Chapter 4 Lexicon Reading and Lecture Notes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Chapter 4 Lexicon Reading and Lecture Notes LING 650 Class Notes Ch.4 by Angelina Rubina & Laura Redden 1 CHAPTER 4 LEXICON READING AND LECTURE NOTES Lexicon – the language user’s mental dictionary. Do speakers memorize entire complex word-forms (e.g. readable, reads, washable)? their component morphemes (e.g. read, -able, -s, wash)? both? Three major positions on the content of the lexicon: a morpheme lexicon (corresponds to the morpheme-based model) contains simple, monomorphemic elements (i.e. roots and affixes) and idiosyncratic complex words (which are still created by rule); a strict word-form lexicon (consistent with the word-based model) contains all complex word-forms (predictable and idiosyncratic); a moderate word-form lexicon (consistent with the word-based model) potentially contains word-forms, morphemes and derived stems. Criticism of the chapter (according to Stan). Some of the problems set up in the chapter were sort of faux-problems used in order to make an argument and distinguish between derivation and inflection. This questions about the difference was used as a sort of “red herring” to lead you down the authors’ argumentation path. 4.1 A MORPHEME LEXICON Advantages of a morpheme lexicon Parallelism between syntax (sentences consist of words), morphology (words consist of morphemes), phonology (morphemes consist of phonemes). Elegant description of language structure: it is maximally economical because some word-forms follow the same morphological patterns in inflection, e.g. writer, reader + -s; in derivation (e.g. read + -er, -able). Problems for a morpheme lexicon Example Non-compositional (unpredictable) meaning Reader (British academic title) does not mean of some derived lexemes read + -er (as in read-er ‘any person who reads’) LING 650 Class Notes Ch.4 by Angelina Rubina & Laura Redden 2 Lack of morpheme segmentability: base modification German plurals e.g. Mutter (SING) vs. Mütter (PL) ‘mother(s)’ Serbian noun paradigm (cumulative/fused cumulative expression expression of number and case), e.g. ovc-a an affix expresses two different (NOMINATIVE, SINGULAR) morphological meanings Finnish third person singular: zero expression/a zero affix oli-n ‘I was’ morphological meaning corresponds to oli-t ‘you were’ no overt form oli ‘he/she was’ oli-mme ‘we were’ oli-tte ‘you (PL) were’ oli-vat ‘they were’ Lezgian non-absolutive noun paradigm, e.g. empty morphs sew-re-n morphemes that have form but no meanings The morpheme lexicon model is on pg. 61 (Figure 4.1) Syntax (sentence) Morphology (words and parts) Phonology (sounds) Spell out The text says that language users don’t usually memorize complex words, but in the blog, Ruthanne posed a question about idioms and phrasal verbs, or combinations of words that are fixed. An example is “let the cat out of the bag”. You can’t say “let the cat out of the box”. So, is this all storied in our brains. Well, we do have it stored that it cannot be plural (cats), for example, but it could be negated (“let’s not let the cat out of the bag”). This question about units that are larger than words points us towards the need for a word based model. In other words, idioms provide counterexamples to the morpheme based model. 4.2 A STRICT WORD-FORM LEXICON Advantages of a strict word-form lexicon The meaning of a word-form does not have to be compositional. LING 650 Class Notes Ch.4 by Angelina Rubina & Laura Redden 3 Morpheme segmentability becomes a lesser problem because word-forms (not morphemes) are the basic units of the lexicon. Unproductive (not used to create new words) morphological patterns do not pose a problem because such word-forms are listed in the lexicon. It is more cognitively realistic than a morpheme lexicon (the more often speakers encounter word-forms, the more easily these word-forms are remembered). Problems for a strict word-form lexicon Examples Lack of elegant description It lists all words Speakers are unlikely to memorize all word- Turkish forms in agglutinative (rich inflectional) languages Some morphological patterns refer to In Dutch, the allomorphy is conditioned by morpheme-based structures the morphological structure of the base: past participles are marked by the prefix ge- (e.g. spreken ‘speak’, ge-sproken ‘spoken’) unless the verb bears a derivational prefix such as be- (e.g. be-spreken ‘discuss’, be-sproken ‘discussed’, not *ge-be-sproken) Some phonological patterns refer to In Italian, casa ‘house’ is pronounced with [z] morpheme-based structures because the s occurs between vowels vs. asimmetrico pronounced with [s], not [z], because of morpheme boundary 4.3 RECONCILING WORD-FORMS AND MORPHEMES A moderate word-form lexicon – morphological structure is fundamentally word-based, but morphological patterns (that are not restricted to roots and affixes) represent secondary generalizations. Thus, both word-forms (primary role) and morphological patterns (secondary