Free Recall of Word Lists Differing in Phonological Similarity, Word Frequency, and Phonotactic Pattern Frequency in SLI

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Free Recall of Word Lists Differing in Phonological Similarity, Word Frequency, and Phonotactic Pattern Frequency in SLI Free recall of word lists differing in phonological similarity, word frequency, and phonotactic pattern frequency in SLI Jeffry A. Coady Julia L. Evans Elina Mainela-Arnold Keith R. Kluender Boston University San Diego State University Penn State University University of Wisconsin Abstract Memory Deficits in SLI Method Results Phonological encoding refers Phonological Similarity to the reciprocal processes in Children with SLI have difficulty with verbal recall. Participants 1. Group. Children with SLI recalled 100 which phonological strings are SLI TYP fewer words overall. broken down into smaller In recall tasks, children with SLI recall fewer items than their typically developing peers. 16 monolingual English-speaking children with SLI and 16 age-matched control children 80 t units for subsequent tasks like 1 c e r 60 Some theories view this as an underlying cause of SLI , while others view memory participated. All children had highly intelligible articulation and nonverbal IQ scores r o 2. List Length. Accuracy decreased as C recall or repetition, or where t n 2 e c r 40 deficits as resulting from a more pervasive linguistic deficit . greater than 85. The children with SLI included 2 with E-SLI and 14 with ER-SLI, 9 e smaller units are combined P list length increased. This effect was into larger phonological 1Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990; 2van der Lely & Howard, 1993; MacDonald & Christiansen, 2002 females and 7 males aged 8;7 to 11;8. The age-matched control children included 8 20 the same for both groups. strings for articulation. It can be measured by having females and 8 males aged 8;5 to 12;3. 0 dis sim dis sim dis sim dis sim dis sim dis sim 3. Phonological Similarity. All children listeners recall lists of words Children with SLI may use less efficient rehearsal strategies. Length 2 Length 3 Length 4 Length 5 Length 6 Overall differing in phonological recalled fewer phonologically similar Group Age CELF CELF RLS PPVT-III EVT NWR CLPT Word Frequency similarity among list items. In Baddeley & Hitch’s working memory model, auditory words are assumed to be stored 100 words. A significant group × similarity Participants typically recall ELS SLI 10;2 72.4* 68.7 91.3* 81.9* 78.9* 36.3* SLI in the phonological loop, subject to decay unless they are actively maintained, such as TYP fewer phonologically similar 80 interaction revealed that children with words, presumably because (1;0) (11.0) (13.2) (10.3) (6.7) (8.2) (13.7) t through rehearsal. Recall is better for items at the beginning and end of a list—primacy c e 60 r SLI showed a smaller phonological r o traces of similar words C t n e and recency effects, respectively. Primacy effects are attributed to rehearsal of initial c interfere with one another in r 40 similarity effect than controls. TYP 10;2 110.7 --- 107.1 98.3 87.5 55.5 e the phonological store. To items, while recency effects are attributed to output of the storage buffer at time of recall. P (1;2) (12.6) (8.5) (11.9) (5.9) (14.4) 20 examine group differences in Children with SLI exhibit reduced primacy effects, suggesting poorer rehearsal. 4. Word Frequency. All children efficiency of phonological 0 hi lo hi lo hi lo hi lo hi lo hi lo recalled more frequently-occurring encoding, 16 children with SLI Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Kirchner & Klatzky, 1985; Mainela-Arnold & Evans, 2005 Length 2 Length 3 Length 4 Length 5 Length 6 Overall (mean age 10;2) and age- Stimulus Materials words. This effect was the same for matched controls recalled lists Phonotactic Probability both groups. of CVC words, two to six Reduced capacity or linguistic impairment? Eighty lists of CVC words ranging from two to six items in length were created. Two 100 SLI items in length, differing in TYP lists at each of five lengths varied along three orthogonal dimensions: phonological 80 5. Phonotactic Probability. All children phonological similarity, Another way to examine rehearsal is by having listeners recall lists of phonologically t c similarity, word frequency, and phonotactic probability. e 60 r without replacement. Word r recalled more words with common o C similar vs. dissimilar words. During rehearsal, traces of similar items interfere with one t n lists also differed orthogonally e c r 40 e P phonotactic patterns (p=.07). A along another two dimensions another so they are recalled less well. Children who use less efficient encoding The 1099 English CVC words rated high in familiarity were separated into high and low affecting recall by typical strategies are less influenced by phonological similarity among items to be recalled. word frequency groups by a median split. They were then separated into high and low 20 significant pp × wf interaction adults—word frequency and 0 revealed that this effect was phonotactic probability. Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Liberman, Shankweiler, Liberman, Fowler & Fischer, 1977 phonotactic probability groups by another median split. Thus, there were four groups of hi lo hi lo hi lo hi lo hi lo hi lo Results reveal that all children words: (1) high word frequency, high phonotactic probability (hi wf, hi pp); (2) hi wf, lo Length 2 Length 3 Length 4 Length 5 Length 6 Overall significant for frequent words, but not recalled fewer words from Serial Position Effects for less frequent words. This was the phonologically similar lists. Children with SLI use less efficient phonological encoding strategies, pp; (3) lo wf, hi pp; and (4) lo wf, hi pp. Words from each of the four groups were 100 same for both groups. While children with SLI combined into 20 lists, four at each length (2 - 6 items). Two lists at each length SLI at least when their memory resources are exceeded. TYP recalled fewer words overall, contained phonologically similar words; two contained phonologically dissimilar words. 80 they were less affected by y 60 6. Serial Position Effects. In longer lists, c a r Children with SLI show the expected phonological similarity effects for shorter word lists, u phonological similarity, Each word appeared in only a single list. c c A all children recalled list initial and list suggesting less efficient but not for longer lists. However, these studies recombined a single set of words, so 40 phonological encoding. All The 320 words were produced by a female hi wf, hi pp hi wf, lo pp 20 final items at higher rates. This effect children recalled more high children heard each word multiple times. Practice effects likely influenced recall. Length 2 Length 3 Length 4 Length 5 Length 6 speaker from the local dialect, with no duration 0 was the same for both groups. frequency words and more Gathercole & Baddeley, 1990; James, van Steenbrugge & Chiveralls, 1994; c.f., Montgomery, 1995; van der Lely & dis sim dis sim 1 2 1 2 3 1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 6 words with frequently List Position Howard, 1993 differences due to wf or pp. Words were occurring sound patterns. excised, normalized for volume, and give rice mouth peak However, the two groups did Conclusions not differ in their sensitivity to Language knowledge also influences recall. concatenated into lists with a 1-s ISI. Time worse light chain tip these measures. Both groups between lists was 5s for two-item lists, and yell wine loop cheap 1. Children with SLI recalled fewer items overall, but showed the same also showed similar serial In the process of redintegration, decaying, partial phonological traces are reconstructed position curves. These increased with list length. sensitivity as children with NLD to word frequency, phonotactic probability, and from established lexical and/or sublexical knowledge1. Adults use both of these, word ride rain wish pitch results suggest that children serial position. This suggests that they can use language knowledge for with SLI have reduced frequency and phonotactic probability, to facilitate recall2. phone line dog keep capacity and less efficient Procedure redintegration, to facilitate recall. phonological encoding 1Thorn, Gathercole & Frankish, 2005; 2Roodenrys, Hulme, Lethbridge, Hinton & Nimmo, 2002 make wait five chip strategies, but remarkably Children were tested individually in a quiet 2. However, because they are less influenced by phonological similarity among lo wf, hi pp lo wf, lo pp similar memory processes. room. They were told that they’d be hearing items to be recalled, their phonological encoding processes are less efficient, at [Research supported by Research Questions NIDCD DC-05263, DC-04072, list of words, and that their job was to repeat dis sim dis sim least when memory resources are exceeded. and DC-005650.] them back in any order. In a blocked design, 1. Do children with SLI encode phonological information less efficiently? Are they less deed pun zoom jug influenced by phonological similarity among items to be remembered? two-item lists were presented first, with list References hip tin peach chick Baddeley, A.D. & Hitch, G.J. (1974). Working memory. In G. Bower (Ed.), Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation, Vol. VIII. NY: Academic Pr. length increasing, concluding with six-item Gathercole, S.E. & Baddeley, A.D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language-disordered children: Is there a causal connection? JML, 29, 336-60. 2. Do children with SLI show the same redintegration processes as children developing James, D., van Steenbrugge, W. & Chiveralls, K. (1994). Underlying deficits in langauge-disordered children with central auditory processing difficulties. App. Psycholing, wool keen knack jerk 15, 311-28. lists. Children’s responses were recorded for Kirchner, D.M. & Klatzky, R.L. (1985). Verbal rehearsal and memory in language-disordered children. JSHR, 28, 556-565.
