S. Laureys et al. (Eds.) Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 177 ISSN 0079-6123 Copyright r 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved

CHAPTER 28

A new era of coma and consciousness science

Adrian M. Owen1,Ã, Nicholas D. Schiff2 and Steven Laureys3

1MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit; Impaired Consciousness Research Group, Wolfson Brain Imaging Center, , UK 2Department of Neurology and Neuroscience, Weill Medical College of Cornell University, New York, NY, USA 3Coma Science Group, Cyclotron Research Center and Department of Neurology, University of Lie`ge, Lie`ge, Belgium

Abstract: In the past ten years, rapid technological developments in the field of neuroimaging have produced a cornucopia of new techniques for examining both the structure and function of the human brain in vivo. In specialized centers, many of these methods are now being employed routinely in the assessment of patients diagnosed with disorders of consciousness, mapping patterns of residual function and dysfunction and helping to reduce diagnostic errors between related conditions such as the vegetative and minimally conscious states. Moreover, such efforts are beginning to provide important new prognostic indicators, helping to disentangle differences in outcome on the basis of a greater understanding of the underlying mechanisms responsible and providing information that will undoubtedly contribute to improved therapeutic choices in these challenging populations. Of course, these emerging technologies and the new information that they provide will bring new ethical challenges to this area and will have profound implications for clinical care and medical–legal decision-making in this population of patients. We review the most recent work in this area and suggest that the future integration of emerging neuroimaging techniques with existing clinical and behavioral methods of assessment will pave the way for new and innovative applications, both in basic neuroscience and in clinical practice.

Keywords: coma; vegetative state; minimally conscious state; locked-in syndrome; functional MRI; consciousness; ethics

Introduction detailed information about brain structure (e.g. anatomy) and connectivity. Thus, in less than ten It has been a tremendously exciting decade for years, low-resolution metabolic group studies and 15 research into disorders of consciousness. Rapid block design ‘activation studies’ using H2 O technological advances have produced a variety positron emission tomography (PET) have largely of novel neuroimaging approaches that allow a given way to event-related functional magnetic comprehensive assessment of brain function resonance imaging (fMRI) investigations that (e.g. cognitive performance) to be combined with combine high-resolution anatomical imaging with sophisticated psychological paradigms. Until recently, such methods were used primarily as ÃCorresponding author. a correlational tool to ‘map’ the cerebral changes Tel.: +44 1223 355294; Fax: +44 1223 359062; that are associated with a particular cognitive E-mail: [email protected] process or function, be it an action, a reaction

DOI: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17728-2 399 400

(e.g. to some kind of external stimulation) or a undetectable because no cognitive output is thought. But recent advances in imaging technol- possible (for a comprehensive discussion of this ogy, and in particular the ability of fMRI to detect issue — including the limitations of behavioral reliable neural responses in individual participants assessment — see Giacino et al., 2009). This in real time, are beginning to reveal patient’s situation may be further complicated when thoughts, actions or intentions based solely on the patients with disorders of consciousness have pattern of activity that is observed in their brain. underlying deficits in the domain of communica- New techniques such as diffusion tensor imaging tion functions, such as aphasia (the consequences (DTI) are also being used to probe the structural of receptive and/or productive aphasia on the integrity of white matter tracts between key brain already limited behavioral repertoire presented in regions and emerging methods such as resting these patients are reviewed in Majerus et al., 15 state fMRI are beginning to reveal how the 2009). ‘Activation’ methods, such as H2 O PET intrinsic functional connectivity of the brain is and fMRI can be used to link residual neural altered by serious brain injury. Throughout this activity to the presence of covert cognitive new volume of Progress in Brain Research — function. A significant development over the past Coma Science: Clinical and Ethical Implications, ten years has been the relative shift of emphasis 15 the influence of this ‘technical revolution’ is from PET activation studies using H2 O metho- palpable; from methodologically themed chapters dology, to functional magnetic resonance imaging on multimodal approaches to assessment, (fMRI; see Chapters by Coleman et al., 2009; advances in fMRI, DTI and MRI spectroscopy Soddu et al., 2009; Sorger et al., 2009; Monti et al., through to more philosophical chapters on the 2009). Not only is MRI more widely available problem of unreported awareness and the moral than PET, it offers increased statistical power, significance of phenomenal consciousness. In the improved spatial and temporal resolution and following sections, we review some of the key does not involve radiation. Given the heteroge- findings in this area and discuss how novel neous of this patient group and the clinical neuroimaging methods and approaches are begin- need to define each individual in terms of their ning to make a significant impact on the assess- diagnosis, residual functions and potential for ment and management of patients with disorders recovery, these technical benefits are of para- of consciousness. The results have profound mount importance in the evaluation of disorders implications for clinical care, diagnosis, prognosis of consciousness. and ethical and medical–legal decision-making, Recent notable examples include Di et al. but are also beginning to address more basic (2007) who used event-related fMRI to measure scientific questions concerning the nature of brain activation in seven vegetative patients and consciousness and the neural representation of four minimally conscious patients in response to our own thoughts and intentions. the patient’s own name spoken by a familiar voice. Two of the vegetative patients exhibited no significant activity at all, three patients exhibited fMRI: from passive paradigms to the assessment activation in primary auditory areas and two of awareness vegetative patients and four minimally conscious patients exhibited activity in ‘higher-order’ asso- An accurate and reliable evaluation of the level ciative temporal-lobe areas. This result is encoura- and content of cognitive processing is of para- ging, particularly because the two vegetative mount importance for the appropriate manage- patients who showed the most widespread activa- ment of patients diagnosed with disorders of tion subsequently improved to a minimally con- consciousness. Objective behavioral assessment scious state in the