www.nature.com/scientificreports

OPEN Neurodynamics and connectivity during facial fear perception: The role of threat exposure and signal Received: 23 June 2017 Accepted: 9 January 2018 congruity Published: xx xx xxxx Cody A. Cushing1, Hee Yeon Im1,2, Reginald B. Adams Jr.3, Noreen Ward1, Daniel N. Albohn3, Troy G. Steiner3 & Kestutis Kveraga 1,2

Fearful faces convey threat cues whose meaning is contextualized by eye gaze: While averted gaze is congruent with facial fear (both signal avoidance), direct gaze (an approach signal) is incongruent with it. We have previously shown using fMRI that the is engaged more strongly by fear with averted gaze during brief exposures. However, the amygdala also responds more to fear with direct gaze during longer exposures. Here we examined previously unexplored oscillatory responses to characterize the neurodynamics and connectivity during brief (~250 ms) and longer (~883 ms) exposures of fearful faces with direct or averted eye gaze. We performed two experiments: one replicating the exposure time by gaze direction interaction in fMRI (N = 23), and another where we confrmed greater early phase locking to averted-gaze fear (congruent threat signal) with MEG (N = 60) in a network of face processing regions, regardless of exposure duration. Phase locking to direct-gaze fear (incongruent threat signal) then increased signifcantly for brief exposures at ~350 ms, and at ~700 ms for longer exposures. Our results characterize the stages of congruent and incongruent facial threat signal processing and show that stimulus exposure strongly afects the onset and duration of these stages.

When we look at a face, we can glean a wealth of information, such as age, sex, health, afective state, and atten- tional focus. Te latter two signals are typically, but not exclusively, carried by emotional expression and eye gaze direction, respectively. Depending on the emotional expression and gaze, we can recognize how happy, angry, or fearful a person is, and infer the source or target of that emotion1. In an initial examination of the interaction between eye gaze and facial emotion, Adams and colleagues introduced the “shared signal hypoth- esis”2–4. Tis hypothesis predicts that there is facilitation of afective processing for combinations of emotional expression and eye gaze that share a congruent, matching signal for approach-avoidance behavior. In support of this hypothesis, using speeded reaction time tasks and self-reported intensity of emotion perceived, Adams and Kleck3,4 found that direct gaze facilitated processing efciency and accuracy, and increased the perceived emotional intensity of approach-oriented emotions (e.g., anger and joy). Conversely, averted gaze facilitated per- ception of avoidance-oriented emotions (e.g., fear and sadness). Several other groups have now also found similar results, including a replication by Sander et al.5 using dynamic threat displays, another using a difusion model of decision making and reaction time6, and another examining efects on refexive orienting to threat7. Perhaps the most compelling behavioral replication of this efect was a study by Milders and colleagues8, who found that direct-gaze anger and averted-gaze fear were detected more readily in an attentional blink paradigm compared to averted-gaze anger and direct-gaze fear, suggesting that congruent pairings (i.e., shared signals) of gaze and emo- tion attract more preconscious attentional awareness. Similar interaction efects have been found at the neural level as well, including in several fMRI studies looking at amygdala responses to diferent gaze directions by threat displays, including our own (see too9–11). Some have also suggested that gaze influences the ambiguity surrounding the source of threat. Whalen and colleagues, for instance, hypothesized that amygdala activation is directly proportional to the amount of

1Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA, USA. 2Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA. 3Department of Psychology, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, PA, USA. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to K.K. (email: [email protected])

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 1 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

ambiguity that surrounds the source of a perceived threat12, which suggests that direct-gaze fear is a more ambig- uous signal than direct-gaze anger. Anger signals both the source of the threat and where it is directed. In the case of fear, the observer knows there is a threat, but direct eye gaze does not indicate the source of the threat - unless it is the observer. Tus, averted eye gaze is more informative in resolving the source of threat for fear, and direct gaze is more informative for anger. In both of these accounts of threat-related ambiguity (shared signals and source of threat detection), direct-gaze fear is considered a more ambiguous combination of cues than averted-gaze fear. Initial eforts to study the neural underpinnings of the perception of these compound threat cues revealed greater amygdala activation in response to incongruent compound threat cues, specifcally fearful faces with a direct gaze and anger faces with averted gaze13,14. Some follow-up studies, however, have found the opposite inter- action: fear with averted eye gaze evoked higher amygdala activation9,10. To address this, Adams et al.15 proposed that presentation duration might help explain these diferences. We hypothesized that brief presentations trigger more refexive processing, which is thought to be preferentially tuned to congruent threat cues (averted-gaze fear), and longer presentations engage more refective processing of the less salient, incongruent threat cues (direct-gaze fear, see e.g.16–18 for discussions of refexive vs. refective processing). Indeed, previous studies fnding stronger amygdala activation in response to averted-gaze fear used relatively brief stimulus exposure times (e.g.9 used 300 ms stimulus durations), whereas those reporting higher amygdala activation in response to direct-gaze fear had longer stimulus exposure times (e.g.,14,19 used 2 s and 1 s stimulus durations, respectively). Adams et al.15 put this hypothesis to the test in the context of three studies using fMRI, with varying presentation parameters during a constant 1.5 s trial. In a direct comparison, this work revealed that amygdala responses were enhanced for fear- ful faces coupled with averted gaze, when rapidly presented (300 ms), and to fearful faces coupled with direct eye gaze, when presented for a sustained duration (1 s). The Current Work The primary goal of the present study was to elucidate the fine-grained neural dynamics of this previously observed response by replicating and extending this work using magnetoencephalography (MEG) to record neu- ral activity in response to brief (250 ms) and longer (883 ms) presentations of fearful faces with direct and averted gaze. MEG allows us to elucidate not only the temporal evolution of neural activity, but also frequency-specifc oscillatory activity in response to the stimulus, including highly temporally resolved interregional connectivity patterns during perception of these compound threat cues from the face. We utilized source localization to obtain good spatial resolution to identify the temporally sensitive contributions of key brain regions in the extended face processing network: the Fusiform Face Area (FFA), (PAC), posterior superior temporal (pSTS), and (OFC), as well as the earliest cortical visual region V1. Tese regions are well known to be involved in either face perception and social communication, or gaze perception, if not both (e.g.,20–24). STS in general has been shown to be sensitive to gaze25, as has the fusiform gyrus26. In addition, both have been shown to be sensitive to facial expression27. Te STS and OFC are also implicated as nodes in the proposed “social brain” consisting of amygdala, OFC, and STS28. Te posterior portion of STS also has been implicated as specializing in inferring intentionality from social cues29,30. Whether or not amygdala activity can be source localized from MEG data is actively debated in the MEG literature. However, there is accumulating evi- dence now that MEG activity can indeed be localized to the subcortical nuclei of the amygdala31–35, but support- ing or advancing this claim is not the goal of our manuscript. Periamygdaloid cortex (PAC) is heavily involved in conveying inputs and outputs of the deeper amygdala nuclei, the contralateral PAC, as well many other cortical regions36,37. While we cannot be certain which of the amygdala nuclei the activity is coming from (a situation similar to all but the highest resolution fMRI studies), given the reliable activation of the amygdala in all of the previous studies using this paradigm it is probable that at least some of the activity may arise in the subcortical nuclei of the amygdala. To truly understand how a network of brain regions responds to a given task, it is necessary to not only look at the response of each region individually, but to also examine how the regions interact38. Tus, we sought to characterize the phase locking between regions in the present work as a measure of functional connectivity within our extended face processing network. Tis approach allows us to build a more complete picture of the neurody- namics at play in the perception of facial threat cues. Examination of phase locking across trials within a region provides estimates of variance across trials that is not directly available in typical evoked response analyses while also having the beneft of being more robust against artifacts39. To examine the frequency-specifc responses in our ROIs and assess functional connectivity between them, we computed phase-locking estimates in and between the evoked responses of these regions. Phase locking (in which the magnitude of oscillatory activity is normal- ized) was chosen over power analyses (a magnitude-dependent measure of synchronized neuronal fring) due to the fact that phase locking is by nature more informative of the timing of activity within a region as well as the timing of functional connectivity between regions40. Phase locking is also thought to be a more trustworthy meas- ure of higher-level functions41. Oscillations in α-band (8–13 Hz) have been implicated as being involved in task selection or disengagement from the task42–45 while β-band (13–30 Hz) activity is indicative of active cognitive processing46–49. By examining activity in these bands both locally and between regions, we sought to elucidate the neurodynamics underlying the diferential processing of threat cues portrayed by direct and averted gaze on a fearful face observed by Adams et al.15 when exposure duration is manipulated. We were primarily interested in characterizing how exposure duration within a constant time frame infuences threat processing as a function of threat cue congruity associated with direct or averted gaze, previously found to evoke diferential activation using fMRI15. An adaptive response to threat cues would be a combination of refexive and refective processes that enables appropriate and timely responses to clear threat signals (e.g., feeing an attacker) while also inhibiting context-inappropriate or maladaptive responses to more ambiguous threat cues (e.g., feeing from someone who is seeking help). Temporally, initial refexive processing has been found to begin as early as 50–100 ms, becoming fully engaged by 300 ms, while intentional responses have been found as early as

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 2 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Figure 1. Stimuli and Task Design. (A) Examples of fearful expression by eye gaze pairings. All faces shown to participants in the actual experiment were taken from the NimStim or Ekman databases (not shown here due to publishing permissions) and displayed a fearful expression with either direct or averted gaze. Participants were instructed to make a button response if the stimulus was a non-face object, ensuring attentive viewing of all faces. (B) Sequence depicting one each of both brief (blue cuboids) and long (red cuboids) exposure trial types. Trials are always 2000 ms: 400–600 ms of a red fxation cross signifying trial start, followed by 250 ms or 883 ms of stimulus (corresponding to trial type), concluding with either 1150–1350 ms or 517–717 ms of a green fxation cross, dependent upon trial type. End-trial jitter is inversely timed with pre-trial jitter such that trial lengths are kept a constant 2000 ms.

500–700 ms50–53. Tus, we predicted that the initial response would be stronger to averted-gaze fear within this timeframe, in agreement with the fndings of Milders et al.8, which found that the congruent averted-gaze fear sig- nal attracted more preconscious attention than direct-gaze fear. On the other hand, we predicted that direct-gaze fear would evoke a stronger secondary response indicative of refective analysis to resolve the conficting signals in the incongruent cue. Moreover, we hypothesized that this late-arising refective response would be greater during longer stimulus exposures, as suggested by the fMRI results14,15,19,54. Finally, we also wanted to test whether the efects reported in van der Zwaag et al.54 and Adams et al.15 can still be evoked with rapid switching of conditions in an event-related design (both direct and averted-gaze fear faces, and brief/longer exposure durations), rather than state-dependent, which could be the case for previous work employing block designs. Terefore, we employed a rapid event-related design in MEG and also scanned a separate cohort in fMRI using an identical paradigm. An additional motivation was to compare the periamygda- loid complex activation in MEG to the fMRI results to test whether the PAC activity we found in MEG is at least broadly comparable to the BOLD activity in fMRI. While BOLD activity is unable to capture the fne-grained temporal dynamics present in the MEG signal, the overall activation diferences might be comparable55. Methods and Materials Experiment 1. Participants. Total 28 undergraduate students (22 females), mean age (s.d.) = 18.75 (1.73), participated in Experiment 1. All the participants had normal color vision and normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Teir informed written consent was obtained according to the procedures of the Institutional Review Board at the Pennsylvania State University. Te participants received partial course credit for their participation. Tis research was performed in accordance with the guidelines and regulations set forth in the Declaration of Helsinki, and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Pennsylvania State University.