Recommended publications
  • Richard Semon's Theory of Memory
    JOURNAL OF VERBAL LEARNING AND VERBAL BEHAVIOR 17, 721--743 (1978) Richard Semon's Theory of Memory DANIEL L. SCHACTER, JAMES ERIC EICH, AND ENDEL TULVlNG University of Toronto In the first decade of the 20th century, Richard Semon put forward a theory of memory that anticipated numerous recent developments in memory research. The theory is discussed both in its historical context and with reference to modem ideas. Semon's theoretical concern for retrieval phenomena is particularly noteworthy. Several reasons are suggested why the theory is virtually unknown today. Current research in the area of human Wolfgang Semon, a German scientist who memory owes many of its present orienting wrote two books on the subject of human attitudes and research techniques to pioneer- memory in the first decade of the 20th century. ing psychologists of the late 19th and early In fact, Semon's work has been almost com- 20th centuries. Among the most well known pletely ignored by mainstream psychologists and important of these early investigators are concerned with processes of memory and Hermann Ebbinghaus, who performed the learning; later in this paper we will explore first systematic laboratory studies of human some of the reasons why Semon's work has memory; William James, whose distinction been bypassed. Yet prominent anatomist J. Z. between primary and secondary memory is Young (1965, p.288) commented that still today the target of much research and "...many modern ideas on the subject [of theorizing~ G. E. Miiller, who performed memoryl go back to Richard Semon...;" important early research on grouping and Gestalt psychologist Kurt Koffka (1935, interference; and Sir Frederic Bartlett, whose p.
    [Show full text]
  • Redintegration and Lexicality Effects in Children: Do They Depend Upon the Demands of the Memory Task?
    Memory & Cognition 2004, 32 (3), 501-510 Redintegration and lexicality effects in children: Do they depend upon the demands of the memory task? JUDY E. TURNER University of Reading, Reading, England LUCY A. HENRY Institute of Psychiatry, London, England and PHILIP T. SMITH and PENELOPE A. BROWN University of Reading, Reading, England The effect of long-term knowledge upon performance in short-term memory tasks was examined for children from 5 to 10 years of age. The emergence of a lexicality effect, in which familiar words were recalled more accurately than unfamiliar words, was found to depend upon the nature of the memory task. Lexicality effects were interpreted as reflecting the use of redintegration, or reconstruction pro- cesses, in short-term memory. Redintegration increased with age for tasks requiring spoken item recall and decreased with age when position information but not naming was required. In a second experi- ment, redintegration was found in a recognition task when some of the foils rhymed with the target. Older children were able to profit from a rhyming foil, whereas younger children were confused by it, suggesting that the older children make use of sublexical phonological information in reconstructing the target. It was proposed that redintegrative processes in their mature form support the reconstruc- tion of detailed phonological knowledge of words. The concept of redintegration has proved to be a pro- memory tasks. For example, long-term knowledge has an ductive approach to examining why long-term memory effect prelexically: Vitevitch and Luce (1999) found that (LTM) knowledge affects performance on short-term decisions could be made more rapidly about auditory memory (STM) tasks.
    [Show full text]
  • Semantic Coherence Stabilizes Phonological Sequences in Short-Term Memory : Semantic Coherence Stabilizes Phonological Sequences in Short-Term Memory
    This is a repository copy of Keeping It Together: Semantic Coherence Stabilizes Phonological Sequences in Short-Term Memory : Semantic coherence stabilizes phonological sequences in short-term memory. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/125356/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Savill, Nicola, Ellis, Rachel, Brooke, Emma et al. (6 more authors) (2017) Keeping It Together: Semantic Coherence Stabilizes Phonological Sequences in Short-Term Memory : Semantic coherence stabilizes phonological sequences in short-term memory. Memory & Cognition. pp. 1-12. ISSN 0090-502X https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-017-0775-3 Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request. [email protected] https://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ Running head: SEMANTIC AND PHONOLOGICAL COHERENCE This is a postprint of a paper to appear in Memory & Cognition DOI: 10.3758/s13421-017-0775-3 Keeping It Together: Semantic Coherence
    [Show full text]
  • Freeing Capacity in WM Through the Use of LTM Representations
    Freeing capacity in WM through the use of LTM representations Lea M. Bartsch1 and Peter Shepherdson2 1 University of Zurich 2 University of Akureyri Author Note We thank Atalia Adank and Dawid Strzelczyk for helping with data collection. The preregistration, data and the analysis scripts can be accessed on the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/7d9aj/). Correspondence should be addressed to Lea M. Bartsch, E-mail: [email protected]. © 2021, American Psychological Association. This paper is not the copy of record and may not exactly replicate the final, authoritative version of the article. Please do not copy or cite without authors' permission. The final article will be available, upon publication, via its DOI: 10.1037/xlm0001024 Freeing capacity in WM through the use of LTM representations 2 Abstract Previous research indicates that long-term memory (LTM) may contribute to performance in working memory (WM) tasks. Across three experiments we investigated the extent to which active maintenance in WM can be replaced by relying on information stored in episodic LTM, thereby freeing capacity for additional information in WM. First, participants encoded word pairs into LTM, and then completed a WM task, also involving word pairs. Crucially, the pairs presented in each WM trial comprised varying numbers of new pairs and the previously learned LTM pairs. Experiment 1 showed that recall performance in the WM task was unaffected when memory set size increased through the addition of LTM pairs, but that it deteriorated when set size increased through adding new pairs. In Experiment 2 we investigated the robustness of this effect, orthogonally manipulating the number of new and LTM pairs used in the WM task.
