The Role of Attributions in Stereotype Threat Effects: Female Achievements in STEM Domains

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Role of Attributions in Stereotype Threat Effects: Female Achievements in STEM Domains DePaul University Via Sapientiae College of Science and Health Theses and Dissertations College of Science and Health Summer 8-17-2012 The Role of Attributions in Stereotype Threat Effects: Female Achievements in STEM Domains Caitlyn Yantis DePaul University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://via.library.depaul.edu/csh_etd Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Yantis, Caitlyn, "The Role of Attributions in Stereotype Threat Effects: Female Achievements in STEM Domains" (2012). College of Science and Health Theses and Dissertations. 14. https://via.library.depaul.edu/csh_etd/14 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the College of Science and Health at Via Sapientiae. It has been accepted for inclusion in College of Science and Health Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Via Sapientiae. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE ROLE OF ATTRIBUTIONS IN STEREOTYPE THREAT EFFECTS: FEMALE ACHIEVEMENT IN STEM DOMAINS A Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science BY CAITLYN ANNE YANTIS JULY, 2012 Department of Psychology College of Science and Health DePaul University Chicago, Illinois ii THESIS COMMITTEE Christine Reyna, Ph.D. Chairperson Ralph Erber, Ph.D. iii VITA The author was born in Baltimore, Maryland on August 30, 1988. She graduated from Loch Raven High School in 2006 and received her Bachelor of Science degree from the University of Mary Washington in 2010. TABLE OF CONTENTS Thesis Committee . ii Vita . iii List of Tables . iv List of Figures . v CHAPTER I. INTRODUCTION . 1 Stereotype Threat. 3 Stereotype Threat Contingencies . 4 Causal Explanations for Stereotype Threat Effects . 6 Attributions . 7 Attributions and Performance . 8 Attributions and Persistence . 9 Attributions and Self-Efficacy . 10 Attributions and Stereotypes . 10 Attributional Model of Stereotypes . 12 Rationale . 14 Statement of Hypotheses . 17 CHAPTER II. METHOD . 21 Procedure . 21 Domain Identification . 21 Attribution Manipulation . 22 Self-Efficacy . 23 Task Performance . 23 Exit Survey . 25 CHAPTER III. RESULTS . 27 Performance . 28 Persistence . 31 Self Efficacy . 34 Domain Identification . 36 Additional Analyses . 38 CHAPTER IV. DISCUSSION . 45 CHAPTER V. SUMMARY. 53 References . 53 Appendix A. Hypotheses . 61 Appendix B. Domain Identification Measure. 63 Appendix C. Attribution Manipulation. 65 Appendix D. Manipulation Check. 70 Appendix E. Demographics Questionnaire. 72 Appendix F. State Self-Esteem Scale . 74 Appendix G. Logical Reasoning Test . 76 Appendix H. Exit Survey . 84 iv List of Tables Table 1. Means and standard deviations of number of correct, attempted problems by group . 29 Table 2. Frequencies of number of correct, attempted problems by group . 30 Table 3. Means, standard deviations, and frequencies for number of problems attempted during persistence . 33 Table 4. Means and standard deviations for self efficacy pre- and post-test . 35 Table 5. Means and standard deviations for domain identification pre- and post-test . 37 Table 6. Correlations between attribution endorsement and achievement outcomes . 42 Table 7. Correlations between attribution endorsement and achievement outcomes (all participants included) . 44 v List of Figures Figure 1. Performance ratios by condition . 30 Figure 2. Performance ratios by condition (after square root transformation) . 31 Figure 3. Desire to persist by condition . 33 Figure 4. Pre- and post-test self efficacy by condition . 36 Figure 5. Pre- and post-test domain identification by condition . 37 Figure 6. Participants’ personal attributions for logical reasoning by condition . 40 Figure 7. Post-test attributions (all participants included) . 44 1 CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION The underrepresentation of women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) is a familiar problem, but the reasoning behind this gender gap is the subject of ongoing study. Although the gender gap in STEM subjects among students has been closing (Buchmann & DiPrete, 2006; Hyde, Lindberg, Linn, Ellis, & Williams, 2008), discrepancies remain on high-stakes exams such as the Scholastic Aptitude Test, Graduate Record Exam, and Advanced Placement tests. On these tests, males out- perform females consistently in science and mathematics (Halpern et al., 2007; Lubinski & Benbow, 2008). It is under the pressure and anxiety of such major diagnostic evaluations that male and female students begin to separate, with women falling behind. These gender differences on critical exams are mirrored in the continuing gender gap in college major selection. Although women are more likely than men to earn a college degree (Buchmann & DiPrete, 2006), there has been little change in female participation in STEM areas, where males typically dominate (Legewie & DiPrete, 2011). Specifically, men are significantly more likely to select engineering, computer science, and mathematics as a college major than are women (Peter & Horn, 2005). Gender differences in college major selection also translate to fewer women in STEM careers. For example, women only make up 5.9% of mechanical engineers and 29.5% of environmental scientists (US Department of Labor, 2010). This pattern holds in academia as well, where 92% of engineering professors and 87% of computer science professors are male (National Science Foundation, 2004). What are the driving forces behind this ongoing gender disparity? 