following months. Staffen et al. of residual cognitive function can be extremely (2006) also used event-related fMRI to compare difficult as motor responses may be minimal, sentences containing the patient’s own name inconsistent, and difficult to document, or may be (e.g. ‘Martin, hello Martin’), with sentences using 401 another first name, in a patient who had been contextually appropriate word meanings may be vegetative for ten months at the time of the scan. intact in some vegetative patients, despite their Differential cortical processing was observed to clinical diagnoses. the patient’s own name in a region of the medial Does the presence of ‘normal’ brain activation prefrontal cortex, similar to that observed in three in patients with disorders of consciousness indi- healthy volunteers. These findings concur closely cate a level of awareness, perhaps even similar to with a recent electrophysiological study, which has that which exists in healthy volunteers when shown differential P3 responses to patient’s own exposed to the same type of information? Many names (compared to other’s names) in some types of stimuli, including faces, speech and pain vegetative patients (Perrin et al., 2006). Selective will elicit relatively ‘automatic’ responses from cortical processing of one’s own name (when it is the brain; that is to say, they will occur without compared directly with another name) requires the need for willful intervention on the part of the the ability to perceive and access the meaning of participant (e.g. you cannot choose to not words and may imply some level of comprehen- recognize a face, or to not understand speech that sion on the part of these patients. However, as is presented clearly in your native language). In several authors have pointed out, a response to addition, there is a wealth of data in healthy one’s own name is one of the most basic forms of volunteers, from studies of implicit learning and language and may not depend on the higher-level the effects of priming (Schacter, 1994) to studies linguistic processes that are assumed to underpin of learning and speech perception during anesthe- comprehension (Perrin et al., 2006; Owen and sia (Davis et al., 2007) that have demonstrated Coleman, 2008; Laureys et al., 2007). that many aspects of human cognition can go on Several recent studies have sought to address in the absence of awareness. Even the semantic this problem of interpretation by adopting an content of masked information can be primed to ‘hierarchical’ approach to fMRI assessment of affect subsequent behavior without the explicit language processing in disorders of consciousness knowledge of the participant, suggesting that (Owen et al., 2005a, b; Coleman et al., 2007, in some aspects of semantic processing may occur press). At the highest level, responses to sen- without conscious awareness (Dehaene et al., tences containing semantically ambiguous words 1998). By the same argument, ‘normal’ neural (e.g. ‘the creak/creek came from a beam in the responses in patients who are diagnosed with ceiling/sealing’) are compared to sentences con- disorders of consciousness do not necessarily taining no ambiguous words (e.g. ‘her secrets indicate that these patients have any conscious were written in her diary’), in order to reveal experience associated with processing those same brain activity associated with spoken language types of stimuli. This logic exposes a central comprehension (Rodd et al., 2005). In one large conundrum in the study of conscious awareness study of 41 patients, 2 who had been diagnosed as and in particular, how it relates to the vegetative behaviorally vegetative were shown to exhibit and minimally conscious states; our ability to ‘normal’ fMRI activity during the speech compre- know unequivocally that another being is con- hension task (Coleman et al., in press), Moreover, sciously aware is determined, not by whether they these fMRI findings were found to have no are aware or not, but by their ability to commu- association with the patients’ behavioral presenta- nicate that fact through a recognized behavioral tion at the time of investigation and thus provide response (for a fuller discussion of this ‘problem additional diagnostic information beyond the of unreportable awareness’ see Adam Zeman’s traditional clinical assessment. These results chapter, 2009). illustrate how technically complex event-related A significant recent development in this field, fMRI designs are now being combined with therefore, has been the development of fMRI well-characterized psycholinguistic paradigms paradigms that render awareness reportable in the to demonstrate that some of the processes absence of an overt behavioral (e.g. motor or involved in activating, selecting and integrating speech) response (Owen et al., 2006; Boly et al., 402

2007). Crucially, these paradigms differ from the and control conditions. Monti et al. (2009) passive tasks described above (e.g. speech percep- describe a study in which healthy volunteers were tion) because ‘normal’ patterns of fMRI activity presented with a series of neutral words, and are only observed when the patient exerts a alternatively instructed to just listen, or to count, willful, or voluntary, response that is not elicited the number of times a given target was repeated. automatically by the stimulus (for a fuller discus- The counting task revealed the frontoparietal sion of this issue, see Monti et al., 2009). Some of network that has been previously associated with these techniques make use of the general obser- target detection and . When vation that imagining performing a particular task tested on this same procedure, a minimally generates a robust and reliable pattern of brain conscious patient produced a very similar pattern activity in the fMRI scanner that is similar to of activity, confirming that he could willfully adopt actually performing the activity itself. For exam- differential mind-sets as a function of the task ple, imagining moving or squeezing the hands will condition and could actively maintain these mind- generate activity in the motor and premotor sets across time. A similar approach has been cortices (Boly et al., 2007) while imagining adopted recently by Schnakers et al. (2008b) who navigating from one location to another will used, as targets, the patient’s own name and other activate the same regions of the parahippocampal ‘non-salient’ names (e.g. similarly frequent names gyrus and the posterior parietal cortex that have that had no relation to the patient or his/her been widely implicated in map-reading and other family). All of the minimally conscious patients so-called spatial navigation tasks (Jeannerod and that were tested exhibited a significant response Frak, 1999; Aguirre et al., 1996). The robustness when passively listening to their own name. In and reliability of these fMRI responses across addition, however, 9 of 14 patients exhibited more individuals means that activity in these regions activity when instructed to count the number of can be used as a neural proxy for behavior, times their own name (or another target name) confirming that the participant retains the ability occurred than when they passively heard it. to understand instructions, to