ADVENTURES OF A PSYCHOLOGIST

In this enlightening biography, award-winning academic psychologist Michael Corballis tells the story of how the field of cognitive psychology evolved and the controversies and anecdotes that occurred along the way. Since the Second World War, psychology has undergone several scientific movements, from behaviourism to cognitive psychology and finally to neuroscience. In this fascinating biography, Corballis recounts his career as a researcher who played a part in these monumental changes in psychology. Beginning with his boarding-school education in New Zealand, Corballis goes on to recount his meanders through university education, his behavioural research into mirror- image discriminations in pigeons, the uprising of the “cognitive revolution” amidst 1960s counterculture and his switch to become a cognitive psychologist, his research into brain asymmetry and the evolution of language and its origin of manual gestures, and the development of mental time travel in animals. Featuring stories of prominent scientists who were integral in psychology’s biggest discoveries and insight into the heated debates and controversies in psychology during a time of great scientific and sociocultural change, this biography is a must-read for those interested in how psychology became established as a science.

Michael Corballis was Professor of Psychology at McGill University and the . He has published widely in experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. In 2003 he was appointed Officer of the New Zealand Order of Merit, and in 2016 he received the Rutherford Medal, New Zealand’s top scientific award.

ADVENTURES OF A PSYCHOLOGIST

Reflections on What Made Up the Mind

Michael Corballis First published 2021 by Routledge 2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN and by Routledge 52 Vanderbilt Avenue, New York, NY 10017 Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business © 2021 Michael Corballis The right of Michael Corballis to be identified as author of this work has been asserted by him in accordance with sections 77 and 78 of the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act 1988. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers. Trademark notice : Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe. British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data A catalog record for this book has been requested

ISBN: 978-0-367-42053-6 (hbk) ISBN: 978-0-367-42054-3 (pbk) ISBN: 978-0-367-82150-0 (ebk)

Typeset in Bembo by Apex CoVantage, LLC CONTENTS

Preface vii

1 Growing up 1

2 Floundering at university 10

3 Hello psychology 16

4 O God! O Montreal! 26

5 Rats and pigeons 35

6 Revolutions 42

7 The lopsided brain 51

8 Doing the splits 60

9 Mirrors 68

10 Gestures, gestures 75

11 On time 83

12 Chomsky, God, and language 92 vi Contents

13 Professional psychology and its discontents 101

14 Anti-science 111

15 A brief reckoning 119

Index 125 PREFACE

There comes a time when you realise you are actually part of history, a living fossil. I was also, I suppose, something of an interloper. My background and early career path had pointed me in quite different directions, and I stumbled across psychology more or less by chance, but somehow it stuck. And here I am still, an octogenarian, and a kind of de facto historian. Psychology as an autonomous academic discipline emerged in New Zealand after World War II, and I was part of it for much of that time – student, junior lecturer, lecturer, professor, emeritus professor, and researcher. Although sci- entific psychology had been well established elsewhere since the late 19th cen- tury, notably in the United States and Germany, it developed rapidly after World War II, spurred partly by advances in the biological sciences and partly as a response to the psychological trauma induced by the war itself. It also underwent quite dramatic changes in approach. Indeed, I scarcely recognise the psychology I was taught in the 1950s – for that matter, I scarcely recognise myself from that grey decade. Although my roots are in New Zealand, I have been witness to much that is international. I taught for some years at McGill University in Montreal, Canada, where Donald Hebb, Canada’s most illustrious psychologist, introduced the brain to psychology. I studied split-brained patients in the laboratory of Nobel Prize winner Roger Sperry in California, as well as in New Hampshire, Ohio, and Italy. I jostled with Noam Chomsky over the nature and evolution of language. I dabbled in brain imaging to try to understand how the two sides of the brain differ. I wrote about memory, brain asymmetry, vision, language, statistics – volumes of stuff, much of it swallowed in the catacombs of science and largely forgotten. Of course I met interesting people, some of whom surface in this book. I have set this account in an autobiographical frame, because my percep- tion of psychology’s tortuous advance toward becoming a science is coloured by viii Preface my own background and experience. I have more or less lived through Shake- speare’s seven ages of man, as recounted by Jaques in As You Like It , although I don’t remember the mewling of infancy, and puking persisted a bit longer, even into my student days. I still have teeth. Bubble reputation? Probably. Looking back, though, I can scarcely recognise any of my former selves, some of whom I wouldn’t wish to know. More important, though, were the cultural changes I witnessed – the early days of dull complacency followed by political and sexual revolutions, then an age of repressive political correctness, with signs now of a populist blowout. These have not only influenced my own demeanour and understanding but also psychology itself. Psychological science went through very different phases since I first encountered it. That is the real story I want to tell. Then there’s the problem of memory, which is fickle, to the point that one’s own account of past events can be very different from those of others. It is full of holes. Looking back through a photo album reveals images and people that seem utterly unfamiliar, as though from a different life. School reunions have much the same disorienting effect, and I now avoid them. A lot of memory is indeed fabricated, often seemingly designed to create you as a person rather than tell what you actually did, or were. Ronald Reagan is said to have based his recollec- tions of past heroics on war movies – this book may well suffer the same defect, although I have no claim to heroism. I make no claim to be inclusive, either, and should warn that I have always been an academic and researcher but never a clinician. There are no suggested remedies or diagnostic hints in this book for errant children, difficult in-laws, or mad dogs. My interest is fundamentally in how the mind works, to borrow a phrase from Steven Pinker, who tells me he once attended my classes. And I will drop other names. I apologise to any who may feel left out. I could probably write another version with entirely different characters; stirring the pot of mem- ory can throw up all sorts of things I have overlooked this time round. By the same token, though, I cannot list all the names of people who have profoundly influenced me – to attempt to do so might just make omissions even more stark. But thank you all anyway, so much. I must nevertheless specifically thank my own family. Barbara has been end- lessly patient and helpful. My son Paul is in the same game as a colleague at the University of Auckland and likes to claim that he taught me everything I know. He’s probably right. My other son Tim is a novelist and now also an academic involved with scientific communication. His wisdom is also appreciated. My three 11-year-old granddaughters, Simone, Lena, and Natasha, have also con- tributed hugely to my late life, mainly through their expertise with electronic devices and their sense of fun. 1 Growing up