2 One explanation for this continuing gender gap is a cultural environment in which gender stereotypes are particularly pervasive and influence not only perceptions of male and female ability in STEM areas, but also students’ performance in these domains. The stereotype that “math is male”, for example, has recently been shown to be acquired as early as second grade (Cvencek, Meltzoff, & Greenwald, 2011). Not only did young students associate math with male, but female elementary students had a much weaker identification with math than did males even at this early age. Studies such as this one indicate that the gender stereotypes related to STEM domains are understood very early and may then contribute to performance on high-stakes exams as well as interest in STEM fields later in life, where STEM gender stereotypes persist (Nosek & Smyth, 2011; Nosek et al., 2009). The internalization of such gender stereotypes can have severe consequences that lead to disengagement from the domain. In order to preserve self- esteem, it is common for individuals to selectively place less value on domains for which their group is stereotyped (Crocker & Major, 1989; Nieva & Gutek, 1981). Therefore, the pervasiveness gender STEM stereotypes can lead to disengagement from an early age, resulting in the gender disparities observed throughout secondary education and in the work force. In general, stereotypes can be debilitating in terms of performance and are influential in the gender STEM problem. Specifically, the phenomenon of stereotype threat can be strong enough to inhibit performance in several different domains, including important diagnostic tests such as the SAT and GRE that students pursuing these fields must take. 3 Stereotype Threat Stereotype threat is defined as “the concrete, real-time threat of being judged and treated poorly in settings where a negative stereotype about one’s group applies (Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002, p. 389).” Feeling threatened by a salient stereotype about one’s group can hinder performance in different domains compared to when no stereotype threat is present. Stereotype threat effects have been observed with various social groups and with many different skills. For example, athletic performance of White participants was impaired compared to Blacks when a sports task was defined as diagnostic of natural ability (Stone, Lynch, Sjomeling, & Darley, 1999). In this case, White participants were threatened by the negative stereotype that Whites as a group are less skillful in athletics than are Blacks, which subsequently interfered with their ability to perform well. Useful skills for the workplace are also affected by stereotype threat. Men’s ability to decode nonverbal cues showed impaired performance when they were told this was a test of social sensitivity, on which women generally score higher (Koenig & Eagly, 2005). The stereotype that women are more sensitive and understanding served as a threat to the male participants, showing that stereotype threat can occur even with typically non-marginalized groups. Similarly, women’s ability to negotiate successfully was impaired when gender was made salient, indicating that females had an existing stereotype of men as superior negotiators (Kray, Galinsky, & Thompson, 2001). Perhaps the most often studied form of stereotype threat is in intellectual or academic domains. This research began with experiments showing that the possibility of confirming a negative racial stereotype about intellectual ability impaired Black students’ 4 performance on difficult verbal reasoning and math tests (Aronson, Quinn, & Spencer, 1998; Steele, 1997; Steele & Aronson, 1995). This effect was observed even when individual skills and level of preparation were accounted for, indicating that although the two racial groups were equally qualified, the negative stereotype present at the time of the test was enough to impair performance. This effect has been replicated with different tasks as well as with different target groups. White males who were highly identified with and proficient in math experienced impaired performance on a math test when comparisons were made with Asians, who are stereotyped as exceling in mathematics (Aronson, Lustina, Good, Keough, Steele, & Brown, 1998; Smith & White, 2002). Furthermore, when a difficult math test was described as producing gender differences, women performed much worse than men even though the two groups were equally qualified (Spencer, Steele, & Quinn,.
