Emotional Mental Imagery Generation During Spontaneous Future Thinking - 7 Relationship with Optimism and Negative Mood 8

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Emotional Mental Imagery Generation During Spontaneous Future Thinking - 7 Relationship with Optimism and Negative Mood 8 1 2 3 4 5 6 Emotional mental imagery generation during spontaneous future thinking - 7 relationship with optimism and negative mood 8 9 10 Julie L. Ji 1, Fionnuala C. Murphy 2, Ben Grafton 1, Colin MacLeod 1, Emily A. 11 Holmes 3 12 13 1 School of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Australia 14 2 Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of 15 Cambridge, Cambridge, UK 16 3 Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Sweden 17 18 19 *Corresponding author information: Dr Julie Lin Ji, M304, University of Western 20 Australia, 35 Stirling Highway, Perth WA 6009, Australia (e-mail: 21 [email protected]). 22 23 1 24 Abstract 25 Optimism is known to buffer against negative mood, thus understanding the factors that 26 contribute to individual variation in optimism may inform interventions for mood disorders. 27 Preliminary evidence suggests that the generation of mental imagery-based representations 28 of positive relative to negative future scenarios is related to optimism. This study investigated 29 the hypothesis that a lower tendency to generate positive relative to negative mental imagery 30 during spontaneous future thinking would be associated with elevated negative mood via its 31 relationship to lower optimism. Participants (N = 44) with varied levels of naturally occurring 32 negative mood reported current levels of optimism and the real-time occurrence and 33 characteristics of spontaneous thoughts during a sustained attention computer task 34 designed. Consistent with hypothesis, lower optimism statistically mediated the relationship 35 between lower proportional frequency of positive relative to negative mental imagery during 36 spontaneous future thinking and elevated negative mood. Further, the relationship between 37 emotional mental imagery and optimism was found for future, but not past, thinking, nor for 38 verbal future or past thinking. Thus, a greater tendency to generate positive rather than 39 negative imagery-based mental representations when spontaneously thinking about the 40 future may influence how optimistic one feels, which in turn may influence one’s level of 41 negative mood. 42 [Word count: 200] 43 Keywords: mental imagery, spontaneous future thinking, optimism, negative mood 44 45 46 2 47 Negative mood states such as feeling sad, anxious and unhappy are common in daily 48 life, and at any one time, with some individuals experiencing higher levels of negative mood 49 than others at any one time. Researchers have sought to understand the cognitive factors 50 driving heightened negative mood, as it may inform intervention development for prevalent 51 mood dysregulation conditions such as depression and anxiety. 52 One factor known to drive the degree of negative mood is optimism, the generalised 53 feeling that one’s own future will turn out well (Carver, Scheier, & Segerstrom, 2010). It is 54 well established that those who are optimistic tend to experience less negative emotional 55 and physiological impacts under stress (Brydon, Walker, Wawrzyniak, Chart, & Steptoe, 56 2009; Carver & Gaines, 1987; Scheier & Carver, 1985; Segerstrom, Taylor, Kemeny, & 57 Fahey, 1998). On the other hand, theory and evidence suggests that negative views of one’s 58 future is one factor driving heightened levels of depression and anxiety (Abramson, 59 Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989; Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979; Miloyan, Bulley, & Suddendorf, 60 2016; Miranda & Mennin, 2007; Muris & Van Der Heiden, 2006; Pyszczynski, Holt, & 61 Greenberg, 1987; Roepke & Seligman, 2016; Strunk, Lopez, & DeRubeis, 2006). Given that 62 optimism buffers against negative mood, understanding factors that drive variation in how 63 optimistic we feel may illuminate new pathways to reducing mood disturbance. 64 Emotional future mental imagery and optimism 65 As the future does not exist, it is likely that our mental representations of the future 66 influences how optimistic we feel. Researchers have postulated that the human capacity to 67 generate mental imagery-based representations of future scenarios that could occur is an 68 important form of future thinking as it allows individuals to simulate and predict possible 69 future outcomes (Bulley, Redshaw, & Suddendorf, 2019; Schacter, Benoit, & Szpunar, 70 2017). Mental imagery refers to internally generated sensori-perceptual experiences that can 71 function as a weak form of veridical perception (Pearson, Naselaris, Holmes, & Kosslyn, 72 2015), thus mental imagery-based representations of future scenarios can allow individuals 73 to pre-experience what might happen in an as-if-real manner (Ji, Heyes, MacLeod, & 74 Holmes, 2016; Lang, 1979; Moulton & Kosslyn, 2009). 3 75 Experimental evidence suggests that generating mental imagery-based 76 representations of hypothetical emotional scenarios can influence one’s attitudes towards 77 such scenarios (Benoit, Paulus, & Schacter, 2019). Further, hypothetical emotional 78 scenarios that are imagined more frequently are judged as more realistic and likely to occur, 79 but only when such scenarios are future-oriented, not past-oriented (De Brigard, Szpunar, & 80 Schacter, 2013; Schacter, Benoit, De Brigard, & Szpunar, 2015; Szpunar & Schacter, 2013). 