It is customary to begin with premonitions – the events, thoughts, and ambitions that led to one’s ultimate calling. Alas, my own upbringing and early education gave little hint. Even so, it is probably worth relating something of the environ- ment in which I grew up, if only because it seems utterly unrelated to what hap- pened later on. Others more insightful than I may see clues as to my eventual turn to academia, but I can scarcely recognise myself in that long-ago place, and the memories that do come back seem astonishing and unreal. Was it really like that? So I offer my early years more as matters of historical and sociological inter- est than as casting any serious light on a future life as an academic psychologist, or on the field itself. And of course my memories are incomplete and fallible, if not downright false. One thing, though, is clear: I was born in 1936. Such is the paucity of human memory that I have no recollection of that particular event. Much later on, as a lecturer in psychology, I asked large introductory classes to date their earliest memories. There were always one or two who claimed to remember actually being born, although they were probably just being deliberately mischievous. For most of us, memories begin at about three or four. As I discovered later, 1936 was an eventful year: A king died and another abdicated; the English rugby team beat the New Zealand All Blacks for the first time; Amy Johnson flew solo from England to Australia; Nazi Germany occupied the Rhineland and Stalin started his Great Purge; at the Olympic Games in Berlin Jesse Owens won the 100-metre dash and the New Zealander Jack Lovelock won the 1500 metres. Oblivious to all of this, I was born in the small farming town of Marton, and grew up on a nearby sheep farm, at least until incarcerated in boarding schools. The town and neighbourhood were in many respects slices of England. The landed gentry still spoke of England as “home,” in spite of the fact that most were several generations removed from the Old Country. Marton itself always seemed 2 Growing up to me eccentric, populated by what appeared to be remittance men or preten- tious toffs, either from Britain or affecting to be so, although the solid farming community was a leavening influence. There were dowager ladies demanding correct pronunciation and manners, and my mother insisted that I phone one of them every year on her birthday, which was on the same day as mine. She referred to the lavatory as “the unsavoury appurtenance,” a phrase I still treasure. Marton also included a coterie of small private schools, one for girls, one for small boys, and one for Maori girls, established in the English tradition. They were all Anglican. Perhaps the lens of time has forced all this into caricature. My mother had grown up in this environment, but my father was something of an outsider. He was the sixth of seven offspring who grew up in County Meath in Ireland. He and the next oldest had worked as farm boys on the estate of the Marquess of Bute in West Scotland before emigrating together in the early 1920s to New Zealand, where they found work on farms in several parts of the country. They were both eventually able to secure wives and farms of their own. They had evidently timed it well in economic terms, although Irish charm probably helped them gain financial support from their newly acquired in-laws. Farming life turned out to be fairly prosperous, in spite of the 1930s depression and the rigours of World War II. My maternal grandfather was a lawyer in Marton, and well esteemed. He was a bookish man who also liked to paint, and he had managed to send his son, my mother’s older brother, to Oxford, where he gained a degree in law. My mother had been a good scholar at her boarding school, but did not go to university. In those days university was mainly for men, and I think my mother faintly disap- proved of university for women, thinking it might give them dangerous ideas. She was not alone in this view, but now the proportions have reversed, with women outnumbering men. So now we all have dangerous ideas. Universities should create dangerous ideas – that’s what they’re for. My father’s family was Anglo-Irish, and although Catholic, they did have social pretensions, which may well have helped my father gain acceptance into the landed gentry of the Rangitikei. The family claimed descent from Barbara Villiers, the most ostentatious and scandalous of the many mistresses of Charles II of England.1 Among her many other descendants were Diana, Princess of Wales, the Mitford sisters, Bertrand Russell, and Anthony Eden. Many others, too, of course – it is a pretentious but fairly meaningless claim, almost on a par with claiming descent from William the Conqueror, or perhaps St Patrick of Ireland. Barbara Villiers’ portrait hangs in the Glin Castle2 in Limerick, Ireland, but on the day that my wife and I visited it had been removed for cleaning. They may have known I was coming. I did find out, though, that I am the seventh cousin once removed of Sir Lau- rence Olivier, although I don’t believe he knew he was related to me.3 I and my three younger brothers were all baptised as Catholics, but my mother had promised the priest that she would raise us as Christians, which he perhaps Growing up 3 understood to mean Catholic but she interpreted as Anglican, or perhaps simply catholic with a small c. It was, I think, a sly arrangement. My mother was sol- idly Anglican in spirit but went to church only at Christmas, and at weddings and funerals, which periodically and decorously punctuated our serene lives. The local Catholic priest would occasionally visit the farm, drink whiskey with my father, and reminisce about Ireland, but I don’t remember even a tincture of religion. Perhaps they waited until I had gone to bed. My father seemed to have nothing to do with religion, although he always refused to enter a Catholic church for fear that he would be struck by a bolt of lightning from above. When he died, he was buried in a defunct Presbyterian churchyard overlooking the farm, in a service delivered by a Catholic priest who at first declined to give him a eulogy, but then