What Is Beyond the Big Five? Plenty!

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

What Is Beyond the Big Five? Plenty! What Is Beyond the Big Five? Plenty! Sampo V. Paunonen Douglas N. Jackson University of Western Ontario ABSTRACT In a recent analysis of personality data, Saucier and Goldberg (1998) sought to answer the question, What is beyond the Big Five? Those authors evaluated numerous clusters of English person-descriptive adjectives that have been suspected of referring to non–Big Five dimensions of personality. Their results led them to conclude that most, if not all, traits of personality can be adequately subsumed within the Big Five factor space. In contrast, our reanalysis of Saucier and Goldberg’s own data, using a more realistic criterion for deciding on whether a variable does or does not fall within a particular factor space, contradicts their claim. We are led to the conclusion that there are plenty of dimensions of behavior beyond the Big Five. Few topics in contemporary psychology have generated as much research and theoretical interest as has the Five-Factor Model of personality. That model has been embraced not only by a personality psychologist, but by researchers in clinical, industrial/organizational, and developmental psy- chology as well. The Five-Factor Model, of course, posits that there is a structure to individual differences in human behavior, such that the traits of personality can be reduced to five orthogonal factors of personal- ity—the so-called Big Five. This research was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Research Grant 410-98-1555 to Sampo V.Paunonen. WethankMichael Ashton and Kibeom Lee for their comments on this article. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sampo V. Paunonen, Department of Psychology, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario N6A 5C2, Canada. Journal of Personality 68:5, October 2000. Copyright © 2000 by Blackwell Publishers, 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA, and 108 Cowley Road, Oxford, OX4 1JF, UK. 822 Paunonen & Jackson As noted by Saucier and Goldberg (1998), the Big Five factors originated with studies of the structure underlying personality ratings, ratings made using words taken from the English-language personality lexicon. When personality adjectives are sampled from the language, either exhaustively or randomly, and used to describe real people, they tend to define five orthogonal clusters. The names applied to those lexically based clusters, in decreasing order by size, are Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional Stability versus Neuroti- cism, and Intellect or Imagination. Roughly the same five dimensions tend to be found in the same order with impressive consistency across different samples of subjects, different selections of personality adjec- tives, and even different cultures and language groups. The Big Five factors of personality are thought to be important precisely because they are big. According to the Lexical Hypothesis, the words we have invented to describe individual differences in personality are reflections of real human behaviors, and the number of words we have invented is in direct proportion to the importance of the behavior domain described. As Goldberg (1982) has said, “Those individual differences that are most significant in the daily transactions of persons with each other become encoded into their language. The more important such a difference is, the more people will notice it and wish to talk of it, with the result that eventually they will invent a word for it” (p. 204). The discovery of the Big Five factors of personality in linguistic data has subsequently led to the structural evaluation of questionnaires and other personality instruments, instruments that may or may not be purposely designed to measure those factors. Sometimes, however, the lexical and nonlexical (i.e., questionnaire) factors have differed in mor- phology. For example, whereas the lexical studies typically find a factor (the smallest) called Intellect, questionnaire-based measures have tended to focus on a dimension (also the smallest) called Openness to Experience (e.g., see Costa & McCrae, 1992). Other differences in Five-Factor Models can also be noted (see reviews by Block, 1995; Digman, 1990; John, 1990; McCrae & John, 1992). The Saucier and Goldberg (1998) Analysis Goldberg has presented, in some past publications, lists of words that can be considered characteristic or prototypical of each of the lexical Big Five, based on his extensive analyses of the factor structure underlying Beyond the Big Five 823 person descriptors in the English language (e.g., Goldberg, 1982, 1990, 1992). As Saucier and Goldberg (1998) have observed, however, some of the personality adjectives evaluated have not been closely aligned with the traditional Big Five factors in certain studies. Those authors, in fact, identified 53 such adjective clusters, each relatively homogeneous but suspected by some as being potentially outside the domain of the