<<

Eye (1990) 4, 787-793

Risk Factors in

JOHAN SJOSTRAND and MATHS ABRAHAMSSON Goteborg, Sweden

Summary Any intervention to prevent serious amblyopia is based on the knowledge about normal versus subnormal visual development. Our ability to predict with high degree of certainty which children will develop amblyopia will be dependant on the characteristics of various risk factors for initiating the development of squint or amblyopia. We have used longitudinal studies of population based cohorts of young children to define some of these risk factors such as refractive errors. Three hundred and ten children with an :;:,:1. 0 D at one year of age were refracted yearly between the age one and four years. Astigmatism and were found to be highly variable during infancy and early childhood. Longitudinal follow-up seems to be needed to separate the normal from the abnormal development, which initiates the development of the amblyopia. Children with constant or increas­ ing astigmatism or anisometropia between one and four years were 'at risk'. In parallel we have studied important factors for successful treatment of amblyo­ pia. Based on these findings we conclude that a population screening at four years of age seems to be advantageous in Sweden in order detect and successfully treat most cases of amblyopia.

Any intervention to prevent serious amblyo­ uations of the efficiencyof the screening pro­ pia has to be based on our knowledge about grams are few.! An abundant literature normal versus abnormal visual development. describes that the three main causes of I will specificallyfocus on the refractive errors amblyopia are , refractive errors as key factors for initiation of amblyopia. and visual deprivation. The major portion of The definition used here for amblyopia is a all amblyopic cases are unilateral ones with condition due to visual deprivation and/or good vision in the other . Most also agree abnormal binocular interaction for which no that besides visual deprivation, abnormal bin­ organic cause can be detected by, for ocular interaction is important for the example, ophthalmoscopy and which is development of amblyopia. In a case with uni­ potentially reversible by therapy. Since our lateral visual deprivation both abnormal bin­ ability to prevent the development of amblyo­ ocular interaction and visual deprivation are pia are limited, an important task in good eye held to be strong amblyogenic factors? The health care is to detect manifest amblyopia at different subgroups with amblyopia are a treatable stage. Programmes for screening generally detected at different ages due to the for amblyopia have therefore been instituted underlying cause (Fig. 1). Well informed in several countries. However, critical eval- parents may detect ocular opacities in the

From: Department of , Sahlgren's Hospital, University of Goteborg, Goteborg, Sweden. Correspondence to: lohan Sjostrand, Department of Ophthalmology, Sahlgren's Hospital, S-413 45 Goteborg, Sweden. 788 J. SJOSTRAND AND M. ABRAHAMSSON

Strabismus , 'x�

Anisometropia

Congenital PA•••

Form deprivation

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

Age (years)

Fig.!. Thefour most common riskfactors associatedwith amblyopia. The bars indicate the period duringwhich amblyopia associated with these riskfactors generally are detected. of their infants and the onset of strabismus effective. Present knowledge does not allow during childhood. However, other amblyopia us to point out which of the refractive errors of types without obvious symptoms have to be infants are potential causes of amblyopia. detected by ophthalmological examination, Several studies have shown that newborn chil­ e.g. refractive errors (anisometropia, oblique dren exhibit a considerable amount of refrac­ astigmatism and ametropia) and small angle tive error at birth and that the magnitude of strabismus. A problem in amblyopia preven­ the decreases as the infant tion and treatment is the late age of presen­ grows older. 7-iO In many cases the refractive tation of cases with straight-eyed amblyopia error has totally disappeared by school going and small angle strabismus. In a study from age. Leicestershire Shaw and co-workers3 found In Goteborg we have been interested in the that the age at which the majority of children natural history of the development of refrac­ with anisometropic amblyopia presented was tive errors and have addressed the question five years or above whereas the strabismic how to separate normal from abnormal cases presented earlier. refractive development. In a longitudinal, The questions addressed here are: How can prospective study of infant astigmatism and its we separate normal from abnormal visual relationship to amblyopiall we have followed development? Can we, with our present the changes in refraction between the ages of knowledge, predict which children will one and four years in 310 infants with astig­ develop amblyopia? Can we prevent amblyo­ matism of 1 D or more at the age of one year pia before it starts by knowing these factors? during this three year period. The children How can we detect amblyopia at an age when were refracted by once a year it is still treatable in most cases? during the test period. Cyc1opentolate was given before retinoscopy. Refractive errors We found that the distribution of the spher­ Several studies have presented simple and ical equivalent in these children showed a effective methods based on photo refraction decrease of the spread during the test period for screening of refractive errors in infants (Fig. 2). This indicates that the emmetropisa­ and young children.4-6 Thus methods for per­ tion process has started. A change in the dis­ forming large scale refraction screening of tribution of the cylindrical refractive error children do exist, but do we have the knowl­ also occurred between one and four years of edge to interpret the results? The object of the age. Approximately one third of the cases screening is to identify, from apparently became non-astigmatic (Fig. 3). healthy individuals, those who are at risk of At the age of four one third of the patients developing a particular defect in the belief had a purely spherical refraction. A similar that early treatment will be easier and more decrease in the incidence of astigmatism has RISK FACTORS IN AMBLYOPIA 789