role) are stored in the lexicon. The challenges for a moderate word-form lexicon: 1) the need to decide how established a word must be to be memorized by speakers; 2) the need to account for how speakers recall words stored in the lexicon. Two types of lexical access (ways to recall a word): decomposition route – by breaking up a word into morphemes (e.g. in-, sane); direct route – by retrieving a complex word-form directly (e.g. insane). LING 650 Class Notes Ch.4 by Angelina Rubina & Laura Redden 4 Some students asked about how we access the lexicon if we are non-native speakers of a language when we are learning the language. Do non-native speakers use a decomposition route or a direct route more frequently? Stan responded that this is probably individualistic, but implied that non-native speakers may be more likely to use the decomposition route and break up a word into morphemes, while a native speaker might have a larger lexicon and just store the words directly. This would depend on speaker competence and the size of her or his lexicon. Speakers try both routes simultaneously. The factors determining which route ‘wins’: Factors Direction of influence relative token frequency of word-form more frequent word-form storage segmentability less segmentable word-form storage allomorphy more effect on base word-form storage individual speaker differences depends? non-native speaker more decomposition route Neologisms – attested novel lexemes that were not observed before. Occasionalisms (or nonce formations) – neologisms that do not really catch on. In the discussion of neologisms and occadionalisms, we considered the word “breathatarian”. This is a completely believable and understandable word, but it is a neologism because it has not been used before. It comes from the word “vegetarian” and “breath” is substituted for “veg.” In neologisms that are neologized with a derivational morpheme, it must be understood where the word came from. For example, “alcoholic” becoming “shopoholic” and “chocoholic”. It is interesting, because the “holic” is not a morpheme, but “alcohol” and “ic” are. Here the “holic” was taken and used to form neologisms, even though the morphmes are not broken up like that. CONCLUSIONS the structure of the lexicon is an active area of research; there is increasing evidence for a moderate word-form lexicon (with the balance in favor of word-based storage); morphology is fundamentally word-based. LING 650 Class Notes Ch.4 by Angelina Rubina & Laura Redden 5 CHAPTER 4 LEXICON LECTURE NOTES Review of Word Based vs. Morpheme Based Models. There was a presentation of Ch 3 CE 4 by Dennis on Tagalog in which he used a word based analysis to analyze Tagalog and explain a morphophonological problem in which the sound and stress indicated a change in word form. If you were to use a morpheme based model, you would need to apply an auto-segmental analysis. Stan explained how question formation with subject-aux inversion realizes itself as a prosodic pattern lying over the whole sentence. “An autosegmental reflex” The wh-word or rising intonation at the end of the sentence. An example is the pitch shift for imperatives in Northern Italian. Stan encouraged us to check out the additional paper that he had posted which analyzes two suffixes, one for passives and one for statives. Examples were given in this article to show that derivational morphology is needed to understand some suffixation, while inflectional morphology is needed to understand the other. This article was provided as a complement to the chapter, because the chapter appears to break apart the two concepts as sort of black and white, either/or issues for the sake of an argument, but examples from the real world shows that it is more nuanced and complicated. We discussed these words: “striped” (one syllable) versus “strip/ed” (two syllables) In Keiko’s language variety (Appalacian?), the two syllable form is grammatical, but most classmates favored the single syllable forms. In fact, the two syllable form comes from history. In English, we have several words in which we use both (“learned” versus “learn-ed”). We would use learned as past tense of learn (inflected), but then learn-ed as a sort of adjective to describe a person (derived). In addition, we have “mark-ed-ness,” and it seems that we tend to add a syllable when we use these words as adjectives. In addition, we discussed the difference between “blessed” and “bless-ed” and thought that it was mostly contextual. The two syllable form appears to come from an older form of English, and is used in more religious contexts. According to Stan, inflectional forms are not supposed to be stored independently. Zero-affix were discussed, as Laura predicted they were more common in third person singular across world languages. Scott and Micheal agreed based on historical linguistics background, and Stan explained why the third person singular was the least marked and therefore made sense as the most likely zero-affix for world languages.