tradi- tional lexical Big Five. Saucier and Goldberg (1998) modified 21 of their 53 adjective clusters to make them even more homogeneous and included all 74 groups of words in their analysis. Their intent was to evaluate the extent to which each cluster of words shows statistical allegiance to the lexical Big Five. To do this, they administered the personality adjectives along with traditional Big Five marker adjectives to subjects in a self-report format. They then correlated scores on each of the 74 word clusters with scores on each of the five factors. Their ultimate test of whether or not a particular word cluster belonged within the space of the Big Five was to compute a multiple correlation between that cluster and the five factor measures. A word cluster that has much variance in common with one or more of the Big Five would, of course, have a high multiple correlation with those factors. The 74 multiple correlations of Saucier and Goldberg’s word clusters with their Big Five marker variables ranged from .09 to .67 with a mean of .38. Using an arbitrary cutoff score of .30 to decide whether a particular word cluster was or was not within the five-factor domain, those authors identified some person descriptor clusters, mainly related to physical characteristics, as being beyond the Big Five. Those clusters, each having a multiple correlation with the factors of less than .30, included words referring to a person’s height, weight, age, and physical attractiveness. Importantly, only one personality-relevant word cluster was likewise judged not to be within the bounds of the traditional Big Five, and it was related to religiosity, containing words such as religious, devout, and reverent. With regard to religiosity, however, Saucier and Goldberg claimed that “many might class- ify this cluster as reflecting individual differences in attitudes or ideology, rather than personality” (p. 506). Saucier and Goldberg’s Data Revisited Saucier and Goldberg (1998) have done a commendable job of identify- ing clusters of personality adjectives that appear, at least at face value, 824 Paunonen & Jackson not to have a strong relation to any of the Big Five factors. But their analysis led them to the conclusion that almost all of those personality variables are, in fact, related to the Big Five nontrivially. Upon our initial read of their article, we found this conclusion a curious one, given the nature of some of the person descriptors involved (see Saucier and Goldberg, 1998, Table 1). Consider, for example, the three dimensions masculine versus feminine, witty and humorous, cunning and sly. None of these seems to belong firmly to any Big Five factor. Saucier and Goldberg, however, concluded that all three dimensions do indeed fit into the space of the Big Five. But notice that a cluster of person descriptive adjectives related to one’s financial wealth (i.e., being prosperous, rich, and well-to-do versus being poor), by Saucier and Goldberg’s own criterion, was found to be more a part of the Big Five factors than were dimensions related to being masculine-feminine, or being witty and humorous, or being cunning and sly (1998, Table 2). Curious indeed. As we are about to describe, our consideration of Saucier and Gold- berg’s (1998) results has led us to the conclusion that many of the variables assessed in that study do not have a lot in common with the traditional lexical Big Five. But this conclusion stands in direct contrast to that of Saucier and Goldberg. The primary reason for this discrepancy is to be found in the choice of the statistical criterion for what constitutes a Big Five indicator. As we explain below, we take issue with Saucier and Goldberg’s criterion as being much too liberal. Table 2 in Saucier and Goldberg’s (1998) article shows the correlations between each of their 74 adjective clusters and each of their Big Five factor measures. Now note how those correlations are similar to factor loadings, being the correlations between variables and factors as found in a normal factor structure matrix. In fact, those correlations are properly interpreted as factor loadings in an extension analysis (see Dwyer, 1937; Gorsuch, 1983; Mosier, 1938). An extension analysis refers to the tech- nique of correlating variables with factors when the variables themselves were not part of the analysis that produced the factors, which is exactly what Saucier and Goldberg have done. Gorsuch (1983) mentioned a good reason for doing an extension analysis that is particularly pertinent to the present discussion. Extension analysis may be needed to test a hypothesis regarding the nature of a particular factor. A well-replicated factor may be hy- pothesized to be that which is measured by variable Z. To include Beyond the Big Five 825 variable Z in an exploratory factor analysis would usually allow it to help define the factor. Because this includes the possibility of capitalizing on chance, it would be better to determine variable Z’s relationship to the factor when the factor is based solely on the other variables.