81 Thus, when spontaneously thinking about the future, individuals who tend to generate 82 mental imagery-based representations of positive rather than negative scenarios may also 83 feel more optimistic about what the future holds, compared to those who are less likely to do 84 so. 85 To our knowledge, no study has directly examined the relationship between biases in 86 one’s tendency to generate positive relative to negative mental imagery during spontaneous 87 (uninstructed) future thinking and optimism. However, some researchers have examined the 88 relationship between the ability to generate positive relative to negative future mental 89 imagery and optimism. Such studies have found that, when instructed to deliberately 90 generate mental imagery-based representations of future scenarios in response to emotional 91 verbal cues, lower subjective imagery vividness for positive, but not negative, scenarios is 92 associated with lower optimism (Blackwell et al., 2013; Ji, Holmes, & Blackwell, 2017; 93 Sharot, Riccardi, Raio, & Phelps, 2007). In addition, clinical researchers have found that 94 instructing participants to generate mental imagery representations of one’s best possible 95 self can increase optimism, and to a greater extent than generating mental imagery 96 representations of daily activities (Meevissen, Peters, & Alberts, 2011; Peters, Flink, 97 Boersma, & Linton, 2010). Thus, positive mental imagery about the future appears to be 98 related to one’s level of optimism. However, biases in instructed future mental imagery 99 generation does not directly bear upon biases in one’s tendency to do so spontaneously (i.e. 100 without instructions or task requirements to do so). 101 Preliminary evidence from a recent experience sampling study suggests that 102 anomalies in mental imagery during spontaneous future thinking may be related to optimism 4 103 (Beaty, Seli, & Schacter, 2018). Beaty et al. (2018) sampled the temporal direction and 104 emotional valence of daily cognition across a seven day period, and found that higher 105 optimism was associated with greater emotional positivity of future-oriented thought content, 106 on average (Beaty et al., 2018). Interestingly, optimism was not related to the overall 107 frequency of future thinking, or to the emotional valence of past-oriented thought content. 108 Thus, optimism may be specifically related to positivity of mental imagery-based thoughts 109 during future thinking. However, it is worth noting that the representational format of 110 cognition was not explicitly assessed in Beaty et al. (2018), as the presence mental imagery- 111 based future thinking was indirectly inferred from vividness ratings participants provided for 112 all thoughts. In addition, the valence of thought content was averaged across all future- 113 oriented thoughts, thus whether optimism was associated with higher frequencies of positive 114 relative to negative future thinking remains unclear. 115 The present study 116 While previous research provides encouraging support for the existence of a link 117 between emotional future mental imagery generation and optimism, the relationship between 118 the tendency to generate positive relative to negative mental imagery during spontaneous 119 future thinking and optimism remains to be directly tested. Most importantly, no study has 120 assessed whether optimism mediates the relationship between individual differences in the 121 tendency to generate positive relative to negative mental imagery during future thinking and 122 negative mood. 123 The present study tested the hypothesis that a lower tendency to generate positive 124 relative to negative mental imagery during spontaneous future thinking contributes to lower 125 optimism, which in turn contributes to elevated negative mood. If this is true, then at any 126 given time, individuals experiencing higher negative mood will be less optimistic about the 127 future (prediction 1), and those feeling less optimistic about the future will exhibit lower 128 tendency to generate positive relative to negative mental imagery during spontaneous future 129 thinking (prediction 2). Further, lower optimism will statistically mediate the relationship 5 130 between lower tendency to generate positive relative to negative mental
Recommended publications
  • Neuroticism, BIS, and Reactivity to Discrete Negative Mood Inductions ⇑ Jennifer Thake, John M
    Personality and Individual Differences 54 (2013) 208–213 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Neuroticism, BIS, and reactivity to discrete negative mood inductions ⇑ Jennifer Thake, John M. Zelenski Department of Psychology, Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, ON, Canada K1S 5B6 article info abstract Article history: Research has established relationships between the personality dimensions of neuroticism and BIS and Received 5 March 2012 broad negative emotional reactivity. However, few researchers have examined the relationships among Received in revised form 4 August 2012 neuroticism, BIS, and discrete negative emotional reactivities. The present study examined whether indi- Accepted 27 August 2012 viduals scoring high on neuroticism and BIS were more reactive across four discrete negative mood Available online 25 September 2012 inductions, relative to those scoring low on these traits. Participants (n = 166) completed personality questionnaires, measures of current mood, viewed a specific mood-inducing film clip (sadness, anger, Keywords: fear or disgust) and then reported their moods a second time. Results revealed that neuroticism/BIS Neuroticism was associated with high reactivity to the fear and sadness inductions. Neuroticism/BIS did not predict Behavioral inhibition system Mood anger or disgust reactivity, but neuroticism/BIS and extraversion/BAS interacted in predicting anger. Emotion Although further research is
