The Social Role of Imitation in Autism Implications for the Treatment of Imitation Deficits

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Social Role of Imitation in Autism Implications for the Treatment of Imitation Deficits Infants & Young Children Vol. 21, No. 2, pp. 107–119 Copyright c 2008 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins The Social Role of Imitation in Autism Implications for the Treatment of Imitation Deficits Brooke Ingersoll, PhD Individuals with autism exhibit significant deficits in imitation skills. This article reviews the impor- tance of imitation in typical development, focusing on the social function of imitation and its role in the development of social communication skills. Second, it reviews evidence suggesting an asso- ciation between imitation deficits and social communication impairments in children with autism. Third, it discusses limitations of the current method for teaching imitation that targets only the learning function of imitation. Finally, it describes a new imitation intervention designed to teach the social use of imitation in young children with autism. Key words: autism, early intervention, imitation, social communication HILDREN WITH AUTISM exhibit signifi- found effect on learning and development Ccant impairment in imitation skills. These in children with autism (Meltzoff & Gopnik, deficits have been reported on a variety of 1994; Rogers & Pennington, 1991), making it tasks including symbolic and nonsymbolic an important focus of intervention. body movements, symbolic and functional This article reviews the importance of imi- object use, vocalizations, and facial expres- tation in development. In particular, it focuses sions (for review, see Smith & Bryson, 1994; on the social function of imitation and its role Williams, Whiten, & Singh, 2004). In typi- in the development of social communication cal infants, imitation emerges early in devel- skills. Second, it reviews evidence suggesting opment (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977) and plays an association between imitation deficits and a crucial role in the development of cog- social communication impairments in chil- nitive and social communication behaviors, dren with autism. Third, it discusses limita- such as language, play, and joint attention tions of the current method for teaching im- (Rogers & Pennington, 1991). This associa- itation that targets only the learning function tion, as well as evidence for the specificity of of imitation. Finally, it describes an imitation the imitation deficit in autism (eg, Charman intervention designed to teach the social use et al., 1997; Rogers, Hepburn, Stackhouse, of imitation in young children with autism. & Wehner, 2003; Stone, Ousley, & Littleford, 1997), has led some researchers to propose ROLE OF IMITATION IN DEVELOPMENT imitation as a primary deficit that has a pro- In typical infants, imitation emerges early in development (Meltzoff & Moore, 1977) and serves 2 distinct functions: a learning Author Affiliation: Lewis & Clark College, Portland, Oregon. function, through which infants acquire new skills and knowledge, and a social function, This study was supported by grants from the Organiza- tion for Autism Research and Cure Autism Now. through which infants engage in social and emotional exchanges with others (Uzgiris, Corresponding Author: Brooke Ingersoll, PhD, 105B Psychology Bldg, Michigan State University, East 1981). It is through this social use of imita- Lansing, MI 48824 ([email protected]). tion that typically developing infants acquire 107 108 INFANTS &YOUNG CHILDREN/APRIL–JUNE 2008 the social communication skills that are found more sophisticated play skills (Morrison & to be deficient in children with autism. Kuhn, 1983). Imitation of peers serves to in- For example, in typical infants, early face- crease and refine peer interactions during to-face interactions with caregivers are often early childhood and remains a strong elicitor characterized by mutual or reciprocal imita- of social interest throughout childhood. tion in which both the caregiver and the in- In sum, the social use of imitation in in- fant engage in imitation of the others’ vocal- fancy and early childhood is associated with izations and facial expressions. It is through the development of more sophisticated social these reciprocal imitation games that infants communication skills. This research would communicate social interest in their partner suggest that a disruption in the early social (Nadel, Guerini, Peze, & Rivet, 1999; Uzgiris, use of imitation might have a significant im- 1981, 1999), develop a sense of shared affec- pact on the development of other social com- tive experience (Malatesta & Izard, 1984), and munication skills, a hypothesis proposed by engage in conversational turn-taking eventu- others (Meltzoff & Gopnik, 1994; Rogers & ally necessary during spoken communication Pennington, 1991). (Trevarthen, Kokkinaki, & Fiamenghi, 1999). Toward the end of the first year, play be- RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN IMITATION tween the infant and caregiver becomes more AND SOCIAL COMMUNICATION object focused and the infant begins to imitate BEHAVIOR IN AUTISM the caregiver’s actions with toys (eg, Uzgiris, 1990). In the second year, imitation games Indeed, the research on autism suggests a often involve affective gestures (Kuczynski, relationship between imitative performance Zahn-Waxler, & Redke-Yarrow, 1987). Among and other social communication skills, includ- mother-child dyads, imitation remains one of ing language, play, and joint attention. There the most common, stable patterns of interac- is considerable support for an association be- tion throughout early childhood (Halliday & tween imitation and language abilities