Recommended publications
  • Behavioural Genetics Studies HERE
    KKaarrmmaaggeenneess BBaacckkggrroouunndd ccoonntteenntt 0011 Behavioral Genetics 0022 The Science Behind Karmagenes 0033 From DNA to Behavior 0044 5 Factors Personality (Big 5/OCEAN) Behavioral Genetics Timeline 380 BC Plato Plato, one of the founders of Western 1610 philosophy, rst Introduced the terms Nature and Nurture, in his Protagoras dialogue. William Shakespeare In Shakespeare's play The Tempest, protagonist 1869 Prospero notes: ‘‘A devil, a born devil, on whose Hereditary Genius nature Nurture can never stick; on whom my pains, Victorian polymath Sir Francis Galton, officially Humanely taken, all, all lost, quite lost; And as with introduced the notion of "Nature vs Nature" in age his body uglier grows, So his mind cankers. I will his book Hereditary Genius (1869), the rst plague them all, Even to roaring." social scientic attempt to s t u d y genius and greatness. He is considered the father of 1913 modern-day behavioural genetics. Theodore Roosevelt Theodore Roosevelt in his letter to Charles Fitter Families 1920 Davenport : ‘’Some day we will realize that the prime duty, the inescapable duty, of a good citizen Fitter Families, were contests rst launched in of the right type is to leave his or her blood behind 1920 in Kansas with the support of the American him in the world.” Eugenics Society, aiming to identify and reward the family with the highest degree of psychological and physical tness among its Eugenic movement member, as measured by a team of medical 1930 doctors. This period witnessed a sharp increase in the number of scientic publications researching heritability of behaviour. Meanwhile, the popularity Buck vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Bottom up Construction of a Personality Taxonomy
    Forthcoming in the European Journal of Psychological Assessment Bottom Up Construction of a Personality Taxonomy David M. Condon*, University of Oregon, USA Dustin Wood, University of Alabama, USA René Mõttus, University of Edinburgh, UK and University of Tartu, Estonia Tom Booth, University of Edinburgh, UK Giulio Costantini, University of Milan-Bicocca, Italy Samuel Greiff, University of Luxembourg, Luxembourg Wendy Johnson, University of Edinburgh, UK Aaron Lukaszewski, California State University, Fullerton, CA, USA Aja Murray, University of Edinburgh, UK William Revelle, Northwestern University, USA Aidan G.C. Wright, University of Pittsburgh, USA Matthias Ziegler, Humboldt Universität zu Berlin, Germany Johannes Zimmermann, University of Kassel, Germany * Correspondence to David Condon, 1227 University St, Eugene, OR, 97403 USA [email protected] This manuscript is based on an Expert Meeting jointly supported by European Association of Personality Psychology and European Association of Psychological Assessment and held from 6th to 8th September 2018 in Edinburgh, Scotland (https://osf.io/fn5pw). The authors are grateful to Mitja Back, Anna Baumert, Jaime Derringer, Sacha Epskamp, Ryne Sherman, David Stillwell, and Tal Yarkoni for their contributions to the Expert Meeting. Not all authors agree with all arguments put forward in this paper. Bottom-Up Construction of a Personality Taxonomy 2 Abstract In pursuit of a more systematic and comprehensive framework for personality assessment, we introduce procedures for assessing personality traits at the lowest level: nuances. We argue that constructing a personality taxonomy from the bottom up addresses some of the limitations of extant top-down assessment frameworks (e.g., the Big Five), including the opportunity to resolve confusion about the breadth and scope of traits at different levels of organization, evaluate unique and reliable trait variance at the item level, and clarify jingle/jangle issues in personality assessment.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to the Five-Factor Model and Its Applications