variability of anisometropia and an almost so unchanged incidence of anisometropia between one and 3 and a half years. Fig. 4 summarises the dynamics of anisometropia in our study8 with new cases entering the group with equivalent numbers becoming non-ani­ sometropic. It is also interesting to note that marked decreases in anisometropia was found -1yaar in some cases; for example in one child with ••••••••• 2years an anisometropia of 4 D, where this refractive

'-3yaars error almost vanished during the three year period. ········· 4yaars Relationship refractive error/amblyopia In order to evaluate refractive errors as risk factors for amblyopia we analysed the relationship between refractive errors and amblyopia in the group of infants with astigmatism.7 The group was followed longitudinally for three years and amblyopia was found in 7% of the children. The refraction data of these chil­ Spherical equivQlent (Dioptef5) dren were compared to the rest of the sample. Fig. 2. The distribution of the spherical equivalent In our follow-up analysis we used the fol­ (the refractive error of the least ametropic axis plus half lowing criteria for amblyopia: the difference between the tw o axes) measured yearly An acuity difference of O.llog unit or more from one tofour years. between the eyes measured with best correc­ tion on at least at two separate consecutive previously been shown by Ingram and Barr9 test sessions in eyes with no signs of disease; and Atkinson and co-workers.12 the acuity of the amblyopic eye improved at The dynamic changes of the astigmatic refraction are well illustrated by the fact that % the majority of all infants (age one year) with an astigmatism equivalent to or greater than two diopters showed a decrease of their astig­ matism. There were, however, some patients 40 with an anomalous behaviour; they showed a stationary or even increasing astigmatism, as 1 year 30 2 years they grew older. Three types of changes with 3 years time could be observed. The largest group 4 years (93%) showed a decrease, another group was stationary and the third group had an increas­ 20 ing refraction of the most hyperopic axis. Dynamic changes were also found with regard to anisometropia in our sample of chil­ dren.8 Eleven per cent of our astigmatic infants had an anisometropia of 1 D or more at the age of one year. At the age of four years there were still about 10% of children that had o I 2 4 anisometropia. However, most of these 'cases Cylindrical refraction (Dlopters were new cases that had developed their ani­ Fig. 3. Distribution of the cylindrical refraction error sometropia during the second, third or fourth measured yearly during the three year test period. A year. Ingram and Barr9 also found a marked plus cylinder convention was used. 790 J. SJOSTRAND AND M. ABRAHAMSSON

Year Year Year Year 1 2 3 4

Anisometropia

Non-anisometropia

Fig. 4. The variability of anisometropia. Non-anisometropia indicates the whole population based sample with no anisometropia. Each square represents one patient. The squares in the anisometropia group indicate patients becoming anisometropic since last year while grey squares in the non-anisometropic group are patients becoming non-anisometropic since last year.