    [Show full text]
  • Dysphoria As a Complex Emotional State and Its Role in Psychopathology
    Dysphoria as a complex emotional state and its role in psychopathology Vladan Starcevic A/Professor, University of Sydney Faculty of Medicine and Health Sydney, Australia Objectives • Review conceptualisations of dysphoria • Present dysphoria as a transdiagnostic complex emotional state and assessment of dysphoria based on this conceptualisation What is dysphoria? • The term is derived from Greek (δύσφορος) and denotes distress that is hard to bear Dysphoria: associated with externalisation? • “Mixed affect” leading to an “affect of suspicion”1,2 1 Sandberg: Allgemeine Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie und Psychisch-Gerichtl Medizin 1896; 52:619-654 2 Specht G: Über den pathologischen Affekt in der chronischen Paranoia. Festschrift der Erlanger Universität, 1901 • A syndrome that always includes irritability and at least two of the following: internal tension, suspiciousness, hostility and aggressive or destructive behaviour3 3 Dayer et al: Bipolar Disord 2000; 2: 316-324 Dysphoria: associated with internalisation? • Six “dysphoric symptoms”: depressed mood, anhedonia, guilt, suicide, fatigue and anxiety1 1 Cassidy et al: Psychol Med 2000; 30:403-411 Dysphoria: a nonspecific state? • Dysphoria is a “nonspecific syndrome” and has “no particular place in a categorical diagnostic system”1; it is neglected and treated like an “orphan”1 1 Musalek et al: Psychopathol 2000; 33:209-214 • Dysphoria “can refer to many ways of feeling bad”2 2 Swann: Bipolar Disord 2000; 2:325-327 Textbook definitions: dysphoria nonspecific, mainly internalising? • “Feeling
    [Show full text]
  • Reward and Emotion: an Affective Neuroscience Approach
    Reward and emotion: An affective neuroscience approach David Sander1 & Lauri Nummenmaa2 1Swiss Center for Affective Sciences (CISA), Campus Biotech, and Laboratory for the Study of Emotion Elicitation and Expression, Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences (FPSE), University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland 2TurKu PET Centre, TurKu University Hospital, and Department of Psychology, University of TurKu, Finland Address Correspondence to: Lauri Nummenmaa Turku PET Centre c/o Turku University Hospital FI-20520 Turku, Finland Email: [email protected] Tel: +358 50 574 7933 Acknowlegements This study was supported by the Academy oF Finland (grants #294897 and #332225), Sigrid Juselius stiftelse and Signe och Anet Gyllenberg’s stiftelse, and by the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant 100019_188966). DS and LN thank Brian Knutson For in-depth discussions concerning several aspects of this paper. Conflicts of interest None Abstract Pleasure and reward are central for motivation, learning, feeling and allostasis. Although reward is without any doubt an affective phenomenon, there is no consensus concerning its relationship with emotion. In this mini-review we discuss this conceptual issue both from the perspective of theories of reward and emotion as well as human systems neuroimaging. We first describe how the reward process can be understood and dissected as intertwined with the emotion process, in particular in light of the appraisal theories, and then discuss how different facets of the reward process can be studied using neuroimaging and neurostimulation techniques. We conclude that future worK needs to focus on mapping the similarities and differences across stimuli and mechanisms that are involved in reward processing and in emotional processing, and propose that an integrative affective sciences approach would provide means for studying the emotional nature of reward.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter 2 Mood Disorders
    A Report on Mental Illnesses in Canada CHAPTER 2 MOOD DISORDERS Highlights • Mood disorders include major depression, bipolar disorder (combining episodes of both mania and depression) and dysthymia. • Approximately 8% of adults will experience major depression at some time in their lives. Approximately 1% will experience bipolar disorder. • The onset of mood disorders usually occurs during adolescence. • Worldwide, major depression is the leading cause of years lived with disability, and the fourth cause of disability- adjusted life years (DALYs). • Mood disorders have a major economic impact through associated health care costs as well as lost work productivity. • Most individuals with a mood disorder can be treated effectively in the community. Unfortunately, many individuals delay seeking treatment. • Hospitalizations for mood disorders in general hospitals are approximately one and a half times higher among women than men. • The wide disparity among age groups in hospitalization rates for depression in general hospitals has narrowed in recent years, because of a greater decrease in hospitalization rates in older age groups. • Hospitalization rates for bipolar disorder in general hospitals are increasing among women and men between 15 and 24 years of age. • Individuals with mood disorders are at high risk of suicide. 31 A Report on Mental Illnesses in Canada What Are Mood Disorders? Mood disorders may involve depression only with the illness progresses. (also referred to as “unipolar depression”) or Both depressive and manic episodes can they may include manic episodes (as in change the way an individual thinks and bipolar disorder, which is classically known as behaves, and how his/her body functions.