carry out different This approach also allowed awareness to be mental tasks in response to those instructions and, identified in a case of complete locked-in syn- therefore, is able to exhibit willed, voluntary drome (Schnakers et al., 2009). behavior in the absence of any overt action. On These types of approach all illustrate a paradig- this basis, they permit the identification of matic shift away from passive (e.g. perceptual) awareness at the single-subject level, without the tasks to more active (e.g. willful) tasks in the fMRI need for a motor response (for discussion, see and (EEG) assessment Owen and Coleman, 2008; Monti et al., 2009). of residual cognitive function in patients with This approach was used recently to demon- disorders of consciousness. What sets such tasks strate that a young woman who fulfilled all apart is that the neural responses required are not internationally agreed criteria for the vegetative produced automatically by the eliciting stimulus, state was, in fact, consciously aware and able to but rather, depend on time-dependent and sus- make responses of this sort using her brain activity tained responses generated by the participant. (Owen et al., 2006, 2007). Thus, when the patient Such behavior (albeit neural ‘behavior’) provides was asked to imagine playing tennis or navigate a proxy for an (e.g. motor) action and is, therefore, her way around her house, significant activity was an appropriate vehicle for reportable awareness observed that was indistinguishable from that (also see Zeman, 2009; Overgaard, 2009). exhibited by healthy volunteers performing the same tasks (Boly et al., 2007). An alternative approach that has been explored fMRI as a form of communication? recently is to target processes that require the willful adoption of ‘mind-sets’ in carefully One major aim of clinical assessment in disorders matched (perceptually identical) experimental of consciousness is to harness and nurture any 403 available response, through intervention, into a ‘decoding thoughts’ using both fMRI and EEG form of reproducible communication, however (e.g. Birbaumer and Cohen, 2007; deCharms, rudimentary. The acquisition of any interactive 2007). However, all of these methods require and functional verbal or nonverbal method of extensive training of participants, the decoding communication represents an important mile- algorithm, or both. Moreover, accuracy rates are stone. Clinically, it demarcates the upper bound- typically in the 60–80% range rendering them of ary of minimally conscious state (MCS) (see limited use in clinical decision-making. In an Giacino et al., 2009). More importantly, from a exciting new development, Sorger et al. (2009) quality of life perspective, it allows such patients have developed a novel information encoding to communicate their wishes (e.g. concerning technique that exploits the fact that the signal-to- treatment options), and, therefore, to exert their noise ratio in fMRI time courses is sufficiently right to autonomy. Thus, a key future question for high to reliably detect BOLD signal onsets and functional neuroimaging is whether fMRI data offsets on a single-trial level with a high degree of could ever be used in this way; that is as a form of accuracy. Eight healthy participants ‘answered’ communication, replacing speech or a motor act multiple-choice questions with 95% accuracy in patients for whom such forms of behavioral by intentionally generating single-trial BOLD expression are unavailable? responses in three tasks that were then ‘decoded’ Several recent studies using fMRI suggest that in real time with respect to three influenceable this may be possible. For example, Haynes et al. signal aspects (source location, onset and offset). (2007) asked healthy volunteers to freely decide Although this ‘proof of concept’ was in healthy which of two tasks to perform (to add or subtract participants, such feats of rudimentary ‘mind- two numbers) and to covertly hold onto that reading’ increase the likelihood that in the near decision during a delay. After the delay they future, some patients with disorders of conscious- performed the chosen task, the result indicating ness may also be able to communicate their which task they had intended to do (and thoughts to those around them by simply mod- eventually executed). A classifier was trained to ulating their own neural activity. recognize the characteristic fMRI signatures associated with the two mental states and in 80% of trials was able to decode from activity in Resting state fMRI medial and lateral regions of prefrontal cortex which of the two tasks the volunteers were Another important new direction in the use of intending to perform. Another previous study has fMRI data in the assessment of patients with shown that fMRI can be used as a ‘brain– disorders of consciousness is in the examination of computer interface’ (BCI) that allows real-time so-called ‘resting-state’ data (for a comprehensive communication of thoughts (Weiskopf et al., discussion of this area see Soddu et al., 2009). 2004); healthy volunteers learned to regulate the Recently, increasing attention has been paid to fMRI signal in a particular brain area using their the ‘intrinsic’ functional connectivity of the brain, own fMRI signal as feedback. In general terms, and this can be revealed by examination of fMRI a brain–computer interface is any system that data collected while the participant is not per- translates an individual’s thoughts and intentions forming any active task (e.g. they are ‘at rest’). into signals to control a computer or communicate Resting state data is very easy to obtain in via external hardware, thereby establishing a vegetative and minimally conscious patients, as it ‘direct’ connection between the brain and the does not require the participant to perform external world without any need for motor output any task. Boly et al. (2009a, b) have recently (Kubler and Neumann, 2005; for further discus- investigated spontaneous activation in patients sion, see Sorger et al., 2009). In recent years, with disorders of consciousness and showed that significant progress has been made in developing resting state connectivity in the ‘default network’ sophisticated noninvasive BCI methods for is decreased in proportion to the degree of 404 consciousness impairment. Specifically, they Diffusion tensor imaging demonstrated that cortico-thalamic BOLD func- tional connectivity (i.e. between posterior cingu- Another significant development in the last late/precuneal cortex and medial thalamus) was decade has been the development of various notably absent, but cortico-cortical connectivity methods for assessing the structural connectivity was preserved within the default network in one of the brain (for a comprehensive discussion of vegetative state patient studied 2.5 years following some of these techniques, see Tshibanda et al., cardio-respiratory arrest (Boly et al., 2009a). In a 2009). DTI is a noninvasive magnetic resonance second study, resting state connectivity was technique that allows examination of white matter investigated using probabilistic independent com- fiber tracts in vivo. In white matter, water ponent analysis in 14 noncommunicative brain diffusion is