Recommended publications
  • The Generative Lexicon
    The Generative Lexicon James Pustejovsky" Computer Science Department Brandeis University In this paper, I will discuss four major topics relating to current research in lexical seman- tics: methodology, descriptive coverage, adequacy of the representation, and the computational usefulness of representations. In addressing these issues, I will discuss what I think are some of the central problems facing the lexical semantics community, and suggest ways of best ap- proaching these issues. Then, I will provide a method for the decomposition of lexical categories and outline a theory of lexical semantics embodying a notion of cocompositionality and type coercion, as well as several levels of semantic description, where the semantic load is spread more evenly throughout the lexicon. I argue that lexical decomposition is possible if it is per- formed generatively. Rather than assuming a fixed set of primitives, I will assume a fixed number of generative devices that can be seen as constructing semantic expressions. I develop a theory of Qualia Structure, a representation language for lexical items, which renders much lexical ambiguity in the lexicon unnecessary, while still explaining the systematic polysemy that words carry. Finally, I discuss how individual lexical structures can be integrated into the larger lexical knowledge base through a theory of lexical inheritance. This provides us with the necessary principles of global organization for the lexicon, enabling us to fully integrate our natural language lexicon into a conceptual whole. 1. Introduction I believe we have reached an interesting turning point in research, where linguistic studies can be informed by computational tools for lexicology as well as an appre- ciation of the computational complexity of large lexical databases.
    [Show full text]
  • THE LEXICON: a SYSTEM of MATRICES of LEXICAL UNITS and THEIR PROPERTIES ~ Harry H
    THE LEXICON: A SYSTEM OF MATRICES OF LEXICAL UNITS AND THEIR PROPERTIES ~ Harry H. Josselson - Uriel Weinreich /I/~ in discussing the fact that at one time many American scholars relied on either the discipline of psychology or sociology for the resolution of semantic problems~ comments: In Soviet lexicology, it seems, neither the tra- ditionalists~ who have been content to work with the categories of classical rhetoric and 19th- century historical semantics~ nor the critical lexicologists in search of better conceptual tools, have ever found reason to doubt that linguistics alone is centrally responsible for the investiga- tion of the vocabulary of languages. /2/ This paper deals with a certain conceptual tool, the matrix, which linguists can use for organizing a lexicon to insure that words will be described (coded) with consistency, that is~ to insure that questions which have been asked about certain words will be asked for all words in the same class, regardless of the fact that they may be more difficult to answer for some than for others. The paper will also dis- cuss certain new categories~ beyond those of classical rhetoric~ which have been introduced into lexicology. i. INTRODUCTION The research in automatic translation brought about by the introduction of computers into the technology has ~The research described herein has b&en supported by the In- formation Systems Branch of the Office of Naval Research. The present work is an amplification of a paper~ "The Lexicon: A Matri~ of Le~emes and Their Properties"~ contributed to the Conference on Mathematical Linguistics at Budapest-Balatonsza- badi, September 6-i0~ 1968.