    [Show full text]
  • Mood Effects on Cognition: Affective Influences on the Content and Process of Information Processing and Behavior Joseph P
    CHAPTER 3 Mood Effects on Cognition: Affective Influences on the Content and Process of Information Processing and Behavior Joseph P. Forgas University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia INTRODUCTION How does affect influence our thinking and behavior? This is an age- old question that philosophers, writers, and artists have struggled with since time immemorial. In the last few decades there have been major ad- vances in the experimental study of the ways that mild, everyday affective states, or moods, may influence the way people process social informa- tion—the way they think, remember, and judge the social world around them. Research on the influence of affect on cognition also contributes to the age-old quest to understand the relationship between the rational and the emotional aspects of human nature. These mood-induced influences on the content and process of thinking have very important implications for everyday interpersonal behavior in general, and for human–computer interactions (HCI) in particular. The evidence increasingly suggests that an evolutionary process shaped the development of all affective responses. Thus, temporary experiences of happiness and sadness, in addition to having a positive or negative hedonic quality, also appear to function as useful signals, spontaneously triggering different information processing strategies that appear to be Emotions and Affect in Human Factors and Human–Computer Interaction. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-801851-4.00003-3 Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 89 90 3. MOOD EFFECTS ON COGNITION highly adaptive to the requirements of different social situations. In this way, positive and negative moods may assist people by recruiting infor- mation and processing strategies that are most appropriate to deal with a given situation.
    [Show full text]
  • Coding for Major Depressive Disorder
    ICD‐10 Resource: Coding for Major Depressive Disorder F32. Major depressive disorder, single episode According to the Fifth Edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM‐5) , five or more of the symptoms listed below must be present during the same 2‐week time period that represents changes in functioning. At least one symptom is either a depressed mood or loss of interest. Depressed mood most of the day, nearly every day, as indicated in the subjective report or in observation made by others Markedly diminished interest in pleasure in all, or almost all, activities most of the day and nearly every day Significant weight loss when not dieting or weight gain, for example, more than 5 percent of body weight in a month or changes in appetite nearly every day Insomnia or hypersomnia nearly every day Psychomotor agitation or retardation nearly every day Fatigue or loss of energy nearly every day Feelings of worthlessness or excessive or inappropriate guilt Diminished ability to think or concentrate, or indecisiveness nearly every day Recurrent thoughts of death The ICD‐10 classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders developed in part by the American Psychiatric Association classifies depression by code. In typical, mild, moderate, or severe depressive episodes the patient suffers from lowering of mood, reduction of energy and decrease in activities. Their capacity for enjoyment, interest, and concentration is reduced and is marked by tiredness after even a minimum of effort is common. Sleep patterns are usually disturbed and appetite diminished along with reduced self‐confidence and self‐esteem. Final code selection is based on severity (mild, moderate, severe) and status.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Regulation As a Primary Goal of Treatment
    Suggested APA style reference information can be found at http://www.counseling.org/knowledge-center/vistas Article 35 Feelings Before Facts: Emotion Regulation as a Primary Goal of Treatment Paper based on a program presentation at the 2013 ACA conference, March 20-24, Cincinnati, OH. Rebecca Huggins Huggins, Rebecca L., is a student in the MPC program at Victory University. Rebecca is currently in her first year as a student and has a passion for working with clients who have a history of trauma and suffer with trauma-related disorders. Her research interests are focused on improving the treatment success for people with complex trauma and the potential benefits of emotion focused treatments, and the use of animal assisted therapies. Emotion regulation refers to the ability a person has to influence their emotional responses (Butler, Lee, & Gross, 2007). Attachment theory suggests that emotional regulation skills are developed as a result of interactions between very young children and their families and that these patterns will remain throughout the individual’s lifespan (Ivey, Ivey, Myers, & Sweeney, 2005). Often, these skills are developed before verbal memory recall is possible (Ivey et al., 2005). Many disorders find their roots in the patient’s inability to regulate emotions. Patients with trauma-related disorders, post- traumatic stress disorder (PTSD; the focus for this research), borderline personality disorder (BPD), and some substance use disorders, often have problems with emotional regulation (Frewen, Dozois, Neufeld, & Lanius, 2012). It is important to focus on emotion regulation skills when treating patients with these disorders. A literature review explains various means of emotion regulation, the skills that accompany these, and the success of these skills with clients who have post-traumatic stress disorder, borderline personality disorder, and substance use disorders.