Stimuli. Te face stimuli in this experiment were identical to those used in the three studies reported in Adams et al.15, with eight models (four female) from the Pictures of Facial Afect56 and eight models (four female) from the NimStim Emotional Face Stimuli database57, all displaying a fearful expression with either a direct gaze or averted gaze (Fig. 1A). Each model had 6 stimuli unique to their identity: one with direct gaze, one with lefward

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 3 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

averted gaze, one with rightward averted gaze, and then the mirror image of each of these iterations. Tis resulted in 96 unique face stimuli used in the experiment. Images of ordinary household objects were also used to ensure observer attention and required a response. Stimuli were presented with Psychtoolbox58,59 in Matlab (Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA). Stimuli were projected onto a screen at the head of the bore and viewed via an angled mirror attached to the head coil subtending approximately 5.72° horizontally and 7.77° vertically of visual angle.

Task Design. We employed an event-related design while keeping presentation parameters as similar to Adams et al.15 as possible within such a design. Each participant viewed 320 trials over the four runs that were randomly, evenly split into brief (15 frames at a refresh rate of 16.67 ms totaling 250 ms) and longer stimulus (53 frames at a refresh rate of 16.67 ms totaling 883 ms) durations to get 160 trials for each presentation duration (128 of which were faces). Each trial lasted 2 seconds, beginning with a randomized 400–600 ms of attending to a central red fxation cross. Te stimulus (either a face or an object) was then presented for either 250 ms or 883 ms, depend- ent upon trial type, followed by a green fxation cross for the remainder of the trial (ranging from 517–1350 ms) before switching back to red to signify the start of a new trial (Fig. 1B). Participants were instructed only to make a manual response via a button box if the stimulus was a non-face object. Tis task would allow us to ensure that participants pay attention to faces without explicitly labeling the emotion displayed, as previous studies have shown that the act of emotion labeling changes neural responsivity to the emotional expression60.

fMRI Data Acquisition and Analysis. fMRI images of brain activity were acquired using a 3 T scanner (Siemens Magnetom Prisma) located at Te Pennsylvania State University Social, Life, and Engineering Sciences Imaging Center. High-resolution anatomical MRI data were acquired using T1-weighted images for the reconstruction of each subject’s cortical surface (TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.28 ms, fip angle = 8°, FoV = 256 × 256 mm2, slice thick- ness = 1 mm, sagittal orientation). Te functional scans were acquired using gradient-echo EPI with a TR of 2000 ms, TE of 28 ms, fip angle of 52° and 64 interleaved slices (3 × 3 × 2 mm resolution). Scanning parameters were optimized by manual shimming of the gradients to ft the brain anatomy of each subject, and tilting the slice prescription anteriorly 20–30° up from the AC-PC line as described in the previous studies61,62 to improve signal and minimize susceptibility artifacts in the brain regions susceptible to signal dropout, such as OFC63. We acquired 456 functional volumes per subject in four functional runs, and the sequence of trials was optimized for hemodynamic response estimation efciency using optseq. 2 sofware (https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/ optseq/). Te acquired fMRI data were analyzed using SPM8 (Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, http:// www.fl.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/sofware/spm8/). Te functional images were corrected for diferences in slice timing, realigned, corrected for movement-related artifacts, coregistered with each participant’s anatomical data, nor- malized to the Montreal Neurological Institute template, and spatially smoothed using an isotropic 8-mm full width half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. ArtRepair sofware was used to correct for excessive movement (http://spnl.stanford.edu/tools/ArtRepair/ArtRepair.htm), and outliers due to movement or signal from preproc- essed fles, using thresholds of 3 s.d. from the mean, 0.75 mm for translation and 0.02 radians rotation, were removed from the data sets64. Data of fve participants among the 28 participants were unusable with ArtRepair identifying >75% of scans as outliers. Terefore, only the remaining 23 participants (19 females and 4 males, mean age (s.d.) = 19.13 (1.36)) were included for further fMRI analyses. Subject-specifc contrasts were estimated using a fxed-efects model. Tese contrast images were used to obtain subject-specifc estimates for each efect. For group analysis, these estimates were then entered into a second-level analysis treating participants as a random efect, using one-sample t-tests at each voxel. We com- puted contrasts between brief and longer exposures within each Treat type (i.e., brief averted-gaze fear vs. longer averted-gaze fear, brief direct-gaze fear vs. longer direct-gaze fear) and between averted-gaze fear and direct-gaze fear within each exposure duration, separately. Te full lists of these whole brain activations are shown in Tables 1 through 3, thresholded at p < 0.001 (t > 3.505) and a minimal cluster size of 10 voxels. Tese parameters are more conservative than those that have been argued to optimally balance between Type 1 and Type 2 errors (65: height p < 0.005, uncorrected, extent: 10 voxels; see also15,46). Although cluster-extent based thresholding has become the most popular approach to dealing with multiple comparisons (e.g.66–68), we chose to report uncorrected p-values using these thresholds for two reasons. First, cluster-extent based thresholding has low spatial specifcity when clusters are large66,68,69, such that p-values of activation at a specifc location or an anatomical area within the cluster are not specifcally determined. Second, we sought to replicate the prior fndings by Adams et al.15, in which uncorrected p-values were reported at threshold of p < 0.005 and k = 10. Tus, reporting uncorrected p-values using these parameters would allow us to ensure spatial specifcity of the statistical signifcance of acti- vation at the areas that have also been reported previously in Adams et al.15. For the regions of interest (ROI) analyses, we extracted the BOLD activity from our a priori ROIs: the amyg- dala, FFA, OFC, and pSTS. We defned another contrast between all the visual stimulation trials (the brief and longer exposures of averted-gaze fear and direct-gaze fear) vs. Null trials. From this contrast, we extracted the percent signal change in our ROIs for all the four conditions using the rfxplot toolbox (http://rfxplot.sourceforge. net) for SPM. We identifed the [x y z] coordinate for each of our ROIs (the MNI coordinates are shown in Fig. 2) then defned a 6 mm sphere around it. Te coordinates for Amygdala, FFA, and OFC were determined based on the previous research that reported the involvement of these regions in processing emotional facial expres- sion (e.g.,14,15). Using the rfxplot toolbox in SPM8, we extracted all the voxels from each individual participant’s functional data within that sphere. Te extracted percent signal change for each of the four trial conditions was subjected to a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with the factors of Exposure duration (2 levels: brief (250 ms) vs. longer (883 ms)) and Treat type (2 levels: averted-gaze fear vs. direct-gaze fear), separately for each of the ROIs. In addition, due to our previous fndings of lef amygdala being specifcally sensitive to direct-gaze fear

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 4 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

MNI coordinates Region Label t-value Extent x y z Averted gaze Brief - Long L Brainstem 4.797 33 −3 −10 −26 R Lingual 3.524 14 30 −67 8 R Amygdala 4.574 11 27 2 −26 L Frontal cortex (Frontal eye felds, BA8) 4.498 31 −24 17 42 L Posterior 4.397 17 −42 −49 14 L Parahippocampal cortex 4.289 16 −24 −7 −30 L 4.232 15 −21 −10 58 L 4.162 73 −30 −55 38 R 4.099 32 42 11 30 Long - Brief L (BA 18) 6.314 221 −18 −100 6 R Visual cortex (BA 17) 6.259 473 15 −97 2 R Visual cortex (BA 18) 3.871 — 33 −94 14 Direct gaze Brief - Long None Long - Brief R Visual cortex (BA 18) 3.566 6496 18 −79 4 9.223 — 21 −94 −8 L 7.34 — −27 −79 −10 R Posterior 8.005 45 −12 −43 26 R Fusiform gyrus 5.315 35 27 2 −36 L 4.951 57 −33 −82 40 R Middle Cingulate Cortex 4.784 26 9 −4 40 L Temporal pole 4.724 43 −39 14 −32 L Fusiform gyrus 4.158 23 −36 −22 −20 R Cerebelum 4.131 41 12 −52 −36 R 4.072 15 57 −7 −20 R 3.908 12 48 −25 40 L Lateral orbitofrontal cortex 3.89 11 −54 29 −10

Table 1. Regions showing increased activation in Experiment 1 in brief- minus longer-exposure, and longer- minus brief-exposure contrasts for averted-gaze fear faces (clear threat) and direct-gaze fear faces (ambiguous threat) (height: p < 0.001 (t(22) > 3.505), extent = 10 voxels). — indicates that this cluster is part of a larger cluster immediately above.

(incongruent threat cue) during the longer exposures15, we performed a planned comparison for lef hemisphere ROIs of longer exposure direct-gaze fear to all other conditions.

Experiment 2. Participants. Sixty participants (40 females), mean age (s.d.) = 26.6 (6.9), with normal or corrected to normal vision completed the study for monetary compensation ($50). Potential subjects were screened via a questionnaire to make sure they were eligible for MEG recording and subsequent MRI structural scans, and had no history of mental illness or use of psychoactive medication. Teir informed written consent was obtained according to the protocol approved by the Institutional Review Board of MGH. Tis research was performed in accordance with the guidelines and regulations set forth in the Declaration of Helsinki, and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the Massachusetts General Hospital.

Stimuli and Task Design. Stimuli and task design were identical to Experiment 1 with the exception of breaks between runs in the MEG being self-paced until the participant was ready to continue. Stimuli were rear-projected onto a translucent screen placed 160 cm from the seated participant to create a 61.5 cm × 38.5 cm display. Stimuli measured 14.1 × 19.2 cm subtending about 5.1° of visual angle horizontally and 6.9° vertically.

MEG Acquisition. Magnetoencephalogram recordings were obtained with a 306-channel Neuromag Vectorview whole-head system (Elekta Neuromag) with 204 planar gradiometers and 102 magnetometers enclosed in a mag- netically shielded room with a shielding factor of 250,000 at 1 Hz (ImedcoAG). Four head position indicator (HPI) electrodes were afxed asymmetrically to each participant’s forehead and the mastoid processes to mon- itor head position in the dewar at the beginning of the recording session. Digitizer data were collected for each

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 5 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Figure 2. Results from Experiment 1 (fMRI study). (A) Shows the percent change in the BOLD signal in bilateral amygdala (lef = lef hemisphere, right = right hemisphere) in response to brief (blue colors) and long (red colors) to clear threat cues (brighter colors) and ambiguous threat cues (darker colors). (B) Shows the percent change in the BOLD signal in bilateral Fusiform Face Area (FFA) (lef = lef hemisphere, right = right hemisphere) in response to brief (blue colors) and long (red colors) to clear threat cues (brighter colors) and ambiguous threat cues (darker colors). Note for both (A) and (B), that a clear threat cue is represented by a fearful face with averted eye-gaze and an ambiguous threat cue is represented by a fearful face with direct eye- gaze.

participant’s head on a Polhemus FastTrack 3D system within a head-coordinate frame defned by anatomical landmarks (lef preauricular area, right preauricular area, and the nasion). HPI positions were marked within this frame, and 150–200 points on the scalp and the face were entered for co-registration with structural MRIs of the subject. Eye movements and blinks were monitored via 4 EOG electrodes: 2 vertical electrodes on the lef eye (one placed just above the eyebrow, the other on the upper cheekbone just below the eye), and 2 horizontal electrodes (placed on either side of the head between the eye and hairline). Cardiac activity was recorded via ECG using electrodes placed on the lef and right chest (2 total). All data from MEG sensors and EOG and ECG electrodes were sampled at 600.615 Hz and were band-pass fltered at 0.1–200 Hz. Recordings were stored for ofine analysis.