    [Show full text]
  • CS474 Natural Language Processing Semantic Analysis Caveats Introduction to Lexical Semantics
    CS474 Natural Language Processing Semantic analysis Last class Assigning meanings to linguistic utterances – History Compositional semantics: we can derive the – Tiny intro to semantic analysis meaning of the whole sentence from the meanings of the parts. Next lectures – Max ate a green apple. – Word sense disambiguation Relies on knowing: » Background from linguistics – the meaning of individual words Lexical semantics – how the meanings of individual words combine to form » On-line resources the meaning of groups of words » Computational approaches [next class] – how it all fits in with syntactic analysis Caveats Introduction to lexical semantics Problems with a compositional approach Lexical semantics is the study of – the systematic meaning-related connections among – a former congressman words and – a toy elephant – the internal meaning-related structure of each word – kicked the bucket Lexeme – an individual entry in the lexicon – a pairing of a particular orthographic and phonological form with some form of symbolic meaning representation Sense: the lexeme’s meaning component Lexicon: a finite list of lexemes Lexical semantic relations: Dictionary entries homonymy right adj. located nearer the right hand esp. Homonyms: words that have the same form and unrelated being on the right when facing the same direction meanings – Instead, a bank1 can hold the investments in a custodial account as the observer. in the client’s name. – But as agriculture burgeons on the east bank2, the river will shrink left adj. located nearer to this side of the body even more. than the right. Homophones: distinct lexemes with a shared red n. the color of blood or a ruby. pronunciation – E.g.
    [Show full text]
  • The Art of Lexicography - Niladri Sekhar Dash
    LINGUISTICS - The Art of Lexicography - Niladri Sekhar Dash THE ART OF LEXICOGRAPHY Niladri Sekhar Dash Linguistic Research Unit, Indian Statistical Institute, Kolkata, India Keywords: Lexicology, linguistics, grammar, encyclopedia, normative, reference, history, etymology, learner’s dictionary, electronic dictionary, planning, data collection, lexical extraction, lexical item, lexical selection, typology, headword, spelling, pronunciation, etymology, morphology, meaning, illustration, example, citation Contents 1. Introduction 2. Definition 3. The History of Lexicography 4. Lexicography and Allied Fields 4.1. Lexicology and Lexicography 4.2. Linguistics and Lexicography 4.3. Grammar and Lexicography 4.4. Encyclopedia and lexicography 5. Typological Classification of Dictionary 5.1. General Dictionary 5.2. Normative Dictionary 5.3. Referential or Descriptive Dictionary 5.4. Historical Dictionary 5.5. Etymological Dictionary 5.6. Dictionary of Loanwords 5.7. Encyclopedic Dictionary 5.8. Learner's Dictionary 5.9. Monolingual Dictionary 5.10. Special Dictionaries 6. Electronic Dictionary 7. Tasks for Dictionary Making 7.1. Panning 7.2. Data Collection 7.3. Extraction of lexical items 7.4. SelectionUNESCO of Lexical Items – EOLSS 7.5. Mode of Lexical Selection 8. Dictionary Making: General Dictionary 8.1. HeadwordsSAMPLE CHAPTERS 8.2. Spelling 8.3. Pronunciation 8.4. Etymology 8.5. Morphology and Grammar 8.6. Meaning 8.7. Illustrative Examples and Citations 9. Conclusion Acknowledgements ©Encyclopedia of Life Support Systems (EOLSS) LINGUISTICS - The Art of Lexicography - Niladri Sekhar Dash Glossary Bibliography Biographical Sketch Summary The art of dictionary making is as old as the field of linguistics. People started to cultivate this field from the very early age of our civilization, probably seven to eight hundred years before the Christian era.