    An Introduction to the Five-Factor Model and Its Applications Robert R. McCrae National Institute on Aging, NIH Oliver P. John University of California at Berkeley ABSTRACT The five-factor model of personality is a hierarchical organi- zation of personality traits in terms of five basic dimensions: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism, and Openness to Experience. Research using both natural language adjectives and theoretically based per- sonality questionnaires supports the comprehensiveness of the model and its applicability across observers and cultures. This article summarizes the history of the model and its supporting evidence; discusses conceptions of the nature of the factors; and outlines an agenda for theorizing about the origins and operation of the factors. We argue that the model should prove useful both for individual assessment and for the elucidation of a number of topics of interest to personality psychologists. What are the basic dimensions of personality, the most important ways in which individuals differ in their enduring emotional, interpersonal, experiential, attitudinal, and motivational styles? Personality theorists have offered hundreds of candidates, and for decades factor analysts attempted to bring order to the resulting confusion by factoring person- ality scales. Instead of resolving the issue, however, these studies only contributed another layer of controversy, most familiar in the compet- We are grateful to Michael Bond, Peter Borkenau, David Buss, Paul Costa, Donald Fiske, Lew Goldberg, Robert Hogan, and Warren Norman for comments on this manu- script, and to Stephen G. West and the associate editors of this journal for their advice and assistance on this special issue. Correspondence may be addressed to Robert R.
    [Show full text]
  • Ubiquitous Personality-Trait Concepts in 13 Diverse and Isolated Languages: a Cluster–Classification Approach
    European Journal of Personality, Eur. J. Pers. 34: 164–179 (2020) Published online 3 March 2020 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com) DOI: 10.1002/per.2246 Ubiquitous Personality-Trait Concepts in 13 Diverse and Isolated Languages: A Cluster–Classification Approach JOSHUA K. WOOD1*, MICHAEL GURVEN2 and LEWIS R. GOLDBERG3 1School of Psychology, Deakin University, Geelong, Australia 2Department of Anthropology, University of California at Santa Barbara, USA 3Oregon Research Institute, USA Abstract: There is longstanding interest in the generalizability of personality across diverse cultures. To investigate the generalizability of personality concepts, we examined the English translations of individual-difference entries from the dictionaries of 12 small-scale societies previously studied for ubiquity of individual differences plus the dic- tionary of an additional society not previously studied in this manner. These 13 societies are highly diverse in geo- graphical location, culture, and language family; their languages developed in isolation from modern-world languages. The goal of our exploratory research was to discover ubiquitous personality concepts in these 13 indepen- dent societies and their languages, providing a window into personality concepts across a broad range of cultures and languages. This study used clusters of empirically related terms (e.g. brave, courageous, and daring), based on a tax- onomy of English-language personality concepts that consisted of 100 personality-trait clusters. English-language definitions of dictionary entries from the 13 languages were matched to the meanings of the synonym clusters. The cluster–classification method uncovered nine ubiquitous personality concepts, plus six that were present in at least 12 of the 13 languages. The nine ubiquitous personality concepts include some not previously identified and suggest a core of possibly universal concepts.