Recommended publications
  • The Perceived Validity of Stereotype Threat As an Explanation for Underperformance
    University of Connecticut OpenCommons@UConn Master's Theses University of Connecticut Graduate School 11-18-2016 The eP rceived Validity of Stereotype Threat as an Explanation for Underperformance Gabriel Camacho University of Connecticut - Storrs, [email protected] Recommended Citation Camacho, Gabriel, "The eP rceived Validity of Stereotype Threat as an Explanation for Underperformance" (2016). Master's Theses. 1034. https://opencommons.uconn.edu/gs_theses/1034 This work is brought to you for free and open access by the University of Connecticut Graduate School at OpenCommons@UConn. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of OpenCommons@UConn. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Running head: THE PERCEIVED VALIDITY OF STEREOTYPE THREAT The Perceived Validity of Stereotype Threat as an Explanation for Underperformance Gabriel Camacho B.A., University of Virginia, 2012 A Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Science at the University of Connecticut 2016 THE PERCEIVED VALIDITY OF STEREOTYPE THREAT ii APPROVAL PAGE Master of Science Thesis The Perceived Validity of Stereotype Threat as an Explanation for Underperformance Presented by Gabriel Camacho, B.A. Major Advisor________________________________________________________________ Diane M. Quinn Associate Advisor_____________________________________________________________ Felicia Pratto Associate Advisor_____________________________________________________________
    [Show full text]
  • Can Self-Persuasion Reduce Hostile Attribution Bias in Young Children?
    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0499-2 Can Self-Persuasion Reduce Hostile Attribution Bias in Young Children? Anouk van Dijk1 & Sander Thomaes1 & Astrid M. G. Poorthuis1 & Bram Orobio de Castro1 # The Author(s) 2018 Abstract Two experiments tested an intervention approach to reduce young children’s hostile attribution bias and aggression: self-persua- sion. Children with high levels of hostile attribution bias recorded a video-message advocating to peers why story characters who caused a negative outcome may have had nonhostile intentions (self-persuasion condition), or they simply described the stories (control condition). Before and after the manipulation, hostile attribution bias was assessed using vignettes of ambiguous provocations. Study 1 (n =83,age4–8) showed that self-persuasion reduced children’s hostile attribution bias. Study 2 (n = 121, age 6–9) replicated this finding, and further showed that self-persuasion was equally effective at reducing hostile attribution bias as was persuasion by others (i.e., listening to an experimenter advocating for nonhostile intentions). Effects on aggressive behavior, however, were small and only significant for one out of four effects tested. This research provides the first evidence that self-persuasion may be an effective approach to reduce hostile attribution bias in young children. Keywords Hostile attribution bias . Self-persuasion . Aggression . Intervention . Experiments Children’s daily social interactions abound with provocations by Dodge 1994). The present research tests an intervention approach peers, such as when they are physically hurt, laughed at, or ex- to reduce hostile attribution bias in young children. cluded from play. The exact reasons behind these provocations, Most interventions that effectively reduce children’s hostile and especially the issue of whether hostile intent was involved, attribution bias rely on attribution retraining techniques (e.g., are often unclear.
