Difficulty of Discrimination Modulates Attentional Capture by Regulating Attentional Focus

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Difficulty of Discrimination Modulates Attentional Capture by Regulating Attentional Focus Difficulty of Discrimination Modulates Attentional Capture by Regulating Attentional Focus Risa Sawaki1,2 and Jun’ichi Katayama1 Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/21/2/359/1759917/jocn.2008.21022.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Abstract & Attentional capture for distractors is enhanced by increas- results revealed that discrimination difficulty had opposite ing the difficulty of discrimination between the standard and effects on the P3a response between central and surround- the target in the three-stimulus oddball paradigm. In this ing locations. With an increase in the difficulty of discrim- study, we investigated the cognitive mechanism of this mod- ination, the P3a response was enhanced when distractor ulation of attentional capture. Event-related brain potentials stimuli were presented in the central location. In contrast, the were recorded from participants while they performed a visual P3a response was reduced when distractor stimuli were three-stimulus oddball paradigm (frequent standard, rare tar- presented in a surrounding location. This finding suggests get, and rare distractor). The discrimination difficulty between that spatial attention was focused by the difficulty of dis- standard and target was manipulated in the central location. crimination, and deviant processing was increased within its Distractor stimuli were presented in the central or surrounding focus but decreased outside its focus. Therefore, attentional locations. The P3a component was elicited by distractor stim- capture for deviant distractors is modulated by top–down uli and was used as a measure of attentional capture. The controlled attentional focus. & INTRODUCTION salience (Theeuwes, 2004; Friedman, Cycowicz, & Gaeta, In the external environment, we are faced with a vast 2001). amount of information. Our cognitive function, however, is limited with regard to its processing capacity, and Two Types of Attentional Capture thus, it is crucial to select the important information of a scene before further processing. Selective attention Attentional allocation for deviant information is well plays a critical role in such situations and it can be known as ‘‘attentional capture’’: attention is allocated broadly considered to comprise ‘‘top–down’’ and ‘‘bottom– to information which deviates in a spatial context (i.e., up’’ processes (Corbetta & Shulman, 2002; Kastner & spatial attentional capture) or in a temporal context (i.e., Ungerleider, 2000; Yantis & Egeth, 1999; Egeth & Yantis, temporal attentional capture) regardless of whether 1997; Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Theeuwes, 1991a). deviant information is relevant to ongoing activity. Be- The top–down process controls attention to select in- cause attentional capture of deviant information enables formation based on task goals (e.g., task-relevant loca- us to be aware of significant changes in our external tion,color,andshape).Sensoryprocessing,reaction environment, such processing is a fundamental cognitive time, and accuracy of target detection can be improved function for successful adaptation to a changing envi- by the top–down mechanism (Giesbrecht, Woldorff, ronment (Sokolov, 1963). Song, & Mangun, 2003; Hopfinger, Buonocore, & Many studies on visual attention have long investi- Mangun, 2000; Mangun, 1995; Mangun & Hillyard, 1988, gated spatial attentional capture by using the visual 1991; Eriksen & St. James, 1986; Posner, 1980). On the search paradigm. Spatial attention is captured by the other hand, the bottom–up process controls attention presence of an irrelevant singleton distractor, such as a to select information based on the properties of stimuli unique red stimulus among green stimuli (Theeuwes, themselves (e.g., salience, novelty, and rarity) regardless 1991a). The most critical factor of attentional capture is of task goals. Thus, attention is preferentially allocated stimulus salience, and visual search for the target is to deviant information which usually possesses stimulus distracted when such salient stimuli are irrelevant to the demanded task (e.g., Theeuwes, 1991a, 1994). In the literature, there is a debate concerning the degree to which top–down attentional control modulates atten- 1Hokkaido University, Sapporo, Japan, 2Japan Society for the tional capture. One view is that attentional capture is Promotion of Science, Japan entirely determined by the physical properties of the D 2008 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21:2, pp. 359–371 Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/jocn.2008.21022 by guest on 27 September 2021 stimuli: Deviant stimuli capture attention involuntarily distractor that occurs at the same spatial location as regardless of any top–down attentional control (e.g., the target, but at a different temporal position, can