in chil- Leslie, 1986). Reciprocal imitation serves to dren with autism. Dawson and Adams (1984) express interest and engagement between the found that children with autism categorized as child and caregiver (Waxler & Yarrow, 1975) high imitators verbalized to the experimenter and is a strategy through which the child significantly more than children categorized learns conventional actions with toys (Uzgiris, as low imitators. Sigman and Ungerer (1984) 1990) and affective gestures (Kuczynski et al., found that children with autism exhibited spe- 1987). cific deficits in vocal and gestural imitation Reciprocal imitation also plays a key role compared with typical children and children in early peer interactions. Performance of with developmental delay. These skills were the same act on the same object initiates correlated with receptive language for all 3 interactions between toddlers (Mueller & groups, and vocal imitation was correlated Lucas, 1975) and often results in maintained with expressive language in typical children or increased social interaction including coun- and children with autism. terimitation (Eckerman & Stein, 1990; Grusec A longitudinal study of imitation and lan- & Abramovitch, 1982). Sustained reciprocal guage in children with autism found an asso- imitation is the predominant mode of so- ciation between gestural imitation and the de- cial interaction and preverbal communication velopment of expressive language 6 months between same-aged toddlers (Baudonniere, later in young children with autism (Stone 1988; Eckerman, 1993). These imitative ex- et al., 1997). In another longitudinal study, changes appear to foster continued social in- Stone and Yoder (2001) found that motor im- teraction by communicating a common un- itation ability at age 2 significantly predicted derstanding of ongoing activities (Eckerman, language outcomes at age 4, suggesting the 1993) and play a role in the acquisition of strong correlation between motor imitation Imitation and Autism 109 and language development in children with relationship between the ability to imitate ac- autism. In contrast, after controlling for devel- tions with objects and the development of opmental age, Rogers et al. (2003) did not find higher level play behaviors; however, the re- a relationship between imitation skills (oral, lationship may be mediated by developmental object, or body) and concurrent language age age. in young children with autism. This finding Imitation deficits may further disrupt the may be due to the use of different imitation development of peer play, as early peer in- tasks (oral-motor vs verbal) or may suggest teractions are heavily based on reciprocal that the relationship between imitation and imitation with toys (Eckerman & Didow, language is mediated by other factors. 1996; Eckerman & Stein, 1990). Indeed, Stone The research also supports a relationship and Lemanek (1990) found that parents of between imitation and play skills in autism. A preschool-aged children with autism reported variety of studies have indicated that young that their child displayed significantly less children with autism are impaired on the imitation of another child at play compared imitation of functional and arbitrary actions with parents of children with developmental with play materials (eg, Charman et al., 1997; delays. 1998; DeMyer et al., 1972; Stone et al., Several studies have found a correlation be- 1997). Hammes and Langdell (1981) found tween imitation and joint attention in autism. that children with autism performed signifi- In nonverbal children with autism, Curcio cantly worse than children with learning dif- (1978) found that higher gesture imitation ficulties on imitation tasks involving 1 real performance predicted more sophisticated object and 1 imaginary object or pantomime communicative gestures. In another study, of an action with 2 imaginary objects. Libby, Abrahamsen and Mitchell (1990) found that Powell, Messer, and Jordan (1997) found that vocal imitation was highly correlated with the children
Recommended publications
  • A Imitation Learning: a Survey of Learning Methods
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by BCU Open Access A Imitation Learning: A Survey of Learning Methods Ahmed Hussein, School of Computing Science and Digital Media, Robert Gordon University Mohamed Medhat Gaber, School of Computing and Digital Technology, Birmingham City University Eyad Elyan , School of Computing Science and Digital Media, Robert Gordon University Chrisina Jayne , School of Computing Science and Digital Media, Robert Gordon University Imitation learning techniques aim to mimic human behavior in a given task. An agent (a learning machine) is trained to perform a task from demonstrations by learning a mapping between observations and actions. The idea of teaching by imitation has been around for many years, however, the field is gaining attention recently due to advances in computing and sensing as well as rising demand for intelligent applications. The paradigm of learning by imitation is gaining popularity because it facilitates teaching complex tasks with minimal expert knowledge of the tasks. Generic imitation learning methods could potentially reduce the problem of teaching a task to that of providing demonstrations; without the need for explicit programming or designing reward functions specific to the task. Modern sensors are able to collect and transmit high volumes of data rapidly, and processors with high computational power allow fast processing that maps the sensory data to actions in a timely manner. This opens the door for many potential AI applications that require real-time perception and reaction such as humanoid robots, self-driving vehicles, human computer interaction and computer games to name a few.