    [Show full text]
  • An Affective Neuroscience Model of Boosting Resilience in Adults
    An Affective Neuroscience Model of Boosting Resilience in Adults Golnaz Tabibnia University of California, Irvine Correspondence should be addressed to: Golnaz Tabibnia Department of Psychological Science University of California, Irvine 4201 Social and Behavioral Sciences Gateway Irvine, CA 92697 (949) 824-3002 Fax [email protected] Page 1 of 123 Abstract Although research has identified dozens of behavioral and psychosocial strategies for boosting resilience in adults, little is known about the common underlying pathways. A comprehensive review of these strategies using an affective neuroscience approach indicates three distinct general routes to resilience (Fig. 1A): 1) down-regulating the negative (e.g., exposure, cognitive reappraisal) by reducing distress-related responses of the amygdala, hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, and autonomic nervous system; 2) up-regulating the positive (e.g., optimism, social connectedness) by activating mesostriatal reward pathways, which in turn can buffer the effects of stress; and 3) transcending the self (e.g., mindfulness, religious engagement) by reducing activation in the default mode network, a network associated with self-reflection, mind-wandering, and rumination. Some strategies (e.g., social support) can boost resilience via more than one pathway. Under- or over-stimulation of a pathway can result in vulnerability, such as over- stimulation of the reward pathway through substance abuse. This tripartite model of resilience-building is testable, accounts for a large body of data on adult resilience, and makes new predictions with implications for practice. Keywords: resilience; well-being; emotion regulation; affective neuroscience; amygdala; stress; reward; default mode Page 2 of 123 1. Introduction Whether surviving a natural disaster, loss of a loved one, or violence, most people experience a traumatic event at some point in their lives.
    [Show full text]
  • Moods and Compliance
    British Journal ofsorial Psychology (1988), 27, 79-90 Printtd in Gmt Britain 79 0 1988 The British Psychological Society Moods and compliance Sandra Milberg Univtrsity of Pittsburgh Margaret S. Clark Carntgit-Mtllon Uniursity, Dtpartmmt of Psychology, Pittsburgh PA IS213, USA This study examined the impact of subjects' moods on their compliance with simple messages. It was predicted and found that recipients of messages: ( 1)complied more when in a happy mood'than when in a neutral state, and (2)complied less when in an angry mood than when in a neutral state. These effects were observed both when high and low pressure messages were used. Possible boundary conditions for the observed effects are discussed. The study described in this paper was designed to test the effects of subjects' moods on compliance with simple messages such as suggestions or orders. It was predicted that there would be (1) more compliance when message recipients were in a positive, happy mood than when in a neutral state and (2) less compliance when message recipients were in a negative, angry mood than when in a neutral state. Several bases exist for these predictions. Why mood should injuence compliance One possible reason why moods might influence compliance with simple messages in the predicted manner is that moods may increase the accessibility of mood-congruent thoughts about the communicator or about the message itself. More specifically, upon listening to a simple message - either a suggestion or a demand - people ordinarily may have a variety of thoughts - some positive, some negative, some neutral - about both the communicator and his or her message.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychological Aftermaths of Pregnancy Loss
    OHSUPsychological Aftermaths of Pregnancy Loss Teni Davoudian, PhD, ABPP Clinical Psychologist Assistant Professor in Psychiatry & Ob/Gyn November 15, 2019 Grief • Grief has physical, emotional, spiritual, and social components • Non-linear process o States rather than stages of grief • OHSUAnger is component of grief 2 Pregnancy Loss Grief • Facilitates bond with deceased as a coping strategy • “Prospective” grieving for the life and relationship projected into the future (versus retrospective grieving) • Peaks at 6 months • Women also grieving: OHSUo Loss of confidence in one’s body o Loss of confidence in medicine o Loss of control 3 Grief vs. Major Depression Grief Major Depression Course Decreases over time Persistent depressed mood Waves of grief (triggered by thoughts/reminders of the deceased) Emotional Normal to experience positive emotions and Pervasive unhappiness Spectrum laughter while grieving Misery Fluctuating ability to feel pleasure Cognitive Thoughts and memories of deceased Self-critical thoughts Processes Negative ruminations Self-Esteem Mostly preserved through grief process Lowered self-esteem OHSU Some concerns about “failing” the deceased Self-loathing Worthlessness Suicidal Ideation If SI occurs, it is in the context of reuniting Focused on ending one’s life with the deceased due to feelings of worthlessness and perceived inability to cope with depression 4 Comorbid Psych Dx • Major Depressive Disorder o RPL: 5x more likely to develop moderate/severe depression (Kolte, 2015) • Generalized anxiety