least one line following the occlusion treat­ which is our basis of knowledge for planning ment. We separated the amblyopic cases in of selective or population screening? subgroups with respect to the severity of their Selective screening: Potential groups for amblyopia. The group with increasing astig­ selective screening are those with heredity, matism and stationary astigmatism had a clear mental retardation and other high risk fac- over-representation of amblyopia. In the sub­ group with increasing astigmatism almost one Table Risk factors for amblyopia in a sample of 310 third of the cases developed amblyopia. children (Abrahamsson et al)8 How informative then is a single refractive Refractive error RR* value? Ingram et al.13 and Atkinson et al.14 Against-the-rule astigmatism 0.1 have, in their pioneering studies, found that Astigmatism decreasing between infants with a bilateral high hypermetropia 1 and 4 years of age 0.2 have an increased risk for squint or Astigmatism �2.0 D at 1 year of age 0.6 amblyopia. Astigmatism <2.0 D at 1 year of age 1.1 Could we predict the risk for amblyopia in Astigmatism unchanged between 1 and 4 years of age 2.0 our sample? Our findingwas that the presence Anisometropia at 4 years of age 2.1 of two diopters of hyperopia in at least one With-the-rule astigmatism 3.1 meridian at one year did not define any 'at Anisometropia at 1 year of age 3.7 risk' population while it was related to some Hyperopia �3.5 D at 1 year of age 3.8 Astigmatism increasing between increased (2x) risk in the four-year group. 1 and 4 years of age 6.7 Choosing +3.5 D at one year as our cri­ Oblique astigmatism 14.5 terion increased the risk to four times that of Strabismus 14.7 the other population. Thus high hypermetro­ Against-the-rule, decreasing astigmatism 0.1 pia at one year of age has in our sample only a With-the-rule, decreasing astigmatism 2.1 moderately predictive value for amblyopia. Hyperopia �3.5 D and decreasing The importance of various refractive errors or astigmatism 2.8 Against-the-rule, increasing astigmatism 3.1 combinations of them as risk factors for With-the-rule, increasing astigmatism 4.1 amblyopia is presented in Table I. Increasing Hyperopia �3.5 D and increasing astigmatism, oblique astigmatism, and stra­ astigmatism 5.8 bismus were the factors with the highest rela­ Oblique, decreasing astigmatism 10.5 Oblique, increasing astigmatism 14.7 tive risk. Relative risk: How many times more likely are Detection by vision screening exposed persons to become diseased, relatively to How can we define the 'at risk' population non-exposed persons in a sample? RISK FACTORS IN AMBLYOPIA 791 tors. Among the early visual disorders the visual defect if they have a refractive error. presence of a family history of My colleagues Aurell and Norrsell (personal may enable us to detect those at high risk. In communication) in Goteborg have made the this group with visual defects in infancy it is interesting observation that only the children most important to detect bilateral visual with heredity in combination with an impairment and we have to screen selectively unchanged high hypermetropia are at risk of at an early age all those with a family history acquiring strabismus. These data in combi­ of congenital . Besides these obvious nation with the fact that strabismus shows evi­ reasons for visual assessment, we also have dence of multifactorial inheritancel8 indicate selectively to screen all mentally retarded that selective screening of siblings of affected children since they run a high risk of having a children at an age of about two years is of visual disorder. In preliminary studies in value. However, we need more information Sweden it has been shown that the relative about the predictive value of various risk fac­ risk of having bilateral of tors and of the possibility of preventing the 6/60 or less is more than one hundred times initiation of amblyopia or strabismus of later that of the normal population. It has also been onset in these cases. shown that the majority of these children need Population screening. The lack of highly due to refractive errors in order to be predictive risk factors for most cases of able to see well. The key to