higher along the direction of fiber damaged patients and 14 healthy controls (Boly bundles (due to axonal organization and the et al., 2009b). Connectivity in all default network myelin sheath). This anisotropy is measured with areas was found to be linearly correlated with the MRI to determine anatomical connectivity. To degree of ‘clinical consciousnesses’ (from healthy date, detailed histopathological studies have controls, to locked-in syndrome, to minimally shown no pathological distinctions between vege- conscious state, vegetative state and coma). tative state and some minimally conscious state Moreover, precuneus connectivity was found to patients (Jennett et al., 2001). This approach has be significantly stronger in minimally conscious been used to great effect recently by Voss et al. patients than vegetative state patients. As might (2006) who used DTI to longitudinally character- be expected given their preserved level of aware- ize brain structural connectivity in a minimally ness, in locked-in syndrome patients, default conscious patient who regained expressive and network connectivity was not significantly differ- receptive language 19 years after sustaining a ent from controls (for further discussion see traumatic brain injury. DTI not only revealed Soddu et al., 2009). In this volume, Boly et al. severe diffuse axonal injury, as indicated by (2009) suggest a theoretical framework for under- volume loss in the medial corpus callosum, but standing the properties that grant a system a state also large regions of increased connectivity of consciousness, and highlight the notion of (relative to healthy controls) in posterior parts ‘integration’ as a necessary (but not sufficient) of the brain (i.e. precuneal areas). In a second component of consciousness. While measures of DTI study 18 months later, these posterior regions integration have been proposed previously, the of white matter anisotrophy were reduced in fact that these may be computationally out of directionality. At the same time point, significant reach for a system such as the brain, makes studies increases in anisotrophy within the midline of intrinsic connectivity (e.g. using resting state cerebellar white matter were shown to correlate data) a relatively crude, but informative, approx- with the observed clinical improvement in motor imation of the levels of functional integration control during the previous 18 months. These available at different levels of consciousness. findings strongly suggest that the observed struc- Using a somewhat different, but related, tural changes within the patient’s white matter approach to understanding consciousness and its played a role in his functional recovery. breakdown, Massimini et al. (2009) propose a Coleman et al. (2009) have also used this theoretically grounded methodology for assessing technique as part of a multimodal approach to a system’s capability for producing consciousness. the assessment of patients with disorders of Adopting a ‘perturbational approach’ they sug- consciousness. In one minimally conscious patient gest that the combination of transcranial magnetic described in detail, DTI revealed reduced stimulation (TMS) and high density EEG may ( 38%) fractional anisotropy in comparison to make it possible to evaluate the amount of healthÀ y control subjects, indicating widespread functional integration of a system — a theoretical loss of white matter integrity. Moreover DTI requisite for conscious experience. revealed a significantly increased apparent 405 diffusion coefficient in comparison to healthy for the subsequent care of the patient and volunteers, suggesting loss of cortico-cortical rehabilitation, as well as legal and ethical deci- connectivity. Indeed, a qualitative view of white sion-making. Unfortunately, the behavior elicited matter paths revealed a loss of the inferior by these patients is often ambiguous, inconsistent temporal and inferior frontal pathways that have and typically constrained by varying degrees of been shown to mediate aspects of speech com- paresis making it very challenging to disentangle prehension in some of the psycholinguistic fMRI purely reflexive from voluntary behaviors (for tasks described above (e.g. Rodd et al., 2005). Of further discussion, see Giacino et al., 2009), a fact note, a prospective cohort study of serial DTI that undoubtedly contributes to the high rate of imaging following severe traumatic brain injury diagnostic error (37–43%) in this patient group and coma found that cognitive and behavioral (Andrews et al., 1996; Childs et al., 1993; improvement correlated with recovery of normal Schnakers et al., 2006). In several recent cases, to supranormal fractional anisotropy in prese- neuroimaging data has been entirely inconsistent lected white matter regions (Sidaros et al., 2008). with the formal clinical diagnosis which remains These findings showed a directional specificity based on standard behavioral criteria. For exam- with improvements in fractional anisotropy seen ple, the patient described by Owen et al. (2006), only in the eigenvectors of the diffusion tensor was clearly able to produce voluntary responses associated with diffusion parallel to the axonal (albeit neural responses) to command, yet was fibers; these results are consistent with the Voss unable to match this with any form of motor et al. (2006) study and supportive of possible response at the bedside. Paradoxically therefore, axonal regrowth. this patient’s (motor) behavior was consistent with In the coming years, we expect that the a diagnosis of vegetative state which effectively increasing use if routinely acquired DTI data in depends on an absence of evidence of awareness or disorders of consciousness will yield larger pro- purposeful response, yet her brain imaging data spective studies in this patient group which will were equally consistent with the alternative ultimately determine whether the sorts of slow hypothesis, that she was entirely aware during structural changes reported by Voss et al. (2006) the scanning procedure. Clearly the clinical occur frequently in severe traumatic brain injury diagnosis of vegetative state based on behavioral and whether they have any influence on func- assessment was inaccurate in the sense that it did tional outcomes (e.g. see Perlbarg et al., 2009; not accurately reflect her internal state of aware- Tollard et al., 2009; Tshibanda, 2009). ness. On the other hand, she was not misdiag- nosed in the sense that no behavioral marker of awareness was missed. Similarly, the minimally The impact on diagnosis and prognosis conscious patient described by Monti et al. (2009) was able to ‘perform’ a complex working memory As the use of multimodal imaging methods in the task in the scanner, in the sense that his brain assessment of disorders of consciousness is trans- activity revealed consistent and repeatable com- lated to clinical routine, the likely effects on mand following. While this ‘behavior’ does not diagnosis and prognosis are beginning to become necessarily alter the patient’s formal diagnosis more apparent. The main goal of the clinical (from ‘low’ MCS) it certainly demonstrated a assessment in the vegetative and minimally con- level of responsively that was not