relented. Comedy always seemed more important than religion. Part of my childhood was spent during the years of World War II. My father was not sent to the front but joined the Home Guard and was stationed for a time in the small Manawatu town of Feilding, which I was sure he stoutly defended. Apart from that, the war did not seem to impinge materially on our lives, at least from my youthful perspective. As a child, though, I was fascinated by the Battle of Britain and learned to recognise silhouettes of Spitfires, Hurricanes, and Messerschmitts – the airborne combatants of the battle – and can probably still do so. The first movie I ever saw was The First of the Few, a 1942 production about the development of the Spitfire fighter, featuring Leslie Howard as R.J. Mitchell, who designed the plane. David Niven also starred as the pilot who flew it. I remember snatches of the movie, including the scene in which Mitchell/ Howard watched seagulls for inspiration. The legendary Howard was killed the following year when the civilian plane he was travelling in was shot down by the Luftwaffe. Although we lived in just one place, I went to no fewer than five schools, and later to four universities. My first school was a small establishment about a mile up a country road, and I walked there in the company of some older boys who lived down the road. They were entrusted to look after me, but my memory is that they teased me mercilessly, partly because I wore glasses. The school closed shortly afterwards, and when I look for it now there is no trace at all. I then took the train into Marton to the district high school, and was met by my grandfather. I remember him asking me whether I was a forward or a back – a reference to rugby – and I replied that I was a backward. This became a standing joke. This arrangement was clumsy, and eventually a rural bus route was set up to send me and my brothers, along with other scattered farm children, to South Makirikiri School some five miles away. The bus driver was a friend of my mother, and I remember that she once ran the bus into the ditch. Or perhaps this was a false memory, based on hearsay about another trip. At age 11 I was sent as a boarder to Huntley School, one of the private schools in Marton. It offered primary education up to standard six (now year eight), but was known as a preparatory school, presumably referring to preparation for 4 Growing up education in a secondary school with similar Christian values. The school was my first rite of passage, and I still shudder at the memory of my first few days – the cold building, the smell of furniture polish, the regime of bells, the over- cooked cabbage and grey meat, and corporal punishment applied, bizarrely, with a wooden spoon. And tripe and onions every Tuesday, a dish I have since been unable to contemplate, let alone eat. It was all in stark contrast to the comfort of the farmhouse and my parents’ benevolence. Of course I adapted to all this, as did a great many other small boys raised in the same culture, but the memory of the initial shock still lingers. I occasionally dream about it. Huntley School gave me my first real exposure to religion, with regular cha- pel services and prayers, which I did not understand. In the evenings before getting into bed we were all obliged to kneel at the bedside with our eyes closed and pray, and I was once punished when a prefect saw me with my eyes open. Only later did it occur to me that the little Fauntleroy who caught me must also have had his eyes open. Religion was in fact a total mystery to me. It seemed to be about ancient people, one or two apparently able to live for many hundreds of years, and presented in obscure language. It seemed to imply that we would live on after death – although at the age of 11 I considered that news of little conse- quence and not helpful, because a dead body couldn’t do much anyway. I failed to understand how a God could be everywhere at once, and apparently invisible. My incomprehension was such that I thought I was probably simply stupid. Per- haps religion came too late; things might have been different if religious instruc- tion had been delivered at an earlier age. A Catholic education might have done the trick, according to the Jesuit promise: “Give me the child for the first seven years and I will give you the man.” My doubt about religion was perhaps the only premonition that psychology might eventually take over my life. We were also taught a smattering of French, Latin, algebra, and Euclid, along with the more serious matters of cricket and rugby. We were encouraged to speak nicely, like little English boys, and an end-of-year prize was awarded to the boy who most upheld the “traditions of an English public schoolboy.” It was not I. The school had a rather murky swimming pool, but I remember that we had swimming lessons on the cricket field, practising swimming movements while lying on the ground. I already knew how to swim from the more rough-and- tumble ways of my earlier life, and won swimming races when we were eventu- ally immersed in the pool itself. The next rite of passage was secondary school. Whanganui Collegiate School was established in 1854 as an Anglican school for the education of native chil- dren, but had developed into the equivalent of a minor English public school. There were a few token Maori and Pacific Island boys there on special schol- arships, but there was otherwise very little to distinguish the school from its counterparts in England, or in other Commonwealth countries such as Aus- tralia, Canada, and South Africa. Many of the traditions observed at Huntley continued – Huntley was after all a preparatory school. Within the first few Growing up 5 days new entrants were given an IQ test to determine which form they should be placed in. My IQ turned out to be about 60, which clearly marked me as a mental defective, and perhaps explained my failure to understand religion. It turned out, though, that the test came in the form of a folded sheet, and I had neglected to unfold it, answering only the questions on the first and last pages – clearly a verification of stupidity anyway. I was then asked to take