    [Show full text]
  • Mental States in Animals: Cognitive Ethology Jacques Vauclair
    Mental states in animals: cognitive ethology Jacques Vauclair This artHe addresses the quegtion of mentaJ states in animak as viewed in ‘cognitive ethology”. In effect, thk field of research aims at studying naturally occurring behaviours such as food caching, individual recognition, imitation, tool use and communication in wild animals, in order to seek for evidence of mental experiences, self-aw&&reness and intentional@. Cognitive ethologists use some philosophical cencepts (e.g., the ‘intentional stance’) to carry out their programme of the investigation of natural behaviours. A comparison between cognitive ethology and other approaches to the investigation of cognitive processes in animals (e.g., experimental animal psychology) helps to point out the strengths and weaknesses of cognitive ethology. Moreover, laboratory attempts to analyse experimentally Mentional behaviours such as deception, the relationship between seeing and knowing, as well as the ability of animals to monitor their own states of knowing, suggest that cognitive ethology could benefit significantly from the conceptual frameworks and methods of animal cognitive psychology. Both disciplines could, in fact, co&ribute to the understanding of which cognitive abilities are evolutionary adaptations. T he term ‘cognirive ethology’ (CE) was comed by that it advances a purposive or Intentional interpretation Griffin in The Question ofAnimd Au~aarmess’ and later de- for activities which are a mixture of some fixed genetically veloped in other publications’ ‘_ Although Griffin‘s IS’6 transmitred elements with more hexible behaviour?. book was first a strong (and certainly salutary) reacrion (Cognitive ethologists USC conceptual frameworks against the inhibitions imposed by strict behaviourism in provided by philosophers (such as rhe ‘intentional stance’)“.
    [Show full text]
  • Much Has Been Written About the Turing Test in the Last Few Years, Some of It
    1 Much has been written about the Turing Test in the last few years, some of it preposterously off the mark. People typically mis-imagine the test by orders of magnitude. This essay is an antidote, a prosthesis for the imagination, showing how huge the task posed by the Turing Test is, and hence how unlikely it is that any computer will ever pass it. It does not go far enough in the imagination-enhancement department, however, and I have updated the essay with a new postscript. Can Machines Think?1 Can machines think? This has been a conundrum for philosophers for years, but in their fascination with the pure conceptual issues they have for the most part overlooked the real social importance of the answer. It is of more than academic importance that we learn to think clearly about the actual cognitive powers of computers, for they are now being introduced into a variety of sensitive social roles, where their powers will be put to the ultimate test: In a wide variety of areas, we are on the verge of making ourselves dependent upon their cognitive powers. The cost of overestimating them could be enormous. One of the principal inventors of the computer was the great 1 Originally appeared in Shafto, M., ed., How We Know (San Francisco: Harper & Row, 1985). 2 British mathematician Alan Turing. It was he who first figured out, in highly abstract terms, how to design a programmable computing device--what we not call a universal Turing machine. All programmable computers in use today are in essence Turing machines.
    [Show full text]
  • Nonverbal Imitation Skills in Children with Specific Language Delay
    City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Dohmen, A., Chiat, S. and Roy, P. (2013). Nonverbal imitation skills in children with specific language delay. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(10), pp. 3288-3300. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.004 This is the unspecified version of the paper. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/3352/ Link to published version: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.06.004 Copyright and reuse: City Research Online aims to make research outputs of City, University of London available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain with the author(s) and/or copyright holders. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to. City Research Online: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/ [email protected] Full title: Nonverbal imitation skills in children with specific language delay Name and affiliations of authors Andrea Dohmen¹˒², Shula Chiat², and Penny Roy² ¹Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, 9 South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK ²Language and Communication Science Division, City University London, Northampton Square EC1V 0HB London, UK Corresponding author Andrea Dohmen University of Oxford / Department of Experimental Psychology 9 South Parks Road Oxford OX1 3UD, UK Email: [email protected] Telephone: 0044/1865/271334 2 Nonverbal imitation skills in children with specific language delay Abstract Research in children with language problems has focussed on verbal deficits, and we have less understanding of children‟s deficits with nonverbal sociocognitive skills which have been proposed to be important for language acquisition.