Hickey, McDonald, & Theeuwes, 2006; Theeuwes, 2004). produce attentional capture (Dalton & Lavie, 2006). The other view is that attentional capture is dynamically The other line of research is based on a series of modulated by top–down attentional control, and several studies, which will be discussed in detail after the next evidences in support of this view have been reported as section, using a three-stimulus oddball paradigm (e.g., discussed below. Sawaki & Katayama, 2007; Katayama & Polich, 1998) and First, attentional capture is dependent on the size of investigating the neural response in such a situation by the attentional focus/window to search a target. When recording event-related brain potentials (ERP). The observers have knowledge about where a target is like- three-stimulus oddball paradigm is based on the random Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/21/2/359/1759917/jocn.2008.21022.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 ly to appear, attention is sharply focused on that loca- occurrence of rare target and rare distractor stimuli that tion. Consequently, a distractor could be ignored even are embedded in a train of frequent standard stimuli. In though it has a high stimulus salience (Theeuwes, 1991b; the visual modality, each stimulus is discretely presented Yantis & Jonides, 1990). Furthermore, when a target at the center of a display, and the participant has to stimulus becomes less salient, participants engage in respond to the target by pressing a button or by silent serial search with a small size of attentional focus, and counting. Although these two research streams utilize a the distraction effect is attenuated (Theeuwes, 1991b, different paradigm and measure of attentional capture, 2004; Theeuwes & Burger, 1998). The size of attentional both have confirmed that a spatial attentional shift is not focus is controlled by top–down processes according to necessary for producing attentional capture. In contrast task demands, and a distractor captures attention only to spatial attentional capture, there are few studies, when parallel search with widely distributed attention is which have investigated cognitive mechanisms of tem- attempted to detect a target. poral attentional capture, in particular the involvement Second, attentional capture is determined by top– of bottom–up and top–down attentional control. The down attentional settings. The distractor captures atten- present study focused on attentional capture in the tion only when a property of the eliciting stimulus (e.g., three-stimulus oddball paradigm. abrupt onset or color) matches with the top–down at- tentional setting of the participants (Folk & Remington, Temporal Attentional Capture in the 1998,1999;Folk,Remington,&Wright,1994;Folk, Three-stimulus Oddball Paradigm Remington, & Johnston, 1992) or when participants adapt ‘‘singleton search mode,’’ in which attention is In the three-stimulus oddball paradigm, target stimuli strategically set to respond to a deviation in general elicit a ‘‘P3’’ or ‘‘P300,’’ which is a large positive-going (Lamy & Egeth, 2003; Bacon & Egeth, 1994). ERP that has a maximum amplitude over central/parietal Finally, attentional capture is influenced by cognitive or parietal electrode sites with a peak latency of about load. The load theory of attention (Lavie, 1995, 2005; 300–600 msec, depending on the stimulus modality and Lavie, Hirst, de Fockert, & Viding, 2004; Lavie & Tsal, task difficulty (Kok, 2001; Verleger, 1997; Picton, 1992; 1994) proposes that distractor processing depends on Sutton, Braren, Zubin, & John, 1965). This type of P3 is the type (perceptual or cognitive) and level (low or high) usually called ‘‘P3b’’ or ‘‘target P3’’ and is thought to of load involved in task-relevant processing. According reflect the neural response regarding a context updating to this theory, perceptual load determines the process- process triggered by task-relevant information (Donchin ing of distractor stimuli at the early perception stage and & Coles, 1988; Donchin, 1981). The P3b response is relates to space-based distractor filtering. On the other reduced and delayed when target stimuli are similar hand, cognitive load determines the efficiency of se- to standard stimuli or when they have a high stimu- lective attention by top–down attentional control and lus probability (Polich, 1986; Courchesne, Hillyard, & relates to the attentional capture modulation. When cog- Courchesne, 1977; Duncan-Johnson & Donchin, 1977). nitive load rises with increasing task demand on working Under certain conditions, distractor stimuli also elicit memory, task-relevance-based top–down attentional P3, but with a more anterior distribution than the tar- control
Recommended publications
  • The Development of the Contingent Negative Variation During an Executive Functioning/Planning Task in Children, Adolescents, and Adults
    THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE CONTINGENT NEGATIVE VARIATION DURING AN EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING/PLANNING TASK IN CHILDREN, ADOLESCENTS, AND ADULTS By KIMBERLY ANDERSON A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2006 1 Copyright 2006 by Kimberly Anderson 2 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to thank my wonderful advisor Dr. Keith Berg for his dedication during my entire time at graduate school. I would also like to thank the many research assistants that gave their time to helping complete this project, it could not have been done without their tireless efforts. Lastly, I want to thank my best friend, Teri DeLucca, for her constant smiles and uplifting prayers. 3 TABLE OF CONTENTS page ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ...............................................................................................................3 LIST OF TABLES...........................................................................................................................6 LIST OF FIGURES .........................................................................................................................7 ABSTRACT.....................................................................................................................................9 CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................................10 2 LITERATURE REVIEW.......................................................................................................11
    [Show full text]
  • Multiple Electrophysiological Markers of Visual-Attentional Processing in a Novel Task Directed Toward Clinical Use
    Hindawi Publishing Corporation Journal of Ophthalmology Volume 2012, Article ID 618654, 11 pages doi:10.1155/2012/618654 Research Article Multiple Electrophysiological Markers of Visual-Attentional Processing in a Novel Task Directed toward Clinical Use Julie Bolduc-Teasdale,1, 2, 3 Pierre Jolicoeur,2, 3 and Michelle McKerral1, 2, 3 1 Centre for Interdisciplinary Research in Rehabilitation and Lucie-Bruneau Rehabilitation Centre, 2275 Laurier Avenue East, Montreal, QC, Canada H2H 2N8 2 Centre for Research in Neuropsychology and Cognition, University of Montreal, C.P. 6128, Succursale Centre-Ville, Montreal, QC, Canada H3C 3J7 3 Department of Psychology, University of Montreal, C.P. 6128, Succursale Centre-Ville, Montreal, QC, Montr´eal, QC, Canada H3C 3J7 Correspondence should be addressed to Michelle McKerral, [email protected] Received 2 July 2012; Accepted 16 September 2012 Academic Editor: Shigeki Machida Copyright © 2012 Julie Bolduc-Teasdale et al. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Individuals who have sustained a mild brain injury (e.g., mild traumatic brain injury or mild cerebrovascular stroke) are at risk to show persistent cognitive symptoms (attention and memory) after the acute postinjury phase. Although studies have shown that those patients perform normally on neuropsychological tests, cognitive symptoms remain present, and there is a need for more precise diagnostic tools. The aim of this study was to develop precise and sensitive markers for the diagnosis of post brain injury deficits in visual and attentional functions which could be easily translated in a clinical setting.
    [Show full text]
  • Emitted P3a and P3b in Chronic Schizophrenia and in First-Episode Schizophrenia
    Emitted P3a and P3b in Chronic Schizophrenia and in First-Episode Schizophrenia by Alexis McCathern Neuroscience and Psychology, BPhil, University of Pittsburgh, 2017 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of University Honors College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Philosophy University of Pittsburgh 2017 UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH UNIVERISTY HONORS COLLEGE This thesis was presented by Alexis McCathern It was defended on April 3, 2017 and approved by John Foxe, PhD, Department of Neuroscience, University of Rochester Michael Pogue-Geile, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh Stuart Steinhauer, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine Thesis Director: Dean Salisbury, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine ii Copyright © by Alexis McCathern 2017 iii EMITTED P3A AND P3B IN CHRONIC SCHIZOPHRENIA AND IN FIRST- EPISODE SCHIZOPHRENIA Alexis McCathern, BPhil University of Pittsburgh, 2017 Neurophysiological biomarkers may be useful for identifying the presence of schizophrenia and the schizophrenia prodrome among at-risk individuals prior to the emergence of psychosis. This study examined the emitted P3 to absent stimuli on a tone counting task in patients with chronic schizophrenia and newly-diagnosed patients. The P3 is biphasic, with the earlier peak (P3a) reflecting automatic orienting and the later peak (P3b) reflecting cognitive processing. Twenty- four individuals with long-term schizophrenia (minimum 5 years diagnosis; SZ) were compared to 24 matched controls (HCSZ), and 23 individuals within 6 months of their first psychotic episode (FE) were compared to 22 matched controls (HCFE). Participants were presented with standard sets of four identical tones (1 kHz, 50 ms, 330 ms SOA, 750 ms ITI).