the test again and given the full allotted time, and since I had already completed half of it my IQ increased to well above what it should have been. I was duly, and fortuitously, placed in the top form. At Collegiate, the cane replaced the wooden spoon, although prefects could administer punishment after lights-out with a strangely reinforced slipper. You were called out of the dormitory and whacked on the behind in the bathroom, while the rest of the dormitory listened in fear – or perhaps schadenfreude. In the morning, we had two minutes in which to get out of bed and take a five-second cold shower, a procedure sometimes monitored and timed by a prefect languish- ing in a hot bath. We then had to run about 400 yards up to the end of a road and back, occasionally under the surveillance of a prefect or master. In class, we were taught English history, and we knew little of the history of New Zealand. We went to chapel every day of the week and twice on Sundays, and our main unspoken challenge was to sing hymns so loudly they could be heard on the other side of the Whanganui River, at least a mile away. Perhaps the more fervent also thought the sound might get through to God. The most prominent activity again, though, was sport. Rugby and cricket were compulsory at least until the third or fourth year, when you could substitute field hockey or rowing, respectively. Individual sports, such as tennis and golf, were less encouraged, although available. I did have a brief crack at golf, but on the first swing of the club the ball simply disappeared, and I never saw it again. Team sports were held to encourage manliness and leadership, and more importantly, perhaps, conformity. Only later in life did it occur to me that sport, rather than religion itself, formed the background of the English public-school system, as well as its colonial outposts, creating loyalty and brotherhood and stifling dissent. Even so, I retain a fondness for both rugby and cricket, in spite of having largely discarded the ethos of the system – although I had actually switched from rugby to hockey because I wore glasses and couldn’t see much on the rugby field without them. Along with corporal punishment, the rugged sporting environment seemed to create a survival bond that encouraged Old Boys to send their sons there, sometimes no doubt against the protests of mothers, heartbroken that their sons were swept away. But many of the mothers, too, went to private girls’ schools with similar ideals, and were hardened to the system. It taught a stoicism and obedience to authority that helps explain why men had earlier gone to war, and perhaps why women, too, learned to suffer indignities without protest. My own mother was a warm person, but I can remember an occasion back on the farm when our own children and their cousins were fooling around, and she exclaimed “They should be sent to boarding school. That would teach them 6 Growing up some manners.” Our boys have long remembered this as a family joke. But on such regimes are empires built – and winning rugby teams. I later learned that the mechanism binding hardship and loyalty was called cognitive dissonance. But like most others I again adapted and blended into school life, and learned to avoid being conspicuous. Overall, I was not an unhappy schoolboy, but I do remember boys who seemed miserable for the time they were there. Some of these nevertheless became loyal Old Boys, and sent their own sons to the school. The system self-perpetuates. Of course it was not unique to Collegiate, but was ingrained in the English public-school system throughout the old Common- wealth. It is well illustrated by the trials of Prince Philip, Duke of Edinburgh, and his son Prince Charles at Gordonstoun in Scotland, a school that is perhaps unusually harsh even by public-school standards, through the influence of its German founder, Kurt Hahn.4 I hasten to add that times have changed. Whan- ganui Collegiate now includes girls, as do many other such schools, has abolished corporal punishment, and is integrated with the public system while retaining an affiliation with the Anglican Church. The headmaster in my time at Collegiate was Frank W. Gilligan, when heads were chosen from the Headmasters’ Conference,5 based in England. Frank came from a distinguished English cricketing family, and his two brothers had cap- tained England. The more famous of the two was Arthur Gilligan, captain of the tour to Australia in 1924–25. He was also a member of the British Union of Fascists, and propagated fascist ideas while on the Australian tour, attracting the of the Australian secret service. He was later a popular cricket com- mentator on radio. Frank himself was a distinguished cricketer and, like his brothers, had also excelled in rugby and athletics as a schoolboy at Dulwich College. As a wick- etkeeper, he captained the Oxford University cricket team and played county cricket for Essex for many years. It was rumoured that he had also been asked to play for England, but his service in World War I may have intervened. He taught for many years at Uppingham School in England before taking his position as headmaster at Collegiate in 1935. He was awarded the OBE in 1955. Frank did not to my knowledge succumb to fascism, and I have since won- dered if he left England to get away from the political ignominy surrounding his brother – although it must be said that fascist views were not uncommon in England in the 1930s. Frank nonetheless preached the virtues of the Christian religion and public-school life, perhaps not so very far removed from fascism, although I always sensed a twinge of hypocrisy on his lips. I fancied that if you inserted butter into his mouth, it would not melt. He spoke with deadpan circumlocution, always referring to the school as “this here our community,” which we abbreviated to THOC, and the rival bunch from the local state sec- ondary school were “our friends from the Technical College.” It surprised me in retrospect that Frank did not coach the cricket eleven. That privilege was given to H.E.B. (“Newt”) Newton, a fanatic of the theory and history of cricket but with no trace of ability. Growing up 7