detection of early ambloypia makes it impossible to prevent or visual disorders otherwise, is probably good detect a major portion of cases presenting parent education in combination with atten­ with straight-eyed amblyopia and small-angle tion and increased awareness of visual strabismus by selective screening. One way to abnormalities and early referral of these cases accomplish the aim of detecting manifest by medical professionals. IS The important visual disorders including amblyopia is by message is: always to listen to the mother if population screening. Few studies have eval­ she tells that there is something abnormal uated the available methods for population about the eyes of her infant. Take infants with screening. Kohler and Stigmarl have under­ as one example. Some lined the importance of high attendance rate may be detected due to the heredity risk fac­ (possible to attain when included in a general tor but since the majority of the cases are idio­ health control) and an efficientacuity testing. pathic we have to rely on the information At four years of age the linear E test detected given by the parents in order to detect them the majority (97%) of eye disorders. I How­ early. According to our experience, in Gote­ ever, evaluations of the long term impli­ borg, the mother is able to give us the earliest cations of population vision screening for information about eye abnormality in two­ general eye health in the population are thirds of the cases (Sjostrand and Abrahams­ sparse. 19 son, unpublished) and the health care system and doctors in only one third of the cases. In Outcome of amblyopia therapy this group of children the importance of early How important is the age at presentation for detection is proven. If we can operate and the outcome of vision in cases with give contact to the child before the end amblyopia? of the development of the fixation reflex In the study from Leicestershire by Shaw et (about three months of age) the child will al.3 they demonstrated that strabismic cases have a much better visual prognosis. 16 generally were found before five years of age What about the risk factors for amblyopia whereas the straight-eyed amblyopes were with an onset after infancy? Can heredity help identified only in 15% of the cases before the us to identify the children at risk and is it age of five(the median age at presentation 6.3 worthwhile selectively to screen children with years of age). How successful is outcome of a family history of strabismus. therapy if amblyopia is detected at a later age? Ingram and Walkerl7 found that siblings of It hqs been shown by Flynn and Cassidy20 and children presenting with squint/amblyopia Oliver and co-workers21 that start of therapy have four times more chance of having a after seven to eight years of age has a worse 792 J. SJOSTRAND AND M. ABRAHAMSSON prognosis. We have turned the question abnormalities detected by parents, for ex stra­ around and asked if the age of presentation of bismus, should be referred for treatment as amblyopia of four years of age or before is early as possible after onset. Future longitudi­ compatible with successful treatment in most nal studies of the emmetropisation process cases. during eye growth seems to be of importance In a prospective study of 52 amblyopic cases for our understanding of refractive errors as aged 2! to eight years in Goteborg we studied key factors for initiation of amblyopia. the outcome of treatment in relationship to References age and type of amblyopia. A new procedure 1 Kohler L and Stigmar G: Vision screening of four­ for testing22 made it possible for year-old children. Acta Paediat Scand 1973, 62: us to test optotype acuity from 2! years and 17-27. onwards. After three and 3! years of age we 2 von Noorden GK: Amblyopia: A Multidisciplinary approach. Invest Ophthalmol & Vis Sci1985,26: tested with linear HOTV and the tumbling E 1704-16. test was used afterthe age of four to fiveyears. 3 Shaw DE, Fielder AR, Minshull C, Rosenthal AR: Our findings in this prospective study were Amblyopia-Factors influencing age of presen­ that the amblyopia was treatable in >95% of tation. Lancet1988, July: 207-9. 