Data Pre-processing and Averaging. All recordings were pre-processed and averaged using a combination of the MNE analysis package70 as well as MNE-Python71 and custom scripts in Python and Matlab. Signal-space projection was applied to the recordings in order to remove noise from external sources72,73. Sensors that were visibly noisy during the recording were noted by the researchers and excluded from analysis. For time course analysis, a low-pass flter of 40 Hz was applied, and recordings were epoched from 200 ms before stimulus onset to 1300 ms post-stimulus. For time-frequency analysis, no flter was applied to the data, and recordings were epoched from 500 ms before stimulus onset until 1440 ms post-stimulus onset. Rejection parameters were set at 4,000 fT/cm for gradiometers, 4000 fT for magnetometers, and 800 uV for EOG. Any epoch where any of these limits were exceeded was excluded from further analysis. A further data quality inspection was performed dur- ing preprocessing and any noisy or fat channels that were not picked up during the recording, but resulted in the rejection of 20% or more of epochs were excluded from analysis to prevent unnecessary epoch rejection. No participants were excluded due to excessive trial rejection. Te lowest number of trials entered into the analyses for a condition for any subject was 51 trials. Tere were no signifcant diferences between conditions for trial count (all p’s > 0.86). We excluded trials where object images were presented from MEG analyses as they were of no experimental interest.

Source Localization. A structural MRI for each participant was acquired on a 1.5 T Siemens Avanto 32-channel “TIM” system. A single compartment boundary-element model was ftted to the intracranial volume of the MRI data in the form of a triangular mesh isomorphic to an icosahedron recursively divided 5 times. Tis model was implemented in a surface-based forward solution to restrict the sources of the MEG signal to the vertices of this triangular mesh (source space) ftted to each individual’s infated cortical surface reconstructed using the Freesurfer analysis package74. Sources closer than 5 mm to the inner skull surface were omitted from the forward solution. Te MRI-head coordinate transformation for each subject was supplied to the forward model by align- ing the digitizer data obtained in the original recording session (see MEG Acquisition) with a high-resolution head surface tessellation constructed from the MRI data. Te inverse operator was prepared with a loose orienta- tion constraint (LOC) parameter of 0.2 in order to improve localization accuracy75. A depth-weighting coefcient of 0.8 was also set for the inverse operator to lessen the tendency of minimum-norm estimates to be localized to superfcial currents in place of deep sources. Only gradiometers were used in the depth weighting process. Both gradiometers and magnetometers were used to source localize the data. MEG data were source localized onto the whole brain using a lambda2 regularization parameter based on Signal-to-Noise Ratio (SNR) equal to 1/(SNR2). Evoked cortical activation was quantifed spatiotemporally by taking only the radial component from a 3-orientation source [x y z] at each vertex in the form of dynamic statistical parametric maps (dSPMs)

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 6 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

based on an inverse solution regularized with an SNR of 3. Tese are a statistical representation of signifcant activity from each source per time point calculated by noise-normalization on the estimated current amplitude (MNE) of a given source according to noise covariance between sensors calculated during a baseline period of 200 ms pre-stimulus76. Te noise covariance estimation model was selected automatically according to rank for each participant77. To analyze the spectral content of the neural response, MEG data were source localized on a trial-by-trial basis using the minimum-norm estimate (MNE) method with the same inverse operator regularized with an SNR of 1 due to the indiscernibility of signal and noise at the single trial level.

ROI Selection and Defnition. Regions of interest (ROIs) were chosen based on their established roles in early visual processing, face perception, threat detection, and emotional processing: early visual cortex (V1), fusiform face area (FFA), posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), periamygdaloid cortex (PAC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). For the MEG data analyses, the ROIs (‘labels’ in the terminology of the mne_analyze sofware) were func- tionally derived in each individual’s anatomical space within a priori anatomical constraints (automatic cortical parcellations) produced with the Freesurfer analysis package74, except for the PAC and pSTS as explained below. Te functional label within the anatomical parcellation was derived from averaged activity from all conditions, so that the activity was independent of trial type. Tis enabled us to account for intersubject variability in regions like FFA, pSTS, and OFC. Functional labels were generated within the anatomical parcellation corresponding to the ROI by isolating the source-space vertex with the highest activation within the anatomical constraints as well as neighboring vertices in the source-space (also within the anatomical constraints) that reach at least 60% of the maximum activation (in dSPM values). Since no suitable a priori parcellation of the amygdala and surrounding periamygdaloid cortex was available, a posteriori anatomical constraints were imposed in the form of user-drawn ROIs in the Freesurfer sofware on the fsaverage infated surface corresponding to the cortex surrounding and including the amygdala, which we will hence refer to simply as periamygdaloid cortex (PAC). Te drawing of the PAC labels was tracked by linking the drawn points to be displayed on the fsaverage MRI volume in tkmedit to ensure that only the cortical surface corresponding to the amygdalae was included in the label. Tese anatomical constraints were then morphed to each individual’s infated surface and used to generate functional PAC ROIs according to the preceding procedure. A similar method was used to obtain the posterior portion of STS as the a priori parcellation generated by Freesurfer extended beyond the true pSTS on many subjects’ cortical surfaces to inferior sulci. User-marked constraints on the fsaverage infated surface were marked around STS and tracked in the fsaverage MRI volume. Te same morphing procedure from above was used, and then the label was split into thirds. Te most posterior third was then taken as each individual’s pSTS to be used as the anatomical constraint when generating the functional pSTS labels.

Time-Course Analysis. Time courses were produced for each ROI by averaging the activity from source-space vertices that fell within the label marked on the individual’s infated cortical surface to be submitted to statistical analysis. Te individual average activity was then further averaged across subjects in order to visualize the grand average.

Phase-Locking Analysis. Using modifed scripts from the MNE-Python package71, the Phase-Locking Factor (PLF) across trials was calculated for each ROI, and Phase-Locking Value (PLV) was calculated to assess func- tional connectivity between two ROIs. Te PLF is a number between 0 and 1 (1 representing perfect synchrony) that represents a magnitude-normalized measure of the phase angle consistency across trials for a particular time-point at a particular frequency78. Tis number was obtained by source localizing each epoch into source space using the Minimum-Norm Estimate (MNE) method with the sign of the signal preserved. Source-space MNE epochs were subjected to spectral decomposition at each time point for each frequency of interest, using a continuous wavelet transformation with a family of complex morlet wavelets containing a number of cycles equal to f/7, where f denotes the frequency of interest. Tis keeps the time window at each frequency identical resulting in stable temporal resolution across frequency ranges. We analyzed frequencies from 8 Hz (representing the lower limit of the α-band) to 30 Hz (representing the upper limit of the β-band). To make these results easier to interpret, and in attempt to localize efects away from spectral leakage inherent in such transformations, PLFs were analyzed in separate frequency ranges: α (8–13 Hz), β (13–30 Hz). Similarly, inter-regional connectivity was assessed with PLVs, also a magnitude-normalized measure of phase-angle consistency across trials between 2 ROIs. Tis was calculated with the same parameters on the same frequencies as above (8–30 Hz) and analyzed by frequency band.

Statistical Analyses. As this work is a direct extension of Adams et al.15, we approached this work with par- ticular efects whose temporal properties we sought to investigate. Specifcally, we knew that averted-gaze fear elicits stronger BOLD activity relative to direct-gaze fear during brief presentations and the opposite is true of longer presentations. Consequently, we performed non-parametric comparisons based on t-tests compar- ing direct vs. averted-gaze fear faces within each presentation duration to describe the temporal evolution of these opposing sensitivities. Additionally, to verify our presentation duration manipulations were functioning as intended (i.e., to perform a reality check), we compared brief vs. longer presentations within each gaze direc- tion, also using non-parametric comparisons based on t-tests. All statistics were computed using non-parametric cluster-level permutation tests based on 5000 permutations with a critical alpha-value of 0.05, following Maris and Oostenveld79. Cluster mass was determined by summing t-values within the cluster rather than counting signifcant pixels/time-points. Reported p-values are Monte Carlo p-values comparing the observed cluster to a null distribution comprising the largest cluster yielded by permuted data sets. Tat is, the reported p-value is the percentage of permuted data sets that yielded a larger cluster than the actual observed cluster (e.g., p = 0.05 means 250 out 5000 permuted data sets yielded a larger cluster than the observed cluster).