    [Show full text]
  • The Combinatory Morphemic Lexicon
    The Combinatory Morphemic Lexicon Cem Bozsahin∗ Middle East Technical University Grammars that expect words from the lexicon may be at odds with the transparent projection of syntactic and semantic scope relations of smaller units. We propose a morphosyntactic framework based on Combinatory Categorial Grammar that provides flexible constituency, flexible category consistency, and lexical projection of morphosyntactic properties and attachment to grammar in order to establish a morphemic grammar-lexicon. These mechanisms provide enough expressive power in the lexicon to formulate semantically transparent specifications without the necessity to confine structure forming to words and phrases. For instance, bound morphemes as lexical items can have phrasal scope or word scope, independent of their attachment characteristics but consistent with their semantics. The controls can be attuned in the lexicon to language-particular properties. The result is a transparent interface of inflectional morphology, syntax, and semantics. We present a computational system and show the application of the framework to English and Turkish. 1. Introduction The study presented in this article is concerned with the integrated representation and processing of inflectional morphology, syntax, and semantics in a unified grammar ar- chitecture. An important issue in such integration is mismatches in morphological, syntactic, and semantic bracketings. The problem was first noted in derivational mor- phology. Williams (1981) provided examples from English; the semantic bracketings in (1a–2a) are in conflict with the morphological bracketings in (1b–2b). (1) a. -ity b. hydro hydro electric electric -ity (2) a. -ing b. G¨odel G¨odel number number -ing If the problem were confined to derivational morphology, we could avoid it by making derivational morphology part of the lexicon that does not interact with gram- mar.
    [Show full text]
  • Changing the Rules: Why the Monolingual Learner's Dictionary Should Move Away from the Native-Speaker Tradition
    127 Changing the rules: Why the monolingual learner's dictionary should move away from the native-speaker tradition Michael Rundell This paper starts from a recognition that the reference needs of people learning English are not adequately met by existing monolingual learner's dictionaries (MLDs). Either the dictionaries themselves are deficient, or their target users have not yet learned how to use them effectively: whichever view one takes — and the truth is probably somewhere in between — it is difficult to escape the conclusion that the MLD's full potential as a language-learning resource has not yet been realized. This is recognized eg by Béjoint 1981: 219: "Monolingual dic­ tionaries are not used as fully as they should be ... many students are not even aware of the riches that their monolingual dictionaries contain" — a view which has been consistently borne out by any user-research we have conducted at Longman. There is a variety of responses to this situation. Compilers of MLDs may feel a certain exasperation with the 'ungrateful' students who fail to recognize the very real progress that has been made in adapting conventional dictionaries to their special needs. More positively, a growing awareness on the part of teachers of the importance of developing students' reference skills (eg Béjoint 1981: 220, Rossner 1985: 99 f., Whitcut 1986: 111) is complemented by a clear commit­ ment on the part of dictionary publishers to make their products as accessible and user-friendly as possible. These two developments seem to offer the beguiling prospect of a scenario in which ever more helpful MLDs are ever more skilfully exploited by their users — thus resolving, to everyone's satisfaction, the problem identified at the beginning of this paper.
    [Show full text]
  • Part 4: Lexical Semantics 2
    Natural Language Processing Part 4: lexical semantics 2 Marco Maggini Tecnologie per l'elaborazione del linguaggio Lexical semantics • A lexicon generally has a highly structured form ▫ It stores the meanings and uses of each word ▫ It encodes the relations between words and meanings • A lexeme is the minimal unit represented in the lexicon ▫ It pairs a stem (the orthographic/phonological form chosen to represent words) with a symbolic form for meaning representation (sense) • A dictionary is a kind of lexicon where meanings are expressed through definitions and examples son noun a boy or man in relation to either or both of his parents. • a male offspring of an animal. • a male descendant : the sons of Adam. • ( the Son) (in Christian belief) the second person of the Trinity; Christ. • a man considered in relation to his native country or area : one of Nevada's most famous sons. • a man regarded as the product of a particular person, influence, or environment : sons of the French Revolution. • (also my son) used by an elder person as a form of address for a boy or young man : “You're on private land, son.” 3 Marco Maggini Tecnologie per l'elaborazione del linguaggio Lexicons & dictionaries • Definitions in dictionaries exploit words and they may be circular (a word definition uses words whose definitions exploit that word) right adj. 1. of, relating to, situated on, or being the side of the body which is away from the side on which the heart is mostly located 2. located nearer to the right hand than to the left 3.