    [Show full text]
  • Modality-Specific and Modality-General Encoding of Auditory and Visual Rhythms
    UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones 5-1-2012 Modality-Specific and Modality-General Encoding of Auditory and Visual Rhythms Amanda Pasinski University of Nevada, Las Vegas Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalscholarship.unlv.edu/thesesdissertations Part of the Cognition and Perception Commons Repository Citation Pasinski, Amanda, "Modality-Specific and Modality-General Encoding of Auditory and Visual Rhythms" (2012). UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones. 1608. http://dx.doi.org/10.34917/4332589 This Thesis is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It has been brought to you by Digital Scholarship@UNLV with permission from the rights-holder(s). You are free to use this Thesis in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/ or on the work itself. This Thesis has been accepted for inclusion in UNLV Theses, Dissertations, Professional Papers, and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Digital Scholarship@UNLV. For more information, please contact [email protected]. MODALITY-SPECIFIC AND MODALITY-GENERAL ENCODING OF AUDITORY AND VISUAL RHYTHMS by Amanda Claire Pasinski Bachelor of Arts University of Nevada, Las Vegas 2007 A thesis document submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Master of Arts in Psychology Department
    [Show full text]
  • Mismatch Negativity and Stimulus-Preceding Negativity in Paradigms of Increasing Auditory Complexity: a Possible Role in Predictive Coding
    entropy Article Mismatch Negativity and Stimulus-Preceding Negativity in Paradigms of Increasing Auditory Complexity: A Possible Role in Predictive Coding Francisco J. Ruiz-Martínez * , Antonio Arjona and Carlos M. Gómez Human Psychobiology Lab, Experimental Psychology Department, University of Sevilla, 41018 Seville, Spain; [email protected] (A.A.); [email protected] (C.M.G.) * Correspondence: [email protected] Abstract: The auditory mismatch negativity (MMN) has been considered a preattentive index of auditory processing and/or a signature of prediction error computation. This study tries to demon- strate the presence of an MMN to deviant trials included in complex auditory stimuli sequences, and its possible relationship to predictive coding. Additionally, the transfer of information between trials is expected to be represented by stimulus-preceding negativity (SPN), which would possibly fit the predictive coding framework. To accomplish these objectives, the EEG of 31 subjects was recorded during an auditory paradigm in which trials composed of stimulus sequences with increasing or decreasing frequencies were intermingled with deviant trials presenting an unexpected ending. Our results showed the presence of an MMN in response to deviant trials. An SPN appeared during the intertrial interval and its amplitude was reduced in response to deviant trials. The presence of Citation: Ruiz-Martínez, F.J.; Arjona, an MMN in complex sequences of sounds and the generation of an SPN component, with different A.; Gómez, C.M. Mismatch Negativity and Stimulus-Preceding amplitudes in deviant and standard trials, would support the predictive coding framework. Negativity in Paradigms of Increasing Auditory Complexity: A Possible Keywords: mismatch negativity; stimulus preceding negativity; contingent negative variation; Role in Predictive Coding.