    [Show full text]
  • Judgements of Attributional Responsibility, Social Dominance Orientation, and the Insanity Defense
    JUDGEMENTS OF ATTRIBUTIONAL RESPONSISBILITY, SOCIAL DOMINANCE ORIENTATION, AND THE INSANITY DEFENSE. By Stephanie Sean Souter A Thesis Presented to The Faculty of Humboldt State University In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Psychology: Academic Research Committee Membership Dr. Gregg J. Gold, Committee Chair Dr. Amanda Hahn, Committee Member Lori Cortez-Regan M.A. Committee Member Dr. Christopher Aberson, Program Graduate Coordinator July 2020 Abstract JUDGEMENTS OF ATTRIBUTIONAL RESPONSIBILITY, SOCIAL DOMINANCE ORIENTATION, AND THE INSANITY DEFENSE Stephanie Sean Souter Issues of race, law, and mental health meet at a cross-section when it comes to cases involving the not guilty by reason of insanity (NGRI) plea in which the defendant is a member of a marginalized group. Although the duty of a juror is to reach a fair and unbiased verdict, the reality is that there are many ways that a person’s thoughts can be biased without them being aware of this (Bargh, 2001). This mock juror experiment (the first of its kind), investigated the role that Social Dominance Orientation (SDO) and attributional responsibility has in the success of NGRI pleas among African American and white defendants diagnosed with schizophrenia. Participants read a police report, a trial summary, and saw a photo where only skin color varied between condition, with either a white or African American defendant. Measures of SDO, attitudes about NGRI, attitudes about mental illness, and perceptions of attributional responsibility were taken. Results from (N = 320) participants suggest negative attitudes about the insanity plea reduces the likelihood of a juror assigning NGRI, R2 = .09, F(1,318) = 31.75, b = -.49, p < .001.
    [Show full text]
  • A CASE STUDY on ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING and SELF-EFFICACY in a HIGH SCHOOL COUNSELING SETTING Heather Laurel Pickett Northern Michigan University
    Northern Michigan University NMU Commons All NMU Master's Theses Student Works 2008 A CASE STUDY ON ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING AND SELF-EFFICACY IN A HIGH SCHOOL COUNSELING SETTING Heather Laurel Pickett Northern Michigan University Follow this and additional works at: https://commons.nmu.edu/theses Recommended Citation Pickett, Heather Laurel, "A CASE STUDY ON ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING AND SELF-EFFICACY IN A HIGH SCHOOL COUNSELING SETTING" (2008). All NMU Master's Theses. 486. https://commons.nmu.edu/theses/486 This Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Works at NMU Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in All NMU Master's Theses by an authorized administrator of NMU Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected],[email protected]. A CASE STUDY ON ATTRIBUTION RETRAINING AND SELF-EFFICACY IN A HIGH SCHOOL COUNSELING SETTING By Heather Laurel Pickett THESIS Submitted To Northern Michigan University In partial fulfillment of the requirements For the degree of MASTER OF ARTS Graduate Studies Office 2008 SIGNATURE APPROVAL FORM This thesis by Heather L. Pickett is recommended for your approval by the student’s thesis committee in the Department of Education and by the Dean of Graduate Studies. ____________________________________________________________________ Committee Chair: Judith M. Puncochar, Ph. D. Date ____________________________________________________________________ First Reader: Derek L. Anderson, Ed. D. Date ____________________________________________________________________ Second
    [Show full text]
  • Social Perception and Attribution 3 Brian Parkinson
    9781405124003_4_003.qxd 10/31/07 2:55 PM Page 42 Social Perception and Attribution 3 Brian Parkinson KEY CONCEPTS actor–observer difference analysis of non-common effects attributional bias augmenting principle causal power causal schema central trait cognitive algebra configural model consensus information consistency information correspondence bias correspondent inference theory covariation theory depressive realism discounting principle distinctiveness information false consensus bias implicit personality theory learned helplessness theory naïve scientist model peripheral trait primacy effect salience self-fulfilling prophecy self-serving biases 9781405124003_4_003.qxd 10/31/07 2:55 PM Page 43 CHAPTER OUTLINE How can we tell what other people are like? How do we explain their actions and experiences (and our own)? This chapter introduces research intended to answer these questions. Studies of social perception show that impressions of others depend on what information is presented, how it is pre- sented, and on prior assumptions about how it fits together. Research into attribution demonstrates that perceivers consistently favour certain kinds of explanation over others. Our impressions and ex- planations are also shaped by our specific reasons for constructing them. In particular, we present social events in different ways to different people under different circumstances. Both social percep- tion and attribution therefore involve communication in combination with private interpretation. Introduction Can you remember when you first met your closest friend? How quickly did you get a sense of what he or she was like, and of how well you would get on together? Did your impression turn out to be correct, and if not, where and why did you go wrong? Now imagine that instead of meeting another person face to face, you are told about them by someone else.
