care?” and those at the Tenth SEAL Scholarship Conference. Further thanks go to Brooklyn Law School for supporting this project with a summer research stipend. 1 “Simplifying to the extreme, I define postmodern as incredulity toward metanarratives.” JEAN-FRANCOIS LYOTARD, THE POSTMODERN CONDITION: A REPORT ON KNOWLEDGE xxiv (Geoff Bennington & Brian Massumi trans., 1984). “If modernity is viewed with Weberian optimism as the project of rationalisation of the life-world, an era of material progress, social emancipation and scientific innovation, the postmodern is derided as chaotic, catastrophic, nihilistic, the end of good order.” COSTAS DOUZINAS ET AL., POSTMODERN JURISPRUDENCE 16 (1991).
    [Show full text]
  • Sociobiology Is a Controversial New Field of Study, Defined by Its Most
    28 SELFiSH GENES, AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR: A BlO-CULTURAL CRITIQUE Jon Marks Sociobiology is a controversial new field of study, defined by its most prolific spokesman as "the systematic study of thebiological basis of all social behavior" (Wilson 1975a:k). Although much excellent work has been done in this field within the areas of entomology and ornithology, the application of sociobiology to humans (Wilson 1975a:5k7ff;Wilson1975b; Hamilton 1975;etc.)has generated considerable acrimonious debate (Caplan 1978), This paper represents an attempt to present sociobiology and social anthropology fairly, and to evaluate the central arguments of sociobiology within a synthetic framework of biology and social anthropology. My purpose in this paper is to explore the foundations upon which human sociobiology is constructed; to demonstrate that human sociobiology is not so much a more scientific approach to anthropology as itis a novel philosophical ap- proach; and to evaluate critically the value of such an approach in the study of human behavior. The Genesis of Sociobiology Classical evolutionary works tended to see natural selection, and thus evolution occurring primarily with respect to the individual: the original definition of natural selection was "this preservation of indivi- dual differences and variations, and the destruction of those which are injurious" (Darwin 1962 [18721:91). However, selection also operated at higher levels: "Natural selection. ..will adapt the structure of each indi- vidual for the benefit of the community;if the community profits by the selected change" (Darwin 1962 [18721:96), With the introduction of genetics Marks 29 into evolutionary theory, this posed a problem. Evolutionary geneticists viewed evolution in terms of altered gene frequencies across generations: however, those genes are expressed in individuals, ft is the individual (i.e., the phenotype) which is exposed to the rigors of the environment, resulting in different fitnesses between individuals.
    [Show full text]
  • The Morality of Evolutionarily Self-Interested Rescues
    Brooklyn Law School BrooklynWorks Faculty Scholarship 2006 The orM ality of Evolutionarily Self-Interested Rescues Bailey Kuklin Follow this and additional works at: https://brooklynworks.brooklaw.edu/faculty Part of the Law and Philosophy Commons, and the Torts Commons Recommended Citation 40 Ariz. St. L. J. 453 (2008) This Article is brought to you for free and open access by BrooklynWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Scholarship by an authorized administrator of BrooklynWorks. THE MORALITY OF EVOLUTIONARILY SELF- INTERESTED RESCUES Bailey Kuklint Introduction ................................................................................................ 453 I. The Rescue Doctrine and Evolutionary Psychology ............................ 456 A . "Peril Invites R escue" ................................................................... 456 B. Evolutionary Psychology ............................................................... 457 1. Kin Selection ............................................................................ 458 2. R eciprocal A ltruism ................................................................. 459 3. Sexual Selection ....................................................................... 466 C. Evolutionary Behavioral Maxims .................................................. 469 II. M orality of R escue ............................................................................... 473 A . U tilitarianism .................................................................................. 477
    [Show full text]
  • Kin Selection and Sexual Selection
    Downloaded from rstb.royalsocietypublishing.org on July 16, 2012 Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B (2012) 367, 2314–2323 doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0281 Review The sociobiology of sex: inclusive fitness consequences of inter-sexual interactions Tommaso Pizzari1,* and Andy Gardner2,3 1Edward Grey Institute, Department of Zoology and 2Department of Zoology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3PS, UK 3Balliol College, University of Oxford, Broad St., Oxford OX1 3BJ, UK The diversity of social interactions between sexual partners has long captivated biologists, and its evolution has been interpreted largely in terms of ‘direct fitness’ pay-offs to partners and their des- cendants. Inter-sexual interactions also have ‘indirect effects’ by affecting the fitness of relatives, with important consequences for inclusive fitness. However, inclusive fitness arguments have received limited consideration in this context, and definitions of ‘direct’ and ‘indirect’ fitness effects in this field are often inconsistent with those of inclusive fitness theory. Here, we use a sociobiology approach based on inclusive fitness theory to distinguish between direct and indirect fitness effects. We first consider direct effects: we review how competition leads to sexual conflict, and discuss the conditions under which repression of competition fosters sexual mutualism. We then clarify indirect effects, and show that greenbeard effects, kin recognition and population viscosity can all lead to episodes of indirect selection on sexual interactions creating potential for sexual altruism and spite. We argue that the integration of direct and indirect fitness effects within a sociobiology approach enables us to consider a more diverse spectrum of evolutionary outcomes of sexual interactions, and may help resolving current debates over sexual selection and sexual conflict.
    [Show full text]