    [Show full text]
  • ERP Peaks Review 1 LINKING BRAINWAVES to the BRAIN
    ERP Peaks Review 1 LINKING BRAINWAVES TO THE BRAIN: AN ERP PRIMER Alexandra P. Fonaryova Key, Guy O. Dove, and Mandy J. Maguire Psychological and Brain Sciences University of Louisville Louisville, Kentucky Short title: ERPs Peak Review. Key Words: ERP, peak, latency, brain activity source, electrophysiology. Please address all correspondence to: Alexandra P. Fonaryova Key, Ph.D. Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences 317 Life Sciences, University of Louisville Louisville, KY 40292-0001. [email protected] ERP Peaks Review 2 Linking Brainwaves To The Brain: An ERP Primer Alexandra Fonaryova Key, Guy O. Dove, and Mandy J. Maguire Abstract This paper reviews literature on the characteristics and possible interpretations of the event- related potential (ERP) peaks commonly identified in research. The description of each peak includes typical latencies, cortical distributions, and possible brain sources of observed activity as well as the evoking paradigms and underlying psychological processes. The review is intended to serve as a tutorial for general readers interested in neuropsychological research and a references source for researchers using ERP techniques. ERP Peaks Review 3 Linking Brainwaves To The Brain: An ERP Primer Alexandra P. Fonaryova Key, Guy O. Dove, and Mandy J. Maguire Over the latter portion of the past century recordings of brain electrical activity such as the continuous electroencephalogram (EEG) and the stimulus-relevant event-related potentials (ERPs) became frequent tools of choice for investigating the brain’s role in the cognitive processing in different populations. These electrophysiological recording techniques are generally non-invasive, relatively inexpensive, and do not require participants to provide a motor or verbal response.
    [Show full text]
  • Perceptual Functions of Auditory Neural Oscillation Entrainment Perceptual Functions of Auditory Neural Oscillation Entrainment
    Perceptual functions of auditory neural oscillation entrainment Perceptual functions of auditory neural oscillation entrainment By Andrew Chang, Bachelor of Science A Thesis Submitted to the School of Graduate Studies in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy McMaster University © Copyright by Andrew Chang July 29, 2019 McMaster University Doctor of Philosophy (2019) Hamilton, Ontario (Psychology, Neuroscience & Behaviour) TITLE: Perceptual functions of auditory neural oscillation entrainment AUTHOR: Andrew Chang (McMaster University) SUPERVISOR: Dr. Laurel J. Trainor NUMBER OF PAGES: xviii, 134 ii Lay Abstract Perceiving speech and musical sounds in real time is challenging, because they occur in rapid succession and each sound masks the previous one. Rhythmic timing regularities (e.g., musical beats, speech syllable onsets) may greatly aid in overcoming this challenge, because timing regularity enables the brain to make temporal predictions and, thereby, anticipatorily prepare for perceiv- ing upcoming sounds. This thesis investigated the perceptual and neural mechanisms for tracking auditory rhythm and enhancing perception. Per- ceptually, rhythmic regularity in streams of tones facilitates pitch percep- tion. Neurally, multiple neural oscillatory activities (high-frequency power, low-frequency phase, and their coupling) track auditory inputs, and they are associated with distinct perceptual mechanisms (enhancing sensitivity or de- creasing reaction time), and these mechanisms are coordinated to proactively track rhythmic regularity and enhance audition. The findings start the dis- cussion of answering how the human brain is able to process and understand the information in rapid speech and musical streams. iii Abstract Humans must process fleeting auditory information in real time, such as speech and music.
    [Show full text]
  • Attention to Features Precedes Attention to Locations in Visual Search: Evidence from Electromagnetic Brain Responses in Humans
    1822 • The Journal of Neuroscience, February 25, 2004 • 24(8):1822–1832 Behavioral/Systems/Cognitive Attention to Features Precedes Attention to Locations in Visual Search: Evidence from Electromagnetic Brain Responses in Humans Jens-Max Hopf,1,2 Kai Boelmans,1 Mircea Ariel Schoenfeld,1 Steven J. Luck,3 and Hans-Jochen Heinze1 1Department of Neurology II, Otto-von-Guericke-University, D-39120 Magdeburg, Germany, 2Leibniz-Institute for Neurobiology, D-39120 Magdeburg, Germany, and 3Department of Psychology, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa 52242-1407 Single-unit recordings in macaque extrastriate cortex have shown that attentional selection of nonspatial features can operate in a location-independent manner. Here, we investigated analogous neural correlates at the neural population level in human observers by using simultaneous event-related potential (ERP) and event-related magnetic field (ERMF) recordings. The goals were to determine (1) whether task-relevant features are selected before attention is allocated to the location of the target, and (2) whether this selection reflects the locations of the relevant features. A visual search task was used in which the spatial distribution of nontarget items with attended feature values was varied independently of the location of the target. The presence of task-relevant features in a given location led to a changeinERP/ERMFactivitybeginningϳ140msecafterstimulusonset,withaneuraloriginintheventraloccipito-temporalcortex.This effect was independent of the location of the actual target. This effect was followed by lateralized activity reflecting the allocation of attention to the location of the target (the well known N2pc component), which began at ϳ170 msec poststimulus. Current source localization indicated that the allocation of attention to the location of the target originated in more anterior regions of occipito-temporal cortex anterior than the feature-related effects.