    [Show full text]
  • Desire for Control and the Illusion of Control: the Effects of Familiarity and Sequence of Outcomes
    JOURNAL OF RESEARCH IN PERSONALITY 20, 66-76 (1986) Desire for Control and the illusion of Control: The Effects of Familiarity and Sequence of Outcomes JERRY M. BURGER University of Santa Clara Two experiments were conducted to examine the relationship between individual differences in the general desire for control and the illusion of control. In Experiment 1, high desire for control college students bet significantly more than lows in a gambling game when they were playing with familiar cards, but not when playing with unfamiliar cards. In Experiment 2, high desire for control subjects demonstrated the illusion of control in a coin-toss game when they experienced success in anticipating the outcome of the coin toss at the beginning of the sequence. These subjects believed they had performed better on the task and anticipated they would do better on upcoming tasks than low desire for control subjects or subjects who experienced failure at the beginning of the sequence. 0 1986 Academic Press, Inc. That people are not always accurate in their assessment of causation has become a well-established fact among exprimental psychologists. Systematic distortions of perceived cause and effect relationships have been found to permeate our efforts to make sense out of the events in the world (cf. Alloy & Abramson, 1979; Nisbett & Ross, 1980). Among the more interesting of these demonstrated errors is the “illusion of control” (Langer, 1975, 1977), the tendency to perceive that one has the ability to influence outcomes that are obviously chance determined. It has been found, for example, that knowing what number to shoot for in a dice game will lead to increased betting, supposedly because the dice thrower believes himself or herself to have a better chance of controlling the outcome (Strickland, Lewicki, & Katz, 1966).
    [Show full text]
  • Reducing Automatic Stereotyping Through Situational Attribution Training
    Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 46 (2010) 221–225 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Social Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp FlashReport Consider the situation: Reducing automatic stereotyping through Situational Attribution Training Tracie L. Stewart a,*, Ioana M. Latu a, Kerry Kawakami b, Ashley C. Myers a a Department of Psychology, Georgia State University, United States b Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada article info abstract Article history: The present research investigated the effectiveness of a new technique for reducing automatic biases Received 23 July 2009 rooted in attribution theory – the Situational Attribution Training Technique. The goal of this strategy Revised 30 August 2009 extends previous work by targeting the fundamental attributional pillars underlying automatic stereo- Available online 13 September 2009 typing. We aimed to circumvent the well-documented tendency for individuals to be overly reliant on dispositional attributions when perceiving negative stereotype-consistent behaviors performed by Keywords: outgroup members. By teaching participants to consider situational attributions for such behaviors, we Stereotyping expected a reduction in outgroup stereotyping. Specifically, White participants were trained extensively Bias reduction to choose situational over dispositional explanations for negative stereotype-consistent behaviors per- Intergroup relations Implicit associations formed by Black men. Across two experiments,
    [Show full text]
  • How Do Social Dominance and Minority Influence Affect The
    Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and private study only. The thesis may not be reproduced elsewhere without the permission of the Author. i How do Social Dominance and Minority Influence affect the Collaboration of Refugee Services? A thesis presented in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Psychology at Massey University, Albany, New Zealand Sarah Helene Hahn 2015 ii Abstract Social Dominance Theory has problematic implications for humanitarian work: It suggests that stakeholders of the humanitarian sector collectively maintain the social hierarchies that disadvantage the very minorities that they are supposed to empower. Minority Influence Theory, on the other hand, suggests that social innovation in the humanitarian sector can emerge from the bottom-up, thus against the grain of social hierarchies. This thesis explores for the refugee service sector of Auckland, New Zealand, (a) if former refugees are indeed marginalised within the inter-organisational