    [Show full text]
  • Five-Factor Model of Personality Christopher J
    Five-Factor Model of Personality Christopher J. Soto, Joshua J. Jackson Introduction The five-factor model of personality (FFM) is a set of five broad trait dimensions or domains, often referred to as the “Big Five”: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroticism (sometimes named by its polar opposite, Emotional Stability), and Openness to Experience (sometimes named Intellect). Highly extraverted individuals are assertive and sociable, rather than quiet and reserved. Agreeable individuals are cooperative and polite, rather than antagonistic and rude. Conscientious individuals are task- focused and orderly, rather than distractible and disorganized. Neurotic individuals are prone to experiencing negative emotions, such as anxiety, depression, and irritation, rather than being emotionally resilient. Finally, highly open individuals have a broad rather than narrow range of interests, are sensitive rather than indifferent to art and beauty, and prefer novelty to routine. The Big Five/FFM was developed to represent as much of the variability in individuals’ personalities as possible, using only a small set of trait dimensions. Many personality psychologists agree that its five domains capture the most important, basic individual differences in personality traits and that many alternative trait models can be conceptualized in terms of the Big Five/FFM structure. The goal of this article is to reference, organize, and comment on a variety of classic and contemporary papers related to the Big Five/FFM. This article begins with papers that introduce the Big Five/FFM structure, approach it from different theoretical perspectives, and consider possible objections to it (General Overviews, Theoretical Perspectives, and Critiques). Next, it discusses papers providing evidence for the Big Five/FFM as a model of basic trait structure (Big Five/FFM Structure).
    [Show full text]
  • A Lexical Approach to Collective Personality Using Nongroup Members As Raters
    Walden University ScholarWorks Walden Dissertations and Doctoral Studies Walden Dissertations and Doctoral Studies Collection 2021 A Lexical Approach to Collective Personality Using Nongroup Members as Raters Robert Cameron Black Walden University Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.waldenu.edu/dissertations Part of the Personality and Social Contexts Commons This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Walden Dissertations and Doctoral Studies Collection at ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Walden Dissertations and Doctoral Studies by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Walden University College of Social and Behavioral Sciences This is to certify that the doctoral dissertation by Robert Black has been found to be complete and satisfactory in all respects, and that any and all revisions required by the review committee have been made. Review Committee Dr. Sandra Mahoney, Committee Chairperson, Psychology Faculty Dr. Brandy Benson, Committee Member, Psychology Faculty Dr. Lisa Scharff, University Reviewer, Psychology Faculty Chief Academic Officer and Provost Sue Subocz, Ph.D. Walden University 2020 Abstract A Lexical Approach to Collective Personality Using Nongroup Members as Raters by Robert Black Dissertation Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Clinical Psychology Walden University February 2021 Abstract The construct of personality exhibits utility in predicting group outcomes. However, quantifying a group personality construct has almost exclusively been accomplished by aggregating individual group member five-factor personality scores to derive the group personality score. This type of operationalizing rests on the assumption that collective personality exists and has a five-factor structure, makes results context specific, and limits cross-comparison of different types of groups.
    [Show full text]
  • The Economics and Psychology of Personality Traits
    IZA DP No. 3333 The Economics and Psychology of Personality Traits Lex Borghans Angela Lee Duckworth James J. Heckman Bas ter Weel DISCUSSION PAPER SERIES DISCUSSION PAPER February 2008 Forschungsinstitut zur Zukunft der Arbeit Institute for the Study of Labor The Economics and Psychology of Personality Traits Lex Borghans ROA, Maastricht University and IZA Angela Lee Duckworth University of Pennsylvania James J. Heckman University of Chicago, American Bar Foundation, University College Dublin and IZA Bas ter Weel CPB Netherlands Bureau for Economic Policy Analysis, UNU-MERIT, Maastricht University and IZA Discussion Paper No. 3333 February 2008 IZA P.O. Box 7240 53072 Bonn Germany Phone: +49-228-3894-0 Fax: +49-228-3894-180 E-mail: [email protected] Any opinions expressed here are those of the author(s) and not those of IZA. Research published in this series may include views on policy, but the institute itself takes no institutional policy positions. The Institute for the Study of Labor (IZA) in Bonn is a local and virtual international research center and a place of communication between science, politics and business. IZA is an independent nonprofit organization supported by Deutsche Post World Net. The center is associated with the University of Bonn and offers a stimulating research environment through its international network, workshops and conferences, data service, project support, research visits and doctoral program. IZA engages in (i) original and internationally competitive research in all fields of labor economics, (ii) development of policy concepts, and (iii) dissemination of research results and concepts to the interested public. IZA Discussion Papers often represent preliminary work and are circulated to encourage discussion.