4 Sjostrand J, Abrahamsson M, Fabian G, Wennhall the cases, if straight-eyed amblyopes were 0: Photorefraction: A useful tool to detect refrac­ detected at four years and strabismic cases tion errors. Acta Ophthalmol 1982, (SuppJ) 157: early after onset. 46-52. The total number of cases with successful 5 Atkinson J, Braddick OJ, Durden K, Watson PG, Atkinson S: Screening for refractive errors in 6-9 treatment to within one line of difference or month old infants by photorefraction. Br J Oph· less between the eyes was high in all age thalmol1984, 68: 105-12. groups if the compliance was good. 6 Kaakinen K, Ranta-Kemppainen L: Screening of The few cases we observed in our study with infants for strabismus and refractive errors with two-flashphoto refraction with and without cyclo­ worse visual outcome were in the group with plegia. Acta Ophthalmol1986, 64: 578-82. bad compliance. These findings are in agree­ 7 Abrahamsson M, Fabian G, Andersson A-K, 0 ment with the observations by Oliver et af. 2 Sjostrand J: A longitudinal study of a population They found in a retrospective study that the based sample of astigmatic children. I: Refraction and amblyopia. Acta Ophthalmol 1990, 68: age of the patient was the most significantfac­ 428-34. tor influencingcompliance and thus the visual 8 Abrahamsson M, Fabian G, Andersson AK, Sj6s­ outcome. The most important factors for a trand J: A longitudinal study of a population successful outcome in our experience are based sample of astigmatic children. II: The summarised as follows: lower age at presen­ changebility of anisometropia. Acta Ophthalmol 1990, 68: 435-40. tation, good compliance, early referral of 9 Ingram RM and Barr A: Changes in refraction strabismic cases, and effective V.A. testing between the ages of 1 and 3 ± years. BrJ Ophthal· from 2! years and onwards. mol 1979; 63: 339-42. 0 1 Gordon RA and Donzis PB: Refractive develop­ ment of the . Invest Ophthalmol VisSci 1985, 103: 785-9. Conclusions 11 Abrahamsson M, Fabian G, Sjostrand J: Changes in Amblyopia is a condition that can be caused astigmatism between the ages of 1 and 4 years: A Br J Ophthalmol 1988, 72: by a number of factors, some of which are longitudinal study. .. 145-9. interrelated such as refractive errors, strabis­ 12 Atkinson J, Braddick 0, French J: Infant astigma­ mus and amblyopia. At present we are lacking tism: Its disappearance with age. VisionResearch easily obtainable risk factors for efficient, 1980,20: 891-3. selective screening in most cases. Identifica­ 13 Ingram RM, Walker C, Wilson JM, Arnold PE, tion of children at risk at a treatable age needs Dally S: Prediction of amblyopia and squint by means of refraction at age 1 year. Br J Ophthal­ different strategies for detection of straight­ mol 1986, 70: 12-15. eyed amblyopes, microstrabismus and large 14 Atkinson J and Braddick 0: Population vision angle strabismus. Our Swedish experience! is screening and individual visual assessment. In Jay that population screening based on efficient B, ed. ')Dectectio'n and Measurement of Visual Impairment in Pre-Vetbal' Children. Dordreclrt, linear acuity testing seems to be advanta­ Boston, Lancaster: Martinus NijhofflDr W. Junk geous. Other ambylopic cases, with Publishers 1986. 376-91. RISK FACTORS IN AMBLYOPIA 793

15 Hall DMB and Hall SM: Early detection of visual old children with and without previous vision defects in infancy. Br Med J 1988, 296: 823--4. screening. Acta Paediatr Scand 1979, 67: 373-7. 16 Parks NM: Visual results in aphakic children. AmJ 20 Flynn JT: Current trends in amblyopia therapy. Ophthalmol1982, 94: 441-9. Trans AmAc ad Ophthalmol Otolaryngol 1978, 17 Ingram RM and Walker C: Refraction as a means of 85: 428-50. predicting squint or amblyopia in preschool sib­ 21 Oliver M, Neumann R, Chaimovitch Y, Gotesman lings of children known to have these defects. BrJ N, Shimshoni M: Compliance and results of treat­ Ophthalmol1979, 63: 238-42. ment for amblyopia in children more than 8 years 18 Griitzner P, Yazawa K, Spivey BE: Heredity and old. AmJ Ophthalmol1986, 102: 340-5. strabismus. Surv Ophthalmol1970, 14: 441-56. 22 Lithander J: 'Kolt-test' Priifung der Sehschiirfe bei 19 Kohler L and Stigmar G: Visual disorders in 7-year- 2jiihrigen. Z praktAu genheilkd 1984, 5: 258.