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 7 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Time Domain. Statistical analyses in the time domain were performed by subtracting each participant’s evoked response for 2 conditions of interest to create a contrast wave. Null distributions were built by means of a sign-fip permutation based on a one-sample t-test. Observed clusters of signifcant time-points whose masses were exceeded by 5 percent of or fewer clusters from the null distribution were considered signifcant. Time-Frequency Domain. Phase-locking maps for each participant (2-dimensional images where the y-axis represents frequency and the x-axis represents time with pixel values corresponding to phase-locking factors or values) were smoothed via a Gaussian image flter with a kernel size of 5 and a sigma of 2 before being submitted to permutations and statistical analysis. Permutations were performed by shufing condition labels for each par- ticipant, such that the condition label of each participant’s phase-locking map was randomized but no one subject ended up with both phase-locking maps (the unit of observation in this case) falling under the same condition. Both the observed and permuted statistical maps were thresholded at an alpha-level of 0.05 with 59 degrees of freedom in order to identify clusters. Observed clusters of contiguous supra-threshold time-frequency points whose masses were exceeded by 5 percent or less of clusters from the null distribution were considered signifcant. Data Availability. All data and code used to perform analyses reported herein are available from the corre- sponding author at reasonable request. Results fMRI Results. Our main interest was in the amygdala responses to averted-gaze vs. direct-gaze fear (con- gruent vs. incongruent threat cues) and their interactions with brief vs. longer stimulus exposures. Figure 2A shows the percentage of BOLD signal change from the baseline for each of the four trial conditions (the brief and longer exposures of averted-gaze fear and direct-gaze fear) in the lef and right amygdala. A two-way repeated measures ANOVA with the factors of the Exposure duration (2 levels: brief [250 ms] vs. longer [883 ms]) and the Treat type (2 levels: averted-gaze fear vs. direct-gaze fear) showed a signifcant main efect of exposure duration in the lef amygdala (F(1,22) = 7.19, p < 0.015), such that the lef amygdala (Fig. 2A, lef panel) showed greater activation for longer exposure than for brief exposure of threat cues (Fig. 2A, lef panel). However, neither the main efect of Treat type (F(1,22) = 1.564, p = 0.224) nor the interaction between the Exposure duration and the Treat type (F(1,22) = 0.496, p = 0.489) were statistically signifcant. Based on our a priori hypothesis that the lef amygdala would be more involved in the sustained processing of the incongruent threat cue (the longer exposure of direct-gaze fear), we conducted a planned comparison to compare the longer exposure of direct-gaze fear with any other conditions and observed a marginally signifcant efect (t(22) = 1.70, p = 0.093). In the right amygdala (Fig. 2A, right panel), both main efects of the Exposure duration (F(1,22) = 0.295, p = 0.593) and of the Treat type (F(1,22) = 0.208, p = 0.652) were not signifcant. However, the predicted interac- tion between Exposure duration and Treat type was signifcant (F(1,22) = 7.19, p < 0.015). Specifcally, the right amygdala responded more strongly to the averted-gaze fear when the exposure was brief and to direct-gaze fear when the exposure was longer. Tese results are consistent with previous fndings that indicate possible amygdala lateralization in orientation and evaluation of facial threat cues80. In the lef FFA (Fig. 2B, lef panel), we found signifcantly greater activation for longer exposure over brief exposure, confrmed by a signifcant main efect of the Exposure duration (F(1,22) = 5.491, p < 0.03). Although a main efect of Treat type (F(1,22) = 0.284, p = 0.600) and the interaction between Exposure duration and Treat type (F(1,22) = 1.766, p = 0.198) were not statistically signifcant, the same planned comparison as in the lef amygdala (i.e., the longer exposure of direct-gaze fear vs. other conditions) showed a signifcant efect (t(22) = 2.076, p < 0.05). In the right FFA (Fig. 2B, right panel), the main efect of Exposure duration was also signifcant (F(1,22) = 6.620, p < 0.02). Furthermore, we observed a similar activation pattern in the right FFA as in the right amygdala: Te brief exposure of averted-gaze fear and the longer exposure of direct-gaze fear elicited stronger right FFA responses than their counterparts (brief exposure of direct-gaze fear and longer exposure of averted-gaze fear, respectively, see Fig. 2B, right panel). However, the interaction between Exposure duration and Treat type (F(1,22) = 1.221, p = 0.281) was not signifcant nor was the main efect of Treat type (F(1,22) = 0.029, p = 0.867). Tus, the lef and right FFA responses somewhat resembled those in the lef and right amygdala, but were weaker: Greater activation was observed for the longer exposure of direct-gaze fear in the lef FFA and greater activation for the brief exposure of averted-gaze fear and the longer exposure of direct-gaze fear in the right FFA. It is also worth noting that the right FFA (Fig. 2B, right panel) showed greater levels of activation than the lef FFA (Fig. 2B, lef panel) in all the conditions, consistent with previous fndings on the right hemisphere dominance in function of FFA81–84. To confrm this statistically, we conducted a separate three-way repeated measures ANOVA with additional factor of Hemisphere (lef FFA versus right FFA) along with the Exposure duration and Treat type, and found a signifcant main efect of Hemisphere (F(1,22) = 7.465, p < 0.015). Consistent with the ROI results, the statistical analyses on the whole brain using a univariate GLM approach showed the greater right amygdala activation for the brief exposure of averted-gaze fear, compared to the longer exposure, and the greater bilateral amygdala for the longer exposure of direct-gaze fear, compared to the brief exposure (Table 1). As shown in Table 1, we also observed that the other ROIs including the OFC, Fusiform area, V1, and the pSTS were diferentially activated as a function of the Exposure duration by Treat type interaction, with greater responses for the brief exposure of averted-gaze fear and the longer exposure of direct-gaze fear. Supporting this fnding at the whole brain level, there is unanimously stronger activity to averted-gaze fear rela- tive to direct-gaze fear during brief exposures and to direct-gaze fear compared to averted-gaze fear during longer exposures (see Table 2).

MEG Results. Efects of Exposure Duration: Time Courses. Te time courses of activation within our ROIs revealed an efect of stimulus exposure duration for both averted-gaze fear and direct-gaze fear. Te time courses can be seen in Fig. 3, and a comprehensive list of the timing and signifcance of these efects can be found in

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 8 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Table 4. In V1 and FFA, we found signifcantly increased activation for brief compared to longer exposures only in the right hemisphere and only for averted-gaze fear for V1 (p = 0.02) and direct-gaze fear for FFA (p = 0.04). Conversely, we observed signifcantly increased activation from longer presentations over brief presentations bilaterally for both gaze directions in V1 and FFA. Bilateral V1 and right FFA were the only regions in which we observed signifcantly greater activity from the longer-exposure condition, while the stimulus was still on the screen, compared to the brief-exposure condition afer stimulus ofset (Fig. 3). In pSTS we observed a sim- ilar temporal progression, but found signifcant diferences in evoked activity between exposure durations only during the viewing of direct-gaze fear faces (see Table 3 for timing and signifcance). PAC and OFC bilaterally demonstrated a distinctly diferent temporal progression of activity compared to the other regions (Fig. 3). Tis was characterized in the brief-exposure condition by a rise in activity starting around 500 ms, hundreds of mil- liseconds afer the stimulus ofset at 250 ms. Tis increase superseded activity in the longer-exposure condition at around 600 ms, even though the stimulus was still being displayed (see Fig. 3 and Table 4 for the timing and statistics of signifcant diferences). Tis activity in response to the brief-exposure stimulus remained stronger until around 1000 ms (750 ms afer stimulus ofset) when a similar pattern began to emerge from the longer exposure, evoked by the removal of the threat cue. Tus, in pSTS, PAC, and OFC, the activity afer the stimulus ofset (post-250 ms) in the brief-exposure condition was higher, and showed a diferent pattern, than activity in the longer-exposure condition in which the stimulus was still present (250–883 ms). In summary, efects of exposure duration dominated the activity in all ROIs we examined. Te nature of these efects seemed to depend on the role of the region in the processing hierarchy, as PAC and OFC, showed strongest responses afer stimulus ofset. To examine these efects in greater detail, we performed phase-locking analyses in all the ROIs for frequencies in the α and β bands.

Efects of Exposure Duration: Phase Locking. Examination of phase synchrony across trials in our ROIs also yielded powerful efects of stimulus exposure duration for both averted-gaze and direct-gaze fear (Fig. 4). See Tables 5 and 6 for the exact timing and signifcance of these efects in the α and β frequency ranges, respec- tively. Similar to the activity we observed in the time courses, there were efects of exposure duration in every ROI examined. Phase-locking exposure efects were quite robust as the vast majority of them corresponded to a non-parametric p value of 0 (i.e., not a single randomly shufed data set in the permutation process out of 5000 yielded a cluster larger than that which was observed in the actual data). V1, FFA, and pSTS bilaterally all had signifcantly greater phase locking to both brief and longer exposures within the trial in both α and β frequency bands (Fig. 4). Te results mirrored those in the time course analyses, in that the phase locking in response to the brief-exposure condition was greater when the stimulus was already of the screen, compared to the longer-exposure condition. Te early visual and face-processing regions V1 and FFA were the only regions to show signifcantly greater phase locking to the longer exposure before the longer-exposure stimulus was removed from the screen (Fig. 4). For PAC, we observed signifcantly greater phase locking to both stimulus exposures for both threat types in the β band. However, activity in the α band was sensitive primarily to longer exposures, with the exception of direct gaze in the lef hemisphere, which evoked responses to both the brief and longer-exposure conditions. OFC was engaged primarily by the longer-exposures trials in both bands. Te only exception was in lef OFC, which displayed stronger β phase locking to the brief exposure during direct-gaze threat cue viewing (p = 0.05).

Efects of Gaze: Time Courses. Permutation cluster tests of the time courses within our ROIs revealed only one efect of gaze. We observed signifcantly increased activation from direct-gaze threat cues during longer expo- sures in V1 of the right hemisphere late in the trial around 500–650 ms (p = 0.02). Given its timing, this efect may be the result of feedback from higher regions.

Effects of Gaze: Phase Locking. Brief Stimulus Exposures (250 ms): Significant phase-locking differences for averted-gaze fear faces. Te frst efect we observed was an increased bilateral response to averted gaze over direct-gaze fear faces. Averted-gaze fear evoked stronger phase locking in the α-band early between lef PAC and OFC (p = 0.013) around 80–160 ms and in right V1 (p = 0.041) around 140–220 ms (Fig. 5A). We also found signifcantly stronger phase locking for averted-gaze compared to direct-gaze fear in the β-band of right PAC at around 120 ms (p = 0.023). Tese fndings support our hypothesis of initial refexive processing being more sensitive to averted-gaze fear, since these congruent cues are thought to be processed more efciently15. In addition, we found stronger phase locking for averted-gaze fear compared to direct-gaze fear very late at the end of the brief-exposure trial. Tis occurred in the β-band between right FFA and PAC around 1200–1300 ms (p = 0.012) as well as in the α-band of right PAC from approximately 1320–1380 ms (p = 0.035) (Fig. 5A). Right PAC’s stronger response to averted gaze during brief expo- sures is in line with our fMRI fndings in the right amygdala, which showed greater activation to averted-gaze fear faces compared to direct-gaze fear faces during brief exposures (Fig. 5A). Overall, we found two distinct time windows during the brief-exposure trials in which phase locking, both across trials within a region and in the connectivity between regions, was signifcantly stronger when exposed to averted-gaze fear faces compared to direct-gaze fear faces: A bilateral response early in the trial around 80–220 ms and then a late response concentrated in the right hemisphere around PAC past 1200 ms (Fig. 5A). Signifcant phase-locking diferences for direct-gaze fear faces. For direct-gaze fear faces, there was signifcantly increased phase locking compared to averted-gaze fear faces during the mid-trial period. We found signifcantly increased phase locking compared to averted-gaze fear faces in the α-band between lef FFA and PAC (p = 0.04), peaking at around 350 ms. Tis occurred following the stimulus ofset at 250 ms, possibly suggesting that removal of the direct-gaze fear face (incongruent threat cue) elicits sensitivity to this type of threat cue (Fig. 5A). Phase locking continued to be stronger to direct-gaze fear in the β-band between lef V1 and PAC around 700 ms

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 9 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

MNI coordinates Region Label t-value Extent x y z Brief Exposure Averted gaze - Direct gaze R Cerebelum 6.019 102 33 −43 −24 7.302 533 12 −40 −28 L Cerebelum 6.775 — −27 −46 −26 4.188 533 −6 −61 −16 R Inferior frontal gyrus 3.871 11 45 11 30 L Inferior frontal gyrus 6.245 113 −39 2 34 L 4.492 121 −33 −1 −30 R 5.238 327 27 −49 10 4.291 — 24 −76 10 R Visual cortex 4.126 — 15 −91 −6 L Visual cortex 4.095 82 −21 −76 4 L Parahippocampal cortex 4.464 13 −15 −31 −2 L Medial temporal pole 4.327 15 −36 14 −32 R Postcentral gyrus 4.325 40 24 −46 58 R Middle cingulate cortex 4.275 18 3 −10 36 L 4.2 82 −27 −55 2 L 3.531 12 −63 −1 30 R Amygdala 3.53 19 21 −1 −24 Direct gaze - Averted gaze None Long Exposure Averted gaze - Direct gaze None Direct gaze - Averted gaze L Cerebelum 5.57 241 −24 −91 −24 5.56 137 15 −52 −12 R Visual cortex 4.918 180 30 −88 −20 L Visual cortex 5.539 195 −15 −49 −12 L Lingual gyrus 4.363 — −12 −64 6 R Fusiform gyrus 4.049 12 39 −16 −32 L Fusiform gyrus 5.015 82 −33 −13 −40 R Temporal pole 4.063 12 33 −1 −42 L Temporal pole 4.949 82 −30 8 −46 4.756 33 30 −34 −12 R Parahippocampal cortex 4.109 11 21 −1 −34 L 4.733 77 −18 −76 30 L 4.702 27 −18 62 20 L Posterior cingulate cortex 4.626 29 −12 −40 36 L 4.352 79 −39 −64 38 L Superior temporal sulcus 4.329 37 −48 −28 −4 R Intraparietal lobule 4.244 131 42 −52 38 L Posterior superior temporal sulcus 4.193 37 −42 58 18 R Inferior frontal gyrus 4.066 29 51 35 30 L Inferior frontal gyrus 4.103 46 −57 11 36 R Inferior temporal gyrus 4.088 63 66 −31 −20 R Prefrontal cortex 3.998 17 9 65 26 L 3.925 12 −60 −13 −18 R Superior frontal gyrus 3.905 20 27 62 24 L Amygdala 3.857 13 −18 2 −22

Table 2. Regions showing increased activation in Experiment 1 in averted-gaze (clear threat) minus direct- gaze (ambiguous threat) and direct-gaze minus averted-gaze contrasts during brief and long exposures (height: p < 0.001 (t(22) > 3.505), extent = 10 voxels). — indicates that this cluster is part of a larger cluster immediately above.