    [Show full text]
  • Deep Reinforcement Learning Via Imitation Learning
    Deep Reinforcement Learning via Imitation Learning Sergey Levine perception Action (run away) action sensorimotor loop Action (run away) End-to-end vision standard features mid-level features classifier computer (e.g. HOG) (e.g. DPM) (e.g. SVM) vision Felzenszwalb ‘08 deep learning Krizhevsky ‘12 End-to-end control standard state low-level modeling & motion motor observations estimation controller robotic prediction planning torques control (e.g. vision) (e.g. PD) deep motor sensorimotor observations torques learning indirect supervision actions have consequences Contents Imitation learning Imitation without a human Research frontiers Terminology & notation 1. run away 2. ignore 3. pet Terminology & notation 1. run away 2. ignore 3. pet Terminology & notation 1. run away 2. ignore 3. pet a bit of history… управление Lev Pontryagin Richard Bellman Contents Imitation learning Imitation without a human Research frontiers Imitation Learning training supervised data learning Images: Bojarski et al. ‘16, NVIDIA Does it work? No! Does it work? Yes! Video: Bojarski et al. ‘16, NVIDIA Why did that work? Bojarski et al. ‘16, NVIDIA Can we make it work more often? cost stability Learning from a stabilizing controller (more on this later) Can we make it work more often? Can we make it work more often? DAgger: Dataset Aggregation Ross et al. ‘11 DAgger Example Ross et al. ‘11 What’s the problem? Ross et al. ‘11 Imitation learning: recap training supervised data learning • Usually (but not always) insufficient by itself – Distribution mismatch problem • Sometimes works well – Hacks (e.g. left/right images) – Samples from a stable trajectory distribution – Add more on-policy data, e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Imitation: Is Cognitive Neuroscience Solving the Correspondence Problem?
    ARTICLE IN PRESS TICS 384 Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.xx No.xx Monthxxxx Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Marcel Brass1 and Cecilia Heyes2 1Department of Cognitive Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Stephanstr. 1A, 04103 Leipzig, Germany 2Department of Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK Imitation poses a unique problem: how does the imi- framework (generalist theories) or whether it depends on tator know what pattern of motor activation will make a special purpose mechanism (specialist theories). We their action look like that of the model? Specialist then review research on the role of learning in imitation theories suggest that this correspondence problem has and observation of biological motion. Finally, we discuss a unique solution; there are functional and neurological a problem for generalist theories: If imitation depends on mechanisms dedicated to controlling imitation. General- ist theories propose that the problem is solved by general mechanisms of associative learning and action control. Box 1. Mirror neurons: What do they do? What are they for? Recent research in cognitive neuroscience, stimulated Mirror neurons in the premotor area F5 of monkeys are active both by the discovery of mirror neurons, supports generalist when the animal observes and when it executes a specific action solutions. Imitation is based on the automatic activation (for a review see [52,53]. The discovery of these cells has had a revolutionary impact, turning perception–action interaction into a of motor representations by movement observation. focus of intensive, interdisciplinary research worldwide. Naturally These externally triggered motor representations are there has been a great deal of speculation about the function of then used to reproduce the observed behaviour.
    [Show full text]
  • How to Use Outlines to Teach Rhetorical Prosody and Structure
    VANCE SCHAEFER AND LINDA ABE United States The Art of Imitation: How to Use Outlines to Teach Rhetorical Prosody and Structure onnative speakers of a language are often at a disadvantage in producing extended speech, as they have differing native (L1) N phonological systems and rhetorical traditions or little experience in giving talks. Prosody in the form of stress, rhythm, and intonation is a difficult but crucial area needed to master extended speech because prosody interacts with structure to play a key role in conveying meaning (i.e., intelligibility) and easing understanding (i.e., comprehensibility) (see Munro and Derwing 1995 for discussion on intelligibility, comprehensibility, and pronunciation). However, second-language (L2) speakers of rhetorical prosody and structure: namely, English might not effectively produce this putting the focus on using outlines. This rhetorical prosody. For example, they may article serves as a primer for practitioners not fully utilize pitch range to signal a shift of English as a second or foreign language in topics (Wennerstrom 1998), potentially (ESL/EFL), describing how to use outlines resulting in lower comprehension levels (Munro as an effective technique to teach rhetorical and Derwing 1995; Wennerstrom 1998). prosody and structure to learners at the level This difficulty appears to arise from a lack of high school, university, and beyond. This of understanding and/or effective practice, article covers the following: as gauged from our teaching experience. While the mechanics of academic writing and 1. a simple overview of prosody and grammar are taught to L2 English learners, the structure in English, described with rhetorical elements of prosody do not appear to concrete features and rules be commonly taught.