    [Show full text]
  • A Conjectural History of Cambridge Analytica's Eugenic Roots
    ARTICLE https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-020-0505-5 OPEN Sordid genealogies: a conjectural history of Cambridge Analytica’s eugenic roots ✉ Michael Wintroub1 “Sordid Genealogies: A Conjectural History of Cambridge Analytica’s Eugenic Roots” explores the history of the methods employed by Cambridge Analytica to influence the 2016 US presidential election. It focuses on the history of psychometric analysis, trait psychology, the 1234567890():,; lexical hypothesis and multivariate factor analysis, and how they developed in close con- junction with the history of eugenics. More particularly, it will analyze how the work of Francis Galton, Ludwig Klages, Charles Spearman, and Raymond Cattell (among others) contributed to the manifold translations between statistics, the pseudoscience of eugenics, the politics of Trumpism, and the data driven psychology of the personality championed by Cambridge Analytica. 1 University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA. ✉email: [email protected] HUMANITIES AND SOCIAL SCIENCES COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 7:41 | https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-020-0505-5 1 ARTICLE HUMANITIES AND SOCIAL SCIENCES COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-020-0505-5 translation of the Old Testament from Hebrew to Greek, they Once King Ptolemy gathered seventy-two elders and put might, in the same breath, have contested the veracity of its them into seventy-two houses, and did not reveal to them content. why he gathered them. He entered each room and said to “ ” Similar conundrums have troubled the ways we have thought them: Write the Torah of Moses your rabbi for me . The about translation since the invention of fiction as a genre in the Holy One, blessed by He gave counsel into each of their early modern period.
    [Show full text]
  • Personality Trait Structure of Shanxi Business-Tycoons in the Chinese Ming and Qing Dynasties
    www.ccsenet.org/ijps International Journal of Psychological Studies Vol. 2, No. 2; December 2010 Personality Trait Structure of Shanxi Business-tycoons in the Chinese Ming and Qing Dynasties Jianjun Zheng (Corresponding author) Institute of Political Science, Chinese Academy of Social Sciences NO. 5 Jian Guo Men Nei Da Jie, Dong Cheng District, Beijing 100732, China E-mail: [email protected] Jiping Yang Department of Psychology, Shanxi University NO. 92 Wucheng Lu, Taiyuan 030006, China E-mail: [email protected] Hao Feng Shanxi Finance and Taxation College NO. 25 Qianfeng North Road, Wanbolin District, Taiyuan 030024, China E-mail: [email protected] Abstract Taking lexical hypothesis of personality trait theory as methodology, this paper attempts to give a quantitative analysis on the personality structure of Shanxi business-tycoons in the Chinese Ming and Qing dynasties (PSSM) from psychology views. Exploratory factor analysis (EFA) of data from a sample of 152 effective questionnaires shows that their personalities consisted of four factors, including “honest and righteous”, “wise and confident”, “calm and persevering” and “determined and promising”. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) of data from another sample of 305 effective questionnaires further confirmed PSSM factorial validity, and fitting indexes were all reasonable. The above results have indicated the cultural inheritance of personality trait as well as the uniqueness and continuity of regional personality trait. Keywords: Chinese personality, Shanxi business-tycoons in the Ming and Qing Dynasty, Personality structure 1. Introduction Jin Merchants in the Ming and Qing dynasties usually refer to the Shanxi merchants of the Ming and Qing dynasties in ancient China.