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 10 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

MNI coordinates Region Label t-value Extent x y z Interaction (Brief averted gaze & Long direct gaze) – (Long averted gaze & Short direct gaze) L Cerebelum 6.944 4804 −15 −52 −16 R Cerebelum 6.526 4804 24 −85 −14 R Cerebelum 6.523 — 15 −49 −14 L Amygdala 6.111 182 −21 −1 −26 L Medial Temporal Pole 4.362 182 −39 14 −32 R Amygdala 5.407 107 27 2 −26 L Inferior frontal gyrus 5.353 185 −48 5 32 L Inferior temporal gyrus 5.284 114 −66 −16 −28 L Pallidum 5.255 68 −9 −1 2 R Middle cingulate cortex 5.126 24 6 −4 38 R Inferior fronta gyrus 5.038 205 57 14 36 R Precentral gyrus 3.912 205 45 −4 36 L Superior temporal sulcus 5.010 19 −39 −28 −4 R Cerebelum 4.830 56 42 −55 −42 L Superior temporal sulcus 4.769 33 −51 −10 −18 R Inferior temporal gyrus 4.734 56 63 −13 −34 L Cerebelum 4.638 29 −27 −52 −44 L Precentral gyrus 4.581 11 −36 −13 44 R 4.580 49 51 32 26 R Angular gyrus 4.536 32 39 −49 36 R Middle temporal gyrus 4.402 49 69 −22 −20 L Posterior cingulate cortex 4.378 28 −12 −40 34 L Putamen 4.318 20 −27 −13 2 L Inferior temporal gyrus 4.290 15 −57 −1 −36 L Orbitofrontal cortex 4.071 13 −51 41 −12 L Posterior superior temporal sulcus 4.044 65 −45 −37 6 L Hippocampus 3.991 11 −27 −19 −18 R 3.944 29 36 −16 42 R Fusiform gyrus 3.883 14 30 −31 −16

Table 3. Regions showing increased activation in Experiment 1 in the interaction between stimulus exposure and gaze direction of fearful faces, measured by the contrast (Brief averted gaze & Long direct gaze) – (Long averted gaze & Short direct gaze) (height: p < 0.001 (t(22) > 3.505), extent = 10 voxels). — indicates that this cluster is part of a larger cluster immediately above.

(p = 0.001), in the right FFA around 900 ms (p = 0.05), and in lef PAC around 1000 ms (p = 0.03). Tus, with brief exposures, there is a relative increase of phase locking to direct-gaze fear compared to averted-gaze fear faces beginning immediately afer removal of the stimulus up until around 1050 ms. In summary, during brief exposures we found stronger early and late phase locking to averted-gaze fear faces, and stronger phase locking to direct-gaze fear faces mid-trial around the 300–1050 ms range, with right PAC responding more to averted-gaze fear and lef PAC responding more to direct-gaze fear. Tese MEG fndings are broadly consistent with the fMRI fndings in the amygdalae using this paradigm (9,15,54, Experiment 1 in this study). Longer Stimulus Exposures (883 ms): Signifcant phase-locking diferences for averted-gaze fear faces. With expo- sures of 883 ms, we likewise observed greater β-band phase locking in response to averted-gaze fear faces early in the trial: in lef V1 around 60–80 ms (p = 0.017), between lef PAC and pSTS around 80–100 ms (p = 0.032), between right FFA and OFC around 140–200 ms (p = 0.033), and between lef PAC and OFC (p = 0.003) at approximately 200–300 ms (Fig. 5B). Tis again demonstrates a refexive response more tuned to averted-gaze fear immediately following exposure to the threat cue, suggesting it to be the more salient threat cue to the face processing network. Averted-gaze fear faces also evoked stronger α-band phase locking in lef V1 (p = 0.042) at around 400 ms (Fig. 5B) compared to direct-gaze fear. In contrast to brief exposures, stronger phase locking for averted-gaze fear face stimuli persisted until 400 ms suggesting that greater averted-gaze fear phase locking early in the trial is not inherently limited to the pre-300 ms early time frame but can be more sustained when the stimulus exposure is longer. Tat is, the exposure duration modulates the initial response to averted-gaze fear faces, with longer exposures resulting in longer processing times for averted-gaze fear, when compared to the processing of direct-gaze fear faces under the same exposure conditions. Signifcant phase-locking diferences for direct-gaze fear faces. Towards the latter half of the trial, phase lock- ing became stronger to fearful faces with direct gaze relative to averted gaze in the α-band between pSTS and

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 11 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Figure 3. Time courses of activation within Regions of Interest (ROIs). Time courses depicting bilateral (Lef = Lef Hemisphere, Right = Right Hemisphere) activation (measured by dSPM values) for both exposure durations (blue = short, red = long) and both threat cue types (brighter shades = averted gaze [“clear” threat], darker shades = direct gaze [“ambiguous” threat]) in our Regions of Interest (ROIs). Standard Error from the Mean (SEM) is represented by dashed lines of the same color above and below the respective time series. Te dashed black line indicates stimulus ofset for brief exposures (250 ms) while the solid black line indicates stimulus ofset for long exposures (883 ms).

FFA around 700–950 ms (p = 0.005) and between FFA and PAC between 1000–1100 ms (p = 0.034), as shown in Fig. 5B. During this same time frame, phase locking in the β-band to direct-gaze fear faces signifcantly increased over averted-gaze fear faces in right OFC around 700–800 ms (p = 0.034) and between right pSTS and FFA at

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 12 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

LH RH ROI Time (ms) p-value Time (ms) p-value Averted Gaze Brief minus Long 150–220 0.01 V1 n.s. n.s. 320–365 0.02 FFA n.s. n.s. n.s n.s pSTS n.s n.s n.s n.s PAC n.s. n.s. 620–820 0.001 OFC 600–800 0.02 600–820 0.01 Long minus Brief V1 500–1300 0.0004 420–1300 0.0002 FFA 900–1300 0.0004 800–1300 0.0002 pSTS n.s n.s n.s n.s PAC 1020–1300 0.002 1100–1300 0.0006 OFC 1075–1300 0.004 1100–1300 0.002 Direct Gaze Brief minus Long V1 n.s. ns. n.s. n.s FFA n.s. n.s. 300–380 0.04 PSTS 380–480 0.05 n.s. n.s. PAC n.s. n.s. 620–780 0.03 OFC n.s. n.s. 620–800 0.02 Long minus Brief V1 500–1300 0.0002 400–1300 0.0002 FFA 900–1080 0.02 800–1300 0.0002 pSTS 975–1300 0.003 950–1100 0.04 PAC 1100–1300 0.03 1100–1300 0.001 OFC 1050–1300 0.01 1050–1300 0.0008

Table 4. Timing of signifcant diferences in activation within ROIs between brief (250 ms) and long (883 ms) exposures (reported p-values are non-parametric and corrected based on cluster permutations thresholded at p < 0.05 uncorrected).

roughly 900–1000 ms (p = 0.01). Tis provides evidence for our hypothesis that longer presentations result in more extensive processing of direct-gaze fear faces, as all of the signifcant phase-locking activity diferences demonstrated higher phase locking for direct-gaze fear faces late in the trial for connections in the right hemi- sphere between PAC, FFA, OFC and pSTS. To conclude, during longer exposures we see similar stronger early phase locking for averted-gaze fear faces relative to direct gaze as we observed in in the brief-exposure trials. However, longer stimulus exposures also prolong this initial processing of averted-gaze fear faces, and delay and prolong processing of direct-gaze fear faces. Returning to greater phase locking for averted-gaze fear faces very late in the trial occurred only during the brief-exposure trials. Discussion Te primary aim of the present study was to characterize the neurodynamics mediating facial threat cue percep- tion with MEG, building on previous fMRI fndings that employed the same stimulus set of congruent (fearful faces with averted eye gaze) or incongruent (fear with direct gaze) cues15. We frst validated our experimental design as a direct comparison to previous eforts by replicating the fndings of block-design experiments9,14,15 using an event-related paradigm in fMRI. In Experiment 1 (fMRI), we replicated the fndings of Adams et al.15, fnding that amygdala responses vary as a function of stimulus exposure duration in response to averted-gaze and direct-gaze fear faces. BOLD activity in the right amygdala was enhanced to averted-gaze fear during brief exposures and direct-gaze fear during longer exposures, whereas the left amygdala activity was greater for longer-exposure durations, particularly for direct-gaze fear. Tese fMRI fndings suggest both that brief stimulus exposures elicit a stronger refexive response to averted-gaze fear, and that longer exposures evoke more refective processing of direct-gaze fear, possibly in order to resolve the incongruity in the latter cue combination. Tese fndings align with previous demonstrations of the amygdala being sensitive to brief exposures of averted-gaze fear faces9,85 as well as longer exposures of the incongruent threat cues conveyed by direct-gaze fear14,86. However, because of the temporal limitations of fMRI and the BOLD signal, they ofer only a partial description of how facial fear cues are processed in the brain, as we detail below. In Experiment 2 (MEG), we aimed to characterize the efects of exposure duration and compound facial threat cues on the neurodynamics of threat perception on a fner temporal scale by examining the time courses and phase locking of activity in the extended face network, including FFA, PAC (the periamygdaloid cortex),

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 13 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Figure 4. Unthresholded statistical maps of brief -versus longer-exposure phase locking across trials by threat cue type. Te lef side shows maps for each ROI from the lef hemisphere according to the threat type conveyed by the face at the top of the column (averted-gaze = clear threat, direct-gaze = ambiguous threat). Te right side shows the same for the right hemisphere. For each map, the y-axis represents frequency (in Hz) while the x-axis represents time (in s) while the pixel value is the t-statistic representing each participant’s PLF (Phase- locking Factor) across trials for brief exposures (represented in blue) compared to long exposures (represented in red). Contour levels map to signifcance based on a two-tailed distribution with 59 degrees of freedom. Green represents no signifcance (i.e. p-values above 0.05). Te three blue shades (cyan, blue, dark/navy blue) represent p-value ranges between 0.05–0.01, 0.01–0.001, and 0.001 and below for brief exposures. Te three red shades (yellow, red, dark red) represent the same p-values for long exposures. All p-values parametric and uncorrected.