    [Show full text]
  • Verbal Short-Term Memory Reflects the Organization of Long-Term
    VERBAL SHORT-TERM MEMORY REFLECTS THE ORGANIZATION OF LONG-TERM MEMORY. FURTHER EVIDENCE FROM SHORT-TERM MEMORY FOR EMOTIONAL WORDS. Steve Majerus 1,2 & Arnaud D‟Argembeau 1,2 1 Université de Liège, Belgium 2 Fonds de la Recherche Scientifique FNRS, Belgium RUNNING HEAD: STM AND EMOTIONAL VALENCE Journal of Memory and Language, 2011, 64, 181-197 Address for correspondence: Steve Majerus, PhD Center for Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience Boulevard du Rectorat, B33 4000 Liège, Belgium Phone : 0032 4 3664656 Fax : 0032 4 3662808 Email : [email protected] 1 ABSTRACT Many studies suggest that long-term lexical-semantic knowledge is an important determinant of verbal short-term memory (STM) performance. This study explored the impact of emotional valence on word immediate serial recall as a further lexico-semantic long-term memory (LTM) effect on STM. This effect is particularly interesting for the study of STM-LTM interactions since emotional words not only activate specific lexico-semantic LTM features but also capture attentional resources, and hence allow for the study of both LTM and attentional factors on STM tasks. In Experiments 1 and 2, we observed a robust effect of emotional valence on pure list recall in both young and elderly adults, with higher recall performance for emotional lists as opposed to neutral lists, as predicted by increased LTM support for emotional words. In Experiments 3 and 4 however, using mixed lists, it was the lists containing a minority of emotional words which led to higher recall performance over lists containing a majority of emotional words. This was predicted by a weak version of the attentional capture account.
    [Show full text]
  • An ERP Study of Lexical and Sublexical Processing
    Immediate Auditory Repetition of Words and Nonwords: An ERP Study of Lexical and Sublexical Processing Xiaorong Cheng1,2,3, Graham Schafer3*, Patricia M. Riddell3 1 Key Laboratory of Adolescent Cyberpsychology and Behavior (CCNU), Ministry of Education, Wuhan, China, 2 School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, China, 3 School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, United Kingdom Abstract ERPs were elicited to (1) words, (2) pseudowords derived from these words, and (3) nonwords with no lexical neighbors, in a task involving listening to immediately repeated auditory stimuli. There was a significant early (P200) effect of phonotactic probability in the first auditory presentation, which discriminated words and pseudowords from nonwords; and a significant somewhat later (N400) effect of lexicality, which discriminated words from pseudowords and nonwords. There was no reliable effect of lexicality in the ERPs to the second auditory presentation. We conclude that early sublexical phonological processing differed according to phonotactic probability of the stimuli, and that lexically-based redintegration occurred for words but did not occur for pseudowords or nonwords. Thus, in online word recognition and immediate retrieval, phonological and/or sublexical processing plays a more important role than lexical level redintegration. Citation: Cheng X, Schafer G, Riddell PM (2014) Immediate Auditory Repetition of Words and Nonwords: An ERP Study of Lexical and Sublexical Processing. PLoS ONE 9(3): e91988. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091988 Editor: Kevin Paterson, University of Leicester, United Kingdom Received December 3, 2013; Accepted February 16, 2014; Published March 18, 2014 Copyright: ß 2014 Cheng et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
    [Show full text]
  • Computational Models of Working Memory: Putting Long-Term Memory Into Context
    Review TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.9 No.11 November 2005 Computational models of working memory: putting long-term memory into context Neil Burgess1 and Graham Hitch2 1Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Anatomy and Developmental Biology, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London, WC1N 3AR, UK 2Department of Psychology, York University, York, YO10 5DD, UK Detailed computational modeling of human memory important for the other and how they interact is a topical has typically been aimed at either short-term (working) question in memory research [12,13]. memory or long-term memory in isolation. However, One of the clearest examples of the dissociation recent research highlights the importance of inter- between STM and LTM comes from the Hebb repetition actions between these systems for both item and effect, in which repeated presentation of a digit sequence order information. At the same time, computational for ISR results in gradual learning of that sequence, even models of both systems are beginning to converge onto though presentation and recall of other sequences a common framework in which items are associated intervenes between the repetitions [14]. Hebb concluded with an evolving ‘context’ signal and subsequently that there must be some change in LTM from a single trial. compete with one another at recall. We review some Milner and colleagues subsequently found that the Hebb of these models, and discuss a common mechanism repetition effect for lists of digits or spatial locations capable of modelling working memory and its inter- depended on the integrity of the medial temporal lobe action with long-term memory, focussing on memory (more specifically, on the left and right hippocampi, for verbal sequences.