    [Show full text]
  • The Project, Its Ratiale and Processes,'Lrcludes A,Descrip-Tionf
    -QED 160 099,. AUTHOR Chenhall, Robert . TITLE The Onomastic Dctopus Repdif. No.3. INSTITUTION California State Univ., Fuller 5c ender .-SPONS N ational Endowment for puB DA NOTE 1 p-. BURS PcE- MF-$0. 3C-S1. Bins Post age DESCRI TORS, k-ctivities; *Catalcgivg; ctaaficetion, Ii'a.grams *Tnf ormati n Needs; *Museums; ru jects; *Subject.. Index Ter *Bysteus Analysis; TheSauti ABSTRACT Itoivities and information -in-museums and .a project titi ertaken,',Xithe Margaret -Woodbury. Strong-Museum to develop syStematic solutiOrp to' problems in cataloging museumcoliections-aze described. Museum-,eibtiwities Are grouped in three categories:41) initial.;--anisition -.:accession, registricn, identification, and restoration; (2)ongo- ng- -rearcb, exhibition, and conservation efforts; and (3) terminal_ -de-accessioniitg. Theongoing are the.,most lizportant'of the three y.pes.of _activities.. In order to carry out theseactivitieseffictitly, all. the information required for each- defined activity must be available at the right tine avd Iladet however- at the pre-sent time there are no systems cfnomenclature that are generally accepled as a basis for defining andidentifying - man-made artifacts.17,5 _.meet this need, this project focused on the creation of a the saur for use at the Strong Museum. This Standardized worpd_l'st, or lexicon, includes, words andphxases,.-and shows synonymous anhierarchical word ,-relationshipi and is not a panacea for all suseux. cataloging dependencies. While_it .1 qrpbleuls, it can serve as a commonstarting Loint. in. identificati activities which are crucial to the catalogin4 process. Asummaryof the project, its ratiale and processes,'Lrcludes a,descrip-tionf. the lexicon with examp es.(BIM) :/- * # *4$4*] x * * * * * * * ** Reproductions supped by EARS arethe hest that can be made the originaldocument.
    [Show full text]
  • SLIDE – a Sentiment Lexicon of Common Idioms
    SLIDE – a Sentiment Lexicon of Common Idioms Charles Jochim, Francesca Bonin, Roy Bar-Haim, Noam Slonim IBM Research fcharlesj,[email protected], froybar,[email protected] Abstract Idiomatic expressions are problematic for most sentiment analysis approaches, which rely on words as the basic linguistic unit. Compositional solutions for phrase sentiment are not able to handle idioms correctly because their sentiment is not derived from the sentiment of the individual words. Previous work has explored the importance of idioms for sentiment analysis, but has not addressed the breadth of idiomatic expressions in English. In this paper we present an approach for collecting sentiment annotation of idiomatic multiword expressions using crowdsourcing. We collect 10 annotations for each idiom and the aggregated label is shown to have good agreement with expert annotations. We describe the resulting publicly available lexicon and how it captures sentiment strength and ambiguity. The Sentiment Lexicon of IDiomatic Expressions (SLIDE) is much larger than previous idiom lexicons. The lexicon includes 5,000 frequently occurring idioms, as estimated from a large English corpus. The idioms were selected from Wiktionary, and over 40% of them were labeled as sentiment-bearing. Keywords: Idiom, Lexicon, Sentiment Analysis 1. Introduction of magnitude larger than previous idiom sentiment lexi- cons and focuses specifically on the most frequently used Multiword expressions (MWE) are a key challenge in Natu- idioms.1 In creating this resource, we are somewhat ag- ral Language Processing (Sag et al., 2002). Among MWEs, nostic to the exact definition of idiom. We are more gener- idioms are often defined as non-compositional multiword ally interested in sentiment analysis that can handle MWEs.