Frank Gilligan had an extraordinary capacity to remember the names of all of the 360 or so boys at the school, and most if not all of their parents. He took great personal interest in individual boys, especially those who were less privileged, but never with any hint of impropriety. One was a Maori boy called Maru- maru, whom Frank affectionately called Maru-squared. I was once summoned to Frank’s house for some indiscretion that would normally result in a caning, but to my surprise he forgot the punishment and showed me round his rose gar- den. He had an MA in English at Oxford, and taught us Latin. When in the fifth form (now called year 11) I was persuaded to drop Latin and take up chemistry, he was sorrowful: “Ah, Corballis,” he said, “you used to be a scholar.” Although only in his late fifties at the time, Frank seemed to be quite ancient and stooped, with fingers gnarled and bent from years of wicketkeeping. He died in 1960, aged 66. I liked him a lot. Occasionally, at morning assembly, we were informed that two boys would no longer be with us, but had been despatched home for “abusing their bod- ies.” A few perhaps understood what this might have meant, but I think most of us were naïve, and a shiver of fear went round the assembly, as we wondered whether this was something we all might have been guilty of. Homosexuality was never overtly discussed, let alone condoned – as might have been the case in the more elite public schools in England. In that sense, at least, we were still colonial country boys. I do seem to remember tales of one or two masters fon- dling some small boy, but in those days this was treated as a something of a joke, although the head of one preparatory school (not Huntley) was found to have drowned himself, either from remorse or the indignity of being found out. While at Collegiate I avidly read the novels of P.G. Wodehouse, along with the short stories of Saki (H.H. Munro), and was once punished for being unable to suppress laughter during prep6 while reading Jerome K. Jerome’s Three Men in a Boat. Perhaps they appealed to me because the school had somehow managed to retain a sense of Edwardian life. The masters all seemed aware of their nicknames – Buzzer, Bertie, Darkest Africa, Dungo, Wetty, Wuzzy – and seemed to delight in caricaturing themselves. This provided amusement in an otherwise rigidly predictable life. I have tried rereading Wodehouse but it now falls flat. This was also the era of what C.P. Snow described a few years later as the “two cultures” – science and the humanities. English and the classics were deemed superior to the sciences, and even when I visited the school as the receiver of a national science award as late as 2016, I sensed that science was still something of an aberration. It’s better a chap should quote Shakespeare than know Newton’s laws of motion or Darwin’s (still dangerous) theory of evolution. Maybe academia in general had not fared so well either; to be recognised as a prominent Old Boy it was always better to be a sporting hero, a captain of industry, or a prominent member of one of the professions. Perhaps the sciences are still seen there as a danger to the established order; C.P. Snow himself once said that “if the sciences have the future in their bones, then the traditional culture responds by wishing the future did not exist.” But in the world at large the order has reversed, with 8 Growing up the humanities now facing radical downsizing, even extermination, in the face of technology, the evil stalk of STEM.7 Perhaps it is the humanities that are now seen as dangerous, likely to threaten material progress in a money-driven world. I did discover at Collegiate that I was not a leader. One of my contemporaries was my first cousin, Neil, and in our final and fifth year we were told that one of us, but probably not both, would be elevated to prefect, an exalted status that allowed us to deliver corporal punishment and stroll around with one hand in the pocket of our grey shorts. By most standards we had been successful students – we were both in the top form where I won a few prizes and Neil provided comic relief, and we were both in the cricket and hockey elevens. But in the end we were both overlooked, perhaps dismissed as being frivolous rather than respon- sible, and perhaps hockey was seen as girlish (as it is in North America, I later discovered). It was better to be a grim-faced rugby forward. I was also embarrassed in my final year to win the prize for divinity. I was still not religious – relentless attendance at chapel saw to that – and evidently won the prize on the strength of a long essay I wrote debunking a book we had been asked to read. I don’t remember what it was called. The gist of the book was that God must exist because the earth was perfectly adapted to host human beings, and only an all-powerful deity could have arranged that. I argued instead that people must have adapted to the earth rather than the other way about, and that there was no need to credit God. My guess is that no one read my pretentious work, and just assumed that it was a paean to the Almighty. I tried to return the prize, on the grounds that my father would laugh at me. I failed, and he did. Ironically, perhaps, this episode may have marked the beginning of my inter- est in evolution, and the possibility of explaining the human mind through sci- ence rather than religion. But that came much later. I won a few other prizes – I think for mathematics and French – and was given the choice of books. I disappointed the masters by choosing volumes of Som- erset Maugham’s novels and short stories, and the short stories of Saki. Dickens or Thackeray would have been more appropriate. I should add that academic standards were not especially high, but there was one exception. Simon Bernau was younger than I was, but in the form above, and was taught mathematics and physics by an exceptional young science graduate who had been hired for one year as a replacement teacher. Simon topped the national Junior Scholarship list by some margin. He was too young to go to university and was asked in the fol- lowing year to remain at school and teach mathematics to the top form – just a handful of us. Of course we hated being taught by someone younger than our- selves and were fairly uncooperative. I was in the fifth form when my parents, along with the school, began to consider what I should do with the rest of my life. I was a top student by the rather non-competitive standards of the school, but without excelling at any- thing in particular, in spite of the prizes. A future in farming was ruled out, and academia (let alone psychology) was never considered, so the main options were the established professions. As the daughter of a small-town lawyer, my mother Growing up 9 contacted men she knew in each of the more respected professions and asked them to write to me, explaining the necessary attributes and possible pitfalls. From memory, these professions included accountancy, architecture, engineer- ing, law, and medicine. I did indeed receive some remarkably insightful and even erudite letters. I was reasonably accomplished in mathematics, although I had also harboured a secret desire to be a cartoonist, and it was eventually decided that engineering might best suit whatever talents I had. It was this that led me to drop Latin in favour of chemistry, to Frank Gilligan’s sorrow. And in truth I enjoyed Latin more and developed a lifelong dislike of chemistry, which I still think of, in the jargon of the time, as “stinks.” Academia was not considered, and psychology would never have stood a chance. In trying to bring to mind my early life, I was astonished at how different it all seems through the lens of today’s world. And yet it was not really out of the ordinary. As L.P. Hartley wrote in his novel The Go-Between: “The past is a for- eign country; they do things differently there.”