revealed by the scious states is to determine whether the patient behavioral examination. retains any purposeful response to stimulation, A second question concerns the implications albeit inconsistent, suggesting they are at least that emerging neuroimaging approaches may partially aware of their environment and/or have for prognosis in this patient group. At themselves. Crucially, this decision separates present, predicting survival, outcome and long- vegetative state from minimally conscious state term cognitive deficits in individual patients with patients and has, therefore, profound implications severe brain injury based on clinical assessment is 406 very difficult (see extensive reviews by Whyte et investigation, but correlated significantly with al., 2009; Katz et al., 2009; Azouvi et al., 2009; subsequent behavioral recovery, six months after Zasler, 2009). It is of interest that in the case the scan. In this case, the fMRI data predicted described by Owen et al. (2006), the patient began subsequent recovery in a way that a specialist to emerge from her vegetative state to demon- behavioral assessment could not. In future, the strate diagnostically relevant behavioral markers full utility of neuroimaging in this context will before the prognostically important 12-month become clearer when even larger studies are threshold was reached (for a diagnosis of perma- conducted, preferably involving multiple centers nent vegetative state), suggesting that early using standardized techniques and paradigms. evidence of awareness acquired with functional neuroimaging may have important prognostic value. Indeed, with a marked increase in the Therapeutic advances number of studies using neuroimaging techniques in patients with disorders of consciousness a At present, there is no empirically proven inter- consistent pattern is starting to emerge. Di et al. vention to facilitate recovery in the vegetative 15 (2008), reviewed 15 separate H2 O PET and state and related disorders of consciousness (e.g. fMRI studies involving 48 published cases which Schnakers et al., 2008a; also see extensive review were classified as ‘absent cortical activation’, by Zafonte et al. (2009) on pharmacotherapy of ‘typical activation’, (involving low level primary arousal and Taira (2009), on intrathecal adminis- sensory cortices) and ‘atypical activation’ (corre- tration of GABA agonists). The favored approach sponding to higher-level associative cortices). The is to create a stable clinical environment for results show that atypical activity patterns appear natural recovery to take place. The greatest to predict recovery from vegetative state with difficulty preventing the development of treat- 93% specificity and 69% sensitivity. That is to say, ment options is the extent and heterogeneity of 9 of 11 patients exhibiting atypical activity pathology underlying these conditions. It is patterns recovered consciousness, whereas 21 of increasingly accepted; therefore, that novel treat- 25 patients with typical primary cortical activity ments designed for the individual or a small group patterns and 4 out of 4 patients with absent of very similar patients will be necessary. One activity failed to recover. This important review such approach is deep brain stimulation (DBS), strongly suggests that functional neuroimaging which uses stereotactically placed electrodes to data can provide important prognostic informa- deliver electrical stimulation to the thalamus (also tion beyond that available from bedside examina- see Moll et al., 2009, on subpallidal DBS-induced tion alone. wakeful unawareness during anesthesia). DBS has In another recent study of 41 patients with recently been employed by Schiff et al. (2007) disorders of consciousness, Coleman et al. (in with startling results in a patient in post traumatic press; also see Coleman et al., 2009) found direct MCS. Electrical stimulation, delivered via electro- evidence of prognostically important information des implanted bilaterally into the central thala- within neuroimaging data that was at odds with mus, was found to produce increased periods of the behavioral assessment at the time of scanning. arousal and responsiveness to command in a 38- Thus, contrary to the clinical impression of a year-old male who had remained in a minimally specialist team using behavioral assessment tools, conscious state for six and a half years following two patients who had been referred to the study the injury. The changes correlated closely with the with a diagnosis of vegetative state, did in fact commencement of DBS and could not be attrib- demonstrate clear signs of speech comprehension uted to gradual recovery over time. Importantly when assessed using fMRI. More importantly however, the patient was at the upper boundary however, across the whole group of patients, the of the minimally conscious state before DBS was fMRI data were found to have no association with commenced. He had also produced inconsistent, the behavioral presentation at the time of the but reproducible evidence of communication and 407 fMRI had shown preservation of cortical language for such a diagnosis (Owen and Coleman, 2008; networks. Yamamoto and Katayama (2005) used Laureys and Boly, 2008). Thus, whether fMRI will a similar technique on more severely impaired ever be used in this context will only become patients and reported positive effects in 8 out of apparent when more patients have been scanned, 21 vegetative patients, who subsequently emerged although if evidence for awareness were to be from vegetative state and obeyed commands. found in a patient who had progressed beyond the However, in that study, DBS was commenced threshold for a diagnosis of permanent vegetative within 3–6 months of brain injury and it is not state, this fact would certainly have profound clear whether the behavioral improvement simply implications for this decision-making process. On reflected natural recovery. There are now wide- the other hand, it is important to point out that spread calls for the methodology of Schiff et al. neuroimaging is unlikely to influence legal pro- (2007) to be extended to a larger number of more ceedings where negative findings have been severely impaired patients in order to evaluate the acquired. False-negative findings in functional potential of this technique to facilitate recovery. neuroimaging studies are common, even in healthy volunteers, and they present particular difficulties in this patient population. For example, Neuroimaging and ethics a patient may have low levels of arousal or even fall asleep during the study (e.g. see review by Neuroimaging of severely brain-injured, noncom- Bekinschtein et al. (2009) on the influence of municative populations of patients raises several arousal fluctuations on patients’ responsiveness) important ethical concerns. Foremost is the or the patient may not have properly heard or concern that diagnostic and prognostic accuracy understood the task instructions, leading to an is assured, as treatment decisions typically include erroneous negative result. Accordingly, single the possibility of withdrawal of life support. In an negative fMRI or EEG findings in patients should excellent discussion of these issues (Fins, 2009), not be