    [Show full text]
  • Ontology and the Lexicon
    Ontology and the Lexicon Graeme Hirst Department of Computer Science, University of Toronto, Toronto M5S 3G4, Ontario, Canada; [email protected] Summary. A lexicon is a linguistic object and hence is not the same thing as an ontology, which is non-linguistic. Nonetheless, word senses are in many ways similar to ontological concepts and the relationships found between word senses resemble the relationships found between concepts. Although the arbitrary and semi-arbitrary distinctions made by natural lan- guages limit the degree to which these similarities can be exploited, a lexicon can nonetheless serve in the development of an ontology, especially in a technical domain. 1 Lexicons and lexical knowledge 1.1 Lexicons A lexicon is a list of words in a language—a vocabulary—along with some knowl- edge of how each word is used. A lexicon may be general or domain-specific; we might have, for example, a lexicon of several thousand common words of English or German, or a lexicon of the technical terms of dentistry in some language. The words that are of interest are usually open-class or content words, such as nouns, verbs, and adjectives, rather than closed-class or grammatical function words, such as articles, pronouns, and prepositions, whose behaviour is more tightly bound to the grammar of the language. A lexicon may also include multi-word expressions such as fixed phrases (by and large), phrasal verbs (tear apart), and other common expressions (merry Christmas!; teach someone ’s grandmother to suck eggs; Elvis has left the building). ! " Each word or phrase in a lexicon is described in a lexical entry; exactly what is included in each entry depends on the purpose of the particular lexicon.
    [Show full text]
  • A Tool Kit for Lexicon Building
    A TOOL KIT FOR LEXICON BUILDING Thomas E. Ahlswede Computer Science Department Illinois Institute of Technology Chicago, Illinois 6e616, USA The interactive lexicon builder will ABSTRACT be the basis for a fully automatic lexicon builder which uses Sager's Linguistic This paper describes a set of String Parser (LSP) to parse machine- interactive routines that can be used to readable text into a relational network create, maintain, and update a computer based on a modified version of Werner's lexicon. The routines are available to NTQ (Modification-Taxonomy-Queueing) the user as a set of commands resembling a schema. Initially this program will be simple operating system. The lexicon pro- applied to Webster's Seventh Collegiate duced by this system is based on lexi- Dictionary and the Longman Dictionary of cal-semantic relations, but is compatible Contemporary English, both of which are with a variety of other models of lexicon available in machine-readable form. structure. The lexicon builder is suit- able for the generation of moderate-sized vocabularies and has been used to construct a lexicon for a small medical expert system. A future version of the LEXICAL-SENANTIC RELATIONS lexicon builder will create a much larger lexicon by parsing definitions from The semantic component of the lexicon machine-readable dictionaries. produced by this system consists princi- pally of a network of lexical-semantic relations. That is, the meaning of a word in the lexicon is indicated as far as possible by its relationships with other words. These relations often have seman- tic content themselves and thus contribute INTRODUCTION to the definition of the words they link.
    [Show full text]
  • Lexicography, Linguistics, and Minority Languages
    JASO 29/3 (1998): 231-242 LEXICOGRAPHY, LINGUISTICS, AND MINORITY LANGUAGES BRUCE CONNELL Introduction THERE is a tendency to assume that anything and everything to do with language comes within the purview of linguistics, which is often characterized as the scien­ tific study of language in all its aspects. For the professional linguist today, this perhaps needs to be amended, as there seem to be many aspects of language, or its scientific study, which are of little concern to linguistics. This could be interpreted in two ways: one, as a signal that some aspects of language are not of sufficient importance to warrant the attention of the trained linguist; or two, as a recognition of the limitations of the discipline, that there exist important aspects of language which we don't yet have the tools to investigate properly_ Compiling and editing dictionaries-Iexicography-certainly has to do with language, but judging by the lack of attention devoted to this practice in linguistics programmes and textbooks, one might be forgiven for concluding that it is either relatively insignificant or a task of such monumental complexity that it is still beyond our grasp. For example, no definition or discussion of lexicography is found in standard introductory text­ books in linguistics, such as Hockett (1954), Robins (1971) or O'Grady et al. (1992), and only passing mention is made in Fromkin and Rodman (1988: 124), who define lexicography simply as 'the editing or making of a dictionary', the aim of which is to prescribe rather than describe the words
    [Show full text]