pSTS, and OFC, as well as the primary visual cortex. We observed robust efects of presentation duration in all the ROIs we examined. Brief stimulus exposures for both direct and averted-gaze fear faces resulted in stronger activity immediately following stimulus ofset, compared with activity when the stimulus was still present during the longer-exposure trials. In PAC and OFC, this activity persisted for hundreds of milliseconds afer the stimulus had been removed in a slow second cycle of processing. Conversely, the longer stimulus exposures elicited signif- icantly greater activation from all our ROIs during the mid-to-late trial period. Tis would suggest that there is indeed an inherent diference in how these areas respond to facial threat cues, driven by the exposure duration. We expected brief stimulus presentations to elicit more refexive threat processing (see16–18), and thus a greater response to averted-gaze fear, which previously had been found to be processed more quickly and efciently than direct-gaze fear (e.g.,8,15). Examination of the phase locking in and between our ROIs indeed revealed a greater initial response to averted-gaze fear in both the α and β frequency ranges. Because of the primary focus on the response in the amygdala complex in previous fMRI studies9,14,15,54, it should be emphasized that we observed stronger phase locking to averted-gaze fear in the initial response of the right PAC, as well as in the initial con- nectivity between lef PAC and OFC, suggesting more preconscious attention to congruent threat cues in these regions. Tis adds to the growing body of neuroimaging evidence that fear is processed quickly by the amygdaloid complex with the help of surrounding connected regions87–90. It is worth noting that these early diferentiations between congruent and incongruent threat cues either temporally paralleled or preceded similar responses in the

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 14 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

LH RH ROI Time (ms) p-value Time (ms) p-value Averted Gaze Brief minus Long V1 320–560 0.007 310–520 0.01 FFA 320–600 0.004 300–660 0.002 390–580 0.002 pSTS 400–610 0.0006 820–940 0.05 PAC n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. OFC n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. Long minus Brief V1 570–1440 0 730–1440 0 FFA 900–1400 0 820–1440 0 990–1340 0 pSTS 1010–1200 0.009 1370–1440 0.02 PAC 980–1420 0 975–1400 0 OFC 980–1400 0.0008 975–1400 0.0002 Direct Gaze Brief minus Long V1 310–570 0.006 320–400 0.01 FFA 350–560 0.03 300–500 0.001 PSTS 380–630 0 380–620 0 PAC n.s. n.s. 620–870 0.01 OFC n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. Long minus Brief V1 590–1440 0 480–1440 0 FFA 940–1300 0 820–1440 0 pSTS 1000–1350 0 1000–1360 0 PAC 970–1400 0 980–1440 0 OFC 980–1330 0 1080–1440 0.0002

Table 5. Timing of signifcant diferences in α phase locking across trials within ROIs between brief (250 ms) and long (883 ms) exposures (reported p-values are non-parametric and corrected based on cluster permutations thresholded at p < 0.05 uncorrected).

early visual cortex (V1) leaving open the possibility of an alternate route to the amygdala via subcortical projec- tions, circumventing V1 and typical initial visual processing90,91. However, increased phase locking between PAC and OFC during this early response indicates it must involve more than subcortical connections. Previous work has shown that rapid magnocellular projections to the OFC enable top-down facilitation of bottom-up processes such as object recognition62. Due to the extensive connections between the amygdala and OFC it is conceivable that similar rapid projections to the OFC are active during facial threat perception, which could then facilitate bottom-up amygdala processing of clear threat messages (congruent threat cues). Additionally, this fnding is consistent with the behavioral fndings of Milders et al.8 and Adams’ shared signal hypothesis4,14,15, suggesting averted-gaze fear is the more salient threat cue. Immediate sensitivity to the more salient threat cue in right PAC is consistent with the right amygdala’s putative function in rapid detection of emotionally salient stimuli92. Once the stimulus was removed at 250 ms in the brief presentation condition, we found that phase lock- ing then increased in response to direct-gaze fear faces from about 300–1000 ms, and then was superseded by averted-gaze fear-related activity very late in the trial. Generally, lef PAC (along with the lef hemisphere as a whole) showed more sensitivity to direct-gaze fear faces during the brief exposures. Tis is also congruent with the lef amygdala’s putative involvement in refective assessment of stimuli following an initial, refexive emotional response from the right side92. Te stronger phase locking to direct-gaze fear during this period reveals a more nuanced picture of PAC activity than that suggested by fMRI results in the amygdala: namely, that the amygdaloid complex appears to be sensitive to threat cues portrayed by both averted and direct-gaze fear at diferent times. Terefore, the greater BOLD signal evoked by averted-gaze fear during brief exposures in fMRI, as in Experiment 1 and in previous fMRI studies9,15 is likely due to the nature of fMRI; that is, summation of (delayed and tempo- rally dilated) BOLD signal, which is unable to resolve fne-grained sensitivity of the amygdala complex to difer- ent threat cues at diferent stages of processing. Tese results might be partially explained by our fnding in MEG that, at the end of the trial (1100–1400 ms), phase locking to congruent threat cues again became stronger than for incongruent threat cues. Signifcant diferences this late in the trial are far too late to be considered “refexive”, indicating that there is more at play in the neurodynamics mediating threat processing than just an automatic, refexive response to the brief exposure, followed by a refective response if stimulus presentation is maintained. Based on the fMRI fndings using this paradigm9,14,15,54, we had predicted that longer stimulus exposures would evoke a stronger late response to direct-gaze fear, possibly indicative of refective analysis to resolve ambiguity in these incongruent threat cues. However, because the brief and longer presentation conditions are

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 15 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

LH RH ROI Time (ms) p-value Time (ms) p-value Averted Gaze Brief minus Long V1 300–690 0 290–650 0 FFA 320–680 0 320–690 0 pSTS 380–630 0 390–580 0.0002 PAC 490–700 0.005 350–570 0.002 OFC n.s. n.s. n.s. n.s. Long minus Brief V1 750–1400 0 830–1440 0 FFA 950–1350 0 920–1440 0 pSTS 980–1330 0 980–1200 0 PAC 970–1390 0 980–1200 0 980–1090 0.007 OFC 1100–1420 0 1100–1400 0 Direct Gaze Brief minus Long V1 310–760 0 320–660 0 330–680 0 FFA 320–640 0 760–900 0.04 PSTS 640–640 0 360–620 0 340–460 0.01 340–480 0.001 PAC 480–710 0.0002 480–790 0.001 OFC 630–790 0.05 n.s n.s. Long minus Brief V1 820–1370 0 830–1420 0 FFA 970–1300 0 950–1420 0 pSTS 980–1330 0 990–1360 0 PAC 970–1390 0 960–1440 0 980–1090 0.003 OFC 1080–1400 0 1090–1440 0

Table 6. Timing of signifcant diferences in β phase locking across trials within ROIs between brief (250 ms) and long (883 ms) exposures (reported p-values are non-parametric and corrected based on cluster permutations thresholded at p < 0.05 uncorrected).

identical for the frst 250 ms, phase locking in and between our ROIs again showed a stronger initial response to averted-gaze fear in both the α and β frequency bands, which resembled the response during the brief stim- ulus exposures. However, this stronger phase locking to averted gaze was sustained longer, compared with the brief-exposure condition, and extended past the point at which direct-gaze fear-related processing had super- seded averted-gaze threat-related processing in the brief duration trials. Similar to brief exposures, mid-trial phase locking during the longer-exposure trials was stronger to direct-gaze fear faces compared to averted-gaze fear faces, but this sensitivity to threat signal incongruity began later and was not superseded by increased phase locking to congruent threat cues later in the trial, as in the brief-exposure trials. In addition to the temporal diferences, we found diferences in the spatial pattern of signifcant phase lock- ing. Compared to the phase locking during brief-exposure trials, which was concentrated around lef PAC, phase locking during longer exposures was centered mostly around right FFA. Refective processing is associ- ated more with the ventral stream16, of which the FFA is a key module. Tus, greater involvement of FFA dur- ing longer-exposure trials may provide some support to our hypothesis of refective processing being biased toward ventral stream processing. A notable connection for right FFA during longer exposures was to right pSTS, which displayed two temporally and spectrally distinct periods of incongruent threat-cue sensitivity immediately preceding and following stimulus ofset. Te late involvement of pSTS during longer exposures could potentially be representative of its function as part of the “social brain”93, as the right pSTS in particular has been linked to internalizing another’s perspective94. It is thought to be a key region for visual integration of social cues, espe- cially when inferring social meaning from combinations of facial expression and eye gaze95, making it a sensible candidate to be recruited in resolving possible ambiguity of the incongruent threat cue during longer exposures. Collectively, the phase locking seen in the right hemisphere supports the idea of slower refective analysis being mediated by the ‘high road’85 during longer exposures to resolve the meaning of the incongruent threat cue. Tis appears at odds with the lef hemisphere’s role as being primarily responsible for refective analysis in previous fndings96. One speculative resolution of this incongruity could lie in the general right hemisphere specialization for face processing97,98 as well as in the right STS’s specialization in gaze perception99. Te nature of such late activity is difcult to interpret, however, particularly since most studies on the topic have been done with fMRI

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 16 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

Figure 5. Signifcant phase locking for averted vs. direct gaze fearful faces in the extended face-processing network. (A) Signifcant phase locking for averted-gaze (bright blue) vs. direct-gaze (dark blue) fearful faces during brief exposures (250 ms). (B) Signifcant phase locking for averted gaze (bright red) vs. direct gaze (dark red) fearful faces during long exposures (883 ms). Lef is the lef hemisphere phase-locking network (denoted by the image of the lef hemisphere). Right is the right hemisphere phase-locking network. Above is the signifcant phase locking overlaid on an infated Freesurfer brain to show anatomical locations of signifcant phase locking. White outlines depict phase locking (PLF) within a region (i.e. phase locking across trials) while black outlines depict phase-locking values (PLV) between regions. Te phase locking’s frequency band is marked by the appropriate Greek letter to the lef (α = 8–13 Hz, β = 13–30 Hz). Below is the signifcant phase locking magnifed and arranged by time of efects as well as frequency band of efects (α on top, β on bottom, as noted on the far right) to display the temporal progression of signifcant phase locking through the trial. Te anatomical location/connection of the phase locking is noted to the lef. Stimulus ofset is marked by the solid black line. For all plots, grey depicts periods of non-signifcant phase locking while colored (see legend/A,B descriptions for color to trial type correspondence) blobs depict signifcant phase locking determined by non-parametric cluster permutation tests as outlined in Methods.

and were not explicitly designed to test the lateralization of the amygdala responses. It is worth noting that there is growing evidence for sex diferences in processing emotional stimuli100. Indeed, other recent work has shown sex diferences in amygdala lateralization in response to congruent and incongruent facial fear and eye gaze cues with fMRI101. Unfortunately, the current fMRI data cannot ofer a clear conclusion on the sex-related diferences due to the highly unequal and small sample size, especially for male participants (N = 4). However, does ofer a direct replication of a previous fMRI study by our group (Adams et al.15) in which the sample size was more balanced across the sexes (15 females to 14 males). To verify that our results here cannot be explained by sex-related dif- ferences, we ran separate non-parametric cluster-based permutation tests based on an independent sample t-test for signifcant diferences in PAC phase locking between males (N = 20) and females (N = 40). No signifcant