    [Show full text]
  • Effects of Irrelevant Auditory Stimuli on a Text Recognition and Text Recall Task
    Psychologia, 2007, 50, 133–149 EFFECTS OF IRRELEVANT AUDITORY STIMULI ON A TEXT RECOGNITION AND TEXT RECALL TASK Michiko MIYAHARA1) and Toru GOSHIKI2) 1)Kyoto University, Japan, 2)Shizuoka University, Japan This article aimed to investigate whether auditory stimuli disrupt the performance of a text recognition task (Experiment1) and a text recall task (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, Spanish speech sounds that disrupted the serial recall task (Miyahara & Goshiki, 2004) were presented during the learning phase of the task. The Spanish speech had a reduced d’, which indicates that the text recognition task was also disrupted. In Experiment 2, Japanese speech sounds or office noise were presented during either the learning only phase, recall only phase, or during both the phases. The results were that both types of auditory stimuli could disrupt the text recall task, and this effect was independent of the meaning of the speech sounds and the presenting phase. Our results could be interpreted by Cowan’s model (1995, 1999) with the inclusion of two modifications. Key words: text recognition, text recall, speech sound, office noise, irrelevant speech effect People carry out everyday cognitive activities in environments that are characterized by various irrelevant auditory stimuli. For example, unwanted noise or speech has been pointed out as a major source of attentional disruption in schools and workplaces (Hyggs, 2003; Landström, Söderberg, Kjellberg, & Nordström, 2002). Similarly, the performance of a cognitive task is disrupted by irrelevant and unattended sounds or speech despite an individual’s best efforts at ignoring them. (For a review, see Beaman, 2005; Banbury, Tremblay, Macken, & Jones, 2001; Jones, 1995).
    [Show full text]
  • Savill, Nicola ORCID: Ellis, Andrew W
    Savill, Nicola ORCID: https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6854-0658, Ellis, Andrew W. and Jefferies, Elizabeth (2016) Newly-acquired words are more phonologically robust in verbal short-term memory when they have associated semantic representations. Neuropsychologia, 98. pp. 85-97. Downloaded from: http://ray.yorksj.ac.uk/id/eprint/2501/ The version presented here may differ from the published version or version of record. If you intend to cite from the work you are advised to consult the publisher's version: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0028393216300690 Research at York St John (RaY) is an institutional repository. It supports the principles of open access by making the research outputs of the University available in digital form. Copyright of the items stored in RaY reside with the authors and/or other copyright owners. Users may access full text items free of charge, and may download a copy for private study or non-commercial research. For further reuse terms, see licence terms governing individual outputs. Institutional Repository Policy Statement RaY Research at the University of York St John For more information please contact RaY at [email protected] Newly-acquired words are more phonologically robust in verbal short-term memory when they have associated semantic representations Nicola Savill, Andrew W. Ellis and Elizabeth Jefferies Department of Psychology, University of York, UK To appear in Neuropsychologia doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.03.006 Corresponding Author: Nicola Savill Department of Psychology, University of York, Heslington, York YO10 5DD UK Email: [email protected] Tel: +44 (0)1904 322937 Fax: +44(0)1904 323181 Keywords: novel word learning; phonological representations; semantic binding; short-term memory; lexicalisation; meaning 1 Abstract Verbal short-term memory (STM) is a crucial cognitive function central to language learning, comprehension and reasoning, yet the processes that underlie this capacity are not fully understood.