    [Show full text]
  • Stimulus Frequency and Masking As Determinants of P300 Latency in Event-Related Potentials from Auditory Stimuli *
    Biological Psycho&v 21 (1985) 309-318 309 North-Holland STIMULUS FREQUENCY AND MASKING AS DETERMINANTS OF P300 LATENCY IN EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS FROM AUDITORY STIMULI * John POLICH Department of Neurology, California College of Medicine, University of California, Irvine, CA 92717, U.S.A. Lawrence HOWARD School of Social Sciences, University of California, Irvine, CA 92717, U.S.A Arnold STARR Department of Neurology, California College of Medicine, University of Californra, twine, CA 92717, U.S.A. Accepted for publication 8 May 1985 The effects of target tone frequency, presence of a masking stimulus. and subject sex on the auditory ERP were studied with an ‘oddball’ paradigm. P300 latency became shorter (about 15 msec) as the difference between the standard (1000 Hz) and target tone frequency increased (1500, 2000, 4000 Hz) but became longer (about 10 msec) with the presence of a white noise masking stimulus. Similar results were obtained for both the P3a and P3b subcomponents of the P300 potential. No significant differences between the adult male and female subjects were observed. The role of stimulus parameters in applied testing situations is discussed. 1. Introduction The P300 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP) is a large (5-15 luv), positive-going waveform that occurs with a modal latency of about 300 msec in normal young adults. Although the neurophysiology underlying the P300 is still being explored (Halgren, Squires, Wilson, Rohrbaugh, Bab and Crandall, 1980; Okada, Kaufman and Williamson, 1983), the cognitive events associated with its generation have received considerable attention (Donchin, 1981; Donchin, Ritter and McCallum, 1978; Pritchard, 1981).
    [Show full text]
  • XXIII National Congress of the Italian Society of Psychophysiology Main
    XXIII National Congress of the Italian Society of Psychophysiology – Scientific Program XXIII National Congress of the Italian Society of Psychophysiology PROCEEDINGS - ABSTRACTS Main lectures Sinigaglia C. The space of action 51 Strata P. The responsible mind 51 Symposia abstracts Addabbo M. - Bolognini N. - Nava E. - Turati C. The processing of others’ touch early in human development 53 Betti V. Dynamic reorganization of functional connectivity 54 during naturalist viewing Bevilacqua V. Home environment perception and virtual reality: cognitive 55 and emotional reactions to domotic induced changes of colours and sounds Bocci T. From metaplasticity to interhemispheric connectivity: 56 electrophysiological interrogation of human visual cortical circuits in health and disease Bortoletto M. TMS-EEG coregistration in the exploration of human cortical 57 connectivity Bove M. An integrated approach to investigate the human neuroplasticity 57 Brattico E. The impact of sound environments on the brain circuits 58 for mood, emotion, and pain Cattaneo L. The motor cortex as integrator in sensorimotor behavior 59 Cecchetti L. How (lack of) vision shapes the morphological architecture 60 of the human brain: congenital blindness affects diencephalic but not mesencephalic structures Neuropsychological Trends – 18/2015 http://www.ledonline.it/neuropsychologicaltrends/ 6 XXIII National Congress of the Italian Society of Psychophysiology – Scientific Program Costantini M. Objects within the sensorimotor system 61 De Cesarei A. - Codispoti M. Relationship between perception and emotional responses 61 to static natural scenes De Pascalis V. - Scacchia P. Influence of pain expectation and hypnotizability 62 on auditory startle responses during placebo analgesia in waking and hypnosis: a brain potential study Garbarini F. Feeling sensations on the other’s body after brain damages 63 Gazzola V.