    [Show full text]
  • The Structure of Phenotypic Personality Traits
    The Structure of Phenotypic Personality Traits Lewis R. Goldberg This personal historical article traces the development of One of the first of these investigators was L. L. Thur- the Big-Five factor structure, whose growing acceptance stone, a pioneer in the development of factor analysis; by personality researchers has profoundly influenced the the report of his initial findings reads today with almost scientific study of individual differences. The roots of this haunting clairvoyance: taxonomy lie in the lexical hypothesis and the insights of Sir Francis Gallon, the prescience of L. L. Thurstone, the Sixty adjectives that are in common use for describing people legacy of Raymond B. Cattell, and the seminal analyses . were given to each of 1300 raters. Each rater was asked to of Tupes and Christal. Paradoxically, the present popu- think of a person whom he knew well and to underline every adjective that he might use in a conversational description of larity of this model owes much to its many critics, each that person. ... the ... correlation . coefficients for the of whom tried to replace it, but failed. In reaction, there sixty personality traits were then analyzed by means of multiple have been a number of attempts to assimilate other models factor methods and we found that five [italics added] factors are into the five-factor structure. Lately, some practical im- sufficient to account for the coefficients.. plications of the emerging consensus can be seen in such It is of considerable psychological interest to know that the contexts as personnel selection and classification. whole list of sixty adjectives can be accounted for by postulating on\y five independent common factors.
    [Show full text]
  • The Big-Five Trait Taxonomy: History, Measurement, and Theoretical
    The Big-Five Trait Taxonomy: History, Measurement, and Theoretical Perspectives Oliver P. John and Sanjay Srivastava University of California at Berkeley Running head: Big Five Trait Taxonomy Final draft: March 5, 1999 Author's Address: Oliver P. John Department of Psychology University of California, MC 1650 Berkeley, CA 94720-1650 W: (510) 642-2178; H: 540-7159; Fax: 643-9334 Email: [email protected]; [email protected] To appear in L. Pervin and O.P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford (in press). 2 Taxonomy is always a contentious issue because the world does not come to us in neat little packages (S. J. Gould, 1981, p. 158). Personality has been conceptualized from a variety of theoretical perspectives, and at various levels of abstraction or breadth (John, Hampson, & Goldberg, 1991; McAdams, 1995). Each of these levels has made unique contributions to our understanding of individual differences in behavior and experience. However, the number of personality traits, and scales designed to measure them, escalated without an end in sight (Goldberg, 1971). Researchers, as well as practitioners in the field of personality assessment, were faced with a bewildering array of personality scales from which to choose, with little guidance and no overall rationale at hand. What made matters worse was that scales with the same name often measure concepts that are not the same, and scales with different names often measure concepts that are quite similar. Although diversity and scientific pluralism are useful, the systematic accumulation of findings and the communication among researchers became difficult amidst the Babel of concepts and scales.
    [Show full text]
  • Paradigm Shift to the Big-Five Trait Taxonomy
    1 John, O. P., Naumann, L., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 114-158). New York, NY: Guilford. Paradigm Shift to the Integrative Big-Five Trait Taxonomy: History, Measurement, and Conceptual Issues Oliver P. John, Laura P. Naumann, and Christopher J. Soto University of California at Berkeley Running head: Big Five Trait Taxonomy Draft: March 4, 2008 Corresponding Author's Address: Oliver P. John Department of Psychology University of California, MC 1650 Berkeley, CA 94720-1650 Phone: (510) 847-6271; Fax: 510-643-9334 Email: [email protected] To appear in O.P. John, R.W. Robins, and L.A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (3rd ed.). New York: Guilford (in press). 2 Since the first version of this chapter (John, 1990) was completed in the late 1980s, the field of personality trait research has changed dramatically. At that time, the Big Five personality dimensions, now seemingly ubiquitous, were hardly known. Researchers, as well as practitioners in the field of personality assessment, were faced with a bewildering array of personality scales from which to choose, with little guidance and no organizing theory or framework at hand. As Allport once put it, “each assessor has his own pet units and uses a pet battery of diagnostic devices” (1958, p. 258). What made matters worse was that scales with the same name might measure concepts that were quite different, and scales with different names might measure concepts that were quite similar.
    [Show full text]