used as evidence for impaired cognitive Joseph Fins notes that ‘the utter and fixed futility function or lack of awareness. of the vegetative state became the ethical and Ethical concerns are also sometimes raised legal justification for the genesis of the right-to-die concerning the participation of severely brain– movement in the United States’ (Fins, 2003, 2006; injured patients in functional neuroimaging stu- Fins et al., 2008). At present, although several of dies (e.g. to assess pain perception; see Demertzi the neuroimaging approaches discussed in this et al., 2009), studies that require invasive proce- chapter hold great promise to improve both dures (e.g. intra-arterial or jugular lines required diagnostic and prognostic accuracy, the standard for quantification of PET data or modeling), or approach remains the careful and repeated the use of neuromuscular paralytics. By definition, neurological exam by a trained examiner. unconscious or minimally conscious patients That said, in future, the routine use of cannot give informed consent to participate in techniques such as fMRI and quantitative EEG clinical research and written approval is typically in the diagnostic process (e.g. for the detection of obtained from family or legal representatives awareness), will raise additional issues relating depending on governmental and hospital guide- to legal decision-making and the prolongation, lines in each country. We side with a proposed or otherwise, of life after severe brain injury ethical framework that emphasizes balancing (see Levy and Savulescu, 2009; Lutte, 2009). At access to research and medical advances alongside present, decisions concerning life support (nutri- protection for vulnerable patient populations. tion and hydration) are generally taken once a Severe brain injury represents an immense social diagnosis of permanent vegetative state has been and economic problem that warrants further made. To date, fMRI has not demonstrated research. Unconscious, minimally conscious and unequivocal signs of awareness in any patient locked-in patients are very vulnerable and that has survived beyond the time point required deserve special procedural protections (also see 408

Lule´ et al., 2009, on quality of life in the locked-in this patient group will undoubtedly fall. Likewise, syndrome). However, it is important to stress that clinical teams will have the best possible informa- these severely brain–injured patients are also tion for planning and monitoring interventions to vulnerable to being denied potentially life-saving facilitate recovery. therapy if clinical research cannot be performed adequately (for further discussion, see Fins, 2009). Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the generous support of Conclusions the James S. McDonnell Foundation. AMO is supported by the Medical Research Council Disorders of consciousness present unique pro- (UK). NS is supported by NS02172, NS43451 and blems for diagnosis, prognosis, treatment and the Charles A. Dana Foundation. SL is a everyday management. In this chapter, we have Research Associate at the Fonds National de la reviewed a number of areas where novel neuroi- Recherche Scientifique de Belgique (FNRS). maging methods and approaches are beginning to make a significant impact on the assessment and management of these patients. For example, cognitive activation studies using event-related References fMRI are now being used to objectively describe (using population norms) the regional distribution Aguirre, G. K., Detre, J. A., Alsop, D. C., & D’Esposito, M. (1996). The parahippocampus subserves topographical of cerebral activity at rest and under various learning in man. Cerebal Cortex, 6, 823–829. conditions of stimulation. Indeed, in several rare Andrews, K., Murphy, L., Munday, R., & Littlewood, C. cases, functional neuroimaging has demonstrated (1996). Misdiagnosis of the vegetative state: Retrospective conscious awareness in patients who are assumed study in a rehabilitation unit. British Medical Journal, 313, to be vegetative, yet retain cognitive abilities that 13–16. Azouvi, P., Vallat-Azouvi, C., Belmont, A., & Azouvi, P. have evaded detection using standard clinical (2009). Cognitive deficits and long-term outcome after methods. Similarly, in some patients diagnosed traumatic coma. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen as minimally conscious, functional neuroimaging (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications – has revealed residual cognitive capabilities that Progress in Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. extend well beyond that evident from even the Bekinschtein, T., Cologan, V., Dahmen, B., & Golombek, D. (2009). You are only coming through in waves: Wakefulness most comprehensive behavioral assessment. variability and assessment in patients with impaired con- Moreover, these detailed functional images are sciousness. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), now being combined with high-resolution infor- Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications – Progress in mation about anatomy and images of structural Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. connectivity, acquired using techniques such as Birbaumer, N., & Cohen, L. G. (2007). Brain–computer interfaces: Communication and restoration of movement in DTI, to produce an increasingly cohesive picture paralysis. Journal of Physiology, 579, 621–636. of normal and abnormal brain function following Boly, M., Coleman, M. R., Davis, M. H., Hampshire, A., serious brain injury. Bor, D., Moonen, G., et al. (2007). When thoughts become Although insufficient population data currently actions: An fMRI paradigm to study volitional brain activity exists, evidence to include the use of such in non-communicative brain injured patients. NeuroImage, 36(3), 979–992. techniques in the formal diagnostic and prognostic Boly, M., Massimini, M., & Tononi, T. (2009). How could procedure in this patient group is accumulating information integration theory be applied to the differential rapidly. The emerging view is not that brain diagnosis of patients in altered states of consciousness? In imaging should replace behavioral assessment, S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma but rather that it should be used, wherever science: Clinical and ethical implications – Progress in Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. possible, to acquire further information about the Boly, M., Tshibanda, L., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Noirhomme, patient and their condition. In doing so, the Q., Schnakers, C., Ledoux, D., et al. (2009a). Functional current alarmingly high rate of misdiagnosis in connectivity in the default network during resting state is 409