    [Show full text]
  • Gamma and Beta Frequency Oscillations in Response to Novel Auditory Stimuli: a Comparison of Human Electroencephalogram (EEG) Data with in Vitro Models
    Gamma and beta frequency oscillations in response to novel auditory stimuli: A comparison of human electroencephalogram (EEG) data with in vitro models Corinna Haenschel*†, Torsten Baldeweg‡, Rodney J. Croft*, Miles Whittington§, and John Gruzelier* *Division of Neuroscience and Psychological Medicine, Imperial College School of Medicine, London W6 8RP, United Kingdom; ‡Institute of Child Health and Great Ormond Street Hospital for Sick Children, University College London, London WC1N 2AP, United Kingdom; and §School of Biomedical Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9NL, United Kingdom Communicated by Nancy J. Kopell, Boston University, Boston, MA, April 10, 2000 (received for review December 15, 1999) Investigations using hippocampal slices maintained in vitro have neuronal assembly within which synchronization is possible (11). demonstrated that bursts of oscillatory field potentials in the This observation is complementary to the demonstration that gamma frequency range (30–80 Hz) are followed by a slower long-range (multimodal) sensory coding is performed at beta oscillation in the beta 1 range (12–20 Hz). In this study, we frequencies, whereas local synchronization occurs at gamma demonstrate that a comparable gamma-to-beta transition is seen frequencies (12). in the human electroencephalogram (EEG) in response to novel Transient bursts of gamma oscillations in the human electro- auditory stimuli. Correlations between gamma and beta 1 activity encephalogram (EEG) can be detected in response to sensory revealed a high degree of interdependence of synchronized oscil- stimulation (13). They can either be tightly time and phase lations in these bands in the human EEG. Evoked (stimulus-locked) locked to the stimulus (termed stimulus-evoked gamma oscilla- gamma oscillations preceded beta 1 oscillations in response to tions) (14), or they may occur with variable latency (termed novel stimuli, suggesting that this may be analogous to the stimulus-induced gamma oscillations) (15).
    [Show full text]
  • Homologues of Human Event-Related Potential Components in Nonhuman
    MONKEY ERPs 1 In press: to appear in The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components. Luck, S.J. & Kappenman, E.S. (Eds.), New York: Oxford University Press. Homologues of human ERP components in nonhuman primates Geoffrey F. Woodman Vanderbilt University, Vanderbilt Vision Research Center, Center for Integrative and Cognitive Neuroscience Running Head: MONKEY ERPs Correspondence from the editor/publisher should be addressed to: Geoffrey F. Woodman Department of Psychology Vanderbilt University PMB 407817 2301 Vanderbilt Place Nashville, TN 37240-7817 615-343-8901 (telephone) 615-343-8449 (fax) [email protected] (e-mail) MONKEY ERPs 2 Like many electrophysiologists who record the electroencephalogram (EEG) and event- related potentials (ERPs) from humans, I was a heavy user of the techniques before I became aware of the fact that EEG activity was originally observed during recordings from animals, including nonhuman primates (Caton, 1875). It was the 50-year-old studies with animals that motivated Hans Berger’s discovery and naming of the EEG recorded from his son, Klaus, and other human subjects (Berger, 1929). Moreover, many users of the ERP technique may be surprised to learn just how rarely the ERP components we use as tools to study human cognition have been studied in other model species, such as nonhuman primates. This chapter chronicles the discovery of ERP components in nonhuman primates. I focus mainly on monkeys but also include evidence from other species when it exists. The discussion generally unfolds chronologically beginning with work from the 19th century and continuing up through current research. During this review I will address differences in the methods and tasks that have been utilized to record ERPs in the different species.
    [Show full text]