Notes 1 The descent was through her daughter Charlotte Fitzroy, “Fitzroy” being the name tra- ditionally given to bastard children of a king. Her grandfather, and my nine times great grandfather, was executed by beheading in 1649. 2 I believe it is now a hotel. 3 A further revelation: I am the twelfth cousin seven times removed of Napoleon I of France. I didn’t like to mention that at first. 4 The hostile receptions given to both Philip and Charles were featured in the TV series The Crown, first launched in 2016. Gordonstoun and Prince Charles have both hit back, claiming the series was exaggerated. A good example, perhaps, of cognitive dissonance in action. 5 An association of heads of English public schools. At the time Whanganui Collegiate was a member of the Conference, along with King’s College and Christ’s College. Headmas- ters are no longer selected from the public-school elite. 6 Supervised preparation for the next day’s classes. 7 Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics. References 1 In those days, men were boys and women were girls, and I don’t remember anyone in between. 2 Colditz Castle had served as a German prisoner-of-war camp in World War II for prison- ers who had repeatedly tried to escape from other camps. Some did manage to escape even from there, a feat portrayed in the 1955 movie The Colditz Story, starring John Mills and Eric Portman. It was widely admired. 3 Actually, I think he elaborated a bit more, but I don’t remember exactly what he wrote. 4 Robert Conquest (1917–2015), “A Grouchy Good Night to the Academic Year,” The Penguin Book of Light Verse (Harmondsworth: Penguin Books, 1980; rpt. 1984), pp. 506–510. 1 He later used his second name and became known as John Adcock, possibly as a result of the spiral aftereffect – although I’m told that it was actually a second marriage that prompted the change. 2 Written on 6 February 1887, and reprinted on p. 262 of The Letters of William James, Vol. 1, edited by his son Henry James (not the novelist), and published by the Atlantic Monthly Press, Boston, in 1920. 3 But not in concrete. 4 A position junior to a lectureship, but since abolished. 1 I had another relative in Whanganui who believed the same of Chinese fruiterers. You have to shout if you want service. 1 Otherwise known as The Great Chain of Being, invented by the early Greeks. To mediaeval Christians it was thought to extend from God down through angels, humans, animals, and eventually to plants and minerals. We now know this is wrong – it’s a kind of tangled tree, and humans have no special status. But we can still measure the evolutionary distance through the tree between humans and other species. 2 The exception was Edward VIII, who insisted on a particular profile out of sequence. And he didn’t last long as king anyway. 1 In 1952, Turing was prosecuted for homosexual acts. After undergoing chemical castra- tion, he died in 1954, probably through suicide, although this has never been proven. 2 When I asked Siri, the helpful assistant on my iPhone, whether she had free will, she replied “This is about you, not about me.” 1 Diversions of Purley is a treatise on grammar, written while Tooke was imprisoned for libel – perhaps reason to suppose his testimony is not to be trusted. Quote is on p. 304. 2 Binnie-Dawson reports similar estimates for Eskimos. 3 These and other examples of late 19th-century concepts of the dual brain, and of sup- posed racial differences, are well described by the historian Anne Harrington. 4 BBS has since moderated its policies and vetted commentaries to reduce their number. 5 Nothing to do with foxes. It stands for Forkhead box P2 if you need to know. 1 Yves had also taken his life in a suicide pact with Justine, in 1994. I have recounted this tragic event in my book How a Distinguished Scholar Was Driven to Kill Herself: The Dark Side of Science. An extraordinary and perhaps telling aspect of Justine’s many scientific publications is that Yves’ contribution was never acknowledged. In all other respects they seemed a devoted couple. 2 You have to flash images quickly so that the patients don’t have time to move their eyes. 3 1994 was also the year in which Justine took her own life. 1 Now at New York University. 1 I have it on Luciano’s authority that he invented the term “mirror neuron,” but he says he intended it as a joke. 2 There’s another old joke: “What is an Italian in handcuffs?” Answer: “Speechless.” 3 Pictorial language may come back, in the form of Instagrams, making speech irrelevant. 4 This excludes native Africans, because interbreeding with Neanderthals, and with their close relatives the Denisovans, took place after the migrations from Africa. 1 Mental time travel is not always accurate. 2 From a conversation with Freddy Gray, published in The Spectator of 10 April 2010. 3 Owen was also famous for inferring the one-time existence of a large flightless bird, later called the moa, from a small fragment of a femur bone. With a few more bones, he was later able to reconstruct the entire skeleton. 1 Ribena is a red blackcurrent concentrate, sometimes given to children as a vitamin C supplement. It was widely distributed by the British government during World War II. 2 I have been told that he has since put it back to 200,000. 1 She has recently retired as Professor of Psychology at the University of Western Australia. 2 And the abandonment of apostrophes, perhaps because they can cause trauma. 3 One of my colleagues placed a sign on his office door proclaiming “I am a psychologist,” defying the authorities to indict him. They didn’t. 4 Maggie Bruck had been a graduate student during my time at McGill. 5 The major book awards in New Zealand, awarded annually. 6 A last-ditch effort has been launched to clear his name, but he sadly died on 4 September 2019. 1 www.elsewhere.org/pomo/ 2 According to Jennifer Schuessler in the New York Times of 18 October 2018, this article was entitled “Our Struggle Is My Struggle,” but I can find no trace of it within the journal Affilia. It seems to have been one of several such articles that were quickly “retracted.” 3 This quote was featured in a news item by Fox News, and the professor who made the statement received a flood of hate mail and threats. I have therefore not mentioned her name or the source. 4 Although I should add that one of my ex-colleagues, Jenni Ogden, was a productive clini- cal neuropsychologist before she went on to become a successful novelist. 1 The Independent, 27 August 2015. 2 You can follow this on Twitter (@neurobollocks). 3 “Stapel betuigt openlijk ‘diepe spijt.’” Brabants Dagblad. 31 October 2011. 4 Although the case for fraud seems overwhelming, the British psychologist (and eugeni- cist) Leslie Hearnshaw claimed that Burt was innocent. 5 American readers may relate more to Rube Goldberg. Bexton, W.H., Heron, W., & Scott, T.H. (1954). Effects of decreased variation in the sensory environment. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 8, 70–76. Corballis, M.C., & Sampson, H. (1963). Scanning and immediate memory in serial add- ing. Australian Journal of Psychology, 15, 99–107. Giles-Bernardelli, B.M. (1950). The decline of intelligence in New Zealand. Population Studies, 4, 200–208. Hebb, D.O. (1949). Organization of behavior. New York: Wiley. Miller, G.A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two. Psychological Review, 63, 81–97. Beale, I.L., Williams, R.J., Webster, D., & Corballis, M.C. (1972). Brain symmetry: Mir- ror-image confusion and the interhemispheric commissures. Nature, 238, 348–349. Corballis, M.C., & Beale, I.L. (1976). The psychology of left and right. Hillsdale, NJ: Law- rence Erlbaum Associates. Corballis, M.C., & Beale, I.L. (1983). The ambivalent mind. Chicago: Nelson-Hall. Hannay, H.J., Ciaccia, P.J., Kerr, J.W., & Barrett, D. (1990). Self-report of right-left con- fusion in college men and women. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 70, 451–457. McConnell, J.V., Jacobson, A.L., & Kimble, D.P. (1959). The effects of regeneration upon retention of a conditioned response in the planarian. Journal of Comparative and Physi- ological Psychology, 32, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048028 Mello, N.K. (1965). Interhemispheric reversal of mirror image oblique lines after mon- ocular training in pigeons. Science, 148, 252–254. Orton, S.T. (1937). Reading, writing, and speech problems in children. New York: W.W. Nor- ton and Co. Ltd. Watson, J.B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158– 177. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0074428 Bregman, A.S. (1994). Auditory scene analysis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Broadbent, D.E. (1958). Perception and communication. London: Pergamon. Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton. Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of Skinner’s “Verbal Behavior.” Language, 35, 26–58. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Skinner, B.F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Annett, M. (1972). The distribution of manual asymmetry. British Journal of Psychology, 63, 343–358. Badzakova-Trajkov, G., Häberling, I.S., & Corballis, M.C. (2010). Cerebral asymmetries in monozygotic twins: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3086–3093. Binnie-Dawson, J.L.M. (1977). An anthropological perspective on the evolution and lateralization of the human brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 299, 424–447. Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur la siège de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d’une observation d’aphémie. Bulletin de la Société anatomique de Paris, 2, 330 –357. Corballis, M.C. (1983). Human laterality. New York: Academic Press. Corballis, M.C. (1991). The lop-sided ape. New York: Oxford University Press. Corballis, M.C., & Morgan, M.J. (1978). On the biological basis of human laterality: I. Evi- dence for a maturational left-right gradient. The Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 2, 261–268. Crow, T.J. (2008). The “big bang” theory of the origin of psychosis and the faculty of language. Schizophrenia Research, 102, 31–52. Harrington, A. (1987). Medicine, mind, and the double brain. Princeton, NJ: Princeton Uni- versity Press. Kiinura, D. (1961). Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 166–171. Kopiez, R. (2011). No disadvantage for left-handed musicians: The relationship between handedness, perceived constraints and performance-related skills in string players and pianists. Psychology of Music, 40, 357–384. McManus, C. (2002). Right hand, left hand: The origins of asymmetry in brains, bodies, atoms and cultures. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson. Mishkin, M., & Forgays, D.G. (1952). Word recognition as a function of retinal locus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 43–48. Morgan, M.J., & Corballis, M.C. (1978). On the biological basis of human laterality: II. The mechanisms of inheritance. The Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 2, 269–277. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. New York: W.H. Freeman. Tooke, J.H. (1840). The diversions of Purley. London: William Tegg & Co. Vargha-Khadem, F., Gadian, D.G., Copp, A., & Mishkin, M. (2005). FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 132–138. Verhaegen, P., & Ntumba, A. (1964). Note on the frequency of left-handedness in African children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 55, 89–90. Corballis, M.C. (1994). Can commissurotomized subjects compare digits between the visual fields? Neuropsychologia, 32, 1475–1486. Corballis, M.C. (2016). How a distinguished scholar was driven to kill herself: The dark side of science. Lewiston, NY: The Edwin Mellen Press. Corballis, M.C., Corballis, P.M., Berlucchi, G., & Marzi, C.A. (2018). Perceptual unity in the split brain: The role of subcortical connections. Brain, 2018, e1. Corballis, M.C., & Sergent, J. (1988). Imagery in a commissurotomized patient. Neuro- psychologia, 26, 13–26. Edwards, B. (1979). Drawing on the right side of the brain: A course in enhancing creativity and artistic confidence. Los Angeles, CA: Tarcher. Gazzaniga, M.S. (2015). Tales from both sides of the brain: A life in neuroscience. New York: Ecco. McGilchrist, I. (2009). The master and his emissary: The divided brain and the making of the western world. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Sergent, J. (1987). A new look at the human split brain. Brain, 110, 1375–1392. Sperry, R.W. (1982). Some effects of disconnecting the cerebral hemisphere. Science, 217, 1223–1227. Block, N.J. (1974). Why do mirrors reverse right/left but not up/down? Journal of Philoso- phy, 71, 259–277. Cooper, L.A., & Shepard, R.N. (1973). Chronometric studies of the rotation of mental images. In W.G. Chase (Ed.), Visual information processing (pp. 75–176). New York: Academic Press. Corballis, M.C. (1988). Recognition of disoriented shapes. Psychological Review, 95, 115–123. Corballis, M.C. (2019). What mirrors do. American Scientist, 107, 224–231. Ogden, J.A. (1996). Fractured minds: A case-study approach to clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press. Ogden, J.A. (2012). Trouble in mind: Stories from a neuropsychologist’s casebook. New York: Oxford University Press. Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: William Morrow. Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. London: Penguin. Shepard, R.N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Sci- ence, 171, 701–703. Corballis, M.C. (2002). From hand to mouth: The origins of language. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Corballis, M.C. (2017). The truth about language: What it is and where it came from. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. Fano, G. (1992). The origins and nature of language. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press (Originally published in Italian in two parts, the first in 1962 and the second in 1973, and translated by S. Petrilli). Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., & Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the pre- motor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609. Häberling, I.S., Corballis, P.M., & Corballis, M.C. (2016). Language, gesture, and hand- edness: Evidence for independent lateralized networks. Cortex, 82, 72–85. Hewes, G.W. (1973). Primate communication and the gestural origins of language. Cur- rent Anthropology, 14, 5–24. Kendon, A. (2011). Vocalisation, speech, gesture, and the language origins debate. Ges- ture, 13, 349–370. Liberman, A.M., Cooper, F.S., Shankweiler, D.P., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Per- ception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–461. McNeill, D. (2012). How language began: Gesture and speech in human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M.A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience, 21, 188–194. Clayton, N.S., Bussey, T.J., & Dickinson, A. (2003). Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 685–691. Corballis, M.C. (2013). Mental time travel: A case for evolutionary continuity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 5–6. Corkin, S. (2013). Permanent present tense: The man with no memory, and what he taught the world. London: Allen Lane. Eiseley, L. (1969). The unexpected universe. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World. Fernández-Armesto, F. (2019). Out of our minds: What we think and how we came to think it. London: Oneworld Publications. Kamil, A.C., & Balda, R.P. (1985). Cache recovery and spatial memory in Clark’s Nut- crackers (Nucifraga columbiana). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Pro- cesses, 85, 95–111. Kingsley, C. (1863). The water babies. London: Macmillan. Knaebe, B., Taylor, A.H., Elliffe, D.M., & Gray, R.D. (2017). New Caledonian crows show behavioural flexibility when manufacturing their tools. Behaviour, 154, 65–91. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003411 Kohler, W. (1927). The mentality of apes (E. Winter, Trans.). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul (Original work published in German, 1917). Kundera, M. (2002). Ignorance (L. Asher, Trans.). New York: HarperCollins. Martin, C.F., Bhui, R., Bossaer, P., Matsuzawa, T., & Camerer, C. (2014). Chimpanzee choice rates in competitive games match equilibrium game theory predictions. Scien- tific Reports, 4(5182). https://doi.org/10.1038/srep05182 Martin, V.C., Schacter, D.L., Corballis, M.C., & Addis, D.R. (2011). A role for the hip- pocampus in encoding simulations of future events. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 13858–13863. Moser, M.B., Rowland, D.C., & Moser, E.I. (2015). Place cells, grid cells, and memory. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology, 7, a021808. O’Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Raghantia, M.A. (2019). Domesticated species: It takes one to know one. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116, 14401–14404. Suddendorf, T. (2013). The gap: The science of what separates us from other animals. New York: Basic Books. Suddendorf, T. (2013). Mental time travel: Continuities and discontinuities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17, 151–152. Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M.C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133–167. Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M.C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel, and is it unique to humans? Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 30, 299–351. Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 1–25. Wearing, D. (2005). Forever today. New York: Doubleday. Berwick, R.C., & Chomsky, N. (2016). Why only us? Language and evolution. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Coles, M. (2001). The Bible in cockney. Abingdon, UK: Bible Reading Fellowship. Corballis, M.C. (2007). Taking up arms. In H. Cohen & B. Stemmer (Eds.), Conscious- ness and cognition: Fragments of mind and brain (pp. 19–27). New York: Academic Press. Corballis, M.C. (2007, June 1). Think so? Review of Boden, M. Mind as machine (Oxford University Press). Times Literary Supplement, 5435, 4–5. Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray. Dor, D. (2015). The instruction of imagination: Language as a social communication technology. New York: Oxford University Press. Everett, D.L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Current Anthropology, 46, 621–646. Grice, H.P. (1989). Studies in the ways of words. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press. Müller, F.M. (1873). Lectures on Mr Darwin’s philosophy of language: Second lecture. Frazer’s Magazine, 7, 659–678. Scott-Phillips, T. (2015). Speaking our minds: Why human communication is different, and how language evolved to make it special. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. Bass, E., & Davis, L. (1988). The courage to heal: A guide for women survivors of child sexual abuse. New York: Perennial Library. Corballis, M.C. (1990). The two profiles of psychology. New Zealand Journal of Psychol- ogy, 19, 2–8. Corballis, M.C. (2002). Malady lingers on. Review of L. Hood, A city possessed: The Christchurch Civic Crèche case. New Zealand Listener, 182(3320), 60. Corballis, M.C. (2003). Memory and the law. New Zealand Listener, 190(3305), 32–35. Corballis, M.C. (2003, October). Psychology and the law. New Zealand Law Journal, 385–388. Editorial. (2002, February). The Ellis case. New Zealand Law Journal, 1. Hood, L. (2002). A city possessed: The Christchurch Civic Crèche case. Dunedin, NZ: Lon- gacre Press. Loftus, E., & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed memory. New York: St. Martin’s Press. Pazder, D., & Smith, M. (1980). Michelle remembers. New York: Pocket Books. Werry, J.S. (1990). Comment on “The two profiles of psychology.” New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 19, 99–100. Best, S., & Kellner, D. (1991). Critical theory and postmodern theory. In S. Best & D. Kellner (Eds.), Postmodern theory. Communications and culture (pp. 215–255). London: Palgrave. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-21718-2_7 Kuhn, T.S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Lindsay, J., & Boyle, P. [Boghossian, P.]. (2017). The conceptual penis as a social con- struct. Cogent Social Sciences, 3, 1330439. Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. New York: W.H. Freeman. Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature: Why violence has declines. New York: Viking Books. Pinker, S. (2018). Enlightenment now: The case for reason, science, humanism, and progress. New York: Viking Books. Potter, J., & Wetherell, M. (1987). Discourse and social psychology: Beyond attitudes and behav- iour. London: Sage Publications Ltd. Sokal, A.D. (1996). Transgressing the boundaries: Toward a transformative hermeneutics of quantum gravity. Social Text, 46–47, 217–252. https://doi.org/10.2307/466856 Corballis, J.H. (1891). Forty-five years of sport. London: Richard Bentley and Son. Fanelli, D. (2009). How many scientists fabricate and falsify research? A systematic review and meta-analysis of survey data. PLoS One, 4(5), e5738. https://doi.org/10.1371/ journal.pone.0005738 Hearnshaw, L.S. (1979). Cyril Burt: Psychologist. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Kamin, L. (1974). The science and politics of IQ. Potomac, MD: Erlbaum. Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological sci- ence. Science, 349(4716). https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716