preserved in a vegetative but not in a brain dead patient. Fins, J. J. (2006). A palliative ethic of care: Clinical wisdom at Human Brain Mapping, 30(8), 2393–2400. life’s end. Sudbury, MA: Jones and Bartlett. Boly, M., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Tshibanda, L., Bruno, M.-A., Fins, J. J., Illes, J., Bernat, J. L., Hirsch, J., Laureys, S., & Boveroux, P., Noirhomme, Q., et al. (2009b). Default Murphy, E. (2008). Neuroimaging and disorders of conscious- network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in ness: Envisioning an ethical research agenda American. non-communicative brain damaged patients. Submitted. American Journal of Bioethics Neuroscience, 8, 3–12. Childs, N. L., Mercer, W. N., & Childs, H. W. (1993). Accuracy Giacino, J. T., Schnakers, C., Rodriguez-Moreno, D., Schiff, of diagnosis of persistent vegetative state. Neurology, 43, N. D., & Kalmar, K., (2009). Behavioral assessment in 1465–1467. patients with disorders of consciousness: Gold standard or Coleman, M. R., Davis, M. H., Rodd, J. M., Robson, T., fool’s gold? In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen Ali, A., Pickard, J. D., et al. (in press). Towards the routine (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications – use of brain imaging to aid the clinical diagnosis of disorders Progress in Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. of consciousness. Brain. Haynes, J.-D., Sakai, K., Rees, G., Gilbert, S., Frith, C., & Coleman, M. R., Monti, M. M., Bekinschtein, T., Pickard, J., & Passingham, R. E. (2007). Hidden intentions in the human Owen, A. M. (2009). A multimodal approach to the brain. Current Biology, 10, 1016. assessment of patients with disorders of consciousness. In Jennett, B., Adams, J. H., Murray, L. S., & Graham, D. I. S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma (2001). Neuropathology in vegetative and severely disabled science: Clinical and ethical implications – Progress in Brain patients after head injury. Neurology, 56, 486–490. Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. Jeannerod, M., & Frak, V. G. (1999). Mental simulation of Coleman, M. R., Rodd, J. M., Davis, M. H., Johnsrude, I. S., action in human subjects. Current Opinions in Neurobiology, Menon, D. K., Pickard, J. D., et al. (2007). Do vegetative 9, 735–739. patients retain aspects of language comprehension? Evi- Katz, D., Polyak, M., Coughlin, D., Nichols, M., & Roche, A. dence from fMRI. Brain, 130, 2494–2507. (2009). Natural history of recovery from brain injury after Davis, M. H., Coleman, M. R., Absalom, A. R., Rodd, J. M., prolonged impairment of consciousness: Patients admitted to Johnsrude, I. S., Matta, B. F., et al. (2007). Dissociating inpatient rehabilitation. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. speech perception and comprehension at reduced levels of Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications – awareness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Progress in Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. 104(41), 16032–16037. Kubler, A., & Neumann, N. (2005). Brain-computer inter- deCharms, R. C. (2007). Reading and controlling human brain faces — The key for the conscious brain locked into a activation using real-time functional magnetic resonance paralyzed body. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 513–525. imaging. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 473–481. Laureys, S., & Boly, M. (2008). The changing spectrum of Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec, H. G., Koechlin, E., coma. Nature Clinical Practice Neurology, 4, 544–546. Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., et al. (1998). Imaging Levy, N., & Savulescu, J. (2009). The moral significance of unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395, 597–600. phenomenal consciousness. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & Demertzi, A., Schnakers, C., Ledoux, D., Chatelle, C., Bruno, A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical M.-A., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., et al. (2009). Different beliefs implications – Progress in Brain Research (this volume). about pain perception in the vegetative and minimally Oxford: Elsevier. conscious states: A European survey of medical and Laureys, S., Perrin, F., & Bre´dart, S. (2007). Self-consciousness paramedical professionals. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & in non-communicative patients. Consciousness and Cogni- A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and tion, 16, 722–741. ethical implications – Progress in Brain Research (this Lule´, D., Zickler, C., Ha¨cker, S., Bruno, M. A., Pellas, F., volume). Oxford: Elsevier. Demertzi, A., et al. (2009). Life may be worth living in locked- Di, H., Boly, M., Weng, X., Ledoux, D., & Laureys, S. (2008). in syndrome. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen Neuroimaging activation studies in the vegetative state: (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications – Predictors of recovery? Clinical Medicine, 8, 502–507. Progress in Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. Di, H. B., Yu, S. M., Weng, X. C., Laureys, S., Yu, D., Li, J. Q., Lutte, I. (2009). Defining personal loss after severe brain et al. (2007). Cerebral response to patient’s own name in the damage. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), vegetative and minimally conscious states. Neurology, 68, Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications – Progress in 895–899. Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier Fins, F. (2009). The ethics of measuring and modulating cons- Majerus, S., Bruno, M. A., Schnakers, C., Giacino, J. T., & ciousness: The imperative of minding time. In S. Laureys, Laureys, S. (2009). The problem of aphasia in the assessment N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical of consciousness in brain damaged patients. In S. Laureys, and ethical implications – Progress in Brain Research N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. and ethical implications – Progress in Brain Research Fins, J. J. (2003). Constructing an ethical stereotaxy for severe (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. brain injury: Balancing risks, benefits and access. Nature Massimini, M., Boly, M., Casali, A., & Tononi, G. A. (2009). A Reviews Neuroscience, 4(4), 323–327. perturbational approach to implement theoretical measures 410

of consciousness at the patient’s bedside. In S. Laureys, Schacter, D. L. (1994). Priming and multiple memory systems: N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical Perceptual mechanisms of implicit memory. In D. L. Schacter and ethical implications – Progress in Brain Research & E. Tulving (Eds.), Memory systems (pp. 233–268). (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Moll, C. K. E., Sharott, A., Hamel, W., Mu¨ nchau, A., Schiff, N. D., Giacino, J. T., Kalmar, K., Victor, J. D., Buhmann, C., Hidding, U., et al. (2009). Waking up the Baker, K., Gerber, M., et al. (2007). Behavioural improve- brain. A case study of stimulation-induced wakeful unaware- ments with thalamic stimulation after severe traumatic brain ness during anesthesia. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. injury. Nature, 448, 600–603. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications Schnakers, C., Giacino, J., Kalmar, K., Piret, S., Lopez, E., – Progress in Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. Boly, M., et al. (2006). Does the FOUR score correctly Monti, M. M., Coleman, M. R., & Owen, A. M. (2009). From diagnose the vegetative and minimally conscious states? V1 to volition: Hierarchical assessment of visual cognition Annals of Neurology, 60, 744–745. and attention with fMRI. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & Schnakers, C., Perrin, F., Schabus, M., Hustinx, R., Majerus, S., A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical Moonen, G., et al. (2009). Detecting consciousness in a total implications – Progress in Brain Research (this volume). locked-in syndrome: An active event related paradigm. Oxford: Elsevier. Neurocase, 25, 1–7. Overgaard, M. (2009). How can we know if patients in coma, Schnakers, C., Hustinx, R., Vandewalle, G., Majerus, S., vegetative state or minimally conscious state are conscious? Moonen, G., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., et al. (2008a). Measuring In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma the effect of amantadine in chronic anoxic minimally science: Clinical and ethical implications – Progress in Brain conscious state. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. Psychiatry, 79, 225–227. Owen, A. M., & Coleman, M. (2008). Functional imaging in the Schnakers, C., Perrin, F., Schabus, M., Majerus, S., Ledoux, D., vegetative state. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 235–243. Damas, P., et al. (2008b). Voluntary brain processing in Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Davis, M. H., Boly, M., disorders of consciousness. Neurology, 71, 1614–1620. Laureys, S., & Pickard, J. D. (2006). Detecting awareness in Sidaros, A., Engberg, A. W., Sidaros, K., Liptrot, M. G., the vegetative state. Science, 313, 1402. Herning, M., Petersen, P., et al. (2008). Diffusion tensor Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Davis, M. H., Boly, M., imaging during recovery from severe traumatic brain injury Laureys, S., & Pickard, J. D. (2007). Response to comments and relation to clinical outcome: A longitudinal study. Brain, on ‘‘Detecting awareness in the vegetative state’’. Science, 131, 559–572. 315, 1221c. Soddu, A., Boly, M., Papa, M., Laureys, S., & Malach, R. Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Menon, D. K., Berry, E. L., (2009). Reaching across the abyss: Recent advances in Johnsrude, I. S., Rodd, J. M., et al. (2005a). Using a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and their hierarchical approach to investigate residual auditory cogni- potential relevance to deficits of consciousness. In S. Laureys, tion in persistent vegetative state. In S. Laureys (Ed.), The N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and boundaries of consciousness: Neurobiology and neuropathol- ethical implications – Progress in Brain Research (this volume). ogy. Progress in Brain Research (Vol. 150, pp. 461–476). Oxford: Elsevier. London: Elsevier. Sorger, B., Dahmen, B., Reithler, J., Gosseries, O., Maudoux, Owen, A. M., Coleman, M. R., Menon, D. K., Johnsrude, I. S., A., Laureys, S., et al. (2009). Another kind of ‘BOLD Rodd, J. M., Davis, M. H., et al. (2005b). Residual auditory response’: Answering multiple choice questions by differ- function in persistent vegetative state: A combined PET ential single-trial fMRI responses. In S. Laureys, N. D. and fMRI study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 15(3–4), Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and 290–306. ethical implications – Progress in Brain Research (this Perlbarg, V., Puybasset, L., Tollard, E., Lehe´ricy, S., Benali, volume). Oxford: Elsevier. H., & Galanaud, D. (2009). Relation between brain lesion Staffen, W., Kronbichler, M., Aichhorn, M., Mair, A., & location and clinical outcome in patients with severe Ladurner, G. (2006). Selective brain activity in response to traumatic brain injury: A diffusion tensor imaging study one’s own name in the persistent vegetative state. Journal of using voxel-based approaches. Human Brain Mapping,in Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 77, 1383–1384. press. Available at http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/ Taira, T. (2009). Intrathecal administration of GABA agonists journal/122440012/abstract?CRETRY=1&SRETRY=0 in the vegetative state. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & Perrin, F., Schnakers, C., Schabus, M., Degueldre, C., A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical Goldman, S., Bredart, S., et al. (2006). Brain response to implications – Progress in Brain Research (this volume). one’s own name in vegetative state, minimally conscious Oxford: Elsevier. state, and locked-in syndrome. Archives of Neurology, 63, Tollard, E., Galanaud, D., Perlbarg, V., Sanchez-Pena, P., Le 562–569. Fur, Y., Abdennour, L., et al. (2009). Experience of diffusion Rodd, J. M., Davis, M. H., & Johnsrude, I. S. (2005). The tensor imaging and 1H spectroscopy for outcome prediction neural mechanisms of speech comprehension: fMRI studies in severe traumatic brain injury: Preliminary results. Critical of semantic ambiguity. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1261. Care Medicine, 37(4), 1448–1455. 411

Tshibanda, J. F., Galanaud, D., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Boly, M., (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications – Laureys, S., & Puybasset, L. (2009). MRI spectroscopy and Progress in Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. diffusion tensor imaging in coma survivors: Promises and Yamamoto, T., & Katayama, Y. (2005). Deep brain stimula- pitfalls. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), tion therapy for the vegetative state. Neuropsychological Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications – Progress in Rehabilitation., 15(3/4), 406–413. Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. Zafonte, R., Hammond, F., Dennison, A., & Chew, E. (2009). Voss, H. U., Uluc-, A. M., Dyke, J. P., Watts, R., Kobylarz, E. J., Pharmacotherapy of arousal: Balancing the risks and McCandliss, B. D., et al. (2006). Possible axonal regrowth in benefits. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), late recovery from the minimally conscious state. The Journal Coma science: Clinical and ethical implications – Progress in of Clinical Investigation, 116, 2005–2011. Brain Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier. Weiskopf, N., Mathiak, K., Bock, S. W., Scharnowski, F., Zasler, N. (2009). Life expectancy and disorders of conscious- Veit, R., Grodd, W., et al. (2004). Principles of a brain- ness after traumatic brain injury. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, computer interface (BCI) based on real-time functional & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma science: Clinical and ethical magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). IEEE Transactions on implications – Progress in Brain Research (this volume). Biomedical Engineering, 51, 966–970. Oxford: Elsevier. Whyte, J., Gosseries, O., Chervoneva, I., DiPasquale, M. C., Zeman, A. (2009). The problem of unreportable awareness. Giacino, J., Kalmar, K., et al. (2009). Predictors of short- In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen (Eds.), Coma term outcome in brain injured patients with disorders of science: Clinical and ethical implications – Progress in Brain consciousness. In S. Laureys, N. D. Schiff, & A. M. Owen Research (this volume). Oxford: Elsevier.