context that is supposed to empower them, (b) if this has detrimental effects on the sector’s performance, and (c) if fostering minority influence might alleviate such effects. The first research question was approached through a stakeholder analysis, which revealed that the social hierarchies within the refugee service sector indeed mirror the marginalisation of former refugees in general New Zealand society. Then, stories of positive and negative incidents of collaboration in the sector were analysed through the lenses of Social Dominance Theory and Minority Influence Theory. A thematic analysis of negative incidents of collaboration gathered accounts of discrimination through disregard, as well as legitimising myths.
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: SELF-DISTANCING and FUNDAMENTAL ATTRIBUTION ERROR 1
    Running Head: SELF-DISTANCING AND FUNDAMENTAL ATTRIBUTION ERROR 1 Effects of a Self-Distancing Perspective on the Fundamental Attribution Error: An Attempt at De-Biasing by Alex Goldberg A Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Arts With Honors in Psychology from the University of Michigan 2011 Advisors: Dr. Ethan Kross, Ryan Bremner SELF-DISTANCING AND FUNDAMENTAL ATTRIBUTION ERROR 2 Abstract This study aimed to attenuate the fundamental attribution error (FAE) by inducing a self- distanced perspective in participants. Participants’ perspectives were manipulated by having them write an essay in either the third-person (self-distanced), the first-person (immersed), or with no instructions (control group 1). A second control group was used in which participants did not write at all. I hypothesized that priming a self-distanced perspective would reduce expression of the fundamental attribution error when rating the true opinions of authors who are directed to take a specific point of view on a controversial topic. A significant decrease in the expression of the FAE was found between the self-distanced groups on the one hand and the immersed, no instruction, and no-writing groups on the other for participants who read essays written by people directed to write essays supporting President Obama. Keywords: self-distancing, fundamental attribution error, correspondence bias, attribution SELF-DISTANCING AND FUNDAMENTAL ATTRIBUTION ERROR 3 Effects of a Self-Distancing Perspective on the Fundamental Attribution Error: An Attempt at De-Biasing Over the years, psychologists have documented a wide range of biases in human cognition, such as the hindsight bias (Fischhoff, 1975), the confirmation bias (Wason, 1960), and the fundamental attribution error (Jones & Harris, 1967; Ross, 1977).
    [Show full text]
  • Social Dominance Theory: the Explanation Behind Social Hierarchy and Oppression?
    Sociological Imagination: Western’s Undergraduate Sociology Student Journal Volume 2 | Issue 1 Article 4 5-4-2013 Social Dominance Theory: The Explanation behind Social Hierarchy and Oppression? Mikala Virley [email protected] Recommended Citation Virley, Mikala (2013) "Social Dominance Theory: The Explanation behind Social Hierarchy and Oppression?,"Sociological Imagination: Western’s Undergraduate Sociology Student Journal: Vol. 2: Iss. 1, Article 4. Virley: Social Dominance Theory: Social Hierarchy and Oppression Social Dominance Theory: The Explanation behind Social Hierarchy and Oppression? Mikala Virley Published by Scholarship@Western, 2013 1 Sociological Imagination: Western’s Undergraduate Sociology Student Journal, Vol. 2 [2013], Iss. 1, Art. 4 Despite our best efforts, oppression, discrimination, brutality and chaos have persisted throughout history and continue to play a role in modern society. Instances of human oppression and suffering are all too common and often result in hierarchal social structures. These hierarchal structures place individuals or groups at the top of the social system, with other individuals or groups barely surviving at the bottom. While journalists and the media have penetrated descriptions and broadcasts of this barbaric behaviour, it is primarily social scientists that have tried to construct a theory behind why societies, across the globe, are structured in this hierarchal way (Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). As a result of this, multiple forms of research on the related topics of stereotyping, prejudice, relations within groups, and race and gender have become extensive. Past literature has placed emphasis on different issues that explain the complex problem of oppression and hierarchies. As there are many separate pieces of the puzzle behind oppression and racism that may be interrelated, it can seem overwhelming and tedious to tie them into a coherent whole.