    [Show full text]
  • An Analysis of Sentence Repetitions in a Single-Talker Interference Task
    AN ANALYSIS OF SENTENCE REPETITIONS IN A SINGLE-TALKER INTERFERENCE TASK Hilary Parlette A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate College of Bowling Green State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE May 2015 Committee: Alexander Goberman, Advisor Miriam Krause Ronald Scherer ii ABSTRACT Listening to speech is a complex process which is made more difficult when distractions, such as noise or other speech, occur at the same time. People often experience competing speech signals in daily life, such as at a noisy restaurant where they must regulate their attention to focus on the target and block out distractions. This study examined complex speech processing using a single-talker interference task, which requires participants to listen to two talkers simultaneously, ignoring a distracter talker while repeating a target talker. The talkers in this study were a native speaker of English (NS) and a non-native speaker of English (NNS). Twelve nonimpaired participants’ listening accuracy (measured by target words repeated) was observed in the contexts of nativeness of speaker, participant characteristics, and performance of standardized measures. No significant correlations were found between cognitive measures and repetition accuracy. An interaction effect was found between the variables of foreign language experience and repetition accuracy of the NS target sentences. This study also included a descriptive observation of participants’ repetition errors, intended as a first step toward identifying possible strategies nonimpaired individuals use in difficult listening situations. The most common types of errors participants made during sentence repetitions were phonetic and semantic errors. There were no significant correlations between error types and accuracy of target repetition.
    [Show full text]
  • S Short-Term Memory: Typical Development and Dyslexia
    JournalMemory & Cognition 2005, ??33 (?),(7), ???-???1210-1219 Phonological similarity neighborhoods and children’s short-term memory: Typical development and dyslexia JENNIFER M. THOMSON University College London, London, England and ULLA RICHARDSON and USHA GOSWAMI University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England In this article, we explore whether structural characteristics of the phonological lexicon affect serial recall in typically developing and dyslexic children. Recent work has emphasized the importance of long-term phonological representations in supporting short-term memory performance. This occurs via redintegration (reconstruction) processes, which show significant neighborhood density effects in adults. We assessed whether serial recall in children was affected by neighborhood density in word and nonword tasks. Furthermore, we compared dyslexic children with typically developing children of the same age or reading level. Dyslexic children are held to have impaired phonological representations of lexical items. These impaired representations may impair or prevent the use of long-term phonological representations to redintegrate short-term memory traces. We report significant rime neighborhood density effects for serial recall of both words and nonwords, for both dyslexic and typically develop- ing children. Phonological short-term memory plays an important rial recall posit a short-term phonological or articulatory role in the development of the lexicon. Gathercole and loop that retains verbal information on a temporary basis
    [Show full text]
  • How Does Chunking Help Working Memory?
    Zurich Open Repository and Archive University of Zurich Main Library Strickhofstrasse 39 CH-8057 Zurich www.zora.uzh.ch Year: 2019 How does chunking help working memory? Thalmann, Mirko ; Souza, Alessandra S ; Oberauer, Klaus Abstract: Chunking is the recoding of smaller units of information into larger, familiar units. Chunking is often assumed to help bypassing the limited capacity of working memory (WM). We investigate how chunks are used in WM tasks, addressing three questions: (a) Does chunking reduce the load on WM? Across four experiments chunking benefits were found not only for recall of the chunked but also ofother not-chunked information concurrently held in WM, supporting the assumption that chunking reduces load. (b) Is the chunking benefit independent of chunk size? The chunking benefit was independent of chunk size only if the chunks were composed of unique elements, so that each chunk could be replaced by its first element (Experiment 1), but not when several chunks consisted of overlapping sets of elements, disabling this replacement strategy (Experiments 2 and 3). The chunk-size effect is not due to differences in rehearsal duration as it persisted when participants were required to perform articulatory suppression (Experiment 3). Hence, WM capacity is not limited to a fixed number of chunks regardless of their size. (c) Does the chunking benefit depend on the serial position of the chunk? Chunks in early list positions improved recall of other, not-chunked material, but chunks at the end of the list did not. We conclude that a chunk reduces the load on WM via retrieval of a compact chunk representation from long-term memory that replaces the representations of individual elements of the chunk.
    [Show full text]