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 17 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

diferences were found between males and females in any of our 4 conditions in either hemisphere (all p’s > 0.24), suggesting our results were not driven by sex-related efects. In our phase-locking results, we did not see much evidence for functional separation between the α and β bands. However, compared to other frequency bands, α-band activity does not ft into a general hypothesis of function102,103. In the specifc context of α activity evoked by viewing emotional faces, α responses have been shown to be higher in response to negative (angry) compared to positive (happy) emotional faces over temporal, parietal, and occipital recording sites104, but weaker in response to valenced faces (both positive and negative) compared to neutral faces105,106. Te evidence for any specifc α-band function in emotional perception is sparse and inconsistent, but none of it suggests the role of inhibition103. We found α-band activity to be sensitive to neg- ative valence, as α phase locking was stronger both in initial refexive processing of averted-gaze fear, suggesting a role in increasing shared-signal processing efciency, and in late refective processing of the direct-gaze fear (incongruent signal). In addition, α-phase locking was mostly congruent with β activity both in efects of stimu- lus duration and efects of eye-gaze, supporting an active non-inhibitory role for α-band oscillations during facial threat cue perception, at least in terms of its phase locking (compared to power or other magnitude-dependent measures). An important limitation of the current work reported here is that we only examined neurodynamics in response to congruent and incongruent threat cues portrayed by fearful faces. Future work will be necessary to supply additional evidence that threat cue congruency is the primary driving factor behind these results, a situation similar to the previous works upon which these studies are based9,14,15,54. In addition, we had no evi- dence from the previous fMRI studies that identical timing parameters would apply to threat cues portrayed by angry faces compared to fearful faces. However, initial eforts with longer exposures do suggest these efects to not simply be an efect of gaze as the original work demonstrated that incongruent cues (direct-gaze fear and averted-gaze anger) elicited stronger amygdala activation than their congruent counterparts despite these cues featuring opposite gaze directions. To provide categorical support for the shared signal hypothesis, future time-resolved explorations will be necessary utilizing other types of congruent and incongruent threat cues, such as anger faces. In summary, with fMRI, we replicated previous stimulus exposure duration efects for fear expression process- ing as a function of gaze perception using a broad stimulus set and employing an event-related design, and show- ing that previous fndings using this paradigm9,14,15,54 are not dependent upon states induced by a block design. Tis set the stage for using this identical paradigm in MEG to examine the temporal dynamics of the efect. We found that the stimulus exposure duration strongly modulated the fne-grained neurodynamics in the extended face perception network, resulting in a slower processing sequence for the longer-exposure stimuli. However, the efects of gaze, while modulated by the exposure duration, were nonetheless evident in greater early phase locking to averted gaze, providing neural evidence at a high temporal resolution that these congruent compound cues are indeed allocated more preconscious attention, and in increased later phase locking to direct gaze, again supporting the idea that slow, refective processing is biased towards the incongruent threat cue. However, the fast neurodynamics in the extended face-processing network in response to these brief and longer exposures observed with MEG show that its response is far more complicated than a simple dichotomy of a refexive response induced by a brief stimulus exposure and a refective response induced by a longer stimulus presentation. Here we have shown that the observed greater BOLD response to averted-gaze fear in the amygdala complex during brief expo- sures is not the result of the processing being curtailed by the brief presentation, and thus cutting of refective processing. Rather, it likely stems from the summation of the hemodynamic signal over the entire trial in fMRI, as we observed stronger mid-trial phase locking to direct-gaze fear during both brief and longer exposures, and the initial refexive response to averted-gaze fear, again regardless of exposure duration. Previous attempts to ration- alize the opposing fndings in fMRI suggested that congruent cues such as averted-gaze fear not only capture attention more readily, but due to their aversive nature, may result in more rapid disengagement, explaining the comparative dwelling of the neural response on direct-gaze fear faces during longer exposures107. Te ability to empirically address this speculation with the high temporal resolution of MEG revealed that this does not in fact seem to be the case, as we observed prolonged cognitive engagement with averted-gaze fear relative to direct-gaze fear under longer exposures. Our fndings thus underscore the importance of investigating brain activity using techniques with high temporal resolution to gain a more complete picture of the neurodynamics underlying per- ception of compound threat cues from the face. References 1. Adams, R. B. Jr., Franklin, R. G. Jr., Nelson, A. J. & Stevenson, M. T. Compound social cues in face processing in Te Science of Social Vision (eds Adams, R. B. Jr., Ambady, N., Nakayama, K. & Shimojo, S.) 90–107 (Oxford Universtiy Press, 2010). 2. Adams, R. B. Jr., Ambady, N., Macrae, C. N. & Kleck, R. E. Emotional expressions forecast approach-avoidance behavior. Motivation and Emotion 30, 179–188 (2006). 3. Adams, R. B. Jr. & Kleck, R. E. Perceived Gaze direction and the processing of facial displays of emotion. Psychological Science 14, 644–647 (2003). 4. Adams, R. B. Jr. & Kleck, R. E. Efects of Direct and Averted Gaze on the Perception of Facially Communicated Emotion. Emotion 5, 3–11 (2005). 5. Sander, D., Grandjean, D., Kaiser, S., Wehrle, T. & Scherer, K. R. Interaction efects of perceived gaze direction and dynamic facial expression: Evidence for appraisal theories of emotion. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 19, 470–480 (2007). 6. Benton, C. P. Rapid reactions to direct and averted facial expressions of fear and anger. Visual Cognition 18, 1298–1319 (2010). 7. Fox, E., Mathews, A., Calder, A. J. & Yiend, J. Anxiety and sensitivity to gaze direction in emotionally expressive faces. Emotion 7, 478–486 (2007). 8. Milders, M., Hietanen, J. K., Leppänen, J. M. & Braun, M. Detection of emotional faces is modulated by the direction of eye gaze. Emotion 11, 1456–1461 (2011). 9. Hadjikhani, N., Hoge, R., Snyder, J. & de Gelder, B. Pointing with the eyes: Te role of gaze in communicating danger. Brain and Cognition 68, 1–8 (2008).

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 18 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

10. N’Diaye, K., Sander, D. & Vuilleumier, P. Self-relevance processing in the human amygdala: gaze direction, facial expression, and emotion intensity. Emotion 9, 798–806 (2009). 11. Sato, W., Yoshikawa, S., Kochiyama, T. & Matsumura, M. Te amygdala processes the emotional signifcance of facial expressions: An fMRI investigation using the interaction between expression and face direction. NeuroImage 22, 1006–1013 (2004). 12. Whalen, P. J. et al. A functional MRI study of human amygdala responses to facial expressions of fear versus anger. Emotion 1, 70–83 (2001). 13. Adams, R. B. Jr. & Janata, P. A comparison of neural circuits underlying auditory and visual object categorization. NeuroImage 16, 361–377 (2002). 14. Adams, R. B. Jr., Gordon, H. L., Baird, A. A., Ambady, N. & Kleck, R. E. Efects of gaze on amygdala sensitivity to anger and fear faces. Science 300, 1536 (2003). 15. Adams, R. B. Jr. et al. Amygdala responses to averted vs direct gaze fear vary as a function of presentation speed. Social Cognitive and Afective 7, 568–577 (2012). 16. Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T. & Trope, Y. Refexion and refection: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 34, 199–249 (2002). 17. Lieberman, M. D. Refexive and refective judgment processes: A social cognitive neuroscience approach in Social judgments: Implicit and explicit processes (eds Forgas, J., Kipling, D. W. & Hippel, W.) 44–67 (Cambridge University Press, 2003). 18. Cunningham, W. A. & Zelazo, P. D. Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(3), 97–104 (2007). 19. Ewbank, M. P., Fox, E. & Calder, A. J. Te Interaction Between Gaze and Facial Expression in the Amygdala and Extended Amygdala is Modulated by Anxiety. Frontiers in human neuroscience 4, 1–11 (2010). 20. Wicker, B., Michel, F., Henaf, M. A. & Decety, J. Brain regions involved in the perception of gaze: a PET study. NeuroImage 8, 221–227 (1998). 21. Hooker, C. I. et al. Brain networks for analyzing eye gaze. Cognitive Brain Research 17, 406–418 (2003). 22. Haxby, J. V., Hofman, E. A. & Gobbini, M. I. Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biological Psychiatry 51, 59–67 (2002). 23. Hardee, J. E., Tompson, J. C. & Puce, A. Te lef amygdala knows fear: Laterality in the amygdala response to fearful eyes. Social Cognitive and Afective Neuroscience 3, 47–54 (2008). 24. Puce, A., Allison, T., Gore, J. C. & McCarthy, G. Face-sensitive regions in human extrastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. Journal of Neurophysiology 74, 1192–1199 (1995). 25. Hofman, E. A. & Haxby, J. V. Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nature neuroscience 3, 80–84 (2000). 26. George, N., Driver, J. & Dolan, R. J. Seen gaze-direction modulates fusiform activity and its coupling with other brain areas during face processing. NeuroImage 13, 1102–1112 (2001). 27. Harris, R. J., Young, A. W. & Andrews, T. J. Morphing between expressions dissociates continuous from categorical representations of facial expression in the . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109, 21164–21169 (2012). 28. Baron-Cohen, S. et al. Social intelligence in the normal and autisitic brain: an fMRI study. European Journal of Neuroscience 11, 1891–1898 (1999). 29. Nummenmaa, L. & Calder, A. J. Neural mechanisms of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13, 135–143 (2009). 30. Adolphs, R. & Birmingham, E. Neural substrates of social perception in Oxford Handbook of Face Perception (ed. Rhodes, G., Calder, A., Johnson, M., & Haxby, J. V.) 577–580 (Oxford University Press, 2012). 31. Attal, Y. et al. Modeling and detecting deep brain activity with MEG & EEG. Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology - Proceedings 4937–4940 (2007). 32. Dumas, T. et al. MEG study of amygdala responses during the perception of emotional faces and gaze. IFMBE Proceedings 28, 330–333 (2010). 33. Attal, Y. & Schwartz, D. Assessment of Subcortical Source Localization Using Deep Brain Activity Imaging Model with Minimum Norm Operators: A MEG Study. PLoS ONE 8, 1–14 (2013). 34. Dumas, T. et al. MEG Evidence for Dynamic Amygdala Modulations by Gaze and Facial Emotions. PLoS ONE 8, 1–11 (2013). 35. Dumas, T., Attal, Y., Dubal, S., Jouvent, R. & George, N. Detection of activity from the amygdala with magnetoencephalography. IRBM 32, 42–47 (2011). 36. Aggleton, J. P. Te Amygdala: a functional analysis. (Oxford University Press, 2000). 37. Pitkänen, A., Pikkarainen, M., Nurminen, N. & Ylinen, A. Reciprocal connections between the amygdala and the , , and in rat. A review. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 911, 369–391 (2000). 38. Varela, F., Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E. & Martinerie, J. Te brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature reviews. Neuroscience 2, 229–239 (2001). 39. Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Delpuech, C. & Pernier, J. Stimulus specifcity of phase-locked and non-phase-locked 40 Hz visual responses in human. Te Journal of Neuroscience 16, 4240–4249 (1996). 40. Klimesch, W., Freunberger, R., Sauseng, P. & Gruber, W. A short review of slow phase synchronization and : Evidence for control processes in diferent memory systems? Brain Research 1235, 31–44 (2008). 41. Düzel, E., Neufang, M. & Heinze, H. J. Te oscillatory dynamics of recognition memory and its relationship to event-related responses. 15, 1992–2002 (2005). 42. Banerjee, S., Snyder, A. C., Molholm, S. & Foxe, J. J. Oscillatory alpha-band mechanisms and the deployment of spatial attention to anticipated auditory and visual target locations: supramodal or sensory-specifc control mechanisms? Te Journal of Neuroscience 31, 9923–9932 (2011). 43. Haegens, S., Nácher, V., Luna, R., Romo, R. & Jensen, O. α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108, 19377–19382 (2011). 44. Romei, V., Gross, J. & Tut, G. On the role of prestimulus alpha rhythms over occipito-parietal areas in visual input regulation: correlation or causation? Te Journal of Neuroscience 30, 8692–8697 (2010). 45. Tut, G. α -Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection. Te Journal of Neuroscience 26, 9494–9502 (2006). 46. Kveraga, K. et al. Early onset of neural synchronization in the contextual associations network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108, 3389–3394 (2011). 47. Okazaki, M., Kaneko, Y., Yumoto, M. & Arima, K. Perceptual change in response to a bistable picture increases neuromagnetic beta-band activities. Neuroscience Research 61, 319–328 (2008). 48. Sehatpour, P. et al. A human intracranial study of long-range oscillatory coherence across a frontal-occipital-hippocampal brain network during visual object processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105, 4399–4404 (2008). 49. Senkowski, D., Molholm, S., Gomez-Ramirez, M. & Foxe, J. J. Oscillatory beta activity predicts response speed during a multisensory audiovisual reaction time task: A high-density electrical mapping study. Cerebral Cortex 16, 1556–1565 (2006).