    [Show full text]
  • Learning by Imitation, Reinforcement and Verbal Rules in Problem-Solving Tasks
    Dandurand, F., Bowen, M. & Shultz, T. R. (2004). Learning by imitation, reinforcement and verbal rules in problem-solving tasks. In the Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Development and Learning (ICDL'04) Developing Social Brains. La Jolla, California, USA: The Salk Institute for Biological Studies. Learning by Imitation, Reinforcement and Verbal Rules in Problem Solving Tasks Frédéric Dandurand, Melissa Bowen, Thomas R. Shultz McGill University, Department of Psychology, 1205 Dr. Penfield Ave., Montréal, Québec, H3A 1B1, Canada, [email protected], [email protected], [email protected] Abstract ponent of intelligence. “The ability to solve problems is one of the most important manifestations of human think- Learning by imitation is a powerful process for ac- ing.” [12]. quiring new knowledge, but there has been little research Information-processing theory is currently the domi- exploring imitation’s potential in the problem solving nant approach to problem solving [12]. Problems are domain. Classical problem solving techniques tend to construed in terms of states, transitions and operators. The center around reinforcement learning, which requires essence of problem solving is a search through the state significant trial-and-error learning to reach successful space for a solution state using the operators available at goals and problem solutions. Heuristics, hints, and rea- each point while satisfying a set of problem-specific con- soning by analogy have been favored as improvements straints. over reinforcement learning, whereas imitation learning has been regarded as rote memorizing. However, re- 1.1. Learning in Problem Solving Tasks search on imitation learning in animals and infants sug- gests that what is being learned is the overall arrange- Several types of learning can occur during problem ment of actions (sequencing and planning).
    [Show full text]
  • Heterogeneity and Diffusion in the Digital Economy: Spain's Case
    Working Paper, Nº 15/29 Madrid, November 2015 Heterogeneity and diffusion in the digital economy: Spain’s case Javier Alonso Alfonso Arellano 15/29 Working Paper November 2015 Heterogeneity and diffusion in the digital economy: Spain’s case* Javier Alonso and Alfonso Arellano. Abstract The traditional Bass model (Bass, 1969) for the adoption and diffusion of new products has customarily been used to gauge the speed at which new products were adopted in a market by estimating innovation (p) and imitation (q) parameters. Rogers (2003) proposed that certain factors influence the diffusion of such products, including the educational level and age of consumers. In this article we estimate the coefficients of innovation and imitation for adopting internet, e-commerce and online banking in Spain’s case, while controlling for heterogeneity of individuals according to educational level and age. We thus find that individuals with very different p and q coefficients can coexist in one market. We then verify that the processes of an ageing population and educational improvement in Spain could give rise to long term effects on Spain’s overall innovative and imitative capacity as a result of its socio-demographic mix. Key words: Digital economy, adoption, diffusion, consumers, Spain. JEL: O30, L81, L86. *: The authors would like to thank assistants to the Conference on Digital Experimentation CODE MIT (October 16th-17th, 2015) for their constructive and useful comments. 2 / 29 www.bbvaresearch.com Working Paper November 2015 1 Introduction The innovations that have been brought in over the last few decades within the ambit of digital media have entailed the most spectacular of changes as regards both the economy and social relations.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Culture Consequences of Sociality What Is Culture? What Is Culture? Cultural Transmission