    [Show full text]
  • An Overview of Mood Disorders/Depression
    An Overview of Mood Disorders/Depression Written by Jennifer L. Horn, PhD, HSPP OVERVIEW OF MOOD DISORDERS Mood disorders are biological disorders that affect children, adolescents, and adults. Depression and other mood disorders are not being ‘down in the dumps’ or easily upset, but serious medical disorders that affect thinking, emotions, and behavior for months to even years if left untreated. Research suggests that about 15- 20% of the population will experience a mood disorder at some point in their life. Females are much more likely to develop a mood disorder, yet males are much more likely to go without treatment. Research has demonstrated physical differences in the brain of people affected with mood disorders. No matter the age of the person, mood disorders present with similar symptoms. Symptoms include sad or unhappy mood that lasts for days at a time, vegetative symptoms such as lack of energy or motivation, somatic symptoms such as changes in eating or sleeping habits, feelings of helplessness, irritability/moodiness, and feelings of hopelessness including suicidal thoughts. Although there is no one cause of mood disorders and depression, research clearly reflects a genetic predisposition to develop a mood disorder. Studies have shown that mood disorders and depression are common among close relatives, and twin studies have also shown a strong genetic basis. Additionally, research has shown that people who tend to be more negative and have low self-esteem are more likely to develop a mood disorder. In addition to the genetic causes of mood disorders, studies have also shown that environmental and interpersonal stressors can trigger mood disorders and depression, especially in those who are already thought to be at risk.
    [Show full text]
  • Self-Generated Thought As the Engine of Neuroticism
    TICS-1465; No. of Pages 7 Opinion Thinking too much: self-generated thought as the engine of neuroticism 1 1 2 3 Adam M. Perkins , Danilo Arnone , Jonathan Smallwood , and Dean Mobbs 1 Department of Psychological Medicine, King’s College London, London, SE5 8AZ, UK 2 Department of Psychology/York Neuroimaging Centre, University of York, Heslington, York, YO10 5DD, UK 3 Department of Psychology, Columbia University, New York, NY 10027, USA Neuroticism is a dimension of personality that captures individual differences perspective, the capacity to think trait individual differences in the tendency to experience beyond the current situation explains the vulnerability negative thoughts and feelings. Established theories that neurotic individuals have for negative thoughts and explain neuroticism in terms of threat sensitivity, but feelings of an apparently abstract nature. Furthermore, have limited heuristic value since they cannot account we now have evidence as to where SGTs come from, since for features of neuroticism that are unrelated to threat, neuroimaging research has identified that the same brain such as creativity and negative psychological states network (the default mode network) is implicated in the experienced in benign, threat-free environments. We episodic memory and semantic processing that occurs address this issue by proposing that neuroticism stems during states of spontaneous thought. The simplest and from trait individual differences in activity in brain cir- most elegant explanation is therefore that we use the cuits that govern the nature of self-generated thought same process to perform all of these different mental (SGT). We argue our theory explains not only the asso- computations and that the tendency to apply these par- ciation of neuroticism with threat sensitivity but also the ticular brain systems to generate negative thoughts and prominence within the neurotic mind of representations feelings will be reflected in high scores on measures of of information that are unrelated to the way the world is neuroticism.
    [Show full text]