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 19 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

50. Posner, M. I. Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32, 3–25 (1980). 51. Driver, J. et al. Gaze perception triggers refexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition 6, 509–540 (1999). 52. Cooper, R. M. & Langton, S. R. H. Attentional bias to angry faces using the dot-probe task? It depends when you look for it. Behaviour Research and Terapy 44, 1321–1329 (2006). 53. Jones, B. C. et al. Facial cues of dominance modulate the short-term gaze-cuing efect in human observers. Proceedings. Biological sciences/Te Royal Society 277, 617–624 (2010). 54. Van Der Zwaag, W., Da Costa, S. E., Zürcher, N. R., Adams, R. B. Jr. & Hadjikhani, N. A 7 tesla fMRI study of amygdala responses to fearful faces. Brain Topography 25, 125–128 (2012). 55. Sharon, D., Hämäläinen, M. S., Tootell, R. B. H., Halgren, E. & Belliveau, J. W. Te advantage of combining MEG and EEG: Comparison to fMRI in focally stimulated visual cortex. NeuroImage 36, 1225–1235 (2007). 56. Ekman, R. Pictures of Facial Afect (Consulting Psychologists Press, 1975). 57. Tottenham, N. et al. Te NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgements from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research 168, 242–249 (2009). 58. Brainard, D. H. Te Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision 10, 433–436 (1997). 59. Pelli, D. G. Te VideoToolbox sofware for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision 10, 437–442 (1997). 60. Lieberman, M., Inagaki, T. K., Tabibnia, G. & Crockett, M. J. Subjective responses to emotional stimuli during labeling, reappraisal, and distraction. Emotion 11, 468–480 (2011). 61. Deichmann, R., Gottfried, J. A., Hutton, C. & Turner, R. Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex. NeuroImage 19, 430–441 (2003). 62. Kveraga, K., Boshyan, J. & Bar, M. Magnocellular projections as the trigger of top-down facilitation in recognition. Te Journal of Neuroscience 27, 13232–13240 (2007). 63. Kringelbach, M. L. & Rolls, E. T. Te functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: Evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology 72, 341–372 (2004). 64. Mazaika, P. K., Hoef, F., Glover, G. H., & Reiss, A. L. Methods and Sofware for fMRI Analysis for Clinical Subjects. 15th annual meeting of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (2009). 65. Lieberman, M. D. & Cunningham, W. A. Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: Re-balancing the scale. Social Cognitive and Afective Neuroscience 4, 423–428 (2009). 66. Friston, K. J., Worsley, K. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., Mazziotta, J. C. & Evans, A. C. Assessing the signifcance of focal activations using their spatial extent. Human Brain Mapping 1, 210–220 (1994). 67. Carp, J. Te secret lives of experiments: Methods reporting in the fMRI literature. NeuroImage 63, 289–300 (2012). 68. Woo, C. W., Krishnan, A. & Wager, T. D. Cluster-extent based thresholding in fMRI analyses: Pitfalls and recommendations. NeuroImage 91, 412–419 (2014). 69. Nichols, T. E. Multiple testing corrections, nonparametric methods, and random feld theory. NeuroImage 62, 811–815 (2012). 70. Gramfort, A. et al. MNE sofware for processing MEG and EEG data. NeuroImage 86, 446–460 (2014). 71. Gramfort, A. et al. MEG and EEG data analysis with MNE-Python. Frontiers in Neuroscience 7, 1–13 (2013). 72. Tesche, C. D. et al. Signal-space projections of MEG data characterize both distributed and well-localized neuronal sources. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 95, 189–200 (1995). 73. Uusitalo, M. A. & Ilmoniemi, R. J. Signal-space projection method for separating MEG or EEG into components. Medical & biological engineering & computing 35, 135–140 (1997). 74. Fischl, B. et al. Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. NeuroImage 23, S69–S84 (2004). 75. Lin, F. H., Belliveau, J. W., Dale, A. M. & Hämäläinen, M. S. Distributed current estimates using cortical orientation constraints. Human Brain Mapping 27, 1–13 (2006). 76. Dale, A. M. et al. Dynamic Statistical Parametric Mapping: Combining fMRI and MEG for High-Resolution Imaging of Cortical Activity. Neuron 26, 55–67 (2000). 77. Engemann, D. A. & Gramfort, A. Automated model selection in covariance estimation and spatial whitening of MEG and EEG signals. NeuroImage 108, 328–342 (2015). 78. Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., Martinerie, J. & Varela, F. J. Measuring phase synchrony in brain signals. Human Brain Mapping 8, 194–208 (1999). 79. Maris, E. & Oostenveld, R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 164, 177–190 (2007). 80. Adams, R. B. Jr. et al. Diferentially tuned responses to restricted versus prolonged awareness of threat: A preliminary fMRI investigation. Brain and Cognition 77, 113–119 (2011). 81. Dien, J. A tale of two recognition systems: Implications of the fusiform face area and the visual word form area for lateralized object recognition models. Neuropsychologia 47, 1–16 (2009). 82. Hamilton, C. R. & Vermeire, B. A. Complementary hemispheric specialization in monkeys. Science 242, 1691–1694 (1988). 83. Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J. & Chun, M. M. Te fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Te Journal of Neuroscience 17, 4302–4311 (1997). 84. Yovel, G., Tambini, A. & Brandman, T. Te asymmetry of the fusiform face area is a stable individual characteristic that underlies the lef-visual-feld superiority for faces. Neuropsychologia 46, 3061–3068 (2008). 85. LeDoux, J. E. Te emotional brain: Te mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. (Simon & Schuster, 1998). 86. Hsu, M., Bhatt, M., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D. & Camerer, C. F. Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making. Science 310, 1680–1683 (2005). 87. Costa, T. et al. Temporal and spatial neural dynamics in the perception of basic emotions from complex scenes. Social Cognitive and Afective Neuroscience 9, 1690–1703 (2014). 88. Diano, M., Celeghin, A., Bagnis, A. & Tamietto, M. Amygdala response to emotional stimuli without awareness: Facts and interpretations. Frontiers in Psychology 7, 1–13 (2017). 89. Diano, M. et al. Dynamic changes in amygdala psychophysiological connectivity reveal distinct neural networks for facial expressions of basic emotions. Scientifc Reports 7, 45260 (2017). 90. Méndez-bértolo, C. et al. A fast pathway for fear in human amygdala. Nature Neuroscience 19, 1041–1049 (2016). 91. Celeghin, A., de Gelder, B. & Tamietto, M. From afective blindsight to emotional consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 36, 414–425 (2015). 92. Rogers, L. J., Vallortigara, G., & Andrew, R. J. Divided : Te Biology and Behaviour of Brain Asymmetries. (Cambrdige Universtiy Press, 2013). 93. Lahnakoski, J. M. et al. Naturalistic FMRI mapping reveals superior temporal sulcus as the hub for the distributed brain network for social perception. Frontiers in human neuroscience 6, 233 (2012). 94. David, N. et al. Diferential involvement of the posterior temporal cortex in mentalizing but not perspective taking. Social Cognitive and Afective Neuroscience 3, 279–289 (2008). 95. Adams, R. B. Jr. et al. Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: an fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, 97–108 (2010).

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 20 www.nature.com/scientificreports/

96. Morris, J. S., Ohman, A. & Dolan, R. J. Conscious and unconscious emotional learning in the human amygdala. Nature 393, 467–470 (1998). 97. Zangenehpour, S. & Chaudhuri, A. Patchy organization and asymmetric distribution of the neural correlates of face processing in monkey inferotemporal cortex. Current Biology 15, 993–1005 (2005). 98. Tsao, D. Y. & Livingstone, M. S. Mechanisms of face perception. Annual review of neuroscience 31, 411–437 (2008). 99. Calder, A. J. et al. Separate Coding of Diferent Gaze Directions in the Superior Temporal Sulcus and Inferior Parietal Lobule. Current Biology 17, 20–25 (2007). 100. Kret, M. E. & De Gelder, B. A review on sex diferences in processing emotional signals. Neuropsychologia 50, 1211–1221 (2012). 101. Im, H. Y. et al. Sex-related diferences in behavioral and amygdalar responses to compound facial threat cues. bioRxiv http://www. biorxiv.org/content/early/2017/08/21/179051 (2017). 102. Başar, E. & Güntekin, B. A short review of alpha activity in cognitive processes and in cognitive impairment. International Journal of Psychophysiology 86, 25–38 (2012). 103. Güntekin, B. & Başar, E. A review of brain oscillations in perception of faces and emotional pictures. Neuropsychologia 58, 33–51 (2014). 104. Güntekin, B. & Basar, E. Emotional face expressions are differentiated with brain oscillations. International Journal of Psychophysiology 64, 91–100 (2007). 105. Balconi, M. & Mazza, G. Brain oscillations and BIS/BAS (behavioral inhibition/activation system) efects on processing masked emotional cues. ERS/ERD and coherence measures of alpha band. International Journal of Psychophysiology 74, 158–165 (2009). 106. Balconi, M., Brambilla, E. & Falbo, L. BIS/BAS, cortical oscillations and coherence in response to emotional cues. Brain Research Bulletin 80, 151–157 (2009). 107. Mogg, K., Garner, M. & Bradley, B. P. Anxiety and orienting of gaze to angry and fearful faces. Biological Psychology 76, 163–169 (2007). Acknowledgements This work was supported with the following funds: NIMH R01MH101194 awarded to KK and RBA, Jr. NIMH K01MH084011 awarded to KK, and NIMH R01107797. NCRR P41RR14075 to A.A. Martinos Center. Instrumentation Grants 1S10RR023401, 1S10RR019307, and 1S10RR023043. Author Contributions C.C. and H.Y. contributed data collection, analysis, and writing the paper. R.B.A., Jr. contributed funding, study design/conception, and writing the paper. N.W., D.N.A., and T.G.S. contributed data collection and manuscript editing. K.K. contributed funding, study design/conception, data collection, analysis, and writing the paper. Additional Information Competing Interests: Te authors declare no competing interests. Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional afliations. Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre- ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. Te images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per- mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

© Te Author(s) 2018

SCIenTIfIC REPOrTS | (2018)8:2776 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-20509-8 21