    Animal culture Consequences of sociality Animals are exposed to behavior, sometimes novel, of others Do animals display culture? What is culture? “The totality of the mental and physical reactions and activities that characterize the behavior of individuals composing a social group collectively and individually in relations to their natural environment, to other groups, to members of the group itself and of each individual to himself” - Franz Boas (1911) “An extrasomatic (nongenetic, nonbodily), temporal continuum of things and events dependent upon symboling. Culture consists of tools, implements, utensils, clothing, ornaments, customs, institutions, beliefs, rituals, games, works of art, language, etc.” - Leslie White (~1949) What is culture? Cultural transmission “Patterns, explicit and implicit, of and for behavior acquired May occur via copying… …or via direct instruction and transmitted by symbols, constituting the distinctive achievement of human groups” - Kroeber and Kluckhohn (1952) “Learned systems of meaning, communicated by means of natural language and other symbol systems, having representational, directive, and affective functions, and capable of creating cultural entities and particular senses of reality” - Roy D'Andrade (~1984) Tool use in chimpanzees “The universal human capacity to classify, codify and communicate their experiences symbolically…a defining feature of the genus Homo” - Wikipedia (2006) Culturally transmitted behavior must persist beyond life of originator 1 Cultural transmission in macaques Cultural transmission
    [Show full text]
  • The Limits to Imitation in Rational Observational Learning∗
    The Limits to Imitation in Rational Observational Learning∗ Erik Eyster and Matthew Rabiny August 8, 2012 Abstract An extensive literature identifies how rational people who observe the behavior of other privately-informed rational people with similar tastes may come to imitate them. While most of the literature explores when and how such imitation leads to inefficient information aggregation, this paper instead explores the behavior of fully rational observational learners. In virtually any setting, they imitate only some of their predecessors, and sometimes contradict both their private information and the prevailing beliefs that they observe. In settings that allow players to extract all rele- vant information about others' private signals from their actions, we identify necessary and sufficient conditions for rational observational learning to include \anti-imitation" where, fixing other observed actions, a person regards a state of the world as less likely the more a predecessor's action indicates belief in that state. Anti-imitation arises from players' need to subtract off the sources of correlation in others' actions, and is mandated by rationality in settings where players observe many predecessors' actions ∗We thank Min Zhang for excellent research assistance as well as Paul Heidhues and seminar participants at Berkeley, DIW Berlin, Hebrew University, ITAM, LSE, Penn, Tel Aviv University, and WZB for their comments. yEyster: Department of Economics, LSE, Houghton Street, London WC2A 2AE, UK. Rabin: Department of Economics, UC Berkeley, 549 Evans Hall #3880, Berkeley, CA 94720-3880, USA. 1 but not all recent or concurrent ones. Moreover, in these settings, there is always a positive probability that some player plays contrary to both her private information and the beliefs of every single person whose action she observes.
    [Show full text]
  • Innovation Has Become the Catchword of the Twentieth and the Twentieth
    Innovation and Imitation: Why is Imitation not Innovation? Benoît Godin 385 rue Sherbrooke Est Montréal, Québec Canada H2X 1E3 Project on the Intellectual History of Innovation Working Paper No. 25 2016 Previous Papers in the Series: 1. B. Godin, Innovation: the History of a Category. 2. B. Godin, In the Shadow of Schumpeter: W. Rupert Maclaurin and the Study of Technological Innovation. 3. B. Godin, The Linear Model of Innovation (II): Maurice Holland and the Research Cycle. 4. B. Godin, National Innovation System (II): Industrialists and the Origins of an Idea. 5. B. Godin, Innovation without the Word: William F. Ogburn’s Contribution to Technological Innovation Studies. 6. B. Godin, ‘Meddle Not with Them that Are Given to Change’: Innovation as Evil. 7. B. Godin, Innovation Studies: the Invention of a Specialty (Part I). 8. B. Godin, Innovation Studies: the Invention of a Specialty (Part II). 9. B. Godin, καινοτομία: An Old Word for a New World, or the De-Contestation of a Political and Contested Concept. 10. B. Godin, Innovation and Politics: The Controversy on Republicanism in Seventeenth Century England. 11. B. Godin, Social Innovation: Utopias of Innovation from circa-1830 to the Present. 12. B. Godin and P. Lucier, Innovation and Conceptual Innovation in Ancient Greece. 13. B. Godin and J. Lane, ‘Pushes and Pulls”: The Hi(S)tory of the Demand Pull Model of Innovation. 14. B. Godin, Innovation after the French Revolution, or, Innovation Transformed: From Word to Concept. 15. B. Godin, Invention, Diffusion and Innovation. 16. B. Godin, Innovation and Science: When Science Had Nothing to Do with Innovation, and Vice-Versa.
    [Show full text]