    [Show full text]
  • Help-Seeking and Causal Attributions for Helping
    Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 112 Help-Seeking and Causal Attributions for Helping BY INGRID OLSSON ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS UPPSALA 2002 Dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology presented at Uppsala University in 2002 ABSTRACT Olsson, I. I. 2002. Help-seeking and Causal Attributions for Helping. Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis. Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Disserta- tions from the Faculty of Social Sciences 112. 93 pp. Uppsala. ISBN 91- 554-5229-9. This thesis investigates help-seeking and effects of help-seeking on causal attributions for helping (i.e., what people believe caused help or lack of help). Additionally, it examines self-serving and other-serving attributions (i.e., to augment a person’s positive sides and diminish the negative ones). Help-seeking was investigated in questionnaires, describing situations where spouses collaborate in doing household chores. A first study showed that women and men report using direct styles (i.e., explicitly verbalising the requests) more often than indirect ones. A second study showed that spouses inaccurately believe that wives in general would report more indirect and less direct styles than husbands in general. Causal attributions for helping were investigated in four studies with different methods, settings, and types of relationships (questionnaires, laboratory experiment; spouses doing chores, students and strangers doing computerized exercises). Consistent support was obtained for a predicted interaction between helping and the clarity of the request for help in determining the attributions. It is sugges- ted that this finding is an effect of people comparing the behavior of one person with their beliefs about how other persons behave (i.e., consensus).
    [Show full text]
  • Spontaneous Mentalizing Predicts the Fundamental Attribution Error
    Spontaneous Mentalizing Predicts the Fundamental Attribution Error The Harvard community has made this article openly available. Please share how this access benefits you. Your story matters Citation Moran, Joseph M., Eshin Jolly, and Jason Paul Mitchell. 2014. "Spontaneous Mentalizing Predicts the Fundamental Attribution Error." Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26 (3): 569–576. Published Version doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00513 Citable link http://nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:13457155 Terms of Use This article was downloaded from Harvard University’s DASH repository, and is made available under the terms and conditions applicable to Other Posted Material, as set forth at http:// nrs.harvard.edu/urn-3:HUL.InstRepos:dash.current.terms-of- use#LAA Spontaneous Mentalizing Predicts the Fundamental Attribution Error Joseph M. Moran1,2, Eshin Jolly1, and Jason P. Mitchell1 Abstract ■ When explaining the reasons for othersʼ behavior, perceivers behavior in response to a specific social situation and later judged often overemphasize underlying dispositions and personality whether that act was attributable to the targetʼsinternaldisposi- traits over the power of the situation, a tendency known as the tions or to external situational factors. Neural regions consistently fundamental attribution error. One possibility is that this bias re- associated with mental state inference—especially, the medial sults from the spontaneous processing of othersʼ mental states, pFC—strongly predicted whether participants later made disposi- such as their momentary feelings or more enduring personality tional attributions. These results suggest that the spontaneous characteristics. Here, we use fMRI to test this hypothesis. Partici- engagement of mentalizing may underlie the biased tendency to pants read a series of stories that described a targetʼsambiguous attribute